Skip to main content
PLOS Medicine logoLink to PLOS Medicine
. 2021 Apr 19;18(4):e1003578. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1003578

Housing environment and early childhood development in sub-Saharan Africa: A cross-sectional analysis

Yaqing Gao 1, Long Zhang 2, Ashish Kc 3, Yinping Wang 1, Siyu Zou 1, Chunyi Chen 1, Yue Huang 1, Xiaoyi Mi 1, Hong Zhou 1,*
Editor: Lars Åke Persson4
PMCID: PMC8092764  PMID: 33872322

Abstract

Background

The influence of the safety and security of environments on early childhood development (ECD) has been under-explored. Although housing might be linked to ECD by affecting a child’s health and a parent’s ability to provide adequate care, only a few studies have examined this factor. We hypothesized that housing environment is associated with ECD in sub-Saharan Africa (SSA).

Methods and findings

From 92,433 children aged 36 to 59 months who participated in Multiple Indicator Cluster Survey (MICS) in 20 SSA countries, 88,271 were tested for cognitive and social–emotional development using the Early Childhood Development Index (ECDI) questionnaire and were thus included in this cross-sectional analysis. Children’s mean age was 47.2 months, and 49.8% were girls. Children were considered developmentally on track in a certain domain if they failed no more than 1 ECDI item in that domain. In each country, we used conditional logistic regression models to estimate the association between improved housing (housing with finished building materials, improved drinking water, improved sanitation facilities, and sufficient living area) and children’s cognitive and social–emotional development, accounting for contextual effects and socioeconomic factors. Estimates from each country were pooled using random-effects meta-analyses. Subgroup analyses were conducted by the child’s gender, maternal education, and household wealth quintiles. On-track cognitive development was associated with improved housing (odds ratio [OR] = 1.15, 95% CI 1.06 to 1.24, p < 0.001), improved drinking water (OR = 1.07, 95% CI 1.00 to 1.14, p = 0.046), improved sanitation facilities (OR = 1.15, 95% CI 1.03 to 1.28, p = 0.014), and sufficient living area (OR = 1.06, 95% CI 1.01 to 1.10, p = 0.018). On-track social–emotional development was associated with improved housing only in girls (OR = 1.14, 95% CI 1.04 to 1.25, p = 0.006). The main limitations of this study included the cross-sectional nature of the datasets and the use of the ECDI, which lacks sensitivity to measure ECD outcomes.

Conclusions

In this study, we observed that improved housing was associated with on-track cognitive development and with on-track social–emotional development in girls. These findings suggest that housing improvement in SSA may be associated not only with benefits for children’s physical health but also with broader aspects of healthy child development.


In a cross-sectional analysis of nationally representative survey data from 20 countries, Yaqing Gao and colleagues investigate associations between housing environment and cognitive and social development in children.

Author summary

Why was this study done?

  • Sub-Saharan Africa (SSA) had the largest number of children (29.4 million) with low cognitive and/or social–emotional development worldwide in 2010.

  • Housing might be linked to early childhood development (ECD) by affecting a child’s health and a parent’s ability to provide adequate care but is neglected in public health, especially in low- and middle-income countries (LMIC).

What did the researchers do and find?

  • We tested the hypothesis that improved housing is associated with on-track development in 2 ECD domains (cognitive and social–emotional).

  • We analyzed data from 88,271 children aged 36 to 59 months in 20 SSA countries collected from nationally representative surveys conducted between 2010 and 2019.

  • Improved housing was associated with 15% higher odds of being developmentally on track in the cognitive domain. Overall, improved housing was associated with on-track social–emotional development in girls but not boys.

What do these findings mean?

  • Housing improvement in SSA may be associated not only with benefits for children’s physical health but also with broader aspects of healthy child development.

  • Future work to establish a causal link between housing and ECD and to assess its underlying mechanisms is crucial for the designing of cost-effective ECD interventions in SSA.

Introduction

Early childhood development (ECD) refers to the development of core age-appropriate perceptual, physical, cognitive, social–emotional, and language skills [1], which influences individual academic, behavioral, and economic accomplishments [2]. In 2010, an estimated 80.8 million children in low- and middle-income countries (LMIC), aged 36 to 59 months, were not on track for cognitive and/or social–emotional development [3]. The Nurturing Care Framework demonstrated that, to reach their full developmental potential, children need 5 components of nurturing care: good health, adequate nutrition, responsive caregiving, opportunities for learning, and safety and security [4]. Most ECD research investigating risk factors and interventions focused on nutritional supplementation, responsive care, and learning opportunities [5], yet the safety and security of children’s environments has received little attention.

There is a lack of research on the influence of housing, as a central pillar of human security, on ECD. The hypothesized mechanisms of the association between housing and ECD are as follows. First, improved housing quality can reduce the exposure to biological (e.g., mold and pests), physical (e.g., noise, extreme temperatures, and poor ventilation), and chemical hazards (e.g., lead and arsenic) [6], which might result in negative health consequences for children [7]. Cross-sectional studies conducted in sub-Saharan Africa (SSA) showed that children living in improved housing (defined as housing with improved drinking water and sanitation facilities, sufficient living area, and finished building materials) had a lower likelihood of malaria, stunting, diarrhea, and anemia [8,9]. Improved physical health, in turn, may benefit the developing brain [10]. Second, a longitudinal study in the United States showed that children living in homes with more rooms and higher levels of cleanliness and safety experienced less physiological stress [11], which might improve their emotional stability and learning [12]. Third, improved housing quality, such as less dampness and warmth, may also reduce mental health problems and stress in parents [13], promoting their ability to engage in stimulating activities with their children. A longitudinal study in the US indicated that mothers’ psychological distress and parenting stress were important mediators in the associations between poor housing quality and children’s internalizing and externalizing problems [12]. Research on the direct associations between housing quality and ECD has been limited and mostly conducted in the US and Europe [7,14]. For example, a longitudinal study carried out in the US showed that children living in a house with environmental deficiencies such as a leaking roof, broken windows, and rodents had lower average reading and math skills [12]. The association between housing and ECD in LMIC, which have more severe housing and ECD problems than high-income countries, remains uncertain.

Africa has the highest rate of population growth and the most acute need for better housing worldwide [15]. The unprecedented modernization and economic growth in Africa have provided a significant opportunity to build healthier homes. Indeed, the prevalence of improved housing doubled in SSA from 2000 to 2015 [16]. However, an unacceptably large proportion of individuals still live in unimproved housing [16]. SSA had the largest number of children (29.4 million) experiencing low cognitive and/or social–emotional development in 2010 [3]. Thus, providing evidence on the association between housing and ECD may shed light upon a possibility to facilitate a faster attainment of the Sustainable Development Goals that advocate for ensuring universal achievement of appropriate ECD and access to adequate, safe, and affordable housing by 2030 [17].

In this study, we used Multiple Indicator Cluster Survey (MICS) data from 20 countries in SSA to test the hypothesis that improved housing is associated with a higher odds of being developmentally on track in either of the 2 ECD domains (i.e., cognitive or social–emotional) among children aged 36 to 59 months. Furthermore, we assessed the heterogeneity of the associations between subpopulations, based on the child’s gender, maternal education, and household wealth, which might influence ECD and/or housing quality [3,18]. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first comprehensive study examining the relationship between housing and ECD in SSA to date.

Methods

This study is reported as per the Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) guideline (S1 Checklist).

Protocol

The prospective analysis plan is included as S1 Analysis Plan. During the peer review process, we made several changes to the analysis plan as per the reviewers’ suggestions: (1) changes in the outcomes, including the removal of the literacy–numeracy domain of the ECDI and replacement of the term “delay” with “on-track” to describe the outcomes; (2) changes in the a priori confounding variables, including adding the availability of books and playthings and treating the age of the child and the child’s household wealth index as linear variables instead of dichotomizing them; and (3) changes in the statistical analysis by applying the conditional logistic model instead of the standard logistic model. This list of changes is also included at the end of S1 Analysis Plan.

Data source

MICS are nationally representative cross-sectional household surveys carried out in LMIC every 5 years [19]. MICS data of SSA countries were included in the analysis if (1) data were from the most recent round of MICS; (2) information on housing quality and ECD was included; and (3) data were publicly available on the MICS website (http://mics.unicef.org/surveys) before May 2020. Registration and login details were required to access the data.

MICS data are internationally comparable due to the use of standardized and validated questionnaires and a uniform 2-stage sampling design. The first stage was cluster formation through the probability proportional to size sampling, based on the most recent national census. In the second stage, the random sampling method was used to select a predetermined number (20 to 30) of households from the clusters. Standardized face-to-face interviews were conducted to obtain information from the household members [19].

Outcomes

ECD was measured by the Early Childhood Development Index (ECDI), which is a questionnaire introduced to the MICS since 2010. The ECDI provides the largest reliable ECD data of children aged 36 to 59 months in LMIC [20]. The 10-item ECDI was developed through pilot tests of the 18-item ECDI in the Philippines, Kenya, and Jordan, which was validated with a Cronbach’s alpha of 0.7 or above on all 4 domains (cognitive, approaches to learning, emotional, and language) [20], as well as through revisions by child development experts [20]. According to the confirmatory factor analysis conducted with children aged 36 to 59 months in 35 LMIC, ECDI demonstrated adequate model fit for the original domains proposed by ECDI developers [3].

The ECDI has 10 items, divided into 4 developmental domains: physical, learning/cognition, social–emotional, and literacy–numeracy [20]. S1 Table presents the complete ECDI questionnaire. Children were classified as developmentally on-track in a certain domain if they failed no more than 1 ECDI item in that domain [20].

To be consistent with previous studies evaluating the global ECD status and exploring the factors that affect ECD [3,21,22], we only selected the development in the cognitive and social–emotional domains as outcomes. Skills covered by these domains are core developmental milestones for children aged 36 to 59 months, such as following commands, independence, and concentration [23].

The physical and literacy–numeracy domains were excluded from this study for several reasons. The pre-academic knowledge, assessed in the literacy–numeracy domain, is substantially more advanced than the literacy–numeracy skills assessed in other questionnaires for this age range (e.g., the Ages & Stages Questionnaire) [24,25]. Therefore, those items are more likely to reflect social norms around early education than children’s developmental capacity [3]. The development of pincer grasp, assessed in the physical domain, was usually achieved before 12 months of age [26]. Consequently, the sensitivity of this item was low [3], resulting in an extremely large number of children being developmentally on track in the physical domain, which was difficult for conducting statistical modeling [27].

Exposures

We defined improved housing based on the definition developed by the United Nations [28,29]. Accordingly, an improved housing has all of the 4 following characteristics: (1) Access to improved water source; (2) Access to improved sanitation facilities; (3) Sufficient living area; and (4) Housing durability. The fifth characteristic “Security of tenure” proposed by the United Nations was excluded from our definition because of the lack of internationally comparable data [16]. Our definition was aligned with 2 recent studies that explored changes in housing and their impact on child health in SSA [9,16].

Table A in S1 Text shows the measurement criteria for the improved water source and sanitation facilities, as outlined by the World Health Organization and United Nations Children’s Fund Joint Monitoring Programme for Water Supply, Sanitation and Hygiene [30]. Living area was considered sufficient for household members if no more than 3 people shared the same habitable room [31]. A house was considered to have a housing durability if at least 2 out of 3 of the materials for the walls, roof, and floor were finished [9,16]. Table B in S1 Text presents the MICS classifications of finished and unfinished materials.

A priori confounding variables

To reduce confounding bias, we included the following covariates: age (months) and gender of the child, maternal education, household wealth index, and the availability of children’s books and playthings. The age of the child was included as a continuous variable. Maternal education was included as a categorical variable, with different classifications across the countries. Most of the countries categorized women into “no education,” “primary education,” and “secondary or higher education” based on the highest education level they achieved. In Lesotho, women with no education were combined with those who had only basic primary education. In Ghana, maternal education was classified into none, primary, middle, and secondary or higher. In Mauritania, the categories were none, Koranic, primary, and secondary or higher. In Nigeria, the categories were none, nonformal, primary, and secondary or higher.

The construction of the wealth index in MICS includes selecting a basket of asset indicator variables, running a principal component analysis, and calculating wealth scores that are based on the first component of the principal component analysis and assigning the score to household members [32]. However, the standard asset indicator variables usually include housing materials, water, and sanitation [32]. A bespoke wealth index excluding these exposure variables is recommended by MICS to avoid concerns relating to tautology, which would bias the estimate toward null [33]. Aligned with previous studies [9,16], we selected the ownerships of the following durable assets as the asset indicator variables: (1) car; (2) motorboat; (3) scooter; (4) cart; (5) bicycle; (6) television; (7) refrigerator; (8) radio; (9) watch; (10) mobile telephone; (11) landline telephone; and (12) electrification of the household. These variables were commonly used in MICS to construct the wealth index [32]. In each country, we excluded indicators where <5% or >95% of households owned the asset. The tailored wealth score was included as a continuous variable as a covariate [9]. When we conducted subgroup analyses according to household wealth, the wealth score was not included in the model as a covariate. The total household population for each country was divided into quintiles based on their tailored wealth score, arranged from poorest (wealth quintile 1) to richest (wealth quintile 5). Children were divided into those belonging to the lower 2 quintiles and those belonging to the upper 3 quintiles according to the wealth quintile of the household they were living in.

We controlled for the availability of children’s books and playthings to account for the variability in individual learning resources [18,34]. The availability of children’s books was defined as having 3 or more children’s books at home in the MICS indicator list [34]. However, the median number of books at home was 0 in all the 20 countries. Therefore, we set the threshold for the availability of children’s books as the presence of at least 1 children’s book or picture book at home, which was consistent with the previous literature [35]. The availability of playthings was defined as the availability of at least 2 types of the following 3 types of playthings at home: (1) homemade toys; (2) toys from a shop or manufactured toys; and (3) household objects or objects found outside [34].

Statistical analysis

In each country, we applied a conditional logistic model to investigate the relationships between housing and ECD. In conditional logistic regression, children living in different housing environments were only compared within the same cluster. Therefore, this approach minimized neighborhood-level confounding factors, such as availability of education services, ECD programs, social norms, and other environmental determinants [8,9].

The outcome of interest was the on-track development in each of the 2 ECD domains, and the exposures were one of the 4 improved housing characteristics, and improved housing as a combined variable. First, we ran a set of models for each individual characteristic of improved housing on the ECD outcomes. The combined variable (improved housing) considering the 4 characteristics was analyzed in a similar manner. Then, the 4 characteristics of improved housing were explored simultaneously in the same model to quantify the extent of the contribution made by each characteristic. In all models, we controlled the same set of a priori confounding variables.

The overall rate of missing data in most countries was below 5%, except for Sierra Leone (5.0%), Mauritania (9.9%), and Chad (10.7%). Missingness was not >10% for any variable in any country (S2 Table). With the assumption of missing completely at random, we used a complete case approach to analyze the data, which is consistent with previous research [9]. Therefore, only children without any missing information on the ECDI, confounding variables, and all the exposures were included in the model.

We assessed the heterogeneity of the estimated associations among countries using the I2 index [36]. We then conducted random-effects meta-analyses with the DerSimonian and Laird method [37] to relate the country-specific odds ratio (OR) of improved housing on ECD to the pooled estimates for the 20 SSA countries.

To further reveal the heterogeneity of the association between improved housing and the odds of being developmentally on track in each ECD domain across different populations, we examined the factors that were expected to impact ECD and/or housing [3,18], including the child’s gender, maternal education (having completed less than secondary education versus secondary or higher education), and household wealth (belonging to the lower 2 versus upper 3 wealth quintiles). We incorporated the 2-way interaction term between improved housing and an additional factor in each country. We computed the expected OR for the association between improved housing and on-track development in each ECD domain for each level of the factor. The overall OR for each subgroup was calculated using a random-effects model. In each country, the interaction was assessed on a multiplicative scale by calculating the OR of the interaction term between improved housing and the factor [38], which was then pooled for all countries using a random-effects model.

A p-value of 0.05 was considered as significant, with all analyses being conducted in SPSS version 25.0 and R version 3.6.1.

Ethics statement

No human participant work or field research was conducted as part of this secondary data analysis. All data used are publicly available and fully de-identified. Ethical approval for the MICS was provided by individual review boards within each participating country at the time of survey implementation.

Results

Study population

Data were extracted from surveys in 20 countries dating from 2010 to 2019. From the total population of 92,433 children aged 36 to 59 months surveyed, complete outcomes, covariates, and exposures data were available for 88,271 children (Table 1). Across all surveys, the children’s average age was 47.2 months, and 49.8% were girls (Table 2). The proportion of rural residence ranged from 49.8% in Benin to 88.9% in Malawi. Zimbabwe, Congo, and Eswatini were the only 3 countries where the proportion of mothers with less than secondary education was lower than 50%. In Chad, 94.4% of the children’s mothers did not complete secondary education. Children’s books were less accessible than having at least 2 types of playthings. The proportion of children having no book at home ranged from 74.3% in Nigeria to 98.0% in DR Congo. The proportion of children having less than 2 types of playthings at home ranged from 21.1% in Zimbabwe to 62.9% in Guinea-Bissau.

Table 1. Survey characteristics of the 20 countries included in the analysis.

Country MICS round Year of investigation Number of children aged 36–59 months (N) Number of children aged 36–59 months without missing information (N)a
Benin MICS5 2014 4,880 4,709
Côte d’Ivoire MICS5 2016 3,730 3,598
Cameroon MICS5 2014 2,846 2,739
Chad MICS4 2010 7,139 6,375
Congo MICS5 2015 3,675 3,526
DR Congo MICS6 2018 8,704 8,453
Eswatini MICS5 2014 1,091 1,038
Gambia MICS6 2018 4,304 4,115
Ghana MICS6 2018 3,668 3,521
Guinea MICS5 2016 3,164 3,099
Guinea-Bissau MICS5 2014 2,970 2,849
Lesotho MICS6 2018 1,326 1,292
Madagascar MICS6 2018 5,142 5,004
Malawi MICS5 2014 7,839 7,536
Mali MICS5 2015 6,550 6,318
Mauritania MICS5 2015 4,446 4,005
Nigeria MICS5 2013 11,648 11,101
Sierra Leone MICS6 2014 4,810 4,568
Togo MICS6 2011 1,989 1,946
Zimbabwe MICS6 2019 2,512 2,479
Total - - 92,433 88,271

aNumber of children without missing information on ECDI, confounding variables, and exposures.

ECDI, Early Childhood Development Index; MICS, Multiple Indicator Cluster Survey.

Table 2. Demographic characteristics of study participants.

Country Number of children aged 36–59 months without missing information (N)a Mean age (months) Girls (%) Rural residence (%) Mother completed less than secondary education (%) Having no book at home (%) Having less than 2 types of playthings at home (%)
Benin 4,709 47.4 50.5 49.8 86.5 89.8 43.1
Côte d’Ivoire 3,598 46.8 48.9 74.0 90.4 93.8 47.6
Cameroon 2,739 46.8 50.7 55.1 67.4 82.1 34.7
Chad 6,375 48.0 50.5 62.7 94.4 96.6 40.0
Congo 3,526 48.2 48.7 69.6 46.0 89.8 43.8
DR Congo 8,453 46.7 51.4 73.3 64.7 98.0 58.1
Eswatini 1,038 47.4 48.7 85.4 47.8 82.6 22.7
Gambia 4,115 47.5 48.5 63.4 79.0 94.6 43.0
Ghana 3,521 47.2 50.9 60.2 87.1 76.0 43.5
Guinea 3,099 47.3 48.4 70.3 88.3 95.5 62.2
Guinea-Bissau 2,849 47.0 50.4 72.9 89.4 97.3 62.9
Lesotho 1,292 47.8 50.5 75.5 58.1 89.9 34.3
Madagascar 5,004 47.3 49.5 77.2 79.1 94.6 40.5
Malawi 7,536 47.3 50.2 88.9 85.1 95.3 46.0
Mali 6,318 46.3 48.5 78.3 90.0 96.4 34.6
Mauritania 4,005 46.8 50.6 57.1 86.4 91.7 56.6
Nigeria 11,101 47.2 49.2 73.5 65.4 74.3 44.5
Sierra Leone 4,568 47.3 49.7 71.3 80.4 91.7 50.7
Togo 1,946 47.2 46.7 69.9 75.0 94.1 54.9
Zimbabwe 2,479 47.6 50.0 70.3 38.1 86.0 21.1
Total 88,271 47.2 49.8 70.6 77.0 90.5 45.5

aNumber of children without missing information on ECDI, confounding variables, and exposures.

ECDI, Early Childhood Development Index.

Improved housing

Across the 20 countries, the median prevalence of improved housing was 18.4%, ranging from 39.8% in Zimbabwe to 3.6% in Madagascar. The median prevalence of finished building materials, improved drinking water, improved sanitation facilities, and sufficient living area were 61.5%, 70.5%, 43.8%, and 63.2%, respectively (Table 3). Across all surveys, the prevalence of improved housing was not statistically different between boys versus girls (17.2% versus 16.7%, p = 0.067), but significantly different between children whose mother completed less than secondary versus secondary or higher education (11.4% versus 35.5%, p < 0.001) and children with household wealth belonging to the lower 2 versus upper 3 quintiles (5.8% versus 26.6%, p < 0.001).

Table 3. Characteristics of housing qualities of study participants.

Country Number of children aged 36–59 months without missing information (N)a Finished building materials (%) Improved drinking water (%) Improved sanitation facilities (%) Sufficient living area (%) Improved housing (%)
Benin 4,709 69.6 73.5 34.8 56.9 17.4
Côte d’Ivoire 3,598 73.7 76.8 36.2 61.1 18.2
Cameroon 2,739 59.0 69.7 49.7 70.6 28.3
Chad 6,375 16.7 54.8 16.1 48.5 4.3
Congo 3,526 59.8 67.4 29.8 60.7 17.4
DR Congo 8,453 19.4 36.9 27.0 59.9 4.5
Eswatini 1,038 88.1 67.3 76.5 66.9 38.0
Gambia 4,115 89.6 88.1 48.4 77.8 33.7
Ghana 3,521 85.1 83.9 52.5 53.5 23.6
Guinea 3,099 71.1 78.5 48.0 65.7 24.5
Guinea-Bissau 2,849 28.5 68.2 11.9 74.3 5.6
Lesotho 1,292 63.2 85.8 65.3 51.5 28.3
Madagascar 5,004 23.2 31.5 8.9 29.8 3.6
Malawi 7,536 34.8 85.1 59.8 69.7 17.4
Mali 6,318 39.2 66.3 41.8 65.4 15.7
Mauritania 4,005 45.0 80.1 45.8 35.2 18.6
Nigeria 11,101 63.7 65.5 46.7 54.3 19.4
Sierra Leone 4,568 50.4 57.1 37.8 73.1 16.6
Togo 1,946 84.0 71.3 36.9 65.9 20.6
Zimbabwe 2,479 79.4 75.2 64.1 71.3 39.8

aNumber of children without missing information on ECDI, confounding variables, and exposures.

ECDI, Early Childhood Development Index.

Early childhood development

The median prevalence of on-track development in cognitive and social–emotional domains were 83.9% (ranging from 50.0% in Chad to 95.7% in Lesotho) and 71.4% (ranging from 61.4% in Congo to 79.3% in Lesotho), respectively (Table 4). The prevalence of on-track cognitive development was higher in girls (79.8% versus 79.2%, p = 0.034), in children whose mother completed secondary or higher education (85.3% versus 77.8%, p < 0.001), and in children with household wealth belonging to the upper 3 quintiles (82.0% versus 76.6%, p < 0.001). The prevalence of on-track social–emotional development was higher in girls (74.2% versus 68.4%, p < 0.001), in children whose mother completed secondary or higher education (72.6% versus 70.9%, p < 0.001), and in children with household wealth belonging to the upper 3 quintiles (71.8% versus 70.6%, p < 0.001).

Table 4. Status of ECD of study participants.

Country Number of children aged 36–59 months without missing information (N)a On-track cognitive development (%) On-track social–emotional development (%)
Benin 4,709 83.0 73.7
Côte d’Ivoire 3,598 86.9 69.8
Cameroon 2,739 85.8 68.0
Chad 6,375 50.0 69.4
Congo 3,526 85.5 61.4
DR Congo 8,453 64.2 78.0
Eswatini 1,038 95.1 66.0
Gambia 4,115 95.5 63.5
Ghana 3,521 82.2 66.9
Guinea 3,099 79.6 62.3
Guinea-Bissau 2,849 86.3 75.9
Lesotho 1,292 95.7 79.3
Madagascar 5,004 84.3 76.1
Malawi 7,536 80.9 75.2
Mali 6,318 84.1 73.6
Mauritania 4,005 83.6 67.0
Nigeria 11,101 80.4 73.5
Sierra Leone 4,568 79.5 61.7
Togo 1,946 72.5 73.0
Zimbabwe 2,479 91.2 77.4

aNumber of children without missing information on ECDI, confounding variables, and exposures.

ECD, early childhood development; ECDI, Early Childhood Development Index.

Association between improved housing and ECD

Fig 1 shows the associations between improved housing and ECD as estimated by the meta-analyses, adjusted for a priori confounding variables. Improved housing was associated with a 15% increase in the odds of being developmentally on track in the cognitive domain (OR = 1.15, 95% CI 1.06 to 1.24, p < 0.001). Similar associations were found for improved drinking water (OR = 1.07, 95% CI 1.00 to 1.14, p = 0.046), improved sanitation facilities (OR = 1.15, 95% CI 1.03 to 1.28, p = 0.014), and sufficient living area (OR = 1.06, 95% CI 1.01 to 1.10, p = 0.018). Finished building materials was not associated with on-track cognitive development (OR = 1.02, 95% CI 0.93 to 1.12, p = 0.666). Overall, there was no association between improved housing and on-track social–emotional development (OR = 1.06, 95% CI 0.98 to 1.14, p = 0.154). The unadjusted associations between improved housing and ECD are shown in S1 Fig. We detected small to moderate heterogeneity between countries for most of the associations, except for the association between improved sanitation facilities and on-track cognitive development (I2 = 60.2), which is positive and statistically significant in Chad, DR Congo, Gambia, Guinea, Guinea-Bissau, and Nigeria (Figure E in S2 Text).

Fig 1. Association between housing quality and ECD in children aged 36–59 months in SSA.

Fig 1

Odds ratios are adjusted for age (months) and gender of the child, maternal education, household wealth index, and the availability of children’s books and playthings. CI, confidence interval; ECD, early childhood development; SSA, sub-Saharan Africa.

S2 Fig shows the associations in the models where we simultaneously adjusted for all 4 improved housing characteristics and a priori confounding variables. Improved sanitation facilities (OR = 1.14, 95% CI 1.02 to 1.27, p = 0.020) and sufficient living area (OR = 1.05, 95% CI 1.01 to 1.10, p = 0.025) were independently associated with a higher likelihood of being developmentally on track in the cognitive domain, while the association between improved drinking water and on-track cognitive development was no longer statistically significant (OR = 1.06, 95% CI 0.99 to 1.13, p = 0.094).

Fig 2 shows the subgroups analyses of the associations between improved housing and ECD by the child’s gender, maternal education, and household wealth quintiles. The association between improved housing and on-track social–emotional development was stronger among girls (p for interaction = 0.005). Improved housing was associated with a 14% increase in the odds of being developmentally on track in the social–emotional domain in girls (OR = 1.14, 95% CI 1.04 to 1.25, p = 0.006). This association was not detected in boys (OR = 0.99, 95% CI 0.91 to 1.08, p = 0.875). The association between improved housing and on-track cognitive development was significant among children with levels of household wealth in the upper 3 quintiles (OR = 1.20, 95% CI 1.08 to 1.32, p < 0.001) but not among children with levels of household wealth in the lower 2 quintiles (OR = 1.01, 95% CI 0.87 to 1.18, p = 0.893), although the differences between these 2 groups were not statistically significant (p for interaction = 0.242).

Fig 2. Subgroup analyses of the association between improved housing and ECD in children aged 36–59 months in SSA.

Fig 2

Odds ratios are adjusted for age (months) and gender of the child, maternal education, household wealth index, and the availability of children’s books and playthings. Pinteraction, p-value for the odds ratio of the interaction term between improved housing and the factor (i.e., p-value for multiplicative interaction); Q1 to Q2, wealth quintile 1 to wealth quintile 2; Q3 to Q5, wealth quintile 3 to wealth quintile 5. ECD, early childhood development; SSA, sub-Saharan Africa.

Discussion

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first multicountry study in SSA examining the association between improved housing and ECD. Overall, we found that improved housing was associated with 15% higher odds of being developmentally on track in the cognitive domain. Individually, we found that improved drinking water, improved sanitation facilities, and sufficient living area were associated with on-track cognitive development. When all housing characteristics were included in the same model, the results suggest that improved sanitation facilities and sufficient living area were independently associated with a higher likelihood of being developmentally on track in the cognitive domain. Across all children, none of the improved housing characteristics or improved housing as a combined variable were associated with on-track social–emotional development. However, we observed a significant association between improved housing and on-track social–emotional development among girls.

Our findings are consistent with prior studies conducted in LMIC that investigated the associations between water and sanitation on cognitive development. A study using MICS data on housing and the Ten Questions Scale to measure ECD suggested that children with more access to protected water or flush toilet had a lower likelihood of cognitive impairment in Thailand and Mongolia [39]. A meta-analysis of studies conducted in Indonesia, Brazil, Pakistan, and India suggested that access to household sanitation was associated with improved cognitive ability in children [40]. A prospective cohort study showed that flush toilet use in infancy was associated with higher cognitive scores in children aged between 18 and 36 months in Tanzania, whereas access to improved water in infancy was not associated with later cognitive development [41].

Previous studies have shown inconsistent associations between building materials and ECD. A study of low-income urban neighborhoods in 3 US cities reported a correlation between the poor physical quality of the house (e.g., leaking roofs, broken windows, and rodents) and lower average reading and math skills in children with an average age of 10 [12]. However, another study showed that earth or dung flooring was not associated with cognitive impairment in children aged between 3 and 9 years in Bangladesh, Lao PDR, Mongolia, and Thailand [39]. We found no association between building materials and ECD. It is possible that a very high standard of improvements, rather than only having finished materials, is needed to promote ECD in LMIC. The association between sufficient living area and ECD has been documented mainly in high-income countries. Residential density (measured as the ratio of the number of people in the household to the number of rooms) during early childhood predicts cognitive development at 36 months both concurrently and prospectively in the US and the United Kingdom [42]. We extended these associations to preschoolers in SSA by showing the positive associations between sufficient living area and on-track cognitive development.

Findings of randomized controlled trials have shown inconsistent associations of water and sanitation intervention with social–emotional development. A trial done in Bangladesh found significant benefits of upgrading latrines and improving water quality on communication and personal social skills at 2 years of age [43]. Similar interventions in Kenya and Zimbabwe showed no effects on children’s social–emotional development [44,45]. We found that on-track social–emotional development was not associated with any of the housing qualities. It is possible that other components of nurturing care in the Nurturing Care Framework [4], such as adequate nutrition and responsive caregiving, would exert an influence on social–emotional development above housing environment [22,45,46]. Moreover, social–emotional development might be more susceptible to neighborhood context than housing environment [47,48].

Several mechanisms might explain the relationship between housing quality and cognitive development. First, improved housing quality can be associated with on-track cognitive development by protecting a child’s physical health. Notably, enteric infections, including intestinal worms caused by poor sanitation, can lead to iron deficiency, inflammation, and poor growth and development [49]. Diarrhea in early childhood has been linked to negative cognitive outcomes [5052], and the sanitation intervention may reduce children’s exposure to this factor [53]. Second, improved housing quality may also be linked with on-track cognitive development by protecting parental mental health [54,55], which could potentially affect parent–child interactions [56,57]. Water and sanitation interventions have shown effectiveness in reducing maternal depressive symptoms [43]. Studies in high-income countries have shown that linkages between higher residential density and children’s poorer cognitive development are largely mediated by diminished maternal responsiveness [42].

We observed that the association between improved housing and on-track social–emotional development was stronger among girls than boys. Previous studies conducted in the US have shown that girls were more sensitive to housing interventions. Girls, rather than boys, who moved to low-poverty neighborhoods experienced significant reductions in conduct disorder and psychological distress, as well as an increase in education achievement [5860]. Moreover, we observed that the association between improved housing and on-track cognitive development was relatively strong in children from relatively rich households; in poorer households, the association between improved housing and ECD may be masked by the larger impact of other risk factors of ECD. For example, a study conducted in Southeast Asia showed that stunting was associated with 28% lower odds of being developmentally on track in ECDI cognitive domain [21]. A study including 17 countries in Europe, Asia, and Africa showed that having books (OR = 1.62) and caregiver counting with the child (OR = 1.47) were associated with on-track cognitive development [22].

This study had several limitations. First, the causal relationship between improved housing and ECD remains unclear, due to the cross-sectional survey design and the residual confounding, which cannot be completely ruled out. Although we controlled for basic proxies of socioeconomic status, especially household materials and learning resources, as well as contextual effects, these a priori factors might not be sufficient for accounting for all potential confounders. Thus, the association between housing and ECD might be distorted due to potential selection bias. Second, the MICS does not contain adequate comprehensive measures for our hypothesized mediators. Additionally, the survey design restricted the causal mediation analysis. Therefore, it is important for future studies to elucidate potential mechanisms in this relationship. Third, other aspects of housing quality may be associated with ECD such as lead paint, rodents, and poor ventilation [61]. However, these characteristics have not been assessed in the MICS. Fourth, although the ECDI has shown validity in field studies [20], this instrument is less sensitive than more complex cognitive assessments used with older age groups. Other aspects of cognition, such as pattern recognition and memory or cultural/context-specific developmental milestones, are not captured in the ECDI and may lead to an inaccurate evaluation of ECD. This inaccuracy would result in non-differential misclassification of the outcomes and bias the effect estimates downwards [62]. Fifth, our analysis only included SSA countries, therefore, the generalizability of our findings to other regions of the world should be done with caution. The heterogeneity of the associations was generally moderate, except for the association between improved sanitation and on-track cognitive development. We believe that random-effects (rather than fixed-effect) models would account for this heterogeneity. More research is needed in these countries to better understand the heterogeneity in these findings.

The main strength of this study is that we investigated the association between housing and ECD by including a multinational sample from 20 countries across SSA. The external validity of our findings was enhanced than the analyses restricted to 1 country. Furthermore, instead of conducting the 1-stage analysis based on the pooled data of all countries, we performed meta-analyses to combine the results of different countries, which varied the effect of confounders in each country and minimized bias [63]. Additionally, incorporating the national survey data in meta-analyses minimized the publication bias.

In SSA, previous research has predominantly focused on the effects of health, nutrition, and responsive caregiving on ECD. The WASH Benefits trial in Kenya assessed the independent and combined effects of water, sanitation, handwashing, and nutrition interventions on children’s social–emotional development [4345]. However, for other aspects of housing quality, such as finished building materials, sufficient living area, and overall housing environment, the evidence base for interventions remains limited. Therefore, given the present intensity of evidence, our study did not imply that scarce resources should be shifted away from services that already have strong evidence of supporting ECD (e.g., preprimary programming and home visiting services) [5]. Instead, our study highlights the need for studies to compare the effect sizes of housing with that of other factors (e.g., nutrition and parenting) on ECD, which is crucial for selecting the target factors of the interventions.

In conclusion, to the best of our knowledge, this is the first study in SSA to demonstrate the associations between multiple aspects of housing quality and children’s on-track development in cognitive and social–emotional domains. Given the cross-sectional nature of the study and the less nuanced representations of ECD outcomes using the ECDI, this study is inherently exploratory/hypothesis generating. Future work to establish a causal link between housing and ECD and to assess its underlying mechanisms is crucial for designing cost-effective ECD interventions and screenings in SSA and thereby, for supporting children in attaining their developmental milestones.

Supporting information

S1 Analysis Plan. Prospective analysis plan.

(DOCX)

S1 Table. The ECDI questionnaire.

ECDI, Early Childhood Development Index.

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Number and percentage of participants with missing data for each variable of interest in each country.

(DOCX)

S1 Text. Classifications of improved/unimproved facility types and finished/unfinished materials.

(DOCX)

S2 Text. Association between housing quality and ECD in children aged 36–59 months in SSA.

ECD, early childhood development; SSA, sub-Saharan Africa.

(DOCX)

S1 Fig. Unadjusted associations between housing quality and ECD in children aged 36–59 months in SSA.

ECD, early childhood development; SSA, sub-Saharan Africa.

(DOCX)

S2 Fig. Mutually adjusted association between housing quality and ECD in children aged 36–59 months in SSA.

ECD, early childhood development; SSA, sub-Saharan Africa.

(DOCX)

S1 Checklist. STROBE checklist.

STROBE, Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology.

(DOC)

Abbreviations

ECD

early childhood development

ECDI

Early Childhood Development Index

LMIC

low- and middle-income countries

MICS

Multiple Indicator Cluster Survey

OR

odds ratio

SSA

sub-Saharan Africa

STROBE

Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology

Data Availability

All data are publicly available on the MICS website (http://mics.unicef.org/surveys). Registration and login details were required to access the data.

Funding Statement

The author(s) received no specific funding for this work.

References

  • 1.Black MM, Walker SP, Fernald LCH, Andersen CT, DiGirolamo AM, Lu C, et al. Early childhood development coming of age: science through the life course. Lancet. 2017;389(10064):77–90. 10.1016/S0140-6736(16)31389-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Daelmans B, Darmstadt GL, Lombardi J, Black MM, Britto PR, Lye S, et al. Early childhood development: the foundation of sustainable development. Lancet. 2017;389(10064):9–11. 10.1016/S0140-6736(16)31659-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.McCoy DC, Peet ED, Ezzati M, Danaei G, Black MM, Sudfeld CR, et al. Early Childhood Developmental Status in Low- and Middle-Income Countries: National, Regional, and Global Prevalence Estimates Using Predictive Modeling. PLoS Med. 2016;13(6):e1002034. 10.1371/journal.pmed.1002034 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.WHO, UNICEF, World Bank. Nurturing care for early childhood development: a framework for helping children survive and thrive to transform health and human potential. 2018.
  • 5.Prado EL, Larson LM, Cox K, Bettencourt K, Kubes JN, Shankar AH. Do effects of early life interventions on linear growth correspond to effects on neurobehavioural development? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet Glob Health. 2019;7(10):e1398–e413. 10.1016/S2214-109X(19)30361-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Govender T, Barnes JM, Pieper CH. Living in low-cost housing settlements in cape town, South Africa-the epidemiological characteristics associated with increased health vulnerability. Bull N Y Acad Med. 2010;87(6):899–911. 10.1007/s11524-010-9502-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Ferguson KT, Cassells RC, MacAllister JW, Evans GW. The physical environment and child development: an international review. Int J Psychol. 2013;48(4):437–68. 10.1080/00207594.2013.804190 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Tusting LS, Bottomley C, Gibson H, Kleinschmidt I, Tatem AJ, Lindsay SW, et al. Housing Improvements and Malaria Risk in Sub-Saharan Africa: A Multi-Country Analysis of Survey Data. PLoS Med. 2017;14(2):e1002234. 10.1371/journal.pmed.1002234 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Tusting LS, Gething PW, Gibson HS, Greenwood B, Knudsen J, Lindsay SW, et al. Housing and child health in sub-Saharan Africa: A cross-sectional analysis. PLoS Med. 2020;17(3):e1003055. 10.1371/journal.pmed.1003055 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Bhutta ZA, Guerrant RL, Nelson CA III. Neurodevelopment, Nutrition, and Inflammation: The Evolving Global Child Health Landscape. Pediatrics. 2017;139(Suppl 1):S12–22. 10.1542/peds.2016-2828D [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Blair C, Raver CC, Granger D, Mills-Koonce R, Hibel L. Allostasis and allostatic load in the context of poverty in early childhood. Dev Psychopathol. 2011;23(3):845–57. 10.1017/S0954579411000344 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Coley RL, Leventhal T, Lynch AD, Kull M. Relations between housing characteristics and the well-being of low-income children and adolescents. Dev Psychol. 2013;49(9):1775–89. 10.1037/a0031033 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Hopton JL, Hunt SM. Housing conditions and mental health in a disadvantaged area in Scotland. J Epidemiol Community Health. 1996;50(1):56–61. 10.1136/jech.50.1.56 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Leventhal T, Newman S. Housing and child development. Child Youth Serv Rev. 2010;32(9):1165–74. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.United Nations. World Population Prospects 2019. United Nations Department of Economic and Social Affairs. 2019.
  • 16.Tusting LS, Bisanzio D, Alabaster G, Cameron E, Cibulskis R, Davies M, et al. Mapping changes in housing in sub-Saharan Africa from 2000 to 2015. Nature. 2019;568(7752):391–4. 10.1038/s41586-019-1050-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.United Nations. Progress towards the Sustainable Development Goals. Report of the Secretary-General. UN Economic and Social Council; 2016.
  • 18.Bradley RH, Putnick DL. Housing quality and access to material and learning resources within the home environment in developing countries. Child Dev. 2012;83(1):76–91. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01674.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.UNICEF. Multiple Indicator Cluster Survey (MICS). [cited 2020 May 22]. http://mics.unicef.org.
  • 20.UNICEF. Data and Analytics Section. Development Of The Early Childhood Development Index In MICS Surveys. New York; 2017.
  • 21.Kang Y, Aguayo VM, Campbell RK, West KP. Association between stunting and early childhood development among children aged 36–59 months in South Asia. Matern Child Nutr. 2018;14(Suppl 4):e12684. 10.1111/mcn.12684 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Salhi C, Beatriz E, McBain R, McCoy D, Sheridan M. Physical Discipline, Deprivation, and Differential Risk of Developmental Delay Across 17 Countries. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Scharf RJ, Scharf GJ, Stroustrup A. Developmental Milestones. Pediatr Rev. 2016;37(1). 10.1542/pir.2014-0103 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Squires J, Bricker D. Ages and Stages Questionnaire (ASQ): A Parent Completed Child Monitoring System. 3rd ed. Baltimore, Maryland: Brooks Publishing Company; 2009. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Gladstone M, Lancaster GA, Umar E, Nyirenda M, Kayira E, van den Broek NR, et al. The Malawi Developmental Assessment Tool (MDAT): the creation, validation, and reliability of a tool to assess child development in rural African settings. PLoS Med. 2010;7(5):e1000273. 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000273 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Augustyn M, Frank DA, Zuckerman BS. Chapter 3—INFANCY AND TODDLER YEARS. In: Carey WB, Crocker AC, Coleman WL, Elias ER, Feldman HM, editors. Developmental-Behavioral Pediatrics. 4th ed. Philadelphia: W.B. Saunders; 2009. p. 24–38. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Nazif-Muñoz JI, Spengler JD, Arku RE, Oulhote Y. Solid fuel use and early child development disparities in Ghana: analyses by gender and urbanicity. J Expo Sci Environ Epidemiol. 2020. 10.1038/s41370-020-0224-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.UN-HABITAT. Affordable Land and Housing in Africa. Nairobi, Kenya: UN-HABITAT; 2012.
  • 29.UN-HABITAT. The challenge of slums: Global report on human settlements 2003 revised and updated version. Nairobi, Kenya: UN-HABITAT; 2010.
  • 30.UNICEF, WHO. Progress on household drinking water, sanitation and hygiene 2000–2017: Special focus on inequalities. 2019.
  • 31.United Nations. SDG Indicators Metadata repository. [cited 2020 May 23]. https://unstats.un.org/sdgs/metadata/.
  • 32.World Food Programme. Vulnerability Analysis & Mapping Guidance Paper-Creation of a Wealth Index. 2017.
  • 33.UNICEF Data and Analytics Section. Review of options for reporting water, sanitation and hygiene coverage by wealth quintile. New York; 2016.
  • 34.UNICEF. MICS6 Indicator List. [cited 2020 Jun 20]. http://mics.unicef.org/files?job=W1siZiIsIjIwMjAvMDcvMjAvMjAvNTIvMDIvMTgvTUlDUzZfSW5kaWNhdG9yc19hbmRfRGVmaW5pdGlvbnNfMjAyMDA2MjYuZG9jeCJdXQ&sha=e5d4afc5db21725c.
  • 35.Manu A, Ewerling F, Barros AJ, Victora CG. Association between availability of children’s book and the literacy-numeracy skills of children aged 36 to 59 months: secondary analysis of the UNICEF Multiple-Indicator Cluster Surveys covering 35 countries. J Glob Health. 2019;9(1):010403. 10.7189/jogh.09.010403 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Higgins JPT, Thompson SG, Deeks JJ, Altman DG. Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. BMJ. 2003;327(7414):557–60. 10.1136/bmj.327.7414.557 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.DerSimonian R, Laird N. Meta-analysis in clinical trials. Control Clin Trials. 1986;7(3):177–88. 10.1016/0197-2456(86)90046-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Brankovic M, Kardys I, Steyerberg EW, Lemeshow S, Markovic M, Rizopoulos D, et al. Understanding of interaction (subgroup) analysis in clinical trials. Eur J Clin Investig. 2019;49(8):e13145. 10.1111/eci.13145 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Llewellyn G, Emerson E, Madden R, Honey A. The Well-being of Children with Disabilities in the Asia Pacific Region: Analysis of UNICEF MICS 3 Survey Data from Bangladesh, Lao PDR, Mongolia and Thailand. Faculty of Health Sciences, The University of Sydney; 2012.
  • 40.Sclar GD, Garn JV, Penakalapati G, Alexander KT, Krauss J, Freeman MC, et al. Effects of sanitation on cognitive development and school absence: A systematic review. Int J Hyg Environ Health. 2017;220(6):917–27. 10.1016/j.ijheh.2017.06.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Sudfeld CR, McCoy DC, Fink G, Muhihi A, Bellinger DC, Masanja H, et al. Malnutrition and Its Determinants Are Associated with Suboptimal Cognitive, Communication, and Motor Development in Tanzanian Children. J Nutr. 2015;145(12):2705–14. 10.3945/jn.115.215996 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Evans GW, Ricciuti HN, Hope S, Schoon I, Bradley RH, Corwyn RF, et al. Crowding and Cognitive Development: The Mediating Role of Maternal Responsiveness Among 36-Month-Old Children. Environ Behav. 2009;42(1):135–48. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Tofail F, Fernald LC, Das KK, Rahman M, Ahmed T, Jannat KK, et al. Effect of water quality, sanitation, hand washing, and nutritional interventions on child development in rural Bangladesh (WASH Benefits Bangladesh): a cluster-randomised controlled trial. Lancet Child Adolesc Health. 2018;2(4):255–68. 10.1016/S2352-4642(18)30031-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Stewart CP, Kariger P, Fernald L, Pickering AJ, Arnold CD, Arnold BF, et al. Effects of water quality, sanitation, handwashing, and nutritional interventions on child development in rural Kenya (WASH Benefits Kenya): a cluster-randomised controlled trial. Lancet Child Adolesc Health. 2018;2(4):269–80. 10.1016/S2352-4642(18)30025-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Gladstone MJ, Chandna J, Kandawasvika G, Ntozini R, Majo FD, Tavengwa NV, et al. Independent and combined effects of improved water, sanitation, and hygiene (WASH) and improved complementary feeding on early neurodevelopment among children born to HIV-negative mothers in rural Zimbabwe: Substudy of a cluster-randomized trial. PLoS Med. 2019;16(3):e1002766. 10.1371/journal.pmed.1002766 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Yousafzai AK, Rasheed MA, Rizvi A, Armstrong R, Bhutta ZA. Effect of integrated responsive stimulation and nutrition interventions in the Lady Health Worker programme in Pakistan on child development, growth, and health outcomes: a cluster-randomised factorial effectiveness trial. Lancet. 2014;384(9950):1282–93. 10.1016/S0140-6736(14)60455-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Ingoldsby EM, Shaw DS. Neighborhood contextual factors and early-starting antisocial pathways. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. 2002;5(1):21–55. 10.1023/a:1014521724498 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Homel R, Burns A. Environmental quality and the well-being of children. Soc Indic Res. 1989;21(2):133–58. [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Ngure FM, Reid BM, Humphrey JH, Mbuya MN, Pelto G, Stoltzfus RJ. Water, sanitation, and hygiene (WASH), environmental enteropathy, nutrition, and early child development: making the links. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2014;1308:118–28. 10.1111/nyas.12330 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Guerrant DI, Moore SR, Lima AA, Patrick PD, Schorling JB, Guerrant RL. Association of early childhood diarrhea and cryptosporidiosis with impaired physical fitness and cognitive function four-seven years later in a poor urban community in northeast Brazil. Am J Trop Med Hyg. 1999;61(5):707–13. 10.4269/ajtmh.1999.61.707 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Niehaus MD, Moore SR, Patrick PD, Derr LL, Lorntz B, Lima AA, et al. Early childhood diarrhea is associated with diminished cognitive function 4 to 7 years later in children in a northeast Brazilian shantytown. Am J Trop Med Hyg. 2002;66(5):590–3. 10.4269/ajtmh.2002.66.590 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Petri WA Jr, Miller M, Binder HJ, Levine MM, Dillingham R, Guerrant RL. Enteric infections, diarrhea, and their impact on function and development. J Clin Invest. 2008;118(4):1277–90. 10.1172/JCI34005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Luby SP, Rahman M, Arnold BF, Unicomb L, Ashraf S, Winch PJ, et al. Effects of water quality, sanitation, handwashing, and nutritional interventions on diarrhoea and child growth in rural Bangladesh: a cluster randomised controlled trial. Lancet Glob Health. 2018;6(3):e302–e15. 10.1016/S2214-109X(17)30490-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Allen J, Balfour R, Bell R, Marmot M. Social determinants of mental health. Int Rev Psychiatry. 2014;26(4):392–407. 10.3109/09540261.2014.928270 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Mensah FK, Kiernan KE. Parents’ mental health and children’s cognitive and social development: families in England in the Millennium Cohort Study. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2010;45(11):1023–35. 10.1007/s00127-009-0137-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Waters SF, Karnilowicz HR, West TV, Mendes WB. Keep it to yourself? Parent emotion suppression influences physiological linkage and interaction behavior. J Fam Psychol. 2020. 10.1037/fam0000664 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Jeong J, McCoy DC, Yousafzai AK, Salhi C, Fink G. Paternal Stimulation and Early Child Development in Low- and Middle-Income Countries. Pediatrics. 2016;138(4). 10.1542/peds.2016-1357 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Osypuk TL, Tchetgen EJT, Acevedo-Garcia D, Earls FJ, Lincoln A, Schmidt NM, et al. Differential mental health effects of neighborhood relocation among youth in vulnerable families: results from a randomized trial. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2012;69(12):1284–94. 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2012.449 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Kessler RC, Duncan GJ, Gennetian LA, Katz LF, Kling JR, Sampson NA, et al. Associations of housing mobility interventions for children in high-poverty neighborhoods with subsequent mental disorders during adolescence. JAMA. 2014;311(9):937–48. 10.1001/jama.2014.607 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar] [Retracted]
  • 60.Leventhal T, Dupéré V. Moving to Opportunity: Does long-term exposure to ‘low-poverty’ neighborhoods make a difference for adolescents? Soc Sci Med. 2011;73(5):737–43. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2011.06.042 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Housing DJR. Healthy Child Development: Known and Potential Impacts of Interventions. Annu Rev Public Health. 2020;41:381–96. 10.1146/annurev-publhealth-040119-094050 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Jurek AM, Greenland S, Maldonado G, Church TR. Proper interpretation of non-differential misclassification effects: expectations vs observations. Int J Epidemiol. 2005;34(3):680–7. 10.1093/ije/dyi060 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Basagaña X, Pedersen M, Barrera-Gómez J, Gehring U, Giorgis-Allemand L, Hoek G, et al. Analysis of multicentre epidemiological studies: contrasting fixed or random effects modelling and meta-analysis. Int J Epidemiol. 2018;47(4):1343–54. 10.1093/ije/dyy117 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

S1 Analysis Plan. Prospective analysis plan.

(DOCX)

S1 Table. The ECDI questionnaire.

ECDI, Early Childhood Development Index.

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Number and percentage of participants with missing data for each variable of interest in each country.

(DOCX)

S1 Text. Classifications of improved/unimproved facility types and finished/unfinished materials.

(DOCX)

S2 Text. Association between housing quality and ECD in children aged 36–59 months in SSA.

ECD, early childhood development; SSA, sub-Saharan Africa.

(DOCX)

S1 Fig. Unadjusted associations between housing quality and ECD in children aged 36–59 months in SSA.

ECD, early childhood development; SSA, sub-Saharan Africa.

(DOCX)

S2 Fig. Mutually adjusted association between housing quality and ECD in children aged 36–59 months in SSA.

ECD, early childhood development; SSA, sub-Saharan Africa.

(DOCX)

S1 Checklist. STROBE checklist.

STROBE, Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology.

(DOC)

Data Availability Statement

All data are publicly available on the MICS website (http://mics.unicef.org/surveys). Registration and login details were required to access the data.


Articles from PLoS Medicine are provided here courtesy of PLOS

RESOURCES