Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2021 Apr 1;10:e67267. doi: 10.7554/eLife.67267

IGF-1 facilitates extinction of conditioned fear

Laura E Maglio 1,2,†,, José A Noriega-Prieto 1,3,, Irene B Maroto 1,4, Jesús Martin-Cortecero 1,5, Antonio Muñoz-Callejas 1, Marta Callejo-Móstoles 1, David Fernández de Sevilla 1,
Editors: Inna Slutsky6, John R Huguenard7
PMCID: PMC8043742  PMID: 33792539

Abstract

Insulin-like growth factor-1 (IGF-1) plays a key role in synaptic plasticity, spatial learning, and anxiety-like behavioral processes. While IGF-1 regulates neuronal firing and synaptic transmission in many areas of the central nervous system, its signaling and consequences on excitability, synaptic plasticity, and animal behavior dependent on the prefrontal cortex remain unexplored. Here, we show that IGF-1 induces a long-lasting depression of the medium and slow post-spike afterhyperpolarization (mAHP and sAHP), increasing the excitability of layer 5 pyramidal neurons of the rat infralimbic cortex. Besides, IGF-1 mediates a presynaptic long-term depression of both inhibitory and excitatory synaptic transmission in these neurons. The net effect of this IGF-1-mediated synaptic plasticity is a long-term potentiation of the postsynaptic potentials. Moreover, we demonstrate that IGF-1 favors the fear extinction memory. These results show novel functional consequences of IGF-1 signaling, revealing IGF-1 as a key element in the control of the fear extinction memory.

Research organism: Rat

Introduction

Conditioned fear is an associative form of learning and memory in which a previous neutral stimulus (called ‘conditioned stimulus’ [CS]; e.g., a tone) comes to elicit a fear response when is associated with an aversive stimulus (called ‘unconditioned stimulus’ [US]; e.g., an electric shock LeDoux, 2000; Maren, 2001; Pape and Pare, 2010). Extinction of conditioned fear is an active learning process involving inhibition of fear expression. It is a decline in conditioned fear responses (CRs) following non-reinforced exposure to the CS. However, fear extinction memory does not erase the initial association between the CS and US but forms a new association (CS–No US) that inhibits expression of the previous conditioned memory (Quirk and Mueller, 2008). Fear extinction depends on specific structures such as the amygdala, the hippocampus, and the prefrontal cortex (PFC) (Milad and Quirk, 2012; Orsini and Maren, 2012). Dysfunctions in the neuronal circuits responsible for fear cause the development of anxiety disorders, including specific phobias and post-traumatic stress (Pavlov, 1927; Quirk and Mueller, 2008).

The consolidation of the extinction memory has been related to long-term synaptic plasticity and increases in the excitability of pyramidal neurons (PNs) from the infralimbic cortex (IL) (Kaczorowski et al., 2012; Koppensteiner et al., 2019; Moyer et al., 1996). The mechanisms of this synaptic plasticity involve NMDA receptors (Burgos-Robles et al., 2007), mitogen-activated protein (MAP) kinases (Hugues et al., 2004), protein kinase A (Mueller et al., 2008), insertion of Ca2+-permeable AMPA receptors (Sepulveda-Orengo et al., 2013), and protein synthesis (Mueller et al., 2008; Santini et al., 2004). Moreover, the excitability of PN from IL is a key determinant for both the acquisition and the extinction of fear, being reduced by fear acquisition and increased by fear extinction (Santini et al., 2008). Indeed, IL layer 5 PNs (L5PNs) of conditioned fear animals show a higher slow post-spike afterhyperpolarization (sAHP) amplitude and lower firing frequency relative to non-conditioned or extinguished animals (Santini et al., 2008).

Insulin-like growth factor-1 (IGF-1) is an endogenous polypeptide with plenty of trophic functions, which can be locally synthesized and released by neurons and astrocytes (Fernandez and Torres-Alemán, 2012). Similarly, its receptor (IGF-1R) is widely expressed among all brain cell types (Quesada et al., 2007; Rodriguez-Perez et al., 2016). IGF-1 increases neuronal firing (Gazit et al., 2016; Nuñez et al., 2003) and modulates excitatory and inhibitory synaptic transmission in the central nervous system (Castro-Alamancos and Torres-Aleman, 1993; Nilsson et al., 1988; Noriega-prieto et al., 2020; Seto et al., 2002). Furthermore, IGF-1 regulates different ion channels, such as A-type K+ channels (Xing et al., 2006) and P/Q-, L-, and N-type voltage-gated Ca2+ channels (Blair and Marshall, 1997; Shan et al., 2003), as well as neurotransmitter receptors activity (Gonzalez de la Vega et al., 2001; Savchenko et al., 2001). However, the role of IGF-1 on the modulation of L5PN of the IL activity and its consequences in the fear extinction memory remain to be clarified.

Here we have examined the effects of IGF-1 in the excitability and synaptic transmission of L5PN from IL and in the extinction of conditioned fear. Our results reveal that IGF-1 induces a long-lasting reduction of the sAHP amplitude and increases the firing frequency of L5PNs. Furthermore, IGF-1 induces a presynaptic long-term depression (LTD) of both excitatory and inhibitory postsynaptic currents (EPSC and IPSCs, respectively) that results in a long-term potentiation (LTP) of the postsynaptic potentials (PSPs). Moreover, we show that IGF-1 facilitates the recall of extinction of fear conditioning when applied intracranially to the IL 30 min before the extinction task. In these animals with the favored extinction by IGF-1, the sAHP is reduced and the excitatory synaptic transmission is depressed. Therefore, we demonstrate for the first time that IGF-1 facilitates the extinction of fear conditioning, increasing the excitability and potentiating the synaptic transmission in L5PN from IL.

Results

IGF-1 increases the excitability of IL-L5PNs

Ca2+-activated K+ currents that mediate the medium and slow post-spike afterhyperpolarization (mAHP and sAHP, respectively) are crucial in the regulation of neuronal excitability (Alger and Nicoll, 1980; Madison and Nicoll, 1984). We first tested the effect of IGF-1 on different components of AHPs of IL L5PNs. In the current-clamp mode, we recorded mAHPs or sAHPs before and during bath application of IGF-1 (10 nM). IGF-1 reduced mAHP (Figure 1A,B) and sAHPs (Figure 1A,C), whereas the fast AHP (fAHP) was unaffected (Figure 1—figure supplement 1A,B). The reduction of mAHP and sAHP was abolished in the presence of the antagonist of the IGF-1 receptor, NVP-AEW 541 (Figure 1A–C). We also checked the effect on neuronal excitability by analyzing the number of action potentials (APs) evoked by increasing current injection steps. We observed that the number of APs evoked by this protocol was higher during IGF-1 perfusion (Figure 1E,F). Moreover, we compared the properties of AP before and after IGF-1 application. There were no significant differences in parameters, such as AP amplitude, AP threshold, AP half-width, AP time to max rise slope, AP max rise slope, AP time to max decay slope, and AP max decay slope (Figure 1—figure supplement 2A–G). However, the input resistance was significantly augmented (Figure 1—figure supplement 2H), suggesting a reduction of ionic conductance. We next performed voltage-clamp recordings to analyze the effect of IGF-1 on the currents that underlie the mAHP and sAHP (Alger and Nicoll, 1980; Storm, 1990) (mIAHP and sIAHP, respectively). IGF-1 decreased mIAHP (Figure 2A–C) and sIAHP (Figure 2A,D,E), while the fast afterhyperpolarization current remained unaltered (Figure 1—figure supplement 1C,D). We observed a gradual current reduction that reached a plateau 20 min after IGF-1 application in both the mIAHP and sIAHP (Figure 2C,E). However, in the absence of IGF-1, the neuronal depolarization did not modify the AHP currents (Figure 2—figure supplement 1A). Interestingly, we observed a long-lasting reduction of mIAHP and sIAHP that remained after the wash-out of IGF-1 (Figure 2—figure supplement 1B,C) and required the activation of the AHPs by the depolarizing protocol (Figure 2—figure supplement 1B,C). Moreover, the IGF-1-dependent modulation of the mIAHP and sIAHP was prevented by the NVP-AEW541 (400 nM, Figure 2A–E), indicating that these effects were mediated by the activation of the IGF-1R.

Figure 1. IGF-1 increases the excitability of IL-L5PNs.

(A) Representative recordings from IL-L5PNs of hyperpolarizing potentials elicited by an 800 ms depolarizing pulse to study medium and slow AHP in control conditions (ACSF, top) and in the presence of NVP-AEW541 (40 nM, bottom), before (black) and during IGF-1 (10 nM, red) (spikes are truncated). The mAHP was measured at the hyperpolarization peak and the gray box indicates where the sAHP was measured. (B) Bar diagram summarizing mAHP amplitudes (n = 9 cells/8 animals; ACSF vs IGF-1 **p<0.01 and ns (non-significant) n = 6 cells/3 animals NVP vs NVP +IGF-1, Student's paired t-test). Mann–Whitney test, n = 9/6 cells ACSF vs NVP, ns. (C) Bar diagram summarizing sAHP amplitudes (n = 9 cells/8 animals; ACSF vs IGF-1 **p<0.01 and ns n = 6 cells/3 animals NVP vs NVP +IGF-1, Student's paired t-test). Mann–Whitney test, n = 9/6 cells ACSF vs NVP, ns. (D). Representative traces recorded from IL-L5PNs after 100 pA current injection in ACSF (gray) and after IGF-1 application (red). (E) Plot showing the frequency of APs as a function of the injected current (pA) for IL-L5PNs in ACSF (gray) and after IGF-1 application (red) (n = 10 cells/8 animals ACSF vs IGF-1 *p<0.05, **p<0.01 and ns, Multiple t-tests with post hoc Holm–Sildak multiple comparison methods). See also Figure 1—figure supplement 1 and Figure 1—figure supplement 2.

Figure 1—source data 1. Source data for Figure 1: IGF-1 increases the excitability of IL-L5PNs.

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. IGF-1 has no effect on fAHP and fIAHP.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

(A) Representative recordings of hyperpolarizing potentials elicited by a 10 ms depolarizing pulse applied to study the fast components of the AHP, in control conditions (ACSF, left) and in the presence of NVP-AEW541 (right), before (black) and during IGF-1 (red) (spikes are truncated). (B) Bar diagram summarizing the lack of effect of IGF-1 on the fAHP (n = 7 cells/5 animals; ACSF vs IGF-1, ns (non-significant) and n = 5 cells/3 animals NVP vs NVP +IGF-1, ns, Student's paired t-test). Mann–Whitney test, n = 7/5 cells ACSF vs NVP, ns. (C) Representative current traces recorded in response to a 10 ms depolarizing pulse from −60 mV to 0 mV in control condition (ACSF, left) and in the presence of (40 nM) NVP-AEW541 (right), before (black) and during IGF-1 (red). The fAHP was measured at the hyperpolarization peak. (D) Bar diagram summarizing the lack of effect of IGF-1 on the current amplitude of fIAHPn = 10 cells/8 animals; ACSF vs IGF-1, ns (non-significant) and n = 6 cells/3 animals NVP vs NVP +IGF-1, ns, Student's paired t-test. Mann–Whitney test, n = 10/6 cells ACSF vs NVP, ns.
Figure 1—figure supplement 1—source data 1. Source data for Figure 1—figure supplement 1.
IGF-1 has no effect on fAHP and fIAHP.
Figure 1—figure supplement 2. Action potential (AP) properties are unaltered by IGF-1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 2.

(A–G) AP amplitude; threshold, half-width, time to rise slope, rise slope, time to decay slope, and decay slope in ACSF and during IGF-1 (n = 9 cells/7 animals) ACSF vs IGF-1 ns Student's paired t-test. (H) Input resistance n = 12 cells/8 animals, ACSF vs IGF-1 ***p<0.001 Student's paired t-test.
Figure 1—figure supplement 2—source data 1. Source data for Figure 1—figure supplement 1: Action potential properties are unaltered by IGF-1.

Figure 2. IGF-1 reduces mIAHP and sIAHP in IL-L5PNs.

(A) Representative current traces recorded from IL-L5PNs in response to an 800 ms depolarizing voltage pulse from −60 mV to 0 mV in the control condition (ACSF, top) and the presence of NVP-AEW541 (40 nM, bottom), before (black), and during 10 nM IGF-1 (red). (B) Bar diagram summarizing the normalized amplitude of mIAHP (n = 9 cells/5 animals; ACSF vs IGF-1 **p<0.01 and ns, non-significant, n = 7 cells/7 animals NVP vs NVP + IGF-1, Student's paired t-test). Mann–Whitney test, n = 9/7 cells ACSF vs NVP, ns. (C) Plot showing the time course of the peak amplitude of the mIAHP in ACSF (red) and in NVP-AEW541 before and during IGF-1 (n = 9 cells/7 animals; ACSF vs IGF-1 ***p<0.0001 and ns n = 7 cells/7 animals NVP vs NVP + IGF-1, Student's paired t-test). Mann–Whitney test, n = 9/7 cells IGF-1 vs NVP + IGF-1, ns. (D and E) Same as B and C respectively but for the sIAHP. (F) Same as A, but in the presence of GDPβs in the patch pipette. Note that only the sIAHP is modulated by IGF-1 under blockade of the G proteins. (G) Bar diagram summarizing the normalized amplitude of mIAHP (left) and normalized area of sIAHP (right) under GDPβs (n = 8 cells/4 animals; mIAHP ACSF vs IGF-1 ns, non-significant and sIAHP, n = 8 cells/4 animals ACSF vs IGF-1 *p<0.05, Student's paired t-test). See also Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

Figure 2—source data 1. Source data for Figure 2.
IGF-1 reduces mIAHP and sIAHP in IL-L5PNs.

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. Time course of mIAHP and sIAHP.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

(A) Plot showing the time course of mIAHP (left) and sIAHP (right) in control conditions (ACSF) after applying 800 ms depolarizing pulses, respectively (n = 4 in each group). Note that the IAHPs are not modulated by the protocol used for their induction in the absence of IGF-1. (B) Plot showing the time course of mIAHP and the persistent effect of IGF-1 after washout; start at 0 min; n = 7 cells/6 animals, ACSF vs IGF-1 **p<0.01 Student'´s paired t-test; and start at 20 min after IGF-1 infusion n = 6 cells/4 animals, ACSF vs IGF-1 **p<0.01 Student's paired t-test. (C) Same as B but for the sIAHP. Note the LTD of IAHP was similar in both groups.
Figure 2—figure supplement 1—source data 1. Source data for Figure 2—figure supplement 1.
Time course of mIAHP and sIAHP.

We perform experiments to block the G protein-dependent modulation of the AHPs induced by metabotropic receptor activation. We used a patch pipette filled with the non-hydrolyzable GDP analogue GDPβs (1 mM) to block the G protein activation. Interestingly, we found that in the presence of GDPβs IGF-1 only reduced the sIAHP (Figure 2F,G). It is noteworthy that although both AHPs were similarly reduced by IGF-1 in the same cells (32.8% for mIAHP and 39.5% for sIAHP) the G protein independency of the mIAHP modulation suggests that different mechanisms may be involved in the modulation of both AHPs. Taken together, these results demonstrate that IGF-1 induced a long-term decrease of mIAHP and sIAHP leading to an increase in the frequency of neuronal firing.

IGF-1 induces LTP of the synaptic transmission

Next, we examined whether IGF-1 modulates the excitatory synaptic transmission. After isolating the EPSCs (see Materials and methods), we measured their amplitude and analyzed the paired-pulse ratio (PPR) and the coefficient of variance. IGF-1 induced a LTD of EPSC peak amplitude (72.63% of baseline, Figure 3A) that was prevented in the presence of NVP-AEW541 (97.72% of baseline, Figure 3A). The IGF-1-mediated depression of the EPSCs was associated with changes in the PPR (Figure 3B) and the coefficient of variation (1/CV2) (Figure 3C), indicating a presynaptic mechanism. Additionally, this modulation was observed when the synaptic stimulation was absent (55.23% of baseline, Figure 3D), suggesting that the evoked synaptic responses were required for this LTD of the EPSCs.

Figure 3. IGF-1 induces long-term potentiation of the synaptic transmission.

(A) Plot showing the time course of EPSCs when the IGF-1 was bath applied for 35 min after a stable (~5 min) baseline, (n = 5 cells/3 animals, ACSF vs IGF-1 ***p<0.001 and ns, n = 5 cells/3 animals NVP vs NVP +IGF-1 Student's paired t-test). Mann–Whitney test, n = 5/5 cells IGF-1 vs NVP +IGF-1, **p<0.01. (B) (top) Superimposed representative paired-pulse EPSCs recorded before (black trace) and during IGF-1 (red trace). (bottom) Bar diagram summarizing the EPSC paired-pulse ratio (PPR) before (black bar) and during IGF-1 (red bar). Student's paired t-test **p<0.01; (n = 5, same as A). (C) (top) Representative EPSCs traces before and during IGF-1. (bottom) Plot of the variance (1/CV2r) as a function of the mean EPSC peak amplitude (M) 30 min after IGF-1 and normalized to control condition (ACSF) (n = 5, same as A). (D) Plot showing the time course of EPSCs when IGF-1 is applied in the absence of synaptic stimulus for 15 min. (n = 5 cells/3 animals, ACSF vs IGF-1 ***p<0.001 Student's paired t-test). (E–H) Same as A–D, respectively, but for the IPSCs. (I) Plot showing the time course of PSP amplitude when, after a stable (~5 min) baseline, IGF-1 was bath applied for 35 min. Student's paired t-test ***p<0.001 (n = 6). (J) Representative PSPs recordings before (black trace) and during IGF-1 (red trace). (K) Bar diagrams summarizing the effect of IGF-1 on the PSP amplitude, PSP area, and the TAU ON and OFF (n = 6 cells/4 animals, ACSF vs IGF-1 *p<0.05 Student's paired t-test). Note that only the amplitude and area of the PSPs are modulated by IGF-1. (L) Representative current-clamp responses recorded before (black trace) and during IGF-1 (red trace). (M) Bar diagram summarizing the number of action potentials (APs) in ACSF and at 5, 10, and 15 min during IGF-1 (n = 6 cells/4 animals, ACSF vs IGF-1 *p<0.05, ***p<0.001 one-way ANOVA with post hoc Dunnett's multiple comparison test). (N) Bar diagram summarizing the lack of effect of IGF-1 in the AP amplitude, the AP half-width, AP time to rise slope, AP rise slope, AP time to decay slope, and AP decay slope (n = 6 cells/4 animals, ACSF vs IGF-1 Student's paired t-test). See also Figure 3—figure supplement 1 and Figure 3—figure supplement 2.

Figure 3—source data 1. Source data for Figure 3.
IGF-1 induces long-term potentiation of the synaptic transmission.

Figure 3.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1. The protocol of stimulation does not induce plasticity.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1.

(A) Plot showing a long-lasting stable recording of the PSPs with the protocol used in Figure 3I but in the absence of IGF-1 ACSF (−5–0 min) vs ACSF (45–50 min), ns (not-significant) Student's paired t-test n = 4 cells/2 animals. (B) Bar diagram summarizing the lack of effect of the protocol of recording the PSPs in the number of action potentials recorded at different time points (0, 5, 10, and 15 min). One-way ANOVA with post hoc Dunnett's multiple comparison test, n = 4 cells/2 animals ns (not-significant).
Figure 3—figure supplement 1—source data 1. Source data for Figure 3—figure supplement 1.
The protocol of stimulation does not induce plasticity.
Figure 3—figure supplement 2. The application of IGF-1 is sufficient to induce plasticity.

Figure 3—figure supplement 2.

(A) Plot showing the time course of PSPs amplitude when, after a stable (~5 min) baseline, IGF-1 was bath applied for 10 min. Student's paired t-test ***p>0.001 (n = 6 cells/2 animals). (B) Bar diagram summarizing the number of action potentials in ACSF and at 5 and 10 min during IGF-1 (n = 6 cells/2 animals) one-way ANOVA with post hoc Dunnett's multiple comparison tests *p<0.05.
Figure 3—figure supplement 2—source data 1. Source data for Figure 3—figure supplement 2.
The application of IGF-1 is sufficient to induce plasticity.

We also analyzed whether IGF-1 regulates inhibitory synaptic transmission. IGF-1 induced a LTD of IPSCs (72.43% of baseline, Figure 3E) that were dependent on IGF-1R activation since this effect was prevented by NVP-AEW541 (99.0% of baseline, Figure 3E). The IGF-1-mediated depression of the IPSCs was associated with changes in the PPR (Figure 3F) and coefficient of variation (1/CV2) (Figure 3G), pointing to a presynaptic mechanism. Nevertheless, the LTD of the IPSC did not require the evoked IPSCs since IGF-1 was able to induce it when the synaptic stimulation was absent (49.76% of baseline, Figure 3H). Interestingly, the IGF-1-dependent depression of EPSCs (Figure 3A,D) and IPSCs (Figure 3E,H) was larger in the absence of synaptic stimulation, suggesting that two different mechanisms may be responsible for the LTD. Finally, we studied the effect of IGF-1 on the PSPs. After recording a stable baseline of PSPs, we increased the intensity of stimulus until that ≈21% of the responses recorded during 5 min were suprathreshold and then we added IGF-1 (Figure 3I,J). IGF-1 induced a significant increase in the number of AP 10 and 15 min after its application (Figure 3M), whereas the properties of these AP were not modified (Figure 3L,N). After returning to the basal intensity of stimulation, a LTP of the PSPs was observed (Figure 3I). This PSP potentiation was not present when using the same protocol but in the absence of IGF-1 (Figure 3—figure supplement 1). According to the observed potentiation, IGF-1 application increased PSP amplitude, without modifying the PSP kinetics (Figure 3J,K). It is worth mentioning that a shorter period of IGF-1 application (10 min) was enough to induce an LTP of PSPs, which is maintained even after washing IGF-1 (Figure 3—figure supplement 2). Together, our results reveal that IGF-1 induces a presynaptic depression of both EPSCs and IPSCs which triggers an LTP of PSPs.

IGF-1 facilitates fear extinction

Since a reduction of the sAHP favors the recall of extinction (Santini et al., 2008), we next analyzed whether IGF-1 was able to improve it. First, we performed a set of experiments to determine the number of sessions required to induced fear conditioning and extinction as previously described (Fontanez-Nuin et al., 2011Figure 4—figure supplement 1). On day 2, the different groups of rats acquired similar levels of conditioned freezing (COND, 74%; EXT, 77%) with three sessions of association (CS-US, fear conditioning). On day 3, rats from the EXT group showed gradual within-session extinction across 20 trials to a final freezing level of 12% (CS-No US), while rats from the COND group remained in their home cage. On day 4, rats from COND group showed high levels of freezing (77–95%) on the test tones, whereas rats from EXT group showed low levels of freezing (50–35%), indicating good recall of extinction during five sessions (Figure 4—figure supplement 1A,B).

To study the effect of IGF-1 on fear extinction memory, we infused saline or IGF-1 or NVP + IGF-1 through a cannula guide previously implanted into the IL (Figure 4A) 30 min before the extinction training (Figure 4B). Although the level of freezing reached on day 2 was comparable in the three groups (saline, 89%; IGF-1, 79%; NVP + IGF-1 81%), the rats from the IGF-1 group showed less freezing in the last session on day 3 (saline, 51%; IGF-1, 14%; NVP + IGF-1, 59%), indicating that IGF-1 treatment favors the acquisition of extinction. On day 4, IGF-1-treated rats exhibited less freezing (37–19%) than the saline (57–41%) and NVP + IGF-1 (63–59%) rats (Figure 4B), unveiling that IGF-1 enhanced the performance of animals to recall of extinction.

Figure 4. IGF-1 facilitates the extinction memory by reducing mIAHP and sIAHP.

(A) (top) Image showing the infralimbic cortex (IL) localization. (bottom) Nissl-stained coronal section of rat IL, the orange arrow indicates the tip of the cannula implanted on the IL. (B) Plot showing the time course of the percentage of freezing during the extinction protocol for the three groups studied (saline, IGF-1, and NVP-+IGF-1). Saline (n = 11), IGF-1 (n = 14), and NVP-AEW541 +IGF-1 (n = 12) were directly applied into the IL through the implanted cannula 30 min before extinction training (day 3). The black arrow indicates the time of infusion. (*p<0.05 **p<0.01 saline vs IGF-1 and NPV + IGF-1 #p<0.05; ##p<0.01; ###p<0.001 IGF-1 vs NPV + IGF-1; One-way ANOVA with post hoc Tukey's multiple comparisons test). (C) Representative mIAHP and sIAHP current traces recorded in three groups of animals studied that showed fear extinction (D) Bar diagrams summarizing the mIAHP for all the groups studied. Recordings were performed in day 4 (test) after finishing the extinction protocol; saline (n = 11 cells/3 animals) vs IGF-1 (n = 7 cells/4 animals) *p<0.05; IGF-1 vs NPV + IGF-1 (n = 9 cells/3 animals) **p<0.01 and saline (n = 11 cells/3 animals) vs NPV + IGF-1 (n = 9 cells/3 animals) ns, one-way ANOVA with post hoc Tukey's multiple comparisons test. (E) Bar diagrams summarizing the sIAHP for all the groups studied. Saline (n = 11 cells/3 animals) vs IGF-1 (n = 7 cells/4 animals) *p<0.05; IGF-1 vs NPV + IGF-1 (n = 9 cells/3 animals) **p<0.01 and saline (n = 11 cells/3 animals) vs NPV + IGF-1 (n = 9 cells/3 animals) ns, one-way ANOVA with post hoc Tukey's multiple comparisons test. (F) For each rat in the extinction groups, the mIAHP and sIAHP for all cells were averaged. Across rats, these values were significantly correlated with freezing levels at test (r = 0.763 for mIAHP and r = 0.787 for sIAHP pointing to that the reduction in both currents is correlated with the extinction recall). (G) Representative traces recorded from IL-L5PNs from animals that showed fear extinction after 200 pA (left) current injection in saline (black); IGF-1 (red) and NVP + IGF-1 (gray). Frequency-injected current relationships for IL-L5PNs in saline (black, n = 10 cells/5 animals) IGF-1 (red, n = 7 cells/3 animals) and NVP + IGF-1 (gray, n = 6 cells/3 animals). (*) IGF-1 vs saline and (#) IGF-1 vs. NVP + IGF-1; one-way ANOVA with post hoc Tukey's multiple comparisons test. See also Figure 4—figure supplement 1 and Figure 4—figure supplement 2.

Figure 4—source data 1. Source data for Figure 4.
IGF-1 facilitates the extinction memory by reducing mIAHP and sIAHP.

Figure 4.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1. Protocol of behavior.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1.

(A) The experimental design of the conditioned fear experiments. (B) Plot showing the time course of the percentage of freezing during the experiment for the two groups studied: conditioned (COND, black circles; n = 7) and extinction (EXT, white circles; n = 11) groups. As expected, animals in the COND group showed higher levels of freezing to the test tones compared with the EXT groups (***p<0.001; **p<0.01; one-way ANOVA with post hoc Tukey's multiple comparisons test).
Figure 4—figure supplement 1—source data 1. Source data for Figure 4—figure supplement 1.
Protocol of behavior.
Figure 4—figure supplement 2. AHPs of the L5PN of the IL recorded from animals after fear extinction.

Figure 4—figure supplement 2.

(A) Bar diagram summarizing fAHP from rats NAÏVE (n = 10 cells/8 animals) and after test behaviors in different groups: EXT (n = 6 cells/2 animals); Ext-Saline (n = 10 cells/3 animals); Ext-IGF-1 (n = 7 cells/3 animals); Ext-NVP +IGF-1 (n = 8 cells/2 animals) One-way ANOVA with post hoc Turkey's multiple comparisons test *p<0.05; **p<0.01. (B) Bar diagram summarizing mAHP from rats NAÏVE (n = 16 cells/10 animals) and after test behaviors in different groups EXT (n = 6 cells/2 animals); Ext-Saline (n = 13 cells/5 animals); Ext-IGF-1 (n = 6 cells/3 animals); Ext-NVP +IGF-1 (n = 11 cells/3 animals) One-way ANOVA with post hoc Turkey's multiple comparisons test *p<0.05; **p<0.01. (C) Bar diagram summarizing sAHP from rats NAÏVE (n = 12 cells/6 animals) and after test behaviors in different groups EXT (n = 6 cells/2 animals); Ext-Saline (n = 11 cells/4 animals); Ext-IGF-1 (n = 6 cells/3 animals); Ext-NVP +IGF-1 (n = 11 cells/3 animals); one-way ANOVA with post hoc Turkey's multiple comparisons test *p<0.05. See also Supplementary file 1.
Figure 4—figure supplement 2—source data 1. Source data for Figure 4—figure supplement 2.
AHPs of the L5PN of the IL recorded from animals after fear extinction.

IGF-1 modulates excitability and synaptic transmission in rats exposed to the extinction memory task

Fear extinction is paralleled by an increase in the excitability of PNs from layer 5 from IL (Santini et al., 2008). Therefore, we tested whether a change in the excitability of IL-L5PNs occurs in the animals in which IGF-1 facilitated the extinction memory. For this purpose, we recorded IL-L5PNs from all behavioral groups after the last extinction session and we observed that the mIAHP and sIAHP of the IGF-1 group were smaller than the mIAHP and sIAHP of the saline group (Figure 4C–E). Importantly, these differences in the mIAHP and sIAHP were not present when comparing the saline group with the group treated with NVP + IGF-1 (Figure 4C–E). Also, we calculated the correlation between the mIAHP and sIAHP amplitudes and freezing levels in all groups. Interestingly, both current amplitude correlated with the freezing (Figure 4F), indicating that the reduction in the currents mediated by IGF-1 facilitates the extinction recall. It is noteworthy that all rat groups that underwent the extinction, except for the Ext-NVP +IGF-1 group, showed a reduced fAHP and mAHP when compared with NAÏVE group (Figure 4—figure supplement 2A,B), which is in agreement with previous results (Santini et al., 2008). However, we only observed a significant reduction in sAHP from Ext-IGF-1 group compared NAÏVE group, suggesting that in this group of animals the reduction of the sAHP is essential for the better level of extinction recall (Figure 4—figure supplement 2C). Moreover, IGF-1-treated group showed an increased AP firing frequency of IL-L5PNs when compared with saline or NVP + IGF-1 groups. These results support that IGF-1 induces the facilitation of extinction through the reduction of mIAHP and sIAHP thus increasing the IL-L5PNs firing frequency (Figure 4G). Finally, we also tested whether the excitatory synaptic transmission was depressed by IGF-1 in animals in which IGF-1 facilitated the extinction. For this purpose, we recorded miniature EPSCs (mEPSCs) in IL-L5PNs from all behavioral groups after the last extinction session on day 4. We observed that the mEPSCs frequency of the IGF-1 group was lower than in saline and NVP + IGF-1 groups (Figure 5A–C), with no changes in the mEPSCs amplitude (Figure 5A,D,E) suggesting a presynaptic modulation of synaptic transmission by IGF-1. Taken together, these results demonstrate that IGF-1 facilitates the establishment of fear extinction memory, generating plasticity of synaptic transmission and neuronal excitability at IL-L5PNs.

Figure 5. IGF-1 decreases the frequency of mEPSC.

Figure 5.

(A) Representative traces recorded at −60 mV in IL-L5PNs from animals that showed fear extinction, in the presence of 1 µM TTX, 50 µM PiTX, and 5 μM CPG-55845. Asterisks denote mEPSC events. Note the decreased mEPSCs frequency induced by IGF-1. (B) Bar diagram of the summary data showing mean mEPSCs frequency from Ext-Saline (black, n = 5 cells/2 animals); Ext-IGF-1 (red, n = 6/3 animals); and Ext-NVP +IGF-1 (gray, n = 6/3 animals). One-way ANOVA with post hoc Tukey's multiple comparisons test. (C) Cumulative probability plots of mean inter-mEPSC interval in Ext-Saline (black); Ext-IGF-1 (red); and Ext-NVP +IGF-1 (gray) (same cells as in B). Note that IGF-1 increased the mean inter-mEPSC interval (p<0.05; Kolmogorov–Smirnov test). (D) Bar diagram of the summary data showing mean mEPSCs amplitude (same cells as in B). (E) Cumulative probability plots of mean amplitude-mEPSC in Ext-Saline (black); Ext-IGF-1 (red); and Ext-NVP +IGF-1 (gray). Note that IGF-1 does not change the mean mEPSCs amplitude (same cells as in B).

Figure 5—source data 1. Source data for Figure 5: IGF-1 decreases the frequency of mEPSC.

Discussion

Interactions among the amygdala, the hippocampus, and the IL PFC are important for the extinction of conditioned fear memory (Milad and Quirk, 2012; Orsini and Maren, 2012; Pape and Pare, 2010; Quirk and Mueller, 2008). Although the effects of IGF-1 on the amygdala (Stern et al., 2014) and the hippocampus (Chen et al., 2011; Stern et al., 2014) have been previously studied, there is no evidence about its possible actions on the IL. Our results demonstrate for the first time that IGF-1 applied to IL favors the extinction of conditioned fear memory causing an increase in L5PN excitability and synaptic plasticity through the activation of the IGF-1R. We present new evidence showing that IGF-1 induced a long-lasting increase in excitability and LTP of the synaptic potentials at IL-L5PNs. The former is mediated by a significant reduction in the mAHP and sAHP, whereas the latter results from the interaction between the presynaptic LTD of the EPSCs and IPSCs. Therefore, our results show a novel functional consequence of IGF-1 signaling on animal behavior in the mPFC through the modulation of the extinction of conditioned fear memory.

IGF-1-induces AHPs reduction leading to enhancement of excitability

The enhancement of excitability is manifested as a reduced spike frequency adaptation in response to prolonged depolarizing current applications, which results from post-burst AHP reduction (Moyer et al., 1996; Sepulveda-Orengo et al., 2013) modulation of intrinsic excitability is linked with the mechanism of consolidation of learning (Sehgal et al., 2014). Specifically, in IL, a reduction of excitability maintains the fear learning (Criado-Marrero et al., 2014) whereas an increase of the excitability regulated the fear extinction learning (Sepulveda-Orengo et al., 2013; Bloodgood et al., 2018). Interestingly, our recordings from IL-L5PNs of animals treated with IGF-1 showed a long-lasting decrease of mAHP and sAHP currents, which is highly related to behavioral recall (Moyer et al., 1996; Santini et al., 2008), suggesting that IGF-1 also modulates the channels responsible for these currents in IL-L5PNs. A previous study demonstrated that the enhancement of the bursting of these neurons after the acquisition of fear extinction is mediated by a reduction of fIAHP due to the blockade of the M-type K+ current (Santini and Porter, 2010). The reduction in the amplitude and the duration of AHPs from IL-L5PNs caused by IGF-1 resulted in a significant increase in their excitability, as reflected in the increase of their firing frequency. In the present work, we demonstrate that IGF-1 does not modify the fIAHP, suggesting that the effects of IGF-1 on IL-L5PN firing frequency and recall of extinction are not mediated by the modulation of the M-current. Moreover, the AP characteristics are not affected by IGF-1, suggesting that the IGF-1-mediated increase in IL-L5PNs excitability is mainly produced by the decrease in the mAHP and sAHP currents. Although IGF-1 inhibits A-type K+ channels in somatosensory neurons in the dorsal column nuclei (Blair and Marshall, 1997; Nuñez et al., 2003; Shan et al., 2003; Xing et al., 2006) this modulation is absent in the IL.

The activation of metabotropic glutamate receptor 5 (mGluR5) produces a reduction of mIAHP and sIAHP, an effect that is prevented in the presence of a mGluR5 antagonist (Fontanez-Nuin et al., 2011; Young et al., 2008), pointing to mGluR5 as responsible for the modulation of these conductances. In our study, inhibition of G protein-coupled receptors by GDPβs abolished the IGF-1-mediated reduction of mIAHP, suggesting that the effect of IGF-1 on mAHP is mediated by the activation of metabotropic receptors. One candidate is the mGLuRs, since it has been demonstrated that the activation of mGluR5 increases IL-L5PN excitability (Fontanez-Nuin et al., 2011) and modulates the recall of extinction (Fontanez-Nuin et al., 2011; Sepulveda-Orengo et al., 2013) in a similar way to what we describe for IGF-1. However, we cannot rule out the involvement of other GPCRs and a further study is required to analyze the type of metabotropic receptor and the signaling pathway mediating the reduction of the mAHP by the IGF-1. On the other hand, we observed that IGF-1-mediated sAHP reduction is independent of the activation of G-protein-coupled receptors, which suggests that IGF-1 could be directly modulating the activity of some AHP-related ion channels, such as SK channels.

IGF-1-induces an LTP of the PSPs

The inhibitory transmission controls the operation of cortical circuits and the modulation of synaptic inhibition plays an important role in the induction of cortical plasticity. In response to glutamate, IGF-1 induces a long-lasting reduction in GABA release from cerebellar Purkinje cells, suggesting that IGF-1 is modulating the glutamatergic transmission in the rat olivo-cerebellar system (Castro-Alamancos and Torres-Aleman, 1993; Castro-Alamancos et al., 1996). IGF-1 was also shown to modulate GABAergic transmission in the olfactory bulb (Liu et al., 2017). However, this is the first demonstration of an IGF-1-mediated presynaptic long-lasting depression of fast GABAergic synaptic transmission in the PFC. Although IGF-1Rs may be present in the inhibitory GABAergic terminals, there are no clear data showing a direct action of IGF-1 acting on presynaptic IGF-1Rs. The only evidence suggesting this mode of action was seen in the hippocampus, where IGF-1 can induce the release of GABA to regulate endogenous ACh release, possibly acting via GABAergic neurons (Seto et al., 2002).

It has been demonstrated that excitatory synaptic transmission is also modulated by IGF-1 in many brain areas (Araujo et al., 1989; Castro-Alamancos and Torres-Aleman, 1993; Maya-Vetencourt et al., 2012; Nilsson et al., 1988; Seto et al., 2002). IGF-1 enhances glutamatergic synaptic transmission through a postsynaptic mechanism involving postsynaptic AMPA, but not NMDA, receptors in CA1 PNs from rat hippocampus (Molina et al., 2012; Ramsey et al., 2005). Additionally, IGF-1 increases the expression levels of hippocampal NMDA receptor subunits 2A and 2B in aged rats (Sonntag et al., 2000), which may facilitate LTP induction. However, here we show that IGF-1 decreases the efficacy of excitatory synaptic transmission by a presynaptic mechanism. Therefore, our observations contribute to the notion that IGF-1 can decrease the release of neurotransmitters both at excitatory and inhibitory synapses throughout the CNS. The net effect of this modulation by IGF-1 is a long-lasting potentiation of the PSPs indicating a greater impact of the reduction of inhibition compared with the reduction of excitation. Interestingly, we observed that two mechanisms coexist in the development of the LTD of EPSCs and IPSCs following IGF-1 application. The first one is dependent on synaptic activation and is characterized by a fast onset, the plateau being achieved at 10–15 min after IGF-1 application. The second one is independent of synaptic activation and is characterized by a slower onset, achieving the plateau at 40 min after IGF-1 application. However, the increase in the input resistance induced by IGF-1 could also explain the resultant LTP of the PSPs nonetheless of the reduction in the release of neurotransmitters. Since SK channels have been shown to modulate NMDA receptors (Jones et al., 2017; Ngo-Anh et al., 2005), we cannot discard a putative IGF-1-dependent modulation of SK channels as the source of the potentiation in the synaptic transmission. The resultant LTP of the PSPs and the increase in the input resistance summed to the decrease of the AHPs would result in the increase of the L5PN activity of the IL and its effect on the fear extinction memory.

IGF-1 facilitates fear extinction memory

There is a growing body of evidence supporting that fear extinction memory is encoded by IL neurons (Quirk and Beer, 2006) that show enhanced responses to extinguished cues during extinction recall (Milad and Quirk, 2002). Pharmacological (Hugues et al., 2004; Laurent and Westbrook, 2009; Sierra-Mercado et al., 2011; Sotres-Bayon et al., 2007), electrical (Milad et al., 2004), or optogenetic (Do-Monte et al., 2015) manipulations of IL have been described to modulate the acquisition of fear extinction. In addition, IL is involved in the recall of this memory since lesions of the IL produce deficits in its retention (Morgan and LeDoux, 1995; Quirk et al., 2000). In the present study, we infused IGF-1 into IL before the extinction protocol and found that the action of IGF-1 in these neurons induced a significant improvement of fear extinction memory compared to the groups treated with saline or NVP + IGF-1. Consistent with that fact, IGF-1 (Llorens-Martín et al., 2010; Trejo et al., 2008), and most recently also IGF-2 (Chen et al., 2011), have been related to cognitive function. Injections of IGF-1, IGF-2, or insulin into the amygdala did not affect memories engaging this region. However, bilateral injection of insulin into the dorsal hippocampus transiently enhances hippocampal-dependent memory, whereas injection of IGF-1 has no effect (Stern et al., 2014). IGF-2 produces the most potent and persistent effect as a memory enhancer on hippocampal-dependent memories (Chen et al., 2011). Like insulin, IGF-2 did not affect amygdala-dependent memories when delivered into the BLA. Contextual fear extinction is facilitated when IGF-2 is injected into the dentate gyrus whereas inhibition of physiological IGF-1 signaling, via intrahippocampal injection of an IGF-1 blocking antibody, did not affect fear extinction (Chen et al., 2011). However, a single intravenous injection of IGF-1 before the training of contextual fear extinction increases the density of mature dendritic spines in the hippocampus and mPFC, favoring the memory of extinction (Burgdorf et al., 2017). All these reports are consistent with the idea that IGF-2 would have a specific role in the hippocampus while IGF-1 could have a selective role in the IL.

The reduction in the AHP amplitude associated with learning mechanisms is transitory since AHP amplitude initial values are recovered within days when training is finished, indicating an intrinsic plasticity phenomenon (Moyer et al., 1996). Cohen-Matslish and coworkers showed that the AHP reduction induced by the synaptic activation requires protein synthesis during a specific time-window for its long-term maintenance (Cohen-Matsliah et al., 2010). It has been shown that the acquisition of extinction memory depends on protein synthesis (Santini et al., 2004). In our study, we show that IGF-1 favors the acquisition of extinction memory when applied directly to the IL in vivo. Therefore, it is tempting to hypothesize that the slow onset kinetics of AHP reduction that we observed after bath perfusion of IGF-1 in brain slices are suggestive of a mechanism involving protein synthesis, although further experiments would be needed to confirm it. Consistent with our observations in brain slices (plateau of AHP reduction is reached after 30 min), AHP amplitude is reduced in IL neurons from rats where the extinction memory was consolidated (recordings from the test day in IGF-1-treated animals). Moreover, we found that a reduction in AHP amplitude was directly correlated with a reduction in the expression of fear, suggesting that mechanisms favoring a greater AHP reduction would produce an enhancement of the acquisition of extinction memory.

Following the acquisition of fear extinction memory, there is a preferential shift toward increased intrinsic excitability in IL-amygdala communication (Bloodgood et al., 2018). In our study, we correlate the reduction in AHP amplitude with an increase in IL neuronal excitability. Under these circumstances, this increase in IL neurons’ bursting would activate inhibitory intercalated cells in the amygdala to produce the inhibition of fear expression, as described previously (Quirk and Mueller, 2008).

Functional relevance of the IGF-1 modulation in the mPFC

IGF-1 is actively transported to the central nervous system from plasma through the choroid plexus (Carro et al., 2000), and it is also locally produced in the brain by neurons and glial cells (Quesada et al., 2007; Suh et al., 2013; Rodriguez-Perez et al., 2016). Interestingly, the uptake of IGF-1 by the brain correlates with frequency-dependent changes in cerebral blood flow in the cortex during information processing (Nishijima et al., 2010). Therefore, the levels of IGF-1 in the IL would depend on both its active transport from the plasma, favored by the activity involved in information processing, and in the IGF-1 locally produced by neurons and astrocytes of the IL. Thus, high levels of IGF-1 are expected in the IL because of its high activity during the extinction of fear conditioning (Milad and Quirk, 2012; Sepulveda-Orengo et al., 2013). Our results demonstrate that a further increase in the IGF-1 levels due to the exogenous application of IGF-1 in the IL favor the extinction of conditioned fear. In natural conditions, these high levels of IGF-1 could be expected after physical exercises because it is known that IGF-1 uptake from the plasma and IGF-1 levels in the brain are increased after running, reaching the higher levels (Carro et al., 2000). Thus, our results could be related to some of the benefits of physical exercise on the brain function by increasing neuronal excitability and favoring synaptic plasticity and learning and memory through the activation of the IGF-1Rs.

In conclusion, the present findings reveal novel mechanisms and functional consequences of IGF-1 signaling in IL. On one hand, IGF-1 induces a reduction in mIAHP and sIAHP and increases the excitability of L5PNs of IL. On the other hand, IGF-1 modulates the excitatory and inhibitory synaptic transmission resulting in a long-lasting enhancement of the synaptic efficacy. Both the synaptic and intrinsic plasticity regulate the neuronal connectivity, which leads to the facilitation of consolidation of fear extinction memory. Altogether, these results strongly support the potential role of IGF-1 as a new therapeutic target for the treatment of anxiety and mood disorders.

Materials and methods

Animals

Male Sprague Dawley rats were group-housed in transparent polyethylene cages. Rats were maintained on a 12:12 hr light/dark scheduled cycle with free access to food and water. All animal procedures were approved by the Universidad Autónoma of Madrid Ethical Committee on Animal Welfare and conform to Spanish and European guidelines for the protection of experimental animals (Directive 2010/63/EU). An effort was made to minimize animal suffering and number.

Electrophysiology

Prefrontal cortical slices were obtained from rats at postnatal day (P20–P30) age. Rats were decapitated and the brain removed and submerged in artificial cerebrospinal fluid (ACSF). Coronal slices (400 µm thick) were obtained with a Vibratome (Leica VT 1200S). To reduce swelling and damage in superficial layers (especially after the behavior tests), brain slices were obtained using a modified ACSF, containing (in mM): 75 NaCl, 2.69 KCl, 1.25 KH2PO4, 2 MgSO4, 26 NaHCO3, 2 CaCl2, 10 glucose, 100 sucrose, one sodium ascorbate, and three sodium pyruvate. After that, brain slices were transferred to regular ACSF (in mM: 124 NaCl, 2.69 KCl, 1.25 KH2PO4, 2 Mg2SO4, 26 NaHCO3, 2 CaCl2, and 10 glucose, 0.4 sodium ascorbate, bubbled with carbogen [95% O2, 5% CO2]) and incubated for >1 hr at room temperature (22–24°C). Slices were then transferred to an immersion recording chamber and superfused with carbogen-bubbled ACSF. Cells were visualized under an Olympus BX50WI microscope. Patch-clamp recordings were obtained from the soma of PNs located at the layer 5 of IL (IL-L5PNs) using patch pipettes (4–8 MΩ) filled with an internal solution that contained (in mM): 130 KMeSO4, 10 HEPES-K, 4 Na2ATP, 0.3 Na3GTP, 0.2 EGTA, 10 KCl (buffered to pH 7.2–7.3 with KOH). Recordings were performed in current- or voltage-clamp modes using a Cornerstone PC-ONE amplifier (Dagan Corporation). Pipettes were placed with a micromanipulator (Narishige). The holding potential was adjusted to −60 mV and the series resistance was compensated to ~80%. Recordings were accepted only when series and input resistances did not change >20% during the experiment. The liquid junction potential was not compensated. Data were low-pass filtered at 3 kHz and sampled at 10 kHz, through a Digidata 1440 (Molecular Devices). The pClamp software (Molecular Devices) was used to acquire the data. Chemicals were purchased from Sigma-Aldrich Quimica and Tocris Bioscience (Ellisville; distributed by Biogen Cientifica) and R and D Systems, Inc (distributed by Bio-Techne). In the current-clamp mode, the afterhyperpolarizing potentials (AHPs) were studied using two different pulse protocols. The fast AHP (fAHP) was evoked by a 10 ms depolarizing pulse whereas an 800 ms depolarizing pulse was used to generate the medium and the slow AHP (mAHP and sAHP, respectively). While the fAHP and mAHP were measured as peak amplitude of the afterhyperpolarization, the sAHP was estimated as the average membrane potential between 50 ms and 280 ms after the 800 ms pulse (Santini et al., 2008). We applied the same protocol but in the voltage-clamp mode to record the currents that underline these potentials. We fixed the membrane potential at −60 mV and then we depolarized the cell at 0 mV during 10 ms and measured the fIAHP at the peak of the currents. In the case of mIAHP and sIAHP we depolarized the cell at 0 mV during 800 ms. We measured the mIAHP at the peak of the current and measured the sIAHP as the area under the curve during 1 s period beginning at the peak of the current. We also analyzed the excitability of IL-L5PNs by measuring the number of APs (spike frequency) elicited in response to a series of long (1 s) depolarizing current steps (25–200 pA for basal condition and 25–350 pA for neurons recorded after the behavioral tests; 25 pA increments) before and during IGF-1. The AP voltage threshold was defined as the first point on the rising phase of the spike with a change in voltage exceeded 50 mV/ms. The spike amplitude was quantified as the difference between the AP voltage threshold and the peak voltage. The spike width was measured at 1/2 of the total spike amplitude. The waveform characteristics of the APs recorded from IL-PNL5s, i.e., rise time, rise slope, decay time, and decay slope, were determined using Clampfit10 software (Axon Instruments).

Bipolar stimulation was applied through a Pt/Ir concentric electrode (OP: 200 μm, IP: 50 μm; FHC) connected by two silver-chloride wires to a stimulator and a stimulus isolation unit (ISU-165 Cibertec). The stimulating electrode was placed at 100 μm below the soma of the recorded neuron (at the level of layer 6), close to the basal dendrites of the recorded IL-L5PNs. Paired pulses (100 μs in duration and 20–100 μA in intensity, 50 ms away) were continuously delivered at 0.33 Hz. EPSCs were isolated in the presence of GABAAR (50 μm picrotoxin; PiTX) and GABABR (5 µM CGP-55845) antagonists. IPSCs were isolated adding AMPAR (20 μm CNQX) and NMDAR (50 μm D-AP5) antagonists. In both cases, after 5 min of stable baseline, we superfused IGF-1 for 35 min to check for long-term synaptic plasticity by analyzing the EPSCs and IPSCs peak amplitudes. In current-clamp mode, after a stable baseline of PSPs, the stimulation intensity was increased until suprathreshold responses reached were ≈21% for 5 min. Next, we applied IGF-1 for 15 min and measured the number of APs; afterward, we returned to initial stimulation intensity and measured the amplitude of PSPs to study the effect of IGF-1 on synaptic transmission. Miniature EPSCs (mEPSCs) were recorded at −60 mV in ACSF in the presence of 1 μm TTX, 20 μm PiTX, and 5 µM CGP-55845 to isolate excitatory synaptic transmission.

Conditioned fear

Rats were anesthetized with ketamine (70 mg/kg i.p.; Ketolar), xylazine (5 mg/kg i.p.; Rompum), and atropine (0,05 mg/kg i.p.; B. Braun Medical S.A) and maintained with isoflurane (2–3% in oxygen). Animals were positioned in a stereotaxic apparatus (David Kopf Instruments, Tujunga, CA, USA) and placed on a water-heated pad at 37°C. The midline of the scalp was sectioned and retracted, and small holes were drilled in the skull. Rats were implanted with a single 26 gauge stainless-steel guide cannula (Plastics One) in the mPFC. Stereotaxic coordinates aiming toward the IL were (AP: 2.8 mm, LM: 1.0 mm, and DV: 4.1 mm) from bregma according to rat brain atlas (Paxinos and Watson, 2007), with the cannula angled 11° toward the midline in the coronal plane as described previously (Santini et al., 2004). A 33-gauge dummy cannula was inserted into the guide cannula to prevent clogging. Guide cannulas were cemented to the skull with dental acrylic (Grip Cement) and the incision was sutured. Buprenorphine hydrochloride (75 mg/kg s.c.; Buprex) was administered for post-surgical analgesia. Rats were allowed at least 7 days for surgery recovery. Trace fear conditioning was conducted in a chamber of 25 × 31 × 25 cm with aluminum and Plexiglas walls (Coulbourn, Allentown, PA). The floor consisted of stainless-steel bars (26 parallel steel rods 5 mm diameter, 6 mm spacing) that can be electrified to deliver a mild shock. A speaker was mounted on the outside wall, and illumination was provided by a single overhead light (miniature incandescent white lamp 28 V). The rectangular chamber was situated inside a sound-attenuating box (Med Associates, Burlington, VT) with a ventilating fan, which produced an ambient noise level of 58 dB. The CS was a 4 kHz tone with a duration of 30 s and an intensity of 80 dB. The US was a 0.4 mA scrambled footshock, 0.5 s in duration, which co-terminated with the tone during the conditioning phase. Between sessions, floor trays and shock bars were cleaned with soapy water and the chamber walls were wiped with a damp cloth. An additional Plexiglas chamber served as a novel context for the auditory cue test. This chamber, which was a triangle with a black smooth Plexiglas floor, was physically distinct from the fear conditioning chamber. All the environmental conditions were completely different regarding the fear conditioning test except for the ventilating fan. Before placing rats into this chamber, the chamber floor and walls were wiped with 30% vanilla solution to provide a background odor distinctive from that used during fear conditioning. The activity of each rat was recorded with a digital video camera mounted on top of each behavioral chamber. 30 min before extinction training, saline (NaCl 0.9%), IGF-1 (10 μM; Preprotech), or the IGF-1R antagonist 7-[cis-3-(1-azetidinylmethyl)cyclobutyl]−5-[3-(phenylmethoxy)phenyl]−7H-pyrrolo[2,3-]pyrimidin-4-amine (NVP-AEW 541, 40 μM; Cayman Chemicals) plus IGF-1 were infused into the IL. For the infusions, cannula dummies were removed from guide cannulas and replaced with 33-gauge injectors, which were connected by polyethylene tubing (PE-20; Small Parts) to 100 μl syringes mounted in an infusion pump (Harvard Apparatus). Drugs were infused at a rate of 0.5 μl/min for 1 min as described previously (Fontanez-Nuin et al., 2011).

Non-cannulated animals

Animals were divided into two groups, the conditioned group (COND) and the extinguished group (EXT). On day 1, rats received three habituation trials (tone-no shock; habituation phase) into two different contexts (context 1: square shock box and context 2: triangle box with soft floor). On day 2, rats received three conditioning trials (tone paired with shock; context 1; condition phase). On day 3, rats of COND group remained in their home cage, whereas rats of EXT group received 20 tone-alone trials (context 2; extinction phase). On day 4, both groups of rats received five tone-alone trials to test for recall of conditioning or extinction (test phase) (Figure 4—figure supplement 1).

Cannulated animals

Animals were divided into the saline group (SAL) and the different drug groups (IGF-1 or NVP + IGF-1). On day 1, rats received three habituation trials into the two contexts aforementioned. On day 2, rats received three conditioning trials. On day 3, rats were infused with saline or drug 30 min before the beginning of the extinction phase. On day 4, all groups of rats received five tone-alone trials in the same chamber (context 2) to test for recall of extinction (Figure 4B). In all phases of the experiment, the interval between successive tones was variable, with an average of 2 min. All groups were tested on the same day to determine the long-term changes occurring in the mPFC that gate subsequent memory retrieval.

Data analysis

Data were analyzed using pClamp (Molecular Devices), Excel (Microsoft), and GraphPad Prism 8.3 software. Twenty responses were averaged except when otherwise indicated. The magnitude of the change in peak EPSCs, IPSCs, and PSPs amplitude was expressed as a percentage (%) of the baseline control amplitude and plotted as a function of time. The presynaptic or postsynaptic origin of the observed regulation of EPSCs and IPSCs amplitudes was tested by estimating the PPR changes, which were considered to be of presynaptic origin (Clark et al., 1994; Creager et al., 1980; Kuhnt and Voronin, 1994) and were quantified by calculating a PPR index (R2/R1), where R1 and R2 were the peak amplitudes of the first and second synaptic currents, respectively. To estimate the modifications in the synaptic current variance, we first calculated the noise-free coefficient of variation (CVNF) for the synaptic responses before and 40 min after applying IGF-1 in the bath with the formula CVNF = √(δXPSC2 − δnoise2)/m; δXPSC2 and δnoise2 (X = E excitatory or X = I inhibitory) are the variances of the peak EPSC or IPSC and baseline, respectively, and m is the mean EPSC or IPSC peak amplitude. The ratio of the CV measured before and 40 min after applying IGF-1 (CVr) was obtained for each neuron as CV after IGF-1 responses/CV control (Clements, 1990). Finally, we constructed plots comparing variation in M (m after IGF-1 responses over m at control conditions) against the changes in response variance of the EPSC or IPSC amplitude (1/CVr2) for each cell. In these plots, values were expected to follow the diagonal if the EPSC or IPSC depression had a presynaptic origin. Results are given as mean ± SEM (N = numbers of cells). There were no gender differences in our experiments. MiniAnalysis software (SynaptoSoft Inc) and pCLAMP software were used for the analysis of the frequency and amplitude of mEPSCs. See Supplementary file 1 for a detailed description of the statistical analyses performed in these experiments.

The total freezing time during the 30 s tone was measured and converted to a percentage of freezing. Freezing was defined as the cessation of all movements except respiration. The percentage of freezing time was used as a measure of conditioned fear (Blanchard and Blanchard, 1972). The behavioral data were analyzed by manual evaluation of the videos and/or using the ImageJ free software with the plugging developed for this purpose (Shoji et al., 2014). No differences were observed with both methods. Values are reported as the means ± SEM. See Supplementary file 1 for a detailed description of the statistical analyses performed in these experiments.

Blind experiments were not performed in the study but the same criteria were applied to all allocated groups for comparisons. Randomization was not employed. The sample size in whole-cell recording experiments was based on the values previously found sufficient to detect significant changes in past studies from the lab.

Statistical analysis was performed using Prism 9 (GraphPad). Prior to any statistical analysis, we assessed normality distribution of our data using Kolmogorov–Smirnov test for normality. Then, we compared data means using t-test or Wilcoxon test for comparisons of two groups. Multiple t-test or ANOVA (one-way or two-way) test for more than two groups for parametric distributions and Mann–Whitney for non-parametric distributions. Specific statistical tests and number of animals are indicated in each figure legend.

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by the following Grants: BFU2013-43668-P and BFU2016-0802-P AEI/FEDER, UE (MINECO) to D Fernández de Sevilla. We thank Dr. Washington Buño, Ricardo Gomez and Ignacio Torres-Aleman for helpful comments and Dr. Cesar Venero for his advice and help on the conditioned fear experiments.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Laura E Maglio, Email: lamaglio@ull.edu.es.

David Fernández de Sevilla, Email: david.fernandezdesevilla@uam.es.

Inna Slutsky, Tel Aviv University, Israel.

John R Huguenard, Stanford University School of Medicine, United States.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • Ministerio de Economía, Industria y Competitividad, Gobierno de España BFU2013-43668-P to David Fernández de Sevilla.

  • Ministerio de Economía, Industria y Competitividad, Gobierno de España BFU2016-0802-P AEI/FEDER to David Fernández de Sevilla.

  • Ministerio de Economía, Industria y Competitividad, Gobierno de España UE to David Fernández de Sevilla.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing - original draft, Writing - review and editing.

Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing - review and editing.

Formal analysis, Investigation.

Formal analysis, Investigation.

Formal analysis, Investigation.

Investigation.

Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Validation, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing - original draft, Project administration, Writing - review and editing.

Ethics

Animal experimentation: All animal procedures were approved by the Universidad Autónoma of Madrid Ethical Committee on Animal Welfare and conform to Spanish and European guidelines for the protection of experimental animals (Directive 2010/63/EU). An effort was made to minimize animal suffering and number.

Additional files

Supplementary file 1. Summary of statistical significance and tests used in each figure.
elife-67267-supp1.docx (107.6KB, docx)
Transparent reporting form

Data availability

All data generated or analysed during this study are included in the manuscript and supporting files.

References

  1. Alger BE, Nicoll RA. Epileptiform burst afterhyperolarization: calcium-dependent potassium potential in hippocampal CA1 pyramidal cells. Science. 1980;210:1122–1124. doi: 10.1126/science.7444438. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Araujo DM, Lapchak PA, Collier B, Chabot JG, Quirion R. Insulin-like growth factor-1 (somatomedin-C) receptors in the rat brain: distribution and interaction with the hippocampal cholinergic system. Brain Research. 1989;484:130–138. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(89)90355-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Blair LA, Marshall J. IGF-1 modulates N and L calcium channels in a PI 3-kinase-dependent manner. Neuron. 1997;19:421–429. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80950-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Blanchard DC, Blanchard RJ. Innate and conditioned reactions to threat in rats with amygdaloid lesions. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology. 1972;81:281–290. doi: 10.1037/h0033521. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bloodgood DW, Sugam JA, Holmes A, Kash TL. Fear extinction requires infralimbic cortex projections to the basolateral amygdala. Translational Psychiatry. 2018;8:60. doi: 10.1038/s41398-018-0106-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Burgdorf J, Colechio EM, Ghoreishi-Haack N, Gross AL, Rex CS, Zhang XL, Stanton PK, Kroes RA, Moskal JR. IGFBP2 produces Rapid-Acting and Long-Lasting effects in rat models of posttraumatic stress disorder via a novel mechanism associated with structural plasticity. International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology. 2017;20:476–484. doi: 10.1093/ijnp/pyx007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Burgos-Robles A, Vidal-Gonzalez I, Santini E, Quirk GJ. Consolidation of fear extinction requires NMDA receptor-dependent bursting in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Neuron. 2007;53:871–880. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.02.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Carro E, Nuñez A, Busiguina S, Torres-Aleman I. Circulating insulin-like growth factor I mediates effects of exercise on the brain. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2000;20:2926–2933. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-08-02926.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Castro-Alamancos MA, Arevalo MA, Torres-Aleman I. Involvement of protein kinase C and nitric oxide in the modulation by insulin-like growth factor-I of glutamate-induced GABA release in the cerebellum. Neuroscience. 1996;70:843–847. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(95)00472-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Castro-Alamancos MA, Torres-Aleman I. Long-term depression of glutamate-induced gamma-aminobutyric acid release in cerebellum by insulin-like growth factor I. PNAS. 1993;90:7386–7390. doi: 10.1073/pnas.90.15.7386. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Chen DY, Stern SA, Garcia-Osta A, Saunier-Rebori B, Pollonini G, Bambah-Mukku D, Blitzer RD, Alberini CM. A critical role for IGF-II in memory consolidation and enhancement. Nature. 2011;469:491–497. doi: 10.1038/nature09667. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Clark KA, Randall AD, Collingridge GL. A comparison of paired-pulsed facilitation of AMPA and NMDA receptor-mediated excitatory postsynaptic currents in the Hippocampus. Experimental Brain Research. 1994;101:272–278. doi: 10.1007/BF00228747. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Clements JD. A statistical test for demonstrating a presynaptic site of action for a modulator of synaptic amplitude. Journal of Neuroscience Methods. 1990;31:75–88. doi: 10.1016/0165-0270(90)90012-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Cohen-Matsliah SI, Motanis H, Rosenblum K, Barkai E. A novel role for protein synthesis in long-term neuronal plasticity: maintaining reduced postburst afterhyperpolarization. Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30:4338–4342. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5005-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Creager R, Dunwiddie T, Lynch G. Paired-pulse and frequency facilitation in the CA1 region of the in vitro rat Hippocampus. The Journal of Physiology. 1980;299:409–424. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1980.sp013133. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Criado-Marrero M, Santini E, Porter JT. Modulating fear extinction memory by manipulating SK potassium channels in the infralimbic cortex. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 2014;8:96. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00096. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Do-Monte FH, Manzano-Nieves G, Quiñones-Laracuente K, Ramos-Medina L, Quirk GJ. Revisiting the role of infralimbic cortex in fear extinction with optogenetics. Journal of Neuroscience. 2015;35:3607–3615. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3137-14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Fernandez AM, Torres-Alemán I. The many faces of insulin-like peptide signalling in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2012;13:225–239. doi: 10.1038/nrn3209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Fontanez-Nuin DE, Santini E, Quirk GJ, Porter JT. Memory for fear extinction requires mGluR5-mediated activation of infralimbic neurons. Cerebral Cortex. 2011;21:727–735. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq147. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Gazit N, Vertkin I, Shapira I, Helm M, Slomowitz E, Sheiba M, Mor Y, Rizzoli S, Slutsky I. IGF-1 receptor differentially regulates spontaneous and evoked transmission via mitochondria at hippocampal synapses. Neuron. 2016;89:583–597. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.12.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Gonzalez de la Vega A, Buño W, Pons S, Garcia-Calderat MS, Garcia-Galloway E, Torres-Aleman I. Insulin-like growth factor I potentiates kainate receptors through a phosphatidylinositol 3-kinase dependent pathway. Neuroreport. 2001;12:1293–1296. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200105080-00047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Hugues S, Deschaux O, Garcia R. Postextinction infusion of a mitogen-activated protein kinase inhibitor into the medial prefrontal cortex impairs memory of the extinction of conditioned fear. Learning & Memory. 2004;11:540–543. doi: 10.1101/lm.77704. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Jones SL, To MS, Stuart GJ. Dendritic small conductance calcium-activated potassium channels activated by action potentials suppress EPSPs and gate spike-timing dependent synaptic plasticity. eLife. 2017;6:e30333. doi: 10.7554/eLife.30333. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Kaczorowski CC, Davis SJ, Moyer JR. Aging redistributes medial prefrontal neuronal excitability and impedes extinction of trace fear conditioning. Neurobiology of Aging. 2012;33:1744–1757. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.03.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Koppensteiner P, Galvin C, Ninan I. Lack of experience-dependent intrinsic plasticity in the adolescent infralimbic medial prefrontal cortex. Synapse. 2019;73:e22090. doi: 10.1002/syn.22090. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Kuhnt U, Voronin LL. Interaction between paired-pulse facilitation and long-term potentiation in area CA1 of guinea-pig hippocampal slices: application of quantal analysis. Neuroscience. 1994;62:391–397. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(94)90374-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Laurent V, Westbrook RF. Inactivation of the infralimbic but not the prelimbic cortex impairs consolidation and retrieval of fear extinction. Learning & Memory. 2009;16:520–529. doi: 10.1101/lm.1474609. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. LeDoux JE. Emotion circuits in the brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2000;23:155–184. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Liu Z, Chen Z, Shang C, Yan F, Shi Y, Zhang J, Qu B, Han H, Wang Y, Li D, Südhof TC, Cao P. IGF1-Dependent synaptic plasticity of mitral cells in olfactory memory during social learning. Neuron. 2017;95:106–122. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.06.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Llorens-Martín M, Torres-Alemán I, Trejo JL. Exercise modulates insulin-like growth factor 1-dependent and -independent effects on adult hippocampal neurogenesis and behaviour. Molecular and Cellular Neuroscience. 2010;44:109–117. doi: 10.1016/j.mcn.2010.02.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Madison DV, Nicoll RA. Control of the repetitive discharge of rat CA 1 pyramidal neurones in vitro. The Journal of Physiology. 1984;354:319–331. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1984.sp015378. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Maren S. Neurobiology of pavlovian fear conditioning. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2001;24:897–931. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.897. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Maya-Vetencourt JF, Baroncelli L, Viegi A, Tiraboschi E, Castren E, Cattaneo A, Maffei L. IGF-1 restores visual cortex plasticity in adult life by reducing local GABA levels. Neural Plasticity. 2012;2012:1–10. doi: 10.1155/2012/250421. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Milad MR, Vidal-Gonzalez I, Quirk GJ. Electrical stimulation of medial prefrontal cortex reduces conditioned fear in a temporally specific manner. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2004;118:389–394. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.118.2.389. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Milad MR, Quirk GJ. Neurons in medial prefrontal cortex signal memory for fear extinction. Nature. 2002;420:70–74. doi: 10.1038/nature01138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Milad MR, Quirk GJ. Fear extinction as a model for translational neuroscience: ten years of progress. Annual Review of Psychology. 2012;63:129–151. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.121208.131631. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Molina DP, Ariwodola OJ, Linville C, Sonntag WE, Weiner JL, Brunso-Bechtold JK, Adams MM. Growth hormone modulates hippocampal excitatory synaptic transmission and plasticity in old rats. Neurobiology of Aging. 2012;33:1938–1949. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.09.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Morgan MA, LeDoux JE. Differential contribution of dorsal and ventral medial prefrontal cortex to the acquisition and extinction of conditioned fear in rats. Behavioral Neuroscience. 1995;109:681–688. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.109.4.681. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Moyer JR, Thompson LT, Disterhoft JF. Trace eyeblink conditioning increases CA1 excitability in a transient and learning-specific manner. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1996;16:5536–5546. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-17-05536.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Mueller D, Porter JT, Quirk GJ. Noradrenergic signaling in infralimbic cortex increases cell excitability and strengthens memory for fear extinction. Journal of Neuroscience. 2008;28:369–375. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3248-07.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Ngo-Anh TJ, Bloodgood BL, Lin M, Sabatini BL, Maylie J, Adelman JP. SK channels and NMDA receptors form a Ca2+-mediated feedback loop in dendritic spines. Nature Neuroscience. 2005;8:642–649. doi: 10.1038/nn1449. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Nilsson L, Sara VR, Nordberg A. Insulin-like growth factor 1 stimulates the release of acetylcholine from rat cortical slices. Neuroscience Letters. 1988;88:221–226. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(88)90130-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Nishijima T, Piriz J, Duflot S, Fernandez AM, Gaitan G, Gomez-Pinedo U, Verdugo JM, Leroy F, Soya H, Nuñez A, Torres-Aleman I. Neuronal activity drives localized blood-brain-barrier transport of serum insulin-like growth factor-I into the CNS. Neuron. 2010;67:834–846. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.08.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Noriega-prieto JA, Maglio LE, Zegarra- JA, Pignatelli J, Fernandez AM, Martinez- L, Fernandes J, Núñez A, Araque A, Alemán T, De SDF. IGF-1 governs cortical inhibitory synaptic plasticity by astrocyte activation. bioRxiv. 2020 doi: 10.1101/2020.02.11.942532. [DOI]
  45. Nuñez A, Carro E, Torres-Aleman I. Insulin-like growth factor I modifies electrophysiological properties of rat brain stem neurons. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2003;89:3008–3017. doi: 10.1152/jn.00089.2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Orsini CA, Maren S. Neural and cellular mechanisms of fear and extinction memory formation. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2012;36:1773–1802. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.12.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Pape HC, Pare D. Plastic synaptic networks of the amygdala for the acquisition, expression, and extinction of conditioned fear. Physiological Reviews. 2010;90:419–463. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00037.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Pavlov I. Conditioned Reflexes. London: Oxford Univ Press; 1927. [Google Scholar]
  49. Paxinos G, Watson C. The Rat Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates. Academic Press; 2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Quesada A, Romeo HE, Micevych P. Distribution and localization patterns of estrogen receptor-beta and insulin-like growth factor-1 receptors in neurons and glial cells of the female rat substantia nigra: localization of ERbeta and IGF-1R in substantia nigra. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2007;503:198–208. doi: 10.1002/cne.21358. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Quirk GJ, Russo GK, Barron JL, Lebron K. The role of ventromedial prefrontal cortex in the recovery of extinguished fear. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2000;20:6225–6231. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-16-06225.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Quirk GJ, Beer JS. Prefrontal involvement in the regulation of emotion: convergence of rat and human studies. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2006;16:723–727. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2006.07.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Quirk GJ, Mueller D. Neural mechanisms of extinction learning and retrieval. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33:56–72. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301555. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Ramsey MM, Adams MM, Ariwodola OJ, Sonntag WE, Weiner JL. Functional characterization of des-IGF-1 action at excitatory synapses in the CA1 region of rat Hippocampus. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2005;94:247–254. doi: 10.1152/jn.00768.2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Rodriguez-Perez AI, Borrajo A, Diaz-Ruiz C, Garrido-Gil P, Labandeira-Garcia JL. Crosstalk between insulin-like growth factor-1 and angiotensin-II in dopaminergic neurons and glial cells: role in Neuroinflammation and aging. Oncotarget. 2016;7:30049–30067. doi: 10.18632/oncotarget.9174. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Santini E, Ge H, Ren K, Peña de Ortiz S, Quirk GJ. Consolidation of fear extinction requires protein synthesis in the medial prefrontal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience. 2004;24:5704–5710. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0786-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Santini E, Quirk GJ, Porter JT. Fear conditioning and extinction differentially modify the intrinsic excitability of infralimbic neurons. Journal of Neuroscience. 2008;28:4028–4036. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2623-07.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Santini E, Porter JT. M-type potassium channels modulate the intrinsic excitability of infralimbic neurons and regulate fear expression and extinction. Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30:12379–12386. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1295-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Savchenko A, Kraft TW, Molokanova E, Kramer RH. Growth factors regulate phototransduction in retinal rods by modulating cyclic nucleotide-gated channels through dephosphorylation of a specific tyrosine residue. PNAS. 2001;98:5880–5885. doi: 10.1073/pnas.101524998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Sehgal M, Ehlers VL, Moyer JR. Learning enhances intrinsic excitability in a subset of lateral amygdala neurons. Learning & Memory. 2014;21:161–170. doi: 10.1101/lm.032730.113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Sepulveda-Orengo MT, Lopez AV, Soler-Cedeno O, Porter JT. Fear extinction induces mGluR5-Mediated synaptic and intrinsic plasticity in infralimbic neurons. Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33:7184–7193. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5198-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Seto D, Zheng WH, McNicoll A, Collier B, Quirion R, Kar S. Insulin-like growth factor-I inhibits endogenous acetylcholine release from the rat hippocampal formation: possible involvement of GABA in mediating the effects. Neuroscience. 2002;115:603–612. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4522(02)00450-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Shan H, Messi ML, Zheng Z, Wang ZM, Delbono O. Preservation of motor neuron Ca2+ channel sensitivity to insulin-like growth factor-1 in brain motor cortex from senescent rat. The Journal of Physiology. 2003;553:49–63. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2003.047746. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Shoji H, Takao K, Hattori S, Miyakawa T. Contextual and cued fear conditioning test using a video analyzing system in mice. Journal of Visualized Experiments. 2014;85:50871. doi: 10.3791/50871. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Sierra-Mercado D, Padilla-Coreano N, Quirk GJ. Dissociable roles of prelimbic and infralimbic cortices, ventral Hippocampus, and basolateral amygdala in the expression and extinction of conditioned fear. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2011;36:529–538. doi: 10.1038/npp.2010.184. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Sonntag WE, Bennett SA, Khan AS, Thornton PL, Xu X, Ingram RL, Brunso-Bechtold JK. Age and insulin-like growth factor-1 modulate N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor subtype expression in rats. Brain Research Bulletin. 2000;51:331–338. doi: 10.1016/S0361-9230(99)00259-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Sotres-Bayon F, Bush DE, LeDoux JE. Acquisition of fear extinction requires activation of NR2B-containing NMDA receptors in the lateral amygdala. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2007;32:1929–1940. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301316. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Stern SA, Chen DY, Alberini CM. The effect of insulin and insulin-like growth factors on Hippocampus- and amygdala-dependent long-term memory formation. Learning & Memory. 2014;21:556–563. doi: 10.1101/lm.029348.112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Storm JF. Understanding the brain through the Hippocampus. the hippocampal region as a model for studying brain structure and function. dedicated to professor Theodor W. Blackstad on the occasion of his 65th anniversary. Progress in Brain Research; 1990. pp. 161–187. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Suh H-S, Zhao M-L, Derico L, Choi N, Lee SC. Insulin-like growth factor 1 and 2 (IGF1, IGF2) expression in human microglia: differential regulation by inflammatory mediators. Journal of Neuroinflammation. 2013;10:805. doi: 10.1186/1742-2094-10-37. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Trejo JL, Llorens-Martín MV, Torres-Alemán I. The effects of exercise on spatial learning and anxiety-like behavior are mediated by an IGF-I-dependent mechanism related to hippocampal neurogenesis. Molecular and Cellular Neuroscience. 2008;37:402–411. doi: 10.1016/j.mcn.2007.10.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Xing C, Yin Y, He X, Xie Z. Effects of insulin-like growth factor 1 on voltage-gated ion channels in cultured rat hippocampal neurons. Brain Research. 2006;1072:30–35. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2005.10.091. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Young SR, Bianchi R, Wong RK. Signaling mechanisms underlying group I mGluR-induced persistent AHP suppression in CA3 hippocampal neurons. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2008;99:1105–1118. doi: 10.1152/jn.00435.2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision letter

Editor: Inna Slutsky1

In the interests of transparency, eLife publishes the most substantive revision requests and the accompanying author responses.

Acceptance summary:

This paper shows that insulin growth factor-1 (IGF-1) plays an important role in regulating intrinsic excitability and synaptic transmission in the infralimbic cortex, facilitating the extinction of fear memory.

Decision letter after peer review:

[Editors’ note: the authors submitted for reconsideration following the decision after peer review. What follows is the decision letter after the first round of review.]

Thank you for submitting your work entitled "IGF-1 facilitates extinction of conditioned fear" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by 3 peer reviewers, one of whom is a member of our Board of Reviewing Editors, and the evaluation has been overseen by a Reviewing Editor and a Senior Editor. The reviewers have opted to remain anonymous.

Our decision has been reached after consultation between the reviewers. Based on these discussions and the individual reviews below, we regret to inform you that your work will not be considered further for publication in eLife.

While reviewers think that the findings linking behavioral and neuronal changes by IGF1 are of potential interest, they pointed to numerous methodological, statistical and biological problems (see the detailed comments below).

Reviewer #1:

This work comes to show the role IGF1 plays in memory extinction. The authors suggest that IGF1 increases intrinsic excitability of L5P neurons in the infralimbic cortex (IL) by decreasing AHP. In addition, it attenuates both IPSCs and EPSCs by presynaptic mechanisms and facilitates a postsynaptic potentiation. Finally, the authors show that animals injected with IGF1 intracranially exhibit increased extinction of fear memory, and that IL-L5P neurons of these animals have reduced AHP current, increased excitability and decreased mEPSC frequency. The authors concluded that IGF1 regulates the cellular and synaptic processes underling fear memory extinction in the IL cortex.

1. Throughout the work the authors show that application of IGF1 causes the aforementioned effects, while they did not address the question whether IGF1 has a physiological role in excitability, synaptic transmission and behavioral phenotypes. It is unclear whether inhibition of IGF1R by NVP-AEW541 caused a decrease in mAHP and sAHP. Moreover, from Fig. S1 it looks like that the IGF1R antagonist has an inhibitory effect on fAHP. The authors should analyze the effect of IGF1R antagonist and show statistics for both, electrophysiological and behavioral phenotypes.

2. The authors show that IGF1 application causes a long-term depression of sAHP, while did not identify the mechanism mediating it. It would be important to identify the channel involved in AHP regulation. Moreover, it would be important to show that block of this channel occludes an increase in I-F ratio. Otherwise, the authors should acknowledge that AHP changes and I-F changes could be mediated by distinct mechanisms.

3. The authors show that IGF1 increases PSP (Fig. 3I). They increased the intensity of stimulation to get some supra-threshold events in the middle of the protocol. The authors must show that this protocol does not cause a potentiation of PSP by itself, without addition of IGF1.

4. Previous study found that extinction causes a decrease in fAHP, but not in sAHP in the IL (Santini et al., 2008). In contrast, conditioning up-regulated sAHP in that study. Moreover, a decrease in fAHP by M-channel inhibitor facilitated extinction (Santini and Porter, 2010). How do the authors explain this discrepancy between the results?

Reviewer #2:

This paper examines the effects of insulin-like growth factor-1 (IGF-1) on intrinsic excitability and synaptic transmission in neurons from the infralimbic cortex and on the extinction of fear memory. The authors report an increase in excitability mediated by the reduction of mAHP and a reduction in excitatory and inhibitory synaptic transmission. They also show that IGF-1 facilitates the extinction of fear memory.

This paper is interesting however, there are many issues. First, IGF-1 application is rather artificial and therefore this does not correspond to a natural way of stimulating neurons. In addition, some protocols and measures must be clarified.

It is not clear what the authors precisely measured when the declare measuring mAHP and sAHP. In contrast with what seems inferred by the authors the medium and slow AHPs can be measured with the same depolarization. In addition, the sIAHP generally peaks at ~0.5-1.0 s while their records display only a current peaking at ~100 ms. So there is no sAHP in the data provided in Figure 1 and 2 and therefore, conclusion about the effect of IGF-1 on sAHP is erroneous.

Effects of IGF-1 on PSCs. The application of IGF-1 is very long (>30 min) and the test time after application is too short (~15 minutes) to really consider the depression as long-lasting. The authors should consider shorter application times (5 or 10 min) and longer test times (at least 30 min) to know whether the persistent effect is due or not to an incomplete elimination of IGF-1 applied in the bath.

Does the effect of IGF-1 on intrinsic excitability long-lasting? To test this, brief application of IGF-1 should be used too.

The rational of the experiment depicted in Fig. 3I en 3J is not clear at all. Please provide details why stimulus intensity is increased during 5 min to obtain 21% of supratheshold responses.

Reviewer #3:

In this manuscript, the authors explore the involvement of IGF-1 in learning, focusing on fear extinction. They show that IGF-1 modulates network and intrinsic firing properties of L5, increasing firing frequency via inhibition of AHP and affecting network properties by reducing the release probability of presynaptic neurons. In addition, they demonstrate a concomitant increase of the firing probability in the postsynaptic L5 neurons. Intriguingly these effects develop rather slowly, with a plateau reached after ~40 min. They further show that in vivo administration of IGF-1 during the extinction phase results in enhanced fear extinction, correlating with the long-lasting effect, of over 24 h, of AHP inhibition and increased firing.

Overall these findings are interesting, linking behavioral and neuronal changes. Nevertheless, important experimental data, that were collected during the performed experiments, are not presented, leaving the reader with a partial view (see below in the comments). Moreover, some controls are missing (see below) and the number of cells in the electrophysiological recordings is rather low (n = 4/5/6 in many key figures).

1. Apart from changes in AHP, were there any additional changes in single AP properties? Did you observe any changes in AP threshold? Kinetics of upstroke or decay? Max amp? Duration? Input resistance?

The examples shown in Fig 1E depicts similar AHP with repetitive firing. If so, additional alteration in AP properties may be responsible for increased firing following IGF-1 application.

2. Fig. 1 - AHP amplitudes are presented as deltas, but the reference point is unclear, was it AP threshold? Also, since these are paired recordings, an illustration highlighting the pairs can be useful.

Were there differences in the time to max AHP? Were there differences between the % change in mAHP and sAHP for each cell? A similar % reduction in both may indicate a common mechanism, while the lack of correlation between mAHP and sAHP may hint that multiple mechanisms are at play. How long did you perfuse IGF-1? (~40 min?) what was the kinetics of this effect?

3. Fig 1B, D. The variability seems to be lower in the NVP group compare to the ACSF group. Could that be attributed to the variable levels of endogenous IGF-1?

4. Fig 1E. Did you observe AHP alterations during prolonged firing? the presented examples depict similar AHP levels after each AP and the potential at the end of the pulse is slightly more hyperpolarized after IGF1 addition, in contrast to Fig. 1A. Are there any other parameters that might support enhanced firing?

5. How many rats were used for each data set? this is not indicated throughout the manuscript.

6. The slow kinetics of the AHP and synaptic currents response to IGF1, as well as the long-lasting effect observed following IGF-1 washout (in slices and in vivo), should be discussed. Such slow kinetics may suggest the possible involvement of protein synthesis or insertion/endocytosis.

7. Line 273 - did you mean "did not require depolarization"? Sup Fig 2 depicts sAHP reduction of over 60% without depolarization. The time courses, with or without stimulation, are somewhat different, but the endpoints (after 50 min) are similar (showing maybe even a bit more inhibition without depolarization). Thus, depolarization may facilitate the process, but is not a prerequisite.

8. Lines 288-290 - "Additionally, this modulation was not observed when the synaptic stimulation was absent (Figure 3D), suggesting that the evoked synaptic responses were required for this LTD of the EPSCs". This statement is confusing and is not supported by the data. Comparing Fig. 3A and 3D, the endpoints (after 50 min) are similar, with inhibition levels of 30-40%. Thus, synaptic stimulation during IGF-1 application is not required for presynaptic LTD. Despite that, as for the kinetics of AHP, depolarization may in fact facilitate this effect. Thus, two mechanisms may coexist: one that supports EPSC inhibition with synaptic activation, with faster kinetics that plateaus within 10 min of IGF1 application, and another slower process, independent of synaptic activation, which plateaus at 50 min. This point requires further clarification and discussion.

9. For IPSCs, the kinetics is similar with or without stimulation. But, the level of inhibition after 50 min is doubled without synaptic stimulation. These properties should also be further discussed.

10. Fig 3I-J - These results are very interesting and important, as mimicking of firing by synaptic activation takes into account changes in multiple presynaptic neurons (excitatory and feedforward inhibition), as well as postsynaptic changes. Indeed, firing in response to current injection into the soma is less physiological. Moreover, SK channels in different cellular locations were shown to confer contradicting effects on EPSPs amplitude and duration, and thus on neuronal excitability (studied by the Stuart lab as well as others). However, valuable information is missing here as well. Did you see changes in PSP duration? changes in feedforward inhibition? AP properties? do you see similar AHP inhibition also with synaptic activation?

The opposing effects observed in voltage clamp and current clamp modes (reduced EPSP amplitude with increased PSP depolarization) need to be discussed. While one possibility for this can be a change in the input resistance, another may reside in alterations of the interplay between dendritic SK and NMDA channels (see Bock et al. 2019). Also, the mechanism for enhanced firing with synaptic stimulation may be different from increased firing with current injection. In addition to the site of depolarization, synaptic stimulations were given at low freq , as oppose the high frequency firing with prolonged depolarization. While the rate of synaptic stimulation is not specified for the current clamp experiments shown (should be added please), the graphical representations suggest 1 stim/min. What was the duration of IGF-1 application here? The methods section indicates 15 min, but the graph and legend specify 35 min. The kinetics for PSP potentiation seems to be faster, compared to the VC experiments. Any indication of the underlying cause?

11. Overall the method section is missing some vital information. Was the age of the rats used for behavioral experiments also P20 - P30? were the surgeries performed at P20? with 7 days recovery the behavioral experiments supposedly spanned P28 - 31, correct? what were the intervals between each step of the trials (cond, ext, test)?

12. Fig 4 - What was the effect of NVP alone? this control is vital. IGF-1R was shown to be fully activated at baseline conditions (Gazit et al. 2016), thus, with NVP and IGF-1, some R might still be functional and activated by IGF1, resulting in partial inhibition by NVP. Moreover, the main conclusion here seems to be drawn from the relatively low levels of fear extinction in the saline group (note the typo - extintion). Comparing the data in Fig 4 to Sup Fig 3, sup Fig 3 illustrates what seems to be further extinction during the extinction phase, as well as during the test phase. Reduction in freezing is missing in the saline group shown in Fig 4. What might reconcile these observations?

13. Was there a correlation between the sAHP amplitude and freezing behavior? One might expect lower sAHP amp in rats with better extinction levels.

14. Did you also observe lower levels of mAHP after in vivo application of IGF1/NVP? any effect on fAHP? is it possible to see a further reduction of sAHP with acute bath application of IGF-1 in mice that were pre-treated with IGF-1 during fear extinction? this would reveal the dynamic range of this long-lasting effect.

15. The variability in mEPSC is large, especially in the IGF-1 group. Recordings from more cells can fix that.

16. The molecular mechanism is not sufficiently addressed. The authors suggest that SK-, m-currents or mGluR5 might be involved. However, no direct evidence for the specific involvement of these channels is provided, nor any explanation for the slow and long-lasting effect observed.

[Editors’ note: further revisions were suggested prior to acceptance, as described below.]

Thank you for resubmitting your work entitled "IGF-1 facilitates extinction of conditioned fear" for further consideration by eLife. Your revised article has been evaluated by John Huguenard (Senior Editor) and a Reviewing Editor.

The manuscript has been improved but there are some remaining issues that need to be addressed, as outlined below:

1. The authors performed new experiments showing that inhibition of IGF1Rs occludes the effects of exogenously applied IGF1, but does not cause any effect on neuronal/synaptic function and memory extinction. This raises the question whether endogenous IGF1 plays a physiological role in these processes? In the discussion the authors claim that "....IGF-1 appears as a key endogenous molecule in the modulation of the extinction of conditioned fear memory, supporting the role of IGF-1 as a crucial piece in behavioral tasks." Unfortunately, the results do not support this central claim.

We would like to ask the authors to modify these claims and to provide some additional discussion on the relevance of the results to some conditions with elevated cerebral IGF1 levels.

2. The authors performed new experiments showing that inhibition of G protein activation by GDPβs prevents the reduction in mAHP, while had no effect on sAHP (Fig. 2F-G). First, in the Fig. 2G the mAHP seems to be altered as well. The p-value for this comparison is 0.06, and an additional single cell may render GDPβs ineffective at all at preventing the effect of IGF1 on AHP (6 cells per group seems rather low). Second, the authors wrote in their discussion: "In our study, inhibition of G protein-coupled receptors by GDPβs abolished the IGF-1-mediated reduction of mIAHP, suggesting that the effect of IGF-1 on mAHP could be mediated by the activation of mGluR5." This conclusion is far reaching since there are many other possible mechanisms may explain this effect. The authors should tone down this claim since no evidence on mGluR5 involvement is provided.

3. Figure 5, which is supposed to present mEPSC data, is missing.

eLife. 2021 Apr 1;10:e67267. doi: 10.7554/eLife.67267.sa2

Author response


[Editors’ note: the authors resubmitted a revised version of the paper for consideration. What follows is the authors’ response to the first round of review.]

Reviewer #1:

This work comes to show the role IGF1 plays in memory extinction. The authors suggest that IGF1 increases intrinsic excitability of L5P neurons in the infralimbic cortex (IL) by decreasing AHP. In addition, it attenuates both IPSCs and EPSCs by presynaptic mechanisms and facilitates a postsynaptic potentiation. Finally, the authors show that animals injected with IGF1 intracranially exhibit increased extinction of fear memory, and that IL-L5P neurons of these animals have reduced AHP current, increased excitability and decreased mEPSC frequency. The authors concluded that IGF1 regulates the cellular and synaptic processes underling fear memory extinction in the IL cortex.

1. Throughout the work the authors show that application of IGF1 causes the aforementioned effects, while they did not address the question whether IGF1 has a physiological role in excitability, synaptic transmission and behavioral phenotypes. It is unclear whether inhibition of IGF1R by NVP-AEW541 caused a decrease in mAHP and sAHP. Moreover, from Fig. S1 it looks like that the IGF1R antagonist has an inhibitory effect on fAHP. The authors should analyze the effect of IGF1R antagonist and show statistics for both, electrophysiological and behavioral phenotypes.

We are grateful to the reviewer for these perceptive comments. We have performed additional statistical analyses to address the reviewer´s concerns. We analysed the effect of IGF-1R antagonist and added the statistical values in the figures, figure legends and supplement table 1, for all experiments performed with IGF-1R antagonist (Figure 1, Figure 2, Figure Supplementary 1, and Figure 4). We did not observe significant differences between ACSF (control condition) and NVP, which discards an inhibitory effect on AHP (fast, medium and slow) by the antagonist of IGF1R (Supplementary Table 1).

2. The authors show that IGF1 application causes a long-term depression of sAHP, while did not identify the mechanism mediating it. It would be important to identify the channel involved in AHP regulation. Moreover, it would be important to show that block of this channel occludes an increase in I-F ratio. Otherwise, the authors should acknowledge that AHP changes and I-F changes could be mediated by distinct mechanisms.

As the reviewer suggested, we did experiments analysing the involvement of metabotropic receptors by blocking G protein activation. As shown in Figure 2F-G, preventing G protein activation by GDPbs in the patch pipette blockade of metabotropic receptor signaling and prevented the reduction of mAHP by IGF-1 while it had no effect on sAHP. These results indicate that the reduction of both AHPs currents would be mediated by different mechanisms and that metabotropic glutamate receptors could be responsible for the reduction of mAHP as previously demonstrated by Young et al. 2007 in the hippocampus.

3. The authors show that IGF1 increases PSP (Fig. 3I). They increased the intensity of stimulation to get some supra-threshold events in the middle of the protocol. The authors must show that this protocol does not cause a potentiation of PSP by itself, without addition of IGF1.

We agree and we show that the protocol does not cause any change in the PSP by itself, without addition of IGF-1 ( see new Figure supplementary 4).

4. Previous study found that extinction causes a decrease in fAHP, but not in sAHP in the IL (Santini et al., 2008). In contrast, conditioning up-regulated sAHP in that study. Moreover, a decrease in fAHP by M-channel inhibitor facilitated extinction (Santini and Porter, 2010). How do the authors explain this discrepancy between the results?

We have performed additional analysis to address the reviewer´s concerns.

First, we have performed analysed of fAHP from NAÏVE rats and all groups of extinction rats (EXT, Ext-Saline, Ext-IGF-1 and Ext-NVP+IGF-1) (new Figure supplementary 6A). The results show a reduction on fAHP in all animals that were exposed at extinction protocols, what is in agreement with previous study published by Santini et al. 2008. We could not include the conditioned group in the statistical analysis since we did not perform electrophysiology experiment in this group. Our object of study was the effect of IGF-1 on the fear extinction memory. Following the reviewer´s comment, we also analysed sAHP from all groups (new Figure supplementary 6C) and we find no significant differences among NAÏVE rats and all extinctions groups, except to IGF-1-treat group. Thus these results do not contradict the previous study published by Santini et. al. 2008. The significant reduction on sAHP from IGF-1-treat group compared with NAÏVE indicates an additional effect of IGF-1 on the extinction.

Reviewer #2:

This paper examines the effects of insulin-like growth factor-1 (IGF-1) on intrinsic excitability and synaptic transmission in neurons from the infralimbic cortex and on the extinction of fear memory. The authors report an increase in excitability mediated by the reduction of mAHP and a reduction in excitatory and inhibitory synaptic transmission. They also show that IGF-1 facilitates the extinction of fear memory.

This paper is interesting however, there are many issues. First, IGF-1 application is rather artificial and therefore this does not correspond to a natural way of stimulating neurons. In addition, some protocols and measures must be clarified.

Although we use a long lasting application of IGF-1 in the manuscript we have used shorter application showing similar results ( see new Figure supplementary 5). Moreover, in the CNS, it has been described that neuronal activity produces both an increase in IGF-1 release (Cao et al., 2011) and the entry of serum IGF-1 into the CNS through the blood– brain barrier (Nishijima et al., 2010). Therefore, during some physiological conditions in which serum IGF-1 levels are increased after physical exercises, a long-lasting increase in the brain areas showing neuronal AP activity is expected. The long lasting IGF-1 application in our manuscript was trying to mimic this natural condition. We have performed new analyses and the description of how the measurements were done has been explained in detail in the new version of the Materials and Methods section.

It is not clear what the authors precisely measured when the declare measuring mAHP and sAHP. In contrast with what seems inferred by the authors the medium and slow AHPs can be measured with the same depolarization. In addition, the sIAHP generally peaks at ~0.5-1.0 s while their records display only a current peaking at ~100 ms. So there is no sAHP in the data provided in Figure 1 and 2 and therefore, conclusion about the effect of IGF-1 on sAHP is erroneous.

We appreciate this constructive comment of the reviewer. We have included new analyses from electrophysiological experiments using the work of Santini and coworkers (Santini et al., 2008) as a reference. We have measured mIAHP as the current peak and sIAHP as the area under the curve for a 1-s period after the current peak. We add a new version of Figure 1, Figure 2, Figure Supplementary 3, and Figure 4. Also, we added a description of how these measurements were done in the Materials and Methods section. Our results show that IGF-1 leads to a reduction in the mAHP and sAHP in IL-L5PN in an IGF-1R activation-dependent manner.

Effects of IGF-1 on PSCs. The application of IGF-1 is very long (>30 min) and the test time after application is too short (~15 minutes) to really consider the depression as long-lasting. The authors should consider shorter application times (5 or 10 min) and longer test times (at least 30 min) to know whether the persistent effect is due or not to an incomplete elimination of IGF-1 applied in the bath.

We agree with the reviewer and we have performed new experiments in which IGF-1 was applied during 10 minutes (see new Figure supplementary 5). The results now clearly shows that IGF-1 induced a long-lasting potentiation of the synaptic transmission that persists after the complete elimination of IGF-1 in the bath .

Does the effect of IGF-1 on intrinsic excitability long-lasting? To test this, brief application of IGF-1 should be used too.

We performed new experiments and evaluated the intrinsic excitability of L5PNs of the IL after 10 min of IGF-1 application. We observed a slight increase in the excitability that is maintained at 60 min although it did not reach statistical significance. Therefore, we decided not to speculate about this possibility in the manuscript.

The rational of the experiment depicted in Fig. 3I en 3J is not clear at all. Please provide details why stimulus intensity is increased during 5 min to obtain 21% of supratheshold responses.

As we state in our response to the major point 1 of this reviewer, we were trying to mimic a possible situation in which AP based activity in L5PNs would induce the increase of the levels of IGF-1 and by entry of serum IGF-1 into the IL through the blood–brain barrier. Therefore, the rational of the experiment was to set an AP activity that did not induce plasticity per se but that could be used to induce it after the increase in the IGF-1 levels, trying to mimic the AP dependent entry of serum IGF-1 into a brain region due to its activity occurring in natural conditions.

Reviewer #3:

In this manuscript, the authors explore the involvement of IGF-1 in learning, focusing on fear extinction. They show that IGF-1 modulates network and intrinsic firing properties of L5, increasing firing frequency via inhibition of AHP and affecting network properties by reducing the release probability of presynaptic neurons. In addition, they demonstrate a concomitant increase of the firing probability in the postsynaptic L5 neurons. Intriguingly these effects develop rather slowly, with a plateau reached after ~40 min. They further show that in vivo administration of IGF-1 during the extinction phase results in enhanced fear extinction, correlating with the long-lasting effect, of over 24 h, of AHP inhibition and increased firing.

Overall these findings are interesting, linking behavioral and neuronal changes. Nevertheless, important experimental data, that were collected during the performed experiments, are not presented, leaving the reader with a partial view (see below in the comments). Moreover, some controls are missing (see below) and the number of cells in the electrophysiological recordings is rather low (n = 4/5/6 in many key figures).

1. Apart from changes in AHP, were there any additional changes in single AP properties? Did you observe any changes in AP threshold? Kinetics of upstroke or decay? Max amp? Duration? Input resistance?

The examples shown in Fig 1E depicts similar AHP with repetitive firing. If so, additional alteration in AP properties may be responsible for increased firing following IGF-1 application.

We have now analyzed the AP characteristics and there are no differences in all the parameters studied (new Figure supplementary 2A-G). However, the application of IGF-1 increased the membrane resistance (Rm) (see new Figure supplementary 2H), suggesting that IGF-1 reduces ionic conductances. This result is in agreement with the IGF-1-mediated reduction of mAHP and sAHP currents.

2. Fig. 1 - AHP amplitudes are presented as deltas, but the reference point is unclear, was it AP threshold? Also, since these are paired recordings, an illustration highlighting the pairs can be useful.

AHP amplitudes were measured as the membrane potential variation from the resting membrane potential. This has now been explained in the Materials and Methods section and the legend of Figure 1. There were not paired recordings. The recordings were performed before and after the IGF-1 application.

3. Were there differences in the time to max AHP? Were there differences between the % change in mAHP and sAHP for each cell? A similar % reduction in both may indicate a common mechanism, while the lack of correlation between mAHP and sAHP may hint that multiple mechanisms are at play. How long did you perfuse IGF-1? (~40 min?) what was the kinetics of this effect?

We thank the Reviewer for this insightful suggestion. We reanalyzed the data and measured the mAHP current at the peak of the current and the sAHP current as of the area under the curve during 1 s period beginning at the peak of the current after a depolarizing pulse of 800 ms, and these values were included in the new version of the manuscript. We also compared the percentage of change for mIAHP and sIAHP and there was no significant difference (32.8% vs. 39.5 %). IGF-1 was present in the bath for 40 min to measure its effect on the AHPs. The results reveal a slow kinetics reaching a stable reduction of AHP at 30 minutes (see Figure 2C,D and Figure supplementary 3 B-C).

4. Fig 1B, D. The variability seems to be lower in the NVP group compare to the ACSF group. Could that be attributed to the variable levels of endogenous IGF-1?

We have performed additional statistical analyses comparing ACSF vs NVP groups and we did not observe differences between them. This analysis has been included in the new version of the manuscript (see Supplementary Table 1).

5. Fig 1E. Did you observe AHP alterations during prolonged firing? the presented examples depict similar AHP levels after each AP and the potential at the end of the pulse is slightly more hyperpolarized after IGF1 addition, in contrast to Fig. 1A. Are there any other parameters that might support enhanced firing?

We have included a new representative example in figure 1G. As aforementioned (Essential revisions point 1), we observed no changes in AP characteristics, suggesting that the enhanced firing may be a consequence of the reduction in AHP currents.

6. How many rats were used for each data set? this is not indicated throughout the manuscript.

The number of rats used in each group of experiments has been included in figure legends. Now, the number of experiments is stated as the “number of cells/number of animals”.

7. The slow kinetics of the AHP and synaptic currents response to IGF1, as well as the long-lasting effect observed following IGF-1 washout (in slices and in vivo), should be discussed. Such slow kinetics may suggest the possible involvement of protein synthesis or insertion/endocytosis.

We now discuss this point in the new version of the manuscript (page 25): “Cohen-Matslish and coworkers showed that the AHP reduction induced by the synaptic activation requires protein synthesis during a specific time-window for its long-term maintenance (Cohen-Matsliah et al., 2010). It has been shown that the acquisition of extinction memory depends on protein synthesis (Santini et al., 2004). In our study, we show that IGF-1 favors the acquisition of extinction memory when applied directly to the IL in vivo. Therefore, it is tempting to hypothesize that the slow onset kinetics of AHP reduction that we observed after bath perfusion of IGF-1 in brain slices are suggestive of a mechanism involving protein synthesis, although further experiments would be needed to confirm it. Consistent with our observations in brain slices (plateau of AHP reduction is reached after 30 min), AHP amplitude is reduced in IL neurons from rats where the extinction memory was consolidated (recordings from the test day in IGF-1treated animals). Moreover, we found that a reduction in AHP amplitude was directly correlated with a reduction in the expression of fear, suggesting that mechanisms favoring a greater AHP reduction would produce an enhancement of the acquisition of extinction memory”.

8. Line 273 - did you mean "did not require depolarization"? Sup Fig 2 depicts sAHP reduction of over 60% without depolarization. The time courses, with or without stimulation, are somewhat different, but the endpoints (after 50 min) are similar (showing maybe even a bit more inhibition without depolarization). Thus, depolarization may facilitate the process, but is not a prerequisite.

We have corrected this issue in the new version of Figure supplementary 2, now Figure supplementary 3. We represent in the same graph the time course of the AHPs with a and without evoking the AHP during the application of IGF-1 during the first 20 min. We show that the % reduction achieved at 60 min is not statistically different. However, the time course is altered and there is a shift to the right indicating that depolarization is not a prerequisite but facilitates the modulation.

9. Lines 288-290 - "Additionally, this modulation was not observed when the synaptic stimulation was absent (Figure 3D), suggesting that the evoked synaptic responses were required for this LTD of the EPSCs". This statement is confusing and is not supported by the data. Comparing Fig. 3A and 3D, the endpoints (after 50 min) are similar, with inhibition levels of 30-40%. Thus, synaptic stimulation during IGF-1 application is not required for presynaptic LTD. Despite that, as for the kinetics of AHP, depolarization may in fact facilitate this effect. Thus, two mechanisms may coexist: one that supports EPSC inhibition with synaptic activation, with faster kinetics that plateaus within 10 min of IGF1 application, and another slower process, independent of synaptic activation, which plateaus at 50 min. This point requires further clarification and discussion.

10. For IPSCs, the kinetics is similar with or without stimulation. But, the level of inhibition after 50 min is doubled without synaptic stimulation. These properties should also be further discussed.

After performing a new analysis, we agree with the reviewer on the coexistence of two mechanisms for the LTD of both the EPSC and IPSC. This possibility is now discussed in the new version of the manuscript accordingly (page 23): “Interestingly, we observed that two mechanisms coexist in the development of the LTD of EPSCs and IPSCs following IGF-1 application. The first one is dependent on synaptic activation and is characterized by a fast onset, the plateau being achieved at 10-15 min after IGF-1 application. The second one is independent of synaptic activation and is characterized by a slower onset, achieving the plateau at 40 min after IGF-1 application”.

Moreover we included the statistical analysis comparing the inhibition levels in the Figure 3A and 3D for EPSC and Figure 3E and 3H for IPSC (see Supplementary Table 1).

11. Fig 3I-J - These results are very interesting and important, as mimicking of firing by synaptic activation takes into account changes in multiple presynaptic neurons (excitatory and feedforward inhibition), as well as postsynaptic changes. Indeed, firing in response to current injection into the soma is less physiological. Moreover, SK channels in different cellular locations were shown to confer contradicting effects on EPSPs amplitude and duration, and thus on neuronal excitability (studied by the Stuart lab as well as others). However, valuable information is missing here as well. Did you see changes in PSP duration? changes in feedforward inhibition? AP properties? do you see similar AHP inhibition also with synaptic activation?

We have shown the lack of effect of IGF-1 in the AP properties and PSP duration (see Figure 3). However we did not study the modulation of the AHP evoked by synaptic activation nor the possible changes in the feedforward inhibition.

12. The opposing effects observed in voltage clamp and current clamp modes (reduced EPSP amplitude with increased PSP depolarization) need to be discussed. While one possibility for this can be a change in the input resistance, another may reside in alterations of the interplay between dendritic SK and NMDA channels (see Bock et al. 2019).

We have now discussed the possible mechanisms for the PSP potentiation in the manuscript (page 23): “However, the increase in the input resistance induced by IGF-1 could also explain the resultant LTP of the PSPs nonetheless of the reduction in the release of neurotransmitters. Since SK channels have been shown to modulate NMDA receptors (Jones et al., 2017; Ngo-Anh et al., 2005), we cannot discard a putative IGF-1dependent modulation of SK channels as the source of the potentiation in the synaptic transmission. The resultant LTP of the PSPs and the increase in the input resistance summed to the decrease of the AHPs would result in the increase of the L5PN activity of the IL and its effect on the fear extinction memory”.

13. Also, the mechanism for enhanced firing with synaptic stimulation may be different from increased firing with current injection. In addition to the site of depolarization, synaptic stimulations were given at low freq , as oppose the high frequency firing with prolonged depolarization. While the rate of synaptic stimulation is not specified for the current clamp experiments shown (should be added please), the graphical representations suggest 1 stim/min.

Recordings were done at 0.33 Hz (1 stim each 3-s) and the average response of 20 recordings (1 min) was represented (e.g., Figure 3I). It is now stated in the Material and Methods section.

14. What was the duration of IGF-1 application here? The methods section indicates 15 min, but the graph and legend specify 35 min. The kinetics for PSP potentiation seems to be faster, compared to the VC experiments. Any indication of the underlying cause?

Thank you for noticing this inconsistence. We have now clarified that the duration of IGF-1 application was 35 minutes (see page 8, lines 139 and 141) in both voltage-clamp (VC) and current-clamp (CC). The development over time of PSP potentiation seems to be faster in CC compared to VC, which could be explained because of the increase in the membrane resistance (see page 14, line 262, and Figure supplementary 2H).

15. Overall the method section is missing some vital information. Was the age of the rats used for behavioral experiments also P20 - P30? were the surgeries performed at P20? with 7 days recovery the behavioral experiments supposedly spanned P28 - 31, correct? what were the intervals between each step of the trials (cond, ext, test)?

Material and Methods section has been extensively rewritten in the new version of the manuscript. For behavioral experiments, rats underwent surgery at postnatal day 21 (after weaning) and were allowed to recover for 7-9 days. Therefore, behavioral tests were started at postnatal day 28-30 and finished at postnatal day 32-34. This same day (P32-P34), animals were sacrificed, and the electrophysiological recordings were done. The interval between each step of the trial was 24 h. All this information is now stated in the Material and Methods section.

16. Fig 4 - What was the effect of NVP alone? this control is vital. IGF-1R was shown to be fully activated at baseline conditions (Gazit et al. 2016), thus, with NVP and IGF-1, some R might still be functional and activated by IGF1, resulting in partial inhibition by NVP. Moreover, the main conclusion here seems to be drawn from the relatively low levels of fear extinction in the saline group (note the typo - extintion). Comparing the data in Fig 4 to Sup Fig 3, sup Fig 3 illustrates what seems to be further extinction during the extinction phase, as well as during the test phase. Reduction in freezing is missing in the saline group shown in Fig 4. What might reconcile these observations?

Although we agree with the reviewer that the application of IGF-1 and NVP could result in partial inhibition of IGF-1R by NVP, this partial inhibition is sufficient to demonstrate the involvement of IGF-1R activation in the modulation caused by IGF-1. We agree that the saline group shows a relatively low levels of fear extinction, however IGF-1 is able to increase this levels of fear extinction.

17. Was there a correlation between the sAHP amplitude and freezing behavior? One might expect lower sAHP amp in rats with better extinction levels.

We have analyzed the correlation between the mAHP/sAHP and the percentage of freezing (see new Figure 4F). The correlation parameters are R = 0.702 for mAHP and R = 0.787 sAHP. This correlation is now discussed in the manuscript (page 25): “Moreover, we found that a reduction in AHP amplitude was directly correlated with a reduction in the expression of fear, suggesting that mechanisms favoring a greater AHP reduction would produce an enhancement of the acquisition of extinction memory”.

18. Did you also observe lower levels of mAHP after in vivo application of IGF1/NVP? any effect on fAHP? is it possible to see a further reduction of sAHP with acute bath application of IGF-1 in mice that were pre-treated with IGF-1 during fear extinction? this would reveal the dynamic range of this long-lasting effect.

a. We did not observe lower levels of mAHP after in vivo application ofNVP+ IGF-1 but we did observe lower levels of mAHP in the in vivo IGF-1 group (see new Figure 4D).

b. There were no significant differences between in vivo fAHP groups where the extinction protocol was performed (see Figure supplementary 6).

c. Although a further reduction of sAHP with acute bath application ofIGF-1 in mice that were pre-treated with IGF-1 during fear extinction would add new information about the dynamic range of the long-lasting effect of IGF-1 in the modulation of the sAHPs, in our opinion this is out of the scope of the present manuscript.

19. The variability in mEPSC is large, especially in the IGF-1 group. Recordings from more cells can fix that.

Although we agree with the reviewer that the application of IGF-1 and NVP could result in partial inhibition of IGF-1R by NVP, this partial inhibition is sufficient to demonstrate the involvement of IGF-1R activation in the modulation caused by IGF-1. We agree that the saline group shows a relatively low levels of fear extinction, however IGF-1 is able to increase this levels of fear extinction.

20. The molecular mechanism is not sufficiently addressed. The authors suggest that SK-, m-currents or mGluR5 might be involved. However, no direct evidence for the specific involvement of these channels is provided, nor any explanation for the slow and long-lasting effect observed.

We have analyzed the correlation between the mAHP/sAHP and the percentage of freezing (see new Figure 4F). The correlation parameters are R = 0.702 for mAHP and R = 0.787 sAHP. This correlation is now discussed in the manuscript (page 25): “Moreover, we found that a reduction in AHP amplitude was directly correlated with a reduction in the expression of fear, suggesting that mechanisms favoring a greater AHP reduction would produce an enhancement of the acquisition of extinction memory”.

[Editors’ note: what follows is the authors’ response to the second round of review.]

The manuscript has been improved but there are some remaining issues that need to be addressed, as outlined below:

1. The authors performed new experiments showing that inhibition of IGF1Rs occludes the effects of exogenously applied IGF1, but does not cause any effect on neuronal/synaptic function and memory extinction. This raises the question whether endogenous IGF1 plays a physiological role in these processes? In the discussion the authors claim that "....IGF-1 appears as a key endogenous molecule in the modulation of the extinction of conditioned fear memory, supporting the role of IGF-1 as a crucial piece in behavioral tasks." Unfortunately, the results do not support this central claim.

We would like to ask the authors to modify these claims and to provide some additional discussion on the relevance of the results to some conditions with elevated cerebral IGF1 levels.

In agreement with the reviewer, we have modified the claim as follows: “Therefore, our results show a novel functional consequence of IGF-1 signalling on animal behavior in the mPFC through the modulation of the extinction of conditioned fear memory”. (page 11, line 215)

We have also provided an additional discussion of the relevance of our results to elevation of cerebral IGF-1 levels observed after running. We have included the following paragraph: “Functional relevance of the IGF-1 modulation in the mPFC. IGF-1 is actively transported to the central nervous system from plasma through the choroid plexus (Carro et al., 2000), and it is also locally produced in the brain by neurons and glial cells (Quesada et al., 2007; Suh et al., 2013; Rodriguez-Perez et al., 2016). Interestingly, the uptake of IGF-1 by the brain correlates with frequency-dependent changes in cerebral blood flow in the cortex during information processing (Nishijima et al., 2010). Therefore, the levels of IGF-1 in the IL would depend on both its active transport from the plasma, favoured by the activity involved in information processing, and in the IGF-1 locally produced by neurons and astrocytes of the IL. Thus, high levels of IGF-1 are expected in the IL because its high activity during the extinction of fear conditioned (Milad and Quirk, 2002; Sepulveda-Orengo et al., 2013). Our results demonstrate that a further increase in the IGF-1 levels due to the exogenous application of IGF-1 in the IL favour the extinction of conditioned fear. In natural conditions, these high levels of IGF-1 could be expected after physical exercises because it is known that IGF-1 uptake from the plasma and IGF-1 levels in the brain are increased after running, reaching the higher levels (Carro et al., 2000). Thus, our results could be related with some of the benefits of physical exercise on the brain function by increasing neuronal excitability and favouring synaptic plasticity and learning and memory through the activation of the IGF-1Rs”. (page 17, line 352)

2. The authors performed new experiments showing that inhibition of G protein activation by GDPβs prevents the reduction in mAHP, while had no effect on sAHP (Fig. 2F-G). First, in the Fig. 2G the mAHP seems to be altered as well. The p-value for this comparison is 0.06, and an additional single cell may render GDPβs ineffective at all at preventing the effect of IGF1 on AHP (6 cells per group seems rather low). Second, the authors wrote in their discussion: "In our study, inhibition of G protein-coupled receptors by GDPβs abolished the IGF-1-mediated reduction of mIAHP, suggesting that the effect of IGF-1 on mAHP could be mediated by the activation of mGluR5." This conclusion is far reaching since there are many other possible mechanisms may explain this effect. The authors should tone down this claim since no evidence on mGluR5 involvement is provided.

We agree with the reviewer. We have increased the number of experiments studying the effect of GDPβs (n = 8) and data confirms that GDPβs prevents the IGF-1-mediated reduction in mAHP (p = 0.109), while had no effect on sAHP reduction (p = 0.023) (Supplementary Table 1).

In agreement with the reviewer, we have tone down this claim by replacing it by the following conclusion: “In our study, inhibition of G protein-coupled receptors by GDPβs abolished the IGF-1-mediated reduction of mIAHP, suggesting that the effect of IGF-1 on mAHP is mediated by the activation of metabotropic receptors. One candidate are the mGluRs, since it has been demonstrated that the activation of mGluR5 increases IL-L5PN excitability (Fontanez-Nuin et al., 2011) and modulates the recall of extinction (Fontanez-Nuin et al., 2011; Sepulveda-Orengo et al., 2013) in a similar way to what we describe for IGF-1. However, we can't rule out the involvement of other GPCRs and a further study is required to analyse the type of metabotropic receptor and the signalling pathway mediating the reduction of the mAHP by the IGF-1”. (page 12, line 246)

3. Figure 5, which is supposed to present mEPSC data, is missing.

The reviewer is completely right. We have now included the missed Figure 5.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Figure 1—source data 1. Source data for Figure 1: IGF-1 increases the excitability of IL-L5PNs.
    Figure 1—figure supplement 1—source data 1. Source data for Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

    IGF-1 has no effect on fAHP and fIAHP.

    Figure 1—figure supplement 2—source data 1. Source data for Figure 1—figure supplement 1: Action potential properties are unaltered by IGF-1.
    Figure 2—source data 1. Source data for Figure 2.

    IGF-1 reduces mIAHP and sIAHP in IL-L5PNs.

    Figure 2—figure supplement 1—source data 1. Source data for Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

    Time course of mIAHP and sIAHP.

    Figure 3—source data 1. Source data for Figure 3.

    IGF-1 induces long-term potentiation of the synaptic transmission.

    Figure 3—figure supplement 1—source data 1. Source data for Figure 3—figure supplement 1.

    The protocol of stimulation does not induce plasticity.

    Figure 3—figure supplement 2—source data 1. Source data for Figure 3—figure supplement 2.

    The application of IGF-1 is sufficient to induce plasticity.

    Figure 4—source data 1. Source data for Figure 4.

    IGF-1 facilitates the extinction memory by reducing mIAHP and sIAHP.

    Figure 4—figure supplement 1—source data 1. Source data for Figure 4—figure supplement 1.

    Protocol of behavior.

    Figure 4—figure supplement 2—source data 1. Source data for Figure 4—figure supplement 2.

    AHPs of the L5PN of the IL recorded from animals after fear extinction.

    Figure 5—source data 1. Source data for Figure 5: IGF-1 decreases the frequency of mEPSC.
    Supplementary file 1. Summary of statistical significance and tests used in each figure.
    elife-67267-supp1.docx (107.6KB, docx)
    Transparent reporting form

    Data Availability Statement

    All data generated or analysed during this study are included in the manuscript and supporting files.


    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES