Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2021 Aug 3;10:e67685. doi: 10.7554/eLife.67685

Cortical magnification in human visual cortex parallels task performance around the visual field

Noah C Benson 1,2,‡,, Eline R Kupers 1,2,§, Antoine Barbot 1,2,#, Marisa Carrasco 1,2,, Jonathan Winawer 1,2,
Editors: Ming Meng3, Joshua I Gold4
PMCID: PMC8378846  PMID: 34342581

Abstract

Human vision has striking radial asymmetries, with performance on many tasks varying sharply with stimulus polar angle. Performance is generally better on the horizontal than vertical meridian, and on the lower than upper vertical meridian, and these asymmetries decrease gradually with deviation from the vertical meridian. Here, we report cortical magnification at a fine angular resolution around the visual field. This precision enables comparisons between cortical magnification and behavior, between cortical magnification and retinal cell densities, and between cortical magnification in twin pairs. We show that cortical magnification in the human primary visual cortex, measured in 163 subjects, varies substantially around the visual field, with a pattern similar to behavior. These radial asymmetries in the cortex are larger than those found in the retina, and they are correlated between monozygotic twin pairs. These findings indicate a tight link between cortical topography and behavior, and suggest that visual field asymmetries are partly heritable.

Research organism: Human

Introduction

The human visual system processes the visual world with an extraordinary degree of spatial non-uniformity. At the center of gaze, we see fine details whereas in the periphery acuity is about 50 times lower (Frisén and Glansholm, 1975). This eccentricity effect is mediated by a far greater density of cone photoreceptors and retinal ganglion cells (RGCs) for the fovea than periphery, as well as by a correspondingly greater amount of cortical surface area per degree of the visual field (Rodieck, 1998).

Importantly, visual performance varies not only across eccentricity, but also as a function of polar angle (Figure 1); at matched eccentricity, performance is better along the horizontal than the vertical meridian (horizontal-vertical asymmetry, or ‘anisotropy’; HVA) and better along the lower than the upper vertical meridian (vertical meridian asymmetry; VMA) (Carrasco et al., 2001; Cameron et al., 2002; Montaser-Kouhsari and Carrasco, 2009; Corbett and Carrasco, 2011; Abrams et al., 2012; Baldwin et al., 2012; Greenwood et al., 2017; Himmelberg et al., 2020; Barbot et al., 2021). These asymmetries are present in several basic perceptual dimensions, including contrast sensitivity (Rovamo and Virsu, 1979; Kroon and van der Wildt, 1980; Robson and Graham, 1981; Regan and Beverley, 1983; Pointer and Hess, 1989; Carrasco et al., 2001; Cameron et al., 2002; Fuller et al., 2008; Corbett and Carrasco, 2011; Abrams et al., 2012; Baldwin et al., 2012Rosén et al., 2014; Himmelberg et al., 2020; Purokayastha et al., 2021) and spatial resolution (Anderson et al., 1992; Mackeben, 1999; Altpeter et al., 2000; Carrasco et al., 2002; Talgar and Carrasco, 2002; Montaser-Kouhsari and Carrasco, 2009; Wilkinson et al., 2016; Greenwood et al., 2017; Barbot et al., 2021).

Figure 1. Task performance around the visual field.

Figure 1.

(A, B) Performance for a Gabor-orientation discrimination task is shown in polar coordinates. The plotted angle indicates the polar angle of the tested stimulus location, and the distance from the origin indicates the performance (percent correct) at that polar angle. The origin indicates chance performance. Gabor patches were presented at either (A) 3.2° or (B) 4.5° of eccentricity, with a spatial frequency of 10 and 12 cycles/deg, respectively. The HVA and VMA are evident in both plots and the asymmetries are more pronounced at the farther eccentricity (Carrasco et al., 2001). (C–E) As the angular distance from the vertical meridian increases, averaged across upper and lower, performance gradually improves, approaching the performance level at the horizontal (C, D), and the difference in performance between the upper and lower hemifields reduces (E, F). These gradual changes around the visual field have been observed for contrast sensitivity (Abrams et al., 2012) (C, E; at 6° eccentricity) and acuity limit (Barbot et al., 2021) (D, F; at 10° eccentricity). Each data point corresponds to the average contrast sensitivity (or spatial frequency threshold) at a given angular location, with ±1 SEM across observers. Spatial frequency was measured as the point at which orientation discrimination performance dropped to halfway between ceiling and chance performance. Redrawn using data from Carrasco et al., 2001; Abrams et al., 2012 and Barbot et al., 2021. HVA, horizontal-vertical asymmetry; VMA, vertical meridian asymmetry.

Variations in performance around the visual field are substantial: they can be as pronounced as that of doubling the stimulus eccentricity (Carrasco et al., 2001; Barbot et al., 2019; Himmelberg et al., 2020). Moreover, these asymmetries are in retinal coordinates: the shape of the performance fields are constant in head-centered (but not screen-centered) coordinates whether subjects are tested with their heads tilted or straight (Corbett and Carrasco, 2011). This result suggests that the effects likely arise relatively early in the visual processing stream. Consistent with this interpretation, radial asymmetries vary with several low-level stimulus properties: in general, both the HVA and the VMA become more pronounced as target eccentricity, target spatial frequency, or number of distractors increase (Carrasco et al., 2001; Cameron et al., 2002; Baldwin et al., 2012). These radial asymmetries are not general hemifield or quarterfield effects; they are largest at the cardinal meridians and decrease gradually as the angular distance from the vertical meridian increases (Abrams et al., 2012; Baldwin et al., 2012; Barbot et al., 2021). Indeed, performance in many tasks is similar across all visual quadrants at 45° intercardinal locations (Carrasco et al., 2001; Cameron et al., 2002; Corbett and Carrasco, 2011; Abrams et al., 2012).

The causes of radial asymmetries in performance are largely unknown. In recent work examining this question (Kupers et al., 2019; Kupers et al., 2020), we simulated a two-alternative forced choice (2-AFC) orientation discrimination task using a computational observer model, implemented in ISETBIO (Cottaris et al., 2019), including optical defocus, fixational eye-movements, photoreceptor sampling, and downstream retinal processing with the ISETBIO software platform (Cottaris et al., 2019). A linear classifier was trained to predict the 2-AFC response based on the outputs of either the cones or the RGCs from retinal patches varying in cone and RGC density. Variation in the accuracy of the classifier in terms of cell density was small compared to the measured performance differences with polar angle in human observers. Substantial additional radial asymmetries must therefore arise in visual processing efferent from the retina—i.e., the LGN or cortex. Here, we investigate whether (1) retinotopic maps in human V1 show stark radial asymmetries in cortical surface area; (2) these asymmetries are larger than those found in the retina; (3) these asymmetries fall off gradually with distance from the cardinal meridians, similar to human behavior; and (4) these asymmetries are correlated between monozygotic twins, less correlated between dizygotic twins, and uncorrelated between unrelated (age- and gender-matched) pairs of subjects.

Results

Polar asymmetries in cortical surface area

To examine polar angle asymmetries in the cortical representation, we used a dataset of 163 subjects for whom retinotopic maps as well as manually labeled iso-polar angle and iso-eccentricity contours are publicly available. These labeled data (Benson et al., 2021b) were produced by annotating the largest publicly available human retinotopy dataset: the Human Connectome Project (HCP) 7 Tesla Retinotopy Dataset (Benson et al., 2018). We calculated the surface areas for a series of regions of interest (ROIs) in each subject defined by their visual field preference. Each series of ROIs was centered on one of the four cardinal meridians. For each meridian, we defined the ROI series to cover a gradually increasing span of polar angles from ±10° to ±50° (Figure 2A).

Figure 2. Cortical surface areas around the visual field.

(A) Polar angle (top left) and eccentricity (bottom left) maps for example HCP subject 177746. The V1/V2 boundaries and the V1 horizontal meridian obtained via Bayesian inference are shown as black and white lines, respectively. The middle panel of A shows ROIs drawn with gradually increasing polar angle widths centered around the horizontal meridian (gray) and the vertical meridian (V1/V2 boundary; magenta), limited to 1–6° of eccentricity in V1 only. The right panel shows ROIs around the upper vertical meridian (ventral V1/V2 boundary; blue) and the lower vertical meridian (dorsal V1/V2 boundary; red) that include both V1 and V2. In this hemisphere, the cortical surface area is greater near the horizontal than the vertical meridian and near the lower than the upper vertical meridian. (B) Mid-gray surface area for increasingly large ROIs centered on the vertical (magenta) or horizontal (gray) meridian. The error bars indicate the 68% confidence intervals (CIs) from bootstrapping. The circular icons show the visual field representations of the ROIs for the nearby data points. The x-values of data points were slightly offset in order to facilitate visualization. The brown dotted line shows the equivalent V1 ROI surface area as predicted by Horton and Hoyt, 1991. (C) Same as B, but for upper (blue) and lower (red) vertical meridians. (D, E) The surface areas are transformed to percent asymmetry, both for the cumulative ROIs (black) and incremental ROIs (green). Whereas cumulative ROIs represent a wedge of the visual field within a certain polar angle distance ± θ of a cardinal axis, incremental ROIs represent dual 10°-wide wedges in the visual field a certain polar angle distance ±θ from a cardinal axis. The percent asymmetry of x to y is defined as 100× (xy)/mean(x, y). Positive values indicate greater area for horizontal than vertical (D) or for lower than upper regions (E). ROI, region of interest.

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. Pial, mid-gray, and white-matter surface areas around the visual field.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

Surface area plots, as calculated in Figure 2, are shown with the addition of ROI surface areas calculated on both the pial and white-matter surfaces. No panel A is shown in order to match Figure 2. (B) Surface area for increasingly large ROIs centered on the vertical (magenta) or horizontal (gray) meridian. The error bars indicate the 68% CI from bootstrapping. The circular icons show the visual field representations of the ROIs for the nearby data points. The x-values of data points were slightly offset in order to facilitate visualization. The brown dotted line shows the equivalent V1 ROI surface area as predicted by Horton and Hoyt, 1991. (C) Same as B, but for upper (blue) and lower (red) vertical meridians. (D, E) The surface areas are transformed to percent asymmetry, both for the cumulative ROIs (black) and incremental ROIs (green). CI, confidence interval; ROI, region of interest.

Analysis of these ROIs indicates that the cortical surface area in V1 is distributed asymmetrically, with remarkably large differences between the horizontal and vertical meridians. For example, within ±10° of the meridians, the surface area is ~80% greater for the horizontal than the vertical representation (Figure 2B and D) and for the lower than the upper meridian (Figure 2C and E). Within these ±10° ROIs, 94% of the 163 subjects have positive HVAs, and 93% have positive VMAs. The asymmetries are largest for narrow wedge ROIs. They decrease gradually with angular distance from the vertical meridian, with the VMA decreasing more sharply with distance than the HVA. Previous estimates of V1 cortical magnification by Horton and Hoyt, 1991 fall about halfway between our measures for the vertical and horizontal meridians as they did not account for angular variation (see Materials and methods). Because the cortical surface area of gyri tends to be large on the pial surface and small on the white-matter surface, whereas the surface area of sulci tends to reverse this trend, we calculated all surface areas on the mid-gray surface. Note, however, that the trends reported above hold for the pial and the white-matter surfaces as well (Figure 2—figure supplement 1).

For the VMA estimates, our measures of surface area straddled the V1/V2 boundaries symmetrically, as depicted in Figure 2A (right image, red and blue ROIs). We defined the ROIs this way to avoid potential artifacts that can arise from measuring receptive field centers near the vertical meridian, as discussed in prior reports (Larsson and Heeger, 2006; Winawer et al., 2010). The advantage of this method is that even if the population receptive field (pRF) centers miss the vertical meridian by a few degrees, the boundaries are well defined on both sides of the ROIs (e.g., 10° into V1 and 10° into V2). If we use the V1 portion only, the general pattern of results holds.

Match between cortical and behavioral asymmetries in distinct subject groups

The cortical patterns we observe match behavioral asymmetries (Figure 1), which also decrease gradually away from the vertical meridian, indicating a close correspondence between psychophysical performance and cortical representations in early visual retinotopic maps. Overall, both behavior and cortical surface area fall off gradually and are highly correlated (Figure 3; Pearson’s r = 0.99). This correlation holds for surface areas calculated on the white-matter surface (r = 0.99), the mid-gray surface (Figure 3; r = 0.99), and the pial surface (r = 0.98).

Figure 3. Asymmetries in behavior and cortical surface area are correlated in distinct subject groups.

Figure 3.

V1 mid-gray surface area measured from the HCP dataset (Benson et al., 2018) is highly correlated with the spatial frequency thresholds measured by Barbot et al., 2021 in matched ROIs. Surface areas (x-axis) were computed for ROIs whose visual field positions were limited to 4–5° of eccentricity in order to match the psychophysical experiments. Areas were summed over regions indicated by the visual field insets (and divided by two for all ROIs except the VM and HM, since the latter two ROIs have half the visual field area of the other ROIs). The Pearson correlation is 0.99. HCP, Human Connectome Project; HM, horizontal meridian; ROI, region of interest; VM, vertical meridian.

Our finding that V1 surface area and acuity threshold covary with polar angle (Figure 3) parallels findings that they covary with eccentricity (Duncan and Boynton, 2003). Other tasks such as motion sensitivity (McKee and Nakayama, 1984), critical flicker frequency (Hartmann et al., 1979), texture segmentation (Gurnsey et al., 1996; Barbot and Carrasco, 2017), and perceptual grouping (Tannazzo et al., 2014) do not show the same systematic decline with eccentricity as acuity. For these tasks, performance might be less tightly linked to V1 surface area.

Asymmetries in cortical surface area by twin status and gender

The strong correlation between behavior and cortical surface area was computed at the group level, given that the two measures come from different subject groups. Nonetheless, there are independent individual differences in both the behavioral (Himmelberg et al., 2020) and cortical surface area (Benson et al., 2018) measures. Because the cortical surface area measures come from the HCP dataset, which includes a large number of twin pairs, we were able to assess whether individual measures in surface area asymmetries (HVA and VMA) were shared between twin pairs. Indeed, for monozygotic twin pairs, the intraclass correlation (ICC) of ROI asymmetry in terms of the ROI’s angular width is ≥0.5 for most ROIs (Figure 4). Interestingly, dizygotic twin pairs have correlations close to 0 for HVA but positive correlations similar to those of the monozygotic twins for the VMA. Age- and gender-matched unrelated pairs have correlations for both HVA and VMA that are not statistically distinct from 0 for all ROIs. In comparison, the correlation of mean V1 thickness for the same monozygotic and dizygotic twins is 0.76 and 0.11, respectively, with 68% CIs across 10,000 bootstraps of 0.67–0.82 and −0.07 to 0.29. For the V1 mid-gray surface area these correlations are 0.90 (68% CI: 0.88–0.93) and 0.71 (68% CI: 0.57–0.81), respectively. The number of twins is not large enough to make a precise estimate of a heritability fraction for asymmetry, as the heritability confidence combines the uncertainty in the monozygotic and the dizygotic data. The general pattern, however, whereby there is a large difference in correlation between monozygotic and dizygotic twins for the HVA, but not for the VMA, suggests a larger heritable component for the HVA, and that the HVA and VMA may be controlled by different mechanisms.

Figure 4. Asymmetries in cortical surface area are correlated across twins.

Figure 4.

Intraclass correlation (ICC) coefficient between twin pairs (monozygotic and dizygotic) and age- and gender-matched unrelated pairs. The correlations were computed for the HVA (left) and VMA (right) for wedge widths, as in Figure 2B and C. An unbiased estimate of the ICC was used (see Materials and methods). HVA, horizontal-vertical asymmetry; VMA, vertical meridian asymmetry.

Because cortical surface area differs between males and females, we examined whether the radial asymmetries do as well. We find that the cortical VMA and HVA do not differ substantially by sex or across age-groups (Appendix 1—figure 1). This is consistent with psychophysical findings that the HVA and VMA are similar in males and females (Barbot et al., 2021; Purokayastha et al., 2021). Whereas the average size of V1 is about 10% larger in males, V1 size normalized to the entire cortical surface area is similar for males and females (Appendix 1—figure 2).

Asymmetries in cortical surface area and retinal cell density

Asymmetries around the visual field exist in many parts of the visual system, beginning with the optics (Jaeken and Artal, 2012; Polans et al., 2015; Liu and Thibos, 2019) and the retinal cone density (Curcio et al., 1990; Song et al., 2011). However, the cortical asymmetries are not simply inherited from the cone photoreceptors. Rather, the asymmetries become larger in measures of RGC density compared to cones, and become substantially larger still in surface area of V1 (Figure 5). Below 8° of eccentricity, cone density is greater on the horizontal than vertical meridian, with an HVA of ~20% (Figure 5ACurcio et al., 1990). Over the same eccentricity range, the HVA measures up to 40% in midget RGC density and 60–80% in visual cortex. In this eccentricity range, the VMA of the cone density is 0 or slightly negative (Figure 5B): the upper visual field (inferior retina) has higher cone density than the lower visual field (superior retina), indicating that cone density cannot be the source of the behavioral VMA (Kupers et al., 2019). The VMA becomes positive in midget RGC density (up to ~20%) and still larger in the cortex (~40–100%). Hence, the radial asymmetries are substantially larger in the cortex compared to both the cone photoreceptors and the mRGCs. Note that neither the changes in polar angle asymmetries from cone density to mRGC density nor from mRGC density to cortex are simple scale factors; rather, the differences across stages of processing vary with eccentricity in a manner that suggest new properties at each stage.

Figure 5. Asymmetries from retina and visual cortex.

Figure 5.

(A) HVA and (B) VMA for cone density (black line) and mRGC density (gray line) when comparing the cardinal meridians, and V1/V2 cortical surface area within the ±10° ROIs (black markers) and ±20° ROIs (green markers) as a function of eccentricity. Cone density data are in counts/deg2 (Curcio et al., 1990). Midget RGC densities represent the receptive field counts/deg2 using the quantitative model by Watson, 2014. V1/V2 cortex data are calculated from the cortical surface area in mm2/deg2 within 1° non-overlapping eccentricity bands from 1° to 6° of the ±10° and ±20° ROIs. Markers and error bars represent the median and standard error across bootstraps, respectively. Data are fitted with a second-degree polynomial. HVA, horizontal-vertical asymmetry; RGC, retinal ganglion cell; ROI, region of interest; VMA, vertical meridian asymmetry.

This finding that polar angle asymmetries are larger across successive stages of visual processing parallels findings regarding eccentricity: the cone density is greatest in the fovea, falling off with increasing eccentricity (Curcio et al., 1990) this fall-off with eccentricity becomes steeper in the midget ganglion cell array (Curcio and Allen, 1990Dacey, 1993; Watson, 2014), and steeper still in V1 (Adams and Horton, 2003). Given that most human visual perception relies on signals passing through V1, the increases in the foveal bias and the radial asymmetries (HVA and VMA) likely have important implications for visual performance. The overall size of V1 and the cortical magnification function (averaged across polar angles) correlate with various measures of perception, including vernier acuity (Duncan and Boynton, 2003; Song et al., 2015), perceived angular size (Murray et al., 2006), and the strength of some illusions (Schwarzkopf et al., 2011). The cortical HVA and VMA in individuals may similarly predict radial asymmetries in their visual performance.

Discussion

Here, we reported cortical magnification at a fine angular resolution around the visual field and in the largest available subject pool, allowing us to measure the organization of cortical magnification with high precision. This precision in turn enabled our comparison between cortical magnification and behavior, between cortical magnification of twins, and between cortical magnification and retinal cell densities. The fine angular resolution is especially important for assessing the VMA. The VMA falls off sharply with distance from the vertical meridian: the size of the VMA within 10° of the vertical meridian is ~80%, whereas within 40° it is only ~10%. Prior studies have also shown radial asymmetries in visual cortex, in fMRI BOLD amplitude (Liu et al., 2006; O'Connell et al., 2016), in cortical volume (O'Connell et al., 2016), in cortical magnification in human (Silva et al., 2018) and Macaque monkey (Van Essen et al., 1984), in pRF size in human (Silva et al., 2018), in spatial frequency preference in human (Aghajari et al., 2020), and in diffusion MRI in human (O'Connell et al., 2016). For instance, O'Connell et al., 2016 found that a greater volume of visual cortex is activated by lower than upper hemifield stimuli. Silva et al., 2018 quantified cortical magnification in visual field quadrants (lower, upper, left, and right) for V1–V3 and reported a greater magnification in the horizontal than the vertical quadrants and in the lower than the upper quadrant. These reports agree broadly with our measurements. However, neither behavior (Carrasco et al., 2001; Cameron et al., 2002; Abrams et al., 2012; Baldwin et al., 2012; Barbot et al., 2021) nor cortical magnification is uniform within quadrants (Figure 2). Silva et al., 2018 additionally quantified the pRF size and cortical magnification of small polar angle bins, and observed a general trend for pRFs to be smaller and for magnifications to be larger on the horizontal than the vertical. However, they observed that their finely binned cortical magnification data were noisy, especially near the vertical, and it is unclear whether the magnification measurements of their polar angle bins in V1 are statistically distinct from each other (Appendix 1—figure 3; see also Figure 4B in Silva et al., 2018). Hence assessing the relation between cortical area and behavior requires measurement at a fine angular resolution and in a large dataset.

In this study, we chose to emphasize cortical surface area as our measure of cortical asymmetry rather than pRF size or BOLD amplitude. Whereas all of these measures reflect important properties of visual system organization, we consider surface area, being closely tied to anatomy, to provide the most direct interpretation and to be most invariant to changes in experimental details. BOLD amplitude is influenced by a variety of non-neural factors, such as vascular density (Lee et al., 1995; Winawer et al., 2010; Kay et al., 2019) and cortical surface orientation relative to the MRI bore (Gagnon et al., 2015). PRF size is dependent on both neural receptive field size and the scatter of neural receptive field centers within a voxel (Amano et al., 2009). The within-voxel scatter is influenced by cortical magnification, such that regions with lower magnification, like the vertical meridian, would have a greater change in neural RF center across the voxel, and hence potentially larger pRFs even if neural RFs were matched. In contrast, pRF size is unlikely to influence surface area or magnification estimates, since these estimates are computed from pRF centers only. Moreover, pRF size can be influenced by stimulus pattern (Le et al., 2017) and task (Kay et al., 2015; Klein et al., 2014).

Although variation in performance with respect to polar angle is less well studied than variation with respect to eccentricity, the size of the polar angle asymmetries in behavior is substantial. At 6° of eccentricity, the VMA and HVA in contrast sensitivity are >40% (Figure 1C, 0° vs. dotted-line; Figure 1E, upper vs. lower at 0°). Additionally, the shapes of these performance fields are resilient. For example, both the HVA and VMA are present for stimuli of different orientation, size, luminance, eccentricity, and spatial frequency and under binocular and monocular viewing conditions (Carrasco et al., 2001; Cameron et al., 2002; Montaser-Kouhsari and Carrasco, 2009; Abrams et al., 2012; Baldwin et al., 2012; Corbett and Carrasco, 2011; Greenwood et al., 2017; Himmelberg et al., 2020; Barbot et al., 2021). They also remain constant after both endogenous (voluntary) (Purokayastha et al., 2021) and exogenous (involuntary) (Carrasco et al., 2001; Cameron et al., 2002) covert attention have exerted their effects on perception. That is, attention improves performance in a variety of tasks at all polar angles to a similar degree, for example, orientation discrimination and acuity, but does not diminish the radial asymmetries. Furthermore, the shape of performance fields is maintained in visual short-term memory (VSTM), implying that the quality with which a visual stimulus is perceived and encoded at different locations affects the quality with which it is maintained in VSTM (Montaser-Kouhsari and Carrasco, 2009). The results reported here—differential resources devoted to the different meridians in V1—may underlie both the perceptual and VSTM phenomena.

Previous work examining the visual cortex in the context of twins and heritability has found some evidence of genetic control. The variance of resting-state fluctuations within the visual cortex has a strong genetic component (Yang et al., 2016), and the surface areas of V1–V3 are more correlated for monozygotic than dizygotic twins (Alvarez et al., 2021; Benson et al., 2021b). Interestingly, our analysis of the asymmetries of twin pairs in the dataset demonstrated differences between HVA and VMA. In all but the ±10° ROIs, monozygotic twins had significantly higher correlations in HVA than dizygotic twins or unrelated pairs (Figure 4). This finding is consistent with a strong genetic effect on HVA, a finding which might reflect a tendency for genetic factors to influence the topography of visual cortex more generally. Surprisingly, however, the twin-pair correlations of the VMA were similar for monozygotic and dizygotic twins. This might be due to measurement limitations (the VMA is estimated from half as much data per subject as the HVA) or it might indicate a separate mechanism from HVA that is more influenced by environmental factors. It is unlikely that measurement noise is the sole explanation because we found a positive correlation for both monozygotic and dizygotic twins for the VMA, whereas noise would tend to reduce correlations.

To conclude, this study reveals a tight link between human perception around the visual field and the anatomical organization of V1: both cortical magnification and visual task performance are greatest on the horizontal meridian, lesser on the lower vertical meridian, and the lowest on the upper vertical meridian. Both the behavioral and cortical HVA/VMA fall off gradually with angular distance from the cardinal meridians, with both performance and magnification reaching equality at the inter-cardinal angles. Moreover, we show that these cortical asymmetries reflect the large-scale organization of the visual pathways, becoming more stark at multiple successive stages, and that they are highly correlated between monozygotic twin pairs. These findings show a clear relation between biological structure and behavior and reveal how an increasingly large radial asymmetry across the visual system contributes to visual perception. Comprehensive models of vision must take into account not only performance and anatomical variations across eccentricity, but also variations as a function of polar angle across iso-eccentric locations.

Materials and methods

Ethics statement

No human subjects data were collected for this paper. All data used in this paper were obtained from previous publications and publicly available datasets. Primarily, analyses were performed using data from the HCP (Van Essen et al., 2012), including data from the HCP that were reanalyzed by subsequent studies (Benson et al., 2018; Benson et al., 2021b). Additionally, Figures 1 and 3 include data replotted from previous publications by the authors (Carrasco et al., 2001; Abrams et al., 2012; Barbot et al., 2021), and Figure 5 includes publicly available data from Curcio et al., 1990. In all cases, informed consent was obtained from subjects in the original studies, and all applicable use policies were followed in the use of the data. No personal health information is included in this paper or in the associated dataset or code.

Code and data availability

All data analyzed in this project was previously available from other sources. The results of all analyses and the source code required to produce them, as well as documentation, are publicly available at an Open Science Framework website (https://osf.io/5gprz/; DOI:10.17605/OSF.IO/5GPRZ) with the exception of analyses that rely on restricted data from the HCP, in which case code that will reproduce the analyses given the restricted data is provided. This paper’s Github repository can be found at https://github.com/noahbenson/performance-fields/ (copy archived at swh:1:rev:81daa65b3bc69a085acab9508610f853f6a7dcb0Benson, 2021a).

Data Sources

Structural preprocessed data were obtained from the HCP Young Adult dataset (https://db.humanconnectome.org/) (Van Essen et al., 2012). Genotype data for these subjects (i.e., twin pairings) were also obtained from the HCP. Retinotopic maps were obtained from the HCP 7 Tesla Retinotopy Dataset (Benson et al., 2018). Manually drawn iso-polar angle and iso-eccentricity contours were obtained from Benson et al., 2021b. Behavioral data were obtained from Abrams et al., 2012 and Barbot et al., 2021. Behavioral data were obtained from a separate set of subjects than the structural and retinotopic mapping data because we have no access to the HCP subjects to administer behavioral experiments. The HCP includes some simple acuity measurements for the subjects, but these measures are not positioned around the visual field.

Cortical calculations

We used the HCP 7 Tesla Retinotopy Dataset (Benson et al., 2018) (n = 163 subjects) to characterize the cortical HVA and VMA. We computed the percent asymmetry in cortical surface area for V1 (HVA) or V1/V2 (VMA) using the formula Δ(a1, a2) = 100 × (a1−a2)/mean(a1, a2), where a1 and a2 are the surface areas of the matched horizontal and vertical ROIs or the matched upper and lower ROIs. To test the angular specificity of these asymmetries, we defined several wedge-shaped ROIs surrounding each cardinal meridian, ranging in width from narrow to wide (±10° to ±50° of polar angle from the meridians). All ROIs were restricted to 1–6° of eccentricity.

ROIs were computed in several steps. Initially, V1 and V2 were divided into six coarse-grain sectors using manually drawn iso-polar angle and iso-eccentricity contours as boundaries (Benson et al., 2021b). These sectors were bounded in the tangential direction by either the UVM and the HM or by the HM and the LVM. In the radial direction, they were bounded by either 1–2°, 2–4°, or 4–7° of eccentricity. These 24 sectors (six per visual area per hemisphere) were then resampled in each subject into a smaller set of sectors corresponding to the ROIs mentioned above. Using the notation in which the UVM, the HM, and the LVM have polar angles of 0°, 90°, and 180°, respectively, these resampled sectors were bounded, in the tangential direction, by 0–10°, 10–20°, 20–30°, and so on, up to 170–180° of polar angle and, in the radial direction, by 1–2°, 2–3°, 3–4°, 4–5°, and 5–6° of eccentricity. These individual fine-grain sectors were then combined into the various ROIs analyzed in this study.

Resampling the coarse-grain sectors published by Benson et al., 2021b into fine-grain sectors was performed by resampling each coarse-grain sector independently. To illustrate the procedure, we describe an example coarse-grain sector bounded by 1–2° of eccentricity and by the HM and the UVM. This coarse-grain sector would be divided into 18 fine-grain sectors, each 10° of polar angle wide (but it would not need to be subdivided further in terms of eccentricity). To ensure that the fine-grain sectors are contiguous and to reduce the effects of measurement noise, we drew fine-grain sector boundaries using a measurement of the distance between the two coarse-grain polar angle boundaries (the HM and the UVM in this case). The distance metric used was r = (d1d2)/(d1+d2), where d1 and d2 are the distances in mm along the cortical surface from each of the respective boundaries (the UVM and HM, respectively, in this case). Note that r is always −1 at one of the two boundaries, is one at the other boundary, and varies smoothly between them. Given this distance metric, we then found the values of r at which the fine-grain sector polar angle boundaries occur. To mitigate noise in the polar angle measurements, we employed isotropic interpolation from the scikit-learn toolbox (Pedregosa, 2011), which yields a bijective continuous relation between r and polar angle that minimizes the prediction error of the polar angles in terms of r. Using this relation we determined the values of r that produced the desired subdivisions of the coarse-grain sector’s polar angle values and divided the sector accordingly. Because polar angle and eccentricity are effectively orthogonal in V1 and V2, and because each sector was bounded by iso-polar angle lines on two opposite sides and by iso-eccentricity lines on the other two sides, the same resampling procedure was used for the radial and tangential directions. Example ROIs are shown in Figure 2A.

A separate set of wedges and eccentricities was used for the data in Figure 3 that was matched to the behavioral experiment. These behavior-matched polar angle wedges were 15°-wide each and were limited to 4–5° of eccentricity. Explicitly, these ROIs represent 0±7.5°, 15±7.5°, 30±7.5°, 45±7.5°, and so on, all the way around the visual field. Similarly, a separate set of wedges and eccentricities were used for the data in Appendix 1—figure 3; these bins were limited to between 1° and 6° of eccentricity and used polar angle bins matched to Figure 4 by Silva et al., 2018. Explicitly, these ROIs represent 0±5.625°, 11.25±5.625°, 22.5±5.625°, 33.75±5.625°, 45±5.625°, and so on, all the way around the visual field. Note that a weakness of the ROI resampling method is that it does not explicitly handle ipsilateral representations of the visual field on cortex. Such representations, according to a previous analysis of the HCP dataset used here, are common near the LVM and to a lesser extent near the UVM on the V1/V2 border (Gibaldi et al., 2021). Our method treats these ipsilateral pRFs as noise around the vertical meridian and lumps them in with the polar angle wedge that is nearest the vertical. When the wedges are small this can result in an overestimation of the vertical meridian surface. Thus, the data point at the LVM should be read and interpreted with caution.

For both the 10°- and the 15°-wide wedges, the surface areas of each ROI (which were calculated from V1 alone) were compared with the areal cortical magnification formula of V1 described by Horton and Hoyt, 1991: mHH(x, y) = (17.3 mm/(ρ(x, y) + 0.75°))2, where ρ(x, y) is the eccentricity in degrees of the visual field coordinate (x, y). The integral of mHH(x, y) over the visual field representation of an ROI yields the cortical surface area cROI predicted by Horton and Hoyt for that ROI: cROI = ∫(x,y)∈ROI mHH(x,y) dx dy. Because Horton and Hoyt’s formula does not incorporate polar angle, its predictions should fall between the surface areas of the vertical ROIs and the horizontal ROIs. For both wedge widths, this was the case. For the 10°-, 20°-, 30°-, 40°-, and 50°-wide ROIs in Figure 2B, Horton and Hoyt predict 2.2, 4.3, 6.5, 8.6, and 10.8 cm2 surface areas, respectively. For the 15°-wide wedges, the predicted surface area for each wedge is 12.8 mm2, which is between the surface areas of the vertical and the horizontal ROIs in Figure 3.

ICC values plotted in Figure 4 were calculated between the 38 monozygotic twins, the 33 dizygotic twins, and the 2858 age- and sex-matched unrelated pairs from the HCP 7 Tesla Retinotopy Dataset (Benson et al., 2018) for which manually annotated iso-angle and iso-eccentricity contours were available (Benson et al., 2021b). The 2858 unrelated pairs represent the subset of the possible 13,203 unique subject pairs from the 163 retinotopy subjects that are not twins or otherwise related, that are of the same gender, and that are in the same HCP age-group. The monozygotic twins included 15 male twin pairs and 23 female twin pairs while the dizygotic twins included 17 male and 16 female twins. Precise ages are not available for subjects, but all subjects in the HCP Young Adult Connectome were 22–40 years old. Error bars are plotted as the 16th and 84th percentiles across 10,000 bootstraps. The V1 mid-gray surface area and V1 gray-matter thickness correlations in Figure 4 were calculated using the manually labeled V1 boundaries (limited to 0–7° of eccentricity); the average thickness of this V1 ROI was used. The formula used for the ICC is an unbiased estimate derived from the one-way ANOVA (McGraw and Wong, 1996): r^ICC=(MSrMSw)/(MSr+MSw), where MSr and MSw are the mean-square of the across-pairs factor and the residuals, respectively. For a 2×n matrix X of twin-pair measurements such that x1,j is the measurement of the first twin of twin pair j and x2,j is the measurement of the second twin, then MSr and MSw are given by Equations 1 and 2.

MSr=12(n1)j=1n(x,j¯x¯) (1)
MSw=1ni{1,2}j=1n(xi,jx,j¯) (2)
wherex,j¯=x1,j+x2,j2,
andx¯=12ni{1,2}j=1nxi,j

Retinal calculations

The raw data used for Figure 5 were previously published or from publicly available analysis toolboxes. Cone density data were extracted from post-mortem retinal tissue of eight human retinae published by Curcio et al., 1990 for 0–8° eccentricities (step size 0.1°) and summarized in the ISETBIO toolbox (https://github.com/isetbio/isetbio) (Cottaris et al., 2019). Midget RGC (mRGC) densities were extracted from the analytic model by Watson, 2014. Cortical asymmetries were computed from the HCP 7 Tesla Retinotopy Dataset (Benson et al., 2018) as percent difference in V1/V2 cortical surface area in ±10° and ±20° wedges centered on the cardinal meridians for 1° bins between 1° and 6° eccentricity. The ±10° and ±20° wedges were identical to those used in Figure 2, and both were plotted due to the possibility of bias introduced by the ipsilateral cortical representation near the vertical meridian discussed in the previous section (see Cortical calculations). Median and standard error of cortical surface area were computed from bootstrapped data across subjects (1000 iterations).

Meridian asymmetries in cone density and mRGC density were defined as percent change in density values at the cardinal meridians (for retina) or V1/V2 cortical surface area within polar angle wedges centered on cardinal meridians as follows:

HVA=100mean(nasal,temporal)mean(superior,inferior)mean(nasal,temporal,superior,inferior)
VMA=100superiorinferiormean(superior,inferior)

Statistical analysis and measurement reliability

This manuscript does not assess statistical significance. CIs computed for Figures 1 and 3 are the standard error of the mean. CIs for Figures 2 and 5 are the standard deviation of the median across bootstraps. CIs for Figure 4 are the 16th and 84th percentiles across 10,000 bootstraps.

Although the behavioral data reanalyzed here were published previously, we have included an overview of their quantification as well. Abrams et al., 2012 measured 75%-correct contrast thresholds in a 2-AFC orientation discrimination task. Participants reported the orientation (±15° of vertical) of Gabor targets (6 cycles/deg) presented at different iso-eccentric locations at 6° of eccentricity. Repeated-measures ANOVAs were used to assess changes in contrast sensitivity as a function of visual hemifield (upper vs. lower) and angular distance from the VM (0°, 15°, 30°, 45°, 67.5°, and 90°). Paired t-tests were corrected for multiple comparisons using a Bonferroni correction. (Barbot et al., 2021) measured orientation discrimination performance as a function of the spatial frequency and angular position of high-contrast grating stimuli presented at 10° of eccentricity. Spatial frequency acuity thresholds were computed from psychometric fits as the stimulus spatial frequency at which performance was 75% correct in the 2-AFC (±45°) orientation discrimination task. Linear mixed-effects models were used to assess the changes in visual acuity between upper and lower hemifields as a function of the angular distance from the VM (0–90°, with 15° steps).

The wedge-shaped ROIs around the visual field were calculated by subdividing visual sectors whose boundaries were hand-drawn on V1 and V2. These sectors were each drawn by four trained raters, and the reliability of these boundaries was assessed extensively in the source publication (Benson et al., 2021b). To summarize: these boundaries are incredibly reliable, with differences between raters accounting for <5% of all variance across subjects and raters. The ROIs were additionally informed by the pRF polar angle and eccentricity measurements from the HCP 7 Tesla Retinotopy Dataset (Benson et al., 2018). Again, reliability of these measurements was discussed extensively in the original paper and is high across independent fMRI scans.

Acknowledgements

The authors thank Antonio Fernández, Michael Jigo, and Drs. Marc Himmelberg and Jan Kurzawski for helpful comments. This work was supported by NIH NEI RO1-EY027401 to MC and JW as well as a Data Science Environments project award from the Gordon and Betty Moore Foundation (Award #2013-10-29) and the Alfred P Sloan Foundation (Award #3835) to the University of Washington eScience Institute. Data were provided in part by the Human Connectome Project, WU-Minn Consortium (Principal Investigators: David Van Essen and Kamil Ugurbil; 1U54MH091657) funded by the 16 NIH Institutes and Centers that support the NIH Blueprint for Neuroscience Research, and by the McDonnell Center for Systems Neuroscience at Washington University.

Appendix 1

Appendix 1—figure 1. Visual field asymmetry in terms of age and gender at ±20° of polar angle.

Appendix 1—figure 1.

Plots of the local (top row) and cumulative (bottom row) asymmetry across subjects in terms of age and gender. The percent horizontal-vertical asymmetry (HVA: horizontal ROI surface area minus vertical ROI surface area, divided by the mean surface area across horizontal and vertical ROIs) is shown in the left column, and the vertical meridian asymmetry (VMA: upper vertical ROI surface area minus lower vertical ROI surface area, divided by the mean surface area across upper and lower ROIs) is shown in the right column. Box-plots show the median (central horizontal line), quartiles (shaded box), and ±95% percentiles (lines).

Appendix 1—figure 2. V1 size and volume differences in terms of sex.

Appendix 1—figure 2.

ROI is limited to V1 between 1° and 6° of eccentricity. Violin plots terminate at the exact extrema of the dataset. Surface area was calculated on the mid-gray surface. Volume calculations include the entire gray-matter layer. Whole-hemisphere calculations for surface area and volume exclude the region corresponding to the corpus callosum.

Appendix 1—figure 3. Comparison to Silva et al., 2018.

Appendix 1—figure 3.

(A) Data from Silva et al., 2018Figure 4, replotted around the visual field. The gray area of the panel indicates the axis limits of the original plot. Plot points and confidence intervals represent the linear cortical magnification of each polar angle bin after regressing out the effect of eccentricity on cortical magnification. (B) The square root of the mid-gray surface area of the 163 subjects analyzed in this paper, plotted for polar angle bins matched to those in panel A. Bins were limited to between 1° and 6° of eccentricity. The square-root of the surface area is used because it should scale linearly with the linear cortical magnification values plotted by Silva et al., 2018 and replotted in panel A. Notably, the LVM has a substantially higher surface area in panel B than the points around it. This is likely due to the prevalence of ipsilateral pRFs near the LVM (Gibaldi et al., 2021). Our method does not explicitly account for ipsilateral representation, and thus parts of cortex with ipsilateral pRFs will tend to be counted as part of the vertical meridian when the polar angle bins are sufficiently small (see also Methods). Thus the data point at the LVM should be read and interpreted with caution. pRF, population receptive field. LVM, lower vertical meridian.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Noah C Benson, Email: nben@uw.edu.

Ming Meng, South China Normal University, China.

Joshua I Gold, University of Pennsylvania, United States.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • National Eye Institute R01-EY027401 to Marisa Carrasco, Jonathan Winawer.

  • Gordon and Betty Moore Foundation #2013-10-29 to Noah C Benson.

  • Alfred P. Sloan Foundation #3835 to Noah C Benson.

Additional information

Competing interests

Reviewing editor, eLife.

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Data curation, Software, Formal analysis, Validation, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing - original draft, Writing - review and editing.

Software, Formal analysis, Investigation, Visualization, Writing - review and editing.

Formal analysis, Investigation, Writing - review and editing.

Conceptualization, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Writing - original draft, Project administration, Writing - review and editing, Resources.

Conceptualization, Resources, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Writing - original draft, Project administration, Writing - review and editing.

Ethics

Human subjects: No human subjects data were collected for this paper. All data used in this paper were obtained from previous publications and publicly-available datasets in which subjects provided informed consent. Primarily, analyses were performed using data from the HCP (DC Van Essen et al. 2012, Neuroimage 62:2222-2231), including data from the HCP that were reanalyzed by subsequent studies (Benson et al. 2018, J Vis 18:23; Benson et al. 2021, bioRxiv 10.1101/2020.12.30.424856). Additionally, Figures 1 and 3 includes data replotted from previous publications by the authors (Carrasco et al. 2001, Spat Vis 15:61-75; Abrams et al. 2012, Vision Res 52:70-78; Barbot et al. 2021, J Vis 21:2), and Figure 5 includes publicly available data from Curcio et al. (1990, J Comp Neurol 292:497-523). In all cases, informed consent was obtained from subjects in the original studies, and all applicable use policies were followed in the use of the data. No personal health information is included in this paper or in the associated dataset or code.

Additional files

Transparent reporting form

Data availability

All source code and data have been permanently archived on the Open Science Framework with https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/5GPRZ.

The following dataset was generated:

Benson NC, Kupers ER, Barbot A, Carrasco M, Winawer J. 2020. Visual Performance Fields. Open Science Framework.

The following previously published datasets were used:

Benson NC. 2018. The Human Connectome Project 7 Tesla Retinotopy Dataset. Open Science Framework.

Van Essen DC. 2012. The Human Connectome Project Young Adult Dataset, 1200 Subject Release. Amazon S3. s3://hcp-openaccess/HCP_1200

Curcio CA. 1990. Human photoreceptor topography. Available as part of the ISETBIO toolbox. github.com/isetbio/isetbio

Benson NC. 2021. Variability of V1, V2, and V3 in a Large Sample of Human Observers. Open Science Framework.

References

  1. Abrams J, Nizam A, Carrasco M. Isoeccentric locations are not equivalent: the extent of the vertical meridian asymmetry. Vision Research. 2012;52:70–78. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2011.10.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Adams DL, Horton JC. A precise retinotopic map of primate striate cortex generated from the representation of angioscotomas. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2003;23:3771–3789. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-09-03771.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Aghajari S, Vinke LN, Ling S. Population spatial frequency tuning in human early visual cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2020;123:773–785. doi: 10.1152/jn.00291.2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Altpeter E, Mackeben M, Trauzettel-Klosinski S. The importance of sustained attention for patients with maculopathies. Vision Research. 2000;40:1539–1547. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(00)00059-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Alvarez I, Finlayson NJ, Ei S, de Haas B, Greenwood JA, Schwarzkopf DS. Heritable functional architecture in human visual cortex. NeuroImage. 2021;239:118286. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118286. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Amano K, Wandell BA, Dumoulin SO. Visual field maps, population receptive field sizes, and visual field coverage in the human MT+ complex. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2009;102:2704–2718. doi: 10.1152/jn.00102.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Anderson RS, Wilkinson MO, Thibos LN. Psychophysical localization of the human visual streak. Optometry and Vision Science. 1992;69:171–174. doi: 10.1097/00006324-199203000-00001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Baldwin AS, Meese TS, Baker DH. The attenuation surface for contrast sensitivity has the form of a witch's hat within the central visual field. Journal of Vision. 2012;12:23. doi: 10.1167/12.11.23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Barbot A, Abrams J, Carrasco M. Distinct mechanisms limit contrast sensitivity across retinal eccentricity and polar angle. Journal of Vision. 2019;19:43. doi: 10.1167/19.10.43. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  10. Barbot A, Xue S, Carrasco M. Asymmetries in visual acuity around the visual field. Journal of Vision. 2021;21:2. doi: 10.1167/jov.21.1.2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Barbot A, Carrasco M. Attention modifies spatial resolution according to task demands. Psychological Science. 2017;28:285–296. doi: 10.1177/0956797616679634. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Benson NC, Jamison KW, Arcaro MJ, Vu AT, Glasser MF, Coalson TS, Van Essen DC, Yacoub E, Ugurbil K, Winawer J, Kay K. The Human Connectome Project 7 Tesla retinotopy dataset: Description and population receptive field analysis. Journal of Vision. 2018;18:23. doi: 10.1167/18.13.23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Benson NC. Visual Performance Fields. swh:1:rev:81daa65b3bc69a085acab9508610f853f6a7dcb0Software Heritage. 2021a https://archive.softwareheritage.org/swh:1:rev:81daa65b3bc69a085acab9508610f853f6a7dcb0
  14. Benson NC, Yoon JMD, Forenzo D, Engel SA, Kay KN, Winawer J. Variability of the surface area of the V1, V2, and V3 maps in a large sample of human observers. bioRxiv. 2021b doi: 10.1101/2020.12.30.424856. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  15. Cameron EL, Tai JC, Carrasco M. Covert attention affects the psychometric function of contrast sensitivity. Vision Research. 2002;42:949–967. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(02)00039-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Carrasco M, Talgar CP, Cameron EL. Characterizing visual performance fields: effects of transient covert attention, spatial frequency, eccentricity, task and set size. Spatial Vision. 2001;15:61–75. doi: 10.1163/15685680152692015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Carrasco M, Williams PE, Yeshurun Y. Covert attention increases spatial resolution with or without masks: support for signal enhancement. Journal of Vision. 2002;2:467–479. doi: 10.1167/2.6.4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Corbett JE, Carrasco M. Visual performance fields: frames of reference. PLOS ONE. 2011;6:e24470. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0024470. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Cottaris NP, Jiang H, Ding X, Wandell BA, Brainard DH. A computational-observer model of spatial contrast sensitivity: effects of wave-front-based optics, cone-mosaic structure, and inference engine. Journal of Vision. 2019;19:8. doi: 10.1167/19.4.8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Curcio CA, Sloan KR, Kalina RE, Hendrickson AE. Human photoreceptor topography. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1990;292:497–523. doi: 10.1002/cne.902920402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Curcio CA, Allen KA. Topography of ganglion cells in human retina. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1990;300:5–25. doi: 10.1002/cne.903000103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Dacey DM. The mosaic of midget ganglion cells in the human retina. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1993;13:5334–5355. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.13-12-05334.1993. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Duncan RO, Boynton GM. Cortical magnification within human primary visual cortex correlates with acuity thresholds. Neuron. 2003;38:659–671. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00265-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Frisén L, Glansholm A. Optical and neural resolution in peripheral vision. Investigative Ophthalmology. 1975;14:528–536. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Fuller S, Rodriguez RZ, Carrasco M. Apparent contrast differs across the vertical meridian: visual and attentional factors. Journal of Vision. 2008;8:11–16. doi: 10.1167/8.1.16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Gagnon L, Sakadžić S, Lesage F, Musacchia JJ, Lefebvre J, Fang Q, Yücel MA, Evans KC, Mandeville ET, Cohen-Adad J, Polimeni JR, Yaseen MA, Lo EH, Greve DN, Buxton RB, Dale AM, Devor A, Boas DA. Quantifying the microvascular origin of BOLD-fMRI from first principles with two-photon microscopy and an oxygen-sensitive nanoprobe. Journal of Neuroscience. 2015;35:3663–3675. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3555-14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Gibaldi A, Benson NC, Banks MS. Crossed-uncrossed projections from primate retina are adapted to disparities of natural scenes. PNAS. 2021;118:e2015651118. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2015651118. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Greenwood JA, Szinte M, Sayim B, Cavanagh P. Variations in crowding, saccadic precision, and spatial localization reveal the shared topology of spatial vision. PNAS. 2017;114:E3573–E3582. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1615504114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Gurnsey R, Pearson P, Day D. Texture segmentation along the horizontal meridian: nonmonotonic changes in performance with eccentricity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 1996;22:738–757. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.22.3.738. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Hartmann E, Lachenmayr B, Brettel H. The peripheral critical flicker frequency. Vision Research. 1979;19:1019–1023. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(79)90227-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Himmelberg MM, Winawer J, Carrasco M. Stimulus-dependent contrast sensitivity asymmetries around the visual field. Journal of Vision. 2020;20:18. doi: 10.1167/jov.20.9.18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Horton JC, Hoyt WF. The representation of the visual field in human striate cortex. A revision of the classic holmes map. Archives of Ophthalmology. 1991;109:816–824. doi: 10.1001/archopht.1991.01080060080030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Jaeken B, Artal P. Optical quality of emmetropic and myopic eyes in the periphery measured with high-angular resolution. Investigative Opthalmology & Visual Science. 2012;53:3405–3413. doi: 10.1167/iovs.11-8993. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Kay KN, Weiner KS, Grill-Spector K. Attention reduces spatial uncertainty in human ventral temporal cortex. Current Biology. 2015;25:595–600. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.12.050. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Kay K, Jamison KW, Vizioli L, Zhang R, Margalit E, Ugurbil K. A critical assessment of data quality and venous effects in sub-millimeter fMRI. NeuroImage. 2019;189:847–869. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.02.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Klein BP, Harvey BM, Dumoulin SO. Attraction of position preference by spatial attention throughout human visual cortex. Neuron. 2014;84:227–237. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.08.047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Kroon JN, van der Wildt GJ. Spatial frequency tuning studies: weighting as a prerequisite for describing psychometric curves by probability summation. Vision Research. 1980;20:253–263. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(80)90110-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Kupers ER, Carrasco M, Winawer J. Modeling visual performance differences 'around' the visual field: A computational observer approach. PLOS Computational Biology. 2019;15:e1007063. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1007063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Kupers ER, Benson NC, Carrasco M, Winawer J. Radial asymmetries around the visual field: from retina to cortex to behavior. bioRxiv. 2020 doi: 10.1101/2020.10.20.347492. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  40. Larsson J, Heeger DJ. Two retinotopic visual areas in human lateral occipital cortex. Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;26:13128–13142. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1657-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Le R, Witthoft N, Ben-Shachar M, Wandell B. The field of view available to the ventral occipito-temporal reading circuitry. Journal of Vision. 2017;17:6. doi: 10.1167/17.4.6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Lee AT, Glover GH, Meyer CH. Discrimination of large venous vessels in time-course spiral blood-oxygen-level-dependent magnetic-resonance functional neuroimaging. Magnetic resonance in medicine. 1995;33:745–754. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910330602. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Liu T, Heeger DJ, Carrasco M. Neural correlates of the visual vertical meridian asymmetry. Journal of vision. 2006;6:1294–1306. doi: 10.1167/6.11.12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Liu T, Thibos LN. Customized models of ocular aberrations across the visual field during accommodation. Journal of Vision. 2019;19:13. doi: 10.1167/19.9.13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Mackeben M. Sustained focal attention and peripheral letter recognition. Spatial vision. 1999;12:51–72. doi: 10.1163/156856899x00030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. McGraw KO, Wong SP. "Forming inferences about some intraclass correlations coefficients": Correction. Psychological Methods. 1996;1:390. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.1.4.390. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  47. McKee SP, Nakayama K. The detection of motion in the peripheral visual field. Vision Research. 1984;24:25–32. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(84)90140-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Montaser-Kouhsari L, Carrasco M. Perceptual asymmetries are preserved in short-term memory tasks. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics. 2009;71:1782–1792. doi: 10.3758/APP.71.8.1782. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Murray SO, Boyaci H, Kersten D. The representation of perceived angular size in human primary visual cortex. Nature neuroscience. 2006;9:429–434. doi: 10.1038/nn1641. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. O'Connell C, Ho LC, Murphy MC, Conner IP, Wollstein G, Cham R, Chan KC. Structural and functional correlates of visual field asymmetry in the human brain by diffusion kurtosis MRI and functional MRI. Neuroreport. 2016;27:1225–1231. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0000000000000682. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Pedregosa F. Scikit-learn: machine learning in Python. Journal of Machine Learning Research. 2011;12:2825–2830. [Google Scholar]
  52. Pointer JS, Hess RF. The contrast sensitivity gradient across the human visual field: with emphasis on the low spatial frequency range. Vision Research. 1989;29:1133–1151. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(89)90061-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Polans J, Jaeken B, McNabb RP, Artal P, Izatt JA. Wide-field optical model of the human eye with asymmetrically tilted and decentered Lens that reproduces measured ocular aberrations. Optica. 2015;2:124–134. doi: 10.1364/OPTICA.2.000124. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  54. Purokayastha S, Roberts M, Carrasco M. Voluntary attention improves performance similarly around the visual field. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics. 2021;1:02316-y. doi: 10.3758/s13414-021-02316-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Regan D, Beverley KI. Spatial-frequency discrimination and detection: comparison of postadaptation thresholds. Journal of the Optical Society of America. 1983;73:1684–1690. doi: 10.1364/josa.73.001684. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Robson JG, Graham N. Probability summation and regional variation in contrast sensitivity across the visual field. Vision Research. 1981;21:409–418. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(81)90169-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Rodieck RW. The First Steps in Seeing. Sinauer Associates; 1998. [Google Scholar]
  58. Rosén R, Lundström L, Venkataraman AP, Winter S, Unsbo P. Quick contrast sensitivity measurements in the periphery. Journal of Vision. 2014;14:3. doi: 10.1167/14.8.3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Rovamo J, Virsu V. An estimation and application of the human cortical magnification factor. Experimental Brain Research. 1979;37:495–510. doi: 10.1007/BF00236819. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Schwarzkopf DS, Song C, Rees G. The surface area of human V1 predicts the subjective experience of object size. Nature Neuroscience. 2011;14:28–30. doi: 10.1038/nn.2706. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Silva MF, Brascamp JW, Ferreira S, Castelo-Branco M, Dumoulin SO, Harvey BM. Radial asymmetries in population receptive field size and cortical magnification factor in early visual cortex. NeuroImage. 2018;167:41–52. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.11.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Song H, Chui TY, Zhong Z, Elsner AE, Burns SA. Variation of cone photoreceptor packing density with retinal eccentricity and age. Investigative Opthalmology & Visual Science. 2011;52:7376–7384. doi: 10.1167/iovs.11-7199. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Song C, Schwarzkopf DS, Kanai R, Rees G. Neural population tuning links visual cortical anatomy to human visual perception. Neuron. 2015;85:641–656. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.12.041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Talgar CP, Carrasco M. Vertical meridian asymmetry in spatial resolution: visual and attentional factors. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2002;9:714–722. doi: 10.3758/BF03196326. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Tannazzo T, Kurylo DD, Bukhari F. Perceptual grouping across eccentricity. Vision Research. 2014;103:101–108. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2014.08.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Van Essen DC, Newsome WT, Maunsell JH. The visual field representation in striate cortex of the macaque monkey: asymmetries, anisotropies, and individual variability. Vision research. 1984;24:429–448. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(84)90041-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Van Essen DC, Ugurbil K, Auerbach E, Barch D, Behrens TE, Bucholz R, Chang A, Chen L, Corbetta M, Curtiss SW, Della Penna S, Feinberg D, Glasser MF, Harel N, Heath AC, Larson-Prior L, Marcus D, Michalareas G, Moeller S, Oostenveld R, Petersen SE, Prior F, Schlaggar BL, Smith SM, Snyder AZ, Xu J, Yacoub E, WU-Minn HCP Consortium The Human Connectome Project: a data acquisition perspective. NeuroImage. 2012;62:2222–2231. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Watson AB. A formula for human retinal ganglion cell receptive field density as a function of visual field location. Journal of Vision. 2014;14:15. doi: 10.1167/14.7.15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Wilkinson MO, Anderson RS, Bradley A, Thibos LN. Neural bandwidth of veridical perception across the visual field. Journal of Vision. 2016;16:1. doi: 10.1167/16.2.1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Winawer J, Horiguchi H, Sayres RA, Amano K, Wandell BA. Mapping hV4 and ventral occipital cortex: the venous eclipse. Journal of Vision. 2010;10:1. doi: 10.1167/10.5.1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Yang Z, Zuo XN, McMahon KL, Craddock RC, Kelly C, de Zubicaray GI, Hickie I, Bandettini PA, Castellanos FX, Milham MP, Wright MJ. Genetic and environmental contributions to functional connectivity architecture of the human brain. Cerebral Cortex. 2016;26:2341–2352. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision letter

Editor: Ming Meng1
Reviewed by: Ming Meng2, Geoffrey K Aguirre3, Xi-Nian Zuo4

Our editorial process produces two outputs: i) public reviews designed to be posted alongside the preprint for the benefit of readers; ii) feedback on the manuscript for the authors, including requests for revisions, shown below. We also include an acceptance summary that explains what the editors found interesting or important about the work.

Acceptance summary:

We believe that this manuscript would be of broad interest to readers in the field of human vision and its cortical topography as well as behavioral and genetic links. The investigation of the neurobiological basis of visual task performance asymmetries represents an important contribution to our understanding of how visual system architecture shapes perception. The key claims of the manuscript are well supported by the data, and the approaches used are thoughtful and rigorous.

Decision letter after peer review:

Thank you for submitting your article "Cortical Magnification in Human Visual Cortex Parallels Task Performance around the Visual Field" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by 3 peer reviewers, including Ming Meng as the Reviewing Editor and Reviewer #1, and the evaluation has been overseen Joshua Gold as the Senior Editor. The following individuals involved in review of your submission have agreed to reveal their identity: Geoffrey K. Aguirre (Reviewer #2); Xi-Nian Zuo (Reviewer #3).

The reviewers have discussed their reviews with one another, and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this to help you prepare a revised submission.

Essential revisions:

1. Clarify the relationship with previous findings.

Firstly, the present results demonstrate that 45 deg angles are relatively under-represented on the cortical surface, but without a concomitant decline in perceptual performance (Figure 3). While a prior study demonstrated radial asymmetries in cortical magnification (Silva 2018), that prior study did not have the power and resolution to show the clear reduction in surface area around 45 deg that is shown here. This result is one of the more novel findings of the current study, but is not discussed. It is noted that in the Barbot (2021) paper, acuity was tested with a grating that was oriented at 45 deg. Could this property of the stimulus interact with the radial orientation bias that has been shown in perception and cortical response (e.g., Sasaki 2006).

Secondly, although the results of twins are very impressive and novel from previous findings such as Silva et al., 2018, we suggest that these twins results should be more thoroughly discussed. For example, the correlations in the MZ twins may not be an independent source of information. That is, could a conclusion be drawn from the correlation in the MZ twins that there is a genetic influence specifically for radial asymmetry of the visual cortex? Instead, there may be genetic influences upon the general shape, folding, and functional organization of the cortex as a whole, of which the visual cortex is just one part. It would be informative, for example, if the correlation in MZ twins for visual cortex radial asymmetry is GREATER than the correlation that is observed for any other cortical property (Chen 2013). While we understand that perceptual data are not available for the twin cohort, it may be helpful to describe any relevant prior work on perceptual variation in twin populations (if such measurements exist).

Thirdly, the Silva 2018 paper included a more "fine scale" analysis of cortical magnification as a function of polar angle (Figure 4B). The error bars in this prior report are an order of magnitude larger than in the current measurements, but it would be helpful to have an evaluation of the degree to which the current measures agree with this prior work.

Fourthly, These is an interesting way of discussing the present findings of the human vision system by looking them at the level of the global brain system (e.g., connectomics), for example, how these vision-related heritable features are related to or implicated for their connectome-level findings (Yang et al., 2016, https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/26891986)?

Lastly but not least, Credit HCP data use: Please visit https://www.humanconnectome.org/study/hcp-young-adult/document/hcp-citations

References:

Sasaki, Yuka, et al. "The radial bias: a different slant on visual orientation sensitivity in human and nonhuman primates." Neuron 51.5 (2006): 661-670.

Chen, Chi-Hua, et al. "Genetic topography of brain morphology." Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 110.42 (2013): 17089-17094.

Silva, Maria Fatima, et al. "Radial asymmetries in population receptive field size and cortical magnification factor in early visual cortex." NeuroImage 167 (2018): 41-52.

Yang Z, Zuo XN, McMahon KL, Craddock RC, Kelly C, de Zubicaray GI, Hickie I, Bandettini PA, Castellanos FX, Milham MP, Wright MJ. Genetic and Environmental Contributions to Functional Connectivity Architecture of the Human Brain. Cereb Cortex. 2016 May;26(5):2341-2352.

2. There is the obvious confound that the horizontal meridian is represented in the depth of a sulcus, while the vertical meridian is represented close to the gyral crowns. It is recommended to add some consideration in the methods or discussion of why cortical