Abstract
Social interactions and relationships are often rewarding, but the neural mechanisms through which social interaction drives positive experience remain poorly understood. Here, we develop an automated operant conditioning system to measure social reward in mice and find that adult mice of both sexes display robust reinforcement of social interaction. Through cell-type-specific manipulations, we identify a crucial role for GABAergic neurons in the medial amygdala (MeA) in promoting the positive reinforcement of social interaction. Moreover, MeA GABAergic neurons mediate social reinforcement behavior through their projections to the medial preoptic area (MPOA) and promote dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens. Finally, activation of this MeA-to-MPOA circuit can robustly overcome avoidance behavior. Together, these findings establish the MeA as a key node for regulating social reward in both sexes, providing new insights into the regulation of social reward beyond the classic mesolimbic reward system.
Introduction
Sociality is essential for the survival and health in many animal species, including humans, and provides significant adaptive benefits1,2. In order to fruitfully navigate a social environment, animals must process social and nonsocial information within immediate and long-term social contexts in order to make appropriate social decisions3. One key piece of information that motivates such decisions is social reward—a rewarding experience associated with social interaction that provides positive reinforcement4. Characteristic abnormalities in reward processing, such as those seen in autism spectrum disorders, depression, and schizophrenia5-7, likely represent inappropriate integration of such information. However, how social interaction is reinforcing and leads to a positive experience remains poorly understood.
Previous studies have shown that social reward processing involves the classic mesolimbic reward system including the nucleus accumbens (NAc) and ventral tegmental area (VTA) as well as the brain areas that directly connect to them8-11. These are the same brain areas that also process non-social reward signals5,9,10. As social reward requires recognition and processing of social cues, the regulation of social reward may also engage circuits beyond the classical reward system, such as those that are specifically involved in social behavior. Although brain areas have been found to directly control specific types of social decisions in a context-dependent manner (e.g. aggression, mating, and parenting), how these social circuits contribute to social reward remains poorly understood12-15. Importantly, a brain area that is implicated in processing social cues and regulating acute behaviors is not necessarily involved in driving the positive reinforcement of social interaction16-18. For example, while the ventromedial hypothalamus has been shown to regulate aggression and aggressive motivation, it does not appear to promote positive reinforcement16. This suggests that social reward signaling requires engagement with specific neural circuits and cell populations even within social brain areas.
The medial amygdala (MeA) is one of the main brain areas mediating social behaviors downstream of olfactory and pheromonal processing in mice19-22. Previous work suggests an important role for the MeA in regulating specific types of social behaviors, including aggressive behavior in males, mating responses in females, and parental care towards offspring12,13,23-26. Since these specific, moment-by-moment behavioral actions are regulated in a manner that is highly sex- and behavioral context-dependent, it remains unclear whether the MeA is involved in positive reinforcement of general social interaction. Here, we developed an automated operant conditioning paradigm to measure general social reward in mice based on previous work16,27-29. We found that adult animals, both male and female, display robust reinforcement of social interaction independent of aggressive or mating contexts. Through cell-type-specific manipulations, we found that activation of the medial preoptic area (MPOA)-projecting MeA circuit is both necessary and sufficient for this positive reinforcement. Interestingly, activating MeA neurons triggers the release of dopamine in the NAc, suggesting that the MeA is a critical circuit component that mediates social reward upstream of the classical reward system.
Results
Adult animals display robust social reward behavior.
To effectively measure social reward in adult animals, we developed an operant conditioning task in which subjects nose-poke to gain access to and interact with a target animal (Fig. 1a) based on previous work16,27-29. Here, a closed-loop, fully automated system was designed to avoid any perturbations from experimenters during the assay (Fig. 1a). Juvenile animals were used as target animals to probe the response to general social interaction without the involvement of aggression or mating behavior30,31. Indeed, both male and female adult animals predominantly displayed close social investigation towards juvenile animals with little aggression or mating during free social interactions (Extended Data Fig. 1a-c).
During this experiment, a subject animal can freely nose-poke two ports. Poking the social port automatically opens a gate 3 seconds after poking, allowing the subject to closely interact with the target animal for 7 seconds (see Methods) (Fig. 1b, c), whereas poking the null port leads to a 10-s time-out. To account for individual bias toward a specific port that is unrelated to social preference, we first measured the subject animal’s baseline preference for the two ports without presentation of a target animal. Each mouse’s less favored port was designated as the social port for this particular animal in subsequent experiments. Within 7 days, the majority of mice developed a strong preference for and increased poking rate toward the social port (Fig. 1c-e, i, j; Extended Data Fig. 1d, h), but did not display this preference when they were trained without presentation of a target animal (gate opening only, Fig. 1f-h, k, l; Extended Data Fig. 1e). Importantly, >90% of the pokes in social port were followed by social interactions, suggesting that poking the social port indeed reflects a motivation to engage in social interaction, rather than a motivation to induce gate opening (Extended Data Fig. 1f). Overall, 70% of the animals showed consistent preference for social port after training (Extended Data Fig. 1g). We also examined the performance of both male and female adult mice and found that both males and females developed a strong preference for social port (Extended Data Fig. 2). This suggests that social interactions are intrinsically rewarding in both males and females and can drive positive reinforcement in an operant conditioning paradigm.
Interestingly, over the course of training, animals not only increased the numbers of pokes in the social port but also exhibited anticipatory behavior that was characterized by waiting in front of the gate during the delay period prior to opening of the gate and subsequent social interaction. The frequency and duration of this behavior increased after seven days of training and were significantly higher with the social port than null port (Fig. 1m, n). Over the course of training, animals also spent increased time engaged in social interaction and displayed reduced latency to initiate social interaction (Fig. 1o, p; as the gate does not open when poking the null port, a social interaction epoch is defined as standing in front of the gate attempting to interact with animals behind the close gate). These behavioral features demonstrate that adult mice of both sexes display robust social reward behavior.
MeApd Vgat+ neurons are required for social reward.
The posterodorsal subdivision of medial amygdala (MeApd) has been implicated in controlling acute social behavioral decisions such as aggression and parenting behavior12,13,24. However, the MeApd has not been associated with positive reinforcement of social behavior. We first asked whether the MeApd is required for social reward behavior. To this end, we performed cell-type-specific ablation of MeApd Vgat+ neurons by expressing Cre-dependent Caspase3 (or EYFP as a control) in the MeApd of Vgat-Cre animals (Fig. 2a). Histological analysis confirmed that ablation significantly reduced the number of Vgat+ neurons in the MeApd (Fig. 2b, c). While the majority of control animals developed consistent preference for social port, a significantly lower proportion of animals with MeApd Vgat+ neurons ablated developed such preference (Fig. 2d). Vgat+ neuron-ablated animals showed no significant preference for social port and no increase in the numbers of pokes in social port compared to control (Fig. 2e-h, Extended Data Fig. 3a-b). These results suggest that MeApd Vgat+ neurons are required for social reward behavior.
To test whether MeApd Vgat+ neurons are also required for the preference for social port after training, we acutely suppressed the activity of Vgat+ neurons after animals developed their preference for social port using a Cre-dependent silencing opsin GtACR (Fig. 2i, j). Efficient photostimulation-dependent silencing of GtACR-expressing Vgat+ neurons was confirmed by electrophysiological recordings in acute brain slices (Extended Data Fig. 3c-d). In the social operant task, silencing of Vgat+ neurons led to an acute suppression of pokes in social port, suggesting that MeApd Vgat+ neurons are also required for processing social reward after training (Fig. 2k, l). In addition, we found that suppressing MeApd Vgat+ neuron activity also reduced the time subject animals spent on close social investigation of juvenile mice in free social interactions (Extended Data Fig. 3e-h).
To further determine whether MeApd Vgat+ neurons are active specifically during social reward, we recorded their neuronal dynamics during the presentation of social vs. nonsocial natural rewarding stimuli (such as chocolate and sucrose solution). We found that Vgat+ neurons responded to social stimuli, but not other naturally rewarding stimuli (Extended Data Fig. 4a, b). Moreover, we found that Caspase3-mediated ablation of MeApd Vgat+ neurons did not impair the animals’ positive preference for food reward (Extended Data Fig. 5a-d). These results suggest that the MeApd is likely specifically involved in social reward.
Activating MeApd Vgat+ neurons promotes reinforcement behavior.
The above results suggest that activity of MeApd Vgat+ neurons may represent positive social signals. If the MeA transforms social cues into reward signals, we hypothesize that direct activation of MeA neurons may bypass the requirement of upstream social inputs and should be sufficient to produce a rewarding experience and drive reinforcement. To determine if activation of Vgat+ neurons promotes reinforcement, we expressed a Cre-dependent activating opsin ChR2 (or EYFP as a control) in the MeApd of Vgat-Cre animals (Fig. 3a, b). Using the real-time place preference assay as a measure for reinforcement behavior, we found that animals developed a consistent positive preference for the chamber coupled with stimulation (Fig. 3c-e). Consistently, in the intracranial self-stimulation assay, animals developed a positive preference to self-stimulate their Vgat+ neurons compared to poking a null port (Fig. 3f-h; Extended Data Fig. 6a). To determine whether the MeApd-mediated reward is specific to a particular sex, we examined the effect of activating the MeApd Vgat+ neurons in both male and female mice. We found that activating these neurons produced a comparably strong preference for stimulation in both sexes, in both the real-time place preference assay and the intracranial self-stimulation assay (Extended Data Fig. 6b-g). These results suggest that activation of MeApd Vgat+ neurons is indeed highly rewarding in both males and females. The finding that activating these neurons can drive reinforcement independent of a social context does not mean that these neurons are nonspecifically involved in general reward but likely reflects an induced gain-of-function that is sufficient to bypass upstream social inputs.
The observation that the MeApd generates reward raised the possibility that the activity of MeApd Vgat+ neurons is sufficient to drive positive reinforcement over days. We used a paradigm that mimicked our social operant task, where the “social” port is coupled with optogenetic stimulation of MeApd Vgat+ neurons in place of presentation of target animals (Fig. 3i). We found that, over three days of training, animals developed a strong preference and increased number of pokes toward the optogenetic port (Fig. 3j-m; Extended Data Fig. 7a-b, e-f), suggesting that activating MeApd Vgat+ neurons is indeed sufficient to drive positive reinforcement over days.
We next asked whether reward is specifically regulated by Vgat+ neurons in the MeApd. The MeApd consists of both Vgat+ and Vglut2+ neurons12. We found that activating Vglut2+ neurons in the real time place preference assay did not promote any positive preference in the stimulated chamber, but rather caused the animals to spend significantly less time there (Extended Data Fig. 8), suggesting that Vglut2+ neuron activity is associated with a negative valence. This result further suggests that the positive reinforcement is specifically controlled by Vgat+ neurons.
Activation of MeApd Vgat+ neurons drives dopamine release.
Release of dopamine is a characteristic associated with rewarding experiences5,32,33. Our results raise an important question of whether the MeApd may regulate dopamine reward system. Using a genetically encoded fluorescent dopamine sensor (dLight), we first examined the patterns of dopamine signals in the Nucleus Accumbens (NAc) when animals were exposed to various social stimuli. We injected an AAV expressing dLight (or EYFP as a control) into the NAc, and examined dopamine signals by measuring dLight fluorescence in awake animals using fiber photometry (Fig. 4a, b). We observed significant increase in dopamine signals when subject animals freely interacted with males, females, or juvenile animals (Fig. 4c-f), suggesting that social interactions induce a robust increase of dopamine signals in the NAc.
We then recorded dLight fluorescence in the NAc while animals performed the social operant task (Fig. 4g-s). We observed a significant increase in dopamine signal during the social interaction period. The signal was highest in the initial phase of training and was reduced when the animals were well trained (Fig. 4p). Interestingly, an elevated signal was observed immediately after gate opening, which indicates the onset of social interaction (Fig. 4q). This increase remained when the animals were well-trained and even during the post-training test in the absence of target animals, suggesting that dopamine release is associated with this conditional cue signaling the onset of social interaction. By contrast, at the end of the social interaction period, dLight signal dropped significantly when the gate started to close at 8 s, which reflects the removal of social reward (Fig. 4r). Finally, nose-poke actions were also associated with an increase of dLight signal (Fig. 4s), which may indicate that the elevation in motivation to nose-poke for social reward is associated with elevated dopamine release. Importantly, all of these dLight signals were not observed or significantly weaker when animals were trained with gate opening alone (without target animals), confirming that the observed changes of dopamine signals are indeed related to social reward rather than simply due to gate opening or closure alone. Collectively, these results suggest that changes of dopamine signals in the NAc are associated with the rewarding social interaction and triggered by related cues.
We next asked if activating MeApd Vgat+ neurons is sufficient to induce dopamine release in the NAc. To this end, we injected an AAV expressing Cre-dependent ChR2 into the MeApd of Vgat-Cre animals, and injected another AAV expressing dLight into the NAc of the same animals (Fig. 5a). We then measured dLight fluorescence in awake animals using fiber photometry while optogenetically activating MeApd Vgat+ neurons. Strikingly, we found that ChR2 activation of Vgat+ neurons triggered a robust, time-locked increase of dLight fluorescence in the NAc, whereas photostimulation of EYFP-expressing control animals did not have this effect (Fig. 5b-d). Following the offset of stimulation, the dLight signal showed a significant reduction (Fig. 5e), which likely reflects the termination of optogenetically induced reward. These results demonstrate that activation of MeApd Vgat+ neurons is sufficient to drive robust, time-locked dopamine release.
MPOA-projecting MeApd neurons mediate social reward behavior.
As the MeA does not directly project to classic reward centers such as the NAc, an important question is how the MeApd processes reward through downstream circuitry. Given that the MeApd projects to the medial preoptic nucleus that directly connects to the VTA11,34,35, the MeApd-to-MPOA pathway is an attractive candidate for mediation of social reward.
To test this, we injected an AAV expressing Cre-dependent ChR2 (or EYFP as a control) in the MeApd and implanted optic fibers above the MPOA to specifically activate the axonal terminals of MeApd Vgat+ neurons that directly project to the MPOA (Fig. 6a, b). Indeed, activating these axonal terminals in the MPOA produced a strong positive preference in both the real-time place preference test and the intracranial self-stimulation test (Fig. 6c-f; Extended Data Fig. 7i), suggesting that MPOA-projecting terminals carry reinforcement signals. To determine whether activation of the MeApd-to-MPOA circuit is also sufficient to drive reinforcement behavior over days, we tested these animals in a test similar to that in Figure 3 (Fig. 6g), and found that activating MPOA-projecting terminals was able to generate a strong preference and increased number of pokes toward the optogenetic port over days (Fig. 6h, i; Extended Data Fig. 7c-d, g-h). These results suggest that activation of the MeApd-to-MPOA circuit is indeed sufficient to promote reinforcement behavior.
To further determine whether the reinforcement behavior is specifically driven by the MeApd-to-MPOA circuit, we first ruled out the possibility that this reinforcement behavior is caused by antidromic activation of collateral projections to other brain areas. To prevent backpropagating action potentials from spreading to other collateral projections, we locally infused lidocaine into the MeApd while activating axonal projections in the MPOA (Extended Data Fig. 9a-b). We found that blocking backpropagating action potentials did not affect the behavioral function of stimulating the MeApd-to-MPOA projection (Extended Data Fig. 9b), suggesting that this behavioral effect is indeed caused by the MeApd-to-MPOA projection but not by other collateral branches. Furthermore, we directly manipulated another major axonal target of MeApd Vgat+ neurons in the ventral premammillary nucleus (PMV) in ChR2-expressing animals, and found that photostimulation of axonal projections to the PMV did not promote reinforcement behavior (Extended Data Fig. 9c-d). Together, these results strongly suggest that the MPOA projection of MeApd Vgat+ neurons is specifically involved in processing social reward and reinforcement.
To test whether the MeApd-to-MPOA circuit is also required for the preference for social port in the social operant task, we acutely suppressed the activity of MPOA-projecting MeApd neurons after animals developed their preference for social port. To specifically label MeApd neurons that project to the MPOA, we injected a retrograde AAV expressing Cre recombinase into the MPOA and a second AAV expressing Cre-dependent GtACR into the MeApd (Fig. 6j-l). Similar to what was observed when silencing all MeApd Vgat+ neurons, silencing MPOA-projecting MeApd neurons by GtACR also led to an acute suppression of pokes in social port. These results suggest that MPOA-projecting MeApd neurons are also required for processing social reward (Fig. 6m, n).
We found that, among MeApd neurons that project to the MPOA, 85.3% are GABAergic (Extended Data Fig. 10a-e). To rule out the possibility that MPOA-projecting glutamatergic neurons may also contribute to reinforcement behavior, we specifically activated the axonal projections of MeApd glutamatergic neurons in the MPOA using optogenetics (Extended Data Fig. 10f, g). We found that activating glutamatergic projections to the MPOA does not promote any positive reinforcement.
To further determine whether the MeApd-to-MPOA circuit is also selective for social reward, we expressed axon-localized GCaMP6 in MeApd Vgat+ neurons and used fiber photometry to record calcium activity in their axonal projections in the MPOA during exposure to social and nonsocial stimuli (Extended Data Fig. 4c, d). We found that, similar to MeApd Vgat+ neurons themselves, axonal projections to the MPOA also displayed strong responses during social interaction but little response to food reward (Extended Data Fig. 4c, d). Furthermore, ablating MPOA-projecting MeApd neurons did not impair the animals’ preference for food reward (Extended Data Figure 5e-h). These results suggest that MPOA-projecting MeApd neurons are not directly involved in promoting food reward.
MeApd-mediated reward overcomes avoidance behavior.
Positive social interaction can buffer against aversive experiences2,36, but the underlying mechanism at the circuit level has been largely unclear. The observation that the MeApd circuit mediates social reward offers an opportunity to ask whether positive signals mediated by the MeApd circuit can overcome negative experiences and suppress the associated behaviors with negative valence. Animals typically display a robust avoidance behavior to move away from locations associated with negative valence. In a large open field, mice innately prefer to stay in the periphery of the arena, as the center is associated with potential danger. When we coupled optogenetic stimulation to the center area (Fig. 7a-c), ChR2-expressing animals spent substantially longer time in the center, whereas control animals did not show this increase (Fig. 7d, e). This suggests that the rewarding effect of activating these neurons is sufficiently strong to act against the innate avoidance of the central area.
Similarly, in an elevated plus maze, animals innately prefer to stay in closed arms and spend much less time in open arms that are associated with a negative valence. When we coupled optogenetic stimulation to open arms, the animals spent substantially longer time in those areas (Fig. 7i-m), suggesting that the rewarding effect of activating MeApd Vgat+ neurons similarly overcomes the innate avoidance of open arms.
Finally, we examined whether this effect is also mediated by the MeApd-MPOA circuit by specifically activating the MeApd Vgat+ axonal terminals in the MPOA (Fig. 7f). When coupling optogenetic activation to the center area in the open field or open arms in the elevated plus maze, we observed a similar increase of time spent in these areas that are associated with negative valence (Fig. 7f-h, n-p). This suggests that activation of the MeApd-MPOA pathway is sufficient to mediate this effect. Together, these results establish a key role for the MeA-to-MPOA circuit in generating positive signals that can overcome avoidance behavior.
Discussion
Using an automated social operant task to measure social reward in mice, we show that adult mice, both male and female, are capable of developing positive reinforcement associated with social reward. Using this assay, we uncover a previously unknown role for the MeA in regulating social reward. We found that Vgat+ neurons in the MeApd are required for social reward behavior, and activating these neurons drive positive reinforcement. We show that dopamine signals in the NAc are associated with social reward-related cues during the social operant task and that activation of MeApd Vgat+ neurons promotes dopamine release in the NAc. Finally, the rewarding effect produced by MeApd Vgat+ neurons is mediated by their projections to the MPOA and is strong enough to overcome avoidance behavior. Together, these results establish a direct causal role for MeA Vgat+ neurons in mediating social reward behavior and present a mechanism for controlling social reward beyond the classical reward system.
Previous studies using social conditioned place preference assay suggest that juvenile, but not adult, mice are able to establish a persistent social preference37. We developed a fully automated operant conditioning system in which animals are trained to nose-poke specific ports to engage in social interactions. Using this system, we present conclusive evidence that adult mice of both sexes exhibit robust preference for social reward, demonstrating that our assay is effective at measuring social reward in adult animals. As this system is fully automated, it increases efficiency and minimizes unnecessary manual disturbance during experiments. The inclusion of a barrier allows us to specifically examine motivation and rewarding effect of behavior initiated by the subject animal, as opposed to passive experiences of behavior initiated by target animals. While animals are able to receive and recognize social cues through the wire grid barrier (such as olfactory, visual, or auditory signals) and can engage in social approach and close social investigation, the presence of a barrier does not allow us to examine full interactions between animals that involve physical contact. As a complementary approach, we have also examined social interactions in a freely moving context prior to performing social operant tasks and showed that adult animals predominantly engaged in close investigation behavior towards juvenile animals.
While reward processing for nonsocial stimuli has been studied extensively, less is known about the brain regions that mediate social reward5. Previous studies have largely focused on brain regions in the classic mesolimbic reward system such as the NAc and the VTA and brain regions that directly connect to them8-11. A common picture from these studies is that the same circuitry that processes non-social reward signals also process social reward9,10,37. Indeed, we found that, in the social operant task, dopamine release in the NAc was significantly elevated during social interaction, as well as during gate opening (as a conditioned cue signaling the onset of social interaction), consistent with the role of dopamine during appetitive food- or drug-seeking behavior32,33,38,39 and during a similar social reward behavior in a recent preprint40.
While MeApd was previously implicated in several social behaviors in a sex- and behavioral context-dependent manner12,13,23-26, whether it is involved in mediating positive reinforcement of social interaction was unclear. Here, we provide multiple lines of evidence that clearly establish the MeApd as a new circuit component in regulating social reward. The activity of MeApd Vgat+ neurons is required for processing social reward and is sufficient to drive reinforcement behavior. Our finding that activating these neurons can drive reinforcement does not mean that these neurons are nonspecifically involved in general reward but rather reflects a gain-of-function that is sufficient to bypass the requirement of social cues. Indeed, the activity of MeApd Vgat+ neurons is not required for mediating food reward and activation of Npy1r+ neurons in the MeA can suppress feeding behavior41. While we cannot exclude the possibility that these neurons are involved in other types of reward that we did not examine, our data suggest that the MeApd is unlikely nonspecifically involved in processing all appetitive stimuli. Interestingly, activation of MeApd Vgat+ neurons drives the release of dopamine in the NAc, which suggests that while the MeApd is not part of the classic mesolimbic system, positive social signals mediated by MeApd Vgat+ neurons likely eventually converge onto the mesolimbic circuit to mediate social reward behavior.
We show that the effect of MeApd neurons on social reward and reinforcement could be mediated by the MPOA-projecting MeApd circuitry. The MPOA is a highly heterogeneous brain structure that contains multiple partially overlapping subpopulations marked by the expression of Nts, Esr1, and Gal genes, each of which consists of a mix of glutamatergic and GABAergic neurons11,34,35. While these various MPOA populations project directly to the VTA, the synaptic mechanism of how these neurons promote dopamine release in the NAc is still largely unclear. Indeed, both glutamatergic and GABAergic populations send direct monosynaptic inputs to the VTA34 and may exert opposite effects. Thus, understanding how MeApd Vgat+ neurons promote dopamine release requires a full picture of the MPOA-VTA-NAc circuitry and remains an important topic for future investigation. Our findings provide new insights into our understanding of the circuitry that connects the amygdala and the hypothalamus and their roles in regulating social behavior and reinforcement. As drugs of abuse lead to addiction by taking control of normal brain reward circuits that reinforce essential behaviors33,42, the MeApd-to-MPOA pathway could be a previously underappreciated target of drugs of abuse that deserve further studies.
Animals engage in specific types of social behavioral actions within immediate social contexts, such as aggressive behavior in males16,43 or sexual attraction between males and females11,26,44,45. The motivation that drives these specific moment-by-moment actions tends to be highly context-dependent and likely involves distinct neural circuits11,16,26,43,44. As the MeA was previously shown to play an important role in aggressive behavior in males and sexual responses in females, the role of MeApd Vgat+ neurons in mediating general social reward towards juvenile animals in both males and females may reflect a distinct behavioral function. Indeed, a different subpopulation of the MeA (Nos1+) promotes female animals’ sexual responses towards male pheromones and can also drive reinforcement behaivor26. These neurons are enriched in the posteroventral subdivision of the MeA (MeApv), which regulates sexual receptivity and mating behavior in females23. As the MeApd consists of heterogeneous subpopulations of GABAergic neurons13,46, it remains to be determined whether distinct GABAergic subtypes may play distinct roles in social reward and/or other social behaviors and whether these diverse behavioral functions are mediated by the same or distinct neuronal subtypes.
Moreover, involvement of a brain area in processing social information does not necessitate its requirement for social reward and/or positive reinforcement. For example, while the ventromedial hypothalamus has been shown to drive specific social behavior, it does not appear to promote positive reinforcement16. Moreover, activation of MeApv Drd+ neuron projection to the bed nucleus of stria terminalis increases aggression, but it does not appear to promote positive reinforcement17. Lastly, activating neurons in the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex that are directly involved in social behavior does not lead to positive reinforcement18. These findings suggest that processing positive social signals and promoting positive reinforcement are not a nonspecific feature of social brain areas, but recruit specific circuits and neuronal populations. Thus, Vgat+ neurons in the MeApd do not simply relay social information but likely represent a specific node among social brain areas that is both necessary and sufficient to drive positive reinforcement. Indeed, Vglu2+ neurons in the MeApd do not promote positive reinforcement, further suggesting that the role of Vgat+ neurons in social reward is specific and is functionally separable from Vglut2+ neurons in the same brain area.
Finally, social impairment is a core symptom of many neurodevelopmental and neuropsychiatric disorders, such as autism spectrum disorders, depression, and anxiety6,7,47. As positive social experiences play a critical role in buffering against aversive experiences, impairment of social functions and resulting lack of social experiences may lead to a wide variety of health problems2,36,48. How social processing overcomes negative experiences, and the neural mechanisms underlying these functions, have been largely unclear. We showed that the rewarding effect produced by the activation of the MeApd circuit is sufficient to overcome behavior associated with negative experiences. This suggests that a potential mechanism of counterbalancing negative experiences is through the activation of circuits that directly regulate social reward.
METHODS
Animals
C57BL/6J males and females (8-12 weeks old) were purchased from Jackson Laboratories and used for behavioral experiments. Slc32a1-ires-Cre (Vgat-Cre) and Slc17a6-ires-Cre (Vglut2-Cre) mice49 were purchased from Jackson Laboratories (stock numbers 028862 and 028863) and were crossed to C57BL/6J mice (purchased from Jackson Laboratories) to produce heterozygous animals (VgatCre/+ and Vglut2Cre/+, 8-16 weeks old, both males and females) for stereotaxic surgery and behavioral experiments. Juvenile animals (4-6 weeks old) used as social stimuli in the social operant task were produced from our breeding colony. Animals were housed in 12 h light-dark cycle (10 p.m. – 10 a.m. light), with food and water available ad libitum. Care and experimental manipulations of all animals were carried out in accordance with the NIH Guide for Care and Use of Laboratory Animals and approved by UCLA IACUC.
Viruses
AAV2-Syn-EYFP, AAV2-EF1α-DIO-EYFP, AAV2-EF1α-FLEX-mCherry, AAV2-EF1α-DIO-hChR2-EYFP, and AAV2-DIO-taCasp3-TEVp50 were purchased from UNC Viral Vector Core. AAV1-hSyn1-SIO-stGtACR2-FusionRed51 (Catalog #105677-AAV1), AAV1-syn-FLEX-jGCaMP7f-WPRE52 (Catalog #104492-AAV1), AAV5-hSynapsin1-FLEX-axon-GCaMP6s53 (Catalog #112010-AAV1), AAV1-mDlx-NLS-mRuby254 (Catalog #99130-AAV1), AAVretro-hSyn-HI-eGFP-Cre (Catalog #105540-AAVrg), and AAV5-hSyn-dLight1.255 (Catalog #111068-AAV5) were purchased from Addgene. CTB-Alexa Fluor 647 was purchased from Thermo Fisher Scientific (Catalog #C34778).
Stereotaxic surgeries
VgatCre/+ and Vglut2Cre/+ animals (8-16 weeks old) were anesthetized with isoflurane and mounted on a stereotaxic device (Kopf Instruments). Both males (>20 g) and females (>16 g) were used in the experiments. Injections were carried out using a pulled, fine glass capillary (WPI).
For Caspase-mediated ablation of MeApd Vgat+ neurons, Vgat-Cre mice were injected bilaterally with 350 nL AAV2-DIO-taCasp3-TEVp and 50 nL AAV2-EF1α-DIO-hChR2-EYFP into the MeApd (ML ±2.05, AP −1.5~−1.6, DV −5.25 from bregma).
For Caspase-mediated ablation of MPOA-projecting MeApd neurons, C57BL/6J mice were bilaterally injected with 300 nL AAV-retro-eGFp-Cre into the MPOA (ML ±0.4, AP −0.1, DV −5.0 from bregma) and 400 nL AAV2-DIO-taCasp3-TEVp into the MeApd (ML ±2.05, AP −1.5~−1.7, DV −5.25 from bregma).
For optogenetic activation or silencing of MeApd neuron cell bodies, Vgat-Cre mice were injected bilaterally with 400 nL AAV1-hSyn1-SIO-stGtACR2-FusionRed into the MeApd for inhibition or 200-250 nL AAV2-EF1α-DIO-hChR2-EYFP into the MeApd for activation (MeApd: ML ±2.05, AP −1.5~−1.7, DV −5.25 from bregma). An optic fiber (200 um core diameter, Inper) was then placed 0.4-0.5 mm above the virus injection site in the MeApd.
For fiber photometry recording of calcium signals, Vgat-Cre mice was injected with 200 nL AAV1-syn-FLEX-jGCaMP7f-WPRE or AAV5-hSynapsin1-FLEX-axon-GCaMP6s into the MeApd (ML ±2.05, AP −1.5~−1.6, DV −5.15~−5.25 from bregma). An optic fiber (200 μm core diameter, Inper) was implanted 0.2 mm above the injection site in the MeApd or at its downstream projection target in the MPOA (ML ±0.4, AP −0.1, DV −5.0 from bregma).
For fiber photometry recording of dopamine signals in the NAc during the social operant task, C57BL/6J mice were injected with 200-250 nL of AAV5-hSyn-dLight1.2 (or AAV2-Syn-EYFP as a control) into the NAc (ML 1.20, AP +1.20, DV −4.30 from bregma). An optic fiber (200 μm core diameter, Inper) was implanted at the virus injection site in the NAc.
For fiber photometry recording of dopamine signals in the NAc in response to activation of MeApd Vgat+ neurons, Vgat-Cre mice were injected with 300 nL AAV5-hSyn-dLight1.2 into NAc (ML 1.20, AP +1.20, DV −4.30 from bregma) and 200-250 nL AAV2-EF1α-DIO-hChR2-EYFP into the MeApd (ML ±2.05, AP −1.5~−1.6, DV −5.25 from bregma). Two optic fibers (200 μm core diameter, Inper) were then implanted, one at the injection site in the NAc and the other 0.5 mm above the injection site in the MeApd.
For optogenetic activation of MeApd projections, Vgat-Cre mice were injected bilaterally (ML ±2.05, AP −1.5~−1.7, DV −5.25 from bregma) with 250 nL AAV2-EF1α-DIO-hChR2-EYFP into the MeApd. Optic fibers were then implanted 0.4-0.5 mm above the MPOA (left side: ML −0.4, AP −0.1, DV −4.75 from bregma; right side, angled 6 degrees, ML 1.0, AP −0.1, DV −4.8 from bregma) or above the PMV (left side: ML −0.5, AP −2.3, DV −5.10 from bregma; right side, ML 1.0, AP −2.3, DV −5.15 from bregma with a 3-degree angle). To rule out the possibility that stimulation of axonal terminals in the MPOA may lead to backpropagation of action potentials to cell bodies in the MeApd and other collateral projections, lidocaine (10 μg / 0.3 μl) was locally infused through a guide cannula into the MeApd.
For optogenetic inhibition of the MeApd-to-MPOA circuit, C57BL/6J mice were bilaterally injected with 300 nL AAV-retro-eGFP-Cre into the MPOA (ML ±0.4, AP −0.1, DV −5.0 from bregma) and 400 nL AAV1-hSyn1-SIO-stGtACR2-FusionRed (ML ±2.05, AP −1.5~−1.7, DV −5.25 from bregma), and then bilaterally implanted with optic fibers above the MeApd (ML ±2.05, AP −1.5~−1.7, DV −4.75 from bregma).
For characterization of MeApd GABAergic and glutamatergic neurons that project to the MPOA, Vglut2-Cre mice were injected with 300 nL AAV-DIO-EYFP and 100 nL AAV-mDlx-NLS-mRuby2 into the MeApd (ML ±2.05, AP −1.5~−1.7, DV −5.25 from bregma). 10 days after the initial injection, 200 nL CTB-Alexa Fluor 647 was injected into the MPOA (ML ±0.4, AP −0.1, DV −5.0 from bregma) of the same animal. Dlx has been previously shown to specifically label GABAergic neurons56. We confirm that the expression of Dlx and Vglut2 promotors shows little overlap in the MeApd (Extended Data Fig. 10d).
All control animals in this study were animals with the same genetic background injected with EYFP-expressing or mCherry-expressing AAVs.
Behavioral assays
Automated system for quantitative analysis of social reward
Based on previous work16,27-29, we developed a fully automated, closed-loop operant conditioning system to quantitatively and efficiently measure social reward. The system consists of two chambers (Fig. 1a, b), a smaller one for holding the target animal, and a larger one for placing the subject animal. The two chambers are separated by a metal wire grid (5 cm x 9 cm) that prevents animals from going to each other’s chambers but allows the subject animal to sniff and investigate the target animal. Behind the wire gird is an opaque plastic door which can be automatically opened or closed by a motor, which is directly controlled by an Arduino microcontroller with custom code. Two 13 mm-diameter nose-poke ports (i.e. social and non-social (null) ports) are located on two sides of the gate. Nose-pokes are detected by infrared beam sensors connected to the Arduino microcontroller. Nose-poking the social port initiates a new 10-s trial, which consists of a 3-s delay period and 7-s social interaction period during which the gate is opened. The gate starts to open at 3 s, becomes fully opened at 5 s, starts to close at 8 s, and becomes fully closed at 10 s (gate opening and closure take 2 s to complete). Nose-poking the null port initiates a 10-s time-out period without gate opening and social interaction (Fig. 1b). Our social operant task consists of three phases, which include baseline (1 day), training (7 days), and post-training (2 days) (Fig. 1b). We implemented a closed-loop and fully automated system for the social operant task with no manual intervention during the experiment. Compared to previous operant conditioning systems for social motivation, we also restricted the target animal in a smaller chamber (while the target animal can still move freely), increasing the chance of close social interaction when the gate opens.
In experiments presented in Fig. 1, wild type mice were housed in our animal colony for at least one week. Prior to experiments, mice were habituated in the behavioral rig for 30 min with the ports blocked for 1-2 days. After habituation, on day 0 (baseline phase), we tested the baseline preference for the ports by allowing the mouse to freely poke the ports without door opening for 30 minutes. To counterbalance individual bias toward a specific port that is unrelated to social preference, the port with fewer nose-pokes for each animal was designated as the social port and the other port as the non-social (null) port for subsequent experiments. On days 1–7 (training phase), each mouse was trained in a 30-minute daily session with door opening and social presentation (Fig 1c, f). An unfamiliar, male juvenile animal was used, and a different juvenile mouse was used each day over the 7-day training sessions. While poking null port does not lead to gate opening or social stimulus, it does lead to a 10-s time-out, which serves as a competing choice that animals have to select for or against during the training process. On days 8–9 (post-training phase), each mouse was trained in a 30-minute daily session without target animals during the “interaction” phase. To rule out the possibility that the preference for social port was solely caused by gate opening, we also carried out control experiments in which mice were trained with gate opening in the absence of target animals when poking the social port.
Preference for social port was calculated as the differences between the percentage of trials with nose-poking in social port and the percentage of trials with nose-poking in null port. To provide a descriptive characterization of animals exhibiting persistent social reward behavior, we chose a threshold for consistent preference (define as showing greater than 20% preference for social port on both day 6 and day 7) (Extended Data Fig. 1). However, statistics was done with the full distribution of preferences from all animals without using any cut-off. Behaviors were recorded and quantified on days 1 and 7. We measured the anticipatory waiting time prior to gate opening, percentage of trials showing anticipatory behavior, latency to social interaction or exploration of the open gate, duration of interaction or exploration, and percentage of trials with interaction. Period of social interaction was defined as the moment when subject animals stood in front of the gate and sniffed the target animal behind the gate. After poking the null port, animals sometimes incorrectly display anticipatory behavior (waiting in front of the gate during the delay period; see Fig. 1m-n) as well as staying in front of the gate attempting to interact with animals behind the gate (Fig. 1o-p).
VgatCre/+ mice produced in our breeding colony tend to show an overall lower number of nose-pokes (in both social and null ports) compared to wild type C57BL/6J animals purchased from Jackson Laboratory, suggesting that the general motivation to engage in nose-poking could be different in animals with different transgenetic backgrounds and/or housing conditions. In all experiments, we did not proceed with training if animals exhibited no or little motivation to engage in nose-poke on both of the first two days of training (defined as less than 10 total pokes in both social and null ports on each day).
To examine behavior subject animals displayed towards juvenile animals during free social interactions prior to being trained in the social operant task, we performed a free social interaction test (Extended Figure 1a-c), in which subject animal freely interacts with an unfamiliar male juvenile mouse for 10 mins in a standard mouse cage. Social behaviors were annotated and quantified, which include sniffing (towards body trunk, anogenital area, or facial area), following, allogrooming, attack, and mounting.
Operant task for food reward
Animals were trained to press levers to obtain food reward (Extended Fig. 5) using an operant task chamber (Med Associates, USA). The training procedure includes 3 days of magazine training and 3 days of operant training. Briefly, mice were initially food deprived for 18 hours prior to the first training day. On days 1-3, animals were subject to daily magazine training sessions, in which each mouse received one food pellet (20 mg; Bio-Serv) per min without lever pressing for 30 mins. Following each daily training session, subject animals were provided excessive amount of food pellets before being food deprived for 14 hours (8 p.m. – 10 a.m.) prior the next training session. On days 4-6, animals were subject to daily operant training sessions, in which each mouse was trained to lever press to obtain food pellets for 30 mins. Pressing the lever paired with food reward led to the delivery of one food pellet after a brief delay (0.5 s on days 4 and 5 for association, and 3 s on day 6 for testing), whereas pressing the other lever (the null lever) leads to a 10-s time-out.
Caspase-3 ablation experiments
In Caspase-3-mediated ablation experiments, preferences for social reward were measured by the social operant task described above. Preferences for food seeking behavior were measured by the operant task for food reward described above. Caspase-3-expressing and EYFP-expressing VgatCre/+ mice were trained in the social operant task (Fig. 2a-h) or food operant task (Extended Data Fig. 5) ~3.5 weeks (for soma ablation) or ~ 5 weeks (for ablating MPOA-projecting MeA neurons) after viral injection. VgatCre/+ mice (both Caspase-3- and EYFP-expressing animals) showed an overall lower number of nose-pokes (in both social and null ports) compared to wild type animals.
GtACR inhibition experiments
Optogenetic inhibition in social operant task
GtACR inhibition experiments (Fig. 2 and Fig. 6) were performed over three consecutive days after animals established a positive preference for social port in the social operant task. Sham stimulation (no laser illumination) was performed on days 1 and 3, and optogenetic inhibition was performed on day 2. Each day consisted of five mins of baseline and five mins of sham or real stimulation (continuous 473 nm laser illumination). Prior to each experiment, a ferrule patch cord was coupled to the ferrule fiber implanted in the mouse using a zirconia split sleeve (Doric Lenses). An Arduino microcontroller board and a custom MATLAB program were used to control the initiation and termination of the test.
Free social interaction assay
A 3-min free social interaction session was performed daily on two consecutive days to examine various social behavior when subject animals freely interaction with a juvenile animal (Extended Fig. 3). Prior to each session, subject animals were habituated in a standard mouse cage for 3 min after being connected a ferrule patch cord. An unfamiliar juvenile male mouse was introduced into the cage for 3 min (different juveniles were used in different sessions). In one of two daily sessions (selected in a counterbalanced manner), continuous blue light stimulation was delivered to subject animals throughout the entire 3-min session. Social behaviors were annotated and quantified, which include sniffing (towards body trunk, anogenital, or facial area), following, allogrooming, attack, and mounting. We compared the percent of time spent on different types of social behaviors between sham stimulation and blue light stimulation.
Three-chamber assay
Three-chamber assay was performed using a three-chamber apparatus that consists of two side compartments (25 cm x 25 cm) and a center compartment (12.5 cm x 25 cm) (illustrated in Extended Data Fig. 9a). In the first session, subject animals were introduced to the center compartment and allowed to freely explore all three compartments for 3 min. A stimulus mouse (4-6 weeks old) was then placed under an inverted wire cup in one side compartment (designated as social compartment), and an empty cup was placed in the other side compartment (designated as nonsocial compartment). Subject animals were allowed to explore all chambers for 3 min and tested daily on two consecutive days. In each daily session, one of the side compartments was selected as the social compartment in a counterbalanced manner on two consecutive days. One session is paired with optical stimulation and the other with no stimulation in a counterbalanced manner. The social preference was calculated as (time in social compartment − time in nonsocial compartment) / (time in social compartment + time in nonsocial compartment).
ChR2 activation experiments
ChR2 stimulations of MeApd neuron cell bodies or MeApd-to-MPOA circuit were performed using the following behavioral paradigms. Fiber attachment and laser control were done as in GtACR silencing experiments.
Real time place preference test (RTPP)
Mice were introduced into a two-chamber apparatus (60 cm x 30 cm x 30 cm for each chamber) and were allowed to freely move between the two chambers. Each test consisted of two consecutive 15-min sessions. In the first session, mice were allowed to freely explore the two compartments for 15 minutes, during which entering into one of the two chambers triggered optogenetic stimulation (ChR2: 473 nm, 20 ms, 20 Hz for soma stimulation or 473 nm, 5 ms, 50 Hz for terminal stimulation, 2.5 mW/mm2, for up to 20-s duration). Exiting the stimulated chamber immediately terminated the photostimulation. During the second session, the opposite chamber was paired with photostimulation. An Arduino microcontroller board and a customized MATLAB program were used to control laser pulses, based on real-time tracking of mouse locations. Preference scores were calculated as the differences between percentages of time spent in the stimulated and unstimulated sides.
To examine the effect of potential backpropagation of action potentials to collateral branches, we performed a lidocaine infusion experiment during the RTPP test. On day 1 (ChR2 only), animals were subject to light stimulations in the RTPP test using procedures described above. On day 2 (ChR2 + lidocaine), a single dose of lidocaine (10 μg / 0.3μl) was locally infused into the MeApd through an implanted guide cannula 10 minutes prior to the delivery of light stimulations in the RTPP test.
We also examined the effect of activating MeApd glutamatergic projections to the MPOA and MeApd GABAergic projections to the PMV in the RTPP test using procedures described above. Two daily sessions were performed on two consecutive days. Sham stimulation was performed on day 1 (baseline) and light stimulation was performed on day 2 (stimulation).
Intracranial self-stimulation
Mice were placed into a customized operant conditioning chamber (30cm x 30cm x 30cm) that has two nose-poke ports (active and inactive) with infrared beam sensors. Nose-poking the active port triggered the delivery of photostimulation (ChR2: 473 nm, 20 ms, 20 Hz for soma stimulation or 473 nm, 5 ms, 50 Hz for terminal stimulation, 2.5 mW/mm2, for up to 1-s duration), and withdrawal of the nose from the port terminated light stimulation. No stimulation was delivered when nose-poking the inactive port. The active and inactive ports were assigned randomly. Mice were allowed to freely explore the chamber for 10 min prior to the experiment for habituation to the experimental environment and were allowed to explore for 30 min during the experiment. An Arduino microcontroller board and a customized MATLAB program were used to control laser pulses.
Optogenetic reward task (mimicking social reward behavior)
We modified the social operant task by replacing social interaction with 2-s light stimulation (ChR2: 473 nm, 20 ms, 20 Hz for soma stimulation or 473 nm, 5 ms, 50 Hz for terminal stimulation, 2.5 mW/mm2) following the 3-s delay period. This test consisted a 5-day training procedure (Fig. 3 and Fig. 6). On day 0 (baseline), we assessed the baseline preference for the ports by placing the mouse into the rig and letting it freely poke the ports without door opening for about 30 minutes. From day 1 to day 3 (training), each mouse was trained in a 30-minute daily session with optical stimulation during the “interaction” phase in the task (Fig 3 and Fig 6). On day 4 (post-training), each mouse was trained in a 30-minute daily session without light delivery during the “interaction” phase. Preference for stimulation-coupled “social” port is calculated by subtracting the percentage of trials with nose-poking in the null port from the percentage of trials with nose-poking in the stimulated port. An Arduino microcontroller board and a customized MATLAB program were used to control laser pulses. We did not perform a social operant assay in which optogenetic stimulation is coupled with the presence of a target animal, as optogenetics stimulation alone is already sufficient to promote robust positive reinforcement and drive learning over days (Fig. 6).
Modified open field test.
The open field test was done in a square chamber (50 × 50 × 50 cm). An experiment consists of a 40 min test, which includes four 10-min sessions of alternating laser stimulations (OFF–ON–ON–OFF). Mice were initially placed in the center of the chamber and allowed to freely explore the chamber. When the animals enter the center zone (25 x 25 cm), optical stimulation is delivered (ChR2: 473 nm, 20 ms, 20 Hz for soma stimulation or 473 nm, 5 ms, 50 Hz for terminal stimulation, 2.5 mW/mm2, for up to 5-s duration). Exiting the center zone immediately terminates the photostimulation. Locations of animals (defined as position of the centroid) were tracked in real-time using a custom program in MATLAB and were used to automatically control Arduino microcontroller in a closed-loop manner.
Modified elevated plus maze.
The EPM apparatus consisted of two open arms (30 × 5 cm) and two enclosed arms (30 × 5 × 30 cm) extending from a central intersection platform (5 × 5 cm). The apparatus was placed 75 cm from the floor. An experiment consists of a 20 min test, which includes four 5-min sessions of alternating laser stimulations (OFF–ON–ON–OFF). Mice were initially placed in the center of the chamber and allowed to freely explore the chamber. When animals enter the open arms, an photostimulation is delivered (ChR2: 473 nm, 20 ms, 20 Hz for soma stimulation or 473 nm, 5 ms, 50 Hz for terminal stimulation, 2.5 mW/mm2, for up to 10-s duration). Exiting the open arms immediately terminates the photostimulation. Locations of animals (defined as position of the centroid) were tracked in real-time using a custom program in MATLAB and were used to automatically control Arduino microcontroller in a closed-loop manner.
Electrophysiological recording in acute brain slices
Mice were anesthetized with isoflurane and decapitated. The brain was rapidly dissected out and transferred to ice-cold oxygenated artificial cerebrospinal fluid (ACSF; 125 mM NaCl, 2.5 mM KCl, 2 mM CaCl2, 1.3 mM NaH2PO4, 1.3 mM MgCl2, 10 mM glucose, and 25 mM NaHCO3). Brain slices containing the MeApd were cut with a vibratome (300 μm; VT1200s, Leica, Germany). Brain slices were transferred to oxygenated artificial cerebrospinal fluid (ACSF) and incubated at 33°C for 1 hr. All chemicals used in slice preparation were purchased from Sigma-Aldrich (St Louis, MO, USA). Slices were then transferred to the recording chamber which was submerged and perfused with ACSF at a rate of 3 ml/min at room temperature. Neurons were identified with differential interference contrast optics (DIC). The recording pipettes (3-5 MΩ) were prepared with a micropipette puller (P1000, Sutter Instrument; USA). For cell attached recordings, the pipettes were filled with an internal solution that contained the following (in mM): 130 K-gluconate, 10 HEPES, 0.6 EGTA, 5 KCl, 3 Na2ATP, 0.3 Na3GTP, 4 MgCl2, and 10 Na2-phosphocreatine (pH 7.2–7.4). The experiments were performed with a computer-controlled amplifier (MultiClamp 700B, National Instruments; USA). The current and voltage signals were low-pass filtered at 3 kHz and digitized at 10 kHz (WinWCP software). 5-s continuous blue light (473 nm) was generated with a digitizer-controlled laser and delivered via an optic fiber cable onto the slice surface. The maximal light intensity reaching the brain tissue was ~10 mW. We analyzed the number of spikes before, during and after light stimulation.
Fiber photometry experiments and data analysis
Photometry experiments were performed as previously described13. Fluorescence signals were acquired with a fiber photometry system (Doric Lenses). The analog voltage signals were digitalized and recorded by a Micro 1401 digitizer (CED, Cambridge, UK) and Spike2 software (version 8.03). The LED power was adjusted at the tip of the optical fiber to 30–50 μW to minimize bleaching. Behaviors were recorded by a video camera (FLIR).
To record neuronal responses of MeApd Vgat+ neurons and their projections to the MPOA during social interactions, subject animals were allowed to freely interact with a novel juvenile for ~20 s. To record neural responses in response to sucrose solution or chocolate, subject animals were allowed to freely consume sucrose solution or chocolate for ~5 minutes. Calcium signals were recorded during the presentation of social or nonsocial stimuli.
To record changes of dopamine signals in the NAc during social interactions (Fig. 4), we sequentially and pseudorandomly presented one male, female, or juvenile mouse for ~10 s, with ~2 min inter-trial interval. We recorded dLight fluorescence at 100 Hz. Time 0 was defined as the moment when subject animals came in close contact with the target animal and begin to investigate and sniff the target animal. Given the brief length (~10 s) of this initial investigation of stimulus animals (which is considered the appetitive phase of interaction), subject animals did not display aggressive or mating behavior, even toward adult conspecifics.
We also recorded dynamics of dopamine signals in the NAc in the social operant task. dLight-expressing animals were trained in the social operant task in the presence or absence of target animals. The behavior paradigm was similar to the procedures used and described in Fig. 1 with a few differences. Here, animals were trained for 7-12 daily sessions with target animals, and the well-trained status was defined as the day when animals reached stable social preference (20% social preference for at least three consecutive days). For animals trained in the presence of a target animal, dLight fluorescence was recorded on days 1 and 4, as well as the day when the animals reached the well-trained status (on day 7-12). On the day after training, an additional session of post-training test was recorded without target animals. For animals trained without target animals, dLight fluorescence was recorded on days 1, 4, and 7, as no animals reached well-trained status.
To record dopamine signals in the NAc during optical stimulation of MeApd Vgat+ neurons, the neuronal activity of the NAc was recorded while delivering laser stimulation (473 nm, 20 Hz, 2.5 mW/mm2, 20 ms pulses, 1 s stimulation duration), with ~ 1 min inter-trial interval. To prevent 473 nm laser stimulation delivered at the MeApd from contaminating fluorescence signals at the NAc, we recorded photometry signals at 1000 Hz. We filtered out data points that were acquired during any laser stimulation pulses (20 ms each at 20 Hz), and only used the data points that are temporally separate from any laser stimulation pulses for further analyses. This allowed us to effectively measure the true fluorescence signals from the NAc without any potential contamination of light signals coming from the laser stimulation at the MeApd.
Photometry data were analyzed using MATLAB. Behavior was scored frame-by-frame and aligned to calcium signals. To measure dynamics of fluorescence intensity immediately before and after the onset of specific behaviors or optogenetic stimulations, (F–F0)/F0 (ΔF/F) was calculated, where F0 was the baseline fluorescence signal averaged over a 1 s window between 5 s and 4 s (Fig. 4c-e; Extended Data Fig. 4), between 3 s and 2 s (Fig. 4h-k and m-o), or between 2 s and 1 s (Fig. 5b, c) prior to the behavior or stimulation onset. ΔF/F values were presented as mean with an SEM envelope. To measure neural activity (i.e. fluorescence changes of GCaMP7f) of MeApd Vgat+ neurons in response to social and non-social stimuli (Extended Data Fig. 4), we calculated the AUC per second between 0 s and 5 s from the behavior onset. To measure dopamine signals (i.e. fluorescence changes of dLight) in the NAc during social interactions (Fig. 4a-e), we calculated the AUC per second between −1 s and 1 s relative to the behavior onset. To measure dopamine signals (i.e. fluorescence changes of dLight) in the NAc during optogenetic stimulation of MeApd Vgat+ neurons (Fig. 5b-e), we calculated the AUCs per second between 0 s and 1 s relative to the stimulation onset and between 0.3 s and 0.6 s following the termination of stimulation.
Histology and Imaging
Animals were sacrificed 4-6 weeks post-injection and perfused with 4% PFA. The brains were dissected out and fixed in 4% PFA for two additional hours at room temperature, rinsed with 1X PBS, and placed in 30% sucrose solution overnight at 4°C. To visualize viral expression and fiber placement, 60 μm sections were cut on a Leica CM1950 cryostat. To examine dLight expression in the NAc, 60 um sections were stained with chicken Anti-GFP antibody (Aves Labs #1020, 1:1000) overnight at 4°C, and with donkey anti-chicken Alexa 488 antibody (Jackson ImmunoResearch #703-545-155, 1:1000) overnight at 4°C. Images were acquired using a confocal microscope (Zeiss LSM 880).
Data analysis and statistics
No statistical methods were used to pre-determine sample sizes, but our sample sizes were selected based on previous experience from related research and literature12,13. Animals were randomly assigned to control and manipulation groups. Data collection and analysis were not performed blind to the conditions of experiments. Animals that exhibited no or little motivation to engage in nose-pokes on the first two days of training (defined as less than 10 total pokes of social and null ports on both days) were excluded from training and data collection. One EYFP-expressing animal in the photometry experiment was excluded based on this criterion. Animals that had low body weight (males <20 g and females <16 g) were excluded from surgeries and experiments. Behavior is analyzed using MATLAB code (https://pdollar.github.io/toolbox/). Figures were plotted using Prism version 9 (GraphPad) or MATLAB 2018b (MathWorks). Data is reported as box plots, dotplots, or mean ± SEM plots. In boxplots, the central mark indicates the median, the bottom and top edges of the box indicate the 25th and 75th percentiles, and the whiskers indicate the 10th and 90th percentiles. Statistical methods used in this study include two-way repeated measures ANOVA, one-way repeated measures ANOVA, one-way ANOVA, Kruskal–Wallis test (one-way ANOVA on ranks), two-sided Mann-Whitney test, two-sided unpaired t test, and two-sided paired t test (see Supplementary Table 1). The data met the assumptions of the statistical tests used. Normality of the data was tested using the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test. Statistically significant differences were established at *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01 and ***P < 0.001; n.s. indicates not significant. All statistic tests are summarized in the Supplementary Table 1.
Life Sciences Reporting Summary
Further information on research design is available in the Nature Research Reporting Summary linked to this article.
Data availability
All data generated and analyzed during this study are either included in this published article or are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.
Code availability
Behavior is analyzed using MATLAB code, available at (https://pdollar.github.io/toolbox/). The code that supports these findings is available upon request from the corresponding author.
Extended Data
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgements
We thank K. Wassum, P. Golshani, L. Kingsbury, T. Raam, D. Wei, L. Gu, F. Sun, and M. Zhang for critical comments on this manuscript and P. Chen, S. Hu, H. Li, Y. Wu, and P. Zhao for technical assistance. This work was supported in part by NIH R01 NS113124, Searle Scholars Award, Klingenstein-Simons Fellowship, Brain Research Foundation grant, Packard Foundation Fellowship, McKnight Scholar Award, Keck Foundation Award, Vallee Scholars Award, and Mallinckrodt Scholar Award to W.H.; Marion Bowen Postdoctoral Grant to R.K.H.
Footnotes
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
References
- 1.Stanley DA & Adolphs R Toward a neural basis for social behavior. Neuron 80, 816–826 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Umberson D, Crosnoe R & Reczek C Social Relationships and Health Behavior Across the Life Course. Annu Rev Sociol 36, 139–157 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Chen P & Hong W Neural Circuit Mechanisms of Social Behavior. Neuron 98, 16–30 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Tamir DI & Hughes BL Social Rewards: From Basic Social Building Blocks to Complex Social Behavior. Perspect Psychol Sci 13, 700–717 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Hu H Reward and Aversion. Annu Rev Neurosci 39, 297–324 (2016). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Chevallier C, Kohls G, Troiani V, Brodkin ES & Schultz RT The social motivation theory of autism. Trends Cogn Sci 16, 231–239 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Zeeland AAS-V, Dapretto M, Ghahremani DG, Poldrack RA & Bookheimer SY Reward processing in autism. Autism Res Official J Int Soc Autism Res 3, 53–67 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Nieh EH et al. Inhibitory Input from the Lateral Hypothalamus to the Ventral Tegmental Area Disinhibits Dopamine Neurons and Promotes Behavioral Activation. Neuron 90, 1286–98 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Hung LW et al. Gating of social reward by oxytocin in the ventral tegmental area. Science 357, 1406–1411 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Dölen G, Darvishzadeh A, Huang KW & Malenka RC Social reward requires coordinated activity of nucleus accumbens oxytocin and serotonin. Nature 501, 179 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.McHenry JA et al. Hormonal gain control of a medial preoptic area social reward circuit. Nat Neurosci 20, 449–458 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Hong W, Kim D-W & Anderson DJ Antagonistic Control of Social versus Repetitive Self-Grooming Behaviors by Separable Amygdala Neuronal Subsets. Cell 158, 1348–1361 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Chen PB et al. Sexually Dimorphic Control of Parenting Behavior by the Medial Amygdala. Cell 176, 1206–1221.e18 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Lin D et al. Functional identification of an aggression locus in the mouse hypothalamus. Nature 470, 221–226 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Wu Z, Autry AE, Bergan JF, Watabe-Uchida M & Dulac CG Galanin neurons in the medial preoptic area govern parental behaviour. Nature 509, 325 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Falkner AL, Grosenick L, Davidson TJ, Deisseroth K & Lin D Hypothalamic control of male aggression-seeking behavior. Nat Neurosci 19, 596–604 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Miller SM, Marcotulli D, Shen A & Zweifel LS Divergent medial amygdala projections regulate approach-avoidance conflict behavior. Nat Neurosci 22, 565–575 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Gutzeit VA et al. Optogenetic reactivation of prefrontal social neural ensembles mimics social buffering of fear. Neuropsychopharmacol Official Publ Am Coll Neuropsychopharmacol 45, 1068–1077 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Swanson LW Cerebral hemisphere regulation of motivated behavior. Brain research 886, 113 164 (2000). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Choi GB et al. Lhx6 delineates a pathway mediating innate reproductive behaviors from the amygdala to the hypothalamus. Neuron 46, 647 660 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Li Y et al. Neuronal Representation of Social Information in the Medial Amygdala of Awake Behaving Mice. Cell 171, 1176 1190 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Bergan JF, Ben-Shaul Y & Dulac C Sex-specific processing of social cues in the medial amygdala. Elife 3, (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Ishii KK et al. A Labeled-Line Neural Circuit for Pheromone-Mediated Sexual Behaviors in Mice. Neuron 95, 123–137 (2017). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Unger EK et al. Medial amygdalar aromatase neurons regulate aggression in both sexes. Cell Reports 10, 453–462 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Yao S, Bergan J, Lanjuin A & Dulac C Oxytocin signaling in the medial amygdala is required for sex discrimination of social cues. Elife 6, 49 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Demir E et al. The pheromone darcin drives a circuit for innate and reinforced behaviours. Nature 578, 137–141 (2020). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Golden SA et al. Compulsive Addiction-like Aggressive Behavior in Mice. Biol Psychiat 82, 239–248 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Venniro M et al. Volitional social interaction prevents drug addiction in rat models. Nat Neurosci 21, 1520 1529 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Martin L & Iceberg E Quantifying Social Motivation in Mice Using Operant Conditioning. J Vis Exp Jove e53009 (2015) doi: 10.3791/53009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Jacobs S, Huang F, Tsien J & Wei W Social Recognition Memory Test in Rodents. Bioprotocol 6, (2016). [Google Scholar]
- 31.Hlinák Z & Krejcí I Social recognition in male rats: age differences and modulation by MIF-I and Alaptide. Physiological Res Acad Sci Bohemoslovaca 40, 59–67 (1991). [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Berke JD What does dopamine mean? Nat Neurosci 21, 787–793 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Volkow ND, Wise RA & Baler R The dopamine motive system: implications for drug and food addiction. Nat Rev Neurosci 18, 741–752 (2017). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Fang Y-Y, Yamaguchi T, Song SC, Tritsch NX & Lin D A Hypothalamic Midbrain Pathway Essential for Driving Maternal Behaviors. Neuron 98, 192 207.e10 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Kohl J et al. Functional circuit architecture underlying parental behaviour. Nature 556, 326 331 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Hennessy MB, Kaiser S & Sachser N Social buffering of the stress response: diversity, mechanisms, and functions. Front Neuroendocrin 30, 470–82 (2009). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Nardou R et al. Oxytocin-dependent reopening of a social reward learning critical period with MDMA. Nature 569, 116–120 (2019). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Schultz W Dopamine reward prediction-error signalling: a two-component response. Nat Rev Neurosci 17, 183–195 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Collins AL et al. Dynamic mesolimbic dopamine signaling during action sequence learning and expectation violation. Sci Rep-uk 6, 20231 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Prévost-Solié C, Girard B, Righetti B, Tapparel M & Bellone C Dopamine neurons of the VTA encode active conspecific interaction and promote social learning through social reward prediction error. Biorxiv 2020.05.27.118851 (2020) doi: 10.1101/2020.05.27.118851. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Padilla SL et al. Agouti-related peptide neural circuits mediate adaptive behaviors in the starved state. Nat Neurosci 19, 734 741 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Berke JD & Hyman SE Addiction, Dopamine, and the Molecular Mechanisms of Memory. Neuron 25, 515–532 (2000). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Golden SA et al. Basal forebrain projections to the lateral habenula modulate aggression reward. Nature 534, 688 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Beny-Shefer Y et al. Nucleus Accumbens Dopamine Signaling Regulates Sexual Preference for Females in Male Mice. Cell Reports 21, 3079–3088 (2017). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Kingsbury L et al. Cortical Representations of Conspecific Sex Shape Social Behavior. Neuron 107, 941–953.e7 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Wu YE, Pan L, Zuo Y, Li X & Hong W Detecting Activated Cell Populations Using Single-Cell RNA-Seq. Neuron 96, 313–329 (2017). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Kupferberg A, Bicks L & Hasler G Social functioning in major depressive disorder. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 69, 313–332 (2016). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Matthews GA et al. Dorsal Raphe Dopamine Neurons Represent the Experience of Social Isolation. Cell 164, 617–631 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Vong L et al. Leptin Action on GABAergic Neurons Prevents Obesity and Reduces Inhibitory Tone to POMC Neurons. Neuron 71, 142 154 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Yang CF et al. Sexually dimorphic neurons in the ventromedial hypothalamus govern mating in both sexes and aggression in males. Cell 153, 896 909 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Mahn M et al. High-efficiency optogenetic silencing with soma-targeted anion-conducting channelrhodopsins. Nat Commun 9, 4125 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Dana H et al. High-performance calcium sensors for imaging activity in neuronal populations and microcompartments. Nat Methods 16, 649–657 (2019). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Broussard GJ et al. In vivo measurement of afferent activity with axon-specific calcium imaging. Nat Neurosci 21, 1272–1280 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Chan KY et al. Engineered AAVs for efficient noninvasive gene delivery to the central and peripheral nervous systems. Nat Neurosci 20, 1172–1179 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Patriarchi T et al. Ultrafast neuronal imaging of dopamine dynamics with designed genetically encoded sensors. Science 360, eaat4422 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Dimidschstein J et al. A viral strategy for targeting and manipulating interneurons across vertebrate species. Nat Neurosci 19, 1743–1749 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
All data generated and analyzed during this study are either included in this published article or are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.