Abstract
Repeated drug use can change dopamine function in ways that promote the development and persistence of addiction. But in what direction? By one view, drug use blunts dopamine neurotransmission, producing a hypodopaminergic state that fosters further drug use to overcome a dopamine deficiency. Another view is that drug use enhances dopamine neurotransmission, producing a sensitized, hyperdopaminergic reaction to drugs and drug cues. According to this second view, continued drug use is motivated by sensitization of drug ‘wanting’. Here we discuss recent evidence supporting the latter view, both from preclinical studies using intermittent cocaine self-administration procedures that mimic human patterns of use, and related human neuroimaging studies. These studies have implications for modeling addiction in the laboratory, and for treatment.
Keywords: Dopamine, Cocaine, Rat, Self-Administration, Tolerance, Sensitization
Animal Models to Isolate Causes of Addiction
Addictions to psychoactive drugs (substance use disorders) are multifaceted disorders, and the propensity for addictions is determined by complex interactions amongst social, psychological, environmental and biological factors. Here we address one of these susceptibility factors; the ability of drugs themselves to produce persistent alterations in brain function, and thereby psychological function(s), in ways that promote and sustain problematic patterns of drug use and addiction. It is hard to specify how drugs change brain and behaviour in humans to promote the transition to addiction, primarily because of the challenge in isolating the consequences of drug use from the effects of many other conditions often associated with addiction (e.g., poor nutrition, high stress, incarceration, poly-drug use, co-morbidities, etc.). For this reason, there has been considerable effort to develop animal models of addiction where causal factors and drug effects can be isolated. Most drug self-administration procedures used in preclinical studies of addiction involve one of three procedures that we will refer to as: Short Access (ShA), Long Access (LgA) or Intermittent Access (IntA). Here we focus on the ways that these different procedures change dopamine (DA) neurotransmission, and we attempt to integrate preclinical studies with neuroimaging studies in human drug users.
Drug Self-Administration in Laboratory Animals
Laboratory animals will expend considerable effort to self-administer most drugs used by humans, and much is known at this stage about the neural systems that mediate the reinforcing and motivational effects of drugs. Drug reward is a multifactorial psychological process [1] involving complex interactions between many different neural systems. Still, much research has pointed to a central role for mesotelencephalic dopamine (DA) systems [2]. Here we focus primarily on ways in which cocaine self-administration can change DA activity, and how this might contribute to the development of addiction-like behaviours. It should be noted that different dopaminergic neuron subpopulations can contribute to different behavioural and psychological aspects of reward-seeking behaviour [5]. We do not address this complexity here. Instead, our focus is on how drug-induced changes in dopamine neurotransmission more generally contribute to the development of addiction.
There are two polarized views concerning which form of cocaine-induced change in DA neurotransmission is critical for promoting addiction – the “Ups and Downs” described by Leyton and Vezina [3, 4] and alluded to in the title of this paper. One view is that repeated cocaine use decreases the ability of rewards, including cocaine, to enhance DA neurotransmission, leading to ‘anhedonia’ [5, 6]. By this view, continued (and escalated) cocaine use is motivated primarily by a desire to overcome this ‘DA deficiency’. As put by Volkow et al.:
“drug consumption triggers much smaller increases in dopamine levels in the presence of addiction (in both animals and humans) than in its absence (…). This attenuated release of dopamine renders the brain’s reward system much less sensitive to stimulation by both drug-related and non–drug-related rewards. As a result, persons with addiction no longer experience the same degree of euphoria from a drug as they did when they first started using it” [7, p. 366]. In other words, “the down-regulation of dopamine signaling (…) dulls the reward circuits’ sensitivity to pleasure”
[7, p. 367].
Note that this seems to adopt the view that DA mediates pleasure (hedonia), because it is otherwise difficult to imagine how a ‘DA deficiency’ could motivate drug seeking. However, that view is incompatible, many would argue, with a large body of evidence showing that DA does not mediate pleasure[8, 9].
In contrast, a second view of addiction is that repeated exposure to addictive drugs, like cocaine, increases DA activity evoked by drug and drug cues (i.e., produces sensitization). This sensitized DA response, in turn, by interacting with glutamate and other signals, leads to heightened motivation for drugs (‘wanting’), but not higher drug ‘liking’ [10–12]. Importantly, more recent studies in rats using an intermittent self-administration procedure that reflects human patterns of cocaine use, have led to a re-examination of hypo- versus hyperdopaminergic states in addiction, in part because this procedure sensitizes DA activity [13, 14].
Preclinical Models of Addiction: Long Access (LgA) versus Short Access (ShA)
One goal of preclinical models is to capture behavioural features of human addiction (“addiction-like behaviours”). DSM-5 criteria for addiction include progressive increases in drug use (i.e., escalation), continued use despite adverse consequences, unsuccessful efforts to cut down and persistent drug craving [15]. Many early drug self-administration studies, especially with cocaine, used Short Access procedures (ShA). With ShA, rats have continuous access to drug for 1–3 hours per day [16, 17]. This usually results in stable cocaine intake across many sessions [18], which is not reflective of addiction. By contrast, early studies using prolonged (many months) oral administration of amphetamine or an opioid (etonitazene) described the emergence of escalated and dysregulated intake, that appeared to much better reflect addiction-like behaviour [19, 20]. But recreational drug users typically prefer routes of administration that produce a more rapid rise in brain drug levels, such as smoking or injection, and this pharmacokinetic variable is very important in determining both the neurobiological response to drugs and the transition to addiction [21]. Thus, a significant advance came with a study that compared rats allowed to continuously self-administer intravenous cocaine for 1 hour/day (ShA), with those allowed to continuously self-administer for 6 h/day (Long Access, LgA) [18]. ShA rats maintained stable levels of drug intake over 22 sessions, as expected. In contrast, LgA rats escalated their intake across sessions. Given that escalation of intake is a central feature of addiction, it was suggested that LgA, “may provide an animal model for studying the development of excessive drug intake and the basis of addiction” [18, p. 298].
Indeed, not only has escalation of intake during LgA been replicated many times [22–25], but LgA cocaine experience is also reported to be more effective than ShA at producing addiction-like behaviours in rats, including: i) a greater increase in motivation for cocaine, as measured by breakpoint on a progressive ratio schedule [26], behavioural economic indicators of cocaine demand [27, 28], running speed in a runway [22] and drug-seeking behaviour [23]; ii) greater drug-taking in the face of an adverse consequence [29]; and iii) greater reinstatement of cocaine-seeking behaviour evoked by re-exposure to the drug [24, 30]. Similarly, LgA more effectively produces addiction-like behaviours following the self-administration of heroin [31], methamphetamine [32], or nicotine [33]. These results have led to widespread adoption of the LgA procedure as a preferred preclinical model of addiction.
Given that LgA and ShA differ in their ability to produce addiction-like behaviours, and the established role for DA in the reinforcing/motivational effects of drugs, there has been considerable focus on determining how LgA may alter DA systems to produce addiction-like behaviours. There are several reports that LgA experience reduces DA transmission, particularly in brain regions that mediate drug-seeking and drug-taking behaviours, like the Nucleus accumbens (NAc; Figure 1). For example, in brain slices taken from rats following LgA experience, stimulated DA release and cocaine-induced inhibition of DA uptake is reduced within the NAc core [14, 34]. This is consistent with decreases in both cocaine-induced DA overflow assessed using in vivo microdialysis, and cocaine-induced psychomotor activity [34]. The findings are also consistent with studies showing that very extended (24 h) [35] or high-dose [36] cocaine self-administration sessions reduce cocaine’s efficacy at the DA transporter and also produce a blunted DA response to the drug. Finally, the phasic DA response that typically accompanies cocaine-seeking behaviour decreases across LgA sessions [37]. These LgA studies seem to support the reward deficiency view that a drug-induced hypo-dopaminergic state (tolerance) promotes the development of addiction-like behaviours. However, as we detail in Box 1, there is evidence that both tolerance and sensitization can develop in parallel, and that tolerance can mask sensitization [38]. Time since the last drug exposure determines which is dominant. Tolerance dissipates after a period of abstinence, revealing sensitization-related changes in brain and behaviour, as indicated for example, by the incubation of cocaine craving after LgA experience in rats [39] (an ‘unmasking’ of DA sensitization can also be seen after ShA cocaine experience [16]). Thus, the LgA model provides evidence for both tolerance and sensitization depending on when measures are made, highlighting the importance of examining DA responses both early and late following the discontinuation of self-administration.
Figure 1. How intermittent- versus long-access cocaine self-administration change dopamine system function.
(A) Representative patterns of cocaine intake, and (B) estimated brain cocaine concentrations in rats self-administering the drug during a long-access (LgA) or intermittent-access (IntA) session (adapted from [13]). (C) Schematic representation of the effects of LgA and IntA cocaine experience on dopamine (DA) system function in the Nucleus Accumbens during the development of addiction. The schematics in (C) represent relative effects in relation to control levels (see below), and summarize effects as measured ex vivo by electrically-evoked DA release and cocaine-induced DA transporter inhibition, and in vivo by cocaine-induced increases in DA concentrations (see text; “Preclinical Models of Addiction: Intermittent Access (IntA)”). The dotted line illustrates control levels, as assessed either in rats with ShA cocaine experience or in cocaine-naïve rats. As shown in (A), under LgA, cocaine is continuously available throughout the session, and rats take cocaine at regular intervals. In contrast, under IntA, cocaine is only available in discrete periods, and rats take cocaine intermittently, which models the intermittency of cocaine use typical in human cocaine users. As shown in (B), LgA produces relatively steady-state brain cocaine concentrations, and IntA produces spiking brain cocaine concentrations.
As illustrated in (C), LgA promotes tolerance and a hypodopaminergic state, whereby cocaine-induced DA responses are blunted. The shading illustrates that some studies report unchanged drug-induced DA responses after LgA cocaine experience, relative to control levels. In contrast to LgA, IntA consistently produces sensitization and a hyperdopaminergic state, whereby cocaine-induced DA responses are enhanced. Of note, studies to date have assessed DA responses within the first week of abstinence, and most studies have also focused on cocaine-induced DA responses. In future work, DA responses to drug, cues and under baseline conditions should be examined across the abstinence period.
BOX 1: Does LgA experience reliably produce tolerance to cocaine’s effects?
As discussed in more detail in the main text, while some studies have reported that LgA experience increases motivation for cocaine, decreases the psychomotor activating effects of cocaine, and decreases DA neurotransmission, the evidence in support of these effects is mixed. A relatively consistent finding is that LgA does not decrease the basal concentration of DA in the NAc [cf. 36, 51, 58, 119]. Furthermore, using in vivo microdialysis in the NAc of rats, LgA was found to have no effect on either the ability of experimenter-administered cocaine challenges to increase extracellular DA, or the efficacy of self-administered cocaine to increase DA [119]. In another study, a single self-administered injection of cocaine increased extracellular DA in the NAc to the same extent in rats with LgA versus ShA experience (although DA overflow was greater in rats with IntA experience), and the magnitude of the DA response to cocaine predicted motivation for cocaine [51]. Similarly, although there are reports that LgA experience increases motivation for cocaine in rats [e.g. 26, 40], there are also reports of no effect [51], of decreased motivation [120 for discussion], or when seen, only a very transient effect [27, 48].
Reports on how LgA experience influences the psychomotor activating effects of cocaine, which are thought to be largely due to increased DA activity, are also mixed. Some early studies suggested LgA produces tolerance to the psychomotor activating effects of cocaine [e.g. 34, 121]. In contrast, ShA and LgA experience have been reported to produce the same degree of psychomotor sensitization, rather than tolerance [30]. A more recent study reported no change in the psychomotor activating effects of cocaine following LgA experience, when rats were tested soon (3 days) after the last self-administration session [60]. However, when tested after 30 days of abstinence, rats with LgA experience showed robust psychomotor sensitization, consistent with an earlier study [23]. It is possible that in some situations the self-administration of very large amounts of cocaine can produce tolerance to a number of cocaine’s effects, which can mask the expression of sensitization [38]. However, tolerance is short-lived, and when it dissipates, underlying sensitization-related neurobehavioural adaptations become evident, similar to what is seen when psychomotor stimulant drugs are experimenter-administered [11, 122], and with incubation of cocaine craving after LgA experience [39].
Preclinical Models of Addiction: Intermittent Access (IntA)
With the ShA and LgA procedures, cocaine is continuously available. This produces high and sustained brain concentrations of cocaine throughout each self-administration session [40–42] (Figure 1). Yet as human users become addicted, they seldom take cocaine continuously in LgA fashion over many hours and days. Instead, they usually consume drug more intermittently, with periods of ingestion spaced apart, both between and within bouts of use [21, 43–45]. This results in a “spiking” pattern of brain cocaine levels over time. To reproduce this spiking pattern within a bout of use, Intermittent Access (IntA) self-administration procedures were developed, whereby periods during which drug is available (5–6 min for cocaine) are interspersed with periods when drug is not available (e.g., 25 min) during a self-administration session [40] (see also [46]; Figure 1).
Crucially, comparing effects of IntA versus LgA indicates that the temporal pattern of drug self-administration may be more important for promoting addiction than the total amount of drug consumed. Previous reviews have compared the ability of these procedures to produce addiction-like behaviour [13, 21]. Specifically, IntA is even more effective than LgA in producing addiction-like behaviours, even though IntA results in much less total cocaine intake/session, with intake levels comparable to ShA [13, 21]. For example, the large amount of cocaine consumption associated with LgA was thought to be necessary for escalation, however IntA experience—which achieves much lower levels of cocaine intake—also results in escalation [42, 47]. In addition, compared to LgA, IntA produces i) a greater and longer-lasting increase in subsequent motivation to obtain cocaine [42, 48], ii) increased drug taking in the face of an adverse consequence ([48] also see [49]), iii) more robust cue-induced reinstatement of cocaine-seeking behaviour ([48, 50, 51] also see [47, 49, 52, 53]), and iv) continued drug seeking when drug is known to no longer be available [47]. Beyond cocaine and other psychostimulants, IntA exposure to other drugs, including alcohol [54] and opioids [55, 56] is also especially effective in producing addiction-like behaviours.
Although admittedly few, studies examining the effects of IntA cocaine experience on DA are consistent with enhancements (sensitization), rather than reductions (tolerance) in drug-evoked DA activity [51] (Figure 1). Initial studies using ex vivo slices showed that IntA cocaine self-administration increases cocaine-induced DA transporter (DAT) inhibition (LgA had the opposite effect) and increases electrically-stimulated DA release in the NAc [14]. As few as 3 IntA cocaine sessions sensitize cocaine’s effects at the DAT in the NAc, and after one week of abstinence, both cocaine potency and stimulated DA release are increased even further [57]. Using in vivo microdialysis in freely moving rats, it has recently been reported that 1–3 days after the last self-administration session, IntA and LgA rats do not differ in basal extracellular DA concentrations, but IntA (not LgA) rats show sensitization of cocaine-induced overflow in the NAc core [51]. Furthermore, IntA cocaine experience enhances amphetamine and methylphenidate’s effects at the DAT [58], and IntA methylphenidate experience produces DA sensitization effects similar to those produced by cocaine [59]. Finally, IntA experience also produces psychomotor sensitization [60–63], which has been related to increased DA neurotransmission [11], and psychomotor sensitization often predicts later motivation for cocaine [61, 62]. Indeed, there is a large literature showing that intermittent treatment with many drugs of abuse produces sensitization of psychomotor activity and of DA [64, 65]. Thus, the available evidence suggests that LgA can produce tolerance, reductions in DA (although see Box 1), and addiction-like behaviours, whereas IntA increases (sensitizes) DA activity and is more effective in producing addiction-like behaviours than LgA, despite less total drug consumption (Figure 1).
Little is known about the mechanism(s) by which IntA cocaine experience sensitizes DA activity. DA in the NAc arises (for the most part) from cells in the ventral tegmental area (VTA). Many neurotransmitter/neuromodulator systems regulate VTA DA neuron firing and DA activity at DA terminals in the NAc. This includes glutamate, GABA, orexin and endogenous cannabinoids and opioids. One of the forthcoming challenges is to determine how IntA drug experience affects these systems to alter DA activity. In this regard, existing studies already show that IntA cocaine ([48], and see also [66]) or fentanyl [55] experience increases the number and activity of orexin neurons in ways that could contribute to enhanced DA release in the NAc, and that this is causal in drug seeking and taking [also see 67]. Although of course, many different players could be involved.
There are well-established sex differences in the transition to addiction, as detailed in a number of reviews [68–70]. For example, in vulnerable cocaine users, women can progress more rapidly from initial drug use to addiction than men do, and women can also be more vulnerable to relapse after abstinence [71, 72]. Similarly, sex differences are seen in rats and monkeys [69, 73]. These sex differences are due largely to activational effects of hormones in females and males, and are thought to be mediated, at least in part, by the ability of gonadal hormones to influence DA systems [74]. In the context of the current review, it is notable that after IntA experience, female rats show more robust psychomotor sensitization than male rats do [60, 62], consistent with earlier studies using experimenter-administered drug [75]. Female IntA rats also more readily develop incentive sensitization than male rats do, as indicated by earlier and greater increases in the motivation to take cocaine [76] and more cocaine seeking when drug is not available [50]. In this regard, future studies should determine the effects of IntA drug experience on DA and other brain systems in females, as there are currently no published studies on the neurobiological effects of IntA in female animals. More generally, future research in addiction should include animals of both sexes and address knowledge gaps due to biased inclusion of males vs. females in past work.
Studies in Humans
In the following, we compare results from preclinical studies to those from neuroimaging studies of DA-related systems in people with a substance use disorder. It is difficult to draw direct parallels between rodent and human studies, and studies in humans also carry many caveats. Still, integrating results across these literatures allows assessing the extent to which findings from preclinical studies are relevant to human drug addiction. As detailed next, we would argue that evidence from human neuroimaging studies is consistent with the thesis that the heightened motivation for drugs seen in addiction is primarily due to a sensitized DA response to drug cues and drugs (see [3, 77] for more thorough reviews).
D-amphetamine increases DA transmission in the human ventral striatum [78] (see also [79, 80]), as indicated by decreased [11C]raclopride binding (but see Box 2 for alternative interpretations of [11C]raclopride binding studies). Alcohol [81], cigarette smoking [82, 83], morphine [84] and Delta 9-tetrahydrocannabinol [85] have a similar effect. With repeated drug exposure, humans show sensitization of drug-induced DA transmission in the striatum. For example, after 3 d-amphetamine doses, there is both psychomotor and DA sensitization to the drug, such that d-amphetamine now produces increased behavioural activation and DA transmission relative to the initial dose [78]. This behavioural and neurochemical sensitization in humans is long-lasting, persisting for up to one year [78].
BOX 2: Do people with addiction have decreased dopamine neurotransmission?
Stimulant drug users are consistently reported to have “a significant decrease in D2/D3 [receptor] availability”, and in “in striatal dopamine release” [111] [also see 112]. This has been interpreted by some to suggest that long-term drug use renders the mesostriatal DA system hypoactive [123]. However, one could make the case that these studies do not provide unambiguous or compelling evidence for this conclusion [3, 4, 10]. There are alternative interpretations for the decrease in D2/D3 binding. i) It could be a compensatory response to increased DA release, or a simple competition effect due to enhanced DA release competing with the neuroimaging tracer, neither of which would be reflective of decreased DA. ii) It could reflect decreased DA autoreceptors (which are also D2-type receptors), which would result in enhanced DA. iii) D2/D3 receptors may be low before drug use, rather than a consequence of drug use—or both. Also, most studies reporting a decrease in D2/D3 receptor availability use ligands like raclopride, a D2/D3 receptor antagonist. However, studies using agonist ligands, such as [11C]-(+)-propyl-hexahydro-naphtho-oxazin (PHNO), which has greater affinity for the postsynaptic, D3 receptor [124], do not support a decrease in D2/D3 receptor availability in addiction [125–128].
Reports of decreased stimulated DA release in people with addiction appear to provide more compelling evidence for DA hypoactivity, but this interpretation has also been questioned [3, 4]. Studies showing blunted DA release typically provide drug in an environment where drug was never experienced before (i.e., in the scanner/laboratory). Thus, they are conducted in the absence of drug-predictive cues, and when drug is known to be unavailable, because users have generally not taken drug in the laboratory environment before. Sensitization is typically not expressed under such conditions, even in rats, because sensitization can be highly context-dependent [3, 77, 122, 129–131]. Indeed, providing drug in the presence of drug-associated cues, or cues signaling drug availability, increases DA release [3, 4, 77, 88]. Furthermore, most studies showing blunted DA release are conducted shortly after abstinence, when preclinical studies indicate sensitization may not be expressed [132, 133]. Lastly, and as noted in the main text, it is not fully clear how a decrease in DA could lead to an increase in motivation.
In addition, and as seen in rats [86], studies using [11C]raclopride and positron emission tomography in humans suggest that drug-paired cues increase striatal DA transmission in humans, and this is linked to drug craving. An environmental context associated with d-amphetamine use increases DA transmission in human striatum, to the same extent as d-amphetamine itself [79]. Presentation of rich, personalized drug cues that signal cocaine availability also increases striatal (and frontocortical) DA release in human users, and the magnitude of this effect is positively correlated with their drug craving scores [87–89].
In humans with substance use disorders, drug-related cues are especially effective at capturing attention, and such cues can evoke craving and renewed drug use [90–93]. Such cue effects are especially evident during daily life outside the laboratory [94]. Some studies have used fMRI to quantify cue-induced neural activations in human users (change in the BOLD signal), and increases in DA activity are thought to be both necessary and sufficient to increase the BOLD signal in the striatum [95–99], although see [100]. Such fMRI studies report that drug-paired cues, as well as the drug itself, produce robust activations in a number of brain regions, including mesostriatal dopamine-rich regions [77, 91, 101–103, also see 104]. These DA-related activations positively correlate with self-report of cue-evoked craving and predict subsequent relapse [105–107]. Interestingly, cocaine cues evoke activations even when people are not consciously aware of seeing the cues [108]. Together, these results further support the notion of a hyperdopaminergic state in drug addiction.
An important question is whether there are mechanistic links between enhanced DA neurotransmission and the development of addictive behaviours in humans. Studies in patients with Parkinson’s Disease and with Dopamine Dysregulation Syndrome (DDS), induced by their L-Dopa or DA receptor agonist medications support such links. In some patients, DDS is accompanied by a pathologically high motivation to take excessive medication, and in many others by the development of compulsive behaviours (e.g., gambling) that abate when the medication is stopped. This has been linked to the pharmacological induction of a hyperdopaminergic state, particularly within the ventral striatum (NAc [109]). Thus, it is an increase, not a decrease, in DA transmission that leads to excessive motivation in DDS. As put by Dagher and Robbins,
“the reward deficiency hypothesis appears to be directly falsified by the premorbid Parkinsonian personality syndrome and by the occurrence of addiction in PD patients when they are overdosed with dopaminergic medication. (…) This suggests that, in the general population as well as in PD patients, factors that lead to enhanced striatal dopaminergic function, whether hereditary or acquired, represent a biological substrate of addictive propensity”
[110, p. 508].
Thus, when DA is pharmacologically enhanced, this promotes addiction-relevant behaviours. Furthermore, these compulsive behaviours cease when pro-dopaminergic medication is terminated. Although data from patients with Parkinson’s disease should be interpreted with caution, these findings nonetheless support the view that addiction involves sensitized DA responses.
The studies in humans discussed above support a role for hyper- and not hypo-dopaminergic states in producing pathological motivations. However, there are also studies that have been interpreted as supporting the opposite view. These latter studies report D2-like receptor downregulation and decreased stimulated DA release in people with substance use disorder [111, 112]. However, as described in Box 2, such findings do not necessarily reflect a hypodopaminergic state [3, 4, 10]. Downregulation of D2-like receptors could reflect a compensatory response to increased DA release and/or decreased D2 autoreceptors, neither of which would be consistent with a blunted DA response. Similarly, the studies showing reduced drug-induced DA release generally tested participants in the absence of drug-predictive cues and contexts, and also tested shortly after abstinence. Such conditions are known to mask the expression of sensitization.
Concluding Remarks
Addiction cannot be reduced to drug-induced changes in DA function alone, as many other neurotransmitter systems, brain systems, and non-pharmacological factors are involved. Nevertheless, clarifying the direction of cocaine-induced changes in DA systems is fundamental for understanding addiction, and carries implications for both modeling addiction in the laboratory and for therapeutics. In this article, we argue that evidence from both rat drug self-administration studies and human neuroimaging studies supports the notion that a sensitized DA response to drugs and drug cues contributes to the development and persistence of addiction. Animal self-administration procedures that reflect the intermittency of human cocaine use are most effective at producing increased motivation for the drug and other addiction-like behaviours, and these procedures consistently produce a sensitized DA response. In parallel, neuroimaging studies in human drug users show that when personalized cues that signal drug availability are present—a condition closer to real-life drug use—the DA response to drug and drug cues is enhanced. This supports the view that drug use produces a hyperdopaminergic reactivity state, and that addiction involves sensitization of DA systems to the incentive effects of drugs and drug cues, leading to pathological drug wanting. In the context of therapeutic approaches, this indicates that when targeting the DA system, treatments should aim to mitigate this sensitized DA state [11]. Indeed, d-amphetamine maintenance therapy decreases cocaine use in humans [113–117], and a recent study in rats shows that d-amphetamine maintenance during IntA cocaine self-administration decreases the expression of psychomotor sensitization and subsequent motivation for cocaine, while reversing the sensitization of cocaine’s action at the DAT [118]. While many questions remain to be addressed (see Outstanding Questions), this emerging literature suggests that treatments that mitigate cocaine-induced sensitization of DA systems may blunt the motivation to take the drug.
Outstanding Questions.
When a given addiction-like behaviour is produced by both IntA and LgA, but effects on DA are opposite, to what extent do changes in other transmitter systems such as glutamate, contribute?
To what extent is the hyperdopaminergic state produced by intermittent cocaine intake causal in the pathological patterns of drug use characteristic of addiction?
By what mechanism(s) does intermittent cocaine intake influence the regulation of dopamine neuron activity?
The temporal pattern of drug use in humans has not been studied systematically. When individuals with addiction have control over cocaine dose and intermittency of intake, what is the pattern of use within and between bouts? Do these patterns differ between women and men?
If sensitization-like changes in DA function in humans are masked by tolerance, how long a period of abstinence is required for sensitization to become evident?
Highlights.
Repeated use of drugs of abuse induces persistent changes in brain function that promote the transition to and persistence of addiction.
Drug self-administration procedures in laboratory animals have been refined to capture behavioural features of addiction.
In animals, self-administration procedures that incorporate the intermittent patterns of drug use seen in humans are more effective at inducing addiction-like patterns of behaviour than procedures that use long, continuous access, and the former consistently increase dopamine function.
Human neuroimaging studies reinforce the notion that drug-induced hyperdopaminergic states contribute to addiction.
Therapeutic strategies to mitigate sensitized hyperdopaminergic states in addiction may be especially efficacious.
Acknowledgements
We thank Drs Kent Berridge and Hans Crombag for comments on an earlier version of this text. We also thank Mr. Patrick Castell and Dr Florence Allain for help in making Figure 1. This work was supported by NIDA grant RO1 DA044204 to TER and CRF, by NIDA grant R21DA045277 and T32DA007268 to CRF, by CIHR grant 168971 to ANS, and by FRQ-S awards to SYK (270051) and ANS (28988).
Glossary
- Addiction-like behaviours
Behaviours displayed by laboratory animal models that are analogous to DSM-5 criteria for substance use disorders. For example, escalation of drug use, considerable time/effort spent to obtain drug, continued drug use despite adverse consequences (punishment resistance) or continued drug seeking when drug is not available (resistance to extinction).
- Incentive-sensitization theory
The incentive-sensitization theory of addiction posits that i) events that activate mesotelencephalic dopamine and associated systems are attributed with incentive salience and become ‘wanted’, ii) in vulnerable people, taking psychoactive drugs produces incremental neuroadaptations in these neural systems, making them sensitized to drugs and drug-associated cues, iii) in turn, these sensitization-related changes maintain excessive drug ‘wanting’ (craving) even after long periods of drug abstinence.
- Drug self-administration
In the context of preclinical studies, drug self-administration refers to a procedure where laboratory animals can make an instrumental behavioural response (e.g., pressing a lever) to obtain a dose of drug.
- Progressive ratio
A schedule of reinforcement where the number of responses required to obtain a single drug infusion increases with each successive infusion. The rate of increase is usually exponential, but other functions can also be used. The maximum requirement met (e.g., in number of lever presses) to obtain a single dose of drug before abandoning the self-administration task is termed the ‘breakpoint’. Breakpoint is used as a measure of motivation for drug, because it reflects the maximum amount of work (e.g., lever pressing) an animal will perform for a single drug infusion.
- Sensitization
The process whereby repeated exposure to the same stimulus (in the context of addiction studies, a given dose of drug, for instance) comes to elicit a progressively greater response to that same stimulus.
- Tolerance
The process by which repeated exposure to the same stimulus (in the context of addiction studies, a given dose of drug, for instance) comes to elicit a progressively reduced response, such that increased doses of drug are required to elicit the same initial response.
- Short access (ShA)
Self-administration procedures where sessions involve continuous drug access, typically for 1–3 hours per session (usually 1 session per day).
- Long access (LgA)
Self-administration procedures where sessions involve continuous drug access, typically for 6+ hours (usually 1 session per day).
- Intermittent access (IntA)
Self-administration involving cycles of drug availability and unavailability within the same session. With cocaine this is achieved by interspersing 5–6 min periods of drug availability with at least 25-min periods when drug is not available. This cycle is then repeated, typically for 4–6 h/session (usually 1 session per day).
- Psychomotor activating effects
The ability of a drug to enhance locomotor activity, rearing, sniffing, darting behaviours, and stereotyped head and forelimb movements. These behaviours require DA transmission in mesolimbic circuits, and changes in the intensity and frequency of these behaviours indicate neuroplasticity in these circuits and of DA in particular.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
Declaration of Interests
The authors declare no conflicts of interest.
References
- 1.Berridge KC and Robinson TE (2003) Parsing reward. Trends Neurosci 26 (9), 507–13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Canchy L et al. (2021) Pharmacokinetics trumps pharmacodynamics during cocaine choice: a reconciliation with the dopamine hypothesis of addiction. Neuropsychopharmacology 46 (2), 288–296. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Leyton M and Vezina P (2013) Striatal ups and downs: their roles in vulnerability to addictions in humans. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 37 (9 Pt A), 1999–2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Leyton M and Vezina P (2014) Dopamine ups and downs in vulnerability to addictions: a neurodevelopmental model. Trends Pharmacol Sci 35 (6), 268–76. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Gondré-Lewis MC et al. (2020) Pre-clinical models of reward deficiency syndrome: A behavioral octopus. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 115, 164–188. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Volkow ND et al. (2019) The neuroscience of drug reward and addiction. Physiological Reviews 99 (4), 2115–2140. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Volkow ND et al. (2016) Neurobiologic Advances from the Brain Disease Model of Addiction. N Engl J Med 374 (4), 363–71. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Berridge Kent C. and Kringelbach Morten L. (2015) Pleasure systems in the brain. Neuron 86 (3), 646–664. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Berridge KC (2007) The debate over dopamine’s role in reward: the case for incentive salience. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 191 (3), 391–431. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Berridge KC and Robinson TE (2016) Liking, wanting, and the incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. Am Psychol 71 (8), 670–679. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Robinson TE and Berridge KC (1993) The neural basis of drug craving: an incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. Brain Res Brain Res Rev 18 (3), 247–91. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.King A et al. (2021) Subjective Responses to Alcohol in the Development and Maintenance of Alcohol Use Disorder. Am J Psychiatry, appiajp202020030247. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Kawa AB et al. (2019) The transition to cocaine addiction: the importance of pharmacokinetics for preclinical models. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 236 (4), 1145–1157. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Calipari ES et al. (2013) Temporal pattern of cocaine intake determines tolerance vs sensitization of cocaine effects at the dopamine transporter. Neuropsychopharmacology 38 (12), 2385–92. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.American Psychiatric Association. (2013) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th Edition, American Psychiatric Association. [Google Scholar]
- 16.Hooks MS et al. (1994) Behavioral and neurochemical sensitization following cocaine self-administration. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 115 (1–2), 265–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Phillips AG and Di Ciano P (1996) Behavioral sensitization is induced by intravenous self-administration of cocaine by rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 124 (3), 279–81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Ahmed SH and Koob GF (1998) Transition from moderate to excessive drug intake: change in hedonic set point. Science 282 (5387), 298–300. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Heyne A (1996) The development of opiate addiction in the rat. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 53 (1), 11–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Heyne A and Wolffgramm J (1998) The development of addiction to d-amphetamine in an animal model: same principles as for alcohol and opiate. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 140 (4), 510–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Allain F et al. (2015) How fast and how often: The pharmacokinetics of drug use are decisive in addiction. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 56, 166–79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Ben-Shahar O et al. (2008) Heightened drug-seeking motivation following extended daily access to self-administered cocaine. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 32 (3), 863–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Ferrario CR et al. (2005) Neural and behavioral plasticity associated with the transition from controlled to escalated cocaine use. Biol Psychiatry 58 (9), 751–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Mantsch J et al. (2004) Effects of extended access to high versus low cocaine doses on self-administration, cocaine-induced reinstatement and brain mRNA levels in rats. Psychopharmacology 175 (1), 26–36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Lynch WJ (2018) Modeling the development of drug addiction in male and female animals. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 164, 50–61. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Paterson NE and Markou A (2003) Increased motivation for self-administered cocaine after escalated cocaine intake. Neuroreport 14 (17), 2229–32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Bentzley BS et al. (2014) Economic demand predicts addiction-like behavior and therapeutic efficacy of oxytocin in the rat. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 111 (32), 11822–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Christensen CJ et al. (2008) Demand for cocaine and food over time. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 91 (2), 209–16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Vanderschuren LJ and Everitt BJ (2004) Drug seeking becomes compulsive after prolonged cocaine self-administration. Science 305 (5686), 1017–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Ahmed SH and Cador M (2006) Dissociation of psychomotor sensitization from compulsive cocaine consumption. Neuropsychopharmacology 31 (3), 563–71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Ahmed SH et al. (2000) Persistent increase in the motivation to take heroin in rats with a history of drug escalation. Neuropsychopharmacology 22 (4), 413–21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Adhikary S et al. (2017) Incubation of extinction responding and cue-induced reinstatement, but not context- or drug priming-induced reinstatement, after withdrawal from methamphetamine. Addiction Biology 22 (4), 977–990. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Paterson NE and Markou A (2004) Prolonged nicotine dependence associated with extended access to nicotine self-administration in rats. Psychopharmacology 173 (1–2), 64–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Calipari ES et al. (2014) Extended access of cocaine self-administration results in tolerance to the dopamine-elevating and locomotor-stimulating effects of cocaine. J Neurochem 128 (2), 224–32. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Mateo Y et al. (2005) Reduced dopamine terminal function and insensitivity to cocaine following cocaine binge self-administration and deprivation. Neuropsychopharmacology 30 (8), 1455–63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Ferris MJ et al. (2011) Cocaine-insensitive dopamine transporters with intact substrate transport produced by self-administration. Biol Psychiatry 69 (3), 201–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Willuhn I et al. (2014) Excessive cocaine use results from decreased phasic dopamine signaling in the striatum. Nat Neurosci 17 (5), 704–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Dalia AD et al. (1998) Transient amelioration of the sensitization of cocaine-induced behaviors in rats by the induction of tolerance. Brain Res 797 (1), 29–34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Wolf ME and Tseng KY (2012) Calcium-permeable AMPA receptors in the VTA and nucleus accumbens after cocaine exposure: when, how, and why? Front Mol Neurosci 5, 72. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Zimmer BA et al. (2012) The motivation to self-administer is increased after a history of spiking brain levels of cocaine. Neuropsychopharmacology 37 (8), 1901–10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Algallal H et al. (2020) Sex differences in cocaine self-administration behaviour under long access versus intermittent access conditions. Addiction Biology 25 (5), e12809. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Allain F et al. (2018) High and escalating levels of cocaine intake are dissociable from subsequent incentive motivation for the drug in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 235 (1), 317–328. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Cohen P, Sas A (1994) Cocaine use in Amsterdam in non deviant subcultures. Addiction Research 2 (1), 71–94. [Google Scholar]
- 44.Beveridge TJ et al. , Analyzing human cocaine use patterns to inform animal addiction model development, College on Problems of Drug Dependence Annual Meeting, Palm Springs, CA, 2012. [Google Scholar]
- 45.Leri F et al. (2004) Heroin and cocaine co-use in a group of injection drug users in Montréal. J Psychiatry Neurosci 29 (1), 40–7. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Deroche V et al. (1999) Cocaine self-administration increases the incentive motivational properties of the drug in rats. Eur J Neurosci 11 (8), 2731–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Kawa AB et al. (2016) Less is more: prolonged intermittent access cocaine self-administration produces incentive-sensitization and addiction-like behavior. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 233 (19–20), 3587–602. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.James MH et al. (2019) Increased Number and Activity of a Lateral Subpopulation of Hypothalamic Orexin/Hypocretin Neurons Underlies the Expression of an Addicted State in Rats. Biol Psychiatry 85 (11), 925–935. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Singer BF et al. (2018) Are Cocaine-Seeking “Habits” Necessary for the Development of Addiction-Like Behavior in Rats? J Neurosci 38 (1), 60–73. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Nicolas C et al. (2019) Incubation of Cocaine Craving After Intermittent-Access Self-administration: Sex Differences and Estrous Cycle. Biol Psychiatry 85 (11), 915–924. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Kawa AB et al. (2019) Incentive and dopamine sensitization produced by intermittent but not long access cocaine self-administration. Eur J Neurosci 50 (4), 2663–2682. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Gueye AB et al. (2019) Intermittent intake of rapid cocaine injections promotes the risk of relapse and increases mesocorticolimbic BDNF levels during abstinence. Neuropsychopharmacology 44 (6), 1027–1035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Allain F and Samaha A-N (2019) Revisiting long-access versus short-access cocaine self-administration in rats: intermittent intake promotes addiction symptoms independent of session length. Addiction Biology 24 (4), 641–651. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Simms JA et al. (2008) Intermittent access to 20% ethanol induces high ethanol consumption in Long-Evans and Wistar rats. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 32 (10), 1816–23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Fragale JE et al. (2020) Intermittent self-administration of fentanyl induces a multifaceted addiction state associated with persistent changes in the orexin system. Addict Biol, e12946. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.O’Neal TJ et al. (2020) Chemogenetic modulation of accumbens direct or indirect pathways bidirectionally alters reinstatement of heroin-seeking in high- but not low-risk rats. Neuropsychopharmacology 45 (8), 1251–1262. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Calipari ES et al. (2015) Brief intermittent cocaine self-administration and abstinence sensitizes cocaine effects on the dopamine transporter and increases drug seeking. Neuropsychopharmacology 40 (3), 728–35. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Calipari ES et al. (2014) Intermittent cocaine self-administration produces sensitization of stimulant effects at the dopamine transporter. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 349 (2), 192–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Calipari ES and Jones SR (2014) Sensitized nucleus accumbens dopamine terminal responses to methylphenidate and dopamine transporter releasers after intermittent-access self-administration. Neuropharmacology 82, 1–10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Carr CC et al. (2020) Intermittent access cocaine self-administration produces psychomotor sensitization: effects of withdrawal, sex and cross-sensitization. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 237 (6), 1795–1812. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Allain F et al. (2017) Intermittent intake of rapid cocaine injections promotes robust psychomotor sensitization, increased incentive motivation for the drug and mGlu2/3 receptor dysregulation. Neuropharmacology 117, 227–237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Algallal H et al. (2020) Sex differences in cocaine self-administration behaviour under long access versus intermittent access conditions. Addict Biol 25 (5), e12809. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Garcia AF et al. (2020) Intermittent but not continuous access to cocaine produces individual variability in addiction susceptibility in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 237 (10), 2929–2941. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Post RM (1980) Intermittent versus continuous stimulation: effect of time interval on the development of sensitization or tolerance. Life Sci 26 (16), 1275–82. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Robinson TE and Becker JB (1986) Enduring changes in brain and behavior produced by chronic amphetamine administration: a review and evaluation of animal models of amphetamine psychosis. Brain Res 396 (2), 157–98. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.James MH et al. (2018) Demand elasticity predicts addiction endophenotypes and the therapeutic efficacy of an orexin/hypocretin-1 receptor antagonist in rats. Eur J Neurosci. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Brodnik ZD et al. (2020) Hypocretin receptor 1 involvement in cocaine-associated behavior: Therapeutic potential and novel mechanistic insights. Brain Res 1731, 145894. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Becker JB et al. (2017) Sex differences, gender and addiction. J Neurosci Res 95 (1–2), 136–147. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Carroll ME and Lynch WJ (2016) How to study sex differences in addiction using animal models. Addict Biol 21 (5), 1007–29. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Becker JB and Koob GF (2016) Sex Differences in Animal Models: Focus on Addiction. Pharmacol Rev 68 (2), 242–63. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.McKay JR et al. (1996) Gender differences in the relapse experiences of cocaine patients. J Nerv Ment Dis 184 (10), 616–22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Elman I et al. (2001) Gender differences in cocaine craving among non-treatment-seeking individuals with cocaine dependence. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse 27 (2), 193–202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Becker JB et al. (2012) Sex differences in the neural mechanisms mediating addiction: a new synthesis and hypothesis. Biol Sex Differ 3 (1), 14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Yoest KE et al. (2018) Rapid effects of ovarian hormones in dorsal striatum and nucleus accumbens. Horm Behav 104, 119–129. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Robinson TE (1984) Behavioral sensitization: characterization of enduring changes in rotational behavior produced by intermittent injections of amphetamine in male and female rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 84 (4), 466–75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Kawa AB and Robinson TE (2019) Sex differences in incentive-sensitization produced by intermittent access cocaine self-administration. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 236 (2), 625–639. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Leyton M (2007) Conditioned and sensitized responses to stimulant drugs in humans. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 31 (8), 1601–13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Boileau I et al. (2006) Modeling sensitization to stimulants in humans: an [11C]raclopride/positron emission tomography study in healthy men. Arch Gen Psychiatry 63 (12), 1386–95. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Boileau I et al. (2007) Conditioned dopamine release in humans: a positron emission tomography [11C]raclopride study with amphetamine. J Neurosci 27 (15), 3998–4003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Martinez D et al. (2003) Imaging human mesolimbic dopamine transmission with positron emission tomography. Part II: amphetamine-induced dopamine release in the functional subdivisions of the striatum. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 23 (3), 285–300. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Boileau I et al. (2003) Alcohol promotes dopamine release in the human nucleus accumbens. Synapse 49 (4), 226–31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Brody AL et al. (2004) Smoking-induced ventral striatum dopamine release. Am J Psychiatry 161 (7), 1211–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Barrett SP et al. (2004) The hedonic response to cigarette smoking is proportional to dopamine release in the human striatum as measured by positron emission tomography and [11C]raclopride. Synapse 54 (2), 65–71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Spagnolo PA et al. (2019) Striatal Dopamine Release in Response to Morphine: A [(11)C]Raclopride Positron Emission Tomography Study in Healthy Men. Biol Psychiatry 86 (5), 356–364. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Bossong MG et al. (2009) Delta 9-tetrahydrocannabinol induces dopamine release in the human striatum. Neuropsychopharmacology 34 (3), 759–66. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Gratton A and Wise RA (1994) Drug- and behavior-associated changes in dopamine-related electrochemical signals during intravenous cocaine self-administration in rats. J Neurosci 14 (7), 4130–46. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Fotros A et al. (2013) Cocaine cue-induced dopamine release in amygdala and hippocampus: a high-resolution PET [(1)(8)F]fallypride study in cocaine dependent participants. Neuropsychopharmacology 38 (9), 1780–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Cox SML et al. (2017) Cocaine Cue-Induced Dopamine Release in Recreational Cocaine Users. Sci Rep 7, 46665. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Milella MS et al. (2016) Cocaine cue-induced dopamine release in the human prefrontal cortex. J Psychiatry Neurosci 41 (5), 322–30. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Field M and Cox WM (2008) Attentional bias in addictive behaviors: a review of its development, causes, and consequences. Drug Alcohol Depend 97 (1–2), 1–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Fadardi JS et al. (2016) Neuroscience of attentional processes for addiction medicine: from brain mechanisms to practical considerations. Prog Brain Res 223, 77–89. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Anderson BA (2021) Relating value-driven attention to psychopathology. Current Opinion in Psychology 39, 48–54. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Cofresí RU et al. (2019) Evidence for incentive salience sensitization as a pathway to alcohol use disorder. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 107, 897–926. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Serre F et al. (2015) Ecological momentary assessment in the investigation of craving and substance use in daily life: a systematic review. Drug Alcohol Depend 148, 1–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Garavan H et al. (2000) Cue-induced cocaine craving: neuroanatomical specificity for drug users and drug stimuli. Am J Psychiatry 157 (11), 1789–98. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Marota JJ et al. (2000) Cocaine activation discriminates dopaminergic projections by temporal response: an fMRI study in Rat. Neuroimage 11 (1), 13–23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Ferenczi EA et al. (2016) Prefrontal cortical regulation of brainwide circuit dynamics and reward-related behavior. Science 351 (6268), aac9698. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Decot HK et al. (2017) Coordination of Brain-Wide Activity Dynamics by Dopaminergic Neurons. Neuropsychopharmacology 42 (3), 615–627. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Roelofs TJM et al. (2017) A novel approach to map induced activation of neuronal networks using chemogenetics and functional neuroimaging in rats: A proof-of-concept study on the mesocorticolimbic system. NeuroImage 156, 109–118. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Lohrenz T et al. (2016) BOLD and its connection to dopamine release in human striatum: a cross-cohort comparison. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 371 (1705). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Anderson BA (2016) The attention habit: how reward learning shapes attentional selection. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1369 (1), 24–39. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Leeman RF et al. (2014) A critical review of the literature on attentional bias in cocaine use disorder and suggestions for future research. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 22 (6), 469–83. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Kuhn S and Gallinat J (2011) Common biology of craving across legal and illegal drugs - a quantitative meta-analysis of cue-reactivity brain response. Eur J Neurosci 33 (7), 1318–26. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Anderson BA et al. (2016) The Role of Dopamine in Value-Based Attentional Orienting. Curr Biol 26 (4), 550–5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Jasinska AJ et al. (2014) Factors modulating neural reactivity to drug cues in addiction: a survey of human neuroimaging studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 38, 1–16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Chase HW et al. (2011) The neural basis of drug stimulus processing and craving: an activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis. Biol Psychiatry 70 (8), 785–93. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Courtney KE et al. (2016) Neural substrates of cue reactivity: association with treatment outcomes and relapse. Addict Biol 21 (1), 3–22. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Childress AR et al. (2008) Prelude to passion: limbic activation by “unseen” drug and sexual cues. PLoS One 3 (1), e1506. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Evans AH et al. (2006) Compulsive drug use linked to sensitized ventral striatal dopamine transmission. Ann Neurol 59 (5), 852–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110.Dagher A and Robbins TW (2009) Personality, addiction, dopamine: insights from Parkinson’s disease. Neuron 61 (4), 502–10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Ashok AH et al. (2017) Association of stimulant use with dopaminergic alterations in users of cocaine, amphetamine, or methamphetamine: A systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry 74 (5), 511–519. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112.Proebstl L et al. (2019) Effects of stimulant drug use on the dopaminergic system: A systematic review and meta-analysis of in vivo neuroimaging studies. Eur Psychiatry 59, 15–24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113.Grabowski J et al. (2001) Dextroamphetamine for cocaine-dependence treatment: a double-blind randomized clinical trial. J Clin Psychopharmacol 21 (5), 522–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114.Greenwald MK et al. (2010) Sustained release d-amphetamine reduces cocaine but not ‘speedball’-seeking in buprenorphine-maintained volunteers: a test of dual-agonist pharmacotherapy for cocaine/heroin polydrug abusers. Neuropsychopharmacology 35 (13), 2624–37. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115.Rush CR et al. (2010) Cocaine choice in humans during D-amphetamine maintenance. J Clin Psychopharmacol 30 (2), 152–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116.Shearer J et al. (2003) Pilot randomized double blind placebo-controlled study of dexamphetamine for cocaine dependence. Addiction 98 (8), 1137–41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117.Lile JA et al. (2020) Pharmacological validation of a translational model of cocaine use disorder: Effects of d-amphetamine maintenance on choice between intravenous cocaine and a nondrug alternative in humans and rhesus monkeys. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 28 (2), 169–180. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118.Allain F et al. (2021) Amphetamine maintenance therapy during intermittent cocaine self-administration in rats attenuates psychomotor and dopamine sensitization and reduces addiction-like behavior. Neuropsychopharmacology 46 (2), 305–315. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 119.Ahmed SH et al. (2003) Escalation of cocaine self-administration does not depend on altered cocaine-induced nucleus accumbens dopamine levels. J Neurochem 86 (1), 102–13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120.Oleson EB and Roberts DC (2009) Behavioral economic assessment of price and cocaine consumption following self-administration histories that produce escalation of either final ratios or intake. Neuropsychopharmacology 34 (3), 796–804. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 121.Ben-Shahar O et al. (2005) Prolonged daily exposure to i.v. cocaine results in tolerance to its stimulant effects. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 82 (2), 411–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122.Robinson TE et al. (1998) Modulation of the Induction or Expression of Psychostimulant Sensitization by the Circumstances Surrounding Drug Administration. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 22 (2), 347–354. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 123.Trifilieff P et al. (2017) Blunted dopamine transmission in addiction: Potential mechanisms and implications for behavior. Semin Nucl Med 47 (1), 64–74. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 124.Boileau I et al. (2015) Imaging the D3 dopamine receptor across behavioral and drug addictions: Positron emission tomography studies with [(11)C]-(+)-PHNO. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 25 (9), 1410–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 125.Matuskey D et al. (2014) Dopamine D(3) receptor alterations in cocaine-dependent humans imaged with [(1)(1)C](+)PHNO. Drug Alcohol Depend 139, 100–5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 126.Worhunsky PD et al. (2017) Regional and source-based patterns of [(11)C]-(+)-PHNO binding potential reveal concurrent alterations in dopamine D2 and D3 receptor availability in cocaine-use disorder. Neuroimage 148, 343–351. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 127.Payer DE et al. (2014) Heightened D3 dopamine receptor levels in cocaine dependence and contributions to the addiction behavioral phenotype: a positron emission tomography study with [11C]-+-PHNO. Neuropsychopharmacology 39 (2), 311–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 128.Boileau I et al. (2012) Higher binding of the dopamine D3 receptor-preferring ligand [11C]-(+)-propyl-hexahydro-naphtho-oxazin in methamphetamine polydrug users: a positron emission tomography study. J Neurosci 32 (4), 1353–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 129.Vezina P and Leyton M (2009) Conditioned cues and the expression of stimulant sensitization in animals and humans. Neuropharmacology 56 Suppl 1, 160–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 130.Duvauchelle CL et al. (2000) Conditioned increases in behavioral activity and accumbens dopamine levels produced by intravenous cocaine. Behav Neurosci 114 (6), 1156–66. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 131.Fontana DJ et al. (1993) Conditioned increases in mesolimbic dopamine overflow by stimuli associated with cocaine. Brain Res 629 (1), 31–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 132.Paulson PE et al. (1991) Time course of transient behavioral depression and persistent behavioral sensitization in relation to regional brain monoamine concentrations during amphetamine withdrawal in rats. Psychopharmacology 103 (4), 480–492. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 133.Paulson PE and Robinson TE (1995) Amphetamine-Induced time-dependent sensitization of dopamine neurotransmission in the dorsal and ventral striatum: A microdialysis study in behaving rats. Synapse 19 (1), 56–65. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]