Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2021 Feb 22.
Published in final edited form as: J Appl Psychol. 2020 Oct 22;105(12):1382–1396. doi: 10.1037/apl0000831

Socioeconomic Status and Well-Being during COVID-19: A Resource Based Examination

Connie R Wanberg 1, Borbala Csillag 1, Richard P Douglass 2, Le Zhou 1, Michael S Pollard 3
PMCID: PMC7899012  NIHMSID: NIHMS1671318  PMID: 33090858

Abstract

The authors assess levels and within-person changes in psychological well-being (i.e., depressive symptoms and life satisfaction) from before to during the COVID-19 pandemic for individuals in the U.S., in general and by socioeconomic status (SES). The data is from two surveys of 1,143 adults from RAND Corporation’s nationally representative American Life Panel, the first administered between April-June, 2019 and the second during the initial peak of the pandemic in the United States in April, 2020. Depressive symptoms during the pandemic were higher than population norms before the pandemic. Depressive symptoms increased from before to during COVID-19 and life satisfaction decreased. Individuals with higher education experienced a greater increase in depressive symptoms and a greater decrease in life satisfaction from before to during COVID-19 in comparison to those with lower education. Supplemental analysis illustrates that income had a curvilinear relationship with changes in well-being, such that individuals at the highest levels of income experienced a greater decrease in life satisfaction from before to during COVID-19 than individuals with lower levels of income. We draw on conservation of resources theory and the theory of fundamental social causes to examine four key mechanisms (perceived financial resources, perceived control, interpersonal resources, and COVID-19 related knowledge/news consumption) underlying the relationship between SES and well-being during COVID-19. These resources explained changes in well-being for the sample as a whole but did not provide insight into why individuals of higher education experienced a greater decline in well-being from before to during COVID-19.

Keywords: Socioeconomic status, conservation of resources, well-being, COVID-19


“People want to talk about this virus as an equal opportunity pathogen, but it’s really not,” said Dr. Ashwin Vasan, a doctor and public health professor at Columbia University. “It’s going right to the fissures in our society.”

New York Times, April 3, 2020

Socioeconomic disparities across life and health outcomes are large and persistent in the United States and other developed countries (Braveman et al., 2011). Given this, the role of socioeconomic status (i.e., SES, an individual’s objective social or economic position in relation to others) during the COVID-19 pandemic has been scrutinized. For example, individuals of lower SES were more likely to be front line workers with higher potential exposure to the virus (Afridi & Block, 2020). In contrast, individuals of higher SES were more likely to be working or furloughed at home with comforts such as a well-stocked pantry, stable internet, and spacious living arrangements (Reeves & Rothwell, 2020). Despite these differences, a national poll by Axios-Ipsos reported that 47% of Americans of higher SES indicated their emotional well-being had gotten worse because of the pandemic, compared to only 34% of lower SES (Talev, 2020).

The first purpose of this study is to examine levels of psychological well-being (i.e., depressive symptoms and life satisfaction) during the COVID-19 pandemic for individuals in the U.S. in general and by SES. We surveyed 1,143 adults from RAND Corporation’s nationally representative American Life Panel during the first U.S. peak of the pandemic in April, 2020. We compare levels of depressive symptoms during the pandemic to pre-COVID population norms (Tomitaka et al., 2018). Our second purpose is to assess within-person changes in well-being (i.e., changes in depressive symptoms and life satisfaction) from before to during the pandemic--in general and by SES. Finally, drawing upon conservation of resources theory (COR theory; Hobfoll, 1989, 2010) and the theory of fundamental social causes (Link & Phelan, 1995), we examine four resource-based mechanisms (perceived financial resources, perceived control, interpersonal resources, and COVID-19 related knowledge/news consumption) underlying the relationship between SES and well-being levels and changes. We operationalize SES as educational attainment and household income. These are “objective and quantifiable indicators of power, prestige, and control over resources” (Diemer et al., 2013, p. 84). These two distinct facets of SES have incremental predictive validity, with education being more specific to human capital and income being more specific to material capital (Oakes & Rossi, 2003).

Our study provides several contributions to the literature. Despite being fundamental aspects of work, educational attainment and income have received scarce attention in industrial-organizational psychology and management research (Côté, 2011; Leana & Meuris, 2015). We present unique nationally representative data about well-being changes from before to during COVID-19, and test mechanisms underlying differential well-being outcomes for individuals of lower and higher SES. Due to the difficulty of accessing representative samples during crises with measures for comparison before the event, most studies of crisis events include convenience samples and only post-event assessments of well-being (Norris et al., 2002). From a theoretical standpoint, our study addresses the need to better understand the pathways that affect well-being changes for individuals of lower and higher SES amidst public health crises (Galama & van Kippersluis, 2019). From a practical standpoint, organizational practices can perpetuate and are also impacted by socioeconomic inequality (Bapuji et al., 2020a). It is thus informative for managerial practice to elucidate the relationship between SES, well-being, and transmitting mechanisms (Christie & Barling, 2009). Further, efforts to minimize risk for reduced well-being during the COVID-19 pandemic and future public health crises are best informed if there is an understanding of the processes that stimulate reduced well-being (Link & Phelan, 1995).

Theory and Hypothesis Development

COR theory is a leading psychological theory of stress and trauma (Hobfoll et al., 2016). According to the theory, humans acquire and safeguard resources to protect themselves and to ease the challenges of daily life. Resources include valued conditions or situations, personal resources such as self-efficacy, and material or energy resources such as money (Hobfoll, 1989). A first tenet of this theory suggests that when individuals lose or fear losing valued resources, well-being is negatively affected (Hobfoll, 1989, 2010). This reduction in well-being stems from both the instrumental and symbolic value of the lost resources.

COVID-19, recognized as a worldwide pandemic by the World Health Organization (WHO) on March 11, 2020, was associated with myriad resource losses for individuals. Millions of employees were furloughed or laid off from their jobs, or began working under new conditions at home or on site. Childcare center and school closures changed family routines. Supply chain interruptions reduced the availability of everyday supplies. Services, such as grooming, physical therapy, public transportation, and many entertainment options, became unavailable or limited. Individuals were isolated from friends and family. Stock markets plummeted in late March 2020, producing financial loss and fear of further loss.

Consistent with COR theory, mental health concerns have been documented in countries that experienced the COVID-19 outbreak before the U.S. (Qiu et al., 2020). For example, of 52,730 individuals surveyed in China during the pandemic, 35% indicated experiencing psychological distress (Qiu et al, 2020). Another study from China reported that the prevalence of depression was 14.6% in a convenience sample (Lei et al., 2020). While such data is informative, within-person comparisons of well-being from before to during COVID-19 are required to assess whether well-being levels have decreased and the effect size of this decrease. In addition to comparing levels of depressive symptoms during the pandemic in the U.S. to established norms, we also examine within-person changes in both depressive symptoms and life satisfaction from before to during the pandemic. We propose:

  • H1: Depressive symptoms assessed during the pandemic will be significantly higher than previous population norms.

  • H2: Individuals will portray an (a) increase in depressive symptoms and (b) a decrease in life satisfaction from before to during the pandemic.

SES and Well-Being During the Pandemic

According to COR theory, SES is fundamentally linked to the availability of resources (Hobfoll, 2010). Individuals with higher education and income have more resources and are better able to protect the resources they have (Hobfoll, 2010). In contrast, low SES makes it difficult to garner resources, even obstructing the protection of one’s resources. Consistent with the COR premise that people with fewer resources will have lower psychological health, lower SES is associated with more depression (Lorant et al., 2003; Wang et al., 2010) and lower life satisfaction (Pinquart & Sorenson, 2000).

The theory of fundamental social causes specifies that the role of SES in health is so robust and enduring that it is imperative to examine SES inequities and the mechanisms behind these inequities (Phelan et al., 2010). While many resources fit under the umbrella of COR theory, the theory of fundamental social causes identifies four key resources underlying SES inequities (Link & Phelan, 1995). First, perceived financial resources refer to perceived fit between accessible material resources and financial needs and wants (Meuris & Leana, 2018). In general, individuals of lower SES worry more about their financial situation and feel more overwhelmed by their financial obligations than individuals of higher SES (Link & Phelan, 1995). Yet, the same level of income may indicate comfort for one and discomfort for another for stemming from divergent financial responsibilities or expenditures (Leana & Meuris, 2015). Second, SES is related to lower levels of power. While power broadly refers to the ability to administer outcomes such as rewards and punishment (Hinkin & Schriesheim, 1989; Raven, 1993), SES is especially related to perceived control over life outcomes in general—“a sense that one’s actions are chronically influenced by external forces outside of one’s individual control and influence” (Kraus et al., 2012, p. 549). Resources and contexts accompanying lower SES result in more disruption to both perceived and actual control over life constraints (Kraus et al., 2012). Third, interpersonal resources refer to having higher levels of social support, social integration, or connectedness (Link & Phelan, 1995). Lower SES is related to smaller social networks and more social isolation and loneliness (Algren et al., 2020; House et al., 1988). Lastly, knowledge refers to possessing facts or information that allow an accurate awareness of a topic. In general, individuals of lower SES have less health-related knowledge (Phelan et al, 2010). Supporting this idea, research early in the COVID-19 crisis found lower COVID-related knowledge among individuals of lower SES (Cutler et al., 2020).

Each of these resources is important to the maintenance of well-being. Specifically, individuals with lower perceived financial resources have lower levels of mental health and life satisfaction stemming from deleterious cognitive energy devoted to fear and discontent about their situation (Meuris & Leana, 2018). Lower sense of control is related to lower well-being because individuals feel powerless about their decisions and influence over others (Anderson et al., 2012; Cheng et al., 2013). With respect to interpersonal resources, social isolation—which can contribute to individuals feeling a lack of support and interpersonal connections—has been associated with risk of depressive symptoms (Teo et al., 2013). Knowledge has been shown to be related to higher levels of physical health—individuals with more health-related knowledge know how to stay healthy (Phelan et al., 2010). The relationship between knowledge and psychological well-being is more ambiguous. In the case of COVID-19, there have been indications that higher satisfaction with knowledge about the virus is related to lower depression (Wang et al., 2020), perhaps because individuals feel efficacious about precautionary measures. Higher consumption of news during a pandemic may reduce psychological well-being, however, perhaps because it amplifies fear and awareness of suffering (Ornell et al, 2020). Indeed, the WHO urged individuals to “minimize watching, reading, or listening to news about COVID-19 that causes you to feel anxious or distressed” (WHO, 2020, p.1). We posit:

  • H3: SES will be (a) negatively associated with depressive symptoms and (b) positively associated with life satisfaction during the pandemic.

  • H4: SES will be positively associated with (a) perceived financial resources, (b) perceived control, (c) interpersonal resources, and (d) COVID-related knowledge and news consumption during the pandemic. These resources will mediate the relationship between SES and depressive symptoms and life satisfaction during the pandemic.

SES and Changes of Well-Being from Before to During COVID-19

The previous hypotheses do not address whether or how well-being might differentially change for individuals of lower and higher SES from before to during COVID-19. Another aspect of COR theory suggests individuals of lower SES may be more likely to experience a greater decline in well-being during a crisis event in comparison to those of higher SES. Specifically, COR theory suggests that individuals with compromised resources are most vulnerable to additional resource loss. In contrast, individuals with higher SES are more likely to have plentiful resource caravans that can be drawn upon to stave off negative emotions and cognitions, and assist overall coping (Hobfoll, 2010). While few studies have examined SES and well-being in the context of crises with pre and post measures (Norris et al, 2002), a few studies support the premise that people with lower SES have the largest decrease in well-being after a crisis (Ginexi et al., 2000; Phifer, 1990). For example, following the 1984 Kentucky flood, individuals of lower SES reported greater increases in depression and anxiety (Phifer, 1990).

In contrast to these findings, as mentioned in our opening paragraph, a nationally representative survey of 1,355 U.S. adults early in the pandemic found that more individuals of higher SES indicated their emotional well-being had gotten worse than individuals of lower SES (Talev, 2020). Although counterintuitive, situationally-specific lower well-being among individuals who typically have more resources is also acknowledged by COR theory. Reduced well-being depends on how one’s unique resources contract in a specific situation (Hobfoll, 2010; Hobfoll et al., 2003). It is possible that loss of resources during COVID-19 may have occurred differentially for individuals of higher and lower SES. For example, higher SES could have been associated with greater loss of interpersonal resources, given that individuals of higher SES were more likely to be newly working at home during the pandemic than those of lower SES. Or, given individuals lower in SES already tend to have low perceived control, individuals of higher SES may have had a relatively bigger drop in perceived control due to COVID-19 related uncertainties. Considering the above, we propose the following competing hypotheses:

  • H5a: There will be a larger increase in depressive symptoms and a larger decrease in life satisfaction from before to during COVID-19 for lower (vs. higher) SES individuals.

  • H5b: There will be a larger increase in depressive symptoms and a larger decrease in life satisfaction from before to during COVID-19 for higher (vs. lower) SES individuals.

Only limited research has empirically examined the role of differential resource loss in explaining SES differences in well-being change (Kiviruusu et al., 2013). A given crisis can affect resources differentially for individuals of higher and lower SES (Warr & Aung, 2019). Data available from a previous assessment of the American Life Panel allowed us to assess actual changes from before to during COVID-19 in two of the resources central to our theorizing: perceived control and interpersonal resources (Link & Phelan, 1995). We examine whether decreases in perceived control and interpersonal resources mediate the relationship between SES and changes in well-being. We also examine whether lower levels of the other resources during COVID-19 (perceived financial resources, COVID-related knowledge, and COVID-related news consumption) explain changes in well-being. Based on the components of both COR theory and the theory of fundamental social causes, we propose:

  • H6: Declines in (a) perceived control and (b) interpersonal resources as well as levels of (c) perceived financial resources and (d) COVID-related knowledge and COVID-related news consumption will mediate the relationship between SES and changes in depressive symptoms and life satisfaction from before to during the pandemic.

Method

Participants and Procedure

We surveyed the RAND American Life Panel (ALP; Pollard & Baird, 2017), a probability-sample based, nationally representative sample. Time 1 (T1) of our study took place before COVID-19 in April-June, 2019 (i.e. the “Health Networks Study”) which focused on U.S. adults between ages 30 and 80. Time 2 (T2) data was collected April 16–19, 2020, during the first estimated 2020 peak of the pandemic in the U.S. as measured by deaths per day and hospital resource use (IHME, 2020). Perceived control, interpersonal resources, depressive symptoms, and life satisfaction were assessed at both T1 and T2. Perceived financial resources, COVID-related knowledge, and COVID-related news consumption were assessed at T2 and were not available at T1. ALP panelists are invited to update demographic and general health status three times a year. An invitation to our study was sent to 1,771 panelists who completed the T1 survey; 1,143 responded (64.5%).1 Demographic data for the sample is shown in Table 1.

Table 1.

Demographic Characteristics of Participants (N = 1,143)

Characteristic n %
Age
 30–39 130 11.4
 40–49 181 15.8
 50–59 215 18.8
 60–69 296 25.9
 70–79 293 25.6
 80–81 28 2.5
Gender
 Male 508 44.4
 Female 635 55.6
Racioethnicity
 Non-Hispanic White 841 73.6
 Non-Hispanic Black 108 9.4
 Hispanic 137 12.0
 Asian or Pacific Islander 34 3.0
 Other 23 2.0
Employment Status in April 2020
 Unemployed and looking for work prior to COVID-19 21 1.8
 Full-time employee 396 34.6
 Part-time employee 60 5.2
 Laid off due to COVID-19 40 3.5
 Furloughed due to COVID-19 32 2.8
 Freelancing or self-employed 79 6.9
 Disabled 59 5.2
 Retired 371 32.5
 Homemaker 42 3.7
 Other 43 3.8
Household Income reported in 2020
 Less than $5,000 19 1.7
 $5,000 to $7,499 8 0.7
 $7,500 to $9,999 16 1.4
 $10,000 to $12,499 17 1.5
 $12,500 to $14,999 16 1.4
 $15,000 to $19,999 38 3.3
 $20,000 to $24,999 46 4.0
 $25,000 to $29,999 53 4.6
 $30,000 to $34,999 59 5.2
 $35,000 to $39,999 42 3.7
 $40,000 to $49,999 93 8.1
 $50,000 to $59,999 101 8.8
 $60,000 to $74,999 156 13.6
 $75,000 to $99,999 134 11.7
 $100,000 to $124,999 122 10.7
 $125,000 to $199,999 139 12.2
 $200,000 or more 81 7.1
Education
 Less than high school 5 .4
 Some high school, no diploma 29 2.5
 High school graduate or equivalent 117 10.2
 Some college, no degree 219 19.2
 Associate degree 152 13.3
 Bachelor’s degree 333 29.1
 Master’s degree 204 17.8
 Professional school degree 41 3.6
 Doctorate degree 43 3.8

Note. Frequencies listed in this table are unweighted. Household income does not total to 1,143 due to missing responses. Respondents were or had previously been employed (e.g., retired, unemployed) in a wide range of occupations, with the three most frequent being education (14%), managerial (10%), and office and administrative (9%).

Measures

The complete list of items in each measure is included in Appendix A. Socioeconomic status was measured with (1) educational attainment assessed in early 2020 and (2) annual household income assessed in 2019 and 2020, averaged across these two assessments. Respondents reported their highest level of education on a 9-point scale (1 = less than high school, 9 = doctorate degree). There was a .98 and above correlation between education used in our analyses and four previous measurements, providing evidence for the reliability of this measure in this sample. Household income represents the total combined income of all family members 15 years or older who lived in the household over the past year on a 17-point scale (1 = less than $5,000, 17 = $200,000 or above). Combining multiple recent years of household income is recommended because household income can change from year to year, especially for individuals of lower SES (Diemer et al, 2013).2

Perceived financial resources

Perceived financial resources were measured by 4 items (Meuris & Leana, 2018) on a 5-point scale (1 = never to 5 = always). Higher scores reflect more satisfaction with, and less worry about, one’s financial resources. Perceived control was measured with 7 items (Lachman & Weaver, 1998) on a 4-point scale (1 = strongly disagree to 4 =strongly agree). Interpersonal resources were assessed with 3 items from the revised UCLA Loneliness Scale (Hughes et al., 2004) on a 3-point scale (1 = hardly ever to 3 = often). This scale is strongly correlated with a longer version (r = .82; Hughes et al., 2004). Higher scores reflect more interpersonal resources. COVID-related knowledge (1 = not at all knowledgeable to 5 = extremely knowledgeable for knowledge) and news consumption (1 = never to 5 = a great deal) were assessed with single items on 5-point scales.

Depressive symptoms

Depressive symptoms were measured by the Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ-8; Kroenke et al., 2009), on a 4-point scale (0 = not at all to 3 = nearly every day). The sum of item responses indicates more depressive symptoms. The PHQ-8 has a high 48-hour test-retest reliability and construct validity as a diagnostic measure (Kroenke et al., 2001; Kroenke et al, 2009). Life satisfaction was assessed with a single item (1 = very dissatisfied to 10 = very satisfied; Kobau et al., 2010). This item is strongly correlated (r = .75; Kobau et al, 2010) with the multi-item Satisfaction with Life Scale (Diener et al., 1985).

Control variables

Control variables included age, racioethnicity (four dummy variables with non-Hispanic White as the referent category, compared to non-Hispanic Black, Hispanic, Asian or Pacific Islander, and Others; 0 = no, 1 = yes), gender (0 = male, 1 = female), and general health status (1 = poor to 5 = excellent). Age and general health status were controlled because they are risk factors for COVID-19 (Bhargava et al., 2020; Mayo Clinic, 2020). Racioethnicity was controlled because racial disparities exist in health outcomes above and beyond SES differences (House & Williams, 2000). We controlled for gender because meta-analytic findings suggest there are more depressive symptoms among women than men (d = 0.27; Salk et al., 2017).3

Analytic Strategy

Weights were incorporated in the estimation of coefficients and standard errors (SEs) in order to account for sampling bias (Asparouhov, 2009).4 We tested H1 using a one-sample t-test and H2 using a paired-sample t-test, with SPSS version 24. To test H3 and H4 we ran structural equation modeling (SEM). To test H5 and H6, we used latent change score (LCS) modeling. In analyses for H3–H6 latent factors were specified for multiple-indicator variables (see McArdle, 2009) and the analyses were conducted in Mplus version 8.3 (Muthén & Muthén, 2017). We used the Monte Carlo method to construct 95% confidence intervals (CIs) of indirect and total effects in R version 4.0.0 using the Modern Applied Statistics with S (MASS) package (Venables & Ripley, 2002).5 Complete responses were provided by 1,117 (98%) respondents. Following Newman (2014), we used maximum likelihood estimation to treat missing data, which also allowed us to utilize the sampling weights.

Results

Preliminary Analyses Results.

Means, standard deviations (SDs), correlations, and alphas for study variables are shown in Table 2. For the repeatedly measured multiple-item scales (i.e., perceived control, interpersonal resources, and depressive symptoms), a confirmatory factor analysis model with time-varying factor loadings did not fit the data significantly better than a model with fixed factor loadings (Satorra-Bentler scaled Δχ2 = 22.16, Δdf = 15, p > .05), supporting measurement equivalence of these scales at T1 and T2.

Table 2.

Means, Standard Deviations, Bivariate Correlations, and Cronbach’s Alpha Coefficients of Study Variables

Variable M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20
1. Gender (0 = male, 1= female) .52 .50
2. Non-Hispanic Black (0 = no, 1 = yes) .09 .28 .09**
3. Hispanic (0 = no, 1 = yes) .24 .42 −.01 −.17**
4. Asian/Pacific Islander (0 = no, 1 = yes) .04 .19 −.10** −.06* −.11**
5. Other racioethnicity (0 = no, 1 = yes) .02 .14 .02 −.05 −.08** −.03
6. Age 53.27 13.67 .03 −.01 −.35** −.10** −.03
7. General health status 3.44 .94 .00 −.09** −.18** .08* −.07* .02
8. Education 4.57 1.81 .03 −.04 −.19** .15** −.04 .00 .27**
9. Income 11.77 3.96 −.13** −.20** −.18** .11** −.08** .05 .28** .45**
10. Perceived financial resources during COVID 3.36 .96 −.06 −.07* −.20** .09** −.03 .27** .31** .24** .37** (.87)
11. Perceived control pre COVID 3.08 .56 .00 .04 −.05 −.05 .01 .16** .27** .14** .20** .34** (.81)
12. Perceived control during COVID 3.02 .54 −.08* .00 −.08** −.05 .01 .17** .33** .10* .19** .45** .63** (.83)
13. Interpersonal resources pre COVID 2.45 .59 −.03 −.07* −.07* .00 .01 .18** .27** .05 .20** .36** .49** .47** (.82)
14. Interpersonal resources during COVID 2.33 .61 −.07* .04 −.05 −.02 .00 .12** .15** .01 .13** .32** .27** .43** .42** (.83)
15. COVID-related knowledge 3.79 .95 .03 −.08* −.25** −.02 −.07* .31** .12** .18** .15** .14** .13** .16** .06* −.01
16. COVID-related news consumption 4.19 .93 .03 .05 −.17** −.17** −.09** .19** −.02 .11** .15** −.05 .08* .05 .01 .01 .37**
17. Depressive symptoms pre COVID 4.18 4.60 .06* −.01 .14** −.07* .03 −.22** −.40** −.11** −.17** −.39** −.49** −.52** −.54** −.26** −.09* .06* (.89)
18. Depressive symptoms during COVID 4.77 4.83 .07* −.01 .13** −.02 −.03 −.25** −.37** −.02 −.14** −.47** −.37** −.54** −.43** −.54** −.08* .06 .56** (.89)
19. Life satisfaction pre COVID 7.76 1.79 −.01 .06* −.10** .01 .01 .14** .41** .07* .18** .33** .43** .41** .46** .27** −.03 −.01 −.50** −.38**
20. Life satisfaction during COVID 7.10 1.90 −.11** .04 .00 .03 .03 .19** .33** −.04 .11** .48** .39** .55** .44** .44** .05 −.10* −.48** −.59** .57**

Note. N = 1117–1143. Racioethnicity is coded as four dummy variables with non-Hispanic White as the referent. Following convention for the PHQ-8 instrument, depressive symptoms are operationalized as the sum of all item scores. Cronbach’s alpha coefficients are reported inside the parentheses along the diagonal.

*

p < .05.

**

p < .01.

H1 and H2 Results.

Using population norms from Tomitaka et al. (2018), one-sample t-test results showed that depressive symptoms during the COVID-19 pandemic (M = 4.77, SD = 4.83) were higher than population norms before the pandemic (M = 3.16; t = 11.28, df = 1142, p < .01, Cohen’s d = .33; see Table 3). Therefore, H1 was supported. Using a paired-sample t-test, results showed that depressive symptoms increased from before (M = 4.18, SD = 4.60) to during COVID-19 (M = 4.77, SD = 4.83; t = 4.55, df = 1142, p < .01, Cohen’s d = .13), supporting H2a. Life satisfaction decreased from before (M = 7.76, SD = 1.79) to during COVID-19 (M = 7.12, SD = 1.90; t = 12.52, df = 1115, p < .01, Cohen’s d = .38), supporting H2b.

Table 3.

Item-level Comparisons on Patient Health Questionnaire (Depressive Symptoms) to Previous Population Norms

Item Present Sample
(N = 1141–1143)
Comparison Sample
(N = 5924)
One-sample t-test results
1. Little interest or pleasure in doing things .67 (.82) .40 11.00**
2. Feeling down, depressed, or hopeless .57 (.75) .35 9.94**
3. Trouble falling or staying asleep, or sleeping too much .85 (.93) .62 8.23**
4. Feeling tired or having little energy .89 (.89) .77 4.53**
5. Poor appetite or overeating .76 (.97) .40 12.68**
6. Feeling bad about yourself – or that you are a failure or have let yourself or your family down .41 (.72) .26 6.93**
7. Trouble concentrating on things, such as reading the newspaper or watching television .43 (.73) .19 10.95**
8. Moving or speaking so slowly that other people could have noticed? Or the opposite – being so fidgety or restless that you have been moving around a lot more than usual .21 (.57) .18 1.60
Total Scale 4.77 (4.83) 3.16 11.28**

Note. Each item is answered on a 4-point scale (0 = not at all to 3 = nearly every day). Higher scores indicate higher levels of depressive symptoms. Under the “Present Sample” column, weighted means are reported with SDs listed inside the parentheses. N = 1141 for Item 4 and N = 1143 for the other items. Comparison sample taken from Tomitaka et al. (2018).

*

p < .05,

**

p < .01,

two-tailed tests.

H3 and H4 Results.

We specified a model that included the effects of education and income on the mediators which in turn had effects on depressive symptoms and life satisfaction during COVID-19. As reported in Table 4, the model explained 62% and 49% of the variance in depressive symptoms and life satisfaction during COVID-19, respectively. Estimates of indirect effects (to test H4) and total effects (to test H3) are reported in Table 5. In contrast to expectations for H3, education had a positive, rather than negative, total effect on depressive symptoms during COVID (total effect = .040, 95% CI = [.022, .060]; H3a) and a negative, rather than positive, total effect on life satisfaction during COVID-19 (total effect = −.169, 95% CI = [−.221, −.116]; H3b). Consistent with expectations for H3, income had a negative total effect on depressive symptoms (total effect = −.014, 95% CI = [−.026, −.002]; H3a) and a positive total effect on life satisfaction (total effect = .055, 95% CI = [.024, .086]; H3b). With regard to H4, education was positively related to COVID-related knowledge (β = .07, SE = .03, p < .01), but COVID-related knowledge was not a significant mediator of the relationships between education and depressive symptoms and life satisfaction (see Table 5). Income was positively related to perceived financial resources (β = .06, SE = .01, p < .01), perceived control (β = .01, SE = .00, p < .05), interpersonal resources (β = .02, SE = .01, p < .05), and COVID-related news consumption (β = .03, SE = .01, p < .05). Each of these resources mediated the relationship between income and depressive symptoms and life satisfaction during COVID-19 (see Table 5). Overall, H4a-d received partial support with regard to income but not education.

Table 4.

Estimates of Unstandardized Structural Path Coefficients Predicting Well-being During COVID-19

Predictor Perceived financial resources during COVID Perceived control during COVID Interpersonal resources during COVID COVID-related knowledge COVID-related news consumption Depressive symptoms during COVID Life satisfaction during COVID
Intercept 3.89 (.08)** 4.28 (.08)** 6.99 (.13)**
Control variables
Gender (0 = male, 1= female) −.05 (.07) −.03 (.02) −.07 (.05) .06 (.08) .05 (.08) .01 (.04) −.22 (.13)
Non-Hispanic Black (0 = no, 1 = yes) .00 (.11) .06 (.03)* .19 (.08)* −.31 (.15)* .09 (.13) −.04 (.07) .61 (.19)**
Hispanic (0 = no, 1 = yes) −.04 (.11) .02 (.03) .05 (.09) −.34 (.13)** −.30 (.13)* −.02 (.07) .69 (.19)**
Asian/Pacific Islander (0 = no, 1 = yes) .24 (.17) −.07 (.05) −.07 (.15) −.19 (.15) −.95 (.47)* −.15 (.11) .52 (.38)
Other racioethnicity (0 = no, 1 = yes) .13 (.13) .10 (.07) .14 (.18) −.47 (.25) −.62 (.23)** −.17 (.11) .68 (.35)*
Age .02 (.00)** .003 (.00)** .01 (.00)** .02 (.00)** .01 (.00)* −.01 (.00)** .01 (.01)**
General health status .19 (.04)** .07 (.02)** .10 (.03)** .04 (.05) −.10 (.05)* −.14 (.03)** .39 (.08)**
SES
Education .03 (.02) .00 (.01) −.03 (.02) .07 (.03)** .04 (.04) .03 (.01)* −.15 (.04)**
Income .06 (.01)** .01 (.00)* .02 (.01)* .01 (.01) .03 (.01)* .01 (.01) −.01 (.02)
Mediators
Perceived financial resources during COVID −.12 (.04)** .53 (.12)**
Perceived control during COVID −.99 (.37)** 2.97 (.78)**
Interpersonal resources during COVID −.42 (.06)** .71 (.15)**
COVID-related knowledge −.01 (.02) .04 (.08)
COVID-related news consumption .05 (.03)* −.16 (.08)*
Residual variances .45 (.06)** .03 (.01)* .29 (.03)** .75 (.05)** .76 (.05)** .15 (.02)** 1.83 (.12)**
R2 .30 .22 .08 .16 .12 .62 .49

Note. N = 1143. Racioethnicity is coded as four dummy variables with non-Hispanic White as the referent category. Age, general health status, education, and income were mean centered. SEs are reported inside the parentheses.

*

p < .05,

**

p < .01,

two-tailed tests. When only education was included as a predictor, it had significant positive effects on perceived financial resources during COVID, COVID-related knowledge, COVID-related news consumption, and depressive symptoms during COVID, and a significant negative effect on life satisfaction during COVID. When only income was included as a predictor, it had significant positive effects on perceived financial resources during COVID, perceived control during COVID, interpersonal resources during COVID, and COVID-related news consumption, and a significant negative effect on life satisfaction during COVID. All the effects of the mediators on the outcomes remained the same in these alternative models.

Table 5.

Indirect Effects, Direct Effects, and Total Effects Between SES and Well-being During COVID-19

Predictor Outcome Indirect effect via… Direct effect Total effect
Perceived financial resources during COVID Perceived control during COVID Interpersonal resources during COVID COVID-related knowledge COVID-related news consumption
Education Depressive symptoms during COVID −.003
[−.010, .002]
.003
[−.009, .016]
.011
[−.001, .023]
−.001
[−.004, .003]
.002
[−.002, .008]
.029
[.005, .053]
.040
[.022, .060]
Income Depressive symptoms during COVID −.007
[−.012, −.002]
−.009
[−.018, −.001]
−.009
[−.017, −.002]
.000
[−.001, .001]
.002
[.000, .004]a
.009
[−.006, .024]
−.014
[−.026, −.002]
Education Life satisfaction during COVID .015
[−.009, .043]
−.010
[−.045, .026]
−.018
[−.043, .002]
.003
[−.008, .017]
−.006
[−.024, .004]
−.153
[−.223, −.083]
−.169
[−.221, −.116]
Income Life satisfaction during COVID .030
[.015, .048]
.027
[.006, .047]
.016
[.003, .033]
.000
[−.002, .003]
−.005
[−.013, .000]b
−.012
[−.053, .029]
.055
[.024, .086]

Note. 95% CIs are reported inside the brackets. 95% CIs that excluded zero are bolded. 20,000 replications were used in the Monte Carlo procedure.

a

The lower limit of this 95% CI was .00004 (with the 90% CI lower limit being .002). The 95% CI therefore did not include zero.

b

The upper limit of this 95% CI was −.000004 (with the 90% CI upper limit being −.001). The 95% CI therefore did not include zero. We interpret these indirect effects as statistically significant, although we encourage future research to further examine the robustness of these effects.

H5 and H6 Results.

We specified a LCS model including the effects of education and income on perceived financial resources during COVID-19, change in perceived control and interpersonal resources, and COVID-related knowledge and news consumption, which in turn had effects on changes in depressive symptoms and life satisfaction. As shown in Table 6, the model explained 28% and 21% of the variance in changes in depressive symptoms and life satisfaction, respectively. A latent change score represents the difference between T1 and T2 (T2 = T1 + change).6 A positive (negative) coefficient of a predictor on a latent change score means a higher level of the predictor is associated with a larger increase (decrease) in the outcome.

Table 6.

Estimates of Unstandardized Structural Path Coefficients Predicting Change in Well-being

Predictor Perceived financial resources during COVID Change in perceived control Change in interpersonal resources COVID-related knowledge COVID-related news consumption Change in depressive symptoms Change in life satisfaction
Intercept .01 (.04) −.06 (.07) 3.89 (.08)** 4.28 (.08)** .08 (.04) −.60 (.15)**
Control variables
Gender (0 = male, 1= female) −.05 (.07) −.08 (.05) −.05 (.06) .06 (.08) .05 (.08) −.04 (.05) −.26 (.14)
Non-Hispanic Black (0 = no, 1 = yes) .00 (.11) −.09 (.08) .19 (.09)* −.31 (.15)* .09 (.13) .02 (.08) .15 (.20)
Hispanic (0 = no, 1 = yes) −.04 (.11) −.07 (.07) −.07 (.10) −.34 (.13)** −.30 (.13)* −.06 (.08) .70 (.25)**
Asian/Pacific Islander (0 = no, 1 = yes) .24 (.17) −.02 (.12) −.10 (.14) −.19 (.15) −.95 (.47)* .02 (.10) .28 (.31)
Other racioethnicity (0 = no, 1 = yes) .13 (.13) −.05 (.11) −.04 (.24) −.47 (.25) −.62 (.23)** −.27 (.13)* .37 (.24)
Age .02 (.00)** .00 (.00) .00 (.00) .02 (.00)** .01 (.00)* .00 (.00) .00 (.01)
General health status .19 (.04)** .03 (.02) −.06 (.04) .04 (.05) −.10 (.05)* −.01 (.03) −.13 (.07)
SES
Education .03 (.02) −.02 (.02) .00 (.02) .07 (.03)** .04 (.04) .03 (.01)* −.11 (.04)*
Income .06 (.01)** .00 (.01) .00 (.01) .01 (.01) .03 (.01)* .00 (.01) −.03 (.03)
Mediators
Perceived financial resources during COVID −.08 (.05) .54 (.15)**
Change in perceived control −.28 (.09)** .76 (.28)**
Change in interpersonal resources −.36 (.07)** .47 (.15)**
COVID-related knowledge −.01 (.03) .28 (.09)**
COVID-related news consumption −.01 (.03) −.19 (.08)*
Residual variances .46 (.06)** .14 (.02)** .26 (.04)** .75 (.05)** .76 (.05)** .18 (.03)** 2.28 (.25)**
R2 .29 .03 .04 .16 .12 .28 .21

Note. N = 1143. Racioethnicity is coded as four dummy variables with non-Hispanic White as the referent category. Age, general health status, education, and income were mean centered. SEs are reported inside the parentheses.

*

p < .05,

**

p < .01,

two-tailed tests. When only education was included as a predictor, it had significant positive effects on perceived financial resources during COVID, COVID-related knowledge, COVID-related news consumption, and change in depressive symptoms, and had a significant negative effect on change in life satisfaction. When only income was included as a predictor, it had significant positive effects on perceived financial resources during COVID and COVID-related news consumption, and significant negative effect on change in life satisfaction but was not significantly related to change in depressive symptoms. All the effects of the mediators on the outcomes remained the same in these alternative models.

Estimates of indirect effects (to test H6) and total effects (to test H5) of education and income on changes in depressive symptoms and life satisfaction are reported in Table 7. Of our competing hypotheses, H5a was not supported. In partial support of H5b, individuals with higher (vs. lower) education reported a larger increase in depressive symptoms (total effect = .032, 95% CI = [.014, .049]) and a larger decrease in life satisfaction (total effect = −.094, 95% CI = [−.137, −.042]) from before to during COVID-19. Income did not have significant total effects on changes in depressive symptoms or life satisfaction. Among the mediators hypothesized in H6, three had significant effects (see Table 7). Mediated by COVID-related knowledge, higher education was associated with a larger increase in life satisfaction (H6d), although overall higher education was associated with a larger decrease in life satisfaction. Mediated by perceived financial resources, higher income was associated with a larger increase in life satisfaction (H6c). However, mediated by COVID-related news consumption, higher income was associated with a larger decrease in life satisfaction (H6d). In other words, these two mediators of income operated in opposite directions, with financial resources contributing to an increase and COVID-related news consumption contributing to a decrease in life satisfaction.

Table 7.

Indirect Effects, Direct Effects, and Total Effects Between SES and Change in Well-being

Predictor Outcome Indirect effect via… Direct effect Total effect
Perceived financial resources during COVID Change in perceived control Change in interpersonal resources COVID-related knowledge COVID-related news consumption
Education Change in depressive symptoms −.002
[−.009, .002]
.004
[−.003, .015]
.001
[−.014, .012]
−.001
[−.005, .004]
−.001
[−.005, .003]
.031
[.004, .057]
.032
[.014, .049]
Income Change in depressive symptoms −.005
[−.011, .001]
.000
[−.005, .004]
.001
[−.005, .009]
.000
[−.001, .001]
.000
[−.004, .002]
.002
[−.017, .020]
−.003
[−.013, .008]
Education Change in life satisfaction .015
[−.009, .048]
−.012
[−.037, .012]
−.001
[−.016, .019]
.019
[.003, .040]
−.007
[−.023, .007]
−.108
[−.191 −.025]
−.094
[−.137, −.042]
Income Change in life satisfaction .030
[.014, .050]
.001
[−.014, .010]
−.002
[−.012, .008]
.002
[−.006, .010]
−.006
[−.015, −.0004]
−.029
[−.077, .020]
−.003
[−.030, .021]

Note. 95% CIs are reported inside the brackets. 95% CIs that excluded zero are bolded. 20,000 replications were used in the Monte Carlo procedure.

Additional Findings.

First, results from the null LCS model for within-person changes in well-being show additional support for H2. There was a significant increase in depressive symptoms (α = .09, SE = .03, p < .01) and a significant decrease in life satisfaction (α = −.65, SE = .08, p < .01) from before to during COVID-19. Second, while changes in resources did not substantially explain the differences in well-being change for individuals of higher and lower SES, they did for the sample in general (see Table 6). For example, individuals who experienced a reduction in perceived control and interpersonal resources experienced a larger increase in depressive symptoms. Finally, as a supplemental analysis we examined the quadratic effects of education and income on the mediators and well-being. As shown in Figures 1 and 2, we found a significant curvilinear relationship between education and depressive symptoms during COVID-19 (β = −.01, SE = .01, p < .05), and between income and change in life satisfaction from before to during COVID-19 (β = −.01, SE = .00, p < .01). Figure 2 portrays alternative support for H5b, for income. Education and income did not have significant curvilinear effects on the mediators. Full tables for the results in Figures 1 and 2 are hosted on the Open Science Framework platform.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Curvilinear relationship between education and depressive symptoms during COVID-19

Note. “Lower” = One SD below the Mean = 2.76, which, when rounded up, corresponds with high school graduate or equivalent. “Medium” = Mean = 4.57, which, when rounded up, corresponds with Associate degree. “Higher” = One SD above the Mean = 6.38, which, when rounded down, corresponds with Bachelor’s degree.

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Curvilinear relationship between income and change in life satisfaction from before to during COVID-19

Note. “Lower” = One SD below the Mean = 7.81, which, when rounded up, corresponds with $25,000-$29,999. “Medium” = Mean = 11.77, which, when rounded up, corresponds with $50,000-$59,999. “Higher” = One SD above the Mean = 15.73, which, when rounded up, corresponds with $125,000-$199,999.

Discussion

A nationally representative sample in the U.S. displayed an increase in depressive symptoms and a decrease in life satisfaction from before to during COVID-19. Levels of depressive symptoms during COVID-19 were also higher than previously established norms (Tomitaka et al., 2018).

Contributing to the important goal of illustrating how the pandemic is affecting individuals of lower and higher SES, our study showed that during the first peak of the pandemic in the U.S., higher education was positively associated with depressive symptoms and negatively associated with life satisfaction. This was contrary to expectations because individuals with lower SES generally have lower well-being. Consistent with expectations, higher income was associated with lower depressive symptoms and higher life satisfaction during the pandemic.

Assessment of change from before to during the pandemic is important to diagnose how the pandemic affected well-being. Individuals with higher education experienced a greater increase in depressive symptoms and a greater decrease in life satisfaction from before to during COVID-19 than individuals with lower education. Income did not have linear relationship with changes in well-being, but supplemental analysis supported a curvilinear relationship showing that individuals at highest levels of income experienced a greater decrease in life satisfaction from before to during COVID-19 than individuals with lower levels of income (see Figure 2).

These findings provide a partial replication of the Axios-Ipsos poll, which indicated that in the U.S., a higher proportion of higher SES individuals reported a decline in their emotional well-being due to the pandemic than those of lower SES (Talev, 2020). A major difference between our study and the Axios-Ipsos poll (beyond our use of comparison data from before the pandemic) is their use of an income and education composite to index SES. Income and education capture different parts of SES and can result in divergent empirical findings (e.g., Christie & Barling, 2009; DeGarmo et al., 1999), which we also reveal in this study.

We examined four resource-based mechanisms to try to explain how SES may transmit to lower and reduced well-being. Tested mediators did not provide good explanatory value, especially for the effect of education. The one significant mediator, COVID-related knowledge, contributed to an increase in life satisfaction from before to during COVID-19, rather than a decrease. As such, COVID-related knowledge was not a valuable explanatory mechanism to explain why individuals with more education displayed an overall well-being decline. Further insight is thus needed. In supplemental analyses, education was not associated with job loss due to COVID-19 (r = −.06, p > .05). We also added having experienced job loss (furloughed or laid off) due to COVID-19 as another control variable. Results were consistent with or without this control. An unmeasured explanation is the increase in work responsibility that individuals of higher education may have encountered. The pandemic meant that many managers had to lead their business units and teams through staffing changes such as layoffs or pay cuts, producing substantial stress (Knight, 2020). Further, educational attainment is a key predictor of participation in the stock market (Cooper & Zhu, 2016), which represents a nuanced aspect of financial resources that our measure might not have fully captured. In the few weeks preceding our T2 assessment, the Dow Jones Industrial Average lost one-third of its total value (S&P Dow Jones Indices, 2020), which may have contributed to a greater loss of wealth (and fear of loss) among individuals with higher levels of education.

Finally, it is plausible that individuals of higher SES experience adaptation or an endowment effect whereby they have a higher expectation for a constant availability of resources (including ones not incorporated in our theorizing), and therefore experience greater declines in well-being when a crisis contracts or threatens their resource supplies (Diener & Biswas-Diener, 2002; Tversky & Kahneman, 1991). This possible explanation is particularly intriguing given that evidence suggests that the pandemic has hit individuals of lower SES very hard. As one of many examples of higher impacts to lower SES individuals, household crowding and higher odds of working on-site have been linked to higher COVID-19 infections (Emeruwa et al., 2020; Oppel et al., 2020).

Our study assessed well-being early in the pandemic and it is possible that the findings of more severe well-being decline among individuals of higher SES are temporary. Future research should examine well-being among groups of higher and lower SES over a longer time during the pandemic as well as moderators of the impact of education (e.g., personality traits). For organizational and managerial practice, as well as mental health practitioners, it will be key to identify the groups for whom the impacts are longer lasting in order to address inequities. It would also be intriguing to examine if our findings replicate in other countries, to consider the role of threat of loss versus actual loss of resources, and to theorize the role of factors such as age and general health as more central predictors of psychological well-being during COVID.

There are several unique aspects to our investigation. Available pre-post studies of SES in the context of other crises have relied on data following versus during the event (Norris et al, 2002). Our study also expands collective knowledge by examining the role of resources in explaining SES differences in levels and changes in well-being during a crisis event. An additional major strength of our study is that it features a probability sample-based, nationally representative panel. This broad sampling strategy was essential to represent both low and high levels of SES, and to provide a more rigorous test of our hypotheses.

We contribute to the conversation on socioeconomic inequality by illuminating how a crisis event afflicts well-being across the SES spectrum. The theory of fundamental social causes has primarily been examined with respect to physical health. Our study extends this theory to the examination of psychological well-being. We found more support for this theory with respect to income as an SES indicator than for education. Moreover, our study contributes to the dynamic testing of COR theory, which emphasizes the velocity of loss spirals underlying chronic resource shortages and suggests the primacy of acute resource losses (Ennis et al., 2000; Hobfoll, 2010). Our findings provide some support for both of these tenets. We found inferior well-being during the pandemic among individuals with lower income and also observed well-being declines to a greater extent among individuals of higher education. Future research is needed to distinguish between the relative impact of chronic resource shortages and acute resource losses. We also invite more managerial research delineating how SES contexts shape psychological experiences in the face of societal and organizational crises (Bapuji et al., 2020b, Fiske & Markus, 2012).

As a limitation, our sample focused on individuals who participated in the “Health Networks” that targeted U.S. adults between 30 and 80 years old. Future research can examine whether our results generalize to those under the age of 30. It is also important to qualify our inferences about COVID-19 per se being the definitive cause of well-being changes from 2019 to 2020. These dynamics may plausibly be explained by other factors that are not associated with the pandemic, such as the political environment. The consistent timing of well-being assessments in 2019 and 2020 mostly rule out alternative explanations related to seasonal effects.

Acknowledgments

A portion of the data reported in this manuscript were obtained from the RAND American Life Panel “Health Networks Study,” [https://grantome.com/grant/NIH/R01-AA025956-01A1], funded by grant R01AA025956 from the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (PI: Pollard). The study questions and relationships examined in the present article have not been examined in previous manuscripts.

Appendix A

Study Measures

Educational attainment

What is the highest level of school you have completed or the highest degree you have received?

  1. Less than high school

  2. Some high school, no diploma

  3. High school graduate or equivalent

  4. Some college, no degree

  5. Associate’s degree

  6. Bachelor’s degree

  7. Master’s degree

  8. Professional school degree

  9. Doctorate degree

Annual household income

Which category represents the total combined income of all members of your family (living here) during the past 12 months? This includes money from jobs, net income from business, farm or rent, pensions, dividends, interest, social security payments and any other money income received by members of your family who are 15 years of age or older.

  1. Less than $5,000

  2. $5,000 to $7,499

  3. $7,500 to $9,999

  4. $10,000 to $12,499

  5. $12,500 to $14,999

  6. $15,000 to $19,999

  7. $20,000 to $24,999

  8. $25,000 to $29,999

  9. $30,000 to $34,999

  10. $35,000 to $39,999

  11. $40,000 to $49,999

  12. $50,000 to $59,999

  13. $60,000 to $74,999

  14. $75,000 to $99,999

  15. $100,000 to $124,999

  16. $125,000 to $199,999

  17. $200,000 or more

Perceived financial resources (Meuris & Leana, 2018)

Since the start of the COVID-19 pandemic in the United Sates, how often have you…

  • 1 = Never

  • 2 = Rarely

  • 3 = Sometimes

  • 4 = Often

  • 5 = Always

  1. Been worried about your financial situation? (R)

  2. Felt satisfied with your financial situation?

  3. Felt overwhelmed by your financial obligations? (R)

  4. Felt that you did not have enough money? (R)

Perceived control (Lachman & Weaver, 1998)

Now we’re going to ask you some questions about feelings you might have. Some of these questions have to do with how much control you feel you have over your life. Some of these questions might make you feel uncomfortable. Remember that you don’t have to answer any question that you don’t want to answer.

  • 1 = Strongly disagree

  • 2 = Disagree

  • 3 = Agree

  • 4 = Strongly agree

  • 5 = Don’t know

  1. I can do just about anything I really set my mind to.

  2. There is really no way I can solve some of the problems I have.

  3. Sometimes I feel that I’m being pushed around in life. (R)

  4. I have little control over the things that happen to me. (R)

  5. What happens to me in the future mostly depends on me. (R)

  6. I often feel helpless in dealing with the problems of life. (R)

  7. There is little I can do to change many of the important things in my life. (R)

Interpersonal resources (Hughes et al., 2004)

Since the start of the COVID-19 pandemic in the United Sates, to what extent have things gotten worse or better for you?

  • 1 = Hardly ever

  • 2 = Some of the time

  • 3 = Often

  1. How often do you feel that you lack companionship? (R)

  2. How often do you feel left out? (R)

  3. How often do you feel isolated from others? (R)

COVID-related knowledge

How knowledgeable would you rate yourself with regards to COVID-19 (e.g., symptoms, how to prevent getting the virus, and prevalence in your state)?

  • 1 = Not at all knowledgeable

  • 2 = Slightly knowledgeable

  • 3 = Somewhat knowledgeable

  • 4 = Moderately knowledgeable

  • 5 = Extremely knowledgeable

COVID-related news consumption

Since the start of the COVID-19 pandemic in the United Sates, how frequently have you followed the news related to the pandemic?

  • 1 = Never

  • 2 = Rarely

  • 3 = Occasionally

  • 4 = A moderate amount

  • 5 = A great deal

Depressive symptoms (Kroenke et al., 2009)

Over the LAST TWO WEEKS, how often have you been bothered by any of the following problems?

  • 0 = Not at all

  • 1 = Several days

  • 2 = More than half the days

  • 3 = Nearly every day

  1. Little interest or pleasure in doing things.

  2. Feeling down, depressed, or hopeless.

  3. Trouble falling or staying asleep, or sleeping too much.

  4. Feeling tired or having little energy.

  5. Poor appetite or overeating.

  6. Feeling bad about yourself—or that you are a failure or have let yourself or your family down.

  7. Trouble concentrating on things, such as reading the newspaper or watching television.

  8. Moving or speaking so slowly that other people could have noticed? Or the opposite—being so fidgety or restless that you have been moving around a lot more than usual.

Life satisfaction (Kobau et al., 2010)

Using a scale of 1 to 10 where 1 means “very dissatisfied” and 10 means “very satisfied”, how do you feel about your life as a whole right now?

General health status

In general, would you say your health is excellent, very good, good, fair, or poor?

Note: (R) indicates reverse scored.

Footnotes

1

Respondents and non-respondents did not significantly differ in general health status, education, and income. Respondents were older and were more likely to be male and non-Hispanic White. Sampling weights were included to adjust for differences between our sample and the general population, and age, gender, and racioethnicity are included in analyses as covariates.

2

Some researchers use occupational prestige as an index of SES among working samples. Among our participants working full or part time in early 2020, Nam-Powers-Boyd occupational prestige scale scores (Boyd & Nam, 2015) were highly correlated with education (r = .51, p < .01) and income (r = .58, p < .01).

3

When control variables are removed, results are all consistent except the effect of income on COVID-related news consumption and the direct effect of education on depressive symptoms were not significant. Detailed results are available on this study’s OSF site.

4

Sampling weights are provided by RAND to match the sample to the U.S. population in multiple demographic characteristics based on data from the Current Population Survey Annual Social and Economic Supplement (administered in March of each year). See technical details of the weighting procedure in Pollard and Baird (2017).

5

Maximum likelihood estimation with robust standard errors (MLR) was used to incorporate weights into our analyses. The MLR method is not compatible with bootstrapping by resampling cases (which is the default bootstrapping procedure in Mplus). Therefore, we used the Monte Carlo method, which resamples estimates of parameters from sampling distributions, to construct 95% CIs of indirect and total effects (MacKinnon et al., 2004).

6

Detailed model specification is provided on this study’s OSF site.

References

  1. Afridi L, & Block L (2020, April 2). Frontline communities hit hardest by COVID-19. Association for Neighborhood & Housing Development. https://anhd.org/blog/frontline-communities-hit-hardest-COVID-19 [Google Scholar]
  2. Algren MH, Ekholm O, Nielsen L, Ersbøll AK, Bak CK, & Andersen PT (2020). Social isolation, loneliness, socioeconomic status, and health-risk behaviour in deprived neighbourhoods in Denmark: A cross-sectional study. SSM-Population Health, 10, 100546 10.1016/j.ssmph.2020.100546 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Anderson C, Kraus M, Galinsky A, & Keltner D (2012). The local-ladder effect: Social status and subjective well-being. Psychological Science, 23(7), 764–771. 10.1177/0956797611434537 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Asparouhov T (2005). Sampling weights in latent variable modeling. Structural Equation Modeling, 12(3), 411–434. 10.1207/s15328007sem1203_4 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bapuji H, Ertug G, & Shaw JD (2020a). Organizations and societal economic inequality: A review and way forward. Academy of Management Annals, 14(1), 60–91. 10.5465/annals.2018.0029 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bapuji H, Patel C, Ertug G, & Allen DG (2020b). Corona crisis and inequality: Why management research needs a societal turn. Journal of Management. Advance online publication 10.1177/0149206320925881 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bhargava A, Fukushima EA, Levine M, Zhao W, Tanveer F, Szpunar SM, & Saravolatz L (2020). Predictors for severe COVID-19 infection. Clinical Infectious Diseases. Advance online publication. 10.1093/cid/ciaa674 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Boyd M, & Nam CB (2015, October 14–16). The newest Nam-Power-Boyd occupational scale: Development and insights [Paper session]. Southern Demographic Association Annual Meeting, San Antonio, TX, United States http://www.npb-ses.info/latest-scores.html [Google Scholar]
  9. Braveman P, Egerter S, & Williams DR (2011). The social determinants of health: Coming of age. Annual Review of Public Health, 32, 381–398. 10.1146/annurev-publhealth-031210-101218 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Cheng C, Cheung S, Chio J, & Chan M (2013). Cultural meaning of perceived control: A meta-analysis of locus of control and psychological symptoms across 18 cultural regions. Psychological Bulletin, 139(1), 152–188. 10.1037/a0028596 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Christie AM, & Barling J (2009). Disentangling the indirect links between socioeconomic status and health: The dynamic roles of work stressors and personal control. Journal of Applied Psychology, 94(6), 1466–1478. 10.1037/a0016847 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Cooper R, & Zhu G (2016). Household finance over the life-cycle: What does education contribute? Review of Economic Dynamics, 20, 63–89. 10.1016/j.red.2015.12.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  13. Côté S (2011). How social class shapes thoughts and actions in organizations. Research in Organizational Behavior, 31, 43–71. 10.1016/j.riob.2011.09.004 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  14. Cutler D, Stantcheva S, Alsan M, & Yang D (2020). Disparities in COVID-19 reported incidence, knowledge, and behavior. medRxiv. Retrieved May 19, 2020 from 10.1101/2020.05.15.20095927 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. DeGarmo DS, Forgatch MS, & Martinez CR Jr. (1999). Parents of divorced mothers as a link between social status and boys’ academic outcomes: Unpacking the effects of socioeconomic status. Child Development, 70(5), 1231–1245. 10.1111/1467-8624.00089 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Diemer MA, Mistry RS, Wadsworth ME, López I, & Reimers F (2013). Best practices in conceptualizing and measuring social class in psychological research. Analyses of Social Issues and Public Policy (ASAP), 13(1), 77–113. 10.1111/asap.12001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  17. Diener E, & Biswas-Diener R (2002). Will money increase subjective well-being? Social Indicators Research, 57(2), 119–169. 10.1023/A:1014411319119 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  18. Diener E, Emmons RA, Larsen RJ, & Griffin S (1985). The Satisfaction with Life Scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49(1), 71–75. 10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Emeruwa UN, Ona S, Shaman JL, Turitz A, Wright JD, Gyamfi-Bannerman C, & Melamed A (2020). Associations between built environment, neighborhood socioeconomic status, and SARS-CoV-2 infection among pregnant women in New York City. JAMA. Advance online publication. 10.1001/jama.2020.11370 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Ennis NE, Hobfoll SE, & Schröder KEE (2000). Money doesn’t talk, it swears: How economic stress and resistance resources impact inner-city women’s depressive mood. American Journal of Community Psychology, 28(2), 149–173. 10.1023/A:1005183100610 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Fiske SK, & Markus HR (Eds.). (2012). Facing social class: How societal rank influences interaction. Russell Sage Foundation. [Google Scholar]
  22. Galama TJ, & Van Kippersluis H (2019). A theory of socio-economic disparities in health over the life cycle. The Economic Journal, 129, 338–374. 10.1111/ecoj.12577 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Ginexi EM, Weihs K, & Simmens SJ (2000). Natural disaster and depression: A prospective investigation of reactions to the 1993 Midwest floods. American Journal of Community Psychology, 28(4), 495–518. 10.1023/a:1005188515149 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Hinkin TR, & Schriesheim CA (1989). Development and application of new scales to measure the French and Raven (1959) bases of social power. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74(4), 561–567. 10.1037/0021-9010.74.4.561 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  25. Hobfoll SE (1989). Conservation of resources: A new attempt at conceptualizing stress. American Psychologist, 44, 513–524. 10.1037/0003-066X.44.3.513 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Hobfoll SE (2010). Conservation of resources theory: Its implications for stress, health, and resilience In Folkman S (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of stress, health, and coping (pp. 127–147). 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195375343.013.0007 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  27. Hobfoll SE, Johnson RJ, Ennis N, & Jackson AP (2003). Resource loss, resource gain, and emotional outcomes among inner city women. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(3), 632–643. 10.1037/0022-3514.84.3.632 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Hobfoll SE, Tirone V, Holmgreen L, & Gerhart J (2016). Conservation of resources theory applied to major stress In Fink G (Ed.), Stress: Concepts, cognition, emotion, and behavior (Vol. 1, pp. 65–71). Academic Press; 10.1016/B978-0-12-800951-2.00007-8 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  29. House JS, Umberson D, & Landis KR (1988). Structures and processes of social support. Annual Review of Sociology, 14, 293–318. 10.1146/annurev.so.14.080188.001453 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  30. House J & Williams DR (2000). Understanding and reducing socioeconomic and racial/ethnic disparities in health In Smedley BD & Syme SL (Eds.), Promoting health: Intervention strategies from social and behavioral research (pp. 81–124). National Academy Press. [Google Scholar]
  31. Hughes ME, Waite LJ, Hawkley LC, & Cacioppo JT (2004). A short scale for measuring loneliness in large surveys: Results from two population-based studies. Research on Aging, 26(6), 655–672. 10.1177/0164027504268574 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. IHME (June 8, 2020). COVID-19 Projections United States of America. Retrieved June 3, 2020 from http://covid19.healthdata.org/united-states-of-america
  33. Kiviruusu O, Huurre T, Haukkala A, & Aro H (2013). Changes in psychological resources moderate the effect of socioeconomic status on distress symptoms: A 10-year follow-up among young adults. Health Psychology, 32(6), 627–636. 10.1037/a0029291 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Knight R (April 7, 2020). How to manage coronavirus layoffs with compassion. Harvard Business Review. https://hbr.org/2020/04/how-to-manage-coronavirus-layoffs-with-compassion [Google Scholar]
  35. Kobau R, Sniezek J, Zack MM, Lucas RE, & Burns A (2010). Well-being assessment: An evaluation of well-being scales for public health and population estimates of well-being among US adults. Applied Psychology: Health and Well-Being, 2(3), 272–297. 10.1111/j.1758-0854.2010.01035.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  36. Kraus MW, Piff PK, Mendoza-Denton R, Rheinschmidt ML, & Keltner D (2012). Social class, solipsism, and contextualism: How the rich are different from the poor. Psychological Review, 119(3), 546–572. 10.1037/a0028756 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Kroenke K, Spitzer RL, & Williams JB (2001). The PHQ-9: Validity of a brief depression severity measure. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 16, 606–613. 10.1046/j.1525-1497.2001.016009606.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Kroenke K, Strine TW, Spitzer RL, Williams JBW, Berry JT, & Mokdad AH (2009). The PHQ-8 as a measure of current depression in the general population. Journal of Affective Disorders, 114(1–3), 163–173. 10.1016/j.jad.2008.06.026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Lachman ME, & Weaver SL (1998). The sense of control as a moderator of social class differences in health and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(3), 763–773. 10.1037/0022-3514.74.3.763 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Leana CR, & Meuris J (2015). Living to work and working to live: Income as a driver of organizational behavior. Academy of Management Annals, 9(1), 55–95. 10.1080/19416520.2015.1007654 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  41. Lei L, Huang X, Zhang S, Yang J, Yang L, & Xu M (2020). Comparison of prevalence and associated factors of anxiety and depression among people affected by versus people unaffected by quarantine during the COVID-19 epidemic in Southwestern China. Medical Science Monitor: International Medical Journal of Experimental and Clinical Research, 26(e924609). 10.12659/MSM.924609 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Link BG, & Phelan J (1995). Social conditions as fundamental causes of disease. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 35, 80–94. 10.2307/2626958 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Lorant V, Deliège D, Eaton W, Robert A, Philippot P, & Ansseau M (2003). Socioeconomic inequalities in depression: A meta-analysis. American Journal of Epidemiology, 157(2), 98–112. 10.1093/aje/kwf182 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. MacKinnon DP, Lockwood CM, & Williams J (2004). Confidence limits for the indirect effect: Distribution of the product and resampling methods. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 39(1), 99–128. 10.1207/s15327906mbr3901_4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Mayo Clinic. (2020). COVID-19: Who’s at higher risk? Retrieved June 12, 2020 from https://www.mayoclinic.org/diseases-conditions/coronavirus/in-depth/coronavirus-who-is-at-risk/art-20483301
  46. McArdle JJ (2009). Latent variable modeling of differences and changes with longitudinal data. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 577–605. 10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163612 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Meuris J, & Leana C (2018). The price of financial precarity: Organizational costs of employees’ financial concerns. Organization Science, 29(3), 398–417. 10.1287/orsc.2017.1187 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  48. Muthén LK, & Muthén BO (1998–2017). Mplus user’s guide (8th ed.). Muthén & Muthén; https://www.statmodel.com/download/usersguide/MplusUserGuideVer_8.pdf [Google Scholar]
  49. Newman DA (2014). Missing data: Five practical guidelines. Organizational Research Methods, 17(4), 372–411. 10.1177/1094428114548590 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  50. Norris FH, Friedman MJ, Watson PJ, Byrne CM, Diaz E, & Kaniasty K (2002). 60,000 disaster victims speak: Part I. An empirical review of the empirical literature, 1981–2001. Psychiatry, 65(3), 207–239. 10.1521/psyc.65.3.207.20173 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Oakes JM, & Rossi PH (2003). The measurement of SES in health research: Current practices and steps toward a new approach. Social Science & Medicine, 56(4), 769–784. 10.1016/S0277-9536(02)00073-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Oppel RA Jr., Gebeloff R, Lai R, Wright W, & Smith M (2020, July 5). The fullest look yet at the racial inequity of coronavirus. The New York Times. https://www.nytimes.com/interactive/2020/07/05/us/coronavirus-latinos-african-americans-cdc-data.html [Google Scholar]
  53. Ornell F, Schuch JB, Sordi AO, & Kessler FHP (2020). “Pandemic fear” and COVID-19: mental health burden and strategies. Brazilian Journal of Psychiatry, 42(3), 232–235. 10.1590/1516-4446-2020-0008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Phelan JC, Link BG, & Tehranifar P (2010). Social conditions as fundamental causes of health inequalities: theory, evidence, and policy implications. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 51(S), S28–S40. 10.1177/0022146510383498 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Phifer JF (1990). Psychological distress and somatic symptoms after natural disaster: Differential vulnerability among older adults. Psychology and Aging, 5(3), 412–420. 10.1037/0882-7974.5.3.412 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Pinquart M, & Sörensen S (2000). Influences of socioeconomic status, social network, and competence on subjective well-being in later life: a meta-analysis. Psychology and Aging, 15(2), 187–224. 10.1037/0882-7974.15.2.187 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Pollard M, & Baird MD (2017). The RAND American Life Panel: Technical description. https://www.rand.org/pubs/research_reports/RR1651.html
  58. Qiu J, Shen B, Zhao M, Wang Z, Xie B, & Xu Y (2020) A nationwide survey of psychological distress among Chinese people in the COVID-19 epidemic: Implications and policy recommendations. General Psychiatry 2020;33:e100213 10.1136/gpsych-2020-100213 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Raven BH (1993). The bases of power: Origins and recent developments. Journal of Social Issues, 49(4), 227–251. 10.1111/j.1540-4560.1993.tb01191.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  60. Reeves RV, & Rothwell J (2020, March 27). Class and COVID: How the less affluent face double risks. The Brookings Institution. https://www.brookings.edu/blog/up-front/2020/03/27/class-and-COVID-how-the-less-affluent-face-double-risks/ [Google Scholar]
  61. Salk RH, & Abramson LY (2017). Gender differences in depression in representative national samples: Meta-analyses of diagnoses and symptoms. Psychological Bulletin, 143(8), 783–822. 10.1037/bul0000102 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. S&P Dow Jones Indices. (2020). Dow Jones Industrial Average. Retrieved July 2, 2020, from https://www.spglobal.com/spdji/en/indices/equity/dow-jones-industrial-average/#overview
  63. Talev M (2020, April 1). Axios-Ipsos Coronavirus Index: Rich sheltered, poor shafted amid virus. https://www.axios.com/axios-ipsos-coronavirus-index-rich-sheltered-poor-shafted-9e592100-b8e6-4dcd-aee0-a6516892874b.html
  64. Teo AR, Choi H, & Valenstein M (2013). Social relationships and depression: Ten-year follow-up from a nationally representative study. PLoS ONE, 8(4), 1–8. 10.1371/journal.pone.0062396 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Tomitaka S, Kawasaki Y, Ide K, Akutagawa M, Yamada H, Ono Y, & Furukawa TA (2018). Distributional patterns of item responses and total scores on the PHQ-9 in the general population: Data from the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey. BMC Psychiatry, 18, 108 10.1186/s12888-018-1696-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Tversky A, & Kahneman D (1991). Loss aversion in riskless choice: A reference-dependent model. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 106(4), 1039–1061. 10.2307/2937956 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  67. Venables WN, & Ripley B (2002). Modern applied statistics with S (4th ed.). Springer; http://www.stats.ox.ac.uk/pub/MASS4 [Google Scholar]
  68. Wang C, Pan R, Wan X, Tan Y, Xu L, McIntyre RS, Choo FN, Tran B, Ho R, Sharma VK, & Ho C (2020). A longitudinal study on the mental health of general population during the COVID-19 epidemic in China. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 87, 40–48. 10.1016/j.bbi.2020.04.028 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Wang JL, Schmitz N, & Dewa CS (2010). Socioeconomic status and the risk of major depression: The Canadian National Population Health Survey. Journal of Epidemiology & Community Health, 64, 447–452. 10.1136/jech.2009.090910 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Warr P, & Aung LL (2019). Poverty and inequality impact of a natural disaster: Myanmar’s 2008 cyclone Nargis. World Development, 122, 446–461. 10.1016/j.worlddev.2019.05.016 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  71. World Health Organization (WHO). (2020). Mental health and psychosocial considerations during the COVID-19 outbreak, 18 March 2020 (No. WHO/2019-nCoV/MentalHealth/2020.1) World Health Organization. https://www.who.int/docs/default-source/coronaviruse/mental-health-considerations.pdf?sfvrsn=6d3578af_2 [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES