Skip to main content
JAMA Network logoLink to JAMA Network
. 2021 May 21;4(5):e2113020. doi: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2021.13020

Long-term Intake of Gluten and Cognitive Function Among US Women

Yiqing Wang 1,2, Benjamin Lebwohl 3,4, Raaj Mehta 1,2, Yin Cao 5,6, Peter H R Green 3, Francine Grodstein 7,8, Manol Jovani 9, Paul Lochhead 1, Olivia I Okereke 10,11,12, Laura Sampson 13, Walter C Willett 10,12,13, Qi Sun 10,12,13, Andrew T Chan 1,2,10,13,
PMCID: PMC8140370  PMID: 34019084

Key Points

Question

Is dietary gluten intake associated with cognitive function in the general population?

Findings

In this cohort study of 13 494 women at midlife without a celiac disease diagnosis, there was no statistical evidence of any association of long- or short-term gluten intake with cognitive function.

Meaning

These findings suggest that in the absence of celiac disease, restriction of dietary gluten to maintain cognitive function is not warranted.


This cohort study examines whether gluten intake is associated with cognitive function in women without celiac disease.

Abstract

Importance

Gluten avoidance has been suggested as having a benefit to cognitive health among the general population, given the link between gluten and cognitive impairment in patients with celiac disease. However, data are lacking in individuals without celiac disease.

Objective

To examine whether gluten intake is associated with cognitive function in women without celiac disease.

Design, Setting, and Participants

This cohort study included US women who participated in the longitudinal, population-based Nurses’ Health Study II and had not previously or subsequently been diagnosed with celiac disease. Dietary data were collected from 1991 to 2015, and data on cognitive function were collected from 2014 to 2019. Data analysis was conducted from October 2020 to April 2021.

Exposures

Energy-adjusted gluten intake, cumulatively averaged across questionnaire cycles prior to cognitive assessment.

Main Outcomes and Measures

Three standardized cognitive scores assessed by the validated Cogstate Brief Battery: (1) psychomotor speed and attention score, (2) learning and working memory score, and (3) global cognition score. Higher scores indicated better performance.

Results

The cohort included 13 494 women (mean [SD] age, 60.6 [4.6] years). The mean (SD) gluten intake was 6.3 (1.6) g/d. After controlling for demographic and lifestyle risk factors in linear regression, no significant differences in standardized cognitive scores (mean [SD], 0 [1]) by quintile of gluten intake were found across highest and lowest quintiles of gluten intake (psychomotor speed and attention: −0.02; 95% CI, −0.07 to 0.03; P for trend = .22; learning and working memory: 0.02; 95% CI, −0.03 to 0.07; P for trend = .30; global cognition: −0.002; 95% CI, −0.05 to 0.05; P for trend = .78). The null associations persisted after additional adjustment for major sources of dietary gluten (ie, refined grains or whole grains), comparing decile categories of gluten intake, using gluten intake updated at each previous questionnaire cycle, or modeling changes in gluten intake. Similarly, these associations were not materially altered in sensitivity analyses that excluded women who had reported cancer or dementia diagnosis or had not completed all dietary assessments.

Conclusions and Relevance

In this study, long-term gluten intake was not associated with cognitive scores in middle-aged women without celiac disease. Our results do not support recommendations to restrict dietary gluten to maintain cognitive function in the absence of celiac disease or established gluten sensitivity.

Introduction

Gluten is a protein present in wheat, barley, and rye.1 It is a ubiquitous component of Western diet, with an estimated intake of 5 to 20 g/d.1 In celiac disease, gluten triggers severe immune responses characterized by autoantibodies and villus atrophy.2 This immune reaction has been linked to multiple neuropsychiatric symptoms, including cognitive impairment, depression, and anxiety, as summarized in several reviews.3,4,5 A longitudinal study of 11 patients aged 22 to 39 years with celiac disease showed that following a gluten-free diet for a year resulted in mucosal healing and modest improvement in cognitive performance.6

Although gluten is not generally believed to cause harm to individuals without celiac disease, gluten-free diets have become increasingly popular in the United States. According to the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey,7 the percentage of people without celiac disease avoiding gluten increased from 0.5% in 2009 to 1.7% in 2014, while the prevalence of celiac disease remained low during the same period (0.7%). In addition, 2 online surveys of more than 1000 US residents conducted in 2015 reported that 20% to 21% of consumers avoided dietary gluten,8,9 with a higher percentage in women (23%) than men (19%). Since the 2013 publication of Grain Brain,10 a New York Times best-selling book that has been translated into more than 30 languages, the assertion that gluten has a deleterious effect on cognitive health has become popularized among the lay public. However, there is a lack of epidemiological study demonstrating the association between gluten intake and cognitive function among individuals without celiac disease or nonceliac gluten sensitivity. Therefore, we aimed to investigate whether long-term gluten intake is associated with cognitive function in a cohort of women at midlife without celiac disease, using dietary data collected over 2 decades and cognitive scores assessed with validated tools.

Methods

Study Population

We used the Nurses’ Health Study II (NHS II), a nationwide prospective cohort of female nurses aged 25 to 42 years when enrolled in 1989.11 Lifestyle and health information was collected using biennial questionnaires. Dietary information had been collected every 4 years since 1991 using a validated food frequency questionnaire (FFQ).12,13,14 The return of mailed questionnaires implied informed consent. More than 90% of potential person-years were retained in follow-ups. Since 2014, 40 082 women with known email addresses and information on trauma exposure and posttraumatic stress disorder were invited to the Cognitive Function Study.15 Women who completed the cognitive assessment and provided plausible dietary information (600-3500 kcal/d) were eligible for the analysis. Participants who responded to this cognitive assessment were similar to the nonresponders in terms of age, body mass index (BMI; calculated as weight in kilograms divided by height in meters squared), alcohol intake, physical activity, and depressive symptoms.15 We excluded women who did not complete the full cognitive battery, had implausibly low cognitive scores that reflected likely technical errors, had previous diagnosis of stroke, or had previous or subsequent diagnosis of celiac disease, as these conditions may affect cognition and gluten intake. This study has been approved by the institutional review board of the Brigham and Women’s Hospital and followed the Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) reporting guideline for cohort studies.

Assessment of Gluten Intake

Our primary exposure was long-term gluten intake estimated every 4 years since 1991 using a 131-item semiquantitative FFQ (eFigure 1 in the Supplement), which has been validated by 1-week food diaries and biomarkers of diet,13,14 with a Spearman correlation coefficient of 0.58 for gluten and 0.60 for gluten-rich food.12,16 We identified gluten-containing foods, such as wheat, rye, and barley, based on year-specific ingredient lists from manufacturers or cookbooks. The quantity of gluten-containing ingredients in each food was calculated by multiplying the serving size with amounts of ingredients per serving. We then estimated gluten content by multiplying the protein proportions of these ingredients by 0.75, in line with previous studies.17,18,19,20,21,22,23 Next, we added up gluten contents in all foods to estimate total gluten intake in grams per day. To account for confounding by energy intake and potential measurement errors, we calculated energy-adjusted intake using residuals from the regression of absolute intake on total energy intake (ie, the residual method).24 We averaged gluten intake from 1991 to the last FFQ preceding the cognitive assessment to quantify long-term intake. We divided energy-adjusted gluten intake by quintiles (0.1 to <5.1, 5.1 to <5.9, 5.9 to <6.6, 6.6 to <7.6, and 7.6 to 18.3 g/d) to assess potential nonlinear associations and threshold of effect.

Assessment of Cognitive Function

The outcomes of this study were 3 standardized cognitive scores measured by the validated, self-administered, online Cogstate Brief Battery that consisted of 4 tasks.25,26,27 This battery has been demonstrated to be a reliable assessment for mild cognitive impairment28 and has been found to have comparable performance in unsupervised and supervised settings.29 We calculated the score for psychomotor speed and attention by averaging task scores on Detection (DET), during which the participant pressed a key each time a card flipped over, and Identification (IDN), during which the participant pressed the yes or no key to indicate whether a red or black card flipped over. We calculated the score for learning and working memory by averaging task scores on One Card Learning (OCL), during which the participant responded yes or no to indicate whether a card had been shown before, and One Back (ONB), during which the participant responded yes or no to indicate whether the card was the same as the previous card. We averaged the scores on all 4 tasks to represent the global cognition score. As previously described,30 we calculated DET, IDN, and ONB scores by reversing the z normalized and log10 transforming mean response times for correct answers, and we calculated the OCL score by z normalizing the arcsine square-root transformed percentage of correct responses. We restandardized these 3 composite cognitive scores using z normalization. Therefore, a higher score indicated better cognitive performance, with each unit increase representing 1 SD higher than the mean score. Maruff et al31 have shown that these composite scores were sensitive in identifying cognitive impairment and had high test-retest reliability (r = 0.95) in 643 healthy adults and 151 adults with mild cognitive impairment or Alzheimer disease.

Statistical Analysis

We used age- and multivariable-adjusted linear regression to examine the associations between gluten intake quintiles and each cognitive score. Consistent with prior analyses of data from our cohort,22,23,32,33 we tested linear trend across gluten categories by assigning the median quintile value to each quintile (4.45, 5.48, 6.23, 7.03, and 8.34 g/d) and testing it as a continuous variable. To help interpret the model results, we calculated age-equivalent differences in cognitive scores by dividing the regression coefficient of gluten by that of age obtained in the same model.

In our multivariable model constructed a priori, we included the following potential confounders as covariates: self-reported race (White or other racial/ethnic group), husband’s education as an indicator for socioeconomic status because nurses had similar education backgrounds (high school or below, college, graduate school, unmarried, or missing [n = 391]), family income (in 2010 US $),34 personal history of hypertension (yes or no),35 diabetes (yes or no), hypercholesterolemia (yes or no), myocardial infarction (yes or no),36 current depression status (yes, indicating diagnosis, treatment, and/or clinically relevant depressive symptoms according to the 10-item Centre for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale [score ≥10], or no, indicating none of the previously listed options),37,38 BMI, smoking status (never, past, current, or missing [n = 12]),39 regular use of aspirin or nonsteroidal anti-inflammatory drugs (NSAIDs; yes or no), use of multivitamins (yes or no), physical activity (metabolic-equivalents [METs]/week),40 menopausal status and menopausal hormone use (premenopausal or postmenopausal with no use, past use, or current use of hormone therapy), total energy intake (kcal/d), alcohol intake (g/d), and the Alternative Healthy Eating Index 2010 (AHEI-10, excluding alcohol and whole grains).33 We acquired these variables from the most recent questionnaire prior to cognitive testing, except for dietary factors, BMI, and physical activity, which were averaged across previous questionnaires to capture long-term patterns and reduce variance.32 Race information was collected for the NHS II because these data could provide valuable information on differences in disease risk and risk factors by race and ethnicity and help reduce health disparities by race and ethnicity. The authors categorized the racial/ethnic groups as White and other racial/ethnic group because the study cohort was predominantly White individuals (96%). When missing values occurred, we imputed with median values for family income (n = 22), BMI (n = 5), and AHEI-10 (n = 8), and carried forward the last nonmissing responses for aspirin or NSAID use (n = 2).

We conducted several secondary analyses. First, because grains are major sources of gluten and are correlated with cognition,41 we additionally adjusted for refined grains (servings per day) and whole grains (servings per day) in the multivariable model. Then, we conducted stratified analyses by age (<65 vs ≥65 years), smoking (never vs ever), BMI (<25 vs ≥25), current depression status, and history of diabetes, hypertension, and hypercholesterolemia to investigate potential effect modification by testing the interaction of gluten and these factors. Next, given that it is possible that only extreme intake of gluten affects cognition, we compared cognitive scores by gluten intake deciles. Last, we modeled the continuous gluten variable in grams per day to test for linear associations. We also conducted several sensitivity analyses to assess the robustness of our findings. First, we additionally excluded participants who (1) had ever reported cancer diagnosis (n = 1761), (2) had every reported dementia diagnosis (n = 8), (3) or had not completed all dietary assessments preceding the cognitive assessment (n = 1848). Second, to assess temporal associations, we modeled gluten intake during each 4-year questionnaire cycle before the cognitive assessment (≤4, 4-8, 8-12, 12-16, 16-20, and 20-24 years), distant past (12-24 years), recent past (4-12 years), and per 1-SD change in mean gluten intake (mean [SD], −0.87 [1.98] g/d) from distant to recent past.

We used SAS version 9.4 (SAS Institute) to perform all analyses. All tests were 2 sided and had a significance level of P < .05. The eMethods in the Supplement provide more details on measurements and analysis.

Results

Among 13 494 women (mean [SD] age, 60.6 [4.6] years; mean [SD] gluten intake, 6.3 [1.6] g/d) in the analytic cohort, the mean (SD) energy-adjusted gluten intake ranged from 4.3 (0.6) g/d in the lowest quintile to 8.6 (1.0) g/d in the highest quintile (Table 1; eFigure 2 in the Supplement). The mean age, BMI, physical activity, and diet quality were similar across gluten intake quintiles. Whereas participants in the lowest quintile tended to have the highest percentages of hypertension (1132 of 2700 [41.9%]), diabetes (236 [8.7%]), and hypercholesterolemia (1666 [61.7%]), participants in the highest quintile tended to have the highest percentages of depression (898 of 2699 [33.3%]) and never smoking (1908 [70.7%]).

Table 1. Age-Adjusted Characteristics of Women Participating in the Nurses’ Health Study II, According to Quintiles of Mean Energy-Adjusted Gluten Intakea.

Characteristics Participants by quintile of mean gluten intake, No. (%)
1, lowest (n = 2700) 2 (n = 2697) 3 (n = 2699) 4 (n = 2699) 5, highest (n = 2699)
Energy-adjusted gluten intake, mean (SD), g/d 4.3 (0.6) 5.5 (0.2) 6.2 (0.2) 7.0 (0.3) 8.6 (1.0)
Age at cognitive testing, mean (SD), yb 61.2 (4.5) 60.7 (4.6) 60.6 (4.6) 60.3 (4.5) 60.0 (4.7)
White individuals 2497 (92.5) 2603 (96.5) 2644 (98.0) 2627 (97.3) 2642 (97.9)
Husband’s educational attainment
≤High school 422 (15.6) 443 (16.4) 419 (15.5) 407 (15.1) 377 (14.0)
College 1185 (43.9) 1185 (43.9) 1178 (43.6) 1117 (41.4) 1094 (40.5)
Graduate school 737 (27.3) 775 (28.7) 763 (28.3) 852 (31.6) 902 (33.4)
Unmarried or missing 356 (13.2) 294 (10.9) 339 (12.6) 323 (12.0) 326 (12.1)
Family income, mean (SD), in thousands of 2010 US $ 82.1 (31.9) 82.1 (31.1) 83.7 (31.6) 84.6 (32.6) 85.6 (32.2)
BMI, mean (SD) 26.5 (5.7) 26.5 (5.5) 26.4 (5.6) 26.0 (5.2) 25.5 (5.4)
Currently diagnosed with depression or had clinically relevant depressive symptoms 784 (29.0) 849 (31.5) 875 (32.4) 871 (32.3) 898 (33.3)
History of hypertension 1132 (41.9) 1114 (41.3) 1046 (38.8) 965 (35.8) 900 (33.4)
History of diabetes 236 (8.7) 220 (8.2) 216 (8.0) 169 (6.3) 150 (5.6)
History of hypercholesterolemia 1666 (61.7) 1611 (59.7) 1620 (60.0) 1547 (57.3) 1434 (53.1)
History of myocardial infarction 33 (1.2) 46 (1.7) 35 (1.3) 31 (1.1) 30 (1.1)
Menopausal status and hormone use
Premenopausal 260 (9.63) 331 (12.3) 324 (12.0) 341 (12.6) 378 (14.0)
Postmenopausal and never used hormones 1021 (37.8) 1079 (40.0) 1084 (40.2) 1148 (42.5) 1183 (43.8)
Postmenopausal and formerly used hormones 547 (32.3) 450 (31.0) 486 (29.8) 484 (26.9) 441 (25.8)
Postmenopausal and currently using hormones 872 (20.3) 837 (16.7) 805 (18.0) 726 (17.9) 697 (16.3)
Smoking status
Never 1645 (60.9) 1785 (66.2) 1768 (65.5) 1826 (67.7) 1908 (70.7)
Past 929 (34.4) 819 (30.4) 856 (31.7) 803 (29.8) 730 (27.0)
Current 124 (4.6) 89 (3.3) 73 (2.7) 67 (2.5) 60 (2.2)
Missing 2 (0.1) 4 (0.1) 2 (0.1) 3 (0.1) 1 (0.04)
Multivitamin use 1536 (56.9) 1527 (56.6) 1531 (56.7) 1441 (53.4) 1408 (52.2)
Regular aspirin or NSAID use 678 (25.1) 679 (25.2) 671 (24.9) 654 (24.2) 630 (23.3)
Physical activity, mean (SD), METs/wk 24.5 (20.3) 22.2 (18.2) 22.9 (18.8) 23.1 (19.6) 22.7 (18.6)
Total calories, mean (SD), kcal/d 1795.0 (436.7) 1823.0 (424.6) 1821.3 (416.2) 1809.6 (438.9) 1744.1 (413.2)
Alcohol intake, mean (SD), g/d 5.9 (8.3) 5.5 (7.4) 5.4 (7.1) 5.3 (7.0) 4.6 (6.2)
AHEI-10 score, mean (SD)c 47.3 (9.6) 45.8 (8.7) 45.8 (8.4) 46.3 (8.4) 45.9 (8.2)
Refined grain intake, mean (SD), servings/d 0.9 (0.4) 1.1 (0.5) 1.1 (0.5) 1.3 (0.6) 1.4 (0.6)
Whole grain intake, mean (SD), servings/d 0.8 (0.5) 1.0 (0.5) 1.1 (0.6) 1.3 (0.7) 1.5 (0.9)

Abbreviations: AHEI-10, Alternative Healthy Eating Index 2010; BMI, body mass index (calculated as weight in kilograms divided by height in meters squared); MET, metabolic equivalent; NSAID, nonsteroid anti-inflammatory drug.

a

Values are adjusted for age (49-55, 56-60, 61-65, and 66-70 y). All variables were updated until the cognitive testing. Dietary intake, BMI, and physical activity were cumulatively averaged through 1991 to the most recent survey (2011 or 2015) before cognitive testing.

b

Value is not age adjusted.

c

The AHEI-10 score excluded whole grains and alcohol, so it included the following 9 components, each scored from 0 to 10, with higher scores indicating healthier diet and better adherence to the index: vegetables, fruits, sugar-sweetened beverages and fruit juice, nuts and legumes, red and/or processed meat, trans fat, long-chain (n3) fatty acids, poly-unsaturated fatty acids, and sodium.

When estimating age-adjusted mean differences in standardized cognitive scores across quintiles of gluten intake (Table 2), we found a slight increase in learning and working memory score between the highest and lowest quintiles (0.05; 95% CI, −0.004 to 0.10; P for trend = .04) and no statistically significant differences in psychomotor speed and attention score (−0.005; 95% CI, −0.06 to 0.05; P for trend = .55) and global cognition score (0.02; 95% CI, −0.03 to 0.07; P for trend = .50). After adjusting for various behavioral and health risk factors in the multivariable model, we observed no difference in psychomotor speed and attention score (−0.02; 95% CI, −0.07 to 0.03; P for trend = .22), learning and working memory score (0.02; 95% CI, −0.03 to 0.07; P for trend = .30), and global cognition score (−0.002; 95% CI, −0.05 to 0.05; P for trend = .78) across quintiles of gluten intake. These multivariable-adjusted estimates were equivalent to mean differences in psychomotor speed and attention, learning and working memory, and global cognition scores associated with 0.5 years, −0.67 years, and 0.05 years of aging, respectively.

Table 2. Differences in Standardized Cognitive Scores Associated With Quintiles of Gluten Intake Among 13 494 Womena.

Model Differences by quintiles of gluten intake, mean (95% CI)
1, lowest 2 3 4 5, highest P value for trendb
Psychomotor speed and attentionc
Standardized score, mean (SD) −0.03 (1.03) 0.01 (1.00) 0.02 (0.96) −0.002 (1.02) 0.02 (0.99) NA
Age-adjusted 0 [Reference] 0.01 (−0.04 to 0.07) 0.02 (−0.03 to 0.07) −0.02 (−0.07 to 0.04) −0.005 (−0.06 to 0.05) .55
Multivariable-adjustedd 0 [Reference] 0.01 (−0.04 to 0.06) 0.01(−0.04 to 0.06) −0.02 (−0.08 to 0.03) −0.02 (−0.07 to 0.03) .22
Multivariable-adjusted, with additional adjustment for refined grainsd 0 [Reference] 0.01 (−0.04 to 0.07) 0.02 (−0.03 to 0.07) −0.02 (−0.07 to 0.04) −0.01 (−0.06 to 0.05) .51
Multivariable-adjusted, with additional adjustment for whole grainsd 0 [Reference] 0.01 (−0.04 to 0.07) 0.02 (−0.03 to 0.07) −0.01 (−0.07 to 0.04) −0.002 (−0.06 to 0.06) .66
Learning and working memoryc
Standardized score, mean (SD) −0.04 (0.98) 0.0001 (0.96) 0.07 (0.98) 0.05 (0.98) 0.05 (0.96) NA
Age-adjusted 0 [Reference] 0.03 (−0.03 to 0.08) 0.09 (0.04 to 0.14) 0.06 (0.01 to 0.11) 0.05 (−0.004 to 0.10) .04
Multivariable-adjustedd 0 [Reference] 0.01 (−0.04 to 0.06) 0.07 (0.02 to 0.13) 0.04 (−0.01 to 0.09) 0.02 (−0.03 to 0.07) .30
Multivariable-adjusted, with additional adjustment for refined grainsd 0 [Reference] 0.01 (−0.04 to 0.06) 0.07 (0.02 to 0.12) 0.04 (−0.02 to 0.09) 0.02 (−0.04 to 0.07) .49
Multivariable-adjusted, with additional adjustment for whole grainsd 0 [Reference] 0.01 (−0.04 to 0.07) 0.07 (0.02 to 0.13) 0.04 (−0.01 to 0.09) 0.02 (−0.03 to 0.08) .33
Global cognitionc
Standardized score, mean (SD) −0.05 (1.02) −0.001 (0.98) 0.05 (0.97) 0.02 (1.02) 0.03 (0.97) NA
Age-adjusted 0 [Reference] 0.02 (−0.03 to 0.08) 0.06 (0.01 to 0.11) 0.02 (−0.03 to 0.07) 0.02 (−0.03 to 0.07) .50
Multivariable-adjustedd 0 [Reference] 0.01 (−0.04 to 0.07) 0.05 (−0.004 to 0.10) 0.01 (−0.05 to 0.06) −0.002 (−0.05 to 0.05) .78
Multivariable-adjusted, with additional adjustment for refined grainsd 0 [Reference] 0.02 (−0.04 to 0.07) 0.05 (−0.002 to 0.10) 0.01 (−0.05 to 0.06) −0.003 (−0.05 to 0.06) .95
Multivariable-adjusted, with additional adjustment for whole grainsd 0 [Reference] 0.02 (−0.04 to 0.07) 0.05 (0.001 to 0.11) 0.01 (−0.04 to 0.07) 0.01 (−0.05 to 0.07) .82

Abbreviation: NA, not applicable.

a

Energy-adjusted gluten intake was cumulatively averaged from 1991 to the last questionnaire cycle preceding the cognitive assessment. To infer age-equivalent differences in standardized cognitive scores, parameter estimates for age in the same multivariable-adjusted models for gluten intake were −0.04 (95% CI, −0.04 to −0.03), −0.03 (95% CI, −0.04 to −0.03), and −0.04 (95% CI, −0.05 to −0.04) for psychomotor speed and attention score, learning and working memory score, and global cognition score, respectively.

b

P value for trend was calculated using the median value for each gluten intake quintile as a continuous variable.

c

Standardized scores for psychomotor speed and attention, learning and working memory, and global cognition were calculated by standardizing the mean standardized scores of the following Cogstate battery tasks: Detection and Identification, One Card Learning and One Back, and all 4 tasks, respectively. Higher scores indicate better performance, with 1 unit increase representing 1 SD higher than the mean.

d

Adjusted for age, race, body mass index, husband’s educational attainment, family income, history of diabetes, history of hypertension, history of hypercholesterolemia, history of myocardial infarction, current depression status, smoking, aspirin or nonsteroid anti-inflammatory drug use, multivitamin use, physical activity, menopausal status and hormone use, total energy intake, alcohol intake, and Alternative Healthy Eating Index score, excluding alcohol and whole grains.

After additionally adjusting for refined grains or whole grains (Table 2), the association between gluten intake and these standardized cognitive scores remained null. The null associations persisted when modeled gluten intake as a continuous variable (eTable 1 in the Supplement) or by decile categories (eTable 2 in the Supplement).

In stratified analyses by age, smoking status, BMI, depression status, and history of diabetes, hypertension, and hypercholesterolemia (Table 3), most of the interactions between gluten and these factors were null. However, the association between gluten intake and standardized psychomotor speed and attention score was potentially modified by smoking status, with higher gluten intake associated with slightly lower score among those who ever smoked (highest vs lowest quintile: −0.09; 95% CI, −0.18 to 0.01; P for trend = .02). However, this association became null after adjustment for refined grains (highest vs lowest quintile: −0.06; 95% CI, −0.17 to 0.04; P for trend = .09) (eTable 3 in the Supplement). Although the association between gluten intake and global cognition was potentially modified by smoking status and history of hypercholesterolemia, there was no difference in global cognition score across gluten quintiles in any stratum of smoking and hypercholesterolemia status (Table 3).

Table 3. Multivariable-Adjusted Differences in Standardized Cognitive Scores Associated With Quintiles of Gluten Intake, According to Different Strataa.

Strata No. Difference by quintiles of gluten intake, mean (95% CI) P value
1, lowest 2 3 4 5 (highest) For trendb For interactionc
Psychomotor speed and attentiond
Age, y
<65 10 234 0 [Reference] 0.02 (−0.04 to 0.08) 0.03 (−0.03 to 0.09) −0.03 (−0.09 to 0.03) −0.02 (−0.08 to 0.04) .15 .62
≥65 3260 0 [Reference] −0.02 (−0.13 to 0.09) −0.05 (−0.16 to 0.06) −0.01 (−0.12 to 0.11) 0.004 (−0.11 to 0.12) .90
Smoking
Never 8932 0 [Reference] 0.02 (−0.05 to 0.08) 0.02 (−0.05 to 0.08) 0.01 (−0.06 to 0.07) 0.01 (−0.05 to 0.08) .88 .02
Ever 4550 0 [Reference] 0.003 (−0.09 to 0.09) 0.01 (−0.08 to 0.09) −0.09 (−0.18 to 0.002) −0.09 (−0.18 to 0.01) .02
BMI
<25 7032 0 [Reference] 0.01 (−0.07 to 0.08) 0.002 (−0.07 to 0.08) −0.06 (−0.14 to 0.01) −0.05 (−0.12 to 0.02) .05 .31
≥25 6462 0 [Reference] 0.01 (−0.06 to 0.09) 0.02 (−0.05 to 0.10) 0.02 (−0.06 to 0.09) 0.005 (−0.07 to 0.08) .91
Current depression status
No 9217 0 [Reference] 0.001 (−0.06 to 0.06) −0.004 (−0.07 to 0.06) −0.01 (−0.07 to 0.05) −0.02 (−0.09 to 0.04) .43 .62
Yes 4277 0 [Reference] 0.03 (−0.07 to 0.13) 0.05 (−0.05 to 0.15) −0.05 (−0.15 to 0.05) −0.05 (−0.15 to 0.05) .33
History of diabetes
No 12 503 0 [Reference] 0.02 (−0.04 to 0.07) 0.02 (−0.04 to 0.07) −0.02 (−0.07 to 0.04) −0.01 (−0.07 to 0.04) .34 .41
Yes 991 0 [Reference] −0.11 (−0.31 to 0.08) −0.06 (−0.26 to 0.14) −0.11 (−0.33 to 0.10) −0.15 (−0.38 to 0.07) .21
History of hypertension
No 8337 0 [Reference] 0.01 (−0.06 to 0.08) 0.01 (−0.06 to 0.08) −0.05 (−0.12 to 0.02) −0.04 (−0.11 to 0.03) .07 .17
Yes 5157 0 [Reference] 0.02 (−0.07 to 0.10) 0.02 (−0.07 to 0.10) 0.02 (−0.07 to 0.11) 0.01 (−0.08 to 0.10) .75
History of hypercholesterolemia
No 5616 0 [Reference] −0.01 (−0.09 to 0.08) −0.04 (−0.12 to 0.05) −0.08 (−0.16 to −0.001) −0.07 (−0.15 to 0.01) .03 .10
Yes 7878 0 [Reference] 0.02 (−0.05 to 0.09) 0.04 (−0.02 to 0.11) 0.01 (−0.06 to 0.08) 0.01 (−0.07 to 0.08) .99
Learning and working memoryd
Age, y
<65 10 234 0 [Reference] 0.02 (−0.03 to 0.08) 0.11 (0.05 to 0.16) 0.07 (0.01 to 0.13) 0.04 (−0.02 to 0.10) .16 .19
≥65 3260 0 [Reference] −0.01 (−0.12 to 0.09) −0.01 (−0.11 to 0.10) −0.04 (−0.14 to 0.07) −0.004 (−0.11 to 0.10) .80
Smoking
Never 8932 0 [Reference] 0.01 (−0.05 to 0.08) 0.06 (−0.01 to 0.12) 0.06 (−0.001 to 0.13) 0.03 (−0.04 to 0.09) .24 .31
Ever 4550 0 [Reference] 0.02 (−0.07 to 0.10) 0.10 (0.02 to 0.19) −0.01 (−0.09 to 0.08) 0.01 (−0.08 to 0.10) .99
BMI
<25 7032 0 [Reference] −0.02 (−0.09 to 0.06) 0.06 (−0.02 to 0.13) 0.05 (−0.02 to 0.12) −0.001 (−0.07 to 0.07) .62 .78
≥25 6462 0 [Reference] 0.04 (−0.03 to 0.12) 0.10 (0.02 to 0.17) 0.02 (−0.05 to 0.10) 0.05 (−0.03 to 0.13) .33
Current depression status
No 9217 0 [Reference] −0.01 (−0.07 to 0.05) 0.06 (0.003 to 0.12) 0.06 (0.0002 to 0.12) 0.03 (−0.03 to 0.09) .10 .13
Yes 4277 0 [Reference] 0.06 (−0.04 to 0.15) 0.09 (−0.001 to 0.19) 0.005 (−0.09 to 0.10) 0.01 (−0.09 to 0.10) .65
History of diabetes
No 12 503 0 [Reference] 0.02 (−0.03 to 0.07) 0.08 (0.03 to 0.14) 0.05 (−0.003 to 0.10) 0.03 (−0.03 to 0.08) .26 .63
Yes 991 0 [Reference] −0.07 (−0.26 to 0.12) −0.03 (−0.22 to 0.16) −0.06 (−0.27 to 0.15) −0.01 (−0.22 to 0.21) .95
History of hypertension
No 8337 0 [Reference] 0.001 (−0.07 to 0.07) 0.07 (0.002 to 0.13) 0.04 (−0.03 to 0.10) 0.02 (−0.04 to 0.09) .36 .94
Yes 5157 0 [Reference] 0.03 (−0.05 to 0.11) 0.08 (0.001 to 0.16) 0.05 (−0.04 to 0.13) 0.02 (−0.07 to 0.11) .57
History of hypercholesterolemia
No 5616 0 [Reference] −0.08 (−0.16 to 0.005) −0.03 (−0.11 to 0.05) −0.03 (−0.11 to 0.05) −0.06 (−0.14 to 0.02) .45 .07
Yes 7878 0 [Reference] 0.07 (0.01 to 0.14) 0.14 (0.08 to 0.21) 0.08 (0.02 to 0.15) 0.07 (0.004 to 0.14) .04
Global cognitiond
Age, y
<65 10 234 0 [Reference] 0.03 (−0.03 to 0.09) 0.08 (0.02 to 0.14) 0.02 (−0.04 to 0.08) 0.004 (−.06 to 0.06) .84 .72
≥65 3260 0 [Reference] −0.02 (−0.13 to 0.08) −0.04 (−0.14 to 0.07) −0.03 (−0.14 to 0.08) 0.0001 (−0.11 to 0.11) .96
Smoking
Never 8932 0 [Reference] 0.02 (−0.05 to 0.08) 0.04 (−0.02 to 0.11) 0.04 (−0.03 to 0.10) 0.02 (−0.04 to 0.09) .46 .03
Ever 4550 0 [Reference] 0.01 (−0.07 to 0.10) 0.06 (−0.03 to 0.15) −0.06 (−0.15 to 0.03) −0.05 (−0.15 to 0.04) .10
BMI
<25 7032 0 [Reference] −0.003 (−0.08 to 0.07) 0.03 (−0.04 to 0.10) −0.01 (−0.09 to 0.06) −0.03 (−0.10 to 0.04) .29 .41
≥25 6462 0 [Reference] 0.03 (−0.04 to 0.11) 0.07 (−0.01 to 0.14) 0.02 (−0.05 to 0.10) 0.03 (−0.05 to 0.11) .55
Current depression status
No 9217 0 [Reference] −0.005 (−0.06 to 0.06) 0.03 (−0.03 to 0.09) 0.03 (−0.04 to 0.09) 0.002 (−0.06 to 0.06) .73 .25
Yes 4277 0 [Reference] 0.05 (−0.05 to 0.15) 0.08 (−0.01 to 0.18) −0.03 (−0.13 to 0.07) −0.01 (−0.11 to 0.09) .37
History of diabetes
No 12 503 0 [Reference] 0.02 (−0.03 to 0.08) 0.06 (0.004 to 0.11) 0.01 (−0.04 to 0.07) 0.01 (−0.05 to 0.06) .96 .41
Yes 991 0 [Reference] −0.11 (−0.31 to 0.08) −0.06 (−0.25 to 0.13) −0.11 (−0.32 to 0.10) −0.11 (−0.32 to 0.11) .37
History of hypertension
No 8337 0 [Reference] 0.01 (−0.06 to 0.07) 0.04 (−0.02 to 0.11) −0.01 (−0.08 to 0.05) −0.01 (−0.08 to 0.05) .47 .34
Yes 5157 0 [Reference] 0.03 (−0.05 to 0.11) 0.06 (−0.03 to 0.14) 0.04 (−0.05 to 0.13) 0.02 (−0.07 to 0.11) .61
History of hypercholesterolemia
No 5616 0 [Reference] −0.05 (−0.13 to 0.04) −0.04 (−0.12 to 0.04) −0.07 (−0.15 to 0.01) −0.07 (−0.15 to 0.01) .06 .04
Yes 7878 0 [Reference] 0.05 (−0.01 to 0.12) 0.11 (0.04 to 0.17) 0.05 (−0.02 to 0.12) 0.04 (−0.03 to 0.11) .28

Abbreviation: BMI, body mass index (calculated as weight in kilograms divided by height in meters squared).

a

Energy-adjusted gluten intake was cumulatively averaged from 1991 to the last questionnaire cycle preceding the cognitive assessment. Model was adjusted for age, race, body mass index, husband’s educational attainment, family income, history of diabetes, history of hypertension, history of hypercholesterolemia, history of myocardial infarction, current depression status, smoking, aspirin or nonsteroid anti-inflammatory drug use, multivitamins use, physical activity, menopausal status and hormone use, total energy intake, alcohol intake, and Alternative Healthy Eating Index score, excluding alcohol and whole grains.

b

P value for trend was calculated using the median value for each gluten intake quintile as a continuous variable.

c

P value for interaction was estimated using an interaction term of gluten intake and the respective stratifying variable.

d

Standardized scores for psychomotor speed and attention, learning and working memory, and global cognition were calculated by standardizing the mean standardized scores of the following Cogstate battery tasks: Detection and Identification, One Card Learning and One Back, and all 4 four tasks, respectively. Higher scores indicate better performance, with 1 unit increase representing 1 SD higher than the mean.

In a sensitivity analysis of women who had never reported cancer diagnosis (eTable 4 in the Supplement), never reported dementia diagnosis (eTable 5 in the Supplement), or had completed all previous dietary assessments (eTable 6 in the Supplement), the null associations between gluten intake and each cognitive scores were similar to the main model. When examining gluten intake estimated during each 4-year questionnaire cycle prior to cognitive assessment (eTable 7 in the Supplement), distant past (12-24 years), and recent past (4-12 years), and change in gluten intake from distant to recent past (eTable 8 in the Supplement), the null associations between gluten intake and cognitive scores remained.

Discussion

In this cohort of middle-aged women without celiac disease, we found no evidence of a meaningful association between long-term gluten intake during 2 decades and subsequent cognitive performance on a validated cognitive battery. Short-term gluten intake and change in gluten intake were likewise not associated with cognitive performance.

The prevalence of gluten avoidance in the general population without celiac disease has markedly increased in the past decade.7,42 Many factors contribute to the popularity of a gluten-free diet, including its touted effects on weight loss, metabolic syndrome, and intestinal symptoms.10,43,44 However, many of these putative benefits lack consistent evidence.7,45 In fact, several studies have raised nutritional concerns, given that some gluten-free foods tend to contain less protein and fiber but higher saturated lipids and sodium than non–gluten-free foods.46,47 To account for potential differences in diet quality associated with different levels of gluten intake, we adjusted for the AHEI-10 score in our model. The null associations between gluten and cognitive scores remained after subsequent adjustment for refined gains or whole grains, which are major sources of dietary gluten, indicating that these null associations were independent of diet quality and gluten sources.

Recently, the concept that gluten could affect cognitive function was popularized by a best-selling book, Grain Brain.10 Concern for such an association is derived in part from patients with celiac disease. Subjective cognitive symptoms, referred to colloquially as brain fog, are often reported by patients with celiac disease preceding diagnosis and after suspected accidental gluten exposure,5 although this symptom has not been formally defined as a medical condition because of limited empirical evidence. Additionally, a large, prospective, population-based study found that celiac disease was not associated with risk for Alzheimer dementia over a median follow-up of 8.4 years, but it was associated with a slight increase in risk for vascular dementia.48

However, current findings on the association between dietary gluten and cognition in celiac disease are inconsistent. In a study of 11 patients aged 22 to 39 years who had been recently diagnosed with celiac disease, Lichtwark et al6 found that, in parallel to mucosal healing, cognitive performance was improved after following a gluten-free diet for 52 weeks. In contrast, a case-control study found that despite more than 1 to 12 years of gluten avoidance, 18 patients with celiac disease older than 65 years had worse cognitive performance compared with 18 age- and-sex-matched controls.49 These results suggest that the possible ameliorating effects of gluten avoidance on cognition may be related to intestinal health and modified by age. In our stratified analyses, we found that the null associations between gluten intake and cognitive scores were not different by age above or below 65 years. Instead, we observed a potential effect modification by smoking status, with higher gluten intake associated with lower psychomotor speed/attention score among those who ever smoked. However, this association became null after adjustment for refined gain consumption. Given that the differences in cognitive scores across the quintiles of gluten intake were extremely small (ie, <0.1 SD) and a total of 21 interactions were tested, these results are unlikely to be clinically meaningful, subject to multiple testing, and should be verified in future study.

Despite the lack of evidence on the gluten-cognition association among individuals without celiac disease or nonceliac gluten sensitivity, proponents of a wider effect of gluten beyond celiac disease have hypothesized that wheat may play a role as an exorphin, exerting central opioidlike effects and thus affecting memory and mood.50,51 Our results, based on a large population-based cohort, does not support this theory and suggest that gluten intake is not associated with cognitive decline, which is in line with the recent position of the Global Council on Brain Health.52

There are several strengths in our study, including its large sample size, high follow-up rate, long-term dietary gluten intake measured prospectively and independently of the primary outcome, and the use of a validated cognitive battery.25,26 In addition, numerous sensitivity analyses that tested gluten intake at different time periods and change in gluten intake confirmed the robustness of our findings by consistently showing a null association between gluten and cognition.

Limitations

This study has limitations. First, the study population consisted of women at midlife from a relatively homogeneous educational and socioeconomic background, potentially limiting its generalizability to other populations. In addition, we had a single measure of cognitive function, which may not adequately represent cognitive trajectories over time, and thus, future research with repeated measures is merited. Furthermore, our FFQs did not ask specifically about gluten-free products or adherence to a gluten-free diet and thus may overestimate gluten intake among participants who selectively consumed such products. Moreover, gluten was calculated based on the estimated percentages of protein in commonly consumed foods containing wheat, rye, and barley. Although this method is widely accepted and has been validated using 1-week diet records,12,13,14,16,22,23 it is insufficiently sensitive to identify individuals consuming a gluten-free diet, and thus, our conclusions should not be extrapolated to apply to that population. Nevertheless, our results showed no significant difference in cognitive scores when comparing the highest decile (median, 9.04 g/d) with the lowest decile (median, 3.97 g/d) of gluten intake. Furthermore, as an observational study, we cannot rule out the possibility of unmeasured residual confounding. However, we adjusted for many potential confounding factors, and multivariate adjustment did not materially alter our age-adjusted estimates.

Conclusions

In this study, we found no significant association of long-term, short-term, or change in gluten intake with cognitive function among women without celiac disease. Our findings suggest that restricting dietary gluten for the purpose of maintaining or improving cognition is not warranted in the absence of celiac disease or established gluten sensitivity. Further longitudinal research in more diverse population is needed to confirm our findings.

Supplement.

eMethods. Supplementary Methods

eTable 1. Mean Differences in Standardized Cognitive Scores Associated With Continuous Gluten Intake in Grams per Day Among 13 494 Women

eTable 2. Mean Differences in Standardized Cognitive Scores Associated With Deciles of Gluten Intake Among 13 494 Women

eTable 3. Multivariable-Adjusted Differences in Standardized Psychomotor Speed and Attention Score Across Quintiles of Gluten Intake by Smoking Status With Additional Adjustment for Refined Grains Among 13 494 Women

eTable 4. Sensitivity Analysis of the Mean Differences in Standardized Cognitive Scores Associated With Quintiles of Gluten Intake Among 11 733 Women Who Had Never Reported Cancer Diagnosis

eTable 5. Sensitivity Analysis of the Mean Differences in Standardized Cognitive Scores Associated With Quintiles of Gluten Intake Among 13 486 Women Who Had Never Reported Dementia Diagnosis

eTable 6. Sensitivity Analysis of the Mean Differences in Standardized Cognitive Scores Associated With Quintiles of Gluten Intake Among 11 646 Women Who Completed All Food Frequency Questionnaires During 24-Year Follow-up Prior to Cognitive Assessment

eTable 7. Multivariable-Adjusted Mean Differences in Standardized Cognitive Scores Associated With Quintiles of Gluten Intake by 4-Year Interval Prior to Cognitive Assessment Among 11 646 Women

eTable 8. Multivariable-Adjusted Mean Differences in Standardized Cognitive Scores Associated With 1 SD (1.98 g/d) Increase in Mean Gluten Intake From Distant Past (12-24 y) to Recent Past (4-12 y) of Cognitive Assessment Among 11 646 Women

eFigure 1. Nurses’ Health Study II Questionnaire in 2015 Questionnaire Cycle

eFigure 2. Study Flow Diagram

eReferences.

References

  • 1.Biesiekierski JR. What is gluten? J Gastroenterol Hepatol. 2017;32(suppl 1):78-81. doi: 10.1111/jgh.13703 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Lebwohl B, Sanders DS, Green PHR. Coeliac disease. Lancet. 2018;391(10115):70-81. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(17)31796-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Makhlouf S, Messelmani M, Zaouali J, Mrissa R. Cognitive impairment in celiac disease and non-celiac gluten sensitivity: review of literature on the main cognitive impairments, the imaging and the effect of gluten free diet. Acta Neurol Belg. 2018;118(1):21-27. doi: 10.1007/s13760-017-0870-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Jackson JR, Eaton WW, Cascella NG, Fasano A, Kelly DL. Neurologic and psychiatric manifestations of celiac disease and gluten sensitivity. Psychiatr Q. 2012;83(1):91-102. doi: 10.1007/s11126-011-9186-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Yelland GW. Gluten-induced cognitive impairment (“brain fog”) in coeliac disease. J Gastroenterol Hepatol. 2017;32(suppl 1):90-93. doi: 10.1111/jgh.13706 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Lichtwark IT, Newnham ED, Robinson SR, et al. Cognitive impairment in coeliac disease improves on a gluten-free diet and correlates with histological and serological indices of disease severity. Aliment Pharmacol Ther. 2014;40(2):160-170. doi: 10.1111/apt.12809 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Choung RS, Unalp-Arida A, Ruhl CE, Brantner TL, Everhart JE, Murray JA. Less hidden celiac disease but increased gluten avoidance without a diagnosis in the United States: findings from the National Health and Nutrition Examination Surveys from 2009 to 2014. Mayo Clinic Proceedings. Published online December 5, 2017. doi: 10.1016/j.mayocp.2016.10.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Hartman Group. Gluten free. Published September 2015. Accessed December 12, 2020. https://www.hartman-group.com/acumenPdfs/gluten-free-2015-09-03.pdf
  • 9.Riffkin R. One in five Americans include gluten-free foods in diet. Gallup. Published July 23, 2015. Accessed March 31, 2021. https://www.gallup.com/poll/184307/one-five-americans-include-gluten-free-foods-diet.aspx
  • 10.Perlmutter D. Grain Brain: The Surprising Truth About Wheat, Carbs, and Sugar—Your Brain's Silent Killers. Hachette Book Group; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Bao Y, Bertoia ML, Lenart EB, et al. Origin, methods, and evolution of the three Nurses’ Health Studies. Am J Public Health. 2016;106(9):1573-1581. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2016.303338 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Yuan C, Spiegelman D, Rimm EB, et al. Validity of a dietary questionnaire assessed by comparison with multiple weighed dietary records or 24-hour recalls. Am J Epidemiol. 2017;185(7):570-584. doi: 10.1093/aje/kww104 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Willett WC, Sampson L, Stampfer MJ, et al. Reproducibility and validity of a semiquantitative food frequency questionnaire. Am J Epidemiol. 1985;122(1):51-65. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a114086 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Rimm EB, Giovannucci EL, Stampfer MJ, Colditz GA, Litin LB, Willett WC. Reproducibility and validity of an expanded self-administered semiquantitative food frequency questionnaire among male health professionals. Am J Epidemiol. 1992;135(10):1114-1126. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a116211 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Sumner JA, Hagan K, Grodstein F, Roberts AL, Harel B, Koenen KC. Posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms and cognitive function in a large cohort of middle-aged women. Depress Anxiety. 2017;34(4):356-366. doi: 10.1002/da.22600 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Petimar J, Yue Y, Willett W, et al. E05-03: validity of estimated intake of flavonoids, gluten, and the whole foods from which they are derived. Poster presented at the 2018 annual conference of the American Society for Nutrition; June 10, 2018; Boston, Massachusetts. Accessed April 28, 2021. https://www.eventscribe.com/2018/Nutrition2018/ajaxcalls/PosterInfo.asp?efp=UlhTRFpZVVI0ODYw&PosterID=147034&rnd=0.3035883
  • 17.van Overbeek FM, Uil-Dieterman IG, Mol IW, Köhler-Brands L, Heymans HS, Mulder CJ. The daily gluten intake in relatives of patients with coeliac disease compared with that of the general Dutch population. Eur J Gastroenterol Hepatol. 1997;9(11):1097-1099. doi: 10.1097/00042737-199711000-00013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Andrén Aronsson C, Lee H-S, Hård Af Segerstad EM, et al. ; TEDDY Study Group . Association of gluten intake during the first 5 years of life with incidence of celiac disease autoimmunity and celiac disease among children at increased risk. JAMA. 2019;322(6):514-523. doi: 10.1001/jama.2019.10329 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Mårild K, Dong F, Lund-Blix NA, et al. Gluten intake and risk of celiac disease: long-term follow-up of an at-risk birth cohort. Am J Gastroenterol. 2019;114(8):1307-1314. doi: 10.14309/ajg.0000000000000255 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Lund-Blix NA, Mårild K, Tapia G, Norris JM, Stene LC, Størdal K. Gluten intake in early childhood and risk of celiac disease in childhood: a nationwide cohort study. Am J Gastroenterol. 2019;114(8):1299-1306. doi: 10.14309/ajg.0000000000000331 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Kasarda DD. Can an increase in celiac disease be attributed to an increase in the gluten content of wheat as a consequence of wheat breeding? J Agric Food Chem. 2013;61(6):1155-1159. doi: 10.1021/jf305122s [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Lebwohl B, Cao Y, Zong G, et al. Long term gluten consumption in adults without celiac disease and risk of coronary heart disease: prospective cohort study. BMJ. 2017;357:j1892. doi: 10.1136/bmj.j1892 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Zong G, Lebwohl B, Hu FB, et al. Gluten intake and risk of type 2 diabetes in three large prospective cohort studies of US men and women. Diabetologia. 2018;61(10):2164-2173. doi: 10.1007/s00125-018-4697-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Willett W, Stampfer MJ. Total energy intake: implications for epidemiologic analyses. Am J Epidemiol. 1986;124(1):17-27. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a114366 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Maruff P, Thomas E, Cysique L, et al. Validity of the CogState brief battery: relationship to standardized tests and sensitivity to cognitive impairment in mild traumatic brain injury, schizophrenia, and AIDS dementia complex. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2009;24(2):165-178. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acp010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Fredrickson J, Maruff P, Woodward M, et al. Evaluation of the usability of a brief computerized cognitive screening test in older people for epidemiological studies. Neuroepidemiology. 2010;34(2):65-75. doi: 10.1159/000264823 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Koyama AK, Hagan KA, Okereke OI, Weisskopf MG, Rosner B, Grodstein F. Evaluation of a self-administered computerized cognitive battery in an older population. Neuroepidemiology. 2015;45(4):264-272. doi: 10.1159/000439592 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Darby D, Maruff P, Collie A, McStephen M. Mild cognitive impairment can be detected by multiple assessments in a single day. Neurology. 2002;59(7):1042-1046. doi: 10.1212/WNL.59.7.1042 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Cromer JA, Harel BT, Yu K, et al. Comparison of cognitive performance on the Cogstate brief battery when taken in-clinic, in-group, and unsupervised. Clin Neuropsychol. 2015;29(4):542-558. doi: 10.1080/13854046.2015.1054437 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Lochhead P, Hagan K, Joshi AD, et al. Association between proton pump inhibitor use and cognitive function in women. Gastroenterology. 2017;153(4):971-979.e4. doi: 10.1053/j.gastro.2017.06.061 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Maruff P, Lim YY, Darby D, et al. ; AIBL Research Group . Clinical utility of the CogsSate brief battery in identifying cognitive impairment in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. BMC Psychol. 2013;1(1):30. doi: 10.1186/2050-7283-1-30 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Bernstein AM, Rosner BA, Willett WC. Cereal fiber and coronary heart disease: a comparison of modeling approaches for repeated dietary measurements, intermediate outcomes, and long follow-up. Eur J Epidemiol. 2011;26(11):877-886. doi: 10.1007/s10654-011-9626-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Chiuve SE, Fung TT, Rimm EB, et al. Alternative dietary indices both strongly predict risk of chronic disease. J Nutr. 2012;142(6):1009-1018. doi: 10.3945/jn.111.157222 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Fotenos AF, Mintun MA, Snyder AZ, Morris JC, Buckner RL. Brain volume decline in aging: evidence for a relation between socioeconomic status, preclinical Alzheimer disease, and reserve. Arch Neurol. 2008;65(1):113-120. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2007.27 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Reitz C, Tang M-X, Manly J, Mayeux R, Luchsinger JA. Hypertension and the risk of mild cognitive impairment. Arch Neurol. 2007;64(12):1734-1740. doi: 10.1001/archneur.64.12.1734 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Gharacholou SM, Reid KJ, Arnold SV, et al. Cognitive impairment and outcomes in older adult survivors of acute myocardial infarction: findings from the translational research investigating underlying disparities in acute myocardial infarction patients' health status registry. Am Heart J. 2011;162(5):860-869.e1. doi: 10.1016/j.ahj.2011.08.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Andresen EM, Malmgren JA, Carter WB, Patrick DL. Screening for depression in well older adults: evaluation of a short form of the CES-D (Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale). Am J Prev Med. 1994;10(2):77-84. doi: 10.1016/S0749-3797(18)30622-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Rock PL, Roiser JP, Riedel WJ, Blackwell AD. Cognitive impairment in depression: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol Med. 2014;44(10):2029-2040. doi: 10.1017/S0033291713002535 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Cervilla JA, Prince M, Mann A. Smoking, drinking, and incident cognitive impairment: a cohort community based study included in the Gospel Oak project. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2000;68(5):622-626. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.68.5.622 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Erickson KI, Hillman C, Stillman CM, et al. ; 2018 Physical Activity Guidelines Advisory Committee . Physical activity, cognition, and brain outcomes: a review of the 2018 physical activity guidelines. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2019;51(6):1242-1251. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0000000000001936 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Ozawa M, Shipley M, Kivimaki M, Singh-Manoux A, Brunner EJ. Dietary pattern, inflammation and cognitive decline: the Whitehall II prospective cohort study. Clin Nutr. 2017;36(2):506-512. doi: 10.1016/j.clnu.2016.01.013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Croall ID, Trott N, Rej A, et al. A population survey of dietary attitudes towards gluten. Nutrients. 2019;11(6):1276. doi: 10.3390/nu11061276 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Agatston A, Geary N. The South Beach Diet Gluten Solution: The Delicious, Doctor-Designed, Gluten-Aware Plan for Losing Weight and Feeling Great—Fast! Rodale Books; 2014. [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Lis DM, Stellingwerff T, Shing CM, Ahuja KD, Fell JW. Exploring the popularity, experiences, and beliefs surrounding gluten-free diets in nonceliac athletes. Int J Sport Nutr Exerc Metab. 2015;25(1):37-45. doi: 10.1123/ijsnem.2013-0247 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Lerner BA, Green PHR, Lebwohl B. Going against the grains: gluten-free diets in patients without celiac disease-worthwhile or not? Dig Dis Sci. 2019;64(7):1740-1747. doi: 10.1007/s10620-019-05663-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Miranda J, Lasa A, Bustamante MA, Churruca I, Simon E. Nutritional differences between a gluten-free diet and a diet containing equivalent products with gluten. Plant Foods Hum Nutr. 2014;69(2):182-187. doi: 10.1007/s11130-014-0410-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Wu JH, Neal B, Trevena H, et al. Are gluten-free foods healthier than non-gluten-free foods? an evaluation of supermarket products in Australia. Br J Nutr. 2015;114(3):448-454. doi: 10.1017/S0007114515002056 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Lebwohl B, Luchsinger JA, Freedberg DE, Green PH, Ludvigsson JF. Risk of dementia in patients with celiac disease: a population-based cohort study. J Alzheimers Dis. 2016;49(1):179-185. doi: 10.3233/JAD-150388 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Casella S, Zanini B, Lanzarotto F, et al. Cognitive performance is impaired in coeliac patients on gluten free diet: a case-control study in patients older than 65 years of age. Dig Liver Dis. 2012;44(9):729-735. doi: 10.1016/j.dld.2012.03.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Zioudrou C, Streaty RA, Klee WA. Opioid peptides derived from food proteins: the exorphins. J Biol Chem. 1979;254(7):2446-2449. doi: 10.1016/S0021-9258(17)30243-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Pruimboom L, de Punder K. The opioid effects of gluten exorphins: asymptomatic celiac disease. J Health Popul Nutr. 2015;33(1):24. doi: 10.1186/s41043-015-0032-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Global Council on Brain Health . Brain-food: GCBH recommendations on nourishing your brain health. Published January 2018. Accessed December 12, 2020. https://www.aarp.org/health/brain-health/global-council-on-brain-health/nutrition.html

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplement.

eMethods. Supplementary Methods

eTable 1. Mean Differences in Standardized Cognitive Scores Associated With Continuous Gluten Intake in Grams per Day Among 13 494 Women

eTable 2. Mean Differences in Standardized Cognitive Scores Associated With Deciles of Gluten Intake Among 13 494 Women

eTable 3. Multivariable-Adjusted Differences in Standardized Psychomotor Speed and Attention Score Across Quintiles of Gluten Intake by Smoking Status With Additional Adjustment for Refined Grains Among 13 494 Women

eTable 4. Sensitivity Analysis of the Mean Differences in Standardized Cognitive Scores Associated With Quintiles of Gluten Intake Among 11 733 Women Who Had Never Reported Cancer Diagnosis

eTable 5. Sensitivity Analysis of the Mean Differences in Standardized Cognitive Scores Associated With Quintiles of Gluten Intake Among 13 486 Women Who Had Never Reported Dementia Diagnosis

eTable 6. Sensitivity Analysis of the Mean Differences in Standardized Cognitive Scores Associated With Quintiles of Gluten Intake Among 11 646 Women Who Completed All Food Frequency Questionnaires During 24-Year Follow-up Prior to Cognitive Assessment

eTable 7. Multivariable-Adjusted Mean Differences in Standardized Cognitive Scores Associated With Quintiles of Gluten Intake by 4-Year Interval Prior to Cognitive Assessment Among 11 646 Women

eTable 8. Multivariable-Adjusted Mean Differences in Standardized Cognitive Scores Associated With 1 SD (1.98 g/d) Increase in Mean Gluten Intake From Distant Past (12-24 y) to Recent Past (4-12 y) of Cognitive Assessment Among 11 646 Women

eFigure 1. Nurses’ Health Study II Questionnaire in 2015 Questionnaire Cycle

eFigure 2. Study Flow Diagram

eReferences.


Articles from JAMA Network Open are provided here courtesy of American Medical Association

RESOURCES