Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2022 May 1.
Published in final edited form as: Patient Educ Couns. 2020 Oct 6;104(5):1049–1058. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2020.10.002

Patient-provider communication patterns among Asian American immigrant subgroups in New York City

Serena Phillips 1,4, Laura C Wyatt 2,4, Monique M Turner 3, Chau Trinh-Shevrin 2, Simona C Kwon 2
PMCID: PMC8021612  NIHMSID: NIHMS1635317  PMID: 33097362

Abstract

Objectives:

Patient-provider communication is essential for patient-centered care, yet Asian American immigrant populations face barriers. We aim to describe: 1) patient-reported communication-related characteristics for 16 disaggregated Asian American subgroups; and 2) the association of patient comprehension of provider communication with socio-demographics, language proficiency and concordance, and perceived cultural sensitivity in this population.

Methods:

Descriptive statistics are presented for 1,269 Asian American immigrants responding to cross-sectional, venue-sampled surveys conducted in New York City. Logistic regression models examine predictors of low comprehension of provider communication.

Results:

Approximately 10% of respondents reported low comprehension of provider communication: lowest among South Asians and highest among Southeast Asians. Eighty-four percent were language-concordant with their provider, 90.1% agreed that their provider understood their background and values, and 16.5% felt their provider looked down on them. Low comprehension of provider communication was significantly associated with Southeast Asian subgroup, less education, limited English proficiency, public health insurance, patient-provider language discordance, and perceived low cultural understanding.

Conclusion:

Among our sample, language and cultural sensitivity are associated with comprehension of provider communication.

Practice Implications:

Strategies improving language access and cultural sensitivity may be important for Asian immigrant patients. These could include interpretation services, bilingual community-based providers, and cultural sensitivity training.

Keywords: Asian American, patient-provider communication, limited English proficiency, cultural competence, immigrant health, New York City

1. Introduction

The Asian American population is the fastest growing racial group in the United States (US) [1]. Roughly 20.4 million people, or 6.3% of the US population identify as Asian alone or in combination with one or more other races [2]. In New York State, nearly two-thirds of Asian Americans are immigrants, and most (71%) of Asians live in New York City (NYC) [3]. Nearly 1.3 million Asian American individuals live in NYC, comprising 15.2% of the total population (U.S. Census Bureau, 2017). Partly due to the model minority myth stereotype, Asian Americans are often believed not to experience health disparities [4]. However, there are differences in various health measures for Asian American subgroups. For example, compared to the overall uninsured population in NYC (9%), Koreans and Chinese are more likely to be uninsured (17% and 11%) [5]. South Asian New Yorkers are at a higher risk for obesity, hypertension, and diabetes. Many Asian groups have high prevalence of gastric cancer compared to other racial groups [6]. Asian American immigrants face challenges such as lack of access to health insurance and health care utilization [7]. Foreign-born individuals are more likely than native-born individuals to be limited English-proficient (LEP), and prevalence of LEP is disproportionately high among Asian Americans [8].

Despite the growing need to further understand the health patterns and needs of Asian American communities, gaps in knowledge and data remain. Most large health surveys are not being conducted in Asian languages, while Asian Americans, especially immigrants, are more likely to be LEP [8], resulting in systematic exclusion from data collection [9]. Moreover, some surveys only have small Asian sample sizes, yielding low statistical power, or aggregate this socially, linguistically, and culturally diverse racial category, masking the distinct health needs and disparities of specific Asian subgroups [9, 10]. In light of these challenges, there is a clear need for more disaggregated data about health and health care experiences of distinct Asian American immigrant communities.

The ability of physicians to effectively and compassionately communicate information is key to a successful patient–physician relationship [11]. The Patient-Centered Culturally Sensitive Health Care Model provides a framework that can help improve health care among low-income minority communities [1214]. This framework describes the following strategies for improving healthcare quality: promoting cultural and linguistic competence in the delivery of healthcare, and promoting culturally sensitive healthcare by improving the patient-provider relationship [14]. According to Tucker et al., health care that is “based on the views of culturally diverse patients rather than the views of health care professionals,” may lead to more positive health behaviors and health outcomes among minority patients [13].

Effective provider-patient communication can establish trust and rapport, transmit health information, empower patients, increase collaborative goal setting, and facilitate agreement on medical decisions [1518]. Provider-patient communication has been strongly linked to patient satisfaction, and less consistently, with other outcomes including comprehension and recall of medical information and treatment adherence [16]. Patient comprehension during medical interactions is critical to the patient’s safety, informed decision-making, and ability to adhere to health-promoting behaviors. Both the provider’s communication skills and the patient’s health literacy level impact the patient’s ability to comprehend health information [19].

Among immigrant populations, language discordance is a potential barrier to effective patient-provider communication [20], and LEP patients are more likely to report lower comprehension of medical situations and poorer quality communication compared to their English-proficient counterparts, especially if they are language-discordant with their providers [2125]. Studies have also associated LEP, low health literacy, and patient-provider language discordance with more distal negative outcomes such as receipt of less health education, poorer self-rated health, and increased risk of death [23, 26, 27].

Deficiencies in cultural sensitivity, the ability to communicate and interact across cultures, also contributes to sub-optimal health care interactions and other negative outcomes. The lack of cultural sensitivity includes implicit and explicit physician bias and discrimination [2833]. In focus groups conducted by Gonzalez and colleagues [34], Black and Latinx patients described negative experiences with race-based assumptions, microaggressions, and inferior treatment by health care staff. There has also been evidence from racially-discordant interactions with Black patients that physician bias is associated with more dominant linguistic patterns, as well as lower patient perceptions of provider respect, friendliness, and patient-centeredness [35, 36].

Potentially due to the model minority myth stereotype, providers may under-recommend breast, cervical, and colorectal cancer screenings to Asian Americans, despite cancer being the leading cause of death for this racial group [37]. Misunderstandings can arise related to differences in values and beliefs; cultural conceptions of health and illness; and expectations of provider, patient, and family roles [29]. The importance of perceived provider cultural sensitivity is underscored in the Patient-Centered Culturally Sensitive Health Care Model, which describes causal links to patient satisfaction, trust, and other outcomes [13, 38].

This study had the following aims: 1) to describe patient-provider communication-related characteristics, measured by an individual’s comprehension of provider communication for 16 disaggregated Asian American subgroups in NYC; and 2) to describe the association of patient comprehension of provider communication with socio-demographics, patient-provider language concordance, language proficiency, and perceived cultural sensitivity in this population. We hypothesize that report of low comprehension of provider communication will be associated with LEP, patient-provider language discordance, and report of poor provider cultural sensitivity.

2. Methods

2.1. Design and Procedure

Data for this study originate from a cross-sectional, large-scale Community Health Resources and Needs Assessment (CHRNA) survey conducted in NYC by the NYU Center for the Study of Asian American Health between 2013 and 2016, with data collection occurring primarily in 2014 and 2015. The survey was administered to Asian American adults aged 18–85 living in NYC. Participants were recruited at community events (e.g. faith-based gatherings, health screenings, and cultural events). Venue-based sampling was used, leveraging partnerships with community-based organizations serving Asian American communities in the NYC metropolitan area to enable survey administration in respondents’ preferred languages. While English surveys were offered, the majority of surveys were completed in Asian languages. Fifteen total Asian American groups were targeted: Asian Indian, Bangladeshi, Pakistani, Himalayan, Sri Lankan, Indo-Caribbean, Chinese, Korean, Japanese, Filipino, Vietnamese, Cambodian, Indonesian, Burmese, and Thai. Outreach occurred separately for each group. In order to account for ethnic subgroup differences, the Himalayan group was further divided into Tibetan and Nepali for analyses. Respondents were asked questions about their socio-demographics, health care experiences, and other health behaviors.

Out of an initial sample of 1,684 survey respondents, individuals born outside of the US, without a regular health care provider, and not answering the main study outcome (comprehension of provider communication) were excluded, resulting in a final sample of 1,269 individuals. Those without a health care provider (answering “I have no regular health care provider” to the question “Do you have a health care provider who speaks a language in which you can comfortably communicate?”) were excluded because additional questions related to comprehension of provider communication were asked only of individuals who had a provider.

2.2. Measures

The dependent variable for analysis is the patient’s level of comprehension of provider communication. Survey participants were first asked: “Do you have a health care provider who speaks a language in which you can comfortably communicate?”) Answers included “yes,” “no,” and “I have no regular health provider.” Among those answering “yes” or “no,” the following question was asked: “During your visit, how much of what the doctor said did you understand?” Those who responded “some” or “only a little” were classified as having low comprehension of provider communication, and those who answered “everything” or “most” were classified as having high comprehension of provider communication. This question was modified from the Commonwealth Fund 2001 Health Care Quality Survey [39, 40], but unlike on the Commonwealth Fund survey, our question does not ask what type of provider/doctor was seen, nor the provider/doctor’s visit referenced.

Asian American subgroups were defined using the community sampling venue and self-reported Asian ethnicity by standard Census classifications [41]. The 16 detailed Asian subgroups were categorized into three broader ethnic subgroups, defined as follows: South Asians (Asian Indian, Bangladeshi, Pakistani, Nepali, Sri Lankan, Indo-Caribbean), East Asians (Chinese, Korean, Japanese, Tibetan), and Southeast Asians (Filipino, Vietnamese, Cambodian, Indonesian, Burmese, Thai). Indo-Caribbeans were classified with South Asians based on Asian Indian ethnicity and ancestor country of origin.

Main independent variables included the respondent’s English fluency, language concordance with his or her health care provider, and perception of the provider’s cultural sensitivity. In keeping with a commonly used operationalization of “limited English proficiency,” English fluency categories were collapsed into a three-level variable: speaking English “very well,” (fluent), “well,” and “not well or “not at all” [42]. Those answering “yes” to the question, “Do you have a health care provider who speaks a language in which you can comfortably communicate?” were considered language-concordant with their provider, while those who answered “no” were not. Our question was adapted from previous survey questions on the Behavioral Risk Factor Surveillance System (BRFSS) [43].

Cultural sensitivity was assessed using two questions. The first was: “I feel that my doctor understands my background and values.” Those answering “strongly agree” or “somewhat agree” had high cultural understanding and those answering “strongly disagree” or “somewhat disagree” had low cultural understanding. The second question was: “I often feel as if my doctor looks down on me and the way I live my life.” Those answering “strongly agree” or “somewhat agree” had high perceived bias and those answering “strongly disagree” or “somewhat disagree” had low perceived bias. These questions were adapted from the Commonwealth Fund 2001 Health Care Quality Survey [39, 40].

Socio-demographic variables included age, gender, years in the US, and education (less than high school, high school/some college, and college graduate). Health insurance was grouped into private, public, and uninsured.

2.3. Statistical analyses

All statistical analyses were conducted using SPSS version 25. Descriptive statistics were tabulated to characterize the overall sample and were presented for each Asian American subgroup (South Asian, East Asian, and Southeast Asian). A multivariable logistic regression model predicting low comprehension of provider communication was constructed for the overall sample while adjusting for Asian American subgroup. This was followed by two subsequent models: Model 1 included Asian American subgroup, age, gender, time in the US, and education; and Model 2 included all variables from Model 1 as well as English fluency, self-rated health, health insurance, patient-provider language concordance, and cultural sensitivity (cultural understanding and perceived bias).

3. Results

3.1. Respondent characteristics

Table 1 presents descriptive sample characteristics overall and by Asian American subgroup. Respondents (n=1,269) reported a mean age of 48.9 years old and averaged 16.8 years of residency in the US. A larger number of respondents were female (58.7%), college graduates (41.3%), and had public insurance (56.0%), with annual household income <$25,000 (30.3%). Distributions for these characteristics varied widely across groups, and one-quarter of participants did not disclose income. Twenty-six percent of the sample spoke English “very well,” while 36.0% spoke English “not well” or “not at all.”

Table 1.

Respondent characteristics from the Community Health Resources and Needs Assessments by Asian American immigrant subgroup, n (%)

Total (n=1,269) South Asian (n=562) East Asian (n=400) Southeast Asian (n=307) p-value
Age, mean ± SD 48.9 ± 15.9 45.7 ± 14.5 55.5 ± 16.2 46.3 ± 15.3 <0.001
Gender 0.001
 Female 744 (58.7) 305 (54.3) 265 (66.4) 174 (56.7)
 Male 524 (41.3) 257 (45.7) 134 (33.6) 133 (43.3)
Education completed <0.001
 Less than high school 345 (27.6) 151 (26.9) 133 (34.0) 61 (20.5)
 High school/some college 389 (31.1) 199 (35.5) 99 (25.3) 91 (30.5)
 College graduate 516 (41.3) 211 (37.6) 159 (40.7) 146 (49.0) <0.001
Number of years in the US, mean (SD) 16.8 ± 10.9 14.1 ± 9.7 20.3 ± 11.6 17.2 ± 11.0 <0.001
Number of years in the US, categories
 ≤5 years 191 (16.0) 105 (19.6) 35 (9.6) 51 (17.3)
 6–10 years 257 (21.5) 140 (26.2) 59 (16.1) 58 (19.7)
 11–20 years 353 (29.5) 167 (31.2) 117 (32.0) 69 (23.5)
 >20 years 394 (33.0) 123 (23.0) 155 (42.3) 116 (39.5)
Annual Household Income 0.002
 <$25,000 385 (30.3) 184 (32.7) 110 (27.5) 91 (29.6)
 $25,000-$55,000 292 (23.0) 148 (26.3) 71 (17.8) 73 (23.8)
 >$55,000 284 (22.4) 112 (19.9) 99 (24.8) 73 (23.8)
 Don’t know/ Refused/ Skipped 308 (24.3) 118 (21.0) 120 (30.0) 70 (22.8)
English proficiency <0.001
 Very well 328 (26.0) 203 (36.2) 43 (10.9) 82 (27.1)
 Well 478 (38.0) 226 (40.3) 130 (32.9) 122 (40.3)
 Not well /Not at all 453 (36.0) 132 (23.5) 222 (56.2) 99 (32.7)
Insurance <0.001
 Public 698 (56.0) 353 (63.3) 207 (53.6) 138 (45.7)
 Private 382 (30.7) 140 (25.1) 124 (32.1) 118 (39.1)
 No insurance 166 (13.3) 65 (11.6) 55 (14.2) 46 (15.2)

Table 2 presents descriptive statistics of comprehension of provider communication, patient-provider language concordance, and cultural sensitivity (cultural understanding and perceived bias). The vast majority (83.6%) of the sample reported language concordance with their provider; concordance was highest among East Asians (88.2%), followed by South Asians (85.6%) and Southeast Asians (73.9%). A total of 90.1% of individuals agreed that their provider (doctor) understands their background and values; this was highest among South Asians (93.2%), followed by East Asians (90.2%) and Southeast Asians (84.0%). Additionally, 16.5% agreed that they often feel as if their provider (doctor) looks down on them and the way they live their life; this was highest among South Asians (17.8%), followed by East Asian (16.0%) and Southeast Asians (14.7%). Out of the entire sample, 10.7% reported some or only a little understanding of what the doctor said; this was highest among Southeast Asians (21.5%), followed by East Asians (9.5%) and South Asians (5.5%).

Table 2.

Patient-provider communication and cultural competence measures by Asian American immigrant subgroup, n (%)

Total (n=1,269) South Asian (n=562) East Asian (n=400) Southeast Asian (n=307) p-value
Do you have a health care provider who speaks a language in which you can comfortably communicate? (Patient-provider language concordance) <0.001
 Yes 1061 (83.6) 481 (85.6) 353 (88.2) 227 (73.9)
 No 208 (16.4) 81 (14.4) 47 (11.8) 80 (26.1)
I feel that my doctor understands my background and values (Cultural understanding) <0.001
 Strongly Agree 660 (56.7) 367 (67.6) 166 (49.1) 127 (45.0)
 Somewhat agree 388 (33.4) 139 (25.6) 139 (41.1) 110 (39.0)
 Somewhat disagree 83 (7.1) 21 (3.9) 23 (6.8) 39 (13.8)
 Strongly disagree 32 (2.8) 16 (3.0) 10 (3.0) 6 (2.1)
I often feel as if my doctor looks down on me and the way I live my life (Perceived bias) <0.001
 Strongly Agree 87 (8.0) 51 (10.0) 26 (8.3) 10 (3.8)
 Somewhat agree 93 (8.5) 40 (7.8) 24 (7.7) 29 (10.9)
 Somewhat disagree 183 (16.8) 71 (13.9) 47 (15.0) 65 (24.4)
 Strongly disagree 728 (66.7) 350 (68.4) 216 (69.0) 162 (60.9)
During your visit, how much of what the doctor said did you understand? (Comprehension of provider communication) <0.001
 Everything 774 (61.0) 381 (67.8) 245 (61.3) 148 (48.2)
 Most 360 (28.4) 150 (26.7) 117 (29.3) 93 (30.3)
 Some 73 (5.8) 23 (4.1) 22 (5.5) 28 (9.1)
 Only a little 62 (4.9) 8 (1.4) 16 (4.0) 38 (12.4)

The appendix (tables ac) includes descriptive statistics of the disaggregated South Asian, East Asian, and Southeast Asian subgroups.

Figure 1 presents comprehension of provider communication by detailed Asian American subgroup. With disaggregation, South Asian groups were shown to have differing rates of low comprehension of provider communication (Sri Lankan – 9.9%, Nepali – 8.3%, Indo-Caribbean – 7.7%, Bangladeshi – 5.8%, Asian Indian – 1.0%, and Pakistani – 1.2%). Among East Asians, Tibetan (47.4%) had higher rates compared to the other groups (Chinese – 7.8%, Japanese – 7.6%, and Korean – 7.4%). Finally, while rates were high among overall Southeast Asians, Burmese (4.8%) and Filipino (8.9%) groups had much lower rates of low comprehension of provider communication compared to other Southeast Asian groups (Cambodian – 56.3%, Thai – 27.6%, Indonesian – 21.7%, and Vietnamese – 19.0%)

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Low comprehension of provider communication by detailed Asian American immigrant subgroup

3.2. Multivariable analyses

In the unadjusted model (n=1,269), Southeast Asians had 4.69 times the odds and East Asians had 1.80 times the odds of low comprehension of provider communication, when compared to South Asians (p<0.001 and p=0.020, respectively). In Model 1, Southeast Asian ethnicity remained statistically significant (aOR=5.80, p<0.001), but East Asian ethnicity did not. In addition, females had 1.59 times the odds compared to males of low comprehension of provider communication (p=0.033). Compared to college graduates, individuals with less than a college education had higher odds of reporting low comprehension of provider communication (less than high school: aOR=9.30, p<0.001, high school/some college: aOR=3.32, p<0.001). In Model 2, Southeast Asian subgroup (aOR=3.39, p<0.001) and less than high school education (aOR=4.04, p=0.006) remained significant, but gender did not. Additional significant factors included: LEP (aOR: 4.24, p=0.012), public health insurance (aOR=2.77, p=0.018), patient-provider language discordance (aOR=5.30, p<0.001), and perceived lower cultural understanding (aOR 6.31, p<0.001). Perceived bias was not a significant predictor of low comprehension of provider communication.

Due to observed gender differences in Model 1, additional logistic regression was run stratifying by gender. Southeast Asian ethnicity, patient-provider language concordance, and provider cultural understanding remained a significant predictor of low comprehension of provider communication for both males and females. However, lower education was a significant predictor of low comprehension of provider communication among females, while insurance (public or uninsured) was a significant predictor of low comprehension of provider communication among males (data not presented).

4. Discussion and Conclusions

4.1. Discussion

Among a large sample of Asian American immigrants living in NYC, we found that Southeast Asian subgroup, lack of patient-provider language concordance, and lower perceived cultural understanding were significant predictors of low comprehension of provider communication. Additionally, speaking English not well or not at all, public insurance, and less than high school education were also significantly associated with low comprehension of the provider in our full model.

LEP and patient-provider language discordance have a demonstrated association with patient-provider communication in past research [22, 24, 25, 44]. However, some detailed Asian American subgroups reported both high rates of LEP and patient-provider language concordance. For example, 88.3% of Chinese American respondents spoke English less than “very well,” yet 94.6% reported language concordance with their doctors; similar findings were seen among Korean respondents (see appendix). This pattern of high language concordance was true for Chinese-speaking respondents regardless of dialect, although LEP was especially high among those speaking dialects outside of Mandarin and Cantonese (data not shown).

While it is possible that respondents felt that they could comfortably communicate with their doctors despite lacking English language skills, it is also likely that the Chinese American respondents in NYC experience high availability of language-concordant doctors who speak a Chinese language, compared to the other Asian American subgroups. Well-established clinics and provider networks offering Chinese in-language health care services in NYC include the Charles B. Wang Community Health Center (CBWCHC), the Chinese American Medical Society, and the Coalition of Asian American Independent Practice Association (CAIPA). CBWCHC is a federally qualified health center that was founded in 1971 and currently operates three sites located in historic Chinatown neighborhoods in NYC, employing mostly doctors who are bilingual in English and Chinese [45]. It served over 58,000 patients in 2017 [45]. The Chinese American Medical Society, a national professional organization dedicated to promoting Chinese American health and medical professionals, is also headquartered in NYC and has been operating since 1964 [46]. CAIPA includes over 1,000 private practice providers in the greater New York area who provide care to almost half-a-million largely Chinese American patients [47]. More recent Asian American immigrant populations reporting lower rates of language concordance, such as Cambodian, Indonesian, and Vietnamese, by contrast, are likely hampered by the lack of an established language-concordant health care workforce who can provide in-language community-based health care services. Language access may be more difficult for these smaller and more recently emerging populations in the NYC area.

Respondents’ reports of lower perceived cultural understanding were also significantly associated with lower comprehension of provider communication. This finding underscores the importance of patient-centered interactions in which doctors successfully convey understanding of the patient’s worldview and values [29]. While cultural sensitivity and patient-centered care have been most strongly tied to patient satisfaction, we provide evidence suggesting connections to patient comprehension of provider communication as well [13, 48].

Our findings also demonstrate that lower socioeconomic status is associated with low comprehension of provider communication, including less than high school education and public health insurance. Past research has found that physicians may have an unconscious bias toward individuals of low socio-economic status, and these biases may affect the health care of these patients [49]. Others have also documented links between patient educational level and both health literacy and communicative style with providers [50, 51]. Further research needs to investigate the possible association between lower socioeconomic status and patient-provider communication among Asian American and immigrant communities.

Study limitations should be mentioned. Generalizability of findings is limited due to use of a non-random venue-based sampling strategy; survey respondents may reflect those more inclined to participate in community-based events or those in greater need of services provided by community-based organizations than the population at large. Proportions are descriptive for our specific sample and cannot be interpreted as estimates of population prevalence. Furthermore, missing data excluded part of our sample from the multivariable analysis, creating another source of potential bias. Observed differences across Asian American subgroups may reflect differences in venue client bases, survey administration, or cultural norms around answering questions, rather than differences in phenomena of interest in the underlying community. Social desirability bias could be an issue if respondents wished to avoid being critical of their health care providers or believed that certain response options were more desirable than others and should be investigated in future research. Patient-provider communication-related constructs were measured with single items available in the survey rather than multi-item validated scales, which decreases the stability of findings. Additionally, if a participant did not have a health provider, they were excluded from answering provider-related questions, and the question “During your visit, how much of what the doctor said did you understand?” does not assess specific information about when the last visit with the doctor/provider was, or what type of doctor/provider was visited (primary or specialist). Future research should take care to include individuals without regular providers and assess additional provider characteristics. Finally, data on patient-provider language concordance were collected, but the language of patient-provider communication was not assessed. This detail is especially important because there is mixed evidence, implying contextual nuances, regarding whether English-proficient patients and LEP patients with language-concordant providers in non-English languages differ in health care communication outcomes [21, 22, 52].

However, a major study strength is our sample’s uncommon ability to offer disaggregated findings for diverse Asian American subgroups, including LEP individuals, who are typically aggregated or excluded from analyses. Data provide the important opportunity to illuminate trends for smaller Asian American immigrant communities for whom little health research exists in the US, such as Himalayan immigrants. The community-based participatory approach to data collection lent language capacity and entre into “hard to reach” populations often overlooked in research. These include individuals who may have limited interaction with governmental or mainstream institutions but may also experience distinct health disparities. We also offer the valuable patient perspective of patient-provider interactions, which is an important compliment to provider-reported data which include possibly unreliable self-report of second language fluency to establish measures like language concordance [24].

Many opportunities exist for further investigation. We demonstrate the feasibility of recruiting sizable samples of diverse LEP populations through community-based partnerships, which is a strategy that others could adopt in future studies to prevent perpetuating the marginalization of these populations in research. Findings should be replicated with validated scales and multi-item measures. As a quantitative analysis of survey data, the present study elucidates trends among Asian American immigrant groups but does not have the ability to explain the patterns observed or describe lived experiences. Interviews, direct observations, and other qualitative methods would provide a richer understanding of the role of patient-provider communication among these communities, and possibly point to other important unexamined factors. These methods would also provide the opportunity to study other important aspects of patient-provider communication beyond our narrow scope of comprehension of information, such as non-verbal communication, rapport-building behaviors, and opportunities to ask questions. Study findings can inform the development of interventions to improve outreach and the quality of health care interactions. Future studies could also examine more distal outcomes beyond understanding of communication, such as health behavior, treatment adherence, and health care service utilization.

4.2. Conclusion

In conclusion, this study demonstrated important differences in our study outcome by Asian American subgroup, with Southeast Asian Americans experiencing lower comprehension of provider communication. Lack of provider cultural understanding, not have a doctor speaking the same language, LEP, and lower socioeconomic status are also significantly associated with low comprehension of provider communication. These findings reinforce that patient-centered culturally sensitive health care may potentially mitigate disparities in comprehension of health care information among Asian American immigrant communities.

4.3. Practice Implications

These findings lend support for initiatives addressing LEP and language discordance in health care, such as interpretation services in Asian languages and diversification of the community-based health care workforce to include bilingual practitioners. Diversification of the health care workforce could improve circumstances by introducing bicultural staff able to understand racial/ethnic minority worldviews in addition to providing in-language services to LEP Asian American immigrant patients. With increasing recognition of the importance of cultural competence, numerous trainings and resources have also been developed to teach health care providers skills in cultural sensitivity, patient-centered communication, and recognition of implicit bias [5356].

Table 3.

Relationship of Asian American immigrant subgroup with low comprehension of provider communication

Unadjusted (n=1,269) Model 1 (n=1,158) Model 2 (n=984)
OR (95% CI) p-value OR (95% CI) p-value OR (95% CI) p-value
Asian American subgroup
 South Asian Reference Reference Reference
 East Asian 1.80 (1.10, 2.94) 0.020 1.48 (0.85, 2.56) 0.167 0.75 (0.36, 1.54) 0.431
 Southeast Asian 4.69 (2.98, 7.38) <0.001 5.80 (3.47, 9.67) <0.001 3.39 (1.80, 6.41) <0.001
Age, years 1.00 (0.99, 1.02) 0.918 0.99 (0.97, 1.01) 0.343
Gender
 Female 1.59 (1.04, 2.45) 0.033 1.47 (0.85, 2.55) 0.164
 Male Reference Reference
Time in US, years 1.00 (0.98, 1.02) 0.697 1.02 (0.99, 1.05) 0.293
Education
 Less than high school 9.30 (5.07, 17.05) <0.001 4.04 (1.73, 9.41) 0.006
 High school/Some college 3.32 (1.80, 6.13) <0.001 1.97 (0.88, 4.41) 0.100
 College graduate Reference Reference
English spoken fluency
 Very well Reference
 Well 2.25 (0.79, 6.43) 0.132
 Not well/Not at all 4.24 (1.38, 13.02) 0.012
Health insurance
 Private Reference
 Public 2.77 (1.19, 6.44) 0.018
 Uninsured 1.38 (0.48, 3.93) 0.548
Patient-provider language concordance
 Yes Reference
 No 5.30 (3.05, 9.18) <0.001
Cultural understanding
 Agree (Strongly or somewhat) Reference
 Disagree (Strongly or somewhat) 6.31 (3.17, 12.54) <0.001
Physician bias
 Agree (Strongly or somewhat) 1.54 (0.81, 2.93) 0.192
 Disagree (Strongly or somewhat) Reference

Highlights.

  • Differences between Asian American subgroups are highlighted, including a lower percentage of Southeast Asians having a doctor with whom they could communicate and feeling that their doctor did not understand their background and values.

  • Southeast Asians experience a significant disadvantage due to low comprehension of provider communication compared to South Asians in adjusted analysis.

  • Lower interpersonal cultural competence by a provider, not having a doctor speaking the same language, limited English proficiency, and lower socioeconomic status (public insurance, lower education) are significantly associated with low comprehension provider communication.

Acknowledgments

Funding sources

This publication is supported in part by the National Institutes of Health–National Institute on Minority Health and Health Disparities award number U54MD000538, the National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences award number UL1TR001445, and the National Cancer Institute award number P30CA016087. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the funding agencies.

Appendix

Table A.

Descriptive statistics of disaggregated South Asian immigrant subgroups (n=562)

Asian Indian (n=96) Bangladeshi (n=139) Pakistani (n=82) Nepali (n=96) Sri Lankan (n=71) Indo-Caribbean (n=78)
Age, mean ± SD 50.6 ± 15.7 40.8 ± 13.0 47.5 ± 15.5 43.6 ± 12.1 45.2 ± 12.6 49.9 ± 15.5
Gender
 Female 51 (53.1) 93 (66.9) 37 (45.1) 48 (50.0) 34 (47.9) 42 (53.8)
 Male 45 (46.9) 46 (33.1) 45 (54.9) 48 (50.0) 37 (52.1) 36 (46.2)
Education completed
 Less than high school 25 (26.0) 31 (22.3) 19 (23.2) 44 (45.8) 6 (8.6) 26 (33.3)
 High school/some college 16 (16.7) 53 (38.1) 28 (34.1) 34 (35.4) 38 (54.3) 30 (38.5)
 College graduate 55 (57.3) 55 (39.6) 35 (42.7) 18 (18.8) 26 (37.1) 22 (28.2)
Years in the U.S., mean ± SD 17.5 ± 11.7 10.8 ± 7.1 15.2 ± 9.7 7.9 ± 5.4 16.3 ± 7.6 20.8 ± 10.4
Insurance
 Public 45 (46.9) 105 (75.5) 56 (69.1) 71 (74.0) 35 (50.7) 41 (53.2)
 Private 36 (37.5) 21 (15.1) 19 (23.5) 10 (10.4) 31 (44.9) 23 (29.9)
 No insurance 15 (15.6) 13 (9.4) 6 (7.4) 15 (15.6) 3 (4.3) 13 (16.9)
Annual Household Income
 <$25,000 35 (36.5) 53 (38.1) 29 (35.4) 35 (36.5) 7 (9.9) 25 (32.1)
 $25,000–$55,000 18 (18.8) 31 (22.3) 20 (24.4) 31 (32.3) 38 (53.5) 10 (12.8)
 >$55,000 26 (27.1) 15 (10.8) 18 (22.0) 7 (7.3) 20 (28.2) 26 (33.3)
 Don’t know/Refused/Skipped 17 (17.7) 40 (28.8) 15 (18.3) 23 (24.0) 6 (8.5) 17 (21.8)
English proficiency
 Very well 42 (43.8) 33 (23.9) 30 (36.6) 11 (11.5) 30 (42.3) 57 (73.1)
 Less than very well 54 (56.2) 105 (76.1) 52 (63.4) 85 (88.5) 41 (57.7) 21 (26.9)
Do you have a health care provider who speaks a language in which you can comfortably communicate? (Patient-provider language concordance)
 Yes 82 (85.4) 119 (85.6) 78 (95.1) 73 (76.0) 58 (81.7) 71 (91.0)
 No 14 (14.6) 20 (14.4) 4 (4.9) 23 (24.0) 13 (18.3) 7 (9.0)
I feel that my doctor understands my background and values (Cultural understanding)
 Strongly Agree 69 (74.2) 105 (76.1) 63 (78.8) 48 (51.6) 41 (60.3) 41 (57.7)
 Somewhat agree 16 (17.2) 28 (20.3) 14 (17.5) 33 (35.5) 23 (33.8) 25 (35.2)
 Somewhat disagree 2 (2.2) 2 (1.4) 1 (1.3) 9 (9.7) 4 (5.9) 3 (4.2)
 Strongly disagree 6 (6.5) 3 (2.2) 2 (2.5) 3 (3.2) 0 (0.0) 2 (2.8)
I often feel as if my doctor looks down on me and the way I live my life (Perceived bias)
 Strongly Agree 15 (18.1) 9 (6.7) 7 (9.1) 5 (5.6) 5 (7.8) 10 (15.6)
 Somewhat agree 9 (10.8) 10 (7.5) 4 (5.2) 7 (7.8) 7 (10.9) 3 (4.7)
 Somewhat disagree 6 (7.2) 22 (16.4) 7 (9.1) 11 (12.2) 11 (17.2) 14 (21.9)
 Strongly disagree 53 (63.9) 93 (69.4) 59 (76.6) 67 (74.4) 41 (64.1) 37 (57.8)
During your visit, how much of what the doctor said did you understand? (Comprehension of provider communication)
 Everything 69 (71.9) 110 (79.1) 68 (82.9) 42 (43.8) 44 (62.0) 48 (61.5)
 Most 26 (27.1) 21 (15.1) 13 (15.9) 46 (47.9) 20 (28.2) 24 (30.8)
 Some 0 (0.0) 6 (4.3) 1 (1.2) 4 (4.2) 6 (8.5) 6 (7.7)
 Only a little 1 (1.0) 2 (1.4) 0 (0.0) 4 (4.2) 1 (1.4) 0 (0.0)

Table B.

Descriptive statistics of disaggregated East Asian immigrant subgroups (n=400)

Chinese (n=166) Korean (n=136) Japanese (n=79) Tibetan (n=19)
Age, mean ± SD 54.2 ± 15.7 56.5 ± 17.8 56.1 ± 14.4 56.4 ± 16.1
Gender
 Female 114 (69.1) 82 (60.3) 56 (70.9) 13 (68.4)
 Male 51 (30.9) 54 (39.7) 23 (29.1) 6 (31.6)
Education completed
 Less than high school 92 (56.8) 22 (16.7) 1 (1.3) 18 (94.7)
 High school/some college 33 (20.4) 42 (31.8) 24 (30.8) 0 (0.0)
 College graduate 37 (22.8) 68 (51.5) 53 (67.9) 1 (5.3)
Years in the U.S. [mean (SD)] 18.8 ± 9.8 21.2 ± 11.4 26.7 ± 14.2 7.8 ± 5.6
Insurance
 Public 95 (58.6) 69 (53.9) 29 (37.2) 14 (77.8)
 Private 49 (30.2) 28 (21.9) 45 (57.7) 2 (11.1)
 No insurance 18 (11.1) 31 (24.2) 4 (5.1) 2 (11.1)
Annual Household Income
 <$25,000 58 (34.9) 36 (26.5) 9 (11.4) 7 (36.8)
 $25,000–$55,000 27 (16.3) 29 (21.3) 13 (16.5) 2 (10.5)
 >$55,000 20 (12.0) 34 (25.0) 41 (51.9) 4 (21.1)
 Don’t know/Refused 61 (36.7) 37 (27.2) 16 (20.3) 6 (31.6)
English proficiency
 Very well 19 (11.7) 5 (3.7) 18 (23.4) 1 (5.3)
 Less than very well 144 (88.3) 131 (96.3) 59 (76.6) 18 (94.7)
Do you have a health care provider who speaks a language in which you can comfortably communicate? (Patient-provider language concordance)
 Yes 157 (94.6) 123 (90.4) 68 (82.9) 7 (36.8)
 No 9 (5.4) 13 (9.6) 14 (17.1) 12 (63.2)
I feel that my doctor understands my background and values (Cultural understanding)
 Strongly Agree 90 (62.9) 45 (36.9) 23 (41.1) 8 (47.1)
 Somewhat agree 42 (29.4) 63 (51.6) 28 (50.0) 6 (35.3)
 Somewhat disagree 6 (4.2) 13 (10.7) 3 (5.4) 1 (5.9)
 Strongly disagree 5 (3.5) 1 (0.8) 2 (3.6) 2 (11.8)
I often feel as if my doctor looks down on me and the way I live my life (Perceived bias)
 Strongly Agree 24 (19.2) 1 (0.8) 0 (0.0) 1 (5.3)
 Somewhat agree 12 (9.6) 6 (4.8) 4 (9.3) 2 (10.5)
 Somewhat disagree 12 (9.6) 33 (26.2) 2 (4.7) 0 (0.0)
 Strongly disagree 77 (61.6) 86 (68.3) 37 (86.0) 16 (84.2)
During your visit, how much of what the doctor said did you understand? (Comprehension of provider communication)
 Everything 116 (69.6) 88 (64.7) 32 (40.5) 9 (47.4)
 Most 37 (22.3) 38 (27.9) 41 (51.9) 1 (5.3)
 Some 10 (6.0) 6 (4.4) 2 (2.5) 4 (21.1)
 Only a little 3 (1.8) 44 (2.9) 4 (5.1) 5 (26.3)

Table C.

Descriptive statistics of disaggregated Southeast Asian immigrant subgroups (n=307)

Southeast Asian (n=307)
Filipino (n=79) Vietnamese (n=63) Cambodian (n=48) Indonesian (n=46) Burmese (n=42) Thai (n=29)
Age, mean ± SD 45.3 ± 15.6 49.4 ± 17.1 51.9 ± 12.9 39.7 ± 11.6 40.7 ± 13.9 51.8 ± 15.3
Gender (Female)
 Female 49 (62.0) 34 (54.0) 29 (60.4) 24 (52.2) 16 (38.1) 22 (75.9)
 Male 30 (38.0) 29 (46.0) 19 (39.6) 22 (47.8) 26 (61.9) 7 (24.1)
Education completed
 Less than high school 1 (1.3) 26 (43.3) 29 (67.4) 1 (2.2) 2 (4.8) 2 (6.9)
 High school/some college 13 (16.7) 26 (43.3) 11 (25.6) 21 (45.7) 8 (19.0) 12 (41.4)
 College graduate 64 (82.1) 8 (13.3) 3 (7.0) 24 (52.2) 32 (76.2) 15 (51.7)
Years in the U.S., mean ± SD 9.7 ± 7.1 22.6 ± 8.9 29.3 ± 5.4 11.9 ± 10.2 11.4 ± 6.1 21.7 ± 11.8
Insurance
 Public 23 (29.1) 39 (63.9) 32 (68.1) 20 (43.5) 19 (47.5) 5 (17.2)
 Private 42 (53.2) 17 (27.9) 12 (25.5) 15 (32.6) 19 (47.5) 13 (44.8)
 No insurance 14 (17.7) 5 (8.2) 3 (6.4) 11 (23.9) 2 (5.0) 11 (37.9)
Annual Household Income
 <$25,000 10 (12.7) 27 (42.9) 21 (43.8) 19 (41.3) 7 (16.7) 7 (24.1)
 $25,000–$55,000 21 (26.6) 14 (22.2) 13 (27.1) 11 (23.9) 5 (11.9) 9 (31.0)
 >$55,000 27 (34.2) 12 (19.0) 6 (12.5) 9 (19.6) 11 (26.2) 8 (27.6)
 Don’t know/Refused 21 (26.6) 10 (15.9) 8 (16.7) 7 (15.2) 19 (45.2) 5 (17.2)
English proficiency
 Very well 37 (48.1) 10 (16.1) 9 (18.8) 8 (17.4) 15 (36.6) 3 (10.3)
 Less than very well 40 (51.9) 52 (83.9) 39 (81.3) 38 (82.6) 26 (63.4) 26 (89.7)
Do you have a health care provider who speaks a language in which you can comfortably communicate? (Patient-provider language concordance)
 Yes 69 (87.3) 46 (73.0) 20 (41.7) 27 (58.7) 42 (100.0) 23 (79.3)
 No 10 (12.7) 17 (27.0) 28 (58.3) 19 (41.3) 0 (0.0) 6 (20.7)
I feel that my doctor understands my background and values (Cultural understanding)
 Strongly Agree 48 (63.2) 29 (52.7) 3 (6.8) 16 (39.0) 20 (48.8) 11 (44.0)
 Somewhat agree 21 (27.6) 18 (32.7) 21 (47.7) 18 (43.9) 21 (51.2) 11 (44.0)
 Somewhat disagree 5 (6.6) 6 (10.9) 19 (43.2) 6 (14.6) 0 (0.0) 3 (12.0)
 Strongly disagree 2 (2.6) 2 (3.6) 1 (2.3) 1 (2.4) 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0)
I often feel as if my doctor looks down on me and the way I live my life (Perceived bias)
 Strongly Agree 4 (5.6) 1 (2.1) 0 (0.0) 4 (10.0) 1 (2.5) 0 (0.0)
 Somewhat agree 6 (8.3) 3 (6.3) 10 (24.4) 4 (10.0) 3 (7.5) 3 (12.0)
 Somewhat disagree 12 (16.7) 12 (25.0) 23 (56.1) 9 (22.5) 2 (5.0) 7 (28.0)
 Strongly disagree 50 (69.4) 32 (66.7) 8 (19.5) 23 (57.5) 34 (85.0) 15 (60.0)
During your visit, how much of what the doctor said did you understand? (Comprehension of provider communication)
 Everything 45 (57.0) 38 (60.3) 7 (14.6) 20 (43.5) 29 (69.0) 9 (31.0)
 Most 27 (34.2) 13 (20.6) 14 (29.2) 16 (34.8) 11 (26.2) 12 (41.4)
 Some 4 (5.1) 5 (7.9) 8 (16.7) 7 (15.2) 2 (4.8) 2 (6.9)
 Only a little 3 (3.8) 7 (11.1) 19 (39.6) 3 (6.5) 0 (0.0) 6 (20.7)

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

Declaration of Completing Interest

None.

Data availability statement

Data may be made available with a Data Use Agreement at the organization’s discretion.

References

  • [1].Lopez G, Ruiz N, Patten E, Key facts about Asian Americans, a diverse and growing population, 2017. https://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2017/09/08/key-facts-about-asian-americans/. (Accessed June 24, 2019.
  • [2].U.S. Census Bureau, ACS demographic and housing estimates: 2013–2017 American Community Survey 5-Year Estimates, 2017.
  • [3].DiNapoli TP, The Asian Community in New York State, Report 9–2016, February 2016. https://www.osc.state.ny.us/sites/default/files/reports/documents/pdf/2020-02/report-9-2016.pdf.
  • [4].Yi SS, Kwon SC, Sacks R, Trinh-Shevrin C, Commentary: Persistence and Health-Related Consequences of the Model Minority Stereotype for Asian Americans, Ethn Dis 26(1) (2016) 133–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [5].King L, Deng WQ, Health Disparities among Asian New Yorkers, New York City Department of Health and Mental Hygiene, March 2018.
  • [6].Taylor VM, Ko LK, Hwang JH, Sin MK, Inadomi JM, Gastric canc er in asian american populations: a neglected health disparity, Asian Pac J Cancer Prev 15(24) (2014) 10565–71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [7].Ye J, Mack D, Fry-Johnson Y, Parker K, Health care access and utilization among US-born and foreign-born Asian Americans, J Immigr Minor Health 14(5) (2012) 731–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [8].Zong J, Batalova J, The Limited English Proficient Population in the United States in 2013, July 8, 2015. https://www.migrationpolicy.org/article/limited-english-proficient-population-united-states-2013. (Accessed August 20, 2020.
  • [9].Islam NS, Khan S, Kwon S, Jang D, Ro M, Trinh-Shevrin C, Methodological issues in the collection, analysis, and reporting of granular data in Asian American populations: historical challenges and potential solutions, J Health Care Poor Underserved 21(4) (2010) 1354–81. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [10].Holland AT, Palaniappan LP, Problems with the collection and interpretation of Asian-American health data: omission, aggregation, and extrapolation, Ann Epidemiol 22(6) (2012) 397–405. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [11].ACOG Committee Opinion No. 587: Effective patient-physician communication, Obstet Gynecol 123(2 Pt 1) (2014) 389–93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [12].Davis K, Schoenbaum SC, Audet AM, A 2020 vision of patient-centered primary care, J Gen Intern Med 20(10) (2005) 953–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [13].Tucker CM, Marsiske M, Rice KG, Nielson JJ, Herman K, Patient-centered culturally sensitive health care: model testing and refinement, Health Psychol 30(3) (2011) 342–50. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [14].Tucker CM, Mirsu-Paun A, Van den Berg JJ, Ferdinand L, Jones JD, Curry RW, Rooks LG, Walter TJ, Beato C, Assessments for measuring patient-centered cultural sensitivity in community-based primary care clinics, J Natl Med Assoc 99(6) (2007) 609–19. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [15].Street RL Jr., Makoul G, Arora NK, Epstein RM, How does communication heal? Pathways linking clinician-patient communication to health outcomes, Patient Educ Couns 74(3) (2009) 295–301. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [16].King A, Hoppe RB, “Best practice” for patient-centered communication: a narrative review, J Grad Med Educ 5(3) (2013) 385–93. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [17].Street RL Jr., How clinician-patient communication contributes to health improvement: modeling pathways from talk to outcome, Patient Educ Couns 92(3) (2013) 286–91. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [18].Jenerette CM, Mayer DK, Patient-Provider Communication: the Rise of Patient Engagement, Semin Oncol Nurs 32(2) (2016) 134–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [19].Kreps GL, Promoting patient comprehension of relevant health information, Isr J Health Policy Res 7(1) (2018) 56. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [20].Green AR, Ngo-Metzger Q, Legedza AT, Massagli MP, Phillips RS, Iezzoni LI, Interpreter services, language concordance, and health care quality. Experiences of Asian Americans with limited English proficiency, J Gen Intern Med 20(11) (2005) 1050–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [21].Schenker Y, Karter AJ, Schillinger D, Warton EM, Adler NE, Moffet HH, Ahmed AT, Fernandez A, The impact of limited English proficiency and physician language concordance on reports of clinical interactions among patients with diabetes: the DISTANCE study, Patient Educ Couns 81(2) (2010) 222–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [22].Wilson E, Chen AH, Grumbach K, Wang F, Fernandez A, Effects of limited English proficiency and physician language on health care comprehension, J Gen Intern Med 20(9) (2005) 800–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [23].Ngo-Metzger Q, Sorkin DH, Phillips RS, Greenfield S, Massagli MP, Clarridge B, Kaplan SH, Providing high-quality care for limited English proficient patients: the importance of language concordance and interpreter use, J Gen Intern Med 22 Suppl 2 (2007) 324–30. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [24].Hsueh L, Hirsh AT, Maupome G, Stewart JC, Patient-Provider Language Concordance and Health Outcomes: A Systematic Review, Evidence Map, and Research Agenda, Med Care Res Rev (2019) 1077558719860708. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • [25].Gonzalez HM, Vega WA, Tarraf W, Health care quality perceptions among foreign-born Latinos and the importance of speaking the same language, Journal of the American Board of Family Medicine : JABFM 23(6) (2010) 745–52. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [26].Tsoh JY, Sentell T, Gildengorin G, Le GM, Chan E, Fung LC, Pasick RJ, Stewart S, Wong C, Woo K, Burke A, Wang J, McPhee SJ, Nguyen TT, Healthcare Communication Barriers and Self-Rated Health in Older Chinese American Immigrants, J Community Health 41(4) (2016) 741–52. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [27].Gardam M, Verma G, Campbell A, Wang J, Khan K, Impact of the patient-provider relationship on the survival of foreign born outpatients with tuberculosis, J Immigr Minor Health 11(6) (2009) 437–45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [28].Clough J, Lee S, Chae DH, Barriers to health care among Asian immigrants in the United States: a traditional review, J Health Care Poor Underserved 24(1) (2013) 384–403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [29].Paternotte E, van Dulmen S, van der Lee N, Scherpbier AJ, Scheele F, Factors influencing intercultural doctor-patient communication: a realist review, Patient Educ Couns 98(4) (2015) 420–45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [30].Zestcott CA, Blair IV, Stone J, Examining the Presence, Consequences, and Reduction of Implicit Bias in Health Care: A Narrative Review, Group Process Intergroup Relat 19(4) (2016) 528–542. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [31].Penner LA, Dovidio JF, Gonzalez R, Albrecht TL, Chapman R, Foster T, Harper FW, Hagiwara N, Hamel LM, Shields AF, Gadgeel S, Simon MS, Griggs JJ, Eggly S, The Effects of Oncologist Implicit Racial Bias in Racially Discordant Oncology Interactions, J Clin Oncol 34(24) (2016) 2874–80. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [32].Yoo HC, Gee GC, Takeuchi D, Discrimination and health among Asian American immigrants: disentangling racial from language discrimination, Soc Sci Med 68(4) (2009) 726–32. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [33].Abramson CM, Hashemi M, Sanchez-Jankowski M, Perceived discrimination in U.S. healthcare: Charting the effects of key social characteristics within and across racial groups, Prev Med Rep 2 (2015) 615–21. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [34].Gonzalez CM, Deno ML, Kintzer E, Marantz PR, Lypson ML, McKee MD, Patient perspectives on racial and ethnic implicit bias in clinical encounters: Implications for curriculum development, Patient Educ Couns 101(9) (2018) 1669–1675. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [35].Hagiwara N, Slatcher RB, Eggly S, Penner LA, Physician Racial Bias and Word Use during Racially Discordant Medical Interactions, Health Commun 32(4) (2017) 401–408. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [36].Institute of Medicine (US) Committee on Understanding and Eliminating Racial and Ethnic Disparities in Health Care, in: Smedley BD, Stith AY, Nelson AR (Eds.), Unequal Treatment: Confronting Racial and Ethnic Disparities in Health Care, Washington (DC), 2003. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [37].Ibaraki AY, Hall GCN, Sabin JA, Asian American Cancer Disparities: The Potential Effects of Model Minority Health Stereotypes, Asian Am J Psychol 5(1) (2014) 75–81. [Google Scholar]
  • [38].Nielsen JD, Wall W, Tucker CM, Testing of a Model with Latino Patients That Explains the Links Among Patient-Perceived Provider Cultural Sensitivity, Language Preference, and Patient Treatment Adherence, J Racial Ethn Health Disparities 3(1) (2016) 63–73. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [39].The Commonwealth Fund, 2001 Health Care Quality Survey, 2002. https://www.commonwealthfund.org/sites/default/files/documents/___media_files_surveys_2001_2001_health_care_quality_survey_qualitysurvey_2001_questionnaire_pdf.pdf. (Accessed April 3, 2020.
  • [40].Johnson RL, Saha S, Arbelaez JJ, Beach MC, Cooper LA, Racial and ethnic differences in patient perceptions of bias and cultural competence in health care, J Gen Intern Med 19(2) (2004) 101–10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [41].Asian Pacific Institute on Gender-Based Violence, Census Data & API Identities, 2017. https://www.api-gbv.org/resources/census-data-api-identities/. (Accessed June 30, 2020.
  • [42].Vickstrom ER, Shin HB, Collazo SG, Bauman KJ, How Well—Still Good? Assessing the Validity of the American Community Survey English-Ability Question, SEHSD (2015).
  • [43].Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, BRFSS Questionnaires, 2019. https://www.cdc.gov/brfss/questionnaires/index.htm. (Accessed April 16, 2020.
  • [44].Lee S, Chen L, Ma GX, Fang CY, What Is Lacking in Patient-Physician Communication: Perspectives from Asian American Breast Cancer Patients and Oncologists, J Behav Health 1(2) (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [45].Charles B Wang Community Health Center, Charles B. Wang Community Health Center, 2011. http://www.cbwchc.org/history. (Accessed August 1 2019).
  • [46].Chinese American Medical Society, Our mission https://chineseamericanmedicalsociety.cloverpad.org/page-1854825. (Accessed August 1 2019).
  • [47].Coalition of Asian-American Independent Practice Associations, About us https://www.caipa.net/caipa/about-us-2/. (Accessed August 1 2019).
  • [48].Alfred M, Ubogaya K, Chen X, Wint D, Worral PS, Effectiveness of culturally focused interventions in increasing the satisfaction of hospitalized Asian patients: a systematic review, JBI Database System Rev Implement Rep 14(8) (2016) 219–56. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [49].Arpey NC, Gaglioti AH, Rosenbaum ME, How Socioeconomic Status Affects Patient Perceptions of Health Care: A Qualitative Study, J Prim Care Communit 8(3) (2017) 169–175. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [50].Rikard RV, Thompson MS, McKinney J, Beauchamp A, Examining health literacy disparities in the United States: a third look at the National Assessment of Adult Literacy (NAAL), Bmc Public Health 16 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [51].Zimmerman E, Woolf SH, Understanding the Relationship between Education and Health. Discussion Paper, Institute of Medicine, Washington, D.C., 2014. https://nam.edu/wp-content/uploads/2015/06/BPH-UnderstandingTheRelationship1.pdf. [Google Scholar]
  • [52].August KJ, Nguyen H, Ngo-Metzger Q, Sorkin DH, Language concordance and patient-physician communication regarding mental health needs, J Am Geriatr Soc 59(12) (2011) 2356–62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [53].Butler M, McCreedy E, Schwer N, Burgess D, Call K, Przedworski J, Rosser S, Larson S, Allen M, Fu S, Kane RL, Improving cultural competence to reduce health disparities, Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality, Rockville, MD, 2016. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [54].Institute of Medicine, Unequal treatment: Confronting racial and ethnic disparities in health care, 2003. [PubMed]
  • [55].Govere L, Govere EM, How Effective is Cultural Competence Training of Healthcare Providers on Improving Patient Satisfaction of Minority Groups? A Systematic Review of Literature, Worldviews Evid Based Nurs 13(6) (2016) 402–410. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [56].Horvat L, Horey D, Romios P, Kis-Rigo J, Cultural competence education for health professionals, Cochrane Database Syst Rev (5) (2014) CD009405. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES