Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2021 May 14;16(5):e0250564. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0250564

The life expectancy of older couples and surviving spouses

Janice Compton 1,#, Robert A Pollak 2,3,4,*,#
Editor: Bryan L Sykes5
PMCID: PMC8121294  PMID: 33989281

Abstract

Individual life expectancies provide information for individuals making retirement decisions and for policy makers. For couples, analogous measures are the expected years both spouses will be alive (joint life expectancy) and the expected years the surviving spouse will be a widow or widower (survivor life expectancy). Using individual life expectancies to calculate summary measures for couples is intuitively appealing but yield misleading results, overstating joint life expectancy and dramatically understating survivor life expectancies. This implies that standard "individual life cycle models" are misleading for couples and that “couple life cycle models” must be substantially more complex. Using the CDC life tables for 2010, we construct joint and survivor life expectancy measures for randomly formed couples. The couples we form are defined by age, race and ethnicity, and education. Due to assortative marriage, inequalities in individual life expectancies are compounded into inequalities in joint and survivor life expectancies. We also calculate life expectancy measures for randomly formed couples for the 1930–2010 decennial years. Trends over time show how the relative rate of decrease in the mortality rates of men and women affect joint and survivor life expectancies. Because our couple life expectancy measures are based on randomly formed couples, they do not capture the effects of differences in spouses’ premarital characteristics (apart from sex, age, race and ethnicity, and, in some cases, education) or of correlations in spouses’ experiences or behaviors during marriage. However, they provide benchmarks which have been sorely lacking in the public discourse.

Introduction

Measures of joint and survivor life expectancy are potentially useful to those designing or evaluating policies affecting older couples and to couples making retirement, savings, and long-term care decisions. However, couple-based measure of life expectancies are virtually unknown in the social sciences. In discussions of intra-household decision making (e.g., regarding the timing of retirement and the claiming of retirement and social security benefits), the difference between the life expectancies of a wife and husband is sometimes treated as an informal measure of survivor life expectancy [13]. As we shall show, neither couple life expectancy nor survivor life expectancies can be inferred from individual life expectancies. This implies that the standard "individual life cycle models" widely used in economics are misleading for couples and that “couple life cycle models” are substantially more complex.

We illustrate the construction of measures of joint and survivor life expectancy for a randomly formed non-Hispanic white couple in which the wife was 60 and the husband 62 in 2010—that is, the wife was born in 1950 and her husband in 1948. We focus on 60-year-old wives and their husbands because these are ages at which many couples make crucial retirement-related decisions such as leaving career employment and claiming retirement and social security benefits. Thus, these are ages at which we would expect joint and survivor life expectancies to be especially salient.

Census data show that in 2010 the average age gap between 60-year-old non-Hispanic white women and their husbands was about 2 years. The 2010 CDC life tables show that the life expectancy of a 60-year-old non-Hispanic white woman was 24.4 years and that of a 62-year-old non-Hispanic white man was 20.2 years. The intuitions that the spouses will die at about the same time (e.g., within 4 or 5 years of each other) and that the wife will not live for a long time after her husband’s death are incorrect. Only if the mortality distributions of the husband and the wife were so tightly concentrated that they did not overlap (e.g., if 60 year old wives always lived longer than their 62 year old husbands) would it would be correct to use the minimum of husband and wife life expectancies as the joint life expectancy and to use the difference between the individual life expectancies as the survivor life expectancy. And, as intuition suggests, if the overlap between the mortality distributions were small, then these measures would be good approximations. But because the overlap is substantial, calculations based on individual life expectancies provide poor approximations of joint life expectancy and very poor approximations of survivor life expectancy.

Although Goldman and Lord [4] proposed couple life expectancy measures more than three decades ago, these measures have not been widely discussed or adopted outside the actuarial literature; see, for example, Bowers et al. [5] Brown and Poterba [6] investigate an alternative notion of "joint life expectancy"–the number of years that at least one spouse is expected to be alive. Similarly, the Internal Revenue Service (IRS) provides race-ethnicity-gender-neutral tables to enable taxpayers to calculate the tax liabilities associated with joint life annuities and distributions from Individual Retirement Accounts (IRAs) [7]. Demographers, economists, gerontologists, and sociologists, however, have generally focused on age-specific mortality rates rather than the summary measures of joint and survivor life expectancies [8, 9]. Although age-specific mortality rates are the basic building blocks of life expectancy measures, they are much less accessible than summary measures such as the probability that the wife will predecease the husband and, we argue, much less accessible than joint and survivor life expectancy.

Using male and female mortality rates from the CDC National Center for Health Statistics (NCHS), we calculate the joint life expectancy of randomly formed older couples and the life expectancy of surviving spouses. These calculations illustrate the extent to which approximations of couple life expectancies based on individual life expectancies are seriously misleading.

We do this separately for non-Hispanic whites, blacks, and Hispanics and, within racial groups for 2010, by educational attainment. For example, the joint life expectancies of couples consisting of 60 year old women and their husbands range from a low of 13.61 years for a black couple in which neither spouse has a college degree to a high of 18.99 years for a non-Hispanic white couple in which both spouses have college degrees. The measures that condition on educational attainment show that the differences in couple life expectancy by education is greater than the racial differences. We also calculate joint and survivor life expectancy for each census year beginning in 1930, and analyze trends in joint and survivor life expectancy from 1930 to 2010. The measures over the decennial years show how the relative declines in the mortality rates of men and women led to increases in both joint life expectancy and the number of years women could expect to be widows.

Our use of randomly formed couples provides measures that are analogous to individual life expectancies. They are summary measures that do not reflect individual differences, but rather provide informative benchmarks. Our decision to base the calculations on easily accessible data requires certain simplifications: (1) we calculate joint and survivor life expectancies from individual age-specific mortality rates; and (2) we use the individual mortality rates calculated from the full population, not from the married populations. These simplifications imply that we cannot take account of the correlation in mortality rates between husbands and wives, except to the extent that they are captured by educational attainment. We discuss each of these in turn.

Our three simplifications ensure that the measures presented here are analogous to standard individual life expectancy measures. First, like the measures presented here, individual life expectancy measures are typically based on cross-section (i.e., period) mortality rates. This assumes stability in age-specific mortality rates, an assumption that is well understood to be improbable. With longitudinal data, researchers can estimate the rate of change in age specific mortality rates and incorporate these forecasts into the life expectancies.

Second, evidence suggests that the age-specific mortality rates of married individuals are less than those of unmarried individuals both because healthier individuals select into marriage and because marriage has protective effects [1014]. Our calculations, which are based on NCHS life tables and randomly formed couples, ignore these effects and assume that individuals’ age-specific mortality rates are independent of their marital status. It would be erroneous to calculate joint and survivor life expectancies based on the age-specific mortality rates of married individuals because doing so assumes that married individuals remain married until death. Clearly, this is rarely true for both spouses and so these calculations will overestimate joint life expectancy. Moreover, it is not clear the extent to which mortality rates based on marital status reflect other attributes–education, race, place of birth, income–that may be related to both mortality and marital status. Therefore, to account for marital status and the protective effects of marriage when calculating joint and survivor life expectancies requires detailed longitudinal data on couples. We leave this to further research.

These two simplifications imply that we are ignoring the correlation in mortality rates of husbands and wives. Although we expect assortative marriage, shared environments, and behavioral habits to create correlations in these mortality rates, we base our calculations on the age-specific mortality of individuals, randomly assigned to form couples based on age and race. The effect of this is to impose the assumption that spouses’ mortality rates are independent of each other. Just as an individual’s life expectancy will differ from the population estimate due to their characteristics, experiences, and life choices, a couple’s joint and survivor life expectancies will differ from the population estimate due to their characteristics, experiences, and choices.

Overall, the calculations of joint and survivor life expectancies based on age-specific mortality rates of men and women–for example, 60 year old non-Hispanic white women randomly matched with 62 year old non-Hispanic white men—certainly do not capture all the nuances of mortality and marriage. They do, however, provide benchmarks which have been sorely lacking in the public discourse. For example, Holden and Kuo [15] analyze responses to questions on expected mortality in the Health and Retirement Study (HRS) and find that, on average, both spouses overestimate their joint years remaining and underestimate their likelihood of being widowed. A better understanding of the joint and survivor life expectancies may aid couples in decision-making.

Method

The calculation of joint and survivor life expectancies requires the age-specific mortality distributions, not individual life expectancies. We use the NCHS life tables [16] for men and women to construct mortality distributions and life tables for randomly constructed couples and from these we calculate joint life expectancy (i.e., the expected number of years that both spouses will live). We then calculate the probability that the wife (husband) will die at each age and use the individual life tables to calculate survivor life expectancy conditional on the sex of the surviving spouse (e.g., if the wife is the surviving spouse, we calculate her expected number of years as a widow). Although we use the terms "widow" and "widower," we actually calculate the expected number of years that both spouses are alive, regardless of whether or not they remain married, and how long a surviving spouse can expect to live after the death of his or her current spouse, regardless of remarriage.

The construction of mortality distributions for couples is straightforward but tedious. To illustrate, we continue to focus on the case in which we form a couple in which the wife was 60 and the husband 62 in 2010. From the individual life tables for men and for women, we calculate the probability that one or both spouses will die in 2010. This probability is the sum of the probabilities of three mutually exclusive events:

  1. the husband will die between 62 and 63 AND the wife will not die between 60 and 61

  2. the wife will die between 60 and 61 AND the husband will not die between 62 and 63, and

  3. the husband will die between 62 and 63 AND the wife will die between 60 and 61.

The sum of these three probabilities is, of course, equal to one minus the probability that neither spouse will die in 2010. Thus, if our only aim were to calculate joint life expectancy, it would be easier to focus on the probability that neither spouse would die at each age or in each year. The drawback of proceeding in this way is that to calculate survivor life expectancies we must first calculate the probability that the wife (husband) will become a widow (widower) at each age.

For couples that survive into 2011, we proceed in the same way, calculating the probability that the husband will die between 63 and 64 and the wife will not die between 61 and 62, etc. These calculations give “mortality rates” for the couple for each year, and from these we can construct a “couple life table.” More specifically, beginning with a cohort of 100,000 couples with the wife aged 60 and the husband aged 62, we can calculate expected transitions to widows, widowers, and “couple death” in each year. This corresponds to the lX and dX columns in standard individual life tables, with the subscript denoting year forward rather than age.

From the couple life table, we calculate the couple’s joint life expectancy by applying to couples the standard life expectancy calculation typically applied to individuals. For our focal couple–a non-Hispanic white couple in which the wife was 60 and the husband 62 in 2010 –joint life expectancy is 17.7 years (recall that her individual life expectancy is 24.4 years and his individual life expectancy is 20.2 years).

Survivor life expectancy answers questions like: “If the wife is the surviving spouse, how many years can she expect to live after her husband’s death?" The survivor life expectancies are appropriately weighted averages of individual life expectancies at each age, where the weights are the probabilities of couple death in each year, conditional on couple survival to that year and conditional on the sex of the surviving spouse.

The probability that our focal 60-year-old wife will predecease her 62-year-old husband is 0.37, a surprisingly high probability that reflects the substantial overlap of their mortality distributions. Hence, the probability that the wife will be the surviving spouse is 0.63 and, if she is the surviving spouse, her survivor life expectancy is 12.5 years. If the husband is the surviving spouse, his survivor life expectancy is 9.5 years.

This example demonstrated the calculation for a non-Hispanic white 60-year-old woman married to a non-Hispanic white 62-year-old man. To generalize the calculations to 60-year-old women married to men of each age, we use the 2010 Census [17] to determine the proportion of 60-year-old women married to men of each age, and use these proportions as weights. Thus the 60 year old woman is the anchor of each calculation: her joint and survivor life expectancies are a weighted average of the joint and survivor life expectancies she would face if married to a man of each age, and the weights are the probability that she is married to a man of each age. Thus, the differences we report in joint and survivor life expectancy across groups reflect differences in both age-specific mortalities and in the age gaps between 60-year-old women and their husbands.

We repeat this calculation for black and Hispanic women in 2010 and for black and white women by education of self and spouse in 2010. To calculate the joint and survivor life expectancies by couple education, we use data from Bound et al. [18] since the NCHS life tables do not condition on education. Finally, we use the decennial census data from 1930–2010 to calculate the joint and survivor life expectancies for 60-year-old white and black women over time.

Results

Joint and survivor life expectancies by race and ethnicity

Table 1 shows the 2010 measures of life expectancy for couples formed at random based on the population age profiles of non-Hispanic white, black, and Hispanic couples. We combine Hispanic blacks with blacks rather than with Hispanics. We define the race and ethnicity of couples by the race and ethnicity of the wife, that is, we consider only same-race couples in the calculation. There is little intermarriage reported in the 2010 census for non-Hispanic white women or for black women aged 60. Fully 96 percent of married non-Hispanic white women are married to non-Hispanic white men, and over 91 percent of married black women are married to black men. Hispanic women are more likely to intermarry; in 2010, 76 percent of married Hispanic women were married to Hispanic men. We present the individual life expectancy of women aged 60, the average life expectancy of spouses, and measures of joint and survivor life expectancy for the appropriately weighted average of couples in which the wife was aged 60.

Table 1. Life expectancy measures, 2010.

By race and ethnicity, wife is aged 60.

Non-Hispanic White Black Hispanic
Wife Life Expectancy 24.40 23.05 26.40
(0.00) (0.00) (0.00)
Husband Life Expectancy 20.17 18.22 21.05
(3.59) (3.74) (4.59)
Joint Life Expectancy 17.66 15.45 18.79
(2.08) (2.12) (2.82)
Survivor Life Expectancy (Wife) 12.48 13.52 13.13
(1.21) (1.19) (1.77)
Survivor Life Expectancy (Husband) 9.48 10.05 9.34
(1.51) (1.77) (1.85)
Probability that Wife is the Surviving Spouse 0.63 0.63 0.65
(0.10) (0.10) (0.12)
Age Gap (HusbandWife) 1.91 1.81 2.25
(4.79) (5.61) (5.90)
Sample Size 10,967 848 620

Calculations by authors. Standard errors in parentheses.

Both Hispanic men and Hispanic women have longer life expectancies than their non-Hispanic white and black counterparts, and Hispanic couples have a higher joint life expectancy. We have taken at face value the mortality distributions and life expectancies for Hispanic men and women reported by the NCHS, but these should be viewed with caution. The NCHS life tables report that Hispanics have substantially longer life expectancies than non-Hispanic whites, despite Hispanics’ lower incomes and lower levels of educational attainment. Demographers have termed this the “Hispanic Paradox” and have offered a variety of explanations [19, 20]. Black et al. [21] argue that the longer Hispanic life expectancies are probably due to measurement error.

Black men and black women have shorter life expectancies than their non-Hispanic white and Hispanic counterparts, and black couples have a lower joint life expectancy. The pattern for survivor life expectancy is the opposite: conditional on becoming a widow, black women have a longer survivor life expectancy (13.5 years) than Hispanic women (13.1 years) and non-Hispanic white women (12.5 years). This pattern reflects the larger gap in the mortality rates of black men and women compared to Hispanic and white men and women. Hispanic men have the shortest survivor life expectancies (9.3 years), followed by white men (9.5 years), with black men having the longest expected survivor life expectancies (10.1 years). While Hispanic men and women have the longest individual life expectancies at age 60, they have the lowest expected number of years in widowhood. black men and women have the shortest life expectancies at age 60, and they have the highest expected number of years in widowhood.

Joint and survivor life expectancies by education

We now discuss the association between these racial and ethnic differences in joint and survivor life expectancy and education, using mortality estimates from Bound et al. [18] to calculate life expectancy measures by education for non-Hispanic white couples and black couples. Bound et al. [18] calculate mortality rates for four education categories—less than high school, high school graduate, some college, and college graduate—for non-Hispanic whites and for blacks, conditional on survival to age 25. We have extended the estimates in two ways. First, we convert their five-year estimates to one-year age estimates using a cubic spline. Second, we extend their estimates for ages above age 85 using the proportional increases in mortality for ages above 86 in the full NCHS data.

We classify randomly formed couples into four education categories, using a "power couples" terminology [22, 23]. Couples are defined as "low-power" if neither the man or woman has a college degree; "half-power-her" if only the woman has a college degree; "half-power-him" if only the man has a college degree; and "full-power" if both have college degrees. The estimates from Bound et al. [18] are based on administrative data that have been criticized for underestimating education, especially at the lower end of the education distribution (Rostron [24], Hendi [25], and Sasson [26]). Because we focus on the differences between college graduates and non-college graduates, concerns about the administrative data used in Bound are less relevant.

Table 2 presents the life expectancy measures by education, with couples formed using weighted averages calculated from 2010 Census data. For both men and women, education is associated with lower mortality and substantially greater life expectancy [18, 2729]. We do not speculate on what portion of this association is causal and what portion reflects correlations between education and other factors, both genetic and environmental. Due to positive assortative marriage on education, the education differences in individuals’ life expectancies translate into substantial differences in joint and survivor life expectancies. Consider first the joint life expectancy measure. For both non-Hispanic white couples and black couples, as we move from low-power couples, to half-power-her, to half-power-him, to full power couples, joint life expectancy increases steadily. For non-Hispanic white couples, joint life expectancy increases across education categories from 15.5 to 19.0 years; for black couples, joint life expectancy increases from 13.6 to 16.4 years.

Table 2. Life expectancy measures, 2010.

By education, wife is aged 60.

Non-Hispanic White Couples Black Couples
Low Power Half Power (Her) Half Power (Him) Full Power All Low Power Half Power (Her) Half Power (Him) Full Power All
Wife Life Expectancy
23.68 26.07 24.73 26.07 24.61 21.70 23.79 22.51 23.79 22.26
(1.79) (0.00) (1.84) (0.00) (1.83) (1.43) (0.00) (1.50) (0.00) (1.53)
Husband Life Expectancy
18.71 19.82 22.06 22.12 20.13 16.95 17.46 19.45 19.76 17.57
(4.17) (4.39) (3.47) (3.46) (4.25) (4.36) (4.22) (3.77) (3.68) (4.36)
Joint Life Expectancy
15.53 17.17 18.31 18.99 16.91 13.61 14.84 15.59 16.39 14.26
(3.04) (3.42) (2.52) (2.82) (3.32) (2.68) (2.59) (2.23) (2.26) (2.78)
Survivor Life Expectancy (Wife)
13.24 13.50 11.70 11.93 12.71 13.40 13.99 12.61 12.87 13.33
(1.58) (1.79) (1.36) (1.38) (1.68) (1.45) (1.52) (1.36) (1.44) (1.49)
Survivor Life Expectancy (Husband)
9.69 8.96 10.07 9.27 9.59 9.89 9.36 10.47 10.05 9.90
(1.71) (1.48) (1.43) (1.28) (1.59) (2.06) (1.70) (1.76) (1.42) (1.94)
Probability that Wife is the Surviving Spouse
0.65 0.68 0.59 0.63 0.64 0.64 0.68 0.59 0.62 0.64
(0.12) (0.12) (0.11) (0.10) (0.12) (0.12) (0.12) (0.11) (0.10) (0.12)
Age Gap (HusbandWife)
1.96 1.93 1.89 1.83 1.91 1.47 2.28 2.79 2.59 1.81
(5.14) (5.16) (4.18) (4.20) (4.79) (5.81) (5.10) (5.31) (4.85) (5.61)
Sample 5,828 994 1,703 2,442 10,967 573 89 72 114 848

Calculations by authors. Standard errors in parentheses.

While joint life expectancies are approximately three years longer for non-Hispanic white couples in each category, survivor life expectancies are slightly higher for black men and women compared with non-Hispanic white men and women. For non-Hispanic white couples and for black couples, the husband survivor life expectancy is highest when he has a college degree and she does not (10.1 and 10.5 years). For these half-power-him couples, his life expectancy is higher than average and hers is lower than average, which results in a higher expected length of widowerhood for the husband. The husband survivor life expectancy is lowest when she has a college degree and he does not (9.0 years for non-Hispanic white couples and 9.4 years for black couples). For these half-power-her couples, his life expectancy is lower than average and hers is higher than average, resulting in lower expected years of widowerhood for the husband.

We find that the differences in life expectancies by education are, in all cases, greater than the racial differences. Similarly, the difference in the probability that the husband dies first is greater across education categories than the differences between blacks and non-Hispanic whites. For black couples and for non-Hispanic white couples, the probability that the wife will die first is lowest for couples in which only the husband has a college degree (half-power-him couples, at 0.59), and highest for couples in which only the wife has a college degree (half-power-her couples, at 0.68). Although these numbers reflect both differences in life expectancy and age gap across education groups, this pattern continues to hold when we fix the age gap at two years and consider only differences in life expectancies between blacks and non-Hispanic whites.

Joint and survivor life expectancies over time

We next describe the trends in joint and survivor life expectancies for white and black couples randomly formed using 1930–2010 Census data. We begin in 1930 because the mortality data for older individuals from 1920 and before is not comparable with the data from 1930 and after. The difficulty is that prior to 1930, the survivor tables end at age 89 (i.e., the probability of death between ages 89 and 90 is one). The tables from 1930 onwards extend to 101. The NCHS life tables for earlier census years do not include mortality distributions for Hispanics. As previously, we analyze couples consisting of 60 year old women and a weighted average of men at each age, with the weights reflecting the population of couples in the data. Thus, the changes we report in joint and survivor life expectancy between 1930 and 2010 reflect both changes in age-specific mortalities and changes in the age gaps between spouses. The gender differences in age-specific mortality have varied throughout the century. For a discussion of the determinants of the gap, see Goldin and Lleras-Muney [30].

Individual life expectancy for 60 year old women has increased steadily over the decades, and the gap between white and black women has remained fairly constant. The individual life expectancy of men married to 60 year old women increased both because the age gap has fallen (so husbands in 2010 are on average younger than husbands in 1930) and because the life expectancy of older men has increased. In Fig 1 we present joint and survivor life expectancies for randomly formed white couples in which the wife was aged 60, and in Fig 2 we present the corresponding life expectancies for randomly formed black couples. The figures are labeled by the race of the wife, but interracial marriages are rare for these cohorts. For all years, the percentage of white married women aged 60 who are married to white men exceeds 93 percent, and has exceeded 96 percent since 1950. For black married women aged 60, the percentage married to black men has exceeded 91 percent for all years since 1950; in 1930 and 1940 the percentage was slightly lower at 89 and 88 percent.

Fig 1. Joint and survivor life expectancies, 1930–2010.

Fig 1

White couples, wife is aged 60.

Fig 2. Joint and survivor life expectancies, 1930–2010.

Fig 2

Black couples, wife is aged 60.

The effect of the increasing life expectancy of older men is evident in the figures. Until 1980, a white woman of age 60 who outlived her husband could expect to spend as much of her remaining life in widowhood as years with her husband. From 1930 to 1980, joint life expectancy for white couples increased from 10.0 years to 14.0 years, and her survivor life expectancy followed a similar pattern, increasing from 10.9 years to 13.4 years. After 1980, the joint life expectancy of white couples continued to increase, while her survivor life expectancy fell. By 2010, joint life expectancy exceeded her survivor life expectancy by 5 years–joint life expectancy was 17.7 years and her survivor life expectancy was 12.5 years. For married black women of age 60, joint life expectancy did not exceed her survivor life expectancy until 2010. Between 1930 and 2000, joint life expectancy for black couples increased from 8.4 years to 13.4 years and her survivor life expectancy increased from 11.3 years to 13.3 years. In 2010, joint life expectancy (15.5 years) finally exceeded her survivor life expectancy (13.5 years).

The survivor life expectancy of the husbands of 60 year old women has slowly increased between 1930 and 2010. In 1930, a white (black) husband who outlived his wife could expect to live 8.5 years (8.7 years) as a widower. By 2010, the survivor life expectancy for white (black) husbands increased to 9.5 years (10.1 years).

The probability that the wife will be the surviving spouse follows an inverted u-shaped pattern similar to her survivor life expectancy. In 1930, the probability that a 60 year old white woman would outlive her husband was 0.56. This probability increased steadily, reaching a peak of 0.69 in 1980 before falling to 0.63 in 2010. The pattern for black women is similar: the probability of being the surviving spouse was 0.58 in 1930, rose to 0.70 in 1990, and then fell to 0.63 in 2010.

Two factors underlie these observed patterns. In recent decades, the life expectancy of older men has increased faster than the life expectancy of older women, implying an increase in joint life expectancy and a decrease in her survivor life expectancy. The age gap between spouses has also fallen, and fell markedly between 2000 and 2010, but the change in the life expectancies of older men and women, not changes in the age gap, are the primary drivers of the observed patterns. We performed counterfactual calculations of joint life expectancy holding the age gap fixed at its 2010 level. Because the average age gap exceeds two years in all census years except 2010, replacing the actual age gaps in each year with the 2010 age gaps raises joint life expectancy and lowers survivor life expectancy. With one exception, however, the decade-to-decade patterns noted above remain.

Dispersion

Of course, like individual life expectancies, joint and survivor life expectancies are summary measures of age-specific mortality distributions and provide no information about their dispersion. To provide information about the dispersion of the mortality distributions around the joint life expectancy and survivor life expectancies, we return to our focal couple (a 60-year-old non-Hispanic white woman and a 62-year-old non-Hispanic white man) and calculate the probability that this randomly formed couple will survive ten, twenty, thirty, or forty years. We present the results in a 4×4 matrix (Table 3). The matrix shows the probabilities of various realizations of the timing of death for the couple and the surviving spouse. The four corner cells, which are the four lowest probabilities in the matrix, correspond to the cases in which one or both spouses die in either the first decade or the last decade. For example, the top right cell, (1, 4), corresponds to the case in which the husband dies within the first decade and the wife lives at least until age 90; thus, the probability that the wife will be a widow for more than 30 years is 0.034. In cells above the principal diagonal, the wife is the surviving spouse; in cells below, the husband is the surviving spouse; in the four cells on the principal diagonal, surviving spouse may be the wife or the husband. If mortality were evenly distributed across the 16 cells of the matrix, each would have a probability of 0.0625. The most likely scenario is that both spouses live between 20 and 30 additional years, but this probability is only 0.129.

Table 3. Distribution of expected mortality.

Wife is aged 60, he is aged 62.

WIFE
HUSBAND 61–70 71–80 81–90 91–100 Total
63–72 0.031 0.057 0.078 0.034 0.200
73–82 0.045 0.084 0.114 0.050 0.293
83–92 0.051 0.094 0.129 0.056 0.331
93–102 0.027 0.050 0.068 0.030 0.175
Total 0.154 0.285 0.390 0.170 1.000

The wide dispersion of these distributions owes little to the differing mortality rates of men and women or to the age gap between spouses, but reflects the large overlap of the underlying mortality distributions. If we perform the same calculation for a same-sex couple consisting of two 60-year-old women or two 62-year-old men, the resulting matrix is very similar. The wide dispersion in mortality rates around joint and survivor life expectancies suggests the need for caution when interpreting summary measures such as joint life expectancy and survivor life expectancy, but similar caution is also required when interpreting individual life expectancy.

Conclusion

We have defined and calculated measures of the joint and survivor life expectancies of couples. Although it is intuitively appealing to calculate couple-based measures of life expectancy directly from men’s and women’s life expectancies, such calculations overstate joint life expectancy and dramatically understate survivor life expectancy of older couples because they fail to take account of the substantial overlap in spouses’ mortality distributions. We calculate joint and survivor life expectancy for randomly formed older couples using 2010 data disaggregated by race and ethnicity and by education, and for white and black couples from 1930–2010. Although the measures are tedious to calculate, tools such as the "Life Expectancy Calculator" on the Social Security website [31] could easily be augmented to calculate couples’ joint and survivor life expectancies.

Joint and survivor life expectancies provide a more complete picture of the life course path faced by married couples. Comparisons by race and ethnicity, and by education, show how even without accounting for correlations, patterns of assortative marriage on race and education amplify differences in life expectancies. Viewing the measures over time, we observe how increases in the life expectancy of one gender relative to the other affects couples’ joint life expectancy and the life expectancy of the surviving spouse. Social science research that focuses on retirement, long-term care and other issues involving older individuals may benefit from incorporating joint life expectancies into the discussion. For example, recent discussions of premature mortality of white men (“deaths of despair”) may consider how a decline in the life expectancy of men impacts the expected length of widowhood, and how this in turn may impact policy.

Our aim is not to provide an in-depth analysis of the determinants of joint and survivor life expectancies across race and ethnicity, education or time. Because we base our calculations on individual life tables, we cannot take account of the possibilities that (a) healthier individuals may select into marriage, (b) marriage may itself increase life expectancy, and (c) mortality rates of husbands and wives may be correlated. Because taking account of these possibilities requires richer data, we leave these refinements for future research. Our goal is to simply highlight the measures and to note that these calculations–even with their limitations–are as important as benchmarks for retirement and long-term healthcare decisions as are individual life expectancies.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to Magali Barbieri, Itzik Faldon, Claudia Goldin, Larry Katz, Andrew Noymer, James Poterba, Peter Wiedenbeck, Robert Willis, and Jeffrey Zax for their comments and suggestions. We are also grateful to participants in the "Women Working Longer" project and those in the MRRC’s Researcher Workshop, the PAA in Austin, the NBER Cohort Studies workshop, and the Michigan Health and Retirement symposium for their comments and suggestions.

Data Availability

The data underlying this study are publicly available from the following sources: Census data: Ruggles S, Genadek K, Goeken R, Grover J, Sobek M. Integrated Public Use Microdata Series: Version 7.0 [dataset]. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota, 2017. https://doi.org/10.18128/D010.V7.0. Mortality rates: CDC, National Center for Health Statistics. Life Tables. https://www.cdc.gov/nchs/products/life_tables.htm. August 3, 2017. Mortality rates by education: Bound J, Geronimus AT, Rodriguez JM, Waidmann TA. Measuring recent apparent declines in longevity: the role of increasing educational attainment. Health Affairs. 2015;34(12):2167-73.

Funding Statement

RAP G-B2012-24 Alfred P. Sloan Foundation https://sloan.org/ The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Browning M. Saving and the intra-household distribution of income. Ricerche Economiche. 1995;49(3):277–92. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Browning M. The saving behavior of a two-person household. The Scandinavian Journal of Economics. 2000;102(2):235–51. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Lundberg S. Family bargaining and retirement behavior. In: Aaron HJ, editor. Behavioral dimensions of retirement economics. Washington D.C.: Brookings Institution Press; 1999. Pp. 253–72. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Goldman N, Lord G. Sex differences in life cycle measures of widowhood. Demography. 1983;20(2);177–95. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Bowers NL, Gerber HU, Hickman JC, Jones DA, Nesbitt CJ. Actuarial mathematics. 2nd ed. The Society of Actuaries; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Brown JR, Poterba JM. Joint life annuities and annuity demand by married couples. Journal of Risk and Insurance. 2000;67(4):527–53. [Google Scholar]
  • 7. https://www.irs.gov/publications/p590b [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Mitchell OS, Poterba JM, Warshawsky MJ, Brown JR. New evidence on the money’s worth of individual annuities. American Economic Review. 1999;89(5):1299–1318. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.van der Klaauw W, Wolpin K. Social security and the retirement and savings behavior of low-income households. Journal of Econometrics. 2008;145(1–2):21–42. 10.1016/j.jeconom.2008.05.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Goldman N. Marriage selection and mortality patterns: inferences and fallacies. Demography. 1993;30(2):189–208. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Smith KR, Zick CD. Linked Lives, dependent demise? Survival analysis of husbands and wives. Demography.1994;31(1):81–93. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Frees EW, Carriere J, Valdez E. Annuity valuation with dependent mortality. The Journal of Risk and Insurance. 1996;63(2):229–61. [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Johnson NJ, Backlund E, Sorlie PD, Loveless CA. Marital status and mortality: the national longitudinal mortality study. Annals of Epidemiology. 2000;10(4):224–38. 10.1016/s1047-2797(99)00052-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Sanders L, Melenberg B. Estimating the joint survival probabilities of married individuals. Insurance: Mathematics and Economics. 2016;67(1):88–106. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Holden KC, Kuo HD. Complex marital histories and economic well-being: the continuing legacy of divorce and widowhood as the HRS cohort approaches retirement. The Gerontologist. 1996;36(3):383–90. 10.1093/geront/36.3.383 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.CDC, National Center for Health Statistics. Life Tables. https://www.cdc.gov/nchs/products/life_tables.htm. August 3, 2017.
  • 17.Ruggles S, Genadek K, Goeken R, Grover J, Sobek M. Integrated Public Use Microdata Series: Version 7.0 [dataset]. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota, 2017. 10.18128/D010.V7.0. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Bound J, Geronimus AT, Rodriguez JM, Waidmann TA. Measuring recent apparent declines in longevity: the role of increasing educational attainment. Health Affairs. 2015;34(12):2167–73. 10.1377/hlthaff.2015.0481 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Smith DP, Bradshaw BS. Rethinking the Hispanic paradox: death rates and life expectancy for us non-Hispanic white and Hispanic populations. American Journal of Public Health. 2006;96(9):1686–93. 10.2105/AJPH.2003.035378 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Goldman N. Will the Latino mortality advantage endure? Research on Aging. 2016;38(3):263–82. 10.1177/0164027515620242 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Black DA, Hsu YC, Sanders SG, Steuerle Schofield L, Taylor LJ. The Methuselah effect: the pernicious effects of unreported deaths on old-age mortality estimates. Demography. 2017; 54(6):2001–24. 10.1007/s13524-017-0623-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Costa DL, Kahn ME. Power couples: changes in the locational choice of the college education, 1940–1990. The Quarterly Journal of Economics. 2000;115(4):1287–1315. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Compton JR. Pollak RA. Why are power couples increasingly located in large metropolitan areas. Journal of Labor Economics. 2007;25(3):475–512. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Rostron BL, Boies JL, Arias E. Education reporting and classification on death certificates in the United States. National Center for Health Statistics. Vital and Health Statistics. 2010;2(151). [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Hendi AS. Trends in U.S. life expectancy gradients: the role of changing educational composition. International Journal of Epidemiology. 2015;33(3):946–55. 10.1093/ije/dyv062 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Sasson I. Trends in life expectancy and lifespan variation by educational attainment: United States, 1990–2010. Demography. 2016;53(2):269–93. 10.1007/s13524-015-0453-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Meara ER, Richards S, Cutler DM. The gap gets bigger: changes in mortality and life expectancy, by education, 1981–2000. Health Affairs. 2008;27(2):350–60. 10.1377/hlthaff.27.2.350 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Olshansky SJ, Antonucci T, Berkman L, Binstock RH, Bӧrsch-Supan A, Cacioppo JT, et al. Differences in life expectancy due to race and educational differences are widening, and many may not catch up. Health Affairs. 2012;31(8):1803–13. 10.1377/hlthaff.2011.0746 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Chetty R, Stepner M, Abraham S, Lin S, Scuderi B, Turner N, et al. The association between income and life expectancy in the United States, 2001–2014. JAMA.2016;315:1750–66. 10.1001/jama.2016.4226 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Goldin C, Lleras-Muney A. XX>XY?: The changing female advantate in life expectancy. Journal of Health Economics. 2019; 67: 102224. 10.1016/j.jhealeco.2019.102224 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.https://www.ssa.gov/oact/population/longevity.html

Decision Letter 0

Bryan L Sykes

24 Sep 2020

PONE-D-20-17148

The Life Expectancy of Older Couples And Surviving Spouses

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Pollak,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

The reviewers and I see great merit in your work. However, there are several areas where the manuscript can be improved prior to publication.  As you will see, the reviewers were in agreement about the importance of your contribution, and offer a number of different ways to strengthen your work (e.g., reorganizing your paper, rounding out the literature review, explicating the methodological approach in more detail, etc.). 

My read of your work is that you offer a compelling approach for how couple decisions around retirement and widowhood matter in life-course research.  In addition to the reviewer comments, there are two additional aspects that I believe would strengthen your manuscript. 

First, it would be helpful to step back and re-frame your paper.  Although your empirical inquiry is of importance to the Social Security Administration, one could easily think of other reasons that might motivate this inquiry.  For instance, it could be that "deaths of despair" have totally shifted ages of death for white men of lower SES in ways that will affect the individual, joint, and survivor life-expectancies.  I would suggest that you draw out the social significance of your inquiry by, potentially, framing your inquiry around Case & Deaton's work on increases in premature mortality among white men. 

Similarly, the period of your study (2010) is right on the tail-end of the Great Recession (2009), which certainly would affect retirement decisions and wealth accumulation.  You may want to engage or theorize how the Great Recession affected retirement decisions among men and women in the age group that you study. 

Second, your findings on race and education are surprising, in large part, because conventional wisdom is that black men have a lower life-expectancy than white men.  The same is true for Black women.  Yet, to find that marriage somehow leads to higher survivor life-expectancy among blacks than whites suggests that the classic economic model of gains to marriage for black men and women need to be reconceptualized to account for mortality differentials.  You may want to discuss these reversals in the context of marital gains. 

Lastly, you may want to rewrite your abstract.  I don't think lines 48 (beginning with "To illustrate,...") through 56 are necessary.  Instead, I would like to encourage you to discuss your method and general findings (including race & education) in the abstract. 

Please submit your revised manuscript by Nov 08 2020 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Bryan L. Sykes, Ph.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. PLOS requires an ORCID iD for the corresponding author in Editorial Manager on papers submitted after December 6th, 2016. Please ensure that you have an ORCID iD and that it is validated in Editorial Manager. To do this, go to ‘Update my Information’ (in the upper left-hand corner of the main menu), and click on the Fetch/Validate link next to the ORCID field. This will take you to the ORCID site and allow you to create a new iD or authenticate a pre-existing iD in Editorial Manager. Please see the following video for instructions on linking an ORCID iD to your Editorial Manager account: https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=_xcclfuvtxQ

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: I like the paper and find the topic important. As stated in my report, I do not always find it easy to follow the estimations in the paper. My report suggests a restructuring and gives examples of where I find the paper unclear.

Reviewer #2: This is a very interesting and well written paper that I enjoyed reading. I think however that the authors need to address some additional comments before publication.

1. All analyses are done using period life expectancy data and referring to period life expectancy. It is important to discuss the relationship between period and cohort life expectancy and to specifically address how their period life expectancy estimates relate to cohort life expediencies (which for the cohorts under consideration is still unknown, but can be estimated).

2. I would like to see a discussion of the interdependence of wife’s and husband’s mortality. For instance, a large body of research shows that widowhood results in an increased risk of death, specifically for men and that is particularly high in the first year after the spouse dies. Although the authors correctly point out that “the couple life cycle models must be substantially complex”, this interdependence is not mentioned. This interdependence is not addressed and I’d like to see a discussion how the authors’ estimates, and specifically the survivor life expectancy, are affected by this interdependence in life expectancy.

3. The observed period life expectancy (used in the analyses) is a mixture of the mortality of married and unmarried individuals. As a result, for married individuals the period life table overestimates mortality, while for unmarried individuals it underestimates. This has implications for the calculated joint life expectancy (underestimating it) and can substantially shift the results. The authors need to address this important issue and discuss how it affects their results.

Points 3 and 4 are very briefly mentioned in the text (lines 193-201), but how these isues affect the authors’ estimates is not discussed. I recommend to expand this discussion and address implications for your results.

4. I am also wondering why the analyses are based on 2010 data and are not using a more recent dataset reflecting more recent trends in the US mortality.

Some additional comments:

Clarify in the abstract which data source is used for this analysis.

The abstract should also mention that the paper investigates differences in joint and survivor life expectancy by race and ethnicity and education.

Clarify how does your approach differ from what Goldman and Lord have done in the past. Related to this, clarify what is exactly the contribution of this paper and how do the present results change our understanding/thinking, what are the implications of these results. The authors point that the concept of joint life expectancy has not bee widely used, and instead age specific mortality rates are widely used in different applications. Why should we instead start using joined life expectancy? Why is the concept presented in this paper advantageous? A broader discussion of these topics will better motivate the paper.

Also, I suggest to move the text in lines 181-201 into the introduction; it is somewhat misplaced in the methods section.

Line 229 – why is race/ethnicity of couples defined by race of wife? If race of husband is used instead, will this change the results?

The differences in joint and survivor life expectancy by race and education are striking, but the implications of these differences are not discussed at all. Expanding this discussion will contribute to the relevance of the paper.

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

Attachment

Submitted filename: Referee report PONE-D-20-17148.pdf

PLoS One. 2021 May 14;16(5):e0250564. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0250564.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


16 Feb 2021

Editor’s Comments

First, it would be helpful to step back and re-frame your paper. Although your empirical inquiry is of importance to the Social Security Administration, one could easily think of other reasons that might motivate this inquiry. For instance, it could be that "deaths of despair" have totally shifted ages of death for white men of lower SES in ways that will affect the individual, joint, and survivor life-expectancies. I would suggest that you draw out the social significance of your inquiry by, potentially, framing your inquiry around Case & Deaton's work on increases in premature mortality among white men.

In the introduction, we included a point on how the consideration of joint and survivor life expectancies could add to the discussion of inequalities by race and ethnicity, and by education. In particular, see lines 112-121. We also note in the conclusion (lines 427-430) that even without accounting for correlations, patterns of assortative mating by race and education amplify differences in life expectancies.

We agree that there is an important dimension of the “deaths of despair” that is missed when the focus is on the individual mortality, namely the impact that individual mortality has on the life course of others in the household. We note this in the conclusion (lines 430-436) as an avenue for future research. However, we did not discuss the issue in detail for two reasons. First, we focus on women who are married at age 60 and their husbands. Given the age and marital status of our focus, we are missing the vast majority of those affected by the increased mortality due to drug overdose and suicide. Second, our focus in this paper is to highlight the importance of joint and survivor life expectancies and the broad trends. It is beyond the scope of this paper to analyze or speculate on the social and economic determinants of the trends and we leave that for future research.

Similarly, the period of your study (2010) is right on the tail-end of the Great Recession (2009), which certainly would affect retirement decisions and wealth accumulation. You may want to engage or theorize how the Great Recession affected retirement decisions among men and women in the age group that you study.

You are certainly correct that the Great Recession would impact the retirement decisions of the couples in whom we are interested. Indeed, this paper is an offshoot from a project that aimed to look at the determinants of women’s labour force participation following the Great Recession. Our focus was initially on how changes in joint and survivor life expectancy alters the time horizon that older couples face when making their retirement and savings decisions. In particular, we noted that there are two horizons to consider when making these decisions - the length of time expected to be alive together, and the length of time expected to be a widow or widower. Our goal here is therefore to explain and document the benchmarks of what these time horizons would look like for randomly formed couples – and to note that the back of the envelope calculations that one might rely on, using individual life expectancies, are quite wrong.

Second, your findings on race and education are surprising, in large part, because conventional wisdom is that black men have a lower life-expectancy than white men. The same is true for Black women. Yet, to find that marriage somehow leads to higher survivor life-expectancy among blacks than whites suggests that the classic economic model of gains to marriage for black men and women need to be reconceptualized to account for mortality differentials. You may want to discuss these reversals in the context of marital gains.

We are glad you found the race and education numbers intriguing. However, the higher survivor life expectancy among black women is not related to the gains to marriage. Our calculations are simply capturing the effect of overlapping age-specific mortality rates of men and women. The survivor life-expectancy of black women is due to the higher gap between mortality of black women and men, compared to gap in mortality of white men and women. In other words, black women can expect longer widowhood.

Lastly, you may want to rewrite your abstract. I don't think lines 48 (beginning with "To illustrate,...") through 56 are necessary. Instead, I would like to encourage you to discuss your method and general findings (including race & education) in the abstract.

Thank you. We have substantially revised the abstract with your suggestions.

Reviewers' comments:

Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes

The data that we use in the paper is publicly available and noted in references number [16-17].

Reviewer 1:

All analyses are done using period life expectancy data and referring to period life expectancy. It is important to discuss the relationship between period and cohort life expectancy and to specifically address how their period life expectancy estimates relate to cohort life expediencies (which for the cohorts under consideration is still unknown, but can be estimated).

Our goal for these calculations was to provide benchmark figures for joint and survivor life expectancies. We kept the calculations as close to the individual life expectancy calculations that are most used, using age-specific mortality rates. We note in lines 130-135 the issue of period and cohort life expectancy calculations however we leave the specific calculations to further research.

I would like to see a discussion of the interdependence of wife’s and husband’s mortality. For instance, a large body of research shows that widowhood results in an increased risk of death, specifically for men and that is particularly high in the first year after the spouse dies. Although the authors correctly point out that “the couple life cycle models must be substantially complex”, this interdependence is not mentioned. This interdependence is not addressed and I’d like to see a discussion how the authors’ estimates, and specifically the survivor life expectancy, are affected by this interdependence in life expectancy.

We deliberately exclude interdependencies with the construction of random couples. Our calculations are by design average measures based on random matches of individuals identified only by age, sex, race and education. We do this so that these benchmark figures are analogous to the standard individual life expectancies. Just as the standard life expectancy does not account for an individual’s choices made over their lifetime (which may lead one to a higher or lower life duration than the average) the population based joint and survivor life expectancies do not account for the choice of which particular spouse to marry, nor to the life choices of the spouses once they are married. We discuss this in the text lines 149-156.

The observed period life expectancy (used in the analyses) is a mixture of the mortality of married and unmarried individuals. As a result, for married individuals the period life table overestimates mortality, while for unmarried individuals it underestimates. This has implications for the calculated joint life expectancy (underestimating it) and can substantially shift the results. The authors need to address this important issue and discuss how it affects their results.

This is true. However, it would not be appropriate to use the mortality rates of married individuals rather than the mortality rates of the full population to calculate life expectancies of married individuals. To do so would assume that the individual is married until death, which is rarely true of both spouses. To account for the differences by married/not, we would need longitudinal data on married couples at age 60 and followed forward until the deaths of both. We leave this for future research, as our goal for this paper was to provide benchmark estimates using the data that are easily available. Moreover, it should be noted that the annual mortality rates of married and unmarried individuals may be capturing the mortality effects of other characteristics that are correlated with both marriage and mortality, and not necessarily reflecting an effect of marriage per se. We discuss this in the text in lines 136-148.

Points 3 and 4 are very briefly mentioned in the text (lines 193-201), but how these isues affect the authors’ estimates is not discussed. I recommend to expand this discussion and address implications for your results.

We expand on these points in the introduction, but are unable to say how exactly these issues affect the estimates. Our estimates in this paper are meant to provide a baseline which can then be compared with refined estimates based on couple rather than individual data.

I am also wondering why the analyses are based on 2010 data and are not using a more recent dataset reflecting more recent trends in the US mortality.

We have added in a section that compares the data from decennial years over time (lines 320-385). This was the initial plan of the paper, and this is why we focussed on 2010 for the cross-section comparisons – it is the latest decennial year available. Rather than discuss deviations due to economic conditions, etc., our goal is to first document long-term trends in joint and survivor life expectancies.

Clarify in the abstract which data source is used for this analysis. The abstract should also mention that the paper investigates differences in joint and survivor life expectancy by race and ethnicity and education.

Thank you. We have substantially altered the abstract to address the comments of the referees and the editor.

Reviewer 2:

Clarify how does your approach differ from what Goldman and Lord have done in the past. Related to this, clarify what is exactly the contribution of this paper and how do the present results change our understanding/thinking, what are the implications of these results. The authors point that the concept of joint life expectancy has not bee widely used, and instead age specific mortality rates are widely used in different applications. Why should we instead start using joined life expectancy? Why is the concept presented in this paper advantageous? A broader discussion of these topics will better motivate the paper.

The approach is not different, our contribution is to highlight how the approach can be applied so that differences over time, by race and ethnicity, and by education provide a fuller picture of joint and survivor LE. We should not consider “Male LE” or “Female LE” in isolation because many decisions are made jointly. Also, when we consider by race and ethnicity, and education, we can see that inequality in individual LE gets compounded into inequalities in joint and survivor LE.

Also, I suggest to move the text in lines 181-201 into the introduction; it is somewhat misplaced in the methods section.

Thank you for this suggestion. We have substantially altered the introduction to clarify the calculations in the paper, and have moved this text as per your suggestion.

Line 229 – why is race/ethnicity of couples defined by race of wife? If race of husband is used instead, will this change the results?

The proportion of older couples who are mixed race is small, whether they are anchored on the husband or the wife. In 2010, 96 percent of married 60 year old non-Hispanic white women have non-Hispanic white husbands; while 94 percent of married 62 year old non-Hispanic white men have non-Hispanic white husbands. The corresponding figures for black and Hispanic couples are lower: 91 percent of black women are married to black men and 88 percent of black men are married to black women; 76 percent of Hispanic women are married to Hispanic men and 78 percent of Hispanic men are married to Hispanic women.

We provide information for only same race/ethnicity couples to highlight assortative marriage but, as with all the summary numbers provided, we are admittedly not capturing all factors that affect life expectancies. The proportion of couples that are represented in the summary measures does not depend on whether we anchor race/ethnicity on the husband or the wife.

The differences in joint and survivor life expectancy by race and education are striking, but the implications of these differences are not discussed at all. Expanding this discussion will contribute to the relevance of the paper.

See above. We add brief discussions of this issue in the introduction and conclusion. A full analysis of the implications of this is beyond the scope of this paper.

Reviewer 3:

Generally, the empirical analysis seems convincing, although it is not always quite clear what theauthors have done in every step, and how the data was used.

Thank you for this comment. We have substantially altered the description of the method.

I think the paper would be more interesting to read if it more clearly explained early on in the paper, perhaps in the Introduction, why we would expect that it is important to take within couple mortality correlations into account. The background and previous literature on intramarriage correlations and sorting into marriage is not introduced and discussed before 7 pages into the paper (lines 181-195), and the literature survey is very short. The paper would improve by extending the literature discussion on why we should expect intra-spouse correlation in health and mortality, citing papers on 1) assortative mating in health, wealth etc. (e.g. Guner,Kulikova & Llull in European Economic Review, Vol. 104, May 2018) and selection into marriage, intra-marriage correlation in health behavior through habits, smoking, drinking, exercise, diet etc., as well as information exchange within couples (e.g. Fadlon and Nielsen, 2019, American Economic Review, “Family Health Behaviors”), 3) coordination of couples’ retirement decisions that may impact health and life expectancy (e.g. Gustman and Steinmeier 2000 in Journal of Labor Economics). This section may also include a short discussion of health effects of bereavement of a spouse.

In this new version of the paper we are more clear in noting that we are specifically not taking into account the within couple mortality correlations. While we believe it is important to address these correlations in future research but we first need a clean slate benchmark measure of couple life expectancies which do not incorporate these correlations. The consideration of how couples’ realized death patterns are correlated requires longitudinal data and is necessarily backward looking. Our aim here is to construct measures that are similar to the individual life expectancy measures and so are based on population averages and do not incorporate individual couple deviations.

The structure of the Methods section is a bit confusing. It starts out with a very brief mention of the data, then goes on to present the idea of constructing life expectancies 0based on individual mortality data and showing some results based on this approach in Table 1, then moves to a short literature discussion (mentioned in point 1 above), and then finally presents the methods used. A restructuring of this section would be helpful.

We have restructured this and hope it is more clear.

Following point 2), I suggest that the authors create a specific Data section explaining in a bit more detail about the 2010 Census and NCHS data for readers who are not quite familiar with these datasets. It was not clear to me whether the authors had access to information at the individual for both partners in a couple? Also, what is the source of the life expectancies from these data? Maybe it is stated in another part of the paper.

In this version, we have been more careful to explain that the couples for which we are calculating joint and survivor life expectancies are randomly formed couples. The life expectancies are calculated from the individual mortality rates.

It is not clear to me what data are used to create Table 1. Moreover, in general, tables should be self-explanatory, without having to read the text in detail. I suggest making a note to Table 1 with instruction about how to read it. Moreover, in line 197: When speaking about “Our calculations, …”, I presume that the authors are speaking about the calculations leading to Table

1?

We have moved this discussion to the end, and believe that in this section, the construction of this table now makes more sense. Thank you for this point.

Methodological contribution: In order to assess the significance of taking proper account of

intra-couple correlation in health and mortality rather than just using individual data, would it be possible e.g. to generate a table like Table 1, but just based on the data for couples?

We have tried to be more clear that we are not using couple data, but rather forming couples randomly and applying individual mortality data.

I found the last four lines of the Conclusion confusing, especially point c), as I thought that the point of the whole exercise in the paper is to show the importance of taking marital status into account when calculating life expectancies for couples.

We hope the goal of this paper is more clear in this version.

Specific and minor comments

Lines 94-98 in the Introduction more or less repeat the text in lines 65-68.

• Thank you. We have changed the introduction substantially based on the comments provided.

Attachment

Submitted filename: PLOS_ONE Response to Editor and Referees.pdf

Decision Letter 1

Bryan L Sykes

12 Apr 2021

The Life Expectancy of Older Couples And Surviving Spouses

PONE-D-20-17148R1

Dear Dr. Pollak,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Bryan L. Sykes, Ph.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: I think the revised manuscript has improved substantially. My main concerns with the prior version were that the structure of the paper was confusing, the description of the data unclear, and the contribution not particularly well articulated. I think the authors have taken very well care of all my comments. The paper now reads well, and the main message is clearly communicated. The paper thus provides a nice contribution to the literature on life expectancy in couples.

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Acceptance letter

Bryan L Sykes

6 May 2021

PONE-D-20-17148R1

The life expectancy of older couples and surviving spouses

Dear Dr. Pollak:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Bryan L. Sykes

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Referee report PONE-D-20-17148.pdf

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: PLOS_ONE Response to Editor and Referees.pdf

    Data Availability Statement

    The data underlying this study are publicly available from the following sources: Census data: Ruggles S, Genadek K, Goeken R, Grover J, Sobek M. Integrated Public Use Microdata Series: Version 7.0 [dataset]. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota, 2017. https://doi.org/10.18128/D010.V7.0. Mortality rates: CDC, National Center for Health Statistics. Life Tables. https://www.cdc.gov/nchs/products/life_tables.htm. August 3, 2017. Mortality rates by education: Bound J, Geronimus AT, Rodriguez JM, Waidmann TA. Measuring recent apparent declines in longevity: the role of increasing educational attainment. Health Affairs. 2015;34(12):2167-73.


    Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES