Abstract
Memory serves critical functions in everyday life, but it is also vulnerable to error and illusion. Two decades ago, I proposed that memory errors could be classified into seven basic categories or “sins”: transience, absent-mindedness, blocking, misattribution, suggestibility, bias, and persistence. I argued that each of the seven sins provides important insights concerning the fundamentally constructive nature of human memory, while at the same time reflecting its adaptive features. In this article I briefly summarize some key developments during the past two decades that have increased our understanding of the nature, consequences, and adaptive functions of the memory sins.
Keywords: Constructive memory, memory errors, misattribution, suggestibility, adaptive functions
Psychologists have long known that memory is subject to various forms of error and illusion. These observations played a key role in the emergence of the view that memory is fundamentally constructive rather than reproductive (Bartlett, 1932; Neisser, 1967). But even by the end of the 20th century, there were few systematic attempts to organize or classify the ways in which memory can go awry. To fill the gap, I argued (Schacter 1999, 2001) that memory’s misdeeds could be classified into seven basic categories or “sins”. Three “sins of omission” refer to different kinds of forgetting: transience (decreasing accessibility of information over time), absent-mindedness (breakdown at the interface of attention and memory), and blocking (temporary inaccessibility of information that is stored in memory). Three “sins of commission” refer to different kinds of distortion: misattribution (attributing a memory or idea to the wrong source), suggestibility (implanted memories that result from suggestion or misinformation), and bias (retrospective distortions produced by current knowledge, beliefs, and feeling). The seventh sin, also one of commission, is persistence (intrusive or pathological remembering of events). Although each of the sins can wreak havoc in everyday life, a key idea in the seven sins framework is that rather than reflecting fatal flaws in the architecture of memory, the sins are more usefully conceived as consequences of processes that contribute importantly to the adaptive functioning of memory in everyday life (Schacter, 2001, Chapter 8).
Two decades have now passed since the original proposal of the seven sins of memory, and during that time there has been much progress in documenting their causes and consequences. I recently completed a revision of the 2001 edition of The Seven Sins of Memory that discusses noteworthy new developments related to each sin (Schacter, in press). In this article, I will briefly summarize a few of these developments regarding the six forgetting and distortion-related sins that speak most directly to the reliability of human memory, and also consider the current status of an adaptive perspective on the memory sins.
The Seven Sins of Memory: Some New and Noteworthy Developments
Transience
The observation that memory performance typically declines over time famously dates to the beginning of the experimental investigation of memory pioneered by Ebbinghaus (1885/1964). It is therefore perhaps ironic that one of the more intriguing observations concerning transience during the past two decades concerns evidence for “anti-transience”: individual differences and experimental manipulations that selectively mitigate forgetting over time. The individual differences evidence comes from observations of individuals identified in studies by McGaugh and colleagues who have come to be known by the term Highly Superior Autobiographical Memory (HSAM; LePort et al., 2012). Beginning with a case study of a woman with a remarkable ability to recall her specific past personal experiences (Parker, Cahill, & McGaugh, 2006), and extending to larger groups of similar individuals (e.g., Patihis et al. 2013, Santangelo et al., 2018), research concerning HSAM has begun to unravel some of the cognitive and neural features that characterize these individuals’ exceptional abilities to recall personal experiences.
Most relevant to the present discussion, LePort et al. (2016) studied 30 HSAM individuals and found that they did not recall any more details than non-HSAM controls about personal events that had occurred a day or a week earlier. By contrast, the HSAM group did recall significantly more details about personal events that had occurred a month, a year, and 10 years earlier, thus indicating reduced forgetting over time. In a separate line of research, studies on the benefits of retrieval practice (also known as the testing effect) have shown a similar kind of anti-transience: In research on story recall, Roediger and Karpicke (2006) showed that compared with restudying, retrieval practice/testing did not benefit recall after a 5-minute delay, but did significantly boost recall relative to re-study after delays of 2 days and 1 week. These observations concerning HSAM and retrieval practice are probably not unrelated: HSAM individuals are known to repeatedly retrieve their past personal experiences, and this self-initiated form of retrieval practice is likely part of the explanation for their reduced transience at long delays. At the same time, these findings highlight the pervasiveness of transience in normal forgetting that is not mitigated by an unusual condition such as HSAM or a specific technique such as retrieval practice.
Absent-mindedness
A characteristic of absent-minded forgetting I emphasized in the first edition of The Seven Sins of Memory (Schacter, 2001) emerged from everyday examples and experimental research showing that when a person’s task is to remember to carry out a specific action at a specific time in the future, if retrieval cues are not available at the moment the action needs to be carried out, and attention is not focused on the task goal, absent-minded forgetting can be severe (e.g., Vortac et al., 1995).
However, neither the prospective memory research nor everyday examples I cited presaged a phenomenon that was little known prior to 2001 but has become all too familiar since: parents who forget that their infant is in a car seat in the back of a hot car, often resulting in the death of the child. Such cases regularly appear each summer, and typically involve a “perfect storm” of circumstances that support catastrophic absent-minded forgetting: a change in routine, absorption with pressing concerns unrelated to the child, reliance on automatic behavior, and an absence of retrieval cues at the moment they are needed. As Weingarten (2009) pointed out in his compelling discussion of these cases, they began to increase after experts recommended moving infant car seats to the rear of the car in order to avoid dangers posed to young children by front seat airbags: “If few foresaw the tragic consequence of the lessened visibility of the child … well, who can blame them? What kind of person forgets a baby?”.
We now know that almost anyone can exhibit such forgetting, including many highly functional and responsible parents. Fortunately, in recent years a variety of external cuing systems have been developed that provide the missing retrieval cue that is crucial to avoiding such tragedies (e.g., Baldwin, 2019; for further discussion of hot car cases and cuing systems, see Schacter, in press). These horrific cases of cue-dependent absent-minded forgetting represent a costly vulnerability of human memory, and yet they reflect the same principles previously demonstrated in the laboratory and in less consequential everyday examples.
Another important post-2001 development related to absent-minded forgetting concerns the phenomenon of mind wandering: when attention drifts away from the task at hand, and focuses instead on task-unrelated thoughts (for conceptual discussion of varieties of mind wandering, see Seli et al., 2018). Mind wandering, which emerged as a major topic of research in cognitive psychology and neuroscience during the first decade of the 21st century (e.g., Smallwood & Schooler, 2006), can operate as a source of divided attention that results in absent-minded forgetting of task-related information. This consequence of mind wandering has been examined in relation to educational settings, where studies of classroom and video-recorded lectures have demonstrated that mind wandering occurs frequently, and that more frequent mind wandering is associated with poorer retention of lecture material (e.g., Lindquist & McLean, 2011; Risko, Anderson, Sarwal, Engelhardt, & Kingstone, 2012; Szpunar, Khan, & Schacter, 2013; for review, see Schacter & Szpunar, 2015). Interpolating brief quizzes regarding lecture content in video-recorded lectures can reduce the frequency of mind wandering and enhance retention of lecture content (Szpunar et al., 2013; see Pan, Schmitt, Bjork, & Sana, 2020, for similar benefits from pretesting lecture content).
Despite the negative educational consequences of mind wandering during lectures, and the tragic costs of forgetting young children in hot cars, it is encouraging that simple interventions can reduce both forms of absent-minded memory lapses.
Blocking
By 2001, much research had been published on various forms of retrieval blocking, including tip-of-the tongue states (e.g., Burke, MacKay, Worthley, & Wade, 1991) and retrieval-induced forgetting (e.g., Anderson, Bjork, & Bjork, 1994). But it was only during 2001 that a new kind of retrieval inhibition was introduced by Anderson and Greene’s (2001) seminal paper using the now well-known think/no-think paradigm to show that trying to suppress retrieval of the target word from a paired-associate (‘no think’ condition) produced a small but significant reduction in later recall of the target compared with both ‘think’ words (i.e., target items recalled in response to the paired cue) and, critically, baseline items that were studied but not presented during the think/no think phase. The phenomenon engendered controversy because of some failures to replicate (e.g., Bulevich, Balota, Roediger, & Butler, 2006), but subsequent meta-analyses have revealed evidence for a modest but significant impairment of recall of “suppress” items in the think-no think paradigm (Anderson & Huddleston, 2012; Stramaccia, Meyer, Rischer, Fawcett, & Benoit, 2020).
Importantly, the effect has proven robust enough to study with neuroimaging procedures that have provided new insights into its neural basis. For example, Anderson et al. (2004) found that when people are trying to suppress a target item during the “no think” phase, a) prefrontal regions associated with cognitive control showed increased activity, b) the hippocampus, a brain region often linked to successful recollection, showed some evidence for decreased activity; and c) these brain changes predicted the extent to which a specific item suffered from impaired recall that is attributable to retrieval inhibition. A more recent study (Schmitz et al., 2017) revealed a possible role for GABA, an inhibitory neurotransmitter, in modulating retrieval inhibition in the think-no think paradigm: there was a positive association between a higher resting concentration of GABA in the hippocampus and a greater ability to suppress “no think” items, and stronger coupling between the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex during attempted suppression. These and related findings provide new insights into the brain mechanisms that produce the kind of retrieval blocking elicited by the think/no think paradigm.
Misattribution
Misattribution can take various forms, including source memory confusion, false recall, and false recognition, which had all been studied extensively by 2001. By contrast, one of the most dramatic forms of misattribution, déjà vu, was still largely a clinical curiosity. That is no longer the case, however, as experimental studies have developed systematic methods to elicit and analyze déjà vu. For example, Cleary and colleagues developed novel virtual reality paradigms to show that déjà vu can result from structural similarity between a new, current visual configuration and a previously experienced one (Cleary et al., 2012), which in turn can provide a basis for an illusory conviction that one knows what will happen next when navigating a virtual environment (Cleary & Claxton, 2018). The development of systematic methods for eliciting déjà vu has even made it possible to study it using fMRI (Urquhart, Sivakurmaran, Macfarlane, & O’Connor, 2018). These kinds of studies are adding to our theoretical understanding of déjà vu and its implications for constructive views of memory (Aitken & O’Connor, 2020).
More generally, fMRI studies of misattribution, especially false recognition, have increased dramatically during the past two decades. Early evidence discussed in Schacter (2001) suggested that true recognition could be distinguished from false recognition based on increased activation in brain regions associated with sensory-perceptual processing, and numerous recent studies have delineated the conditions under which such sensory reactivation effects are and are not observed (Schacter, Carpenter, Devitt, & Thakral, in press). Importantly, such studies have also begun to identify brain regions consistently associated with false retrieval (for a meta-analysis, see Kurkela & Dennis, 2016), thereby clarifying the basis of certain kinds of subjectively compelling but illusory memories.
Suggestibility
During the 1990s, psychologists developed experimental paradigms for inducing through suggestion what Loftus (2003) later called “rich false memories” – detailed but inaccurate recollections of everyday experiences. By the end of the 1990s, studies using procedures based on Loftus and Pickrell’s (1995) “lost-in-the-mall” paradigm showed that suggestive procedures (e.g., visualization, social influence) resulted in false memories of various kinds of everyday events for 20–30% of participants (e.g., Hyman & Pentland, 1996; Porter, Yuille, & Lehmann, 1999).
More recently, Shaw and Porter (2015) reported that a potent suggestive procedure drawing on visualization and social pressure induced 70% of their college student sample to develop a false memory of committing a crime as an adolescent, which greatly exceeded the proportion of false memories previously produced in similar paradigms. However, the interpretation of this striking result was questioned by Wade, Garry, and Pezdek (2018), who identified problems with the criteria that Shaw and Porter used to classify a participant’s report as a “false memory.” Citing earlier work that distinguished between false memories and false beliefs, Wade et al. argued that most of the reports that Shaw and Porter classified as “false memories” are more appropriately characterized as “false beliefs”, i.e., people accepted a suggestion as true and speculated about specifics of what might have happened, but did not report a subjective experience of remembering the event (for empirical and conceptual considerations regarding the relation between belief and recollection, see Scorboria, Jackson, Talarico, Hanczakowski, Wysman, & Mazzoni, 2014). After re-scoring Shaw and Porter’s results using criteria from previous studies that distinguish between false memories and false beliefs, they found that only about 25–30% of Shaw and Porter’s participants falsely remembered committing a crime, in line with earlier results.
Despite this important qualification, Shaw and Porter’s (2015) findings nonetheless provide a notable extension of previous work on suggestibility by showing that a combination of social coercion and imagination can lead a surprisingly high proportion of young adults to develop a false belief that they committed a crime. Wade et al.’s (2018) observation that only a much smaller proportion develop full-blown false memories is important theoretically, but the high proportion of false beliefs in this study nonetheless highlights the vulnerability of memory reports to corruption from suggestive influence.
Bias
A common form of retrospective bias is known as consistency bias, where people rewrite the past to make it consistent with their current knowledge, beliefs, and feelings (e.g., Ross & Wilson, 1999). During the past decade, evidence has emerged linking consistency bias with false recollections in the domain of political cognition. For example, Frenda, Knowles, Saletan, and Loftus (2013) used an online survey to examine memory for three actual political events and five fabricated ones. They found that 27% claimed to remember seeing the fabricated events on the news (and about 50% claimed some type of memory for the fabricated event). Consistency biases influenced these false memories, with conservative participants especially likely to report false memories for fabricated events that depicted President Obama in a negative light, and liberal participants especially likely to report false memories for fabricated events that depicted President Bush in a negative light.
More recently, Murphy, Loftus, Grady, Levine, and Greene (2019) reported related data in a study conducted just prior to the May 2018 referendum in Ireland concerning abortion rights. The eighth amendment to the constitution of Ireland guaranteed the unborn a right to life, resulting in extremely restrictive abortion laws. The “yes” side voted to repeal the eighth amendment and won a landslide victory over the “no” side that voted to preserve it. The week before the election, Murphy and colleagues administered an online survey to over 3000 adults, most of whom indicated that they would be voting in the referendum. The survey included photos and headlines of four true and two fake new stories pertaining to both the “yes” and “no” campaigns. Critically, there were two versions of each of the fake news stories, one that reflected negatively on the “yes” side and another that reflected negatively on the “no” side.
Participants were asked to choose among five options regarding their memory for the news stories: 1) I remember seeing/hear this; 2) I don’t remember seeing/hearing this but I remember it happening; 3) I don’t remember this but I believe it happened; 4) I remember this differently; 5) I don’t remember this. False memories occurred frequently for the fake news stories: 48% of participants choose either option 1 or 2, and the percentage increased to 63% when option 3 (i.e., false belief) was included. There was also some evidence that consistency bias played a role: “yes” supporters were more likely than “no” supporters to falsely “remember” fake news involving a fabricated scandal about the “no” side, whereas “no” supporters showed the opposite pattern. These findings are not only relevant to current political divisions in the United States, where the nature and even existence of “truth” has become a partisan issue, but also highlight a novel role for consistency bias in shaping the emergence of false memories.
An Adaptive Perspective
As noted earlier, a key concept in the seven sins framework is that rather than reflecting fatal flaws in the architecture of memory, the sins can be viewed as consequences of adaptive features of memory that contribute to its usefulness in everyday life. In the initial articulation of this perspective (Schacter, 1999, 2001), I relied on functional and evolutionary analyses of memory, some developed in previous work on adaptive aspects of forgetting (e.g., Anderson & Milson, 1989; Anderson & Schooler, 1991; Bjork & Bjork, 1988), to make the case for this approach to conceptualizing the seven sins.
However, whereas there was little experimental evidence to support this adaptive perspective in 2001, during the past two decades an increasingly impressive body of such evidence has emerged. Much of this evidence has been reviewed elsewhere and I refer the interested reader to those sources for in-depth discussion (Ditta & Storm, 2018; Fawcett & Hulbert, 2020; Howe, 2011; Schacter, in press; Schacter et al., in press; Schacter, Guerin, & St. Jacques, 2011). Recent evidence is particularly strong for misattribution errors, including findings that a) false memories in the Deese-Roediger-McDermott false recognition paradigm (Roediger & McDermott, 1995) can boost performance on subsequent tests of creative problem solving (Howe & Garner, 2018) and analogical reasoning (Howe, Garner, Threadgold, & Ball, 2015); b) source misattributions are increased when people make correct (vs. incorrect) associative inferences regarding overlapping features of related episodes (AB, BC) by retrieving and recombining elements of those episodes, including contextual features that are mistakenly combined (Carpenter & Schacter, 2017, 2018); and c) episodic simulation and retrieval processes that support the adaptive function of future thinking can also increase memory errors and biases (Devitt & Schacter, 2018, 2019; Dewhurst, Anderson, Grace, & van Esch, 2016; Dewhurst, Anderson, Grace, & Howe, 2019; Thakral, Madore, Devitt, & Schacter, 2019; for a theoretical analysis, see Schacter & Addis, 2007, 2020).
Conclusion
Overall, the picture of the memory sins painted during the past twenty years supports a constructive perspective on memory by providing new examples of the manifestations and consequences of each of the sins, as well as novel insights into the mechanisms that produce them. The emerging evidence on the benefits of the memory sins supports the view that they reflect the operation of adaptive constructive processes (Schacter, 2012), which play a functional role in memory but produce errors and distortions as a consequence of doing their adaptive jobs. A constructive memory is not always a reliable one, but it need not be a dysfunctional one either: the same processes that can undermine memory’s reliability sometimes do so while contributing to the effective operation of a variety of mental functions.
Funding
Preparation of this manuscript was supported by National Institute on Aging [grant number R01 AG008441] and National Institute of Mental Health [grant number RO1MH060941] to DLS. I thank Ethan Harris for assistance.
References
- Aitken CBA, & O’Connor AR (2020). Converging on an understanding of the déjà vu experience. In Cleary AM & Schwartz BL (Eds.) Memory quirks: The study of odd phenomena in memory (pp. 288–305). London: Routledge. [Google Scholar]
- Anderson JR, & Milson R (1989). Human memory: An adaptive perspective. Psychological Review, 96, 703–719. [Google Scholar]
- Anderson JR, & Schooler LJ (1991). Reflections of the environment in memory. Psychological Science, 2, 396–408. [Google Scholar]
- Anderson MC, Bjork RA, & Bjork EL (1994). Remembering can cause forgetting: Retrieval dynamics in long-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 1063–1087. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anderson MC, & Greene C (2001). Suppressing unwanted memories by executive control. Nature, 410, 366–369. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anderson MC, & Huddleston E (2012). Toward a cognitive and biological model of motivated forgetting. In Belli RF (Ed.), True and false recovered memories: A reconciliation of the debate (pp.53–120). New York: Springer [Google Scholar]
- Anderson MC, Ochsner KN, Kulh B, Cooper J, Gabrieli SW, Glover GH, and Gabrieli JDE (2004). Neural systems underlying the suppression of unwanted memories. Science, 303, 232–235. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baldwin D (2019, August 21). Life-saving car seat alarms that remind parents there’s a baby in the back seat. https://www.fatherly.com/gear/best-car-seat-alarms/
- Bartlett FC (1932). Remembering Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Bjork RA, & Bjork EL (1988). On the adaptive aspects of retrieval failure in autobiographical memory. In Gruneberg MM, Morris RE, and Sykes RN (Eds.), Practical aspects of memory: Current research and issues, 1 (pp. 283–288). Chichester, England: John Wiley. [Google Scholar]
- Bulevich JB, Roediger HL, Balota DA, & Butler AC (2006). Failures to find suppression of episodic memories in the think/no think paradigm. Memory & Cognition, 34, 1569–1577. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burke D MacKay DG, Worthley JS, & Wade E (1991). On the tip of the tongue: What causes word failure in young and older adults? Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 237–246. [Google Scholar]
- Carpenter AC, & Schacter DL (2017). Flexible retrieval: When true inferences produce false memories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 43, 335–349. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carpenter AC, & Schacter DL (2018). False memories, false preferences: Flexible retrieval mechanisms supporting successful inference bias novel decisions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 147, 988–1004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cleary AM, Brown AS, Sawyer BD, Nomi JS, Ajoku AC, & Ryals AJ (2012). Familiarity from the configuration of objects in 3-dimensional space and its relation to déjà vu: A virtual reality investigation. Consciousness and Cognition, 21, 969–975. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cleary AM, & Claxton AB (2018). Déjà vu: An illusion of prediction. Psychological Science, 29, 635–644. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Devitt AL & Schacter DL (2018). An optimistic outlook creates a rosy past: The impact of episodic simulation on subsequent memory. Psychological Science, 29, 936–946. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Devitt AL & Schacter DL (2019). Looking on the bright side: Aging and the impact of emotional future simulation on subsequent memory. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbz041 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dewhurst SA Anderson RJ, Grace L & van Esch L (2016). Adaptive false memories: Imagining future scenarios increases false memories in the DRM paradigm. Memory & Cognition, 44, 1076–1084. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dewhurst SA, Anderson RJ, Grace L, & Howe D (2019). Simulation, false memories, and the planning of future events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 45, 26–36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ditta AS, & Storm BC (2018). A consideration of the seven sins of memory in the context of creative cognition. Creativity Research Journal, 30, 402–417. [Google Scholar]
- Ebbinghaus H (1885/1964). Memory: A contribution to experimental psychology. New York: Dover. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fawcett JM, & Hulbert JC (2020). The many faces of forgetting: Toward a constructive view of forgetting in everyday life. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 9, 1–18. [Google Scholar]
- Frenda SJ, Knowles ED, Saletan W, & Loftus EF (2013). False memories of fabricated political events. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49, 280–286. [Google Scholar]
- Howe ML (2011). The adaptive nature of memory and its illusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 312–315. [Google Scholar]
- Howe ML, & Garner SR (2018). Can false memories prime alternative solutions tambiguous problems? Memory, 26, 96–105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hyman IE Jr., & Pentland J (1996). The role of mental imagery in the creation of false childhood memories. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 101–17. [Google Scholar]
- Howe ML, Garner SR, Threadgold E, & Ball LJ (2015). Priming analogical reasoning with false memories. Memory & Cognition, 43, 879–895. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kurkela KA, & Dennis NA 2016. Event-related fMRI studies of false memory: An activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia, 81, 149–67. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LePort AKR, Mattfield AT, Dickinson-Anson H, Fallon JH, Stark CEL, Kruggel F, Cahill L, & McGaugh JL (2012). Behavioral and neuroanatomical investigation of Highly Superior Autobiographical Memory (HSAM). Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 98, 78–92. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LePort AKR, Stark SM, McGaugh JL, & Stark CEL (2016). Highly Superior Autobiographical Memory: Quality and quantity of retention over time. Frontiers in Psychology. 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.02017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lindquist SL, & McLean JP (2011). Daydreaming and its correlates in an educational environment. Learning and Individual Differences, 21, 158–167. [Google Scholar]
- Loftus EF (2003). Make-believe memories. American Psychologist, 58, 867–873. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Loftus EF, & Pickrell JE (1995). The formation of false memories. Psychiatric Annals, 25, 720–725. [Google Scholar]
- Murphy G, Loftus EF, Grady RH, Levine LJ, & Greene CM (2019). False memories for fake news during Ireland’s abortion referendum. Psychological Science, 30, 1449–1459. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neisser U (1967). Cognitive psychology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. [Google Scholar]
- Pan SC, Schmitt AG, Bjork EL, & Sana F (2020). Pretesting reduces mind wandering and enhances learning during online lectures. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 9, 542–5534. [Google Scholar]
- Parker ES, Cahill L, & McGaugh JL (2006). A case of unusual autobiographical remembering. Neurocase, 12, 35–49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Patihis L, Frenda SJ, LePort AKE, Petersen N, Nichols RM, Stark CEL, McGaugh JL, & Loftus EF (2013). False memories in highly superior autobiographical memory individuals. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 110, 20947–20952. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Porter S, Yuille JC, & Lehman DR (1999). The nature of real, implanted, and fabricated memories for emotional childhood events: Implications for the re-covered memory debate. Law and Human Behavior, 23, 317–337. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Risko EF, Anderson N, Sarwal A, Engelhardt M, and Kingstone A (2012). Everyday attention: Variation in mind wandering and memory in a lecture. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 26, 234–242. [Google Scholar]
- Roediger HL III, & Karpicke J (2006). Test-enhanced learning: Taking memory tests improves long-term retention. Psychological Science, 17, 249–255. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roediger HL III, & McDermott KB (1995). Creating false memories: Remembering words not presented in lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 21, 803–814. [Google Scholar]
- Ross M, & Wilson AE (1999). Constructing and appraising past selves. In Schacter DL and Scarry E (Eds.), Memory, brain, and belief. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Santangelo V, Cavallina C, Colucci C, Santori A, Macri S, McGaugh JL, & Campolongo P (2018). Enhanced brain activity associated with memory access in highly superior autobiographical memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 115, 7795–7800. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schacter DL (1999). The seven sins of memory: Insights from psychology and cognitive \neuroscience. American Psychologist, 54, 182–203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schacter DL (2001). The seven sins of memory: How the mind forgets and remembers. Boston and New York: Houghton Mifflin. [Google Scholar]
- Schacter DL (2012). Adaptive constructive processes and the future of memory. American Psychologist, 67, 603–613. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schacter DL (in press). The seven sins of memory: How the mind forgets and remembers (2nd edition). Boston and New York: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt. [Google Scholar]
- Schacter DL & Addis DR (2007). The cognitive neuroscience of constructive memory: Remembering the past and imagining the future. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society (B), 362, 773–786. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schacter DL & Addis DR (2020). Memory and imagination: Perspectives on constructive episodic simulation. In Abraham A (Ed.), The Cambridge Handbook of the Imagination (pp. 111–131). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Schacter DL, Carpenter AC, Devitt AL, & Thakral PP (in press). Memory errors and distortion. In Kahana MJ and Wagner AD (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Human Memory. New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Schacter DL & Szpunar KK (2015). Enhancing attention and memory during video-recorded lectures. Scholarship of Teaching and Learning in Psychology, 1, 60–71. [Google Scholar]
- Schacter DL, Guerin SA, & Jacques PL St. (2011). Memory distortion: An adaptive perspective. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15, 467–474. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmitz TW, Correia MM, Ferreira CS, Prescot AP, & Anderson MC 2017. Hippocampal GABA enables inhibitory control over unwanted thoughts. Nature Communications. 8, 1311 DOI: 10.1038/s41467-017-00956-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scorboria A, Jackson DL, Talarico J, Hanczakowski M, Wysman L, & Mazzoni G (2014). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143, 1242–1258. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seli P, Kane MJ, Smallwood J, Schacter DL, Maillet D, Schooler JW, & Smilek D (2018). Mind-wandering as a natural kind: A family-resemblances view. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22, 479–490. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shaw J, & Porter S (2015). Constructing rich false memories of committing crime. Psychological Science, 26, 291–301. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smallwood J, and Schooler JW (2006). The restless mind. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 946–958. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stramaccia DF, Meyer A-K, Rischer KM, Fawcett JM, & Benoit RG (2020). Memory suppression and its deficiency in psychological disorders: A focused meta-analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 10.1037/xge0000971 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Szpunar KK, Kahn NY, & Schacter DL (2013). Interpolated memory tests reduce mind wandering and improve learning of online lectures. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 110, 6313–6317. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thakral PP, Madore KP, Devitt AL, & Schacter DL (2019). Adaptive constructive processes: An episodic specificity induction impacts false recall in the Deese-Roediger-McDermott paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 148, 1480–1493. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vortac OU, Edwards MB, & Manning CA (1995). Functions of external cues in prospective memory. Memory, 3, 201–219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Urquhart JA, Sivakurmaran MH, Macfarlane JA, & O’Connor A (2018). FMRI evidence supporting the role of memory conflict in the déjà vu experience. Memory. 10.1080/09658211.2018.1524496 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wade KA, Garry M, & Pezdek K (2018). Deconstructing rich false memories of committing crime: Commentary on Shaw and Porter (2015). Psychological Science, 29, 471–476. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weingarten G (2009, March 8). Fatal distraction. https://www.washingtonpost.com/lifestyle/magazine/fatal-distraction-forgetting-a-child-in-thebackseat-of-a-car-is-a-horrifying-mistake-is-it-a-crime/2014/06/16/8ae0fe3a-f580-11e3-a3a5-42be35962a52_story.html?noredirect=on