Skip to main content
Cerebral Cortex (New York, NY) logoLink to Cerebral Cortex (New York, NY)
. 2021 Apr 7;31(8):3856–3871. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhab054

Distinct Regionalization Patterns of Cortical Morphology are Associated with Cognitive Performance Across Different Domains

C E Palmer 1,, W Zhao 2, R Loughnan 2, J Zou 3, C C Fan 1,4, W K Thompson 3, A M Dale 2,5,6,7, T L Jernigan 1,2,5,7
PMCID: PMC8258441  PMID: 33825852

Abstract

Cognitive performance in children is predictive of academic and social outcomes; therefore, understanding neurobiological mechanisms underlying individual differences in cognition during development may be important for improving quality of life. The belief that a single, psychological construct underlies many cognitive processes is pervasive throughout society. However, it is unclear if there is a consistent neural substrate underlying many cognitive processes. Here, we show that a distributed configuration of cortical surface area and apparent thickness, when controlling for global imaging measures, is differentially associated with cognitive performance on different types of tasks in a large sample (N = 10 145) of 9–11-year-old children from the Adolescent Brain and Cognitive DevelopmentSM (ABCD) study. The minimal overlap in these regionalization patterns of association has implications for competing theories about developing intellectual functions. Surprisingly, not controlling for sociodemographic factors increased the similarity between these regionalization patterns. This highlights the importance of understanding the shared variance between sociodemographic factors, cognition and brain structure, particularly with a population-based sample such as ABCD.

Keywords: adolescence, cognition, cortical morphology, development, multivariate, neuroimaging

Introduction

Two-factor theories of intellectual development have divided cognitive function into two broad components: fluid abilities refer to one’s propensity to solve problems, reason, act quickly, and adapt to novel situations; whereas, crystallized abilities encompass task-specific knowledge that accrues throughout the lifespan (Horn and Cattell 1966; Deary 2012). Despite these constructs being seemingly dissociable, performance across different tasks probing these cognitive processes is often correlated (Spearman 1904; Wechsler 1946; Akshoomoff et al. 2013). The latent factor explaining this shared variance, termed “g,” is heritable (Bouchard and McGue 1981; Gottfredson and Deary 2004; Plomin and Spinath 2004; Panizzon et al. 2014) and predictive of social and academic outcomes (Gottfredson and Deary 2004; Arendt and Nielsen 2005; Cutler et al. 2012). A pervasive view in society is that “g” represents a single causal underlying trait that influences performance across multiple domains. Although genome-wide-association-studies (GWAS) demonstrate that cognitive phenotypes, including “g,” can be associated with specific genetic variants (Davies et al. 2018; Savage et al. 2018), some of which are also associated with brain structure (Grasby et al. 2020; Lett et al. 2020), these associations are highly polygenic, and though the mediating pathway from genetics to cognition is still poorly understood, there is little support for a causal pathway associated with a unitary neural correlate. Cognitive development is highly complex and some models of intellectual development have posited that correlations in performance across multiple tasks, the so-called “positive manifold,” may emerge via positive interactions among developing cognitive systems, which may have distinct neural substrates (Van Der Maas et al. 2006, 2017; Hampshire et al. 2012). This framework is difficult to test empirically; however, one prediction from this mutualism model would be that individual differences in cognitive performance would be associated with different distributions of neural phenotypes across individuals.

Previous studies have reported associations of general cognitive ability with larger brain volume and distributed structural and functional neural correlates particularly across frontal and parietal regions (Duncan et al. 2000; Colom et al. 2009, 2013; Gläscher et al. 2010; Basten et al. 2015). Cortical volume, as measured with magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), is comprised of two genetically and developmentally distinct components, surface area (CSA) and apparent thickness (CTH) (Panizzon et al. 2009; Winkler et al. 2010; Jernigan et al. 2011), which have shown different associations with individual variability in cognition. Positive associations with cortical surface area (CSA) and cognition have been consistently reported (Schnack et al. 2015; Walhovd et al. 2016; Schmitt et al. 2019; Grasby et al. 2020); however, associations between cortical thickness (CTH) and cognition have been less consistent (Sowell et al. 2004; Shaw et al. 2006; Brouwer et al. 2014; Burgaleta et al. 2014). This is likely due to limited statistical power to identify replicable associations, differences in neuroimaging processing protocols and cognitive assessments used to estimate general cognitive ability, and the age of the participants. Few studies have measured individual differences in the regionalization of cortical morphology (i.e., the relative increase or decrease in CTH or CSA within a particular region relative to mean CTH or total CSA, respectively). Complex patterns of positive and negative associations of relative CSA and CTH have been associated with general cognitive function (Fjell et al. 2015; Vuoksimaa et al. 2016; Reardon et al. 2018), as well as with more specific measures of cognitive performance (Fjell et al. 2012; Newman, Jernigan, et al. 2016a; Newman, Thompson, et al. 2016b; Curley et al. 2018). However, many previous studies have been underpowered to detect significant effects of relative cortical configuration across the whole cortex particularly when using univariate statistical methods with stringent control for multiple comparisons (Vuoksimaa et al. 2016). However, we know that during embryonic development, the relative areal expanse associated with different functional regions occurs via the graded expression of transcription factors across the cortical plate (O’Leary et al. 2007; Rakic et al. 2009). Individual variability in this patterning could therefore result in subtle changes to the whole configuration of the cortex, which may in turn influence cognitive development.

In the current study, we used a multivariate analysis to measure the association between the regionalization of CSA and apparent CTH in a large sample (n = 10 145) of 9–11-year-old children from the Adolescent Brain and Cognitive DevelopmentSM (ABCD) study. This large-scale study of 11 880 9 and 10-year-old children, uses neuroimaging, genetics, and a multidimensional battery of assessments to investigate the role of various biological, environmental, and behavioral factors in brain, cognitive, and social/emotional development. Given the biology of cortical development and growing evidence that models encompassing distributed behavioral associations across the brain better predict behavioral outcomes (Reddan et al. 2017; van der Meer et al. 2020; Zhao et al. 2020), we used a multivariate approach to assess the significance of the effects of the brain phenotypes on behavior when aggregated across all cortical vertices. We subsequently examined the degree to which the distributed cortical patterns associating with cognitive performance measured with the fluid and crystallized composite scores from the NIH Toolbox exhibited effects of a common underlying cortical architecture or distinct patterns of cortical regionalization. These composite scores generated from the toolbox have been validated against gold standard measures of intelligence in both adults and children (Akshoomoff et al. 2013; Hodes et al. 2013; Heaton et al. 2014).

The ABCD study sample was recruited to resemble the population of the United States of America as closely as possible; therefore, the participants are from diverse racial, ethnic, and socioeconomic backgrounds. Confounding associations between these sociodemographic variables, brain structure and cognitive performance complicate the interpretation of model results that control for demographic covariates. We have therefore performed our analyses with and without controlling for these important sociodemographic variables in order to highlight the implications of this for the interpretation of our findings. This is an important aspect of analyzing individual differences in large population-based samples such as the ABCD study.

Materials and Methods

Sample

The ABCD study is a longitudinal study across 21 data acquisition sites enrolling 11 880 children starting at 9–11-years old. This paper analyzed the baseline sample from release 2.0.1 (NDAR DOI: 10.15154/1504041). The ABCD study used school-based recruitment strategies to create a population-based, demographically diverse sample, however, it is not necessarily representative of the US national population (Garavan et al. 2018; Compton et al. 2019). Due to the inclusion of a wide range of individuals across different races, ethnicities and socioeconomic backgrounds, it is important to carefully consider how to control for these potentially confounding factors and the implications of this on our effects of interest. Sex and age were used as covariates in all analyses. Only subjects who had complete data across all of the measures analyzed were included in the neuroimaging analyses creating a final sample of 10 145 subjects. Additional sample details can be found in the Supplementary Material. Supplementary Table 1 displays the names of each variable used in these analyses from data release 2.0.1. Supplementary Table 2 shows the demographic characteristics of the sample.

Neurocognitive Assessment

The ABCD study neurocognitive assessment at baseline consisted of the NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery (NIHTBXCB), the WISC-V matrix reasoning task, the Little Man Task (LMT), and the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning task (RAVLT). All of the tasks were administered using an iPad with support or scoring from a research assistant where needed. The NIHTBXCB is a widely used battery of cognitive tests that measures a range of different cognitive domains. It includes the Toolbox Oral Reading RecognitionTask (TORRT), Toolbox Picture Vocabulary Task (TPVT), the Toolbox Pattern Comparison Processing Speed Test (TPCPST), Toolbox List Sorting Working Memory Test (TLSWMT), Toolbox Picture Sequence Memory Test (TPSMT), Toolbox Flanker Task (TFT), and Toolbox Dimensional Change Card Sort Task (TDCCS). Details of each task can be found in Supplementary Materials. In the current study, the uncorrected scores for each task were used for statistical analyses. Composite scores estimating crystallized intelligence (mean of TPVT and TORRT), fluid intelligence (mean of TPCPST, TLSWMT, TPSMT, TFT, and TDCCS), and a total cognition score (mean of all tasks) are also provided by the NIHTBCB (https://www.healthmeasures.net/explore-measurement-systems/nih-toolbox/intro-to-nih-toolbox/cognition) and were also analyzed. These measures have been validated against “gold standard” measures of general cognitive ability, and the constructs referred to as crystallized and fluid intelligence in adults (Heaton et al. 2014) and children (Akshoomoff et al. 2013). To address remaining concern that the total composite score may not adequately reflect estimates of “g” as a global latent factor, we conducted a principal component analysis (PCA) to generate a latent factor for “g,” including all of the tasks administered in the ABCD neurocognition battery.

