Skip to main content
Nature Portfolio logoLink to Nature Portfolio
. 2019 Jun 21;26(3):1019–1028. doi: 10.1038/s41380-019-0444-y

Do ADHD-impulsivity and BMI have shared polygenic and neural correlates?

Edward D Barker 1,2,✉,#, Alex Ing 1,2,#, Francesca Biondo 1,2, Tianye Jia 1,2,3,4, Jean-Baptiste Pingault 5, Ebba Du Rietz 1, Yuning Zhang 1,2, Barbara Ruggeri 1,2, Tobias Banaschewski 6, Sarah Hohmann 6, Arun L W Bokde 7, Uli Bromberg 8, Christian Büchel 8, Erin Burke Quinlan 1,2, Edmund Sounga-Barke 1, April B Bowling 9, Sylvane Desrivières 1,2, Herta Flor 10,11, Vincent Frouin 11,22, Hugh Garavan 12, Philip Asherson 1, Penny Gowland 13, Andreas Heinz 14, Bernd Ittermann 15, Jean-Luc Martinot 16, Marie-Laure Paillère Martinot 17, Frauke Nees 6,10, Dimitri Papadopoulos-Orfanos 11,22, Luise Poustka 18, Michael N Smolka 19, Nora C Vetter 20, Henrik Walter 14, Robert Whelan 21, Gunter Schumann 1,2; IMAGEN Consortium
PMCID: PMC7910212  PMID: 31227801

Abstract

There is an extensive body of literature linking ADHD to overweight and obesity. Research indicates that impulsivity features of ADHD account for a degree of this overlap. The neural and polygenic correlates of this association have not been thoroughly examined. In participants of the IMAGEN study, we found that impulsivity symptoms and body mass index (BMI) were associated (r = 0.10, n = 874, p = 0.014 FWE corrected), as were their respective polygenic risk scores (PRS) (r = 0.17, n = 874, p = 6.5 × 10−6 FWE corrected). We then examined whether the phenotypes of impulsivity and BMI, and the PRS scores of ADHD and BMI, shared common associations with whole-brain grey matter and the Monetary Incentive Delay fMRI task, which associates with reward-related impulsivity. A sparse partial least squared analysis (sPLS) revealed a shared neural substrate that associated with both the phenotypes and PRS scores. In a last step, we conducted a bias corrected bootstrapped mediation analysis with the neural substrate score from the sPLS as the mediator. The ADHD PRS associated with impulsivity symptoms (b= 0.006, 90% CIs = 0.001, 0.019) and BMI (b= 0.009, 90% CIs = 0.001, 0.025) via the neuroimaging substrate. The BMI PRS associated with BMI (b= 0.014, 95% CIs = 0.003, 0.033) and impulsivity symptoms (b= 0.009, 90% CIs = 0.001, 0.025) via the neuroimaging substrate. A common neural substrate may (in part) underpin shared genetic liability for ADHD and BMI and the manifestation of their (observable) phenotypic association.

Subject terms: Psychology, Psychiatric disorders

Introduction

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder with a typical onset during childhood, but also shows mixed patterns of continuity and remittance, along with an adulthood onset [1]. Previous studies indicate an association between adult ADHD and being overweight and, in extremis, obesity [2, 3]—conditions which are relatively common and have marked long-term health effects [4, 5]. Research has identified factors contributing to the comorbidity between ADHD and obesity, including  fetal programming,  psychosocial stress, and factors directly related to energy balance, reduced physical exercise and sleep patterns alterations [6]. The bulk of current research has focused on how neuropsychological impairments (e.g., impulsivity) associated with ADHD may increase dysregulated and excessive eating [2, 3]. Importantly, neural and genetic liabilities may be important components underlying the phenotypic association between ADHD and overweight/obesity [6].

With regard to neural correlates, a recent ADHD ENIGMA mega-analysis [7] reported smaller subcortical volumes for ADHD (n = 20,183) compared to controls (n = 35,191) in the accumbens, amygdala, caudate nucleus, hippocampus and putamen. Importantly, some of these brain areas are involved in inhibitory control and reward-related pathways [7]. Neuroimaging studies in individuals who are overweight or obese have identified similar regions implicated in inhibitory control and reward-related pathways [8], including the amygdala, hippocampus and prefrontal cortex as well sensory areas (precentral gyrus) that could relate to food cues [9]. Genome wide association studies (GWAS) have identified susceptibility loci for ADHD [10] and obesity [11]. A polygenic risk score (PRS) – a cumulative genetic risk profile from across the genome – derived from the ADHD GWAS [10] showed associations with obesity-related indices of body mass index (BMI [12]), waist-to-hip ratio, childhood obesity, HDL cholesterol and Type 2 diabetes. Another study [13] using the ADHD PRS reported positive associations with BMI, depression and substance use, with an inverse relationship with numeric reasoning, suggesting genetic overlap with neurocognitive performance (see also [14]). Indeed, with regard to neurology, loci from the ADHD GWAS [10] have been associated with both ADHD risk and lower intracranial volume, as well as the amygdala, caudate nucleus and putamen [15].

PRS approaches have been applied to brain imaging studies. Although this has yet to be done with ADHD and obesity simultaneously, a PRS based on the recent ADHD GWAS [10] associated with smaller caudate volume [16]. The use of PRS in schizophrenia has been associated with abnormalities in both brain structure [17] and function [18]. With regard to structure, Neilson et al. [17] reported an association with bilateral frontal gyrification, which is a hypothesized neural endophenotype of schizophrenia [19].

These findings suggest that a better understanding of the relationship between ADHD and overweight/obesity may be gained by using an approach that incorporates both genetic and neural variation. We used the rich dataset of the ongoing IMAGEN study, comprising impulsivity symptoms, BMI, genome-wide loci and whole brain structural and functional neuroimaging measures at age 19. Firstly, we aimed to examine associations between PRS scores and phenotypes for ADHD and BMI, to assess genetic and phenotypic relationships between these traits. Secondly, we aimed to assess the associations between the PRS scores for ADHD and BMI, whole-brain structural variation, as well as a functional neuroimaging measure that assesses reward-related impulsivity. We were also interested in examining the degree to which the PRS scores of ADHD and BMI associated with the phenotypic measurements of impulsivity symptoms and BMI via the identified neural imaging substrate (a mediational model). We had no a priori hypotheses about associations between the PRS scores and brain regions; we ran the analyses in an exploratory manner.

Methods

Participants

We conducted all analyses on individuals drawn from the IMAGEN study (www.imagen-europe.com), a large-scale imaging genetic study aimed at identifying genetic, neuroimaging, and behavioural bases of individual variability in psychiatric disorders. Genetic data was derived from blood samples collected at the age of 14, while clinical, physical and neuroimaging data analysed in the present study were collected at the age of 19. Psytools software (Delosis Ltd, London, UK) was used to conduct the behavioural characterization via its internet-based platform. The assessment battery of questionnaires and cognitive tasks was self-administered both in participants’ homes and at the neuroimaging facilities. The study was approved by local ethics research committees at each site. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants as well as from their legal guardians. Measures.

Impulsivity symptoms at age 19 were assessed using self-reported scores taken from the well-validated Barratt Impulsivity Scale (BIS) [20]. The BIS is a questionnaire that is widely used to assess trait-impulsivity in a variety of studies including ADHD [21], neuroimaging [22], personality [23], over-eating [24] and in relation to genetic influence and criminal behaviours [25]. At the time the study was conducted, the IMAGEN consortium had BIS data for 1319 individuals at 19 years-old.

Body mass index (BMI)

BMI at age 19 was derived from height and weight measurements. Height was measured using a standard protocol to the last complete centimeter, weight was measured to the nearest 100 g. BMI was calculated as weight (kg)/height(m2). BMI values were recorded for 1347 subjects at the time the study was conducted. We excluded 114 participants who either were underweight (BMI < 18.5) extremely overweight (BMI > 50).

Genetic data

After quality control, 1982 cases were included in our sample totalling 506,932 SNPs available for PRS. Population stratification was controlled for in all analyses via the use of the first eight principal components of the genetic data, which were used as covariates. Full details on acquisition and initial processing are given in the supplementary materials of this paper.

MRI data

In this investigation, we used voxel-based morphometry measures of grey matter derived from T1 weighted MRI acquisitions and activation maps derived from the Monetary Incentive Delay (MID) task, to examine neural responses to reward anticipation and reward outcome. Full details on the MRI acquisitions [26] pre-processing [27] and confounds used in the analysis are given in the supplementary materials of this paper.

A Task Based fMRI Acquisition of the Monetary Incentive Delay (MID) task was used to examine neural responses to reward anticipation and reward outcome [28]. The task consisted of 66 10-s trials. In each trial, participants were presented with one of three cue shapes (cue, 250 ms) denoting whether a target (white square) would subsequently appear on the left or right side of the screen and whether 0, 2 or 10 points could be won in that trial. After a variable delay (4000–4500 ms) of fixation on a white crosshair, participants were instructed to respond with left/right button-press as soon as the target appeared. Feedback on whether and how many points were won during the trial was presented for 1450 ms after the response. Using a tracking algorithm, task difficulty (i.e., target duration varied between 100 and 300 ms) was individually adjusted such that each participant successfully responded on ~66% of trials. Based on prior research suggesting reliable associations between ADHD symptoms and fMRI blood-oxygen-level dependent (BOLD) responses measured during reward anticipation, the current study used the contrast ‘anticipation of high-win vs anticipation of no-win’.

Statistical analyses

The analyses proceeded in three steps (code used in statistical analyses is available upon request). In Step 1, PRS scores were created for ADHD and BMI and we examined associations between the scores and phenotypic measures. We also calculated associations between the ADHD and BMI PRS scores themselves, in order to investigate a potential genetic comorbidity between ADHD and BMI.

To derive the ADHD PRS, summary statistics were downloaded from the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium (http://www.med.unc.edu/pgc/results-and-downloads; 20,183 cases and 35,191 controls, all European descent). To calculate the BMI PRS, GWAS summary statistics from 339,224 European descents were downloaded from the GIANT Consortium (http://portals.broadinstitute.org/collaboration/giant/index.php). Deriving a PRS necessitates the use of a significance threshold for inclusion of SNPs in the calculation of the score (e.g., all SNPs associated at p < 0.05). In investigations where the researcher is only interested in the relation between the score and the phenotype, it is possible to vary that threshold, and select that which results in the highest variance explained between the PRS and the phenotype [29, 30]. However, in the present investigation, we were also interested in both the PRS scores themselves, and in their relation to functional and structural neuroimaging measures. In this case, varying the threshold for SNP inclusion in the PRS scores for ADHD and BMI, respectively, can lead to overfitting on the phenotype of interest [30]. Given the nature of our research question, which involves multiple phenotypes and multiple brain markers, this overfitting could lead to circularity problems in further analyses [31]. Using multiple PRS scores also poses a problem with multiple comparisons: if several PRS scores are calculated on the same phenotype, a correction must be carried out over these tests to ensure significance [30]. We therefore chose to use a nominal threshold of p = 0.05 for the inclusion of SNPs in the calculation of the score. This approach avoids both overfitting and multiple testing. All genetic data processing and analyses were performed using the R package PRSice [15] and PLINK [17]. In order to minimise the statistical assumptions necessary for valid inference, all significance levels reported here were ascertained using permutation testing. As we are explicitly looking for comorbidities between different data types, and the sPLS analysis only identifies positive associations, all significance tests were one-sided.

In Step 2, we investigated whether ADHD and BMI share a common underlying neural substrate. We used a sparse formulation of partial least squares (sPLS) for this purpose [18, 19]. This method is designed to establish associations between multiple sets of variables by finding the weighted sum of variables in each set, which correlate maximally with the weighted sum of variables that are connected via a path diagram (see Supplementary Figure S1). We used five-fold cross-validation to quantify the strength and significance of the associations of individual PRS scores and phenotypes, with the VBM and MID measures they were connected to via the path diagram [32].

Although PLS methods are very powerful, results can be difficult to interpret, as all variables contribute to associations between data-views. This is particularly problematic with neuroimaging data, which is high dimensional. It would be useful to know which neuroimaging features were associated with PRS scores and phenotypes of impulsivity and BMI. For this reason, we used a method that induces sparsity by setting some PLS weights to zero through the application of an L1 penalty, applied under resampling [33, 34]. Through this approach, one can identify weighted sets of brain regions that are associated with the PRS scores and phenotypes. This set of brain regions may therefore be considered as an endophenotype for ADHD and BMI. We used a resampling procedure to ensure that the PLS approach only retained features that were robustly associated with phenotypes and genetic measures [33]. Significance was ascribed using a permutation testing procedure. A detailed description of the exact analysis approach used is detailed in the Supplementary Materials. In all analyses, we controlled for genetic population stratification (i.e., the 8 principle components), gender, imaging site, age and total intracranial volume. Many association tests were carried out in this investigation. We corrected for multiple comparisons using the Holm-Bonferroni method [35].

In Step 3, we examined the degree to which the set of brain regions identified by sPLS may act as an intermediary endophenotype between the PRS scores and the impulsivity and BMI phenotypes. In other words, we examined if the brain regions could help explain (or mediate) the observable association between ADHD and BMI genetic vulnerabilities and the impulsivity and BMI phenotypes. The mediation (or indirect) pathways were defined by the product term of the pathways of interest (e.g., PRS to brain BY brain to phenotype). There were four overall possible effects: 1) ADHD PRS → brain regions → impulsivity symptoms; 2) ADHD PRS → brain regions → BMI; 3) BMI PRS →brain regions → BMI; and 4) BMI PRS →brain regions → impulsivity. Because standard errors underlying mediation pathways (i.e., the product terms) are known to be skewed, we bootstrapped all indirect effects 10,000 times with bias corrected (90% and 95%) confidence intervals. The mediation pathways reported here are based on the bootstrapped variability around the product of non-standardized path coefficient estimates (i.e., b). Mediation pathways were programmed in Laavan [36] in the statistical package R [37].

Results

Step 1: PRS scores for ADHD and BMI

At the time the study was conducted, n = 874 subjects had complete genetic and phenotypic data. The BMI PRS score was significantly associated with BMI (r = 0.23, n = 874, p = 2.2 × 10−11 FWE corrected) and the PRS score for ADHD was significantly associated with impulsivity symptoms (r = 0.10, n = 874, p= 0.014 FWE corrected). We also note that impulsivity symptoms were associated with BMI (r = 0.10, n = 874, p = 0.014 FWE corrected), and that the PRS scores for ADHD and BMI were significantly cross-correlated (r = 0.17, n = 874, p = 6.5 × 10−6, FWE corrected). Of interest, using Steiger’s test for dependent correlations [38], we found that the correlation between the PRS scores of ADHD and BMI was higher (p = 0.036 FWE corrected) than the correlation between the phenotypic measures of impulsivity and BMI.

Step 2: the shared neural correlates of the PRS scores of ADHD and BMI

Of the 874 subjects who had both complete genetic and phenotypic data, 604 had both T1 and fMRI data that passed QC. Using sPLS, we found that both the ADHD and BMI phenotypes, and their respective polygenic risk scores, are significantly associated with a common neural substrate, constructed from T1 and fMRI data, which we term the ‘neural endophenotype’. The PRS for BMI was associated with the neural endophenotype at r = 0.12, n = 604, p = 9.5 × 10−3, FWE corrected, whilst the ADHD PRS was significant at r = 0.087, n = 604, p = 0.036 FWE corrected. ADHD and BMI phenotypes were also associated with this set of brain regions, with ADHD associated at r = 0.091, n = 604, p = 0.035 FWE corrected, and BMI associated at r = 0.15, n = 604, p = 9.0 × 10−4 FWE corrected respectively. These correlations are summarized in a matrix and path diagram in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

a The panel on the left shows the path diagram illustrating associations between the different biological and phenotypic measures used in this investigation. Associations that are statistically significant are marked with an asterisk. b The panel on the right shows correlation values between the different biological and phenotypic measures investigated in this study, the lower triangular matrix shows correlation values between the various biological measures, whilst the upper triangular matrix shows the FWE-corrected significance levels of these associations

The neural endophenotype is made up of grey matter regions and regions of activation derived from the MID task. Grey matter regions contributing to this neural endophenotype were located bilaterally in the cerebellum, amygdala, hippocampus, para-hippocampus and orbital frontal cortex, which all present bilaterally. There was also a largely left lateralised grey matter cluster in the inferotemporal cortex. sPLS weights in each of these brain regions were negative, implying an inverse association between PRS scores/phenotypes and grey matter. Neural endophenotype clusters that came up in the MID task were largely left lateralised and included the fusiform gyrus and para-hippocampus, postcentral and parietal inferior, calcarine and occipital superior and frontal superior medial cortex. These results are displayed in Fig. 2. sPLS weights in each of these brain regions were positive, implying a positive association between the MID contrast map and the PRS/phenotype scores. The full set of VBM and MID clusters identified and localised in the sPLS analysis are tabulated in Supplementary Tables S1 and S2.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

a The scatter plots in panel (a) of the figure show relations between neuroimaging and ADHD and BMI PRS scores. b The brain regions in panel (b) of the figure illustrate the VBM and MID regions that are associated with ADHD and BMI PRS scores, and their associated phenotypes. c The scatter plots in panel (c) of the figure show relations between neuroimaging and ADHD and BMI phenotypes

Step 3: Neural endophenotype as a mediator in the link between PRS scores and ADHD and BMI

In Step 3, we saved the neuroimaging factor score from the sPLS and conducted the multivariate bias corrected bootstrapped mediation analyses. The four mediation pathways were observed, albeit at a 90% CIs for ADHD and 95% CIs BMI, suggesting that the neural endophenotype may act as an intermediary variable in the link between the PRS scores and the impulsivity and BMI phenotypes. These four mediation pathways were: 1) ADHD PRS → neuroimaging score → impulsivity (b= 0.006, 90% CIs = 0.001, 0.019); ADHD PRS → neuroimaging score → BMI (b= 0.009, 90% CIs = 0.001, 0.023); 3) BMI PRS → neuroimaging score → BMI (b= 0.014, 95% CIs = 0.003, 0.033); and 4) BMI PRS → neuroimaging score → impulsivity (b= 0.009, 95% CIs = 0.001, 0.025).

Discussion

This study set out to examine if there are shared genetic and neural correlates of the ADHD (impulsivity) and BMI [39] phenotypes. Our results support previous ADHD-related research in showing an association between these two phenotypes [3]. Further analyses showed that the PRS scores for ADHD and BMI were correlated with each other and with grey matter across the brain and contrast maps derived from fMRI scans conducted during the MID task. Importantly, these neuroimaging measures also associated with the phenotypes of impulsivity and BMI. These results extend present knowledge of the biological underpinnings of the association between ADHD and BMI in three main ways.

Firstly, using previously established genome-wide evidence, and a multi-modal whole-brain approach, this study showed that the PRS scores and phenotypes of ADHD and BMI simultaneously associated with decreases in grey matter in similar brain areas as reported by the case-control ADHD ENIGMA mega-analysis [7] as well as certain case-control designs of obesity [9]. These areas include the cerebellum, amygdala, hippocampus, orbitofrontal and inferotemporal cortex. The functions of these brain areas underscore the importance of impairments in cognitive and behavioural control and impulsivity in both ADHD and BMI. For example, the fronto-cerebellar networks have been implicated in impairments of cognitive control (e.g., over-riding impulses) in both ADHD and obesity [40, 41]; the ability to consider future consequences/adjust behaviours [41]; and the inhibition of eating behaviour [42]. Likewise, the amygdala and hippocampus have been associated with impulsivity symptoms in ADHD [43] as well as weight gain and over-eating behaviours such as continuing to eat even when sated [4447]. Finally, the orbital frontal cortex has been linked to impairments in cognitive and attentional control in ADHD [48], but also potentially to selection and consumption of calorie-rich foods [49] and sensitized reactivity to (food-related) reward [50]. The second set of findings relate to the reward anticipation fMRI task. There was an increased BOLD response for the posterior visual and association areas, postcentral, cerebellum and frontal medial cortex. This was consistent with previous findings from both the MID task and other fMRI paradigms used to assess reward processes. With regard to the MID task, our findings converged with previous research that reported abnormalities in frontal-parietal and cerebellar-parietal networks [51, 52]. Supporting other reward processing studies, we identified increased BOLD response in the cerebellum and postcentral gyri [47, 53], in addition to reward areas in the medial frontal regions [54, 55]. Importantly, both the MID-related results and previously mentioned grey matter results showed abnormalities in visual processing areas (i.e., inferotemporal and association cortices for grey matter and posterior visual and association cortices for the MID task). These abnormalities may be implicated in visual attention biases to reward-related cues (e.g., food or reward) in both obesity and ADHD [3739].

Third, we examined a multivariate mediation model. Here, the associations between the ADHD and BMI PRS scores and the impulsivity and BMI phenotypes were partially carried by the brain areas identified by sPLS. We therefore suggest that these brain areas (structural + functional) may act as a neural endophenotype [56] between the genetic liabilities of ADHD and obesity and the manifestation of their (observable) phenotypic association. By definition neurobiological endophenotypes mark genetic vulnerability but are also independent of disease state [57]. Our suggestion of a neural endophenotype is consistent with the idea that a limited number of neural systems may engender risk for a range comorbid psychiatric syndromes [56]. As such, this hypothesized neural endophenotype might extend as vulnerability for known comorbidities of ADHD and BMI, such as substance use and addiction [58] and depression [59, 60], to name a few.

How might the results of the present study bear on future research? Are there (potential) clinical implications? It is important to note that our findings of genetic influence on ADHD and BMI are non-deterministic and can be context dependent [61]. Indeed, Barcellos et al. [57], taking advantage of a natural experiment, showed that an additional year of compulsory schooling in the UK benefitted individuals with higher genetic risk for obesity, reducing the gap in unhealthy body size in the top and bottom terciles of genetic risk of obesity from 20 to 6 percentage points. This is an interesting result when it is considered that certain interventions targeting ADHD can result in weight reduction and, potentially, vice-versa. Although findings are mixed, studies on Methylphenidate show that a reduction in ADHD symptoms can associate with decreased BMI, especially for overweight/obese adolescents [3, 62]. Methylphenidate has also been proposed as a treatment for obesity [63, 64]. Future research should examine if individuals at high genetic risk for both ADHD and BMI will especially benefit from interventions tailored for one—or both—of these conditions.

Our findings should be considered in light of a number of limitations. Firstly, our models were statistically driven—although theoretically informed. The models are therefore dependent on the properties of the measures included. While our sPLS model identified simultaneous association between the PRS scores, the two neuroimaging modalities and the phenotypic measurements of ADHD and BMI, this does not mean that this model—or the study variables contained within it—is the best (or only) solution. Secondly, it is also important to note that PRS and neural correlates only explain a small proportion of the liability for many psychiatric difficulties and other traits. Thirdly, the current findings differ from reward anticipation in case-control ADHD designs [65, 66] and for clinical remission [67]. Relatedly, it will be of interest to replicate the present results in clinical samples, for ADHD and BMI separately, across different age groups, and across different ethnic groups. Fourth, in the mediation analyses, the ADHD-related pathway had larger bias corrected CIs (90%) compared to the BMI-related pathway (95%). There are at least two reasons for these different CIs. The GWAS discovery sample for ADHD (n = 46,350) is smaller than BMI (n = 339,224), which can affect the relative predictive power of these PRS scores [68]. In addition, we examined the ADHD PRS in relation to impulsivity symptoms, which is less precise than examining the BMI PRS in relation to BMI.

Conclusion

Our results indicate that ADHD and BMI PRS scores are significantly correlated, suggesting shared genetic risk between the traits. In a data-driven model, both the PRS scores were associated with the phenotypic measures of ADHD and BMI, as well as shared neural correlates of impairments in inhibitory control and reward processing.

Supplementary information

Supplemental Material (789.9KB, docx)

Acknowledgements

This specific manuscript is funded by a grant from Economic and Social Research Council (ES/R005516/1) to Edward D. Barker. IMAGEN is supported by the European Union–funded FP6 Integrated Project IMAGEN (Reinforcement-Related Behaviour in Normal Brain Function and Psychopathology) (LSHM-CT-2007-037286), the Horizon 2020–funded ERC Advanced Grant STRATIFY (Brain Network Based Stratification of Reinforcement-Related Disorders) (695313), ERANID (Understanding the Interplay Between Cultural, Biological, and Subjective Factors in Drug Use Pathways) (PR-ST-0416-10004), BRIDGET (JPND: Brain Imaging, Cognition, Dementia, and Next-Generation Genomics) (MR/N027558/1), the FP7 projects IMAGEMEND (602450; Imaging Genetics for Mental Disorders), MATRICS (603016), the Innovative Medicine Initiative Project EU-AIMS (115300-2), the Medical Research Council Grant c-VEDA (Consortium on Vulnerability to Externalizing Disorders and Addictions) (MR/N000390/1), the Swedish Research Council FORMAS, the Medical Research Council, the National Institute for Health Research Biomedical Research Centre at South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust and King’s College London, the Bundesministerium für Bildung und Forschung (BMBF grants 01GS08152, 01EV0711, eMED SysAlc01ZX1311 A, Forschungsnetz AERIAL), and the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG grants SM 80/7-1, SM 80/7-2, SFB 940/1). Further support was provided by grants from the Agence Nationale de la Recherche (project AF12-NEUR0008-01-WM2NA, and ANR-12-SAMA-0004), the Fondation de France, the Fondation pour la Recherche Médicale, the Mission Interministériellede Lutte Contre les Drogues et les Conduites Addictives (MILDECA), the Assistance Publique Hôpitaux de Paris and INSERM (interface grant), Paris Sud University IDEX 2012. Tianye Jia is supported by: 111 Project (No. B18015) National Natural Science Foundation of China (No. 91630314 & No. 81801773) the Key Project of Shanghai Science & Technology (No.16JC1420402) Shanghai Municipal Science and Technology Major Project (No.2018SHZDZX01) and ZJLab Shanghai Pujiang Project (18PJ1400900).

Author contributions

EDB and AI had full access to data for this study and take responsibility for the accuracy of the results reported here. Study concept and design: EDB, AI and GS. Acquisition, analysis or interpretation: All authors. Drafting of manuscript: Barker, AI, FB, FJ, BR, JBP, EDR, GS. Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: All authors. Statistical analysis: AI, EDB, FJ. Obtained funding: IMAGEN consortium, EDB. Administrative, technical or material support: AI, EDB, EBQ, YZ. Study supervision: EDB, AI.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

TB has served as an adviser or consultant for Actelion, Bristol-Myers Squibb, Desitin Arzneimittel, Eli Lilly, Hexal Pharma, Medice, Novartis, Pfizer, Shire, UCB, and Vifor Pharma; he has received conference attendance support, conference support, or speaking fees from Eli Lilly, Janssen McNeil, Medice, Novartis, Shire, and UCB; he has been involved in clinical trials conducted by Eli Lilly, Novartis, Shire, and Vifor Pharma; and he has received royalties from CIP Medien, Hogrefe, Kohlhammer, and Oxford University Press. EDB has received grant support, including Ph.D. support, from General Electric Healthcare; he has served as a consultant for IXICO, and he has received an honorarium for teaching in a scanner programming course run by General Electric Healthcare. ALWB has received funding from the Science Foundation Ireland Stokes Programme (07/SK/B1214a). PG has received a research grant from Lyndra and an honorarium paid to her employer from GlaxoSmithKline. LP has received conference attendance support or speaking fees from Medice, Novartis, and Shire. HW has received a speaking honorarium from Servier. Dr. Buitelaar has served as a consultant, advisory board member, or speaker for Eli Lilly, Janssen-Cilag BV, Novartis, Lundbeck, Roche, Servier, and Shire. Dr. Glennon has served as a consultant for Boehringer Ingelheim. The remaining authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Footnotes

Publisher’s note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

These authors contributed equally: Edward D Barker, Alex Ing

Supplementary information

The online version of this article (10.1038/s41380-019-0444-y) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

References

  • 1.Agnew-Blais JC, Polanczyk GV, Danese A, Wertz J, Moffitt TE, Arseneault L. Young adult mental health and functional outcomes among individuals with remitted, persistent and late-onset ADHD. Br J Psychiatry. 2018;213:526–34. doi: 10.1192/bjp.2018.97. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Nigg JT, Johnstone JM, Musser ED, Long HG, Willoughby M, Shannon J. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and being overweight/obesity: New data and meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Rev. 2016;43:67–79. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2015.11.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Cortese S, Moreira-Maia CR, St. Fleur D, Morcillo-Peñalver C, Rohde LA, Faraone SV. Association between ADHD and obesity: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Am J Psychiatry. 2016;173:34–43. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2015.15020266. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Biederman J. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a selective overview. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57:1215–20. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.10.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Wang YC, McPherson K, Marsh T, Gortmaker SL, Brown M. Health and economic burden of the projected obesity trends in the USA and the UK. Lancet. 2011;378:815–25. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(11)60814-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Hanć T, Cortese S. Attention deficit/hyperactivity-disorder and obesity: a review and model of current hypotheses explaining their comorbidity. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2018;92:16–28. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.05.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Hoogman M, Bralten J, Hibar DP, Mennes M, Zwiers MP, Schweren LS, et al. Subcortical brain volume differences in participants with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children and adults: a cross-sectional mega-analysis. Lancet Psychiatry. 2017;4:310–9. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(17)30049-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Seymour KE, Reinblatt SP, Benson L, Carnell S. Overlapping neurobehavioral circuits in ADHD, obesity, and binge eating: evidence from neuroimaging research. CNS Spectrums. 2015;20:401–11. doi: 10.1017/S1092852915000383. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Carnell S, Gibson C, Benson L, Ochner C, Geliebter A. Neuroimaging and obesity: current knowledge and future directions. Obes Rev. 2012;13:43–56. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-789X.2011.00927.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Demontis, D, Walters, RK, Martin, J, Mattheisen, M, Als, TD, Agerbo, E et al. Discovery of the first genome-wide significant risk loci for attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Nat Gene. 2018;51:63–75. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 11.Locke AE, Kahali B, Berndt SI, Justice AE, Pers TH, Day FR, et al. Genetic studies of body mass index yield new insights for obesity biology. Nature. 2015;518:197. doi: 10.1038/nature14177. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Anttila V, Bulik-Sullivan B, Finucane HK, Walters RK, Bras J, Duncan L, et al. Analysis of shared heritability in common disorders of the brain. Science. 2018;360:eaap8757. doi: 10.1126/science.aap8757. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Du Rietz E, Coleman J, Glanville K, Choi SW, O’Reilly PF, Kuntsi J. Association of polygenic risk for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder with co-occurring traits and disorders. Biol Psychiatry Cognitive Neurosci Neuroimag. 2018;3:635–43. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2017.11.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Frazier‐Wood AC, Carnell S, Pena O, Hughes SO, O’Connor TM, Asherson P, et al. Cognitive performance and BMI in childhood: Shared genetic influences between reaction time but not response inhibition. Obesity. 2014;22:2312–8. doi: 10.1002/oby.20862. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Klein M, Walters RK, Demontis D, Stein JL, Hibar DP, Adams HH, et al. Genetic markers of ADHD-related variations in intracranial volume. Am J Psychiatry. 2019;176:228–38. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2018.18020149. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Alemany, S, Jansen, PR, Muetzel, RL, Marques, N, El Marroun, H, Jaddoe, VW et al. Common polygenic variations for psychiatric disorders and cognition in relation to brain morphology in the general pediatric population. J Am Acad Child Adoles Psychiatry. 2019;58:600–7. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 17.Neilson E, Bois C, Clarke T-K, Hall L, Johnstone E, Owens D, et al. Polygenic risk for schizophrenia, transition and cortical gyrification: a high-risk study. Psychol Med. 2018;48:1532–9. doi: 10.1017/S0033291717003087. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Erk S, Mohnke S, Ripke S, Lett T, Veer I, Wackerhagen C, et al. Functional neuroimaging effects of recently discovered genetic risk loci for schizophrenia and polygenic risk profile in five RDoC subdomains. Transl Psychiatry. 2017;7:e997. doi: 10.1038/tp.2016.272. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.White T, Gottesman I. Brain connectivity and gyrification as endophenotypes for schizophrenia: weight of the evidence. Curr Topics Med Chem. 2012;12:2393–403. doi: 10.2174/156802612805289953. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Patton JH, Stanford MS, Barratt ES. Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. J Clin Psychol. 1995;51:768–74. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(199511)51:6<768::aid-jclp2270510607>3.0.co;2-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Chamberlain SR, Ioannidis K, Leppink EW, Niaz F, Redden SA, Grant JE. ADHD symptoms in non-treatment seeking young adults: relationship with other forms of impulsivity. CNS Spectrums. 2017;22:22–30. doi: 10.1017/S1092852915000875. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Johnston JA, Wang F, Liu J, Blond BN, Wallace A, Liu J, et al. Multimodal neuroimaging of frontolimbic structure and function associated with suicide attempts in adolescents and young adults with bipolar disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2017;174:667–75. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2016.15050652. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Lange F, Wagner A, Müller A, Eggert F. Subscales of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale differentially relate to the Big Five factors of personality. Scandinavian J Psychol. 2017;58:254–9. doi: 10.1111/sjop.12359. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Loxton NJ. The role of reward sensitivity and impulsivity in overeating and food addiction. Curr Addict Rep. 2018;5:212–22. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Stetler DA, Davis C, Leavitt K, Schriger I, Benson K, Bhakta S, et al. Association of low-activity MAOA allelic variants with violent crime in incarcerated offenders. J Psychiatr Res. 2014;58:69–75. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2014.07.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Ashburner J, Friston KJ. Unified segmentation. Neuroimage. 2005;26:839–51. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Grellmann C, Bitzer S, Neumann J, Westlye LT, Andreassen OA, Villringer A, et al. Comparison of variants of canonical correlation analysis and partial least squares for combined analysis of MRI and genetic data. Neuroimage. 2015;107:289–310. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.12.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Knutson B, Westdorp A, Kaiser E, Hommer D. FMRI visualization of brain activity during a monetary incentive delay task. Neuroimage. 2000;12:20–27. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0593. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Dudbridge F. Power and predictive accuracy of polygenic risk scores. PLoS Genet. 2013;9:e1003348. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1003348. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Euesden J, Lewis CM, O’reilly PF. PRSice: polygenic risk score software. Bioinformatics. 2014;31:1466–8. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btu848. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Kriegeskorte N, Simmons WK, Bellgowan PS, Baker CI. Circular analysis in systems neuroscience: the dangers of double dipping. Nature neuroscience. 2009;12:535. doi: 10.1038/nn.2303. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Friedman J, Hastie T & Tibshirani R. The elements of statistical learning. Vol. 1. New York: Springer; 2001.
  • 33.Meinshausen N, Bühlmann P. Stability selection. J Royal Stat Soc Series B. 2010;72:417–73. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Witten DM, Tibshirani R, Hastie T. A penalized matrix decomposition, with applications to sparse principal components and canonical correlation analysis. Biostatistics. 2009;10:515–34. doi: 10.1093/biostatistics/kxp008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Holm, S. A simple sequentially rejective multiple test procedure. Scandinavian J Stat. 1979;6:65–70.
  • 36.Rosseel Y, Oberski D, Byrnes J, Vanbrabant L, Savalei V, Merkle E et al. Package ‘lavaan’. Latent Variable Analysis. Version 0.6-3. 2018. https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/lavaan/lavaan.pdf.
  • 37.R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. 2013. https://www.R-project.org/.
  • 38.Steiger JH. Tests for comparing elements of a correlation matrix. Psychol Bul. 1980;87:245. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Cortese S, Castellanos FX. The relationship between ADHD and obesity: implications for therapy. Exp Rev Neurotherap. 2014;14:473–9. doi: 10.1586/14737175.2014.904748. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Bauer C, Moreno B, González‐Santos L, Concha L, Barquera S, Barrios F. Child overweight and obesity are associated with reduced executive cognitive performance and brain alterations: a magnetic resonance imaging study in M exican children. Pediatric Obes. 2015;10:196–204. doi: 10.1111/ijpo.241. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Bonath B, Tegelbeckers J, Wilke M, Flechtner H-H, Krauel K. Regional gray matter volume differences between adolescents with ADHD and typically developing controls: further evidence for anterior cingulate involvement. J Attention Disord. 2018;22:627–38. doi: 10.1177/1087054715619682. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Park B-y, Seo J, Park H. Functional brain networks associated with eating behaviors in obesity. Sci Rep. 2016;6:23891. doi: 10.1038/srep23891. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Tajima-Pozo K, Ruiz-Manrique G, Yus M, Arrazola J, Montañes-Rada F. Correlation between amygdala volume and impulsivity in adults with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Acta Neuropsychiatr. 2015;27:362–7. doi: 10.1017/neu.2015.34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Coppin G. The anterior medial temporal lobes: Their role in food intake and body weight regulation. Physiol Behav. 2016;167:60–70. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2016.08.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Fazzari G, Zizza M, Di Vito A, Alò R, Mele M, Bruno R, et al. Reduced learning and memory performances in high-fat treated hamsters related to brain neurotensin receptor1 expression variations. Behav Brain Res. 2018;347:227–33. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2018.03.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Sun X, Kroemer NB, Veldhuizen MG, Babbs AE, de Araujo IE, Gitelman DR, et al. Basolateral amygdala response to food cues in the absence of hunger is associated with weight gain susceptibility. J Neurosci. 2015;35:7964–76. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3884-14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Wang G-J, Yang J, Volkow ND, Telang F, Ma Y, Zhu W, et al. Gastric stimulation in obese subjects activates the hippocampus and other regions involved in brain reward circuitry. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2006;103:15641–5. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0601977103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Rubia K. Cognitive Neuroscience of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) and Its Clinical Translation. Front Human Neurosci. 2018;12:100. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Grabenhorst F, Rolls ET, Bilderbeck A. How cognition modulates affective responses to taste and flavor: top-down influences on the orbitofrontal and pregenual cingulate cortices. Cerebral Cortex. 2007;18:1549–59. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Moreno-Lopez L, Contreras-Rodriguez O, Soriano-Mas C, Stamatakis EA, Verdejo-Garcia A. Disrupted functional connectivity in adolescent obesity. NeuroImage Clin. 2016;12:262–8. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2016.07.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Verdejo-Román J, Fornito A, Soriano-Mas C, Vilar-López R, Verdejo-García A. Independent functional connectivity networks underpin food and monetary reward sensitivity in excess weight. NeuroImage. 2017;146:293–300. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.11.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.von Rhein D, Beckmann CF, Franke B, Oosterlaan J, Heslenfeld DJ, Hoekstra PJ, et al. Network‐level assessment of reward‐related activation in patients with ADHD and healthy individuals. Human Brain Map. 2017;38:2359–69. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23522. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Carnell S, Benson L, Pantazatos SP, Hirsch J, Geliebter A. Amodal brain activation and functional connectivity in response to high‐energy‐density food cues in obesity. Obesity. 2014;22:2370–8. doi: 10.1002/oby.20859. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Burger KS, Sanders AJ, Gilbert JR. Hedonic hunger is related to increased neural and perceptual responses to cues of palatable food and motivation to consume: evidence from 3 independent investigations, 2. J Nutr. 2016;146:1807–12. doi: 10.3945/jn.116.231431. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Rothemund Y, Preuschhof C, Bohner G, Bauknecht H-C, Klingebiel R, Flor H, et al. Differential activation of the dorsal striatum by high-calorie visual food stimuli in obese individuals. Neuroimage. 2007;37:410–21. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.05.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Beauchaine TP, Constantino JN. Redefining the endophenotype concept to accommodate transdiagnostic vulnerabilities and etiological complexity. Biomarkers in medicine. 2017;11:769–80. doi: 10.2217/bmm-2017-0002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Barcellos SH, Carvalho LS & Turley P. Education can reduce health differences related to genetic risk of obesity. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 10.1073/pnas.1802909115. 2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 58.Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Tomasi D, Baler RD. Obesity and addiction: neurobiological overlaps. Obes Rev. 2013;14:2–18. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-789X.2012.01031.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Biederman J, Ball SW, Monuteaux MC, Mick E, Spencer TJ, McCREARY M, et al. New insights into the comorbidity between ADHD and major depression in adolescent and young adult females. J Am Acad Child Adols Psychiatry. 2008;47:426–34. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e31816429d3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Lazarevich I, Camacho MEI, del Consuelo Velázquez-Alva M, Zepeda MZ. Relationship among obesity, depression, and emotional eating in young adults. Appetite. 2016;107:639–44. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2016.09.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Pingault JB, O’Reilly PF, Schoeler T, Ploubidis GB, Rijsdijk F & Dudbridge F. Using genetic data to strengthen causal inference in observational research. Nat Rev Genet. 2018;19:566–80. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 62.Mellström E, Forsman C, Engh L, Hallerbäck MU & Wikström S. Methylphenidate and reduced overweight in children with ADHD. J Attention Disord. 1087054718808045 (2018). 10.1177/1087054718808045. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 63.Leddy JJ, Epstein LH, Jaroni JL, Roemmich JN, Paluch RA, Goldfield GS, et al. Influence of methylphenidate on eating in obese men. Obes Res. 2004;12:224–32. doi: 10.1038/oby.2004.29. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Danilovich N, Mastrandrea LD, Cataldi L, Quattrin T. Methylphenidate decreases fat and carbohydrate intake in obese teenagers. Obesity. 2014;22:781–5. doi: 10.1002/oby.20574. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Scheres A, Milham MP, Knutson B, Castellanos FX. Ventral striatal hyporesponsiveness during reward anticipation in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;61:720–4. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.04.042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Ströhle A, Stoy M, Wrase J, Schwarzer S, Schlagenhauf F, Huss M, et al. Reward anticipation and outcomes in adult males with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Neuroimage. 2008;39:966–72. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.09.044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Stoy M, Schlagenhauf F, Schlochtermeier L, Wrase J, Knutson B, Lehmkuhl U, et al. Reward processing in male adults with childhood ADHD—a comparison between drug-naive and methylphenidate-treated subjects. Psychopharmacology. 2011;215:467–81. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2166-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Choi SW, Mak TSH & O’Reilly P. A guide to performing Polygenic Risk Score analyses. bioRxiv. 10.1101/416545. 2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplemental Material (789.9KB, docx)

Articles from Molecular Psychiatry are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES