Skip to main content
ACS Pharmacology & Translational Science logoLink to ACS Pharmacology & Translational Science
. 2021 Mar 5;4(2):424–435. doi: 10.1021/acsptsci.1c00014

Predicting Reactions to Psychedelic Drugs: A Systematic Review of States and Traits Related to Acute Drug Effects

Jacob S Aday †,*, Alan K Davis ‡,§, Cayla M Mitzkovitz , Emily K Bloesch , Christopher C Davoli
PMCID: PMC8033773  PMID: 33860172

Abstract

graphic file with name pt1c00014_0002.jpg

Psychedelic drugs are increasingly being incorporated into therapeutic contexts for the purposes of promoting mental health. However, they can also induce adverse reactions in some individuals, and it is difficult to predict before treatment who is likely to experience positive or adverse acute effects. Although consideration of setting and dosage as well as excluding individuals with psychotic predispositions has thus far led to a high degree of safety, it is imperative that researchers develop a more nuanced understanding of how to predict individual reactions. To this end, the current systematic review coalesced the results of 14 studies that included baseline states or traits predictive of the acute effects of psychedelics. Individuals high in the traits of absorption, openness, and acceptance as well as a state of surrender were more likely to have positive and mystical-type experiences, whereas those low in openness and surrender or in preoccupied, apprehensive, or confused psychological states were more likely to experience acute adverse reactions. Participant sex was not a robust predictor of drug effects, but 5-HT2AR binding potential, executive network node diversity, and rACC volume may be potential baseline biomarkers related to acute reactions. Finally, increased age and experience with psychedelics were individual differences related to generally less intense effects, indicating that users may become slightly less sensitive to the effects of the drugs after repeated usage. Although future well-powered, placebo-controlled trials directly comparing the relative importance of these predictors is needed, this review synthesizes the field’s current understanding of how to predict acute reactions to psychedelic drugs.

Keywords: psychedelic, predictor, baseline, individual differences, screening


“The mental effects of this material [LSD] are tremendously variable. They are completely unpredictable. You cannot tell for any given individual if he’s going to have a good trip or a bad trip.” – US Navy Training Film, 19671

Anderson Cooper: “Can you tell who is going to have a bad experience; who is going to have a transcendent experience?”

Dr. Roland Griffiths: “Our ability to predict that is almost none at all.” – 60 Minutes Interview, October 13, 20192

Psilocybin, lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD), ayahuasca/N,N-dimethyltryptamine (DMT), and mescaline are classic psychedelic drugs that can induce myriad cognitive,3 emotional,4 and neurological5 effects. They were investigated for their uses in treating psychiatric conditions during the mid-20th century until legal changes in the United States (US), and subsequently worldwide, halted most ongoing research.6 Recently, public and scientific interest into the drugs has grown as a result of promising preliminary findings published on psychedelic-assisted psychotherapy’s effects on depression,7 substance misuse,8 and emotional distress associated with a variety of other chronic health conditions.911

A pressing problem for scientific research in this area, however, is that psychedelics can also induce experiences that are markedly psychologically challenging, particularly when used without adequate preparation and support,12 but little is known about participant states or traits that may predict such a reaction. Identification of states and traits predicting acute psychedelic outcomes may increase the likelihood of positive therapeutic effects, reduce the prevalence of adverse reactions, and improve confidence that one is selecting a safe and effective treatment. The need for a more nuanced understanding of states and traits related to acute psychedelic reactions has become even more pressing with the recent legalization of psilocybin therapy in Oregon, which will require individuals to pass a risk assessment for goodness of fit.13

Although there is currently little consensus on predicting how participants will react to psychedelics based on their individual characteristics, there is some agreement regarding the influence of various factors external to the individual. Most, but not all,14 researchers believe that the quality or phenomenology of the acute psychedelic experience is fundamental to the drugs’ therapeutic benefits.1517 Therefore, the setting or context in which a psychedelic is given may be critical to predicting if individuals will experience therapeutic effects.18,19 Cross-culturally, psychedelic users often place emphasis on environmental factors such as ceremony, song, and ritual in the healing process.20 In contemporary psychedelic trials, researchers design treatment rooms to be as comforting as possible (e.g., often set up like a living room).21 There are typically two guides/therapists for support, and music is played through headphones. The focus on setting is also reflected in the way many recreational psychedelic users prepare for personal sessions. When surveying individuals using 5-MeO-DMT in naturalistic settings, Lancelotta and Davis22 found that users employed environmental (e.g., preparing music), medical (e.g., obtaining a predrug medical checkup), and social (e.g., ensuring an available emergency contact) preparations in order to enhance their experience. Some indigenous cultures adopt a much different approach to the setting and healing process; for example, it may be the shaman, rather than the patient, who ingests the drug as the shaman is thought to be highly trained in interpreting and passing on spiritual and therapeutic revelations to the patient.23

Another important external factor that may predict the effects of a psychedelic is dosage. When comparing dosages of 0, 5, 10, 20, and 30 mg/70 kg of psilocybin, Griffiths and colleagues24 found dose-dependent long-term increases in life satisfaction, well-being, altruism, spirituality, positive attitudes about life and self, and positive mood changes. Importantly, ratings of fear or feeling trapped increased substantively with 30 mg/70 kg dose compared to the 20 mg/70 kg dose, suggesting somewhere in this range may be the ideal dose for optimizing treatment outcomes while minimizing adverse reactions. Dovetailing with these findings, Holze et al.25 identified a ceiling effect with LSD, where positive drug effects systematically increased with dosages of 25, 50, and 100 μg, but plateaued at the next dosage of 200 μg, which also increased anxiety. Research has demonstrated that mystical-type experiences (i.e., marked by a sense of transcendence, joy, and awe) are more likely to be induced with higher dosages of psychedelics,2628 but one study found that not all facets of mystical-type experiences are dose-dependent.29 Given that mystical-type experiences have been identified as a mechanism underlying many of the benefits linked to psychedelics,3032 it appears that dosage can be an important predictor of the drugs’ therapeutic effects. However, much is still unknown about individual state or trait factors that may interact with known predictors like dosage.

Exclusion criteria used to screen potential participants are another useful source of information, as these criteria are ostensibly in place because researchers and/or regulators believe these variables may influence the acute experience of psychedelic drugs or increase risk for postsession complications. Individuals are generally excluded on the basis of their psychiatric or medical history (see Johnson et al.21 for safety guidelines). In terms of psychiatric screening, those with a personal history of schizophrenia, psychotic disorders, or bipolar disorders are excluded as there is evidence that psychedelics can trigger psychotic episodes in a subset of users;33 those with a first- or second-degree relative with these conditions can be excluded on this basis as well.21 Individuals with uncontrolled hypertension are excluded because psychedelics can acutely increase pulse as well as systolic and diastolic blood pressure.34,35 Pregnant women or those with inadequate birth control practices are excluded as well.21 Tricyclic antidepressants and lithium,35 acute use of serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs),37 and haloperidol38 have been shown to potentiate the effects of hallucinogens, whereas chronic use of SSRIs and monoamine oxidase inhibitors have been shown to suppress their effects.36 Therefore, individuals using these medications are often excluded on the basis of safety and/or experimental control.

The Current Review

There is currently little consensus in the field on participant characteristics that can be used to predict what valence of acute effects an individual will have after taking a psychedelic drug. In modern studies, considering dosage and setting as well as excluding those with predispositions toward psychotic conditions has resulted in a high degree of safety and generally positive outcomes.39 However, if the medicalization of psychedelic drugs is to continue its expansion, it is imperative that researchers and clinicians develop more specific criteria to predict the valence of acute effects for any one given individual. Moreover, improved screening methods for psychedelic treatment should reduce adverse reactions and time spent on ineffective treatments. To address this important gap in the literature, we systematically reviewed the contemporary research on baseline variables that have been shown to predict acute psychedelic drug effects. Although Studerus et al.40 conducted an initial overview with some similar intentions, their analysis was not a systematic review, and only included studies that used psilocybin and data from their own laboratory.

Methods

Search Strategy

Our systematic review was conducted in accordance with the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses guidelines (PRISMA).41 Two of the authors (J.S.A. and C.M.M.) independently completed the systematic search in PubMed (Figure 1). Six psychedelic-related terms (e.g., “ayahuasca”, “LSD”, “lysergic acid diethylamide”, “mescaline”, “psilocybin”, and “n,n-dimethyltryptamine”) were systematically cross-referenced into PubMed’s search engine with seven predictor-related terms (e.g., “predict”, “correlate”, “associate”, “pre-drug”, “individual difference”, “baseline”, and “risk factor”) for a sum of 42 distinct searches (e.g., “ayahuasca predict”, “ayahuasca correlate”, etc.). An initial filter was applied in PubMed to select for language (English), species (human), and year (1994–present). We chose 1994 as our cutoff for contemporary research because Strassman et al.42 was the first study to administer classic psychedelic compounds to human participants in the US after several decades of prohibition.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Flow diagram depicting the systematic search process.

Selection Criteria

After filtering for language, species, and year, articles were examined for those which met the following criteria: (1) They were published in a peer-reviewed journal. (2) Classic psychedelic drugs were used. (3) Predrug measures were predictive of an acute effect. Book chapters, case reports, and reviews were excluded. Discrepancies regarding study inclusion were resolved by evaluating the study together with multiple members of the research team.

Results

Study Selection

The 42 PubMed searches generated a total of 1635 articles, which were assessed by the two searchers for those that fulfilled the selection criteria. This led to the identification of 10 articles that met the criteria for our review, and 4 others were identified by the authors from outside sources (e.g., referenced in other articles, published after our systematic search, etc.), for a total of 14 articles included in the systematic review (Table 1).

Table 1. Articles That Met Selection Criteria.

authors year population/diagnosis drug dosage (ROA) sample size control condition baseline predictor variable state (S) or trait (T) outcome predicted direction of relationship (±)
Bienemann et al.a,43 2020 unselected psilocybin varied 346 no gender (female) T “bad trip” +
Bouso et al.53 2013 users ayahuasca 100 mL (oral) 24 yes lifetime ayahuasca use T acute impairment in executive function
Carbonaro et al.a,12 2016 unselected psilocybin varied 1993 no age S difficulty of experience
history of psychedelic use T difficulty of experience
Carhart-Harris et al.51 2015 healthy LSD 40–80ug (IV) 10 yes conscientiousness T acute changes in suggestibility +
mental imagery ability T acute changes in mental imagery +
Haijen et al.54 2018 unselected varied varied 212–654 no history of psychedelic use T wellbeing  
openness T wellbeing
absorption T mystical experience +
    challenging experience +
    visual effects +
clear intention S mystical experience +
    visual effects +
set S challenging experience +
recreational intention S challenging experience
Lebedev et al.44 2015 healthy psilocybin 2 mg (IV) 15 yes executive network node diversity T ego dissolution
Lewis et al.45 2020 healthy psilocybin 0.16 or 0.215 mg/kg (oral) 55 no right rACC volume T intentsity of acute emotional experience +
Russ et al.a,46 2019 unselected psilocybin varied 183 no surrender S mystical expereience +
    ego dissolution +
absorption T mystical experience +
    ego dissolution +
spiritual motivations S mystical experience +
    ego dissolution +
    apprehension/mystical experience relationship +
deservingness S mystical experience
    ego dissolution
apprehension S mystical experience
    ego dissolution
    adverse experience +
gender (female) T apprehension/mystical experience relationship +
preoccupation S adverse experience +
confusion S adverse experience +
openness T adverse experience -
Russ et al.a,47 2019 unselected psilocybin varied 143 no surrender S mystical experience +
    dread
absorption T mystical experience +
    dread
barriers S mystical experience
age S mystical experience
    dread
preoccupation S dread +
confusion S dread +
Smigielski et al.48 2019 expert Buddhist meditators psilocybin 315 ug/kg (oral) 39 yes optimismtic attitude toward life T visionary restructuralization +
    oceanic boundlessness +
    mystical experience +
openness T oceanic boundlessness +
    mystical experience +
reappraisal of emotions T anxious ego dissolution
acceptance T mystical experience +
meditation depth T mystical experience +
    oceanic boundlessness +
Stauffer et al.49 2020 long-term AIDS survivors psilocybin 0.3–0.36 mg/kg 18 no attachment anxiety T mystical experience +
Attachment avoidance T challenging experience +
Stenbaek et al.50 2020 healthy psilocybin 0.2–0.3 mg/kg (oral) 16 no 5-HT2AR binding potential T peak plateau latency
subjective drug intensity comedown latency +
mystical experience
Studerus et al.40 2012 healthy psilocybin 115–315 mg/kg (oral) 261 yes age S acute impaired control and cognition
history of psychedelic use T acute disembodiment
    visionary restucturalization
    acute changed meaning of percepts
cannabis consumption T acute blissful state +
alcohol consumption T acute audio–visual synesthesia +
    complex imagery +
emotional excitability S spiritual experience +
    acute anxiety +
    acute general consciousness alteration +
    insightfulness +
    acute audio–visual synesthesia +
general psychological distress S acute oceanic boundlessness
    acute blissful state
    complex imagery
absorption T acute oceanic boundlessness +
    visionary restructuralization +
sociability T spiritual experience
    acute audio–visual synesthesia +
Terhune et al.52 2016 healthy LSD 40–80ug (IV) 10 yes absorption T stimulus-color consistency
a

Retrospective study design.

The review included 10 articles using psilocybin,12,40,4350 2 using LSD,51,52 1 using ayahuasca,53 and 1 examining psychedelic use in general.54 Sample sizes ranged from 10–1993 (M = 108.75, SD = 91.95), and 6 out of 14 articles included control conditions. The median year of publication was 2018, suggesting that this is an emerging area of research. Baseline predictors of psychedelic drug effects could be broadly broken down into two categories: states and traits (Table 2; see also Supplementary Table 1 to see data organized by outcomes rather than by baseline predictors).

Table 2. Summary of Relevant States and Traits.

  baseline predictor outcome(s) and direction of relationship (±) refs
states age mystical experience (−), dread (−), acute impaired control and cognition (−), adverse experience (−) (12, 40, 47)
apprehension mystical experience (−), ego dissolution (−), adverse experience (+) (46, 47)
barriers mystical experience (−) (47)
clear intention mystical experience (+), visual effects (+) (54)
confusion dread (+) (46, 47)
deservingness mystical experience (−), ego dissolution (−) (46)
emotional excitability spiritual experience (+), acute anxiety (+), general consciousness alteration (+), insightfulness (+) (40)
general psychological distress oceanic boundlessness (−), blissful state (−), complex imagery (−) (40)
preoccupation dread (+) (47)
recreational intention challenging experience (−) (54)
set challenging experience (−) (54)
spiritual motivations mystical experience (+), ego dissolution (+), apprehension/mystical experience relationship (+) (46)
surrender mystical experience (+), ego dissolution (+), spiritual change (+), dread (−) (46, 47)
traits 5-HT2AR binding peak plateau latency (−), subjective drug intensity comedown latency (+), mystical experience (−) (50)
absorption mystical experience (+), challenging experience (+), visual effects (+), ego dissolution (+), dread (−), spiritual change (+), acute oceanic boundlessness (+), acute visionary restructuralization (+), stimulus-color consistency (−) (40, 46, 47, 52, 54)
acceptance mystical experience (+) (48)
alcohol consumption audio–visual synethesia (+), visual effects (+) (40)
attachment anxiety mystical experience (+) (49)
attachment avoidance challenging experience (+) (49)
cannabis consumption blissful state (+) (40)
conscientiousness suggestibility (+) (51)
executive network node diversity ego dissolution (+) (44)
gender (female) “bad trip” (+), apprehension/mystical experience relationship (+) (43, 46)
history of psychedelic use acute impairment in executive functioning (−), adverse experience (−), wellbeing (−), acute disembodiment (−), visionary restructuralization (−) (12, 40, 53, 54)
meditation depth mystical experience (+), oceanic boundlessness (+) (48)
mental imagery ability acute changes in mental imagery (+) (51)
openness adverse experience (−), oceanic boundlessness (+), mystical experience (+) (54, 46, 48)
optimistic attitude toward life visionary restructuralization (+), oceanic boundlessness (+), mystical experience (+) (48)
reappraisal of emotions anxious ego dissoution (−) (48)
right rACC volume intensity of acute emotional experience (+) (45)
sociability spiritual experience (−), acute audio–visual synesthesia (+) (40)

Drugs

Psilocybin

We will begin by reviewing the studies that identified biological measures that were predictive of the effects of psilocybin. Although they will need further verification, these variables could potentially be used as biomarkers for screening participants in future research or clinical settings. One study found that predrug individual differences in 5-HT2AR binding potential predicted how individuals responded to psilocybin.50 Lower baseline 5-HT2AR binding was associated with longer peak effects, a more rapid decrease in subjective drug intensity (SDI), and higher ratings of mystical-type experience. The authors suggested that greater predrug serotonin receptor availability could lead to more effective receptor stimulation and more intense effects. Longer peak effects could allow mystical-type experiences to intensify, and a rapid return to baseline consciousness may make the effects more salient. Therefore, predrug 5-HT2AR binding may be an important biomarker for predicting the effects of psychedelics. The small sample size (N = 16) and lack of control group in this study are important limitations, however.

In a placebo-controlled trial, Lebedev et al.44 found that individuals with a lower diversity of executive network nodes at baseline (i.e., those with more efficient network segregation) were more likely to experience ego dissolution under the influence of psilocybin. The authors interpreted these results to be in line with the notion that efficient network segregation is related to firm ego boundaries and that individuals with typically firm boundaries are more sensitive to the ego-dissolving effects of psilocybin. However, the study was limited by a small and homogeneous sample (N = 15; female = 2). Nonetheless, given that ego dissolution has been identified as a mechanism underlying some of the positive long-term benefits of psychedelics,5557 executive network node diversity may be an important predictor to evaluate further.

Another placebo-controlled study found that structural neural measures could be used to predict aspects of the psychedelic experience.45 Here, baseline right hemisphere rostral anterior cingulate (rACC) thickness positively predicted feelings of spiritual experience and insightfulness while healthy volunteers were under the influence of psilocybin. Given the rACC’s high concentration of 5-HT2A receptors and role in emotional processing,58 the authors hypothesized that activity in the cingulate cortex may underlie the profound emotional experiences induced by psychedelics. Although the study was limited by using varied dosages within the treatment condition (e.g., 0.16 mg/kg or 0.215 mg/kg), it was strengthened by the use of a relatively large and heterogeneous sample (N = 55; female = 22).

Bienemann et al.43 performed a quantitative text analysis of negative psilocybin reports posted to the website Erowid. They found that “bad trips” were more frequently reported by female users, suggesting that sex may be a predrug variable related to how individuals respond in a recreational setting. However, the effect size was small (V = 0.20), and this study was hampered by a number of methodological limitations, including self-selection and recall biases, varied dosages, varied settings, unknown drug purities, and a large amount of missing data.

Contradicting these results, Russ et al.46 found few sex differences other than that for females apprehension was significantly more negatively correlated with mystical-type experiences compared to that in males. This study also identified that predrug states of preoccupation and confusion were positively related to adverse reactions, whereas the trait of openness was negatively correlated with adverse outcomes. Ego dissolution and mystical-type experiences were positively related to baseline measures of surrender (i.e., willful release of one’s goals, constructs, habits, and preferences), absorption (i.e., individual’s openness to diverse cognitive, perceptual, and imaginative experiences as well as value of aesthetics and nature), and spiritual motivations, as well as negatively correlated with measures of deservingness and apprehension. Although the study had a large and heterogeneous sample (N = 183; female = 85), it was limited by a retrospective design and varied dosages.

In another study, Russ and colleagues47 once again identified that baseline measures of surrender (r = 0.72) and absorption (r = 0.61) moderately to strongly predicted the extent to which one had a mystical-type experience. Here, surrender was also positively related to spiritual change and long-term positive change, as well as negatively associated with acute experiences of dread and long-term negative change. Measures of absorption were also positively correlated with spiritual change and long-term positive change and negatively related to dread. Experiences of dread were positively related to baseline measures of preoccupation (r = 0.64) and confusion (r = 0.58) and were negatively correlated with age (r = −0.23). Finally, baseline measures of age and barriers (i.e., tendency to reject phenomena that have no rational explanation) were negatively related to acute mystical-type experiences. This study had similar strengths and weaknesses as Russ et al.;44 the sample was large and sex-diverse (N = 143; female = 62) but used a retrospective design and therefore could not control dosage, setting, or drug purity.

When evaluating the use of psilocybin in combination with a mindfulness retreat in a sample of expert Buddhist meditators, Smigielski et al.48 identified several traits that were predictive of drug effects. Mystical-type experiences were positively related to baseline measures of optimistic attitudes toward life, openness, acceptance, and meditation depth. Optimistic attitudes toward life were also predictive of acute experiences of visionary re-structuralization (i.e., changes in elementary and complex imagery) and oceanic boundlessness (i.e., mystical-type experiences). Predrug measures of openness and meditation depth were also positively related to oceanic boundlessness. Although some participants had previous experience with psychedelics and others did not, in general, this study was methodologically strong as it was placebo-controlled and double-blind, had a respectable sample size (N = 38; female = 15), and included a long-term follow-up (e.g., 4 months).

Using a pooled analysis of 23 studies from the same laboratory involving 409 psilocybin sessions with 261 healthy volunteers, Studerus et al.40 identified a number of states and traits that were predictive of acute effects. First, absorption was positively related to most facets of oceanic boundlessness (e.g., experience of unity, spiritual experience, insightfulness, and disembodiment) and all facets of visionary re-structuralization (e.g., complex imagery, elementary imagery, audio–visual synesthesia, and changed meaning of percepts). Individuals high in the trait of sociability were less likely to have a spiritual experience and more likely to experience audio–visual synesthesia. Histories of cannabis and alcohol consumption were related to increases in blissful states and audio–visual synesthesia, respectively. In terms of state predictors, they found that age was negatively related to acute impairments in control and cognition. Emotional excitability predicted acute increases in spiritual experience, insightfulness, audio–visual synesthesia, general consciousness alteration, and anxiety. Last, baseline state-level measures of general psychological distress were negatively correlated with oceanic boundlessness, blissful states, and complex imagery. Although dosages varied across some of the experiments, this analysis was methodologically rigorous given its large sample size (N = 261; female = 100), and all studies were placebo-controlled.

Stauffer and colleagues49 administered 0.3–0.36 mg/kg of psilocybin to long-term AIDS survivors experiencing demoralization. They found that those high in the trait of attachment anxiety at baseline were more likely to have mystical-type experiences (r = 0.53), and those high in attachment avoidance were more likely to have challenging experiences (r = 0.62). The authors hypothesized that hyperactivating strategies (i.e., cognitive and behavioral efforts to reduce distance from intimate others) commonly used by those with high attachment anxiety may prime individuals for interconnectedness, a defining feature of mystical-type experiences. The correlation between attachment avoidance and challenging experiences was thought to reflect the aversion to emotional vulnerability and difficulty with novel experiences characteristic of those high in attachment avoidance. However, this study was limited because of its open-label design, small sample size (N = 18), and homogeneous sample (i.e., almost exclusively older men with history of psychedelic use).

Finally, Carbonaro et al.12 surveyed psilocybin users about their most challenging experience with the drug. Weak, but statistically significant, relationships were found between age (r = −0.06) and history of hallucinogen use (r = −0.05) with difficulty of the experience. Although the study had a large sample size (N = 1993), its retrospective design, homogeneous sample (78% male, 89% White, and 87% had at least some college education), and variable dosages as well as settings are important limitations to keep in mind.

LSD

Two articles were included that identified baseline measures predicting the acute effects of LSD, but it seems both studies relied on the same sample of participants. The researchers first found that baseline measures of conscientiousness were strongly correlated with acute changes in suggestibility (r = 0.89).51 Given that suggestibility has been argued to be one factor underlying the drugs’ transdiagnostic benefits,39 baseline measures of conscientiousness could be an important predictor to include when screening participants. The authors also found that predrug mental imagery ability was positively correlated with acute increases in vividness of mental imagery (r = 0.72).

In another analysis of the same sample of participants as Carhart-Harris et al.,51 researchers found that absorption was negatively related to stimulus-color consistency during synesthesia-like experiences with LSD (r = −0.67).52 The authors concluded that synesthesia-like experiences are not more consistent under LSD than placebo and that proneness to states of absorption may moderate this relationship. Although these two studies were placebo-controlled, they were limited by a small and nondiverse sample (N = 10; female = 1), variable dosages (e.g., 40–80 μg), and participants had considerable past experience with the drug (M number of LSD experiences = 65).

Ayahuasca

Only one study identified a predictor of the effects of ayahuasca in our search. Bouso et al.53 found that while under the influence of ayahuasca, experienced users (i.e., >60 uses) had less impairment than occasional users (i.e., 8–60 uses) on a test of executive functioning. Additionally, across both groups, impaired performance in executive functioning was inversely correlated with lifetime ayahuasca use (r = −0.62). This indicates that previous experience with psychedelics may confer compensatory or neuromodulatory effects that buffer against acute cognitive impairments. Although the sample size was modest (N = 24; female = 12), the study was sex-balanced and included a control group consisting of drug-naïve participants to rule out learning effects.

Psychedelics Not Otherwise Specified

Given that classic psychedelics generally share more similarities than differences in their effects,59 some studies have examined predictors related to psychedelic use in general. In a prospective study, researchers surveyed users 1 week prior, 1 day before, 1 day after, 2 weeks after, and 4 weeks following the day they planned to use a psychedelic.54 The trait of absorption was once again an important predictor of effects as it was positively related to mystical-type experiences, visual effects, as well as challenging experiences. Having a clear intention and positive “set” (i.e., one’s state of mind immediately prior to drug intake) were conducive to mystical-type experiences and visual effects and decreased the likelihood of a challenging experience. A recreational intention was also negatively related to challenging experiences. Although the study was strengthened by a large sample size (N = 212–654) and multiple follow-ups, it was limited by attrition as well as varied substances, dosages, and settings.

Discussion

Individual responses to psychedelic drugs are notoriously difficult to predict; however, this review indicates that there may be baseline traits and states that are important to consider. Given that the median year of publication for the included articles was 2018, it seems researchers are just beginning to understand how to predict psychedelic drug effects. Next, we will coalesce this burgeoning research.

Traits

In terms of biological predictors, 5-HT2AR binding potential,50 executive network node diversity,44 and rACC volume54 were shown to be related to the acute cognitive and emotional effects of psychedelic drugs. Given the high cost of collecting neuroimaging measures, it may be some time before these indices can be practically implemented into clinical screening procedures. Nevertheless, there is clear utility in researchers continuing to delineate biomarkers predictive of drug effects, as they can reduce participant and experimenter biases.60 One biological measure that was collected in every study was participant sex; however, only a single study found that adverse reactions with psilocybin were more likely to be reported by female users.43 Other studies that included measures of adverse reactions did not identify sex differences,12,48,54 and there are alternative explanations to the findings of Bienemann et al.43. For example, given their self-report design, the results could be explained by the tendency for women to be more likely to disclose negative emotional experiences than men.61 Moreover, research has demonstrated that there are no sex differences in 5-HT2AR distribution62 nor in the pharmacokinetic profiles of psychedelics.6365 In short, it seems that psychedelic drugs induce generally similar effects in males and females.

Absorption was the personality trait most consistently linked to psychedelic drug reactions, as it was related to higher ratings of mystical-type experience54 and ego dissolution.46 Individuals high in absorption were also particularly susceptible to the perceptual effects of the drugs, as absorption predicted heightened visual effects54 and visionary restructuralization40 as well as decreased stimulus–color consistency during synesthetic-like experiences.52 These findings are in line with previous research noting that absorption is a predisposing trait for hallucinatory experiences across a variety of altered states.66 Critically, across several studies and research groups, absorption was a replicable predictor of mystical-type experiences.40,46,47,54 Given the role of mystical-type experiences in facilitating therapeutic changes with psychedelics,39 it appears the trait of absorption may be an important variable to consider when evaluating if psychedelics are likely to be an effective treatment for an individual. Moreover, absorption has been shown to be related to 5-HT2AR binding potential,67 which is consistent with recent research indicating that 5-HT2AR binding potential may be a biomarker for predicting psychedelic drug effects.50

Openness to experience, a personality trait highly correlated with absorption,68 was also related to positive effects, such as mystical-type experiences48 and fewer adverse reactions.46 Additionally, acceptance was related to mystical-type experiences.48 Thus, absorption, openness, and acceptance may represent a set of related personality traits that prime individuals to fully immersive themselves in, and be more accepting of, a nonordinary state.

In addition to personality traits, one’s personal history of psychedelic use was shown to predict how individuals responded on several measures. In general, drug effects were less intense in those with greater psychedelic usage in the past, suggesting that users may habituate or become desensitized to the effects of psychedelics over time. Indeed, history of psychedelic use was negatively related to disembodiment,40 visionary restructuralization,40 and acute impairment in executive functioning.53 These changes did not seem to be valence-specific, as greater previous use was related to reduced difficulty of the experience12 as well as reduced improvements in well-being post-treatment.54 One interpretation of these results is that users may habituate to the effects of psychedelics after repeated usage, and it may be important to not overuse psychedelic substances in order to continue deriving the same benefits from them. Many users report psychedelic states as being drastically different than typical waking consciousness; this novelty may be one factor contributing to “pivotal mental states” that underlie rapid psychological transformation with the drugs.69 Although it is not unusual in some cultures to continue taking psychedelics on a regular basis (e.g., ayahuasca as much as 4 times per week),70 it is unclear if these users continue to experience the same intensity of acute effects or long-term benefits from any given session.

States

An individual’s expectations and mood immediately prior to drug intake, commonly referred to as one’s “set”, have been long argued to be critical variables in determining acute psychedelic effects.7173 Those who approach the session with an open and positive mindset are thought to be more likely to derive benefits, whereas those in an unstable state or negative mindset are more likely to experience adverse effects.74,75 This notion was validated by our findings, as having a positive set decreased the likelihood of having a challenging experience.54 Similarly, states of apprehension, confusion, and distress predicted more adverse reactions and less likelihood for a mystical-type experience.40,46,47 A state of surrender, in particular, emerged from our synthesis of the literature as an important predictor of drug effects. Those high in surrender were more likely to experience ego dissolution as well as mystical-type experiences and were less likely to experience acute dread.46,47 A state of surrender may contribute to the positive effects of psychedelics in a similar way as the traits of absorption, openness, and acceptance, that is, by making individuals more susceptible to immersing themselves in the experience and “letting go” of previous self-narratives or attitudes.76

In addition to expectations and mood, the motivations and intentions for a psychedelic experience were predictive of how individuals responded. Spiritual motivations were related to increased likelihood of mystical-type experiences and ego dissolution.46 Clear intentions were also predictive of mystical-type experiences, and those with a recreational intention were less likely to report challenging experiences.54 Therefore, in addition to traits, expectations, and mood, researchers and clinicians should consider patients’ intentions when predicting how an individual will respond to a psychedelic drug.

The age at which participants consumed a psychedelic substance was related to several outcomes. As age increased, participants tended to experience slightly less intense effects, both positive and negative. Age was negatively related to difficulty of the experience,12 experiences of dread,47 mystical-type experiences,47 and acute impaired control and cognition.40 Thus, older individuals may be somewhat less sensitive to the effects of psychedelics drugs and/or potentially require higher doses. This is in line with other research indicating that there may be concomitant changes with age that can interact with drug potencies (e.g., increased body weight, higher number of other medications, and decreases in 5-HT2AR binding potential).72,77

Limitations

There are a number of important limitations to keep in mind when interpreting findings from this review. First, our methodological critique of the literature revealed that many studies were compromised by open-label designs, small sample sizes, and/or homogeneous samples. Additionally, retrospective designs using online crowd-sourcing methods may have been compromised by self-selection biases. However, these are not problems inherent to psychedelic science per se as they could be rectified with increased funding for larger, placebo-controlled trials with diverse samples. Another limitation is that many of the relationships identified in this review have yet to be replicated. However, given that many of the studies identifying baseline predictors of drug effects were recently published, it may be that researchers have not been fully aware of the findings or have not yet had sufficient time to pursue replication work; our review can address the first issue by bringing these variables to researchers’ attention and inspiring further study in this area. Last, it is worth noting that researchers often fail to report when there is an absence of a relationship between variables.79 This “file drawer” problem may have produced unpublished results that run contradictory to our findings, resulting in potential Type I errors. Nonetheless, this review can provide guidance for future researchers aiming to identify potential replicable predictors.

Future Directions

In addition to addressing the aforementioned limitations, there are myriad future directions researchers can explore to improve our understanding of how to predict acute effects of psychedelic drugs. First, now that a set of baseline variables related to drug effects has been identified, future researchers should compare the relative importance of these predictors by including them within the same studies and statistical models. Doing so can ultimately inform an optimal and concise set of screening procedures for individuals planning to take a psychedelic. We particularly endorse future researchers evaluating the trait of absorption and state of surrender (which are moderately correlated with one another),47 as they were consistently related to positive experiences with psychedelics and the strength of these relationships were often moderate-to-strong. Across cultures and spiritual orientations, those high in absorption are more likely to have spiritual experiences;72 our results suggest that this effect generalizes to mystical-type experiences under psychedelics, which are often described as spiritually significant.

If states and traits that facilitate the benefits of psychedelics become firmly established, then a natural question will be how to alter those baseline variables to enhance treatment effects. For example, if absorption is an important individual difference related to positive responses to psychedelics, then increasing absorption pretreatment may make the therapy more effective. Speculatively, given that microdosing psychedelics (i.e., regularly taking a non-hallucinogenic dose) has been shown to increase absorption,81 it is conceivable that incorporating microdosing before a full treatment dose could be beneficial. However, state-level factors would ostensibly be easier to alter than traits, which are more stable. That is, participants’ mood, expectations, and intentions can be primed during predrug preparatory counseling to optimize treatment outcomes.

Conclusion

For decades, identification of baseline variables that predict the effects of psychedelic drugs has proven illusory. Although the findings presented here are not definitive, they can serve as an important synthesis of the evidence scientists have compiled to date. This information will become increasingly valuable as psychedelic substances continue to be decriminalized and incorporated into medical contexts.72 Those high in the traits of absorption, openness, and acceptance as well as a state of surrender may represent ideal candidates for psychedelic therapy. In contrast, individuals low in those traits or that are in preoccupied, apprehensive, or confused states are more likely to experience adverse reactions. Although participant sex was not a robust predictor, 5-HT2AR binding potential, executive network node diversity, and rACC volume were baseline biological measures related to acute reactions. Increased age and experience with psychedelics were individual differences related to generally less intense effects with the drugs. Future studies should address current experimental limitations by using well-powered, placebo-controlled designs with more diverse samples. Nonetheless, this review serves as an important synthesis of how to predict reactions to psychedelic drugs. Application of this knowledge could have broad implications for psychedelic science, including increasing confidence in treatment selection, saving resources spent on ineffective treatments, attenuating the prevalence of adverse reactions, and improving therapeutic efficacy.

Supporting Information Available

The Supporting Information is available free of charge at https://pubs.acs.org/doi/10.1021/acsptsci.1c00014.

  • Table with the results of our review organized by outcome measure rather than by predictors (XLSX)

Effort for Dr. Davis was provided by Tim Ferriss, Matt Mullenweg, Craig Nerenberg, Blake Mycoskie, and the Steven and Alexandra Cohen Foundation. The funders had no role in study design, data analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

The authors declare the following competing financial interest(s): Dr. Davis is a board member of Source Research Foundation. This organization was not involved in the design/execution of this study or the interpretation or communication of findings.

Supplementary Material

pt1c00014_si_001.xlsx (10.4KB, xlsx)

References

  1. U.S. Navy . (1967) LSD [TV broadcast], U.S. Navy Training Film. [Google Scholar]
  2. CBS . (2019, October 13) Researchers experimenting with psychedelics to treat addiction. Depression and anxiety, 60 Minutes [TV series episode]. [Google Scholar]
  3. Davis A. K.; Barrett F. S.; Griffiths R. R. (2020) Psychological flexibility mediates the relations between acute psychedelic effects and subjective decreases in depression and anxiety. J. Contex Behav Sci. 15, 39–45. 10.1016/j.jcbs.2019.11.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Roseman L.; Haijen E.; Idialu-Ikato K.; Kaelen M.; Watts R.; Carhart-Harris R. L. (2019) Emotional breakthrough and psychedelics: Validation of the emotional breakthrough inventory. J. Psychopharmacol. 33, 1076–1087. 10.1177/0269881119855974. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Carhart-Harris R. L.; Erritzoe D.; Williams T.; Stone J. M.; Reed L. J.; Colasanti A.; Tyacke R. J.; Leech R.; Malizia A. L.; Murphy K.; et al. (2012) Neural correlates of the psychedelic state as determined by fMRI studies with psilocybin. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 109, 2138–2143. 10.1073/pnas.1119598109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Aday J. S.; Davoli C. C.; Bloesch E. K. (2019) 2018: A watershed year for psychedelic science. Drug Sci., Policy Law 10.1177/2050324519872284. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  7. Davis A. K.; Barrett F. S.; May D. G.; Sepeda N.; Cosimano M. P.; Finan P.; Johnson M. W.; Griffiths R. R. (2020) Effects of psilocybin-assisted therapy for major depressive disorder: A randomized clinical trial. JAMA Psychiatry 3285. 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2020.3285. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Johnson M. W.; Garcia-Romeu A.; Cosimano M. P.; Griffiths R. R. (2014) Pilot study of the 5-HT2AR agonist psilocybin in the treatment of tobacco addiction. J. Psychopharmacol. 28, 983–992. 10.1177/0269881114548296. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Anderson B. T; Danforth A.; Daroff P. R.; Stauffer C.; Ekman E.; Agin-Liebes G.; Trope A.; Boden M. T.; Dilley P. J.; Mitchell J.; Woolley J. (2020) Psilocybin-assisted group therapy for demoralized older long-term AIDS survivor men: An open-label safety and feasibility pilot study. EClinicalMedicine 27, 100538. 10.1016/j.eclinm.2020.100538. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Ross S.; Bossis A.; Guss J.; Agin-Liebes G.; Malone T.; Cohen B.; Mennenga S. E; Belser A.; Kalliontzi K.; Babb J.; et al. (2016) Rapid and sustained symptom reduction following psilocybin treatment for anxiety and depression in patients with life-threatening cancer: A randomized controlled trial. J. Psychopharmacol. 30, 1165–1180. 10.1177/0269881116675512. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Spriggs M. J.; Kettner H.; Carhart-Harris R. L. (2020) Positive effects of psychedelics on depression and wellbeing scores in individuals reporting an eating disorder. Eating and Weight Dis-Studies on Anorexia, Bulimia and Obesity 1–6. 10.1007/s40519-020-01000-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Carbonaro T. M.; Bradstreet M. P.; Barrett F. S.; MacLean K. A.; Jesse R.; Johnson M. W.; Griffiths R. R. (2016) Survey study of challenging experiences after ingesting psilocybin mushrooms: Acute and enduring positive and negative consequences. J. Psychopharmacol. 30, 1268–1278. 10.1177/0269881116662634. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Oregon Secretary of State . (2019, July 2) Complete text of initiative #34. http://oregonvotes.org/irr/2020/034text.pdf.
  14. Olson D. E. (2020) The subjective effects of psychedelics may not be necessary for their enduring therapeutic effects. ACS Pharmacol. Transl. Sci. 10.1021/acsptsci.0c00192. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Roseman L.; Nutt D. J.; Carhart-Harris R. L. (2018) Quality of acute psychedelic experience predicts therapeutic efficacy of psilocybin for treatment-resistant depression. Front. Pharmacol. 8, 974. 10.3389/fphar.2017.00974. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Sepeda N. D.; Clifton J. M.; Doyle L. Y.; Lancelotta R.; Griffiths R. R.; Davis A. K. (2019) Inhaled 5-methoxy-N, N-dimethyltryptamine: Supportive context associated with positive acute and enduring effects. J. Psychedelic Studies: 1–9. 10.1556/2054.2019.033. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  17. Yaden D. B., and Griffiths R. R. (2020) The subjective effects of psychedelics are necessary for their enduring therapeutic effects. ACS Pharmacol. Transl. Sci. 10.1021/acsptsci.0c00194. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Aday J. S.; Davoli C. C.; Bloesch E. K. (2020) Psychedelics and virtual reality: Parallels and applications. Ther. Adv. Psychopharmacol. 10.1177/2045125320948356. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Carhart-Harris R. L.; Roseman L.; Haijen E.; Erritzoe D.; Watts R.; Branchi I.; Kaelen M. (2018) Psychedelics and the essential importance of context. J. Psychopharmacol. 32, 725–731. 10.1177/0269881118754710. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Kaasik H.; Kreegipuu K. (2020) Ayahuasca users in Estonia: Ceremonial practices, subjective long-term effects, mental health, and quality of life. J. Psychoact. Drugs 52, 255. 10.1080/02791072.2020.1748773. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Johnson M. W.; Richards W. A.; Griffiths R. R. (2008) Human hallucinogen research: Guidelines for safety. J. Psychopharmacol. 22, 603–620. 10.1177/0269881108093587. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Lancelotta R. L.; Davis A. K. (2020) Use of benefit enhancement strategies among 5-methoxy-N, N-dimethyltryptamine (5-MeO-DMT) users: Associations with mystical, challenging, and enduring effects. J. Psychoact. Drugs 52, 273. 10.1080/02791072.2020.1737763. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Pinchbeck D. (2019) When Plants Dream: Ayahuasca, Amazonian Shamanism and the Global Psychedelic Renaissance,Watkins Publishing. [Google Scholar]
  24. Griffiths R. R.; Johnson M. W.; Richards W. A.; Richards B. D.; McCann U.; Jesse R. (2011) Psilocybin occasioned mystical-type experiences: Immediate and persisting dose-related effects. Psychopharm 218, 649–665. 10.1007/s00213-011-2358-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Holze F.; Vizeli P.; Ley L.; Muller F.; Dolder P.; Stocker M.; Duthaler U.; Varghese N.; Eckert A.; Borgwardt S.; Liechti M. E. (2021) Acute dose-dependent effects of lysergic acid diethylamide in a double-blind placebo-controlled study in healthy subjects. Neuropsychopharmacology 46, 537. 10.1038/s41386-020-00883-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Barsuglia J.; Davis A. K.; Palmer R.; Lancelotta R.; Windham-Herman A.-M.; Peterson K.; Polanco M.; Grant R.; Griffiths R. R. (2018) Intensity of mystical experiences occasioned by 5-MeO-DMT and comparison with a prior psilocybin study. Front Psych 9, 2459. 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02459. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Kangaslampi S.; Hausen A.; Rauteenmaa T. (2020) Mystical experiences in retrospective reports of first times using a psychedelic in Finland. J. Psychoact. Drugs 52, 309. 10.1080/02791072.2020.1767321. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Lyvers M.; Meester M. (2012) Illicit use of LSD or psilocybin, but not MDMA or nonpsychedelic drugs, is associated with mystical experiences in a dose-dependent manner. J. Psychoact. Drugs 44, 410–417. 10.1080/02791072.2012.736842. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Nicholas C. R; Henriquez K. M; Gassman M. C; Cooper K. M; Muller D.; Hetzel S.; Brown R. T; Cozzi N. V; Thomas C.; Hutson P. R (2018) High dose psilocybin is associated with positive subjective effects in healthy volunteers. J. Psychopharmacol. 32, 770–778. 10.1177/0269881118780713. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Griffiths R. R.; Richards W. A.; Johnson M. W.; McCann U. D.; Jesse R. (2008) Mystical-type experiences occasioned by psilocybin mediate the attribution of personal meaning and spiritual significance 14 months later. J. Psychopharmacol. 22, 621–632. 10.1177/0269881108094300. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Griffiths R. R; Johnson M. W; Richards W. A; Richards B. D; Jesse R.; MacLean K. A; Barrett F. S; Cosimano M. P; Klinedinst M. A (2018) Psilocybin-occasioned mystical-type experience in combination with meditation and other spiritual practices produces enduring positive changes in psychological functioning and in trait measures of prosocial attitudes and behaviors. J. Psychopharmacol. 32, 49–69. 10.1177/0269881117731279. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. James E.; Robertshaw T. L.; Hoskins M.; Sessa B. (2020) Psilocybin occasioned mystical-type experiences. Hum. Psychopharmacol. 35, e2742 10.1002/hup.2742. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Dos Santos R. G.; Bouso J. C.; Hallak J. E. (2017) Ayahuasca, dimethyltryptamine, and psychosis: A systematic review of human studies. Ther. Adv. Psychopharmacol. 7, 141–157. 10.1177/2045125316689030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Griffiths R. R.; Richards W. A.; McCann U.; Jesse R. (2006) Psilocybin can occasion mystical-type experiences having substantial and sustained personal meaning and spiritual significance. Psychopharm 187, 268–283. 10.1007/s00213-006-0457-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Isbell H. (1959) Comparison of the reactions induced by psilocybin and LSD-25 in man. Psychopharmacologia 1, 29–38. 10.1007/BF00408109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Bonson K. R.; Murphy D. L. (1995) Alterations in responses to LSD in humans associated with chronic administration of tricyclic antidepressants, monoamine oxidase inhibitors or lithium. Behav. Brain Res. 73, 229–233. 10.1016/0166-4328(96)00102-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Fiorella D.; Helsley S.; Rabin R. A.; Winter J. C. (1996) Potentiation of LSD-induced stimulus control by fluoxetine in the rat. Life Sci. 59, PL283–PL287. 10.1016/0024-3205(96)00490-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Vollenweider F. X.; Vollenweider-Scherpenhuyzen M. F.; Bäbler A.; Vogel H.; Hell D. (1998) Psilocybin induces schizophrenia-like psychosis in humans via a serotonin-2 agonist action. NeuroReport 9, 3897–3902. 10.1097/00001756-199812010-00024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Aday J. S.; Mitzkovitz C. M.; Bloesch E. K.; Davoli C. C.; Davis A. K. (2020) Long-term effects of psychedelic drugs: A systematic review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 113, 179–189. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.03.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Studerus E.; Gamma A.; Kometer M.; Vollenweider F. X. (2012) Prediction of psilocybin response in healthy volunteers. PLoS One 7, e30800 10.1371/journal.pone.0030800. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Moher D.; Liberati A.; Tetzlaff J.; Altman D. G. (2009) Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: The PRISMA statement. PLoS Med. 6, e1000097 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Strassman R. J.; Qualls C. R.; Uhlenhuth E. H.; Kellner R. (1994) Dose-response study of N, N-dimethyltryptamine in humans: II. Subjective effects and preliminary results of a new rating scale. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 51, 98–108. 10.1001/archpsyc.1994.03950020022002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Bienemann B.; Ruschel N. S.; Campos M. L.; Negreiros M. A.; Mograbi D. C. (2020) Self-reported negative outcomes of psilocybin users: A quantitative textual analysis. PLoS One 15, e0229067 10.1371/journal.pone.0229067. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Lebedev A. V.; Lövdén M.; Rosenthal G.; Feilding A.; Nutt D. J.; Carhart-Harris R. L. (2015) Finding the self by losing the self: Neural correlates of ego-dissolution under psilocybin. Hum Brain Map 36, 3137–3153. 10.1002/hbm.22833. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Lewis C. R.; Preller K. H.; Braden B. B.; Riecken C.; Vollenweider F. X. (2020) Rostral anterior cingulate thickness predicts the emotional psilocybin experience. Biomedicines 8, 34. 10.3390/biomedicines8020034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Russ S. L.; Carhart-Harris R. L.; Maruyama G.; Elliott M. S. (2019) Replication and extension of a model predicting response to psilocybin. Psychopharm 236, 3221–3230. 10.1007/s00213-019-05279-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Russ S. L.; Carhart-Harris R. L.; Maruyama G.; Elliott M. S. (2019) States and traits related to the quality and consequences of psychedelic experiences. Psych of Consciousness: Theory, Res, and Practice 6, 1. 10.1037/cns0000169. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  48. Smigielski L.; Kometer M.; Scheidegger M.; Krähenmann R.; Huber T.; Vollenweider F. X. (2019) Characterization and prediction of acute and sustained response to psychedelic psilocybin in a mindfulness group retreat. Sci. Rep. 9, 1–13. 10.1038/s41598-019-50612-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Stauffer C. S., Anderson B. T., Ortigo K. M., and Woolley J. (2020). Psilocybin-assisted group therapy and attachment: Observed reduction in attachment anxiety and influences of attachment insecurity on the psilocybin experience. ACS Pharmacol. Transl. Sci. 10.1021/acsptsci.0c00169. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Stenbæk D. S., Madsen M. K., Ozenne B., Kristiansen S., Burmester D., Erritzoe D., Knudsen G. M., and Fisher P. M. (2020) Brain serotonin 2A receptor binding predicts subjective temporal and mystical effects of psilocybin in healthy humans. J. Psychopharmacol. 10.1177/0269881120959609. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Carhart-Harris R. L.; Kaelen M.; Whalley M. G.; Bolstridge M.; Feilding A.; Nutt D. J. (2015) LSD enhances suggestibility in healthy volunteers. Psychopharm 232, 785–794. 10.1007/s00213-014-3714-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Terhune D. B.; Luke D. P.; Kaelen M.; Bolstridge M.; Feilding A.; Nutt D.; Carhart-Harris R.; Ward J. (2016) A placebo-controlled investigation of synaesthesia-like experiences under LSD. Neuropsychologia 88, 28–34. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.04.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Bouso J. C.; Fábregas J. M.; Antonijoan R. M.; Rodríguez-Fornells A.; Riba J. (2013) Acute effects of ayahuasca on neuropsychological performance: differences in executive function between experienced and occasional users. Psychopharm 230, 415–424. 10.1007/s00213-013-3167-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Haijen E. C. H. M.; Kaelen M.; Roseman L.; Timmermann C.; Kettner H.; Russ S.; Nutt D.; Daws R. E.; Hampshire A. D. G.; Lorenz R.; Carhart-Harris R. L. (2018) Predicting responses to psychedelics: A prospective study. Front. Pharmacol. 9, 897. 10.3389/fphar.2018.00897. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Kettner H.; Gandy S.; Haijen E. C.; Carhart-Harris R. L. (2019) From egoism to ecoism: Psychedelics increase nature relatedness in a state-mediated and context-dependent manner. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 16, 5147. 10.3390/ijerph16245147. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Uthaug M. V.; Lancelotta R.; van Oorsouw K.; Kuypers K. P. C.; Mason N.; Rak J.; Sulakova A.; Jurok R.; Maryska M.; Kuchar M.; et al. (2019) A single inhalation of vapor from dried toad secretion containing 5-methoxy-N, N-dimethyltryptamine (5-MeO-DMT) in a naturalistic setting is related to sustained enhancement of satisfaction with life, mindfulness-related capacities, and a decrement of psychopathological symptoms. Psychopharmacology 236, 2653–2666. 10.1007/s00213-019-05236-w. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Uthaug M. V.; van Oorsouw K.; Kuypers K. P. C.; van Boxtel M.; Broers N. J.; Mason N. L.; Toennes S. W.; Riba J.; Ramaekers J. G. (2018) Sub-acute and long-term effects of ayahuasca on affect and cognitive thinking style and their association with ego dissolution. Psychopharmacology 235, 2979–2989. 10.1007/s00213-018-4988-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Aday J. S., Rizer W., and Carlson J. M. (2017) Neural mechanisms of emotions and affect, in Emotions and Affect in Human Factors and Human-Computer Interaction (Jeon M., Ed.) pp 27–87, Academic Press, San Diego, CA. [Google Scholar]
  59. Griffiths R. R.; Hurwitz E. S.; Davis A. K.; Johnson M. W.; Jesse R. (2019) Survey of subjective” God encounter experiences“: Comparisons among naturally occurring experiences and those occasioned by the classic psychedelics psilocybin, LSD, ayahuasca, or DMT. PLoS One 14, e0214377 10.1371/journal.pone.0214377. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Mayeux R. (2004) Biomarkers: Potential uses and limitations. NeuroRx 1, 182–188. 10.1602/neurorx.1.2.182. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Snell W. E.; Miller R. S.; Belk S. S.; Garcia-Falconi R.; Hernandez-Sanchez J. E. (1989) Men’s and women’s emotional disclosures: The impact of disclosure recipient, culture, and the masculine role. Sex Roles 21, 467–486. 10.1007/BF00289098. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  62. Beliveau V.; Ganz M.; Feng L.; Ozenne B.; Højgaard L.; Fisher P. M.; Svarer C.; Greve D. N.; Knudsen G. M. (2017) A high-resolution in vivo atlas of the human brain’s serotonin system. J. Neurosci. 37, 120–128. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2830-16.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Brown R. T.; Nicholas C. R.; Cozzi N. V.; Gassman M. C.; Cooper K. M.; Muller D.; Thomas C. D.; Hetzel S. J.; Henriquez K. M.; Ribaudo A. S.; Hutson P. R. (2017) Pharmacokinetics of escalating doses of oral psilocybin in healthy adults. Clin. Pharmacokinet. 56, 1543–1554. 10.1007/s40262-017-0540-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Dolder P. C.; Schmid Y.; Haschke M.; Rentsch K. M.; Liechti M. E. (2016) Pharmacokinetics and concentration-effect relationship of oral LSD in humans. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 19, pyv072 10.1093/ijnp/pyv072. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Holze F.; Duthaler U.; Vizeli P.; Müller F.; Borgwardt S.; Liechti M. E. (2019) Pharmacokinetics and subjective effects of a novel oral LSD formulation in healthy subjects. Br. J. Clin. Pharmacol. 85, 1474–1483. 10.1111/bcp.13918. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Glicksohn J.; Barrett T. R. (2003) Absorption and hallucinatory experience. App Cog Psych: J. Soc. App Res. in Mem and Cog 17, 833–849. 10.1002/acp.913. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  67. Ott U.; Reuter M.; Hennig J.; Vaitl D. (2005) Evidence for a common biological basis of the absorption trait, hallucinogen effects, and positive symptoms: Epistasis between 5-HT2a and COMT polymorphisms. Am. J. Med. Genet., Part B 137B, 29–32. 10.1002/ajmg.b.30197. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Radtke H. L.; Stam H. J. (1991) The relationship between absorption, openness to experience, anhedonia, and susceptibility. Int. J. Clin Exp Hypnosis 39, 39–56. 10.1080/00207149108409617. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Brouwer A., and Carhart-Harris R. L. (2020). Pivotal mental states. J. Psychopharmacol. 10.1177/0269881120959637. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Santos R. D.; Landeira-Fernandez J.; Strassman R. J.; Motta V.; Cruz A. P. M. (2007) Effects of ayahuasca on psychometric measures of anxiety, panic-like and hopelessness in Santo Daime members. J. Ethnopharmacol. 112, 507–513. 10.1016/j.jep.2007.04.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Leary T.; Litwin G. H.; Metzner R. (1963) Reactions to psilocybin administered in a supportive environment. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 137, 561. 10.1097/00005053-196312000-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Aday J. S.; Bloesch E. K.; Davoli C. C. (2020) 2019: A year of expansion in psychedelic research, industry, and deregulation. Drug Sci., Policy Law 6, 1–6. 10.1177/2050324520974484. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  73. Richards W. A.; Rhead J. C.; DiLeo F. B.; Yensen R.; Kurland A. A. (1977) The peak experience variable in DPT-assisted psychotherapy with cancer patients. J. Psychedelic Drugs 9, 1–10. 10.1080/02791072.1977.10472020. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  74. McWilliams S. A.; Tuttle R. J. (1973) Long-term psychological effects of LSD. Psychol Bull. 79, 341–351. 10.1037/h0034411. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Hartogsohn I. (2016) Set and setting, psychedelics and the placebo response: An extra-pharmacological perspective on psychopharmacology. J. Psychopharmacol. 30, 1259–1267. 10.1177/0269881116677852. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Wolff M.; Evens R.; Mertens L. J.; Koslowski M.; Betzler F.; Gründer G.; Jungaberle H. (2020) Learning to let go: A cognitive-behavioral model of how psychedelic therapy promotes acceptance. Front Psychiatry 11, 5. 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Hughes S. G. (1998) Prescribing for the elderly patient: Why do we need to exercise caution?. Br. J. Clin. Pharmacol. 46, 531. 10.1046/j.1365-2125.1998.00842.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Rosenthal R. (1979) The file drawer problem and tolerance for null results. Psych Bull. 86, 638–641. 10.1037/0033-2909.86.3.638. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  79. Polito V.; Stevenson R. J. (2019) A systematic study of microdosing psychedelics. PLoS One 14, e0211023 10.1371/journal.pone.0211023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

pt1c00014_si_001.xlsx (10.4KB, xlsx)

Articles from ACS Pharmacology & Translational Science are provided here courtesy of American Chemical Society

RESOURCES