MRI Acquisition and Image Preprocessing

The ABCD MRI data were collected across 21 research sites using Siemens Prisma, GE 750 and Philips 3T scanners. Scanning protocols were harmonized across sites. Scanner ID was included in all analyses to control for any differences in image acquisition across sites and scanners. Full details of all the imaging acquisition protocols used in ABCD are outlined by Casey et al. (2018). Preprocessing of all MRI data for ABCD was conducted using in-house software at the Center for Multimodal Imaging and Genetics (CMIG) at University of California San Diego (UCSD) as outlined in Hagler et al. (2019). Cortical surfaces were constructed from T1-weighted structural images for each subject and segmented to calculate measures of apparent cortical thickness and surface area using FreeSurfer v5.3.0 (Dale et al. 1999; Fischl et al. 1999, 2004; Fischl and Dale 2000; Jovicich et al. 2006). Cortical maps were smoothed using a Gaussian kernel of 20 mm full-width half maximum (FWHM) and mapped into standardized spherical atlas space. Vertex-wise data for all subjects for each morphometric measurement were concatenated into matrices in MATLAB R2017a and entered into general linear models (GLM) for statistical analysis using custom written code. More details can be found in the Supplementary Materials.

Statistical Analysis

Behavioral Analysis

All behavioral variables were standardized (z-scored) prior to analysis. Linear mixed effects models were used to residualize all behavioral variables for age, sex, and a random effect of family ID prior to pairwise analysis. Partial pairwise Pearson’s correlations were conducted to determine associations between all of the cognitive tasks. This analysis was then repeated additionally residualizing all behavioral variables by race/ethnicity, household income, and highest parental education to determine the impact of controlling for these sociodemographic factors on the pairwise associations. We additionally ran a PCA across all single cognitive tasks. A scree plot of eigenvalues demonstrated that the first unrotated principal component (PC1) explained substantially more variance across the tasks than the subsequent components. The loadings of each single task with PC1 are shown in Supplementary Figure 1 alongside the scree plot. PC1 encompasses the shared variance across cognitive tasks, which is often described in the literature as an estimate of “g”: general cognitive ability. This was also included in the correlation matrices.

Vertex-wise Effect Size Maps

Vertex-wise data were standardized (z-scored) prior to analysis. We applied a GLM univariately at each vertex (n = 1284) associating a given behavior from a set of covariates (age, sex, scanner ID, race/ethnicity, household income, and parental education) and the vertex-wise morphology data. Mass univariate standardized beta surface maps were created showing the vertex-wise associations for each analysis. Vertex-wise effect size maps were calculated for all of the 13 cognitive measures predicted by relative CSA (controlling for total CSA) and relative CTH (controlling for mean CTH) separately resulting in N = 26 independent vertex-wise analyses. Models determining the association between CSA and behavior included total CSA (sum of CSA across vertices) as an additional predictor and models analyzing CTH included the mean CTH across vertices. These analyses were repeated with and without including race/ethnicity, income, and parental education covariates.

Determining the Significance of the Effect Size Maps Using an Omnibus Test

In order to determine whether there was a significant association between vertex-wise cortical morphology (controlling for global measures) and cognitive task performance, we employed a permutation test using a univariate statistic (min-P) and three multivariate statistics (MOSTest, Stouffer, and Fisher). Using the least-squares estimates Inline graphic from each GLM at each vertex, we computed the Inline graphicvector of Wald statistics Inline graphicacross the whole cortex and accompanying P-values: Inline graphic. Using these values, we calculated four different test statistics for the permutation test outlined in Table 1.

Table 1.

Univariate and multivariate test statistics measured

Univariate Tippett (Min-P) Inline graphic
Multivariate MOSTest Inline graphic
Multivariate Fisher Inline graphic
Multivariate Stouffer Inline graphic

The univariate statistic, min-P was defined as the most significant (smallest) P-value across the cortex. This is a commonly used neuroimaging omnibus test, but does not take into account the distributed nature of many brain-behavior associations. The multivariate statistical omnibus test (MOSTest) (Shadrin et al. 2020; van der Meer et al. 2020) is a more pertinent omnibus test, as it determines the significance of the whole pattern of effects across the cortex taking into account the covariance structure across vertices. Inline graphic was calculated as the estimated squared Mahalanobis norm of Inline graphic. The Fisher and Stouffer methods are additional alternative multivariate methods for aggregating effects across the cortex, but do not take into account the covariance across vertices. These are likely less optimal statistics, but do not rely on the regularization of a high-dimensional matrix, therefore, were included to show convergence of results across multiple methods. Extended methodological details can be found in the Supplementary Materials.

To correctly estimate the null distribution from permutations, subject labels were shuffled according to exchangeability blocks (EBs) defined based on the family structure within ABCD (Winkler et al. 2015). This permutation procedure was used to determine the distribution of each test statistic under the global null hypothesis H0. We rejected H0 if the observed test statistic was greater than the value of the permuted test statistic at the critical threshold corresponding to an alpha level of 0.0038 (0.05 corrected for the 13 cognitive tests analyzed).

Comparison Across Associations Maps

To determine the shared variance between the effect size maps for the fluid (F) and crystallized (C) composite scores for each imaging phenotype, additional maps were created controlling for the other composite measure by including that measure as an additional covariate within the design matrix. This produced a vertex-wise standardized beta effect size surface map for F independent of the association between cortical morphology and C (FC) and C independent of F (CF). Surface maps of the difference between the vertex-wise beta coefficients as well as vertex-wise Pearson’s correlation coefficients were calculated for the following contrasts: F – C, F – FC, and C – CF. The magnitude of effects across the cortex for each map was calculated using the following formula: Inline graphic. In order to determine the relative size of the effects across the difference maps compared with the individual maps, we calculated a root mean square (RMS) ratio using the following formula: Inline graphic, with Inline graphic representing the RMS of the difference map and RMS1 and RMS2 as the RMS for each individual map used to produce the difference.

Quantifying the Magnitude of the Association Between Brain Structure and Cognition Using a Mass Univariate Polyvertex Score

Univariate polyvertex score (PVSU) were calculated for the fluid and crystallized composite scores to quantify the behavioral variance explained by the vertex-wise cortical morphology. The method used here was the mass univariate PVSU method outlined in detail by Zhao and colleagues (Zhao et al. 2020). In contrast to the Bayesian-PVS highlighted in their paper, the mass univariate PVSU method does not take into account the covariance structure of the imaging phenotype across vertices. For vertex-wise structural imaging measures, the Bayesian-PVS does not increase predictive power (unlike for functional MRI), therefore, for parsimony, the mass-univariate method was used here. PVSU were calculated within a 10-fold cross-validation framework, such that each model was trained on 90% of the sample and scores then calculated by predicting the behavior using the estimated model in the remaining 10% until a PVSU was calculated for each subject. All behavioral and imaging data were preresidualized using the covariates of no interest within each cross-validation fold prior to model estimation. PVSU were calculated with and without controlling for sociodemographic variables. Global CSA and CTH measures specific to each modality were included in the preresidualization as covariates. This allowed us to determine the unique association between relative cortical morphology and cognition and compare this with the predictive power of global measures. The association between the relative imaging phenotype and behavior across the whole sample was calculated as the squared correlation (R2) between the observed behavior and the predicted behavior (the PVSU).

In order to explore the proportion of shared variability in cognitive performance explained by brain structure and the sociodemographic variables, we generated several linear models with differing predictors. These models were generated separately for each imaging modality (when these were included) and with either the fluid or the crystallized scores as the dependent variable. Each model was trained on 90% of the sample and tested in a 10% hold-out set within a 10-fold cross-validation framework to produce a robust, out-of-sample R2. The models are outlined in Figure 7.

Figure 6 .


Figure 6

Controlling for sociodemographic factors reduces the variance in cognitive performance predicted by cortical morphology. These bar charts show the percentage R2 in the fluid (left column) and crystallized (right column) composite scores predicted by several models including sociodemographic and brain measures. Model 1 shows the total variance in behavior explained by the full model including the covariates of no interest (age, sex, and scanner), the sociodemographic variables (parental education, household income, and race/ethnicity) and either the regionalization of CTH and mean CTH (A; top row) or the regionalization of CSA and total CSA (B; bottom row). Model 2 shows the unique variance in behavior explained by the sociodemographic factors after preresidualizing both the dependent (DV) and independent (IV) variables for the covariates of no interest. Additionally, preresidualizing for the imaging phenotypes (model 3) lead to a reduction in this R2. Conversely, model 4 shows the unique variance in behavior explained by the structural measures (global and regionalization) after preresidualizing for the covariates of no interest only. Additionally, preresidualizing for the sociodemographic factors (model 5) lead to a large decrease in the variance explained by these structural measures, again showing the shared variance between brain structure, cognition, and sociodemographic factors. Controlling for the sociodemographic factors reduced the variance explained in the composite scores by ~4–5-fold for CSA and ~4–8-fold for CTH.

Additional models were run to estimate the out of sample variance in PC1 predicted by the relative and global imaging variables as well as the fluid and crystallized PVSU in order to understand the overlap in variance across these behavioral measures. Outside of the cross-validation framework, partial correlation coefficients were estimated across the whole sample by preresidualizing both the dependent and independent variables by the covariates of no interest and/or sociodemographic variables and then correlating the residuals. All out-of-sample R2 estimates and partial correlation coefficients can be found in Supplementary Table 3.

Results

Behavioral Data

Figure 1A displays pairwise Pearson’s correlation coefficients describing the phenotypic relationship between all of the cognitive tasks measured at baseline in the ABCD study, the composite scores and the latent g-factor (PC1) controlling for age, sex, and a random effect of family ID. As expected, performance across all tasks was positively correlated. Reading recognition and picture vocabulary performance were most highly correlated with each other (out of the single task measures; r = 0.45). These scores were averaged to produce the crystallized composite score, therefore, show a high correlation with this measure (r = 0.82–0.87). The Toolbox measures used to produce the fluid composite score (flanker, dimensional card sorting, pattern processing speed, picture sequence, and list working memory) were less correlated with each other (r = 0.13–0.39), therefore, showed slightly lower correlations with the fluid composite measure compared with the tasks contributing to the crystallized composite score (r = 0.59–0.68). The total composite score (mean of fluid and crystallized) was more highly correlated with the fluid measure (r = 0.90) compared with the crystallized measure (r = 0.73). The matrix reasoning task showed similar correlations across the composite scores (r = 0.36–0.43), which were lower than expected given that matrix reasoning is often used as a measure of fluid intelligence. As expected, the RAVLT showed the highest correlations with the verbal and memory related tasks from the NIH Toolbox. The LMT showed low correlations across all the cognitive tasks (r = 0.12–0.22). PC1 derived from all single cognitive tasks showed a strong correlation with the total composite score from the NIH Toolbox as expected (r = 0.95) and similar correlations with all of the other tasks as the total composite score.

Figure 1 .


Figure 1

Phenotypic correlations between all of the cognitive measures at baseline in ABCD. (A) Partial Pearson’s correlation coefficients for all cognitive tasks, composite scores, and the latent factor (PC1) controlling for age, sex, and a random effect of family. (B) Correlations after additionally controlling for sociodemographic covariates (race/ethnicity, household income, parental education). Cognitive performance was preresidualized for all covariates and then correlated using a Pearson’s correlation. Darker colors indicate a higher correlation coefficient. The composite scores and PC1 are highlighted in bold. The other cognitive measures are single tasks. Correlation coefficients among the single task measures decreased after controlling for the sociodemographic variables demonstrating that these additional measures explained a proportion of the variance in cognitive performance among these tasks.

Figure 1B shows the same correlation matrix after additionally preresidualizing all of the cognitive measures for sociodemographic measures (race/ethnicity, household income, and parental education) in order to show the partial phenotypic correlations after controlling for these confounding factors. There was a slight decrease across the correlation coefficients; however, the overall pattern of these relationships remained consistent.

Determining the Association Between the Regionalization of Cortical Morphology and Cognition

We used a MOSTest (Shadrin et al. 2020; van der Meer et al. 2020) to measure the association between the regionalization of CSA and CTH and individual differences in cognitive performance on the fluid and crystallized composite scores from the NIH Toolbox. This multivariate approach aggregates effects across the entire cortex, and therefore is better able to detect associations that are distributed compared with a standard univariate neuroimaging omnibus test that assumes effects are sparse and localized. The MOSTest also takes into account the covariance across the brain. This statistical procedure implements a permutation test with 10 000 permutations to determine statistical significance. For all associations, the observed multivariate statistic fell beyond the null distribution of permuted statistics (P < 0.0001; Table 2). To calculate a more precise P-value for the associations, we extrapolated beyond the null distribution (see Supplementary Methods). All associations were also significant when using more widely used univariate omnibus statistics (Min-P; P < 0.0005), but were smaller in magnitude than when using the MOSTest. This suggests that the distributed signal across the cortex may be important for predicting cognitive performance. The regionalization of CTH showed greater associations with cognitive performance than CSA. All of these analyses controlled for all sociodemographic factors (race/ethnicity, household income, and parental education) as well as the global parameter (total CSA or mean CTH), age, sex, and scanner ID. In addition, we measured all associations using two alternative multivariate statistics, Stouffer and Fisher, to determine method invariance of these brain-behavior associations. All of these methods converged (Supplementary Fig. 2).

Table 2.

Cortical morphology was significantly associated with both fluid and crystallized composite scores

Permuted P-values Extrapolated P-values
Min-P MOST Min-P MOST
CSA ~ fluid score + covariates 5.00E-04 1.00E-04 1.29E-04 2.93E-10
CSA ~ crystallized score + covariates 1.00E-04 1.00E-04 9.34E-06 1.53E-07
CTH ~ fluid composite score + covariates 1.00E-04 1.00E-04 3.96E-05 5.59E-19
CTH ~ crystallized score + covariates 2.00E-04 1.00E-04 4.13E-08 8.71E-18

Notes: Permuted P-values demonstrate statistical significance based on the rank of the observed statistic in the distribution of permuted statistics. For all associations, both the observed univariate (min-P) and multivariate (MOST) test statistics were in the extreme of the respective null distributions, therefore, we rejected the null hypothesis that the observed statistics came from the empirical null distribution. Extrapolated P-values provide an estimate of the likelihood of the observed statistics beyond the range that can be directly estimated from the permutations. The magnitude of effects was larger using the multivariate test statistic and for associations between cognitive performance and CTH (controlling for mean CTH)

Distinct Patterns of Association Between the Regionalization of CSA and the Fluid and Crystallized Composite Scores

After establishing significant associations between cortical morphology and cognitive performance, we aimed to measure the similarity across the estimated effect size maps for these associations. Interestingly, on visualizing the estimated effect size maps of the associations between the regionalization of CSA (controlling for total CSA) and the fluid and crystallized composite scores, we saw an unique structural pattern of association for each composite score (Fig. 2A,B). Indeed, similarity maps demonstrated very little overlap between these vertex-wise associations (Supplementary Fig. 4AC). Figure 2C shows the difference between the beta coefficients for the fluid (F) and crystallized (C) surface maps. To quantify the magnitude of these vertex-wise differences relative to the original associations, we calculated a ratio of the variance (root-mean squared) of the beta coefficient differences divided by the variance (root-mean squared) of the average beta coefficients across the F and C estimated effect size maps (RMS ratio for F – C = 1.21).

Figure 2 .


Figure 2

Distinct estimated effect size maps of association between the regionalization of cortical morphology and fluid and crystallized composite scores. All maps display the vertex-wise mass univariate standardized beta coefficients unthresholded for CSA (left) and CTH (right). A+D) The association between relative CSA and the fluid composite score and (B+G) the crystallized composite score. C) The difference in standardized beta coefficients between A+B. E) The association between relative CSA and the fluid composite score controlling for the crystallized composite score. F) The difference in standardized beta coefficients between D+E. H) The association between relative CSA and the crystallized composite score controlling for the fluid composite score. I) The difference in standardized beta coefficients between G+H. J+M) The association between relative CTH and the fluid composite score and (K+P) the crystallized composite score. L) The difference in standardized beta coefficients between J+K. N) The association between relative CTH and the fluid composite score controlling for the crystallized composite score. O) The difference in standardized beta coefficients between M+N. Q) The association between relative CTH and the crystallized composite score controlling for fluid composite score. R) The difference in standardized beta coefficients between P+Q. The estimated effect size maps showing the association between cortical morphology and the fluid and crystallized composite scores have distinct patterns. These behavioral measures show very little overlapping variance with the regionalization of CSA and CTH.

In order to determine the unique pattern of association between the relative configuration of CSA and the fluid and crystallized composite scores, we generated a vertex-wise association map between the regionalization of CSA and the fluid composite score controlling for the crystallized composite score (FC; Fig. 2E), and the crystallized composite score controlling for the fluid composite score (CF; Fig. 2H). The pattern of associations was almost identical to that obtained when the respective composite score was not included as a covariate in the model. Indeed, the surface map of the vertex-wise “differences” between the associations for each composite score and that composite score controlling for the other were very small in magnitude compared with the original F − C effect size maps (Fig. 2F,I; RMS ratio for C – CF = 0.27; RMS ratio for F – FC = 0.38).

Moreover, the vertex-wise correlation between the estimated beta coefficients for F versus FC was high (r = 0.92) as was the correlation between the beta coefficients for C versus CF (r = 0.95). In contrast, the vertex-wise correlation between the beta coefficients for F versus C was much lower (r = 0.30). This further implies that there was minimal shared variance between the associations for the fluid and crystallized scores and the regionalization of CSA. The minimal overlap in the cortical configuration associated with these measures supports that the configurations of relative CSA associated with these measures were relatively distinct.

Distinct Patterns of Association Between the Regionalization of CTH and the Fluid and Crystallized Composite Scores

Estimated effect size maps showing the vertex-wise associations between the relative configuration of CTH and the fluid and crystallized composite scores are shown in Figure 2J,K. The structural pattern of association between these composite scores and CTH were more similar than with CSA, particularly on the medial surface and similarity maps highlighting overlap in (same direction) associations showed a moderate degree of overlap (Supplementary Fig. 4D,F). However, there were key regions with distinct differences in relative CTH associations between the fluid and crystallized composite scores. Figure 2L shows the difference between the beta coefficients for the F and C surface maps, which were relatively large compared with the original estimated effect sizes (RMS ratio for F – C = 0.92). In order to determine the unique pattern of association between the regionalization of CTH and F and C, we generated a vertex-wise map of the association between the regionalization of CTH and FC (Fig. 2N) and CTH and CF (Fig. 2Q). As with CSA, the pattern of associations was almost identical to when the respective intelligence measure was not included in the model. Indeed, the estimated effect size map of the vertex-wise differences between the associations for each composite score and each composite score controlling for the other were very small in magnitude (Fig. 2O,R; RMS ratio for C – CF = 0.31; RMS ratio for F – FC = 0.30). The vertex-wise correlations between the map of associations for each composite score and that composite score controlling for the other were high (F vs. FC: r = 0.94; C vs. CF: r = 0.95); whereas the vertex-wise correlation between the maps of association for the fluid and crystallized composite scores was much lower (F vs. C: r = 0.40). The minimal overlap in the cortical configurations associated with these measures supports that the configurations of CTH associated with these measures were relatively distinct.

Distinct Patterns of Association for the Regionalization of CTH and CSA Across Cognitive Tasks

Visualizing the effect size maps between each cortical measure and each cognitive task revealed relatively distinct patterns of association across the cognitive tasks (Fig. 3). This was further supported by the low vertex-wise correlations across these associations (Fig. 4). The estimated beta coefficients for the regionalization of CTH and cognition were more correlated across tasks than for the regionalization of CSA and cognition. Effect size maps were more similar for tasks using similar underlying cognitive processes and the composite measures appeared to reflect mixtures of the patterns of association for each of the tasks that were averaged to produce the composite scores. Estimated effect size maps for each cortical measure and each cognitive task and composite score without controlling for the sociodemographic variables of race/ethnicity, household income, and parental education were more highly correlated (Fig. 4C,D). This shows that variance in the sociodemographic factors is shared with the cognitive and structural measures. The association maps for PC1 and the total composite score from the NIH Toolbox were remarkably similar further validating the total composite score as a measure of general cognitive ability.

Figure 3 .


Figure 3

Distinct patterns of association for the regionalization of CTH and CSA across cognitive tasks. Estimated effect size maps showing the mass univariate standardized beta coefficients for the association between each cognitive task and (A) the regionalization of CSA and (B) the regionalization of CTH. Performance on all of the cognitive tasks and the composite scores showed significant associations with the regionalization of cortical morphology across the whole cortical surface using the MOSTest as shown in Palmer et al. (2019).

Figure 4 .


Figure 4

Similarity of regionalization association patterns across cognitive tasks is modulated by demographic factors. Pairwise vertex-wise correlations between the estimated beta coefficients across all cognitive tasks, the composite scores and PC1 predicted by (A) the regionalization of CSA and (B) the regionalization of CTH controlling for the sociodemographic variables of race/ethnicity, household income, and parental education. (C, D) Pairwise vertex-wise correlations for all of the cognitive tasks and (C) the regionalization of CSA and (D) the regionalization of CTH “not” controlling for the specified sociodemographic variables (but controlling for age, sex and scanner). The correlation among associations was larger when these demographic variables were not included in the GLMs used to produce the estimated effect size maps.

Association Maps When Not Controlling for Sociodemographic Variables

We computed the estimated effect size maps for the F and C composites scores associated with the regionalization of CSA (Fig. 5A,B) and CTH (Fig. 5D,E) without controlling for the sociodemographic variables (but controlling for age, sex, and scanner). For both morphology measures, the vertex-wise correlation between the F and C maps increased (CSA: r = 0.69; CTH: r = 0.89). However, the F − C difference maps were remarkably similar to when the sociodemographic variables were controlled for as shown by a vertex-wise correlation between the F − C difference maps with and without controlling for sociodemographic factors (CSA: r = 0.96; CTH: r = 0.94). This strongly suggests that these confounding variables in aggregate index common shared variance between cortical morphology and cognition across domains that is not specific to a particular task performance. Estimated effect size maps for the association between the regionalization of CSA and CTH and cognitive performance across tasks without controlling for sociodemographic factors can be found in Supplementary Figure 5.

Figure 5 .


Figure 5

Similar estimated effect size maps for the association between the regionalization of cortical morphology and the fluid and crystallized composite scores when “not” controlling for sociodemographic factors. All maps display the vertex-wise mass univariate standardized beta coefficients unthresholded for each association. The top part of the figure shows the association between the regionalization of CSA and the fluid composite score (A) and the crystallized composite score (B). (C) The difference in standardized beta coefficients between A + B. The bottom part of the figure shows the association between the regionalization of CTH and the fluid composite score (D) and the crystallized composite score (E). (F) The difference in standardized beta coefficients between D + E. The demographic variables of race/ethnicity, household income, and parental education were not included in the mass univariate GLMs to produce these estimated effect size maps. The exclusion of these confounding factors increased the magnitude of the estimated beta coefficients and the similarity between the maps for the fluid and crystallized composite scores. However, the difference maps (C + F) closely reflect the pattern of differences seen in Figures 2C and L respectively.

Partitioning the Variance Between Brain Structure and Cognition

In order to quantify the variance in each composite score predicted by the vertex-wise imaging data for the regionalization of CSA and CTH, we calculated a mass PVSU. Much like a polygenic risk score, the PVSU represents a linear weighted sum of the vertex-wise associations, which are projected from a training set to an independent hold-out set within a cross-validation framework. A comparison of the subject-wise PVSU with cognitive performance therefore provides a conservative, out-of-sample, lower bound estimate of the unique variance in behavior predicted by the regionalization of cortical morphology independent of the global brain measures. When controlling for age, sex, and scanner, the regionalization of CSA explained a similar proportion of the total variability in cognition compared with the regionalization of CTH (for fluid scores: total CSA = ~2.59%R2; CSA PVSU = ~1.27%R2; CTH PVSU = ~1.88%R2; for crystallized scores: total CSA predicted ~7.15%R2; CSA PVSU predicted ~2.39%R2; CTH PVSU = ~3.85%R2; Fig. 6). Mean CTH was not predictive of cognitive performance. Total CSA predicted more variance in crystallized scores than fluid scores.

Without controlling for brain structure, but whilst controlling for age, sex, and scanner, the sociodemographic factors collectively predicted more variance in crystallized (~23%R2) compared to fluid scores (~10%R2). Across imaging measures, the sociodemographic variables accounted for ~70–90% of the variation in fluid and crystallized scores that was predicted by brain structure. CTH measures (mean CTH and CTH PVSU) explained ~22% of the association between the sociodemographic variables and the crystallized score and ~27% for the fluid score, whilst CSA (total CSA and CSA PVSU) explained ~37% of the association between the sociodemographic variables and the crystallized score and ~44% for the fluid score. This shows that the relationship between brain structure and cognition was strongly related to the sociodemographic factors of race/ethnicity, parental education, and household income.

We additionally estimated the variance in PC1 predicted by regional CSA and CTH measures using the PC1 PVSU when controlling and not controlling for sociodemographic variables. Cross-validated R2 estimates were greatly reduced when controlling for sociodemographic variables (Supplementary Table 3). When controlling for sociodemographic variables in particular, the fluid and crystallized PVSU predicted less variance in the behavior they were not trained on highlighting key differences in the associated regionalization patterns. Moreover, a model predicting PC1 from both the fluid and crystallized PVSU (for CSA and CTH independently) explained a similar proportion of the variance to the sum of R2 across separate models with the fluid and crystallized PVSU predicting PC1. This was less clear for CTH due to the greater similarity in relative association patterns. When not controlling for sociodemographic variables, the overlap in the variance in PC1 explained by the fluid and crystallized PVSU was much greater. This emphasizes that much of the shared variance between cognitive tasks and associated regionalization patterns is associated with sociodemographic factors.

Discussion

In this study, we have shown that the regionalization of cortical morphology, independent of global brain measures, was significantly associated with individual differences in cognitive performance in a large sample of 9–11-year-old children (N = 10 145). Moreover, we showed that individual differences in composite scores of fluid and crystallized performance were associated with distinct maps of relative cortical areal expansion and apparent thickness. This suggests that regional cortical architecture relates differently to these two types of cognitive performance variability. The relatively distinct association maps for the single tasks used to generate these composite measures provide evidence that patterns of cortical morphology differ among individuals and can explain individual variability across different cognitive processes. These patterns do not appear to represent a single, neural construct that could explain the positive manifold. Furthermore, we have shown that sociodemographic diversity impacts the association between cortical morphology and cognition similarly across cognitive domains, which suggests these factors are likely explaining confounding variance to the relationship between brain structure and cognition. Since the treatment of sociodemographic variables in these models led to substantial differences in the magnitude of effects of interest, users of ABCD data should select covariates carefully and discuss any sociodemographic factors potentially related to both explanatory variables and outcomes.

Relative Cortical Configuration is Relevant for Understanding Individual Variability in Cognition

There is a high degree of individual variability in the regionalization of the cortex, however, the extent to which that can explain individual variability in behavior is not well understood. A number of studies have highlighted associations between regional differences in cortical morphology, controlling for global imaging measures, and cognition, however, they have lacked the power to measure associations based on the entire configuration of cortical morphology (Vuoksimaa et al. 2016). Individual differences in the regionalization of CSA and CTH have been associated with distinct, continuous gradients of genetic influences across the cortex (Chen et al. 2011; Chen, Fiecas, et al. 2013a; Chen, Gutierrez, et al. 2013b) that coincide with the gene expression patterns that dictate specialization of the neocortex during embryonic development (O’Leary et al. 2007; Rakic et al. 2009). Individual differences in this molecular signaling could therefore lead to subtle alterations in this cortical configuration, which could lead to variability in behavior. Here, we have used a multivariate statistical approach to measure the significance of distributed effects, which reflect the graded and distributed nature of the biology of regionalization.

We have shown that individual variability in cortical regionalization is robustly associated with individual variability in performance across multiple cognitive tasks. For both morphology measures, there was a clear pattern of positive and negative associations with cognitive performance, which suggests that, at this developmental stage, “relative” differences in the size and thickness of cortical regions mirror individual difference variability in cognition. Although we have corrected for age in all of our models, we are only measuring associations at a single time-point, therefore, phase differences in cortical maturation may be contributing to variability in regionalization. Nevertheless, the regionalization of CSA predicted additional unique variance in cognition not explained by total CSA. By partitioning the variance in this way, we can determine the relative importance of these components of CSA for cognition. This appeared to differ depending on the cognitive task, with more language-related measures having a greater association with total CSA compared to the regionalization of CSA. Comparatively, the proportion of variance in behavior explained by regional CTH was relatively similar across the different measures. Consistent with previous studies (Vuoksimaa et al. 2015, 2016; Schmitt et al. 2019; Grasby et al. 2020), mean CTH did not predict cognitive performance across any measures.

Distinct Associations Between Cortical Morphology and Different Cognitive Domains

The estimated effect size maps between the regionalization of CSA and the crystallized composite score (comprised of reading and vocabulary measures) showed that greater areal expansion in regions previously associated with language functions, such as the left temporal and middle frontal regions (Brown et al. 2001; Hickok and Poeppel 2007; Martin et al. 2015), was associated with higher crystallized scores; whereas, greater areal expansion in a different set of regions, previously implicated in cognitive control mechanisms required for the fluid tasks, such as the anterior cingulate and insula cortices (Botvinick et al. 2004; Taylor et al. 2009; Menon and Uddin 2010; Fjell et al. 2012; Curley et al. 2018), was associated with higher fluid scores. The patterns of apparent CTH associated with these measures were more similar, however, there were key differences in lateral frontal, insula, superior medial, and entorhinal cortices.

Interestingly, the areas most strongly associated with the total composite score were a combination of those most strongly associated with either the fluid or the crystallized composite scores and were very similar to the association maps for PC1. Consistent with previous literature, the CSA and CTH maps for our measures of general cognitive ability were very similar to those observed by Vuoksimaa et al. (2016) despite the difference in age between the two samples. In particular, negative associations with relative CTH and general cognitive ability were found in medial and lateral frontal cortex and positive associations around the central gyrus; and for relative CSA, we observed similar positive associations across the temporal lobe, medial frontal and anterior cingulate cortices. Our results also mirror some associations in highly evolutionary and developmentally expanded regions, namely the frontal cortex, which has been shown previously to associate with general cognitive ability (Fjell et al. 2015; Reardon et al. 2018). The maps of association between both the regionalization of CSA and CTH and the total composite score (and PC1) represent a mixture of the fluid and crystallized associations. Indeed, the same can be seen for the fluid and crystallized composite score maps, which appear to represent a mixture of the association maps for each of the individual tasks that contributed to those scores. This is reflected within the correlation matrix of the vertex-wise associations (Fig. 4A,B). This observation has implications for theories of intellectual development.

Implications for Theories of Intellectual Development

Factor analyses of cognitive measures frequently reveal a higher order general latent factor “g” that can explain (statistically) individual differences in cognitive performance. However, this observation alone does not necessarily reveal anything about neural underpinnings of “g.” There is a pervasive view within society that “g” represents a single causal underlying trait that influences performance across multiple domains. GWAS have begun to reveal some genetic loci associated with general cognitive ability that exhibit pleiotropy with structural brain phenotypes, which point to biological pathways from our genes to “g” via brain phenotypes (although the direction of causality is not clear) (Davies et al. 2018; Savage et al. 2018; Grasby et al. 2020). However, this does not necessarily suggest that there is a unitary pathway underlying the development of cognitive function across individuals. Indeed, polygenic scores for educational attainment and general cognitive ability differentially predict cognitive performance in different domains (Elliott et al. 2019; Loughnan et al. 2019; Mitchell et al. 2020). Here, we do not find that there is a single pattern of regionalization that could represent a causal neuroanatomical substrate underlying “g.” Indeed, we find that, on average, individuals with one regionalization pattern perform better on more language-related tasks, while individuals with a different regionalization pattern perform better on more executive function-related tasks. Therefore, this reveals important heterogeneity among children with comparable levels of performance on measures of general cognitive ability. Our results do not test any causality between brain structure and behavior and do not rule out that a single or global neural phenotype underlying (or contributing to) “g” could be represented within a different neural modality, such as functional connectivity or latency of neuronal signaling; however, there is a lack of supporting evidence for a dominant neural “g” phenotype when measuring cortical morphology in the current study.

Total CSA was modestly associated with cognitive performance in the current study; however, before controlling for sociodemographic factors, total CSA only predicted ~7% of the variability in crystallized scores and ~2% in fluid scores. Due to the unprecedented statistical power in this study, we can infer that total CSA is unlikely to account for substantially more individual variability in cognitive scores in children of this age. Indeed, this effect is much smaller than the shared variance across cognitive measures that has been attributed to “g” (~37% in the current sample) and is of a similar magnitude to that observed by Reardon et al. (2018; 1%). Moreover, the relative importance of this measure for predicting individual variability in cognition differed as a function of the type of intelligence measured, which is inconsistent with this being a global mediator of “g.”

Our results may be more compatible with alternative models of intellectual development in which a single structural phenotype is not necessarily required to produce the latent g-factor. For example, according to the mutualism model (Van Der Maas et al. 2006, 2017), at the beginning of development, individuals start with initially uncorrelated, differentially weighted resources that underlie different cognitive processes. Over time, these resources interact in a mutually beneficial way such that high performance of one system aids the development of another system leading to the positive manifold in performance. This is an important distinction from the more conventional “g” model as here individual differences in mature cognitive performance emerge from different combinations of these resources in early development, such that, for example, the same general cognitive performance score later in development could arise from high initial memory resources in one individual, but high initial processing speed in another individual. Given the development of the positive manifold throughout childhood, we would not necessarily expect to see a large influence of these initial biases on individual variability in cognitive performance at 9–10-years old. Indeed, we can see that multiple environmental and experiential factors contribute to differences in cognitive performance across individuals. It is possible that the small, dissociable variance across cognitive domains associated with cortical regionalization, after controlling for sociodemographic factors, reflects these initial biases in resource allocation. Only in tasks that disproportionately recruit specific cognitive processes can we really detect the small differences in performance across individuals related to cortical regionalization. This may explain why we see such a small association between cortical regionalization and individual variability in cognitive performance.

Given this hypothesis, it is possible that this variability in regionalization is mediated by genetic variability in cortical arealization. Indeed, the regionalization of the cortex is moderately heritable (Winkler et al. 2010; Chen et al. 2011; Eyler et al. 2011, 2012; Chen, Gutierrez, et al. 2013b) and during early embryonic development, gradients of morphogens dictate the expression of transcription factors across the dorsal proliferative zone, which determines the eventual spatial location and functional specialization of cortical projection neurons (O’Leary et al. 2007; Rakic et al. 2009). Small biases in the arealization process, reflected in variability in cortical regionalization, may influence developing cognitive functions in individuals in ways that advantage some functional domains relative to others, which creates important diversity within our population. Although the current study does not measure genetic variation directly, this hypothesis supports the magnitude of the observations here. Exposure to different environments, such as access to good education, can then disproportionately enhance specific genetic effects as has previously been shown (Dickens and Flynn 2001; Harden et al. 2007; Kan et al. 2013; Loughnan et al. 2019) and contribute to individual differences in cognitive function. We have not directly tested this theory in the current paper; however, our observations are in line with what we would expect based on these models. Alternative models such as sampling models and hierarchical factor models could also explain the observations here. Longitudinal, developmental studies are required to tease apart the predictions of these different models at different developmental stages.

It is unclear how heterogeneous the regionalization of cortical morphology is, however, if there is a mixture distribution, as we expect from the given observations and the mutualism model, then this would also reduce the estimated effect size from the mean association maps across the full sample. Future analyses should use data-driven approaches to classify individuals based on similarity in cortical architecture and determine subsequent associations with behavior. This would also elucidate the extent to which these small associations are driven by subgroups of individuals who may have occult or prodromal pathology.

Understanding Individual Variability in Cognition Related to Both Brain Structure and Sociodemographic Factors

After controlling for age, sex, and scanner, but before controlling for sociodemographic variables, the proportion of variance in the composite measures of cognitive performance shared with the brain phenotypes in aggregate was ~9% for the crystallized scores and ~4% for the fluid scores. Overall, the full model including all the phenotypes together accounted statistically for ~32% of the variance in crystallized scores and ~20% of the variance in fluid scores. These results suggest that many factors relating to individual differences in cognitive test scores remain unaccounted for in these statistical models. However, the results are sufficiently powerful to constrain future hypotheses by restricting the range of plausible causal effects of well-measured variables, such as total CSA.

The regionalization of CSA and CTH both accounted for additional unique variance in cognition independent of global measures. Even after preresidualizing for sociodemographic factors that could index cultural, environmental, and other experiential effects on cognitive test scores, as well as global brain measures, the associations between the cortical regionalization phenotypes and cognitive test scores were statistically robust; but only explained ~0.1% of the residual variation. Here, we have used a very conservative, out-of-sample effect size; we are therefore providing the lower bound for the estimated effect size between regionalization and cognition, which is not influenced by any confounding measures. Given the above hypothesis regarding what would be predicted from the mutualism model, it is perhaps unsurprising that we see such a small brain-behavior association, particularly as we are measuring individual variability in typically developing children and not pathology. Here, much of the individual variability in regionalization is attributable to environmental factors, such as sociodemographic variables, although gene–environment correlations may also be contributing to this shared relationship. Moreover, small effects are common-place among samples of this size where effect sizes are not inflated by sampling variability or publication bias (Dick et al. 2020). Large brain-behavior associations in smaller studies with less precision around effect size estimates should be interpreted very cautiously.

Our results show that the sociodemographic variables accounted for a substantial proportion of the shared variance between the brain phenotypes and composite measures of cognitive function. This may result from many small effects of factors indexed by these variables, such as nutrition, limited access to healthcare and education, untreated prenatal complications, systemic racism, high levels of stress, or exposure to environmental toxins. These factors may affect both brain and cognitive development, however, it is not clear to what extent these follow overlapping or independent pathways, and therefore, to what extent these sociodemographic factors are mediators versus confounders in the relationship between brain structure and cognition. However, the finding that these sociodemographic factors predicted similar variance across cognitive tasks, and the association maps were more similar across tasks when not controlling for these factors, suggests that these variables are more likely to be confounding. Only by controlling for these measures do we remove that confounding variance and reveal specific associations, which may more closely represent causal relationships between brain structure and cognition, however, importantly, we cannot infer any causality from the current analyses.

Conclusions

The graded and distributed nature of the neurobiology underlying the regionalization of the cortex supports the importance of studying the whole configuration of cortical morphology associated with behavior using multivariate statistics and continuous pattern comparison methods. Here, we have shown a robust association between cortical regionalization and cognitive performance and have highlighted key differences in the variability of cortical regionalization that can explain individual variability across different components of cognition. Using the unprecedented ABCD dataset, we now have the power to discover novel brain-behavior associations and understand individual variability in behavior among a diverse sample of participants. With future releases of the ABCD data, we will aim to better understand the heterogeneity of cortical regionalization across the sample and track how the relationship between regionalization and cognitive performance may change over time within an individual.

Supplementary Material

SupplementaryMaterial_PUBLICATION_UPLOAD_bhab054

Funding

Data used in the preparation of this article were obtained from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study (https://abcdstudy.org), held in the NIMH Data Archive (NDA). This is a multisite, longitudinal study designed to recruit more than 10 000 children age 9–10 and follow them over 10 years into early adulthood. The ABCD study is supported by the National Institutes of Health and additional federal partners under award numbers U01DA041022, U01DA041028, U01DA041048, U01DA041089, U01DA041106, U01DA041117, U01DA041120, U01DA041134, U01DA041148, U01DA041156, U01DA041174, U24DA041123, U24DA041147, U01DA041093, and U01DA041025. A full list of supporters is available at https://abcdstudy.org/federal-partners.html. A listing of participating sites and a complete listing of the study investigators can be found at https://abcdstudy.org/Consortium_Members.pdf. ABCD consortium investigators designed and implemented the study and/or provided data but did not all necessarily participate in analysis or writing of this report. This manuscript reflects the views of the authors and may not reflect the opinions or views of the NIH or ABCD consortium investigators. The ABCD data repository grows and changes over time. The data were downloaded from the NIMH Data Archive ABCD Collection Release 2.0.1 (DOI: 10.15154/1504041).

Notes

The authors wish to thank the youth and families participating in the ABCD study and all ABCD staff involved in data collection and curation. Dr Dale reports that he was a Founder of and holds equity in CorTechs Labs, Inc., and serves on its Scientific Advisory Board. He is a member of the Scientific Advisory Board of Human Longevity, Inc. He receives funding through research grants from GE Healthcare to UCSD. The terms of these arrangements have been reviewed by and approved by UCSD in accordance with its conflict of interest policies. Dr Dale also reports that he has memberships with the following research consortia: Alzheimers Disease Genetics Consortium (ADGC); Enhancing Neuro Imaging Genetics Through Meta Analysis (ENIGMA); Prostate Cancer Association Group to Investigate Cancer Associated Alterations in the Genome (PRACTICAL); Psychiatric Genomics Consortium (PGC). Conflict of Interest: Dr. Dale reports that he was a Founder of and holds equity in CorTechs Labs, Inc., and serves on its Scientific Advisory Board. He is a member of the Scientific Advisory Board of Human Longevity, Inc. He receives funding through research grants from GE Healthcare to UCSD. The terms of these arrangements have been reviewed by and approved by UCSD in accordance with its conflict of interest policies.

References

  1. Akshoomoff  N, Beaumont  JL, Bauer  PJ, Dikmen  SS, Gershon  RC, Mungas  D, Slotkin  J, Tulsky  D, Weintraub  S, Zelazo  PD  et al.  2013. NIH toolbox cognition battery (CB): composite scores of crystallized, fluid, and overall cognition. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev.  78:119–132. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Arendt, Nielsen  J. 2005. Does education cause better health? A panel data analysis using school reforms for identification. Econ Educ Rev.  24:149–160. [Google Scholar]
  3. Basten  U, Hilger  K, Fiebach  CJ. 2015. Where smart brains are different: a quantitative meta-analysis of functional and structural brain imaging studies on intelligence. Intelligence.  51:10–27. [Google Scholar]
  4. Botvinick  MM, Cohen  JD, Carter  CS. 2004. Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: an update. Trends Cogn Sci.  8:539–546. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bouchard  T, McGue  M. 1981. Familial studies of intelligence: a review. Science (80-).  212:1055–1059. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Brouwer  RM, van  Soelen  ILC, Swagerman  SC, Schnack  HG, Ehli  EA, Kahn  RS, Hulshoff Pol  HE, Boomsma  DI. 2014. Genetic associations between intelligence and cortical thickness emerge at the start of puberty. Hum Brain Mapp.  35:3760–3773. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Brown  WE, Eliez  S, Menon  V, Rumsey  JM, White  CD, Reiss  AL. 2001. Preliminary evidence of widespread morphological variations of the brain in dyslexia. Neurology.  56:781–783. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Burgaleta  M, Johnson  W, Waber  DP, Colom  R, Karama  S. 2014. Cognitive ability changes and dynamics of cortical thickness development in healthy children and adolescents. Neuroimage.  84:810–819. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Casey  BJ, Cannonier  T, Conley  MI, Cohen  AO, Barch  DM, Heitzeg  MM, Soules  ME, Teslovich  T, Dellarco  DV, Garavan  H  et al.  2018. The Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) study: imaging acquisition across 21 sites. Dev Cogn Neurosci.  32:43–54. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Chen  C-H, Fiecas  M, Gutierrez  ED, Panizzon  MS, Eyler  LT, Vuoksimaa  E, Thompson  WK, Fennema-Notestine  C, Hagler  DJ, Jernigan  TL  et al.  2013a. Genetic topography of brain morphology. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA.  110:17089–17094. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Chen  C-H, Panizzon  MS, Eyler  LT, Jernigan  TL, Thompson  W, Fennema-Notestine  C, Jak  AJ, Neale  MC, Franz  CE, Hamza  S  et al.  2011. Genetic influences on cortical regionalization in the human brain. Neuron.  72:537–544. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Chen  C, Gutierrez  ED, Thompson  W, Panizzon  MS, Terry  L, Eyler  LT, Fennema-notestine  C, Jak  AJ, Michael  C, Franz  CE  et al.  2013b. Hierarchical genetic organisation of human cortical surface area. Science; 335:1634–1636. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Colom  R, Burgaleta  M, Román  FJ, Karama  S, Álvarez-Linera  J, Abad  FJ, Martínez  K, Quiroga  MÁ, Haier  RJ. 2013. Neuroanatomic overlap between intelligence and cognitive factors: morphometry methods provide support for the key role of the frontal lobes. Neuroimage.  72:143–152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Colom  R, Haier  RJ, Head  K, Álvarez-Linera  J, Quiroga  MÁ, Shih  PC, Jung  RE. 2009. Gray matter correlates of fluid, crystallized, and spatial intelligence: testing the P-FIT model. Intelligence.  37:124–135. [Google Scholar]
  15. Compton  WM, Dowling  GJ, Garavan  H. 2019. Ensuring the best use of data. JAMA Pediatr. 173:809–810. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Curley  LB, Newman  E, Thompson  WK, Brown  TT, Hagler  DJ, Akshoomoff  N, Reuter  C, Dale  AM, Jernigan  TL, Jernigan  TL. 2018. Cortical morphology of the pars opercularis and its relationship to motor-inhibitory performance in a longitudinal, developing cohort. Brain Struct Funct.  223:211–220. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Cutler  DM, Lleras-Muney  A, Cutler  D, Lleras-Muney  A. 2012. Education and health: insights from international comparisons. Washington, DC: NBER program.
  18. Dale  AM, Fischl  B, Sereno  MI. 1999. Cortical surface-based analysis. Neuroimage.  9:179–194. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Davies  G, Lam  M, Harris  SE, Trampush  JW, Luciano  M, Hill  WD, Hagenaars  SP, Ritchie  SJ, Marioni  RE, Fawns-Ritchie  C  et al.  2018. Study of 300,486 individuals identifies 148 independent genetic loci influencing general cognitive function. Nat Commun.  9:2098. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Deary  IJ. 2012. Intelligence. Annu Rev Psychol.  63:453–482. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Dick  AS, Watts  AL, Heeringa  S, Lopez  DA, Bartsch  H, Chieh Fan  C, Palmer  C, Reuter  C, Marshall  A, Haist  F  et al.  2020. Meaningful effects in the adolescent brain cognitive development study. bioRxiv. 2020.09.01.276451. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Dickens  WT, Flynn  JR. 2001. Heritability estimates versus large environmental effects: the IQ paradox resolved. Psychol Rev.  108:346–369. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Duncan  J, Seitz  RJ, Kolodny  J, Bor  D, Herzog  H, Ahmed  A, Newell  FN, Emslie  H. 2000. A neural basis for general intelligence. Science (80-).  289:457–460. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Elliott  ML, Belsky  DW, Anderson  K, Corcoran  DL, Ge  T, Knodt  A, Prinz  JA, Sugden  K, Williams  B, Ireland  D  et al.  2019. A polygenic score for higher educational attainment is associated with larger brains. Cereb Cortex.  29:3496–3504. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Eyler  LT, Chen  CH, Panizzon  MS, Fennema-Notestine  C, Neale  MC, Jak  A, Jernigan  TL, Fischl  B, Franz  CE, Lyons  MJ  et al.  2012. A comparison of heritability maps of cortical surface area and thickness and the influence of adjustment for whole brain measures: a magnetic resonance imaging twin study. Twin Res Hum Genet.  15:304–314. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Eyler  LT, Prom-Wormley  E, Panizzon  MS, Kaup  AR, Fennema-Notestine  C, Neale  MC, Jernigan  TL, Fischl  B, Franz  CE, Lyons  MJ  et al.  2011. Genetic and environmental contributions to regional cortical surface area in humans: a magnetic resonance imaging twin study. Cereb Cortex.  21:2313–2321. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Fischl  B, Dale  AM. 2000. Measuring the thickness of the human cerebral cortex from magnetic resonance images. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA.  97:11050–11055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Fischl  B, Salat  DH, van der  Kouwe  AJW, Makris  N, Ségonne  F, Quinn  BT, Dale  AM. 2004. Sequence-independent segmentation of magnetic resonance images. Neuroimage.  23:S69–S84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Fischl  B, Sereno  MI, Dale  AM. 1999. Cortical surface-based analysis II: inflation, flattening, and a surface-based coordinate system.9:195–207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Fjell  AM, Walhovd  KB, Brown  TT, Kuperman  JM, Chung  Y, Hagler  DJ, Venkatraman  V, Roddey  JC, Erhart  M, McCabe  C  et al.  2012. Multimodal imaging of the self-regulating developing brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA.  109:19620–19625. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Fjell  AM, Westlye  LT, Amlien  I, Tamnes  CK, Grydeland  H, Engvig  A, Espeseth  T, Reinvang  I, Lundervold  AJ, Lundervold  A  et al.  2015. High-expanding cortical regions in human development and evolution are related to higher intellectual abilities. Cereb Cortex.  25:26–34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Garavan  H, Bartsch  H, Conway  K, Decastro  A, Goldstein  RZ, Heeringa  S, Jernigan  T, Potter  A, Thompson  W, Zahs  D. 2018. Recruiting the ABCD sample: design considerations and procedures. Dev Cogn Neurosci.  32:16–22. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Gläscher  J, Rudrauf  D, Colom  R, Paul  LK, Tranel  D, Damasio  H, Adolphs  R. 2010. Distributed neural system for general intelligence revealed by lesion mapping. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA.  107:4705–4709. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Gottfredson  LS, Deary  IJ. 2016. Intelligence predicts health and longevity, but why?  Curr Dir Psychol Sci.  13:1–4. [Google Scholar]
  35. Grasby  KL, Jahanshad  N, Painter  JN, Colodro-Conde  L, Bralten  J, Hibar  DP, Lind  PA, Pizzagalli  F, Ching  CRK, McMahon  MAB  et al.  2020. The genetic architecture of the human cerebral cortex. Science (80-).  367:eaay6690. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Hagler  DJ, Hatton  S, Cornejo  MD, Makowski  C, Fair  DA, Dick  AS, Sutherland  MT, Casey  BJ, Barch  DM, Harms  MP  et al.  2019. Image processing and analysis methods for the adolescent brain cognitive development study. Neuroimage.  202:116091. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Hampshire  A, Highfield  RR, Parkin  BL, Owen  AM. 2012. Fractionating human intelligence. Neuron.  76:1225–1237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Harden  KP, Turkheimer  E, Loehlin  JC. 2007. Genotype by environment interaction in adolescents’ cognitive aptitude. Behav Genet.  37:273–283. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Heaton  RK, Akshoomoff  N, Tulsky  D, Mungas  D, Weintraub  S, Dikmen  S, Beaumont  J, Casaletto  KB, Conway  K, Slotkin  J  et al.  2014. Reliability and validity of composite scores from the NIH toolbox cognition battery in adults. J Int Neuropsychol Soc.  20:588–598. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Hickok  G, Poeppel  D. 2007. The cortical organization of speech processing. Nat Rev Neurosci.  8:393–402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Hodes  RJ, Insel  TR, Landis  SC, NIH Blueprint for Neuroscience Research . 2013. The NIH toolbox: setting a standard for biomedical research. Neurology.  80:S1–S1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Horn  JL, Cattell  RB. 1966. Refinement and test of the theory of fluid and crystallized general intelligences. J Educ Psychol.  57:253–270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Jernigan  TL, Baaré  WFC, Stiles  J, Madsen  KS. 2011. Postnatal brain development. Structural imaging of dynamic neurodevelopmental processes. In: Progress in brain research. Elsevier, 9: pp. 77–92. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Jovicich  J, Czanner  S, Greve  D, Haley  E, van der  Kouwe  A, Gollub  R, Kennedy  D, Schmitt  F, Brown  G, MacFall  J  et al.  2006. Reliability in multi-site structural MRI studies: effects of gradient non-linearity correction on phantom and human data. Neuroimage.  30:436–443. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Kan  K-J, Wicherts  JM, Dolan  CV, van der  Maas  HLJ. 2013. On the nature and nurture of intelligence and specific cognitive abilities. Psychol Sci.  24:2420–2428. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Lett  TA, Vogel  BO, Ripke  S, Wackerhagen  C, Erk  S, Awasthi  S, Trubetskoy  V, Brandl  EJ, Mohnke  S, Veer  IM  et al.  2020. Cortical surfaces mediate the relationship between polygenic scores for intelligence and general intelligence. Cereb Cortex.  30:2708–2719. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Loughnan  RJ, Palmer  CE, Thompson  WK, Dale  AM, Jernigan  TL, Fan  CC. 2019. Polygenic score of intelligence is more predictive of crystallized than fluid performance among children. bioRxiv. 637512. [Google Scholar]
  48. Martin  A, Schurz  M, Kronbichler  M, Richlan  F. 2015. Reading in the brain of children and adults: a meta-analysis of 40 functional magnetic resonance imaging studies. Hum Brain Mapp.  36:1963–1981. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Menon  V, Uddin  LQ. 2010. Saliency, switching, attention and control: a network model of insula function. Brain Struct Funct.  214:655–667. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Mitchell  BL, Cuéllar-Partida  G, Grasby  KL, Campos  AI, Strike  LT, Hwang  LD, Okbay  A, Thompson  PM, Medland  SE, Martin  NG  et al.  2020. Educational attainment polygenic scores are associated with cortical total surface area and regions important for language and memory. Neuroimage.  212:116691. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Newman  E, Jernigan  TL, Lisdahl  KM, Tamm  L, Tapert  SF, Potkin  SG, Mathalon  D, Molina  B, Bjork  J, Castellanos  FX  et al.  2016a. Go/No Go task performance predicts cortical thickness in the caudal inferior frontal gyrus in young adults with and without ADHD. Brain Imaging Behav.  10:880–892. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Newman  E, Thompson  WK, Bartsch  H, Hagler  DJ, Chen  C-H, Brown  TT, Kuperman  JM, McCabe  C, Chung  Y, Libiger  O  et al.  2016b. Anxiety is related to indices of cortical maturation in typically developing children and adolescents. Brain Struct Funct.  221:3013–3025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. O’Leary  DDM, Chou  SJ, Sahara  S. 2007. Area patterning of the mammalian cortex. Neuron.  56:252–269. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Palmer  CE, Zhao  W, Loughnan  R, Fan  CC, Thompson  W, Jernigan  TL, Dale  AM. 2019. Determining the association between cortical morphology and cognition in 10,145 children from the Adolescent Brain and Cognitive Development (ABCD) study using the MOSTest. bioRxiv. 816025. [Google Scholar]
  55. Panizzon  MS, Fennema-Notestine  C, Eyler  LT, Jernigan  TL, Prom-Wormley  E, Neale  M, Jacobson  K, Lyons  MJ, Grant  MD, Franz  CE  et al.  2009. Distinct genetic influences on cortical surface area and cortical thickness. Cereb Cortex.  19:2728–2735. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Panizzon  MS, Vuoksimaa  E, Spoon  KM, Jacobson  KC, Lyons  MJ, Franz  CE, Xian  H, Vasilopoulos  T, Kremen  WS. 2014. Genetic and environmental influences of general cognitive ability: is g a valid latent construct?  Intelligence.  43:65. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Plomin  R, Spinath  FM. 2004. Intelligence: genetics, genes, and genomics. J Pers Soc Psychol.  86:112–129. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Rakic  P, Ayoub  AE, Breunig  JJ, Dominguez  MH. 2009. Decision by division: making cortical maps. Trends Neurosci.  32:291–301. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Reardon  PK, Seidlitz  J, Vandekar  S, Liu  S, Patel  R, Park  MTM, Alexander-Bloch  A, Clasen  LS, Blumenthal  JD, Lalonde  FM  et al.  2018. Normative brain size variation and brain shape diversity in humans. Science (80-).  360:1222–1227. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Reddan  MC, Lindquist  MA, Wager  TD. 2017. Effect size estimation in neuroimaging. JAMA Psychiatry. 74:207–208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Savage  JE, Jansen  PR, Stringer  S, Watanabe  K, Bryois  J, De Leeuw  CA, Nagel  M, Awasthi  S, Barr  PB, Coleman  JRI  et al.  2018. Genome-wide association meta-analysis in 269,867 individuals identifies new genetic and functional links to intelligence. Nat Genet.  50:912–919. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Schmitt  JE, Neale  MC, Clasen  LS, Liu  S, Seidlitz  J, Pritikin  JN, Chu  A, Wallace  GL, Lee  NR, Giedd  JN. 2019. A comprehensive quantitative genetic analysis of cerebral surface area in youth. J Neurosci.  39:3028–3040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Schnack  HG, van  Haren  NEM, Brouwer  RM, Evans  A, Durston  S, Boomsma  DI, Kahn  RS, Hulshoff Pol  HE. 2015. Changes in thickness and surface area of the human cortex and their relationship with intelligence. Cereb Cortex.  25:1608–1617. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Shadrin  AA, Kaufmann  T, van der  Meer  D, Palmer  CE, Makowski  C, Loughnan  RM, Jernigan  TL, Seibert  TM, Hagler  DJ, Smeland  OB  et al.  2020. Multivariate genome-wide association study identifies 1735 unique genetic loci associated with cortical morphology. bioRxiv. 2020.10.22.350298. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Shaw  P, Greenstein  D, Lerch  J, Clasen  L, Lenroot  R, Gogtay  N, Evans  A, Rapoport  J, Giedd  J. 2006. Intellectual ability and cortical development in children and adolescents. Nature.  440:676–679. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Sowell  ER, Thompson  PM, Leonard  CM, Welcome  SE, Kan  E, Toga  AW. 2004. Longitudinal mapping of cortical thickness and brain growth in Normal children. J Neurosci.  24:8223–8231. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Spearman  C. 1904. “General intelligence,” objectively determined and measured. Am J Psychol.  15:201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Taylor  KS, Seminowicz  DA, Davis  KD. 2009. Two systems of resting state connectivity between the insula and cingulate cortex. Hum Brain Mapp.  30:2731–2745. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Van Der Maas  H, Kan  K-J, Marsman  M, Stevenson  CE. 2017. Network models for cognitive development and intelligence. J Intell.  5:16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Van Der Maas  HLJ, Dolan  CV, Grasman  RPPP, Wicherts  JM, Huizenga  HM, Raijmakers  MEJ. 2006. A dynamical model of general intelligence: the positive manifold of intelligence by mutualism. Psychological Review, 113:842–861. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. van der  Meer  D, Frei  O, Kaufmann  T, Shadrin  AA, Devor  A, Smeland  OB, Thompson  WK, Fan  CC, Holland  D, Westlye  LT  et al.  2020. Understanding the genetic determinants of the brain with MOSTest. Nat Commun.  11:1–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Vuoksimaa  E, Panizzon  MS, Chen  C-H, Fiecas  M, Eyler  LT, Fennema-Notestine  C, Hagler  DJ, Fischl  B, Franz  CE, Jak  A  et al.  2015. The genetic association between neocortical volume and general cognitive ability is driven by global surface area rather than thickness. Cereb Cortex.  25:2127–2137. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Vuoksimaa  E, Panizzon  MS, Chen  C-H, Fiecas  M, Eyler  LT, Fennema-Notestine  C, Hagler  DJ, Franz  CE, Jak  AJ, Lyons  MJ  et al.  2016. Is bigger always better? The importance of cortical configuration with respect to cognitive ability. Neuroimage.  129:356–366. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Walhovd  KB, Krogsrud  SK, Amlien  IK, Bartsch  H, Bjørnerud  A, Due-Tønnessen  P, Grydeland  H, Hagler  DJ, Håberg  AK, Kremen  WS  et al.  2016. Neurodevelopmental origins of lifespan changes in brain and cognition. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA.  113:9357–9362. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Wechsler  D. 1946. The measurement of adult intelligence. 3rd ed. Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins Co. [Google Scholar]
  76. Winkler  AM, Kochunov  P, Blangero  J, Almasy  L, Zilles  K, Fox  PT, Duggirala  R, Glahn  DC. 2010. Cortical thickness or grey matter volume? The importance of selecting the phenotype for imaging genetics studies. Neuroimage.  53:1135–1146. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Winkler  AM, Webster  MA, Vidaurre  D, Nichols  TE, Smith  SM. 2015. Multi-level block permutation. Neuroimage.  123:253–268. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Zhao  W, Palmer  CE, Thompson  WK, Chaarani  B, Garavan  HP, Casey  BJ, Jernigan  TL, Dale  AM, Fan  CC. 2020. Individual differences in cognitive performance are better predicted by global rather than localized BOLD activity patterns across the cortex. Cereb Cortex.  31:1478–1488. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

SupplementaryMaterial_PUBLICATION_UPLOAD_bhab054

Articles from Cerebral Cortex (New York, NY) are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press

RESOURCES