Abstract
Accumulating evidence indicates that the actin regulator cofilin is overactivated in Alzheimer’s Disease (AD), but whether this abnormality contributes to synaptic and cognitive impairments in AD is unclear. In addition, the brain region and cell types involved remain unknown. In this study, we specifically manipulate LIMK1, the key protein kinase that phosphorylates and inactivates cofilin, in the hippocampus of APP/PS1 transgenic mice. Using local injections of the AAV virus containing LIMK1 under the control of the CaMKIIα promoter, we show that expression of LIMK1 in hippocampal excitatory neurons increases cofilin phosphorylation (i.e., decreases cofilin activity), rescues impairments in long-term potentiation, and improves social memory in APP/PS1 mice. Our results suggest that deficits in LIMK1/cofilin signaling in the hippocampal excitatory neurons contribute to AD pathology and that manipulations of LIMK1/cofilin activity provide a potential therapeutic strategy to treat AD.
Supplementary Information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1186/s13041-021-00833-3.
Keywords: APP/PS1 transgenic mice, LTP, LIMK1, Cofilin, Learning and memory
Introduction
Alzheimer’s Disease (AD) is the leading cause of dementia, but no effective treatment or cure is currently available. A prevailing hypothesis is that the production from sequential cleavage of the amyloid precursor protein (APP) by secretases and accumulation of amyloid peptides (Aβ) drives the progressive neuronal damage and cognitive impairment in AD [1]. Aβ molecules are released to the extracellular space and can aggregate to form soluble oligomers (AβO) or further accumulate into insoluble plaques [1–3]. How these peptides lead to neuronal degeneration and memory loss remains unclear; however, recent evidence indicates that soluble oligomers (AβO) are sufficient to cause synaptic deficits prior to the deposition of the plaques and these synaptic effects are the best correlates of cognitive impairments in AD [3–7]. It has been shown that AβO, but not monomers, induce loss of synaptic proteins and dendritic spines [3–9]. In addition, AβO can inhibit long-term potentiation (LTP), facilitate long-term depression, and impair memory without causing neuronal death [4–7, 9–15]. LTP is an extensively studied form of synaptic plasticity widely regarded as key mechanisms of memory formation [16, 17]. LTP deficits are also widely reported in animal models of AD [5–7]. The molecular mechanisms by which these synaptic processes are affected in AD are still poorly understood.
LIM domain kinase 1 (LIMK1) is a key signaling molecule critical for actin regulation in the brain. LIMK1 can be activated by various surface proteins, including glutamate receptors, and exerts its effects on actin dynamics by directly phosphorylating and inactivating the actin binding protein cofilin [18, 19]. Together with cofilin phosphatases, LIMK1 regulates the balance of phosphorylated (inactive) and dephosphorylated (active) cofilin to control actin reorganization [20, 21]. Perturbations of LIMK1 and cofilin have been shown to impair spine morphology, synaptic plasticity and memory, underscoring the crucial role of LIMK1/cofilin signaling in synaptic and brain function [22–25]. Recent studies have also shown that abnormalities in both LIMK1 and cofilin are associated with AD patients and animal models of AD [25–42]. For example, the level of phosphorylated cofilin was found to be decreased by the Aβ treatment as well as in the brain of AD mouse models [25, 28–38], suggesting that overactivation of cofilin may contribute to AD pathology. Interestingly, increased phosphorylated cofilin (i.e., decreased cofilin activity) has also been found to be associated with AD models under some circumstances [25, 29–33]. These results indicate complex effects of AD pathology on LIMK1/cofilin signaling, and therefore, whether and how LIMK1/cofilin contributes to AD pathogenesis remains unclear.
In this study, we investigated the role of hippocampal LIMK1 and cofilin using APP/PS1 transgenic mice. We used 3-month old mice because previous studies indicated that there were little or no amyloid plaques detected at this age, which allowed us to examine the effects of LIMK1/cofilin before the plaque formation [43, 44]. We showed that these APP/PS1 transgenic mice exhibited impairments in social memory and LTP in the hippocampus. Hippocampal level of phosphorylated cofilin was also decreased. Viral expression of LIMK1, specifically in the hippocampal excitatory neurons, increased cofilin phosphorylation and LTP and improved social memory. These results established the critical roles of hippocampal LIMK1/cofilin in early synaptic and memory deficits in APP/PS1 mice and suggested that LIMK1/cofilin signaling pathway may serve as potential therapeutic target to treat AD.
Results
Impaired social memory in APP/PS1 mice
We focused on social behavior and memory in 3-month old mice because this aspect is relatively less studied compared to other cognitive processes in APP/PS1 transgenic mice. We assessed social interaction and memory using the three-chamber social interaction test and the five-trial social memory test. In the three-chamber social interaction test (Fig. 1A) that consisted of three stages (stage 1: habituation; stage 2: sociability; stage 3: social memory), both APP/PS1 and wild-type (WT) littermates spent more time interacting with the stranger 1 (S1) than the empty cage, suggesting that sociability was not altered in APP/PS1 mice (Fig. 1B). However, during stage 3, while WT mice spent more time interacting with the novel stranger (S2) than S1, APP/PS1 mice spent similar amount of time interacting with S1 and S2, suggesting impaired social recognition memory (Fig. 1C, D). In the five-trial social memory test (Fig. 1E), both WT and APP/PS1 mice spent gradually less time interacting with the stranger mouse during the repeated exposures (trial 1–5), but showed increased interaction when a novel stranger mouse was introduced on trial 6 (Fig. 1F). However, APP/PS1 mice spent significantly less time interacting with the novel stranger than WT mice on trial 6 (Fig. 1F), suggesting impaired social recognition memory. These results suggest that APP/PS1 mice are impaired in social recognition memory but not sociability. We also conducted the open field test but found no significant differences between WT and APP/PS1 mice in travel distance/speed or the amount of time spent in center/periphery zone of the arena (Fig. 1G–J). Similarly, there were no differences in total travel distance and the amount of time spent in the closed or open arms during the elevated plus maze test (Fig. 1L–M). These results suggest that locomotor activity and anxiety-like behavior were not significantly altered in 3-month old APP/PS1 mice.
Impaired LTP and reduced cofilin phosphorylation in APP/PS1 mice
To investigate cellular mechanisms underlying memory deficits in APP/PS1 mice, we carried out electrophysiological recordings at the Schaffer collateral-commissural pathway (CA1 synapse). We first examined basal synaptic transmission using various stimulation intensities but found no differences in input/output curves of field excitatory postsynaptic potentials (fEPSPs) between WT and APP/PS1 mice (Fig. 2A). Presynaptic function as judged by paired-pulse facilitation (PPF) was also not altered in APP/PS1 mice (Fig. 2B). We then compared LTP induced by a theta burst stimulation (TBS) protocol. As shown in Fig. 2C and D, TBS-induced LTP was significantly lower in APP/PS1 compared to WT mice. These results indicate that APP/PS1 mice were impaired in synaptic plasticity at the age of 3 months. Next, we analyzed LIMK1 and cofilin expression using Western blot analysis of protein lysates prepared from the hippocampus. As shown in Fig. 3, while the total protein levels of LIMK1 and cofilin were not altered, phosphorylated (i.e., inactive) cofilin (P-Cofilin) was significantly reduced in APP/PS1 mice. Therefore, cofilin activity is abnormally upregulated in the hippocampus of APP/PS1 mice.
Hippocampal expression of LIMK1 increases phosphorylated cofilin in APP/PS1 mice
To investigate whether increased cofilin activity in the hippocampus is responsible for the synaptic and cognitive deficits in APP/PS1 mice, we employed local injections of AAV virus, which expressed LIMK1-EGFP (LIMK1 fused to EGFP) or control EGFP under the control of the excitatory neuronal promoter CaMKIIα, bilaterally into the hippocampus. We reasoned that overexpression of LIMK1-EGFP would increase cofilin phosphorylation and therefore normalize cofilin activity. As shown in Fig. 4, LIMK1-EGFP expression was restricted to the hippocampus (Fig. 4A). EGFP signals and colocalized with the neuronal marker, NeuN, as well as LIMK1, but not with the astrocytic marker, GFAP (Fig. 4B and C). To confirm the expression of LIMK1, we isolated the hippocampus and performed Western blot analysis. As shown in Figs. 5 and 6, the level of P-Cofilin was significantly increased in both WT (Fig. 5) and APP/PS1 (Fig. 6) mice expressing LIMK1-EGFP compared to EGFP control virus. Total protein level of cofilin was comparable between LIMK1-EGFP and EGFP expressing mice for both WT and APP/PS1 mice. Thus, viral expression of LIMK1 decreases cofilin activity in the hippocampus via increasing cofilin phosphorylation.
Hippocampal expression of LIMK1 improves LTP in APP/PS1 mice
To investigate the effects of decreased cofilin activity caused by overexpressing LIMK1, we first examined synaptic function at CA1 synapses in both WT and APP/PS1 mice. As shown in Fig. 7, expression of LIMK1-EGFP had no effects on basal synaptic transmission, PPF and TBS-LTP in WT mice. In contrast, expression of LIMK1-EGFP significantly enhanced TBS-LTP without affecting basal synaptic strength or PPF (Fig. 8) in APP/PS1 mice. These results suggest that decreasing cofilin activity was sufficient to rescue the TBS-LTP impairment in APP/PS1 mice. It is possible that in WT mice, endogenous cofilin activity is already low that further reduction does not affect LTP.
Hippocampal expression of LIMK1 improves social memory in APP/PS1 mice
To further investigate the functional consequence of overexpressing LIMK1, we examined behavioral responses in both WT and APP/PS1 mice expressing EGFP or LIMK1-EGFP. Expression of LIMK1-EGFP had no effects on social recognition memory in the three-chamber test or anxiety-like behavior in the elevated plus maze test in WT mice (Fig. 9A–C, I-K), but impaired social recognition in the five-trial test (Fig. 9D) and decreased locomotor activity in the open field test (Fig. 9E–H). These results indicate that overexpression of LIMK1 in WT mice negatively affected social behavior, suggesting that a balanced level of P-Cofilin is important. In contrast, expression of LIMK1-EGFP in APP/PS1 mice significantly improved social recognition memory in both three-chamber and five-trial repeated exposure tests (Fig. 10A–D), without affecting locomotor activity or anxiety-like behavior (Fig. 10E–K). Therefore, the elevated cofilin activity in the hippocampus is an important cause for impaired social memory in APP/PS1 mice.
Discussion
Although changes in LIMK1/cofilin signaling in animal models of AD appear to be complex being affected by subcellular compartments and age of the mice [25, 27, 30–33], increased cofilin activity as determined by decreased P-Cofilin and the formation of aberrant cofilin-actin rods (characterized by accumulation of active cofilin) are fairly consistent observations [25, 28–40]. In addition, reducing cofilin activity by expressing constitutively inactive cofilin (S3D) [41] or manipulating cofilin regulators can rescue Aβ42-induced spine loss as well as deficits in LTP and contextual memory in APP/PS1 mice [38, 39]. Furthermore, a phosphorylated cofilin peptide that presumably prevents cofilin activation also partially improves working memory and novel object recognition in the 5XFAD AD mouse model [42]. These studies suggest that increased cofilin activity may be responsible for the cognitive and synaptic deficits associated with AD, but no direct evidence is yet available by using genetic manipulations of LIMK1/cofilin signaling. In addition, the brain regions and cell types that are involved remain unknown.
In this study, we have addressed these questions by expressing full-length LIMK1 specifically in hippocampal excitatory neurons. In order to avoid the effect of amyloid plaques, we choose to use three-month old mice because little or no plaque formations have been detected at this age [43, 44, Additional file 1: Figure S1]. First, we show that APP/PS1 mice are impaired in social recognition memory. The social memory impairments in APP/PS1 mice are evident in both the three-chamber and five-trial social interaction tasks. In both tests, sociability appears not affected. Second, we show that LTP at CA1 synapse is significantly reduced whereas basal synaptic strength and presynaptic function are intact. The impaired LTP is accompanied by increased cofilin activity without changes in total LIMK1 or cofilin in the hippocampus. These results are consistent with many previous studies [4–7] showing that synaptic and cognitive deficits occur in APP/PS1 mice even before Aβ plaque formation. Third, we show that overexpression of LIMK1 in hippocampal excitatory neurons decreases cofilin activity and improves LTP in APP/PS1 mice. Interestingly, overexpression of LIMK1 in WT hippocampus does not improve LTP although it does decrease cofilin activity. It is possible that the endogenous cofilin activity is already sufficiently low in WT mice that a further decrease does not affect LTP. However, overexpression of LIMK1 in WT hippocampus does negatively affect social memory in the five-trial test, suggesting that this effect is cofilin-independent. Finally, we show that overexpression of LIMK1 improves social memory in APP/PS1 mice. These results suggest that the abnormally high cofilin activity in 3-month APP/PS1 mice is an important contributor to the synaptic and cognitive impairments in these mice. Because the expression of LIMK1 is restricted to the hippocampal excitatory neurons, our results reveal that increased cofilin activity in hippocampal excitatory neurons may be particularly important for early synaptic and cognitive deficits in APP/PS1 transgenic mice.
Previous studies have shown that Aβ42 oligomers can activate LIMK1 and its upstream regulators (e.g., the Rho GTPases Rac1 and their associated protein kinases p21- activated kinases and Rho-kinase ROCK2) in neurons [26, 27, 31]. In addition, increased LIMK1 and ROCK2 activity has also been reported in the hAPPJ20 AD mouse model [27]. Consistent with these results, pharmacological inhibition of LIMK1 has been shown to prevent dendritic spines loss induced by Aβ42 oligomers as well as spine loss in the hAPPJ20 mouse model [27]. These results appear to be inconsistent with findings in the present study where increasing LIMK1 rescues synaptic and cognitive deficits. However, there are a number of potential reasons that could explain the discrepancy between the pharmacological inhibition and our study. First, the animal models (APP/PS1 versus hAPPJ20) used in these two studies are different and therefore the changes in LIMK1/cofilin signaling could be different. Second, the age of mice used in these studies (3-month versus 6-month old) is also different. It is known that changes in LIMK1/cofilin signaling are affected by the age of the animals [30]. Third, the present study uses molecular manipulations specifically in the hippocampal excitatory neurons rather than global inhibition of LIMK1. It is possible that changes in LIMK1/cofilin is region/cell-specific, so that global and region/cell cell-specific manipulations of LIMK1 may have different effects on synaptic and cognitive impairments in these mice. Previous studies have shown that both dorsal and ventral hippocampal regions, including dorsal CA2 and dentate gyrus (DG) as well as ventral CA1, are important for social recognition memory [45–47]. Although we injected the virus in the dorsal hippocampus, which suggests a critical role of LIMK1 in this region, it remains unknown whether dorsal CA2 or DG or both are required. It would be important to distinguish these possibilities in future studies.
In summary, we have demonstrated that cofilin activity is increased in 3-month old APP/PS1 transgenic mice and that overexpression of LIMK1, specifically in the hippocampal excitatory neurons, improves synaptic plasticity and social memory. These results suggest that changes in LIMK1/cofilin in hippocampal excitatory neurons are a key event that may underlie the neuropathology of 3-month old APP/PS1 mice before the amyloid plaque formation. It remains to be investigated whether these changes are present at other ages or in other AD models and whether manipulations of LIMK1 have a similar rescuing effect. Nevertheless, our results suggest that intervention of LIMK1/cofilin may provide a potential strategy to improve synaptic function and memory impairment associated with AD patients.
Materials and methods
Animals
APP/PS1 transgenic mice (#34829-JAX) were obtained from the Jackson Laboratory and were housed (2–5 mice per cage) on a 12 h/12 h light/dark cycle with food and water ad libitum. The following PCR primers were used for genotyping APP/PS1 mice: oIMR 1644: AATAGAGAACGGCAGGAGCA; oIMR 1645:GCCATGAGGGCACTAATCAT. All experimental procedures were conducted according to the guidelines of the Canadian Council on Animal Care (CCAC) and approved by the Animal Care Committee at the Hospital for Sick Children, Canada. All experiments were performed blind to the genotype of the mice. Both male and female mice were used but no differences were noted between sexes, therefore the data were pooled together for statistical analyses between genotypes.
Surgical procedures
For virus injections, the AAV2/DJ-CaMKIIα-LIMK1-EGFP (LIMK1 fused to EGFP, 7.6 × 1012) and AAV2/DJ-CaMKIIα-EGFP (1.3 × 1013) (produced through Canadian Neurophotonics Platform, Laval University, Canada) were injected bilaterally to the dorsal hippocampus as previously described [46]. Briefly, mice were anaesthetized with isoflurane (1.5–2% in 1 L/minute oxygen) and placed onto a stereotaxic frame. Body temperature was maintained at 37 °C using a temperature controller. A midline scalp incision was made followed by craniotomies using a 0.6 mm drill bit. The viruses were injected into the dorsal hippocampus (AP: − 2.00 mm, DV: − 1.50 mm, ML: ± 1.50 mm relative to bregma; coordinates derived from Paxinos and Franklin, 2007). 1 μL of the virus was infused bilaterally at a rate of 0.5 μL/minute via an internal cannula connected by tygon tubing to a 10 μL Hamilton needle syringe. After infusion, the internal cannula was left in place for 8 min to allow for diffusion. The surgically operated mice were recovered for 4 and 6 weeks to allow for LIMK1-EGFP and EGFP expression before behaviour tests were performed. The expression pattern of LIMK1-EGFP and EGFP as well as the injection sites were confirmed by immunohistochemical staining of fixed brain sections after behaviour tests.
Slice electrophysiology
All the electrophysiological recordings were done at the Schaffer collateral-commissural pathway (CA1 synapse) in the hippocampus as previously described [22, 48]. In brief, the mouse brains were removed and 350 µm brain slices prepared in ice-cold artificial cerebrospinal fluid (ACSF) saturated with 95% O2/5% CO2. ACSF contained (in mM): 120.0 NaCl, 3.0 KCl, 1.2 MgSO4, 1.0 NaH2PO4, 26.0 NaHCO3, 2.0 CaCl2, and 11.0 D-glucose. The slices were recovered at 28℃ for 15 min then for at least 2 h at room temperature before a single slice was transferred to a submersion chamber constantly perfused with 95% O2/5%CO2 saturated ACSF. Perfusion flow rate was maintained at 2 ml/min. Synaptic transmission was evoked by stimulation at 0.067 Hz and recorded with glass pipettes (3–4 MΩ) filled with ACSF. For input–output experiments, the stimulus intensity was increased gradually. Paired-pulse facilitations were obtained at inter-pulse intervals of 25 ms, 50 ms, 100 ms, 200 ms, 500 ms or 1 s, and calculated as the ratios of the second response peak values over the first response peak values. LTP was induced by three trains of theta burst stimulations (TBS, five pulses at 100 Hz every 200 ms) with an intertrain interval of 10 s. LTP was calculated and statistically evaluated by comparing the mean values of the last 10 min of the recording and the mean values of the entire baseline. All data acquisition and analysis were done using pCLAMP 10.6 (Axon Instruments, Foster City, California, USA).
Western blot analysis
Protein lysates were prepared from hippocampal slices as previously described [48]. Briefly, hippocampus was isolated quickly on ice and dissolved in ice-cold lysis buffer containing (in mM): 20 Tris–HCl (pH 7.5), 150 NaCl, 1 EDTA, 1 EGTA, 1%Triton X-100, 1 Na3VO4, 20 NaF, and 1% protease inhibitor cocktail and phosphatase inhibitor (Roche) and kept at 4 ℃ for 40 min and debris was removed by centrifugation at 10,000g for 15 min. The protein samples were mixed with 25% volume of 4 × SDS loading buffer (250 mM Tris–HCl, 10% SDS, 0.5% bromophenol blue, 50% glycerol, 5% beta-mercaptoethanol, pH 7.4) and loaded and separated on a SDS-PAGE ployacrylamide gel followed by electrotransfer to a nitrocellulose filter. The filter was then blocked with 5% dry milk in TBST (20 mM Tris–HCl, 9% NaCl, 1% Tween-20, pH 7.6) and incubated overnight at 4 °C with indicated primary antibodies in TBST. Following washing and incubation with appropriate secondary antibodies, the filter was washed and developed using an enhanced chemiluminescence (Thermo-Fisher, CAT#34579) detection method. The images were analyzed using the image studio software as per manufacturer’s instruction. The amount of total protein loaded was controlled by normalizing each tested protein with anti-GAPDH immunoreactivity on the same blot. The antibodies used included: anti-Cofilin (Cell Signaling Technology, rabbit, CAT#51755), anti-LIMK1 (Cell Signaling Technology, rabbit, CAT#3842S), anti-P-Cofilin (Santa Cruz Biotechnology, rabbit, CAT#LO115), anti-GAPDH (Cell Signaling Technology, rabbit, CAT#2118S), and HRP-linked goat anti-rabbit IgG (Cell Signaling Technology, CAT#7074S).
Immunohistochemistry
Previously described procedure was followed [46]. Briefly, mice were anesthetized by ketamine and perfused with 0.1M phosphate-buffered saline (PBS) followed by 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA in PBS). The brain was then dissected and post-fixed in 4% PFA for 24 h, and then transferred to 30% sucrose in PBS solution until it was fully saturated. The brain was then embedded in Tissue-Tek® O.C.T. compound, frozen in liquid nitrogen and sliced to 25 μm coronal or horizontal cryostat sections at − 20 °C (Leica CM1950, Concord, Ontario, Canada). The brain sections were transferred to a glass slide. Sections were washed with PBS, permeabilized by 0.3% Triton for 1 h, blocked with 5% fetal bovine serum for 1 h, and incubated with primary antibodies overnight at 4 °C, and then appropriate secondary antibodies at room temperature for 2 h. After washing, the coverslips were mounted using the ProLong Diamond Antifade mounting medium DAPI for imaging. For thioflavin-S staining, brain sections were sequentially washed in 70% and 80% ethanol for 5 min each followed by incubation in 1 thioflavin-S for 8 min at room temperature. The sections were then washed with 80%, 70% ethanol and PBS again for 5 min each. The slides were dried and cover slipped with mounting media. Images were collected on a Leica epi-fluorescence microscope and a Nikon A1R confocal microscopes and analyzed using ImageJ software (NIH, Betheseda, Maryland, USA). Primary antibodies included: anti-LIMK1 (Cell Signaling Technology, CAT#3842S), anti-NeuN (Cell Signaling Technology, CAT#12943S) and anti-GFAP (Cell Signaling Technology, CAT#3670S). Secondary antibodies included: Alexa Fluor 555 anti-rabbit IgG for anti-LIMK1 and anti-NeuN primary antibodies (Thermofisher Scientific, CAT#A31572), and Alexa Fluor 555 anti-rabbit IgG for anti-GFAP primary antibody (Thermofisher Scientific, CAT#A31570). The excitation/emission used were 402/460 (nm) for DAPI, 488/509 (nm) for GFP, and 562/580 (nm) for Alexa Fluor 555.
Behavioral tests
All the mice were tested at the age of 3 ± 0.5 months. The mice were injected with viruses at the age of 8 weeks and behavioral tests were performed 4 weeks later. All behavioral tests were performed during the light cycle. The mice were tested in open field, elevated plus maze, three-chamber social interaction and five-trial repeated social test. At least three day intervals were given after each test. The detailed procedures of these tests were described previously [45], but briefly explained below.
The open field apparatus was a rectangular Plexiglas box (40 cm long × 40 cm wide × 35 cm high) comprising four walls and an open roof. The illumination in the room was provided by centrally placed in-ceiling dim lights. All mice were individually tested in one 5 min session. Each subject was introduced to the apparatus in the same place of the arena near the center and allowed to explore the apparatus for 10 min. The apparatus was cleaned thoroughly with 75% ethanol before each subject was tested. The movement of the mouse was video tracked and analyzed using ANY-maze software (USA). The open field was divided into central (center 20 cm diameter) and peripheral zones for analysis.
The elevated plus maze consisted of two open arms (35 cm long × 5 cm wide) and two closed arms of the same size with 10 cm high side walls. The apparatus was placed 50 cm above the ground. The tested mice were individually placed in the center and allowed for 10 min free exploration. The entries to and time spent in the open arms, center zone and closed arms were recorded. The maze was cleaned thoroughly with 75% ethanol between mice. The movement was tracked and analyzed using ANY-maze software (USA).
For three-chamber social interaction test, tree chambers (60 cm long × 40 cm wide × 22 cm high) connected by removable partitions in the plexiglass walls that allowed animals to freely move between the chambers. Mice were handled twice a day for 3 days before the test. Prior to the day of test, the handled mice were each habituated to the empty apparatus for 10 min. During the test, a stranger mouse was placed in a cylindrical wired cage (8 cm diameter, 17 cm high) with bars spaced 1 cm apart positioned in left and/or right chamber. The middle chamber was left empty all the time. Each test session had three stages: stage 1: 10 min habituation stage with two empty cages; stage 2: 5 min sociability test with a stranger mouse (S1) and an empty cage; stage 3: 5 min social memory test with the previously encountered stranger (S1) and a second novel stranger (S2). Each stage was separated by a 45 s-1 min interval. The amount of interaction was recorded using sniff time when the animal oriented its nose within 1 cm of the mouse contained in the wired cage. Sniff time was recorded by an experimenter using a stop-watch. Data were analyzed as a percentage time spent investigating the target cage over the total time interacting with either cage. The interaction was also tracked using ANY-maze software (IL, USA).
For five-trial social interaction test, the subject mouse was placed in a chamber (40 cm long × 20 cm wide × 22 cm high) and presented with a same sex juvenile, strange mouse in a cylindrical wired cage (8 cm diameter, 17 cm high) with bars spaced 1 cm apart. Six consecutive 1 min interaction trials with a 30–45 s inter-trial interval were used for each subject. On the last trial, a novel stranger juvenile mouse of the same sex was presented in the cage. A stop-watch was used to record the sniff time when the animal oriented its nose within 1 cm of the stranger mouse in the wired cage. The interaction was also tracked and analyzed using ANY-maze software (IL, USA).
Statistical analyses
All the averaged data in the graphs were stated as mean ± SEM and statistically evaluated by Student’s t-test for comparisons of two groups or by ANOVA (one-way, two-way or repeated measures, as appropriate) for comparisons of more than two groups followed by post hoc Fisher’s LSD multiple comparison test using the SPSS program. P < 0.05 was considered significant. The details of statistical data, including statistical methods, P values and sample size, were provided in respective figure legends.
Supplementary Information
Acknowledgements
We are grateful to all members of Jia labs for their technical assistance and comments on the manuscript.
Abbreviations
- AD
Alzheimer’s Disease
- AβO
Beta amyloid oligomers
- LIMK1
Lim-domain protein kinase 1
- APP/PS1
B6;C3-Tg(APPswe,PSEN1dE9)85Dbo/Mmjax
- LTP
Long-term potentiation
- TBS
Theta-burst stimulation
- fEPSP
Field excitatory postsynaptic potential
Authors' contributions
YW and ZJ designed the study. HZ, YB, DL, HZ, and YM performed the experiments. HZ, YB, AL, DL, CZ and XL analyzed and interpreted the data. HZ, YB, YW and ZJ wrote the paper. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Authors’ information
HZ is an exchange graduate student between Department of Physiology, University of Toronto and Guizhou Medical University, Guiyang, Guizhou Province, China. YB and HZ are graduate students of Department of Physiology, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada. AL is an Associate Investigator of the Key Laboratory of Developmental Genes and Human Disease, MOE, Southeast University, Nanjing, China. DL is a Research Assistant at the Program in Neurosciences and Mental Health, The Hospital for Sick Children, Toronto, ON, Canada. YM is a Research Associate at the Program in Neurosciences and Mental Health, The Hospital for Sick Children, Toronto, ON, Canada. CZ is a Professor of the Department of Geriatrics, The Second Medical Center & National Clinical Research Center for Geriatric Diseases, Beijing, China. XL is a Professor of the Department of Cardiovascular Medicine, The Second Affiliated Hospital of Guizhou University of Traditional Chinese Medicine, Guiyang, China. YW is a Professor of the Department of Psychiatry, Affiliated Hospital of Guizhou Medical University, Guiyang, China, and Mental Health and Counseling Center of College Students, Guizhou Medical University, China. ZJ is a Professor and Senior Scientist in the Department of Physiology, University of Toronto, and Program in Neurosciences and Mental Health, the Hospital for Sick Children.
Funding
This work was supported by Grants from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR, MOP119421, PJT-155959, PJT168922, ZJ), Canadian Natural Science and Engineering Research Council (NSERC RGPIN 341498, RGPIN-2017-06295, ZJ), National Natural Science Foundation of China (81761128036, 81960262, 31760294, YW), High Level Innovative Talents Cultivation Program of Guizhou Province (Qian Ke He SY[2018]5802, [2016]5679, YW), Guiyang City Science and Technology Projects, Zhu Subjects Contract ([2018]1-94, YW), Guizhou Science and Technology Cooperation Support (2020, No. 4Y198, YW), China Scholarship Council (CSC NO 201908520052, HZ) and the Hospital for Sick Children Foundation.
Availability of data and materials
The data used in our study are available from the authors on reasonable request.
Declarations
Ethics approval and consent to participate
All animal experiment procedures were approved the Hospital for Sick Children (Toronto, Canada) Animal Care and Use Committees.
Consent for publication
Not applicable.
Competing interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Contributor Information
Yiming Wang, Email: 754603457@qq.com.
Zhengping Jia, Email: zhengping.jia@sickkids.ca.
References
- 1.Hardy J, Selkoe DJ. The amyloid hypothesis of Alzheimer's disease: progress and problems on the road to therapeutics. Science. 2002;297:353–356. doi: 10.1126/science.1072994. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Walsh DM, Minogue AM, Sala Frigerio C, Fadeeva JV, Wasco W, Selkoe DJ. The APP family of proteins: similarities and differences. Biochem Soc Trans. 2007;35(Pt 2):416–420. doi: 10.1042/BST0350416. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Selkoe DJ. Alzheimer's disease is a synaptic failure. Science. 2002;298(5594):789–791. doi: 10.1126/science.1074069. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Haass C, Selkoe DJ. Soluble protein oligomers in neurodegeneration: lessons from the Alzheimer'samyloid beta-peptide. Nat Rev Mol Cell Biol. 2007;8(2):101–112. doi: 10.1038/nrm2101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Palop JJ, Mucke L. Amyloid-beta-induced neuronal dysfunction in Alzheimer's disease: from synapses toward neural networks. Nat Neurosci. 2010;13(7):812–818. doi: 10.1038/nn.2583. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Mucke L, Selkoe DJ. Neurotoxicity of amyloid β-protein: synaptic and network dysfunction. Cold Spring Harb Perspect Med. 2012;2(7):a006338. doi: 10.1101/cshperspect.a006338. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Sheng M, Sabatini BL, Südhof TC. Synapses and Alzheimer's disease. Cold Spring Harb Perspect Biol. 2012; 4(5): a005777. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 8.Almeida CG, Tampellini D, Takahashi RH, Greengard P, Lin MT, Snyder EM, Gouras GK. Beta-amyloid accumulation in APP mutant neurons reduces PSD-95 and GluR1 in synapses. Neurobiol Dis. 2005;20(2):187–198. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2005.02.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Hsieh H, Boehm J, Sato C, Iwatsubo T, Tomita T, Sisodia S, Malinow R. AMPAR removal underlies Abeta-induced synaptic depression and dendritic spine loss. Neuron. 2006;52(5):831–843. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.10.035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Lambert MP, Barlow AK, Chromy BA, Edwards C, Freed R, Liosatos M, Morgan TE, Rozovsky I, Trommer B, Viola KL, Wals P, Zhang C, Finch CE, Krafft GA, Klein WL. Diffusible, nonfibrillar ligands derived from Abeta1-42 are potent central nervous system neurotoxins. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1998;95(11):6448–6453. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.11.6448. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Walsh DM, Klyubin I, Fadeeva JV, Cullen WK, Anwyl R, Wolfe MS, Rowan MJ, Selkoe DJ. Naturally secreted oligomers of amyloid beta protein potently inhibit hippocampal long-term potentiation in vivo. Nature. 2002;416(6880):535–539. doi: 10.1038/416535a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Hu NW, Smith IM, Walsh DM, Rowan MJ. (2008) Soluble amyloid-beta peptides potently disrupt hippocampal synaptic plasticity in the absence of cerebrovascular dysfunction in vivo. Brain. 2008;131(Pt 9):2414–2424. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn174. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Li S, Hong S, Shepardson NE, Walsh DM, Shankar GM, Selkoe D. Soluble oligomers of amyloid Beta protein facilitate hippocampal long-term depression by disrupting neuronal glutamate uptake. Neuron. 2009;62(6):788–801. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.05.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Cleary JP, Walsh DM, Hofmeister JJ, Shankar GM, Kuskowski MA, Selkoe DJ, Ashe KH. Natural oligomers of the amyloid-beta protein specifically disrupt cognitive function. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8(1):79–84. doi: 10.1038/nn1372. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Shankar GM, Li S, Mehta TH, Garcia-Munoz A, Shepardson NE, Smith I, Brett FM, Farrell MA, Rowan MJ, Lemere CA, Regan CM, Walsh DM, Sabatini BL, Selkoe DJ. Amyloid-beta protein dimers isolated directly from Alzheimer's brains impair synaptic plasticity and memory. Nat Med. 2008;14:837–842. doi: 10.1038/nm1782. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Bliss TV, Collingridge GL. A synaptic model of memory: long-term potentiation in the hippocampus. Nature. 1993;361:31–39. doi: 10.1038/361031a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Kandel ER, Dudai Y, Mayford MR. The molecular and systems biology of memory. Cell. 2014;157:163–186. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2014.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Bernard O. Lim kinases, regulators of actin dynamics. Int J Biochem Cell Biol. 2007;39(6):1071–1076. doi: 10.1016/j.biocel.2006.11.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Scott RW, Olson MF. LIM kinases: function, regulation and association with human disease. J Mol Med (Berl) 2007;85(6):555–568. doi: 10.1007/s00109-007-0165-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Bamburg JR. Proteins of the ADF/cofilin family: essential regulators of actin dynamics. Annu Rev Cell Dev Biol. 1999;15:185–230. doi: 10.1146/annurev.cellbio.15.1.185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Bamburg JR, Bernstein BW. ADF/cofilin Current Biology: CB. 2008;18(7):R273–R275. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.02.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Meng Y, Zhang Y, Tregoubov V, Janus C, Cruz L, Jackson M, Lu WY, MacDonald JF, Wang JY, Falls DL, Jia Z. Abnormal spine morphology and enhanced LTP in LIMK-1 knockout mice. Neuron. 2002;35(1):121–133. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00758-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Meng Y, Takahashi H, Meng J, Zhang Y, Lu G, Asrar S, Nakamura T, Jia Z. Regulation of ADF/cofilin phosphorylation and synaptic function by LIM-kinase. Neuropharmacology. 2004;47(5):746–754. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2004.06.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Zhou Z, Hu J, Passafaro M, Xie W, Jia Z. GluA2 (GluR2) regulates metabotropic glutamate receptor-dependent long-term depression through N-cadherin-dependent and cofilin-mediated actin reorganization. J Neurosci. 2011;31(3):819–833. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3869-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Zablah BY, Merovitch N, Jia Z. The role of ADF/Cofilin in synaptic physiology and Alzheimer's Disease. Front Cell Dev Biol. 2020;2020(8):594998. doi: 10.3389/fcell.2020.594998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Henderson BW, Gentry EG, Rush T, Troncoso JC, Thambisetty M, Montine TJ, Herskowitz JH. Rho-associated protein kinase 1 (ROCK1) is increased in Alzheimer's disease and ROCK1 depletion reduces amyloid-β levels in brain. J Neurochem. 2016;138(4):525–531. doi: 10.1111/jnc.13688. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Henderson BW, Greathouse KM, Ramdas R, Walker CK, Rao TC, Bach SV, Curtis KA, Day JJ, Mattheyses AL, Herskowitz JH. Pharmacologic inhibition of LIMK1 provides dendritic spine resilience against β-amyloid. Sci Signaling. 2019;12(587):aaw9318. doi: 10.1126/scisignal.aaw9318. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Bamburg JR, Bernstein BW. Actin dynamics and cofilin-actin rods in Alzheimer disease. Cytoskeleton (Hoboken, N. J.) 2016;73(9):477–497. doi: 10.1002/cm.21282. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Kang DE, Woo JA. Cofilin, a master node regulating cytoskeletal pathogenesis in Alzheimer's Disease. J Alzheimer's Dis. 2019;72(s1):S131–S144. doi: 10.3233/JAD-190585. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Barone E, Mosser S, Fraering PC. Inactivation of brain Cofilin-1 by age, Alzheimer's disease and γ-secretase. Biochimica et Biophysica Acta. 2014;1842(12 Pt A):2500–2509. doi: 10.1016/j.bbadis.2014.10.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Mendoza-Naranjo A, Contreras-Vallejos E, Henriquez DR, Otth C, Bamburg JR, Maccioni RB, Gonzalez-Billault C. Fibrillar amyloid-β1-42 modifies actin organization affecting the cofilin phosphorylation state: a role for Rac1/cdc42 effector proteins and the slingshot phosphatase. J Alzheimer's Dis. 2012;29(1):63–77. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2012-101575. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Gu Z, Cheng J, Zhong P, Qin L, Liu W, Yan Z. Aβ selectively impairs mGluR7 modulation of NMDA signaling in basal forebrain cholinergic neurons: implication in Alzheimer's disease. J Neurosci. 2014;34(41):13614–13628. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1204-14.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Rush T, Martinez-Hernandez J, Dollmeyer M, Frandemiche ML, Borel E, Boisseau S, Jacquier-Sarlin M, Buisson A. Synaptotoxicity in Alzheimer's Disease Involved a dysregulation of actin cytoskeleton dynamics through Cofilin 1 phosphorylation. J Neurosci. 2018;38(48):10349–10361. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1409-18.2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Davis RC, Marsden IT, Maloney MT, Minamide LS, Podlisny M, Selkoe DJ, Bamburg JR. Amyloid beta dimers/trimers potently induce cofilin-actin rods that are inhibited by maintaining cofilin-phosphorylation. Mol Neurodegener. 2011;6:10. doi: 10.1186/1750-1326-6-10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Rahman T, Davies D, Tannenberg R, Fok S, Shepherd C, Dodd P, Cullen K, Goldsbury C. Cofilin rods and aggregates concur with tau pathology and the development of Alzheimer's disease. J Alzheimer's Dis. 2014;42(4):1443–1460. doi: 10.3233/JAD-140393. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Han F, Zhuang TT, Chen JJ, Zhu XL, Cai YF, Lu YP. Novel derivative of Paeonol, Paeononlsilatie sodium, alleviates behavioral damage and hippocampal dendritic injury in Alzheimer's disease concurrent with cofilin1/phosphorylated-cofilin1 and RAC1/CDC42 alterations in rats. PloS one. 2017;12(9):e0185102. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0185102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Walsh KP, Minamide LS, Kane SJ, Shaw AE, Brown DR, Pulford B, Zabel MD, Lambeth JD, Kuhn TB, Bamburg JR. Amyloid-β and proinflammatory cytokines utilize a prion protein-dependent pathway to activate NADPH oxidase and induce cofilin-actin rods in hippocampal neurons. PloS one. 2014;9(4):e95995. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0095995. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Woo JA, Boggess T, Uhlar C, Wang X, Khan H, Cappos G, Joly-Amado A, De Narvaez E, Majid S, Minamide LS, Bamburg JR, Morgan D, Weeber E, Kang DE. RanBP9 at the intersection between cofilin and Aβ pathologies: rescue of neurodegenerative changes by RanBP9 reduction. Cell Death Dis. 2015;6(3):1676. doi: 10.1038/cddis.2015.37. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Woo JA, Jung AR, Lakshmana MK, Bedrossian A, Lim Y, Bu JH, Park SA, Koo EH, Mook-Jung I, Kang DE. Pivotal role of the RanBP9-cofilin pathway in Aβ-induced apoptosis and neurodegeneration. Cell Death Differ. 2012;19(9):1413–1423. doi: 10.1038/cdd.2012.14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Sun Y, Liang L, Dong M, Li C, Liu Z, Gao H. Cofilin 2 in serum as a novel biomarker for Alzheimer's Disease in Han Chinese. Front Aging Neurosci. 2019;11:214. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2019.00214. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Shankar GM, Bloodgood BL, Townsend M, Walsh DM, Selkoe DJ, Sabatini BL. Natural oligomers of the Alzheimer amyloid-beta protein induce reversible synapse loss by modulating an NMDA-type glutamate receptor-dependent signaling pathway. J Neurosci. 2007;27(11):2866–2875. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4970-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Deng Y, Wei J, Cheng J, Zhong P, Xiong Z, Liu A, Lin L, Chen S, Yan Z. Partial amelioration of synaptic and cognitive deficits by inhibiting Cofilin dephosphorylation in an animal model of Alzheimer's Disease. J Alzheimer's Dis. 2016;53(4):1419–1432. doi: 10.3233/JAD-160167. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Jankowsky JL, Fadale DJ, Anderson J, Xu GM, Gonzales V, Jenkins NA, Copeland NG, Lee MK, Younkin LH, Wagner SL, Younkin SG, Borchelt DR. Mutant presenilins specifically elevate the levels of the 42 residue beta-amyloid peptide in vivo: evidence for augmentation of a 42-specific gamma secretase. Hum Mol Genet. 2004;13(2):159–170. doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddh019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Yan P, Bero AW, Cirrito JR, Xiao Q, Hu X, Wang Y, Gonzales E, Holtzman DM, Lee JM. Characterizing the appearance and growth of amyloid plaques in APP/PS1 mice. J Neurosci. 2009;29(34):10706–10714. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2637-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Hitti FL, Siegelbaum SA. The hippocampal CA2 region is essential for social memory. Nature. 2014;508(7494):88–92. doi: 10.1038/nature13028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Leung C, Cao F, Nguyen R, Joshi K, Aqrabawi AJ, Xia S, Cortez MA, Snead OC, 3rd, Kim JC, Jia Z. Activation of entorhinal cortical projections to the dentate gyrus underlies social memory retrieval. Cell Rep. 2018;23(8):2379–2391. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2018.04.073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Okuyama T, Kitamura T, Roy DS, Itohara S, Tonegawa S. Ventral CA1 neurons store social memory. Science. 2016;353(6307):1536–1541. doi: 10.1126/science.aaf7003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Zhou Z, Liu A, Xia S, Leung C, Qi J, Meng Y, Xie W, Park P, Collingridge GL, Jia Z. The C-terminal tails of endogenous GluA1 and GluA2 differentially contribute to hippocampal synaptic plasticity and learning. Nat Neurosci. 2018;21:50–62. doi: 10.1038/s41593-017-0030-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The data used in our study are available from the authors on reasonable request.