Abstract
Acute high-intensity interval training (HIIT) is a time-efficient strategy to improve physical health; however, the effect of acute HIIT on executive function (EF) is unclear. The aim of this study was to systematically review the existing evidence and quantify the effect of acute HIIT on overall EF and the factors affecting the relationship between acute HIIT and EF. Standard databases (i.e., the PubMed, Medline, Scopus, and CENTRAL databases) were searched for studies that examined the effect of acute HIIT on EF and were published up until January 2021. The overall EF and factors grouped by three categories, namely, EF assessment characteristics, exercise intervention characteristics, and sample and study characteristics, were analyzed by percentage of comparison for positive or null/negative effects. Overall, 35 of 57 outcomes (61%) across 24 studies revealed that acute HIIT has a positive effect on overall EF. In terms of factors, the results indicated that among EF assessment characteristics, groups, inhibition, updating, and the assessment occurring within 30 min may moderate the effect of acute HIIT on EF, while among exercise intervention characteristics, total time within 11 to 30 min may moderate the effect. Finally, among sample characteristics, age under 40 years may moderate the effect. Acute HIIT is generally considered a viable alternative for eliciting EF gains, with factors related to EF components, timing of the assessment, exercise total time, and age potentially moderating the effect of HIIT on EF.
Keywords: acute exercise, cognitive function, high-intensity interval training, executive function, exercise, systematic review
1. Introduction
Acute exercise, which refers here to a single bout of exercise, has received considerable and increasing interest due to its facilitating effect on cognitive function [1,2,3]. Indeed, it is currently recommended by both the American College of Sport Medicine guidelines (ACSM, 2020) and various national physical activity guidelines based in part on its beneficial effects on cognitive function [4]. Along with being positively associated with various aspects of cognitive function (e.g., attention, information processing, and memory), acute exercise has been found to have a pronounced effect on executive function (EF), a subset of top-down cognitive control processes for purposeful and goal-directed behavior [5,6,7,8]. Acute exercise has been observed to improve EF performance behaviorally [9,10,11], in addition to positively affecting EF task-associated neuroelectric activity [12,13,14] and neural networks [15,16]. The facilitation of EF following acute exercise has been further confirmed by several reviews [17,18,19]. However, such effects on specific modality of exercise (e.g., high-intensity interval training (HIIT)) remains for further investigation.
HIIT, a unique form of exercise, has been found to improve health in general [4]. HIIT consists of repeated, brief durations of high-intensity exercise (i.e., exercise resulting in ≥85% maximal heart rate) interspersed with periods of low-intensity exercise or rest [20,21,22]. Compared to typical exercise (e.g., aerobic exercise, resistance exercise), HIIT appears to constitute a more effective strategy for eliciting a range of physiological health benefits such as improved cardiorespiratory fitness [23,24], improved insulin sensitivity [25], and decreased body fat and blood pressure [26]. In addition to these physiological alterations, the beneficial effects of HIIT have also been found to extend to EF [27,28,29].
Recently, Chang and colleagues [30] proposed a “3W1H” framework aimed at further understanding the effects of acute exercise on cognitive function, in which it is suggested that the nature of cognitive functions and the exercise characteristics (i.e., what), moderators linked to sample characteristics and the timing of the assessment (i.e., who and when), and the potential mechanisms involved (i.e., how) should be considered. Relatedly, it is expected that the effects of acute HIIT on EF could be further enhanced based upon the framework. In specific terms, EF can be divided into two sub-domains consisting of core EF (i.e., inhibition, shifting, and updating/working memory) and higher-order EF (i.e., planning) [7,31], and the question of whether HIIT affects general EF or these specific EF sub-domains remains unanswered [19,32]. It is also unclear whether specific HIIT characteristics affecting EF could be investigated because while the nature of typical exercise characteristics (e.g., exercise duration, exercise mode, and exercise intensity) have been observed to affect EF [1,33], HIIT contains different exercise features (i.e., working/recovery time ratio and rest interval). Lastly, sample and study characteristics (i.e., age, gender, fitness level, design, and comparison) as well as the timing of the assessment have been found to influence the effects of acute exercise on cognitive function, but whether these factors affect the relationship between HIIT and EF has yet to be determined [1].
To extend the existing knowledge regarding the effect of acute HIIT on EF, systematic evaluations of the available evidence [27,28,29] and comprehensive reviews of the consequences of overall and moderating factors are required [34,35]. The present systematic review thus sought to investigate the effect of acute HIIT on EF while also considering three categories of potentially influential factors, including EF assessment characteristics (i.e., EF components and the timing of the assessment), exercise intervention characteristics (i.e., total time, type, modality, work/recovery time ratio, rest interval, and intensity), and sample and study characteristics (i.e., age, gender, fitness level, study design, and comparator), in order to further our understanding.
2. Methods
This systematic review was designed in accordance with the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-analysis (PRISMA) guidelines [36], and the methods used were selected based on the Cochrane Guidelines for Systematic Reviews for literature search and selection [37].
2.1. Search Strategy
The literature search consisted of a computer-based search of the PubMed, Medline, Scopus, and CENTRAL databases for articles published between January 1990 and January 2021. The search query was [“high intensity interval training” (Title/Abstract) OR “HIIT” (Title/Abstract) OR “high intensity interval exercise” (Title/Abstract) OR “HIIE” (Title/Abstract)] AND [“cognition” (Title/Abstract) OR “cognitive function” (Title/Abstract) OR “executive function” (Title/Abstract) OR “inhibition” (Title/Abstract) OR “updating” (Title/Abstract) OR “shifting” (Title/Abstract) OR “planning” (Title/Abstract)]. Additional articles were added by reviewing previous systematic reviews [24,27], and we also supplemented the search using complementary databases (i.e., the Google Scholar database). All the articles considered in the search were limited to peer-reviewed publications written in English.
2.2. Selection Criteria
The selection criteria were based on the PICOS criteria in order to define the characteristics of the included studies. Population: studies including healthy participants with no limitations for age were included; Intervention: studies evaluating the effect of acute HIIT on executive function and providing clear HIIT protocol were included; Comparator: studies comparing an active control (e.g., stretching) or passive control (e.g., watching television, sitting, and resting) were included; Outcomes: studies assessing EF were included; Study design: studies with any type of design (i.e., within-subject and between-subject designs) were included. One reviewer (JYA) initially performed the article search, after which two reviewers (JYA and FTC) screened the titles and abstracts of studies identified for potential selection by the search. Any disagreements were discussed with the third reviewer (YKC), until a consensus was achieved.
2.3. Quality Assessment
The Cochrane Collaborations’ domain-based assessment of risk of bias [38], a seven-item quality assessment tool, was employed. The seven items assessed can be described as follows: (1) random sequence generation refers to selection bias due to inadequate generation of a randomized sequence. (2) Allocation concealment refers to selection bias due to inadequate concealment of allocations prior to assignment. (3) Blinding refers to performance bias. (4) Blinding of the outcome assessment refers to detection bias. (5) Incomplete outcome data refers to attrition bias. (6) Selective reporting refers to reporting bias. (7) Other bias refers to bias due to problems not covered elsewhere in the domain-based assessment (e.g., a potential source of bias related to the specific study design). The methodological quality of each study was evaluated by two reviewers (JYA and FTC), and disagreements were resolved by consulting the third reviewer (YKC).
2.4. Data Extraction
Information on study details (i.e., author, year, and participant location), participants (i.e., sample size, gender, mean age, and fitness level), design characteristics (i.e., experimental design and comparator), exercise intervention (total time, type (modality), protocol (i.e., set, work and recovery time, and rest interval), and intensity), and the EF assessment (i.e., timing of the assessment, task, and components) were extracted from all the included studies. Data from included studies were extracted independently by two of the reviewers (JYA and FTC) and any discrepancies were solved by consulting a third reviewer (YKC).
For understanding potential moderators, the review further utilized the percentages of positive and null/negative effects. EF outcomes were extracted and response time (RT) data was included, if RT and accuracy were simultaneously coexisting [39]. We further categorized potential moderators into three groups to examine their possible moderating role in the effects of acute HIIT on EF (with the three categories being EF assessment characteristics, exercise intervention characteristics, and sample and study characteristics). The EF assessment characteristics considered included EF components (i.e., inhibition, updating, shifting, and planning) and the timing of the assessment (i.e., ≤10 min, 11–20 min, 21–30 min, or >30 min after the exercise). The exercise intervention characteristics considered included the total exercise time (i.e., ≤10 min, 11–20 min, 21–30 min, or >30 min), type (i.e., aerobic exercise or combined exercise), modality (i.e., running, cycling, circuit training, or boxing), rest interval (i.e., active or passive), work/recovery time ratio (i.e., >1, <1, or = 1), and intensity (i.e., maximal or submaximal). The sample and study characteristics considered consisted of age (i.e., ≤18 years old, 19~40 years old, or >40 years old), gender (i.e., >50% male, <50% male, or equal), fitness level (i.e., fit or sedentary), study design (i.e., within-subject or between-subject), and comparator (i.e., active control or passive control).
3. Results
3.1. Search Results and Overall EF
A total of 521 articles for potential inclusion were initially retrieved through the search, and 338 articles were identified through other sources (n = 859). After removing duplicates, 446 articles remained for further screening of titles and abstracts. Afterward, 152 full-text articles were assessed for examination of their eligibility. Finally, 24 articles were included in the systematic review (Figure 1).
All of the included articles were studies addressing the effects of an acute HIIT intervention on EF; an overview of the included studies is provided in Table 1. The present review found that the number of participants in each study ranged from 11 to 64, and the studies included studies conducted in twelve countries, including Brazil [40], China [41,42], Colombia [43], France [44], Germany [45,46], Japan [47,48,49,50,51], Korea [52], Lithuania [53], Poland [54,55], Spain [56], Switzerland [57], the United States [58,59,60,61], and the United Kingdom [62,63]. Additionally, 13 task-related EF components (i.e., inhibition, updating, shifting, and planning) were categorized (Table 2). Overall, the results found that 35 of 57 (61%) outcomes showed a positive effect on overall EF and 22 of 57 (39%) outcomes showed a null/negative effect on overall EF (Table 3).
Table 1.
Study | Participant | Design | Exercise Intervention | EF Assessment | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Author (year), Location | N (males %) Mean age (SD) Fitness level |
Exp. Design Comparator | Total time | Type | Protocol: Set, WRT (rest interval) |
Intensity | Time exam. | Task | Comp. |
Alves et al. (2014) [40] Brazil |
N = 22 (41%) 53.7 (4.7) Fit |
Within-subject Active |
20 min | Aerobic exercise (cycling) | 10 sets, 1 min and 1 min (active) | Submax. | Immediate | Digit span test-backward Stroop test | Updating Inhibition |
Burin.et al. (2020) [47] Japan |
N = 45 (53%) 23.7 (4.5) Fit |
Within-subject Passive |
8 min | Aerobic exercise (running) | 8 sets, 30 s and 30 s (active) | Maximal | Immediate | Stroop test | Inhibition |
Chang et al. (2017) [52] Korea |
N = 36 (0%) 21.4 (1.6) Fit |
Between-subject Passive |
30 min | Combined exercise (circuit training) | 3 sets, 1:2 (passive) | Submax. | 15 min | Stroop test | Inhibition |
Cooper et al. (2016) [62] UK |
N = 44 (48%) 12.6 (0.6) Fit |
Within-subject Passive |
10 min | Aerobic exercise (running) | 10 sets, 10 s and 50 s (active) | Maximal | Immediate, 45 min | Corsi block test Stroop test-complex level | Updating Inhibition |
Dupuy et al. (2018) [44] France |
N = 20 (100%) 28.0 (4.8) Fit |
Within-subject Passive |
36 min | Aerobic exercise (cycling) | 6 sets, 3 min and 3 min (passive) | Submax. | 15, 30, 45, and 60 min | Modified Stroop test-interference | Shifting |
Gmiat et al. (2017) [55] Poland |
N = 14 (0%) 22.7 (3), 41.7 (4) Sedentary |
Between-subject Passive |
27 min | Combined exercise (circuit training) | 3 sets, 30 s and 10 s (passive) | Maximal | 60 min | Corsi block test Stroop test | Inhibition Updating |
Hashimoto et al. (2018) [48] Japan |
N = 14 (100%) 24 (1) Sedentary |
Within-subject Passive |
28 min | Aerobic exercise (cycling) | 4 sets, 4 min and 3 min (active) | Submax. | Immediate, 10, 20, 30, 40, 50 min | Stroop test | Inhibition |
Kao et al. (2018) [58] USA |
N = 36 (50%) 21.5 (0.5) Sedentary |
Within-subject Passive |
16 min | Aerobic exercise (running) | 8 sets, 1 min and 1 min (active) | Submax. | 12 min | Flanker task-interference score | Inhibition |
Kao et al. (2017) [59] USA |
N = 64 (42%) 19.2 (0.8) Sedentary |
Within-subject Passive |
7.5 min | Aerobic exercise (running) | 3 sets, 1.5 min and 1 min (active) | Submax. | 20 min | Flanker task-incongruent | Inhibition |
Kujach et al. (2018) [49] Japan |
N = 25 (64%) 21.0 (1.6) Sedentary |
Within-subject Passive |
8 min | Aerobic exercise (cycling) | 8 sets, 30 s and 30 s (passive) | Submax. | 15 min | Stroop test | Inhibition |
Kujach et al. (2019) [50] Japan |
N = 36 (100%) 21 (1.29) Fit-low |
Between-subject Passive |
30 min | Aerobic exercise (cycling) | 6 sets, 30 s and 4.5 min (passive) | Maximal | 20 min | Stroop test TMT-B |
Inhibition Shifting |
Lambrick et al. (2016) [63] UK |
N = 20 (45%) 8.8 (0.8) Sedentary |
Within-subject Passive |
15 min | Aerobic exercise (running) | 6 sets, 55 s and 95 s (active) | Maximal | 1 min, 15 min, and 30 min | Stroop test | Inhibition |
Ligeza et al. (2018) [54] Poland |
N = 18 (100%) 24.9 (2.2) Fit |
Within-subject Passive |
24 min | Aerobic exercise (cycling) | 4 sets, 3 min, 3 min (active) | Submax. | 13 min | Flanker task-incongruent | Inhibition |
Ludyga et al. (2019) [57] Switzerland |
N = 94 (100%) 13.9 (0.7) Sedentary |
Between-subject Passive |
16 min | Combined exercise (circuit training) | 10 sets, 60 s and 30 s, 30 s and 30 s (passive) | Maximal | Immediate, 30 min, 60 min | Flanker task-incongruent | Inhibition |
Martínez et al. (2020) [56] Spain |
N = 25 (100%) (2.1) Fit |
Within-subject Passive |
20 min | Aerobic exercise (cycling) | 10 sets, 1 min and 1 min (passive) | Submax. | Immediate, 30 min | Digit span test | Updating |
Miller et al. (2019) [60] USA |
N = 25 (48%) 23 (2.79) Fit |
Within-subject Passive |
10 min (LV) and 20 min (MV) | Aerobic exercise (cycling) | 5 sets, 1 min and 1 min (active) (LV); 10 set, 1 min and 1 min (active) (MV) | Submax. | 1 min | Stroop test | Inhibition |
Quintero et al. (2018) [43] Colombia |
N = 36 (100%) 23.55 (3.4) Sedentary |
Between-subject Passive |
32 min | Aerobic exercise (running) | 4 sets, 4 min and 4 min (active) | Submax. | Immediate | Stroop test-interference | Inhibition |
Schwarck et al. (2019) [45] Germany |
N = 39 (100%) 23.33 (3.23) Fit |
Between-subject Passive |
25 min | Aerobic exercise (running) | 5 sets, 2 min and 3 min (active) | Submax. | 10 min | Stroop test-incongruent TMT-B |
Inhibition Shifting |
Slusher et al. (2018) [61] USA |
N = 13 (100%) 23.62 (1.06) Sedentary |
Within-subject Passive |
5 min | Aerobic exercise (cycling) | 10 sets, 20 s and 10 s. (active) | Maximal | Immediate | Wisconsin card sorting task | Updating |
Solianik et al. (2020) [53] Lithuania |
N = 11 (100%) 22.8 (2.9) Fit |
Within-subject passive |
12 min | Aerobic exercise (boxing) | 3 sets, 3 min and 1 min (passive) | Maximal | 21–30 min | Go/No-Go task Procedural reaction time Mathematical processing |
Inhibition Shifting Updating |
Sun et al. (2019) [42] China |
N = 20 (50%) 23.9 (2.5) Sedentary |
Within-subject Passive |
6 min | Aerobic exercise (cycling) | 10 sets, 6 s and 30 s (passive) | Maximal | Immediate | Go/No-Go task | Inhibition |
Tsukamoto et al. (2016) [51] Japan |
N = 12 (100%) 22.9 (0.4) Fit |
Within-subject Passive |
28 min | Aerobic exercise (cycling) | 4 sets, 4 min and 3 min (active) | Submax. | Immediate, 10, 20, and 30 min | Stroop test-incongruent | Inhibition |
Wilke et al. (2020) [46] Japan |
N = 35 (49%) 26.7 (3.6) Fit |
Between-subject Passive |
15 min | Combined exercise (circuit training) | 30 sets, 20 s and 10 s (passive) | Maximal | Immediate | Stroop TMT-B Digit span test |
Inhibition Shifting Updating |
Xie et al. (2020) [41] China |
N = 16 (100%) 24.5 (5.09) Sedentary |
Within-subject Passive |
20 min | Aerobic exercise (cycling) | 10 sets, 1 min and 1 min (active) | Submax. | 15 min | Flanker task-incongruent | Inhibition |
Note: WRT = work and recovery time; NR = not report; TMT-B = trail making test-B; UK = United Kingdom; USA = United States; Immediate ≤10 min; Exam. = Examination; Comp. = Component; Submax. = Submaximal.
Table 2.
Inhibition | Updating | Shifting | Planning |
---|---|---|---|
Go/No-Go task Flanker task Stroop test |
Corsi blocks test Digit span test-backward Mathematical processing Symbol digits modality Wisconsin card sorting task |
Modified Stroop test Procedural reaction time Switching task-costs Trail making task-B |
None |
Table 3.
N of Outcome | N of Positive Effect (%) | N of Null Effect (%) | |
---|---|---|---|
Overall | 57 | 35 (61%) | 22 (39%) |
EF assessment | |||
Component | |||
Inhibition | 39 | 27 (69%) | 12 (31%) |
Updating | 11 | 6 (55%) | 5 (45%) |
Shifting | 7 | 2 (29%) | 5 (71%) |
Planning | -- | -- | -- |
Time examination | |||
≤10 min | 24 | 16 (67%) | 8 (33%) |
11–20 min | 11 | 9 (88%) | 2 (12%) |
21–30 min | 10 | 7 (70%) | 3 (30%) |
>30 min | 12 | 3 (25%) | 9 (75%) |
Exercise intervention | |||
Total time | |||
≤10 min | 8 | 3 (38%) | 5 (62%) |
11–20 min | 24 | 17 (71%) | 7 (29%) |
21–30 min | 20 | 13 (65%) | 7 (35%) |
>30 min | 5 | 2 (40%) | 3 (60%) |
Type | |||
Aerobic exercise | 43 | 28 (65%) | 15 (35%) |
Combined exercise | 14 | 7 (50%) | 7 (50%) |
Modality | |||
Running | 12 | 7 (58%) | 5 (42%) |
Cycling | 28 | 19 (69%) | 9 (31%) |
Circuit training | 14 | 7 (50%) | 7 (50%) |
Boxing | 3 | 2 (67%) | 1 (33%) |
Rest interval | |||
Active | 30 | 21 (70%) | 9 (30%) |
Passive | 27 | 14 (52%) | 13 (48%) |
Work recovery ratio | |||
>1 | 25 | 16 (64%) | 9 (36%) |
<1 | 13 | 7 (54%) | 6 (46%) |
=1 | 19 | 12 (63%) | 7 (37%) |
Intensity | |||
Maximal | 21 | 12 (57%) | 9 (43%) |
Submaximal | 36 | 23 (64%) | 13 (36%) |
Sample and study characteristics | |||
Age | |||
≤18 years | 13 | 7 (54%) | 6 (46%) |
19~40 years | 40 | 27 (68%) | 13 (32%) |
>40 years | 4 | 1 (25%) | 3 (75%) |
Gender | |||
>50% male | 33 | 22 (67%) | 11 (33%) |
<50% male | 23 | 12 (52%) | 11 (48%) |
Equal | 1 | 1 (50%) | 1 (50%) |
Fitness level | |||
Fit | 53 | 33 (62%) | 20 (38%) |
Sedentary | 4 | 2 (50%) | 2 (50%) |
Study Design | |||
Within-subject | 38 | 25 (66%) | 13 (34%) |
Between subject | 19 | 10 (53%) | 9 (47%) |
Comparator | |||
Active control | 2 | 1 (50%) | 1 (50%) |
Passive control | 55 | 34 (62%) | 21 (38%) |
Note: EF = executive function.
3.2. EF Assessment
In terms of individual EF components, we found greater percentages of positive effects for inhibition (69% vs. 31%) and updating (55% vs. 45%), whereas the results revealed that the number of null/negative effects was greater than the number of positive effects for shifting (29% vs. 71%). No EF outcomes for planning were included in the reviewed studies. In the terms of the timing of assessment, the number of positive effects was greater than the number of null/negative effects when the assessment was performed less than 10 min (67% vs. 33%), 11–20 min (88% vs. 12%), or 21–30 min (70% vs. 30%) after the exercise. In contrast, the number of null/negative effects was greater than the number of positive effects when the assessment was conducted more than 30 min (25% vs. 75%) after the exercise.
3.3. Exercise Intervention
This systematic review also examined the effects of acute exercise on EF through consideration of six exercise intervention characteristics, including typical exercise features (i.e., total time, type, and modality) and HIIT features (i.e., rest interval, work/recovery time ratio, and intensity).
Regarding typical exercise features, the analysis indicated that greater percentages of positive effects were found for total time between 11 and 20 min (71% vs. 29%) and total time between 21 and 30 min (65% vs. 35%), but not for total time of less than 10 min (38% vs. 62%) or total time of more than 30 min (40% vs. 60%). Regarding exercise type, the percentage of positive effects was greater than that of null/negative effects for aerobic exercise (65% vs. 35%), while the percentages of positive effects and null/negative effects were similar for combined exercise (50% vs. 50%). In terms of exercise modality, the percentage of positive effects was greater than that of null/negative effects for running (58% vs. 42%), cycling (69% vs. 31%), and boxing (67% vs. 33%). In addition, similar percentages of positive and null/negative effects were found for circuit training (50% vs. 50%).
Regarding HIIT features, the percentage of positive effects was greater than the percentage of null/negative effects for active interval control (70% vs. 30%) and for passive control (52% vs. 48%) during the rest interval. In terms of work/recovery time ratio, the analysis indicated that the percentage of positive effects was greater than that of null/negative effects for all categories, including >1 (64% vs. 36%), <1 (54% vs. 46%), and = 1 (63% vs. 37%). In terms of intensity, the percentage of positive effects was greater than the percentage of null/negative effects for maximal intensity (57% vs. 43%) and submaximal intensity (64% vs. 36%).
3.4. Sample and Study Characteristics
This systematic review considered also another five factors, including sample characteristics (i.e., age, gender, and fitness level) and study characteristics (study design and comparison type).
Regarding the sample characteristics, the results for age indicated that the percentage of positive effects was greater than the percentage of null/negative effects for participants aged less than 18 years (54% vs. 46%) and participants aged 19 to 40 years old (68% vs. 32%), whereas the percentage of null/negative effects was greater than percentage of positive effects for participants aged more than 40 years old (25% vs. 75%). In terms of gender, regardless of the percentage of male participant, the percentage of positive effects was greater than that of null/negative effects (52~67% vs. 33~48%). In terms of fitness level, the percentage of positive effects was greater than that of null/negative effects for fit participants (62% vs. 38%), whereas the percentages were similar for sedentary participants (50% vs. 50%). In terms of study characteristics, the percentage of positive effects was greater than that of null/negative effects for within-subject designs (66% vs. 34%) and between-subject designs (53% vs. 47%). In terms of comparators, the percentage of positive effects was greater than that of null/negative effects for passive control (62% vs. 38%) and similar for active control (50% vs. 50%).
3.5. Quality Assessment
Most of the studies revealed unclear risks of bias in terms of selection bias, performance bias, and detection bias, as well as low risks of bias in terms of attrition bias, reporting bias, and other bias. High risks of bias were reported in terms of performance bias, detection bias, reporting bias, and other bias, but with relative low percentages. These results demonstrated that the included studies were of low to moderate quality in general. The risk of bias results are summarized in Figure 2.
4. Discussion
This systematic review identified 57 outcomes across 24 articles in order to investigate the effect of acute HIIT on EF and to examine whether factors grouped into three categories, namely, EF assessment characteristics, exercise intervention characteristics, and sample and study characteristics, affect the relationship between HIIT and EF. Specifically, we used percentage of comparison (positive effects or null/negative effects) as an operational definition to identify the effect of acute HIIT on EF. Overall, the results revealed that acute HIIT generally tends to have a positive effect on EF. Moreover, they indicated that EF assessment characteristics (i.e., components, timing of the assessment), exercise intervention characteristics (i.e., total time) and sample characteristics (i.e., age) may all moderate the relationship between acute HIIT and EF.
4.1. Overall Effect
Sixty-one percent (35 of 57) outcomes from our analysis suggested that acute HIIT tends to have a positive effect on overall EF. The observed beneficial effect on EF effect of acute HIIT extends previous studies that utilized acute types of aerobic and resistance exercises [1] as well as high-intensity exercise [19] to determine their effects on EF. Remarkably, the results indicating the facilitation of EF following acute HIIT were provided by more updated, comprehensive (e.g., multiple exercise modality), and objective evaluations in this review compared to a previous systematic review [27].
The facilitation of EF possibly results from physiological alterations induced by HIIT (e.g., heart rate, lactate, catecholamine, and blood flow alterations), which in turn lead to an individual increasing his or her attentional sources when engaging in cognitive performances [64,65]. Previous studies have also provided evidence indicating that acute HIIT affects the prefrontal cortex, the brain region associated with EF, by increasing prefrontal cortex activation and oxygenation [59,63]. Furthermore, the observed EF facilitation might be associated with exercise-induced brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF), a biomarker associated with EF [61,66]. Specifically, a single bout of HIIT induces higher brain H2O2 and TNF-α levels [67], after which these molecules activate the signaling of peroxisome proliferator-activated receptor-γ coactivator (PGC-1α) to enhance neuron BDNF synthesis [61,67]. It should be noted that these potential mechanisms have not been directly examined in terms of their role in the relationship between acute HIIT and EF, so more studies are needed to clarify their influence.
4.2. EF Assessment
Few studies have examined how acute HIIT affects EF from the perspectives of specific EF components and timing of the assessment. Our review reveals that acute HIIT tends to have positive effects on two specific components (i.e., inhibition and updating, but not shifting) and based on the timing of the assessment (i.e., within 30 min of the exercise but not more than 30 min after the exercise), implying that experimental variations affect the observed effects of acute HIIT on EF.
Most of the studies included in this review focused on inhibition (n = 39), and 69% of the outcomes indicated a positive effect of acute HIIT on EF. Inhibition is believed to reflect the ability to inhibit automatic responses when engaged in cognitive processes and has been observed to be positively linked to acute high-intensity exercise [68]. Given that inhibition was assessed by the studies in our review through multiple EF tasks including the Stroop test, the Flanker task, and the Go/No-Go task, our results suggest a positive effect of acute HIIT on inhibition regardless of the assessment task employed.
A total of 55% of outcomes indicated a positive effect of HIIT on updating, but only 29% of outcomes showed a positive effect on shifting, implying that acute HIIT has varying effects on specific EF components. Although updating and shifting are recognized as aspects of core EF, the concepts and involved brain regions of these two EF components seem to be different. Specifically, updating (also known as working memory), a capacity for processing and restoring temporary information [69,70], is associated with the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of the middle frontal gyrus, while shifting (also known as cognitive flexibility), which refers to the ability to perceive alternative explanations for occurrences and to modify responses while overriding automatic behaviors [71], is associated with the anterior cingulate (located at anterior and posterior regions). Our findings suggest that acute HIIT may affect these two components differently. Notably, however, given that the numbers of outcomes regarding updating (n = 11) and shifting (n = 7) were relatively limited, the results should be interpreted with caution. Furthermore, it is noteworthy that there were no studies focused on planning. Planning is believed to reflect an ability to manage a strategy and set goals and is considered a higher-level and primary component of EF [7]. Our results therefore call for caution in examining the specific EF components to further our understating regarding the impacts of acute HIIT on EF.
In terms of timing of the assessment, our findings indicate that positive effects of acute HIIT on EF were observed for assessments conducted within 30 min of the exercise (i.e., ≤10 min, 11–20 min, 21–30 min) but not for assessments conducted more than 30 min after the termination of the exercise suggest that the beneficial effects of acute HIIT on EF were diminished after 30 min. The finding of decreased effects on EF with longer time following acute exercise is partially supported by a previously conducted meta-analysis [1], in which larger positive effects on cognitive function were observed within 11 to 20 min following acute exercise (effect size (ES) = 0.26) and smaller effects were showed after 20 min (ES = 0.17). Indeed, Cooper, Bandelow, Nute, Dring, Stannard, Morris and Nevill [62] hypothesized that the attenuated magnitude of the improvement of cognitive performance at 30 min following the cessation of HIIT is likely associated with higher neuromuscular fatigue during exercise at 95% of maximal power output. It is thus possible that the beneficial effect on EF would be diminished along with the passing of time following the termination of acute HIIT.
4.3. Exercise Intervention
The present review included past studies that investigated both typical exercise characteristics (i.e., total time, type, and modality) and specific HIIT characteristics (i.e., rest interval, work/recovery time ratio, and intensity) in order to examine the role of these exercise characteristics in the relationship between acute HIIT and EF.
Acute HIIT interventions with total time between 11 and 20 min (17 of 24 outcomes, 71%) or between 21 and 30 min (13 of 20 outcomes, 65%) tended to have positive effects on EF, but those with total time of less than 10 min or more than 30 min did not consistently have positive effects on EF (38~40% for positive effects). These findings confirm previous empirical studies indicating that acute exercise durations of 11 to 30 min are required to increase EF. For example, Chang et al. [72] focused on younger adults to examine the dose–response relationship between acute exercise duration and EF and found that acute exercise for 30 min (i.e., 5 min warm-up, 20 min main exercise, and 5 min cool-down) has beneficial effects on EF, whereas acute exercise for 10 min or 45 min showed negligible effects. Their follow-up study focused on late-middle-aged adults further demonstrated that acute exercise bouts for 30 min show more enhanced EF compared to those of less than 20 min [73]. However, given that these studies focused on acute aerobic exercise, whether or not there is a dose–response relationship between HIIT duration and EF requires further examination.
Interestingly, HIIT types (i.e., aerobic exercise and combined exercise) and HIIT modality (i.e., running, cycling, and boxing) favor positive effects (i.e., 58 to 69%) on EF, wherein HIIT types (i.e., combined exercise) and HIIT modality (i.e., circuit training) show 50% outcomes favor positive effects. Along with a study by Hsieh, Chueh, Huang, Kao, Hillman, Chang, and Hung [27] indicating that HIIT with aerobic exercise type facilitates EF, our review extends previous findings by suggesting that HIIT type and modality generally have positive influences on EF.
Regarding rest interval, 70% positive outcomes for active rest (e.g., low-intensity exercise) and 52% positive outcomes for passive rest (i.e., full rest) were observed. The results suggest that while both rest types benefit EF, HIIT with active rest may lead to more positive effects. Indeed, the beneficial effects of acute HIIT with active rest were observed not only immediately after [63] but also at 60 min after acute HIIT [57]. Our review also examined the role of work/recovery time ratios categorized into “>1” (i.e., exercise time is greater than rest time), “<1” (i.e., rest time is greater than exercise time), and “=1” (i.e., exercise time and rest time are equal) and observed that all the categories showed greater positive percentages (i.e., from 54% to 64%). While Hsieh, Chueh, Huang, Kao, Hillman, Chang, and Hung [27] have suggested that a ratio of 1 is better than other ratios based upon a specific empirical study [57], our results from other objective evaluations demonstrate that the work/recovery time ratio of HIIT is not a sensitive factor in terms of the effects of HIIT on EF.
Our findings indicated that acute HIIT tends to have a positive effect on EF regardless of its intensity (i.e., submaximal or maximal), which is in contrast with previous hypotheses, such as the inverted-U theory [74], reticular-activation hypofrontality theory [75], and neurochemical hypotheses [64], which suggest that acute exercise of high intensity seems to impair EF. It is worth noting, however, that those hypotheses were proposed based upon aerobic exercises with continuous rhythms, which are different from HIIT containing numerous short bouts of high-intensity exercise and rest, suggesting that the special form of HIIT might result in different effects on EF.
4.4. Sample and Study Characteristics
Three individual sample characteristics including age, gender, and fitness level were analyzed. We categorized age so as to present the adolescents, adults, and middle-aged to older adults. We believe the ages would be considered as main moderators between HIIT and EF. Greater percentages of positive effects were found, for both populations under 18 years of age, and between 19 to 40 years of age (i.e., 54% and 68%, respectively), but not for those of more than 40 years of age (i.e., 25%). However, these results should be interpreted with caution, because the number of studies focusing on individuals older than 40 years was limited (n = 4), and none of the selected studies targeted older adults. The limited numbers of such studies may be linked to HIIT features, namely, the high-intensity and quick rhythm of HIIT, which raise safety issues and are a major exercise barrier for its (more) widespread implementation, particularly among older individuals [76]. Clearly, optimal HIIT in terms of duration, intensity, type, and modality, must be considered for middle-aged or older populations.
The results for all genders (i.e., >50% male, <50% male) and fitness levels (i.e., fit, sedentary) showed greater percentages of positive effect, with males and individuals with fit status showing higher positive percentages (62 to 67%). These findings suggest that acute HIIT might have both general and specific effects on EF in terms of these two individual characteristics.
Study characteristics were categorized into two dimensions (i.e., study design and comparator). More studies (n = 38) employed within-subject designs than between-subject designs (n = 19), which was consistent with previous studies regarding the effects of acute exercise on cognitive function indicating that cross-over designs are typical employed [1]. Although within-subject designs showed greater percentages of positive effects (i.e., 66%) compared to between-subject designs (i.e., 53%), the results of within-subject and between-subject study designs should be interpreted differently, given that a within-subject design has more power but potentially suffers from confounds, while a between-subject design is more conservative but has limitations in some cases [77]. We therefore suggest the choice of design should be further considered. Lastly, we also included both active and passive controls to confirm whether the observed effects of acute HIIT on EF were affected by comparator, and the results indicated that passive control is showed more effects on EF (62%) compared to active control (50%). This finding is important because it indicates that acute HIIT tends to have positive effects regardless of the comparator. However, further studies are needed to replicate these findings because only a small number of active controls were utilized (n = 2).
4.5. Strength and Limitations
This was first systematic review to have examined the effects of HIIT on EF with consideration of multiple factors, as well as considering evaluations by using objective approaches; however, it also had some limitations. The first was the small number of studies included overall. HIIT is a relatively new exercise modality and was initially utilized mainly to improve athletic performance [78]. Most studies of HIIT have thus focused on its effects on physical health [4,79], while the examination of its effects on mental health, including cognitive function, began relatively recently, which limited the number of studies included in this review. Additionally, for fully understanding the research issue, unpublished articles could be further considered, in order to reduce publication bias (e.g., file drawer problem). Secondly, only a few studies were judged as low risk, and the associated methodological restrictions increased the possibility that the results were swayed by confounding variables, meaning that caution should be used in drawing any conclusions. Furthermore, we excluded several studies (n = 11) because the details of HIIT intervention (e.g., work/recovery time, intensity) were missing. Future studies are suggested in order to provide competed information about HIIT intervention. Lastly, while the percentages of positive verses negative effects provide a relatively objective means for evaluating the results, statistical analysis of effect sizes, obtained via meta-analysis, provides more precise conclusions, and therefore is suggested for use in future studies.
5. Conclusions
The current systematic review indicated that the majority of HIIT analyses indicated positive effects of HIIT on EF, and that the beneficial effects on EF associated with acute HIIT occurred regardless of exercise type, modality, rest interval, work/recovery time ratio, intensity, gender, fitness level, and comparator, with various EF assessment characteristics (i.e., components, timing of the assessment), exercise intervention characteristics (i.e., total time), and sample characteristics (i.e., age) potentially moderating the observed relationship between acute HIIT and EF. We believe our findings have important applications for enhancing the effects of acute HIIT on EF, especially for those seeking time-efficient designs of HIIT interventions to immediately facilitate EF.
Author Contributions
Y.-K.C., S.-S.H., J.-Y.A., and F.-T.C. contributed to the conception of the work. Y.-K.C., S.-S.H., J.-Y.A., F.-T.C., S.-C.K., T.-M.H., and A.-G.C. contributed to the design of the work. J.-Y.A., F.-T.C., and Y.-K.C. conducted the literature search, selection, data extraction, and analysis. J.-Y.A., S.-C.K., T.-M.H., and A.-G.C. conducted the assessment of study quality. J.-Y.A., Y.-K.C., and S.-S.H. wrote the first draft of the manuscript with support from A.-G.C. All authors contributed to the manuscript revisions and agreed with final approval of the version. All authors ensured the accuracy of research. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
Funding
This work was supported by part of a grant from Ministry of Science and Technology in Taiwan (MOST 107-2628-H-003-003-MY3) and National Taiwan Normal University from the Higher Education Sprout Project by the Ministry of Education (MOE) in Taiwan to Y.-K.C. This research was supported partly by grants received by A.-G.C. from the National Natural Science Foundation of China (31771243).
Institutional Review Board Statement
Not applicable.
Informed Consent Statement
Not applicable.
Data Availability Statement
All datasets generated for this research are included in this published article.
Conflicts of Interest
The authors no conflict of interest.
Footnotes
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Chang Y.K., Labban J.D., Gapin J.I., Etnier J.L. The effects of acute exercise on cognitive performance: A meta-analysis. Brain Res. 2012;1453:87–101. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2012.02.068. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Loprinzi P.D., Blough J., Crawford L., Ryu S., Zou L., Li H. The Temporal Effects of Acute Exercise on Episodic Memory Function: Systematic Review with Meta-Analysis. Brain Sci. 2019;9:87. doi: 10.3390/brainsci9040087. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Pindus D.M., Drollette E.S., Raine L.B., Kao S.C., Khan N., Westfall D.R., Hamill M., Shorin R., Calobrisi E., John D., et al. Moving fast, thinking fast: The relations of physical activity levels and bouts to neuroelectric indices of inhibitory control in preadolescents. J. Sport Health Sci. 2019;8:301–314. doi: 10.1016/j.jshs.2019.02.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Piercy K.L., Troiano R.P., Ballard R.M., Carlson S.A., Fulton J.E., Galuska D.A., George S.M., Olson R.D. The physical activity guidelines for Americans. JAMA. 2018;320:2020–2028. doi: 10.1001/jama.2018.14854. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Bento-Torres J., Bento-Torres N.V.O., Stillman C.M., Grove G.A.J., Huang H., Uyar F., Watt J.C., Wollam M.E., Erickson K.I. Associations between cardiorespiratory fitness, physical activity, intraindividual variability in behavior, and cingulate cortex in younger adults. J. Sport Health Sci. 2019;8:315–324. doi: 10.1016/j.jshs.2019.03.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Audiffren M., André N. The exercise-cognition relationship: A virtuous circle. J. Sport Health Sci. 2019;8:339–347. doi: 10.1016/j.jshs.2019.03.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Diamond A. Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2013;64:135–168. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Chen F.T., Chan K.H., Feng S.H., Wu T.Y., Kao S.C., Chang Y.K. New perspective: Effect of exercise and nutritional supplements on cognitive function in older adults. Phys. Educ. J. 2019;52:1–15. doi: 10.3966/10247297201903520S001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Chang Y.K., Chen F.T., Kuan G., Wei G.X., Chu C.H., Yan J., Chen A.G., Hung T.M. Effects of acute exercise duration on the inhibition aspect of executive function in late middle-aged adults. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2019;11:227. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2019.00227. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Peiffer R., Darby L.A., Fullenkamp A., Morgan A.L. Effects of acute aerobic exercise on executive function in older women. J. Sports Sci. Med. 2015;14:574–583. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Tsukamoto H., Takenaka S., Suga T., Tanaka D., Takeuchi T., Hamaoka T., Isaka T., Hashimoto T. Impact of exercise intensity and duration on postexercise executive function. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2017;49:774–784. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0000000000001155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Wu C.H., Karageorghis C.I., Wang C.C., Chu C.H., Kao S.C., Hung T.M., Chang Y.K. Effects of acute aerobic and resistance exercise on executive function: An ERP study. J. Sci. Med. Sport. 2019;22:1367–1372. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2019.07.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Chu C.H., Kramer A.F., Song T.F., Wu C.H., Hung T.M., Chang Y.K. Acute exercise and neurocognitive development in preadolescents and young adults: An ERP study. Neural Plast. 2017;2017:2631909. doi: 10.1155/2017/2631909. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Chang Y.K., Alderman B.L., Chu C.H., Wang C.C., Song T.F., Chen F.T. Acute exercise has a general facilitative effect on cognitive function: A combined ERP temporal dynamics and BDNF study. Psychophysiology. 2017;54:289–300. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12784. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Mehren A., Diaz Luque C., Brandes M., Lam A.P., Thiel C.M., Philipsen A., Özyurt J. Intensity-dependent effects of acute exercise on executive function. Neural Plast. 2019;2019:8608317. doi: 10.1155/2019/8608317. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Li L., Men W.W., Chang Y.K., Fan M.X., Li L., Wei G.X. Acute aerobic exercise increases cortical activity during working memory: A functional MRI study in female college students. PLoS ONE. 2014;9:e99222. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0099222. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Ludyga S., Gerber M., Brand S., Holsboer-Trachsler E., Pühse U. Acute effects of moderate aerobic exercise on specific aspects of executive function in different age and fitness groups: A meta-analysis. Psychophysiology. 2016;53:1611–1626. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12736. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Li J.W., O’Connor H., O’Dwyer N., Orr R. The effect of acute and chronic exercise on cognitive function and academic performance in adolescents: A systematic review. J. Sci. Med. Sport. 2017;20:841–848. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2016.11.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Moreau D., Chou E. The acute effect of high-intensity exercise on executive function: A meta-analysis. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2019;14:734–764. doi: 10.1177/1745691619850568. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Norton K., Norton L., Sadgrove D. Position statement on physical activity and exercise intensity terminology. J. Sci. Med. Sport. 2010;13:496–502. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2009.09.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Gibala M.J., Little J.P., Macdonald M.J., Hawley J.A. Physiological adaptations to low-volume, high-intensity interval training in health and disease. J. Physiol. 2012;590:1077–1084. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2011.224725. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Gray S.R., Ferguson C., Birch K., Forrest L.J., Gill J.M. High-intensity interval training: Key data needed to bridge the gap from laboratory to public health policy. Br. J. Sports Med. 2016;50:1231–1232. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2015-095705. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Costa E.C., Hay J.L., Kehler D.S., Boreskie K.F., Arora R.C., Umpierre D., Szwajcer A., Duhamel T.A. Effects of high-intensity interval training versus moderate-intensity continuous training on blood pressure in adults with pre-to established hypertension: A systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized trials. Sports Med. 2018;48:2127–2142. doi: 10.1007/s40279-018-0944-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Costigan S.A., Eather N., Plotnikoff R.C., Taaffe D.R., Lubans D.R. High-intensity interval training for improving health-related fitness in adolescents: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Br. J. Sports Med. 2015;49:1253–1261. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2014-094490. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Gibala M.J., Gillen J.B., Percival M.E. Physiological and health-related adaptations to low-volume interval training: Influences of nutrition and sex. Sports Med. 2014;44(Suppl. 2):S127–S137. doi: 10.1007/s40279-014-0259-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Batacan R.B., Duncan M.J., Dalbo V.J., Tucker P.S., Fenning A.S. Effects of high-intensity interval training on cardiometabolic health: A systematic review and meta-analysis of intervention studies. Br. J. Sports Med. 2017;51:494–503. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2015-095841. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Hsieh S.S., Chueh T.Y., Huang C.J., Kao S.C., Hillman C.H., Chang Y.K., Hung T.M. Systematic review of the acute and chronic effects of high-intensity interval training on executive function across the lifespan. J. Sports Sci. 2020;39:10–22. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2020.1803630. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Cooper S.B., Dring K.J., Nevill M.E. High-Intensity Intermittent Exercise: Effect on Young People’s Cardiometabolic Health and Cognition. Curr. Sports Med. Rep. 2016;15:245–251. doi: 10.1249/JSR.0000000000000273. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Costigan S.A., Eather N., Plotnikoff R.C., Hillman C.H., Lubans D.R. High-intensity interval training for cognitive and mental health in adolescents. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2016;48:1985–1993. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0000000000000993. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Chang Y.K., Erickson K.I., Stamatakis E., Hung T.M. How the 2018 US physical activity guidelines are a call to promote and better understand acute physical activity for cognitive function gains. Sports Med. 2019;49:1625–1627. doi: 10.1007/s40279-019-01190-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Álvarez-Bueno C., Pesce C., Cavero-Redondo I., Sánchez-López M., Martínez-Hortelano J.A., Martínez-Vizcaíno V. The effect of physical activity interventions on children’s cognition and metacognition: A systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Am. Acad. Child. Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2017;56:729–738. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2017.06.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Browne S.E., Flynn M.J., O’Neill B.V., Howatson G., Bell P.G., Haskell-Ramsay C.F. Effects of acute high-intensity exercise on cognitive performance in trained individuals: A systematic review. Prog. Brain Res. 2017;234:161–187. doi: 10.1016/bs.pbr.2017.06.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Lambourne K., Tomporowski P. The effect of exercise-induced arousal on cognitive task performance: A meta-regression analysis. Brain Res. 2010;1341:12–24. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.03.091. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Etnier J.L., Chang Y.K. Exercise, cognitive function, and the brain: Advancing our understanding of complex relationships. J. Sport Health Sci. 2019;8:299–300. doi: 10.1016/j.jshs.2019.03.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Chen F.T., Etnier J.L., Chan K.H., Chiu P.K., Hung T.M., Chang Y.K. Effects of exercise training interventions on executive function in older adults: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Sports Med. 2020;50:1451–1467. doi: 10.1007/s40279-020-01292-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Liberati A., Altman D.G., Tetzlaff J., Mulrow C., Gøtzsche P.C., Ioannidis J.P., Clarke M., Devereaux P.J., Kleijnen J., Moher D. The PRISMA statement for reporting systematic reviews and meta-analyses of studies that evaluate healthcare interventions: Explanation and elaboration. BMJ. 2009;339:b2700. doi: 10.1136/bmj.b2700. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Moher D., Liberati A., Tetzlaff J., Altman D.G. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: The PRISMA statement. Ann. Intern. Med. 2009;151:264–269. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-151-4-200908180-00135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Higgins J.P., Thomas J., Chandler J., Cumpston M., Li T., Page M.J., Welch V.A. Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions. John Wiley & Sons; Hoboken, NJ, USA: 2019. [Google Scholar]
- 39.Liu S., Yu Q., Li Z., Cunha P.M., Zhang Y., Kong Z., Lin W., Chen S., Cai Y. Effects of Acute and Chronic Exercises on Executive Function in Children and Adolescents: A Systemic Review and Meta-Analysis. Front. Psychol. 2020;11:554915. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.554915. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Alves C.R.R., Tessaro V.H., Teixeira L.A.C., Murakava K., Roschel H., Gualano B., Takito M.Y. Influence of acute high-intensity aerobic interval exercise bout on selective attention and short-term memory tasks. Percept. Mot. Skills. 2014;118:63–72. doi: 10.2466/22.06.PMS.118k10w4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Xie C., Alderman B.L., Meng F., Ai J., Chang Y.K., Li A. Acute high-intensity interval exercise improves inhibitory control among young adult males with obesity. Front. Psychol. 2020;11:1291. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01291. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Sun S., Loprinzi P.D., Guan H., Zou L., Kong Z., Hu Y., Shi Q., Nie J. The effects of high-intensity interval exercise and hypoxia on cognition in sedentary young adults. Medicina. 2019;55:43. doi: 10.3390/medicina55020043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Quintero A.P., Bonilla-Vargas K.J., Correa-Bautista J.E., Domínguez-Sanchéz M.A., Triana-Reina H.R., Velasco-Orjuela G.P., García-Hermoso A., Villa-González E., Esteban-Cornejo I., Correa-Rodríguez M., et al. Acute effect of three different exercise training modalities on executive function in overweight inactive men: A secondary analysis of the BrainFit study. Physiol. Behav. 2018;197:22–28. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2018.09.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Dupuy O., Billaut F., Raymond F., Benraiss A., Theurot D., Bosquet L., Fraser S., Tremblay J. Effect of acute intermittent exercise on cognitive flexibility: The role of exercise intensity. J. Cogn. Enhanc. 2018;2:146–156. doi: 10.1007/s41465-018-0078-z. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Schwarck S., Schmicker M., Dordevic M., Rehfeld K., Müller N., Müller P. Inter-individual differences in cognitive response to a single bout of physical exercise—A randomized controlled cross-over study. J. Clin. Med. 2019;8:1101. doi: 10.3390/jcm8081101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Wilke J. Functional high-intensity exercise is more effective in acutely increasing working memory than aerobic walking: An exploratory randomized, controlled trial. Sci. Rep. 2020;10:1–7. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-69139-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Burin D., Liu Y., Yamaya N., Kawashima R. Virtual training leads to physical, cognitive and neural benefits in healthy adults. Neuroimage. 2020;222:117297. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117297. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Hashimoto T., Tsukamoto H., Takenaka S., Olesen N.D., Petersen L.G., Sørensen H., Nielsen H.B., Secher N.H., Ogoh S. Maintained exercise-enhanced brain executive function related to cerebral lactate metabolism in men. FASEB J. 2018;32:1417–1427. doi: 10.1096/fj.201700381RR. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Kujach S., Byun K., Hyodo K., Suwabe K., Fukuie T., Laskowski R., Dan I., Soya H. A transferable high-intensity intermittent exercise improves executive performance in association with dorsolateral prefrontal activation in young adults. Neuroimage. 2018;169:117–125. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.12.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Kujach S., Olek R.A., Byun K., Suwabe K., Sitek E.J., Ziemann E., Laskowski R., Soya H. Acute sprint interval exercise increases both cognitive functions and peripheral neurotrophic factors in humans: The possible involvement of lactate. Front. Neurosci. 2020;13:1455. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2019.01455. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Tsukamoto H., Suga T., Takenaka S., Tanaka D., Takeuchi T., Hamaoka T., Isaka T., Hashimoto T. Greater impact of acute high-intensity interval exercise on post-exercise executive function compared to moderate-intensity continuous exercise. Physiol. Behav. 2016;155:224–230. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.12.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Chang H., Kim K., Jung Y.J., Kato M. Effects of acute high-Intensity resistance exercise on cognitive function and oxygenation in prefrontal cortex. J. Exerc. Nutr. Biochem. 2017;21:1–8. doi: 10.20463/jenb.2017.0012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Solianik R., Bružas V., Mockus P., Vadopalas K., Streckis V. Acute effects of high-intensity interval training on cognition and retinal microcirculation in experienced amateur boxers. J. Sports Med. Phys. Fit. 2020 doi: 10.23736/s0022-4707.20.11352-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Ligeza T.S., Maciejczyk M., Kałamała P., Szygula Z., Wyczesany M. Moderate-intensity exercise boosts the N2 neural inhibition marker: A randomized and counterbalanced ERP study with precisely controlled exercise intensity. Biol. Psychol. 2018;135:170–179. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2018.04.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Gmiat A., Micielska K., Kozłowska M., Flis D.J., Smaruj M., Kujach S., Jaworska J., Lipińska P., Ziemann E. The impact of a single bout of high intensity circuit training on myokines’ concentrations and cognitive functions in women of different age. Physiol. Behav. 2017;179:290–297. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2017.07.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Martínez-Díaz I.C., Escobar-Muñoz M.C., Carrasco L. Acute effects of high-intensity interval training on brain-derived neurotrophic factor, cortisol and working memory in physical education college students. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2020;17:8216. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17218216. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Ludyga S., Pühse U., Lucchi S., Marti J., Gerber M. Immediate and sustained effects of intermittent exercise on inhibitory control and task-related heart rate variability in adolescents. J. Sci. Med. Sport. 2019;22:96–100. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2018.05.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Kao S.C., Drollette E.S., Ritondale J.P., Khan N., Hillman C.H. The acute effects of high-intensity interval training and moderate-intensity continuous exercise on declarative memory and inhibitory control. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 2018;38:90–99. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2018.05.011. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Kao S.C., Westfall D.R., Soneson J., Gurd B., Hillman C.H. Comparison of the acute effects of high-intensity interval training and continuous aerobic walking on inhibitory control. Psychophysiology. 2017;54:1335–1345. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12889. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Miller M.G., Hanson N., Tennyck J., Plantz K. A comparison of high-intensity interval training (HIIT) volumes on cognitive performance. J. Cogn. Enhanc. 2019;3:168–173. doi: 10.1007/s41465-018-0107-y. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Slusher A.L., Patterson V.T., Schwartz C.S., Acevedo E.O. Impact of high intensity interval exercise on executive function and brain derived neurotrophic factor in healthy college aged males. Physiol. Behav. 2018;191:116–122. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2018.04.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Cooper S.B., Bandelow S., Nute M.L., Dring K.J., Stannard R.L., Morris J.G., Nevill M.E. Sprint-based exercise and cognitive function in adolescents. Prev. Med. Rep. 2016;4:155–161. doi: 10.1016/j.pmedr.2016.06.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Lambrick D., Stoner L., Grigg R., Faulkner J. Effects of continuous and intermittent exercise on executive function in children aged 8–10 years. Psychophysiology. 2016;53:1335–1342. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12688. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.McMorris T. Exercise-cognition Interaction: Neuroscience Perspectives. Elsevier Academic Press; Amsterdam, The Netherlands: 2016. History of research into the acute exercise–cognition interaction: A cognitive psychology approach. [Google Scholar]
- 65.McMorris T., Tallon M., Williams C., Sproule J., Draper S., Swain J., Potter J., Clayton N. Incremental exercise, plasma concentrations of catecholamines, reaction time, and motor time during performance of a noncompatible choice response time task. Percept. Mot. Skills. 2003;97:590–604. doi: 10.2466/pms.2003.97.2.590. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Yang J.L., Lin Y.T., Chuang P.C., Bohr V.A., Mattson M.P. BDNF and exercise enhance neuronal DNA repair by stimulating CREB-mediated production of apurinic/apyrimidinic endonuclease 1. Neuromol. Med. 2014;16:161–174. doi: 10.1007/s12017-013-8270-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Saucedo Marquez C.M., Vanaudenaerde B., Troosters T., Wenderoth N. High-intensity interval training evokes larger serum BDNF levels compared with intense continuous exercise. J. Appl. Physiol. 2015;119:1363–1373. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00126.2015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Oberste M., Javelle F., Sharma S., Joisten N., Walzik D., Bloch W., Zimmer P. Effects and moderators of acute aerobic exercise on subsequent interference control: A systematic review and meta-analysis running title: Acute exercise and interference control. Front. Psychol. 2019;10:2616. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02616. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Van der Linden M., Brédart S., Beerten A. Age-related differences in updating working memory. Br. J. Psychol. 1994;85:145–152. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1994.tb02514.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Eldreth D.A., Patterson M.D., Porcelli A.J., Biswal B.B., Rebbechi D., Rypma B. Evidence for multiple manipulation processes in prefrontal cortex. Brain Res. 2006;1123:145–156. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.07.129. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Friederich H.C., Herzog W. Behavioral Neurobiology of Eating Disorders. Springer; Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany: 2010. Cognitive-behavioral flexibility in anorexia nervosa; pp. 111–123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Chang Y.K., Chu C.H., Wang C.C., Wang Y.C., Song T.F., Tsai C.L., Etnier J.L. Dose-response relation between exercise duration and cognition. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2015;47:159–165. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0000000000000383. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Chen F.T., Etnier J.L., Wu C.H., Cho Y.M., Hung T.M., Chang Y.K. Dose-response relationship between exercise duration and executive function in older adults. J. Clin. Med. 2018;7:279. doi: 10.3390/jcm7090279. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Yerkes R.M., Dodson J.D. The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit-formation. J. Comp. Neur. 1908;18:459–482. doi: 10.1002/cne.920180503. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Dietrich A., Audiffren M. The reticular-activating hypofrontality (RAH) model of acute exercise. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2011;35:1305–1325. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.02.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Calverley T.A., Ogoh S., Marley C.J., Steggall M., Marchi N., Brassard P., Lucas S.J.E., Cotter J.D., Roig M., Ainslie P.N. HIITing the brain with exercise: Mechanisms, consequences and practical recommendations. J. Physiol. 2020;598:2513–2530. doi: 10.1113/JP275021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Charness G., Gneezy U., Kuhn M.A. Experimental methods: Between-subject and within-subject design. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 2012;81:1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jebo.2011.08.009. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Bishop N.A., Lu T., Yankner B.A. Neural mechanisms of ageing and cognitive decline. Nature. 2010;464:529–535. doi: 10.1038/nature08983. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Martland R., Mondelli V., Gaughran F., Stubbs B. Can high-intensity interval training improve physical and mental health outcomes? A meta-review of 33 systematic reviews across the lifespan. J. Sports Sci. 2020;38:430–469. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2019.1706829. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
All datasets generated for this research are included in this published article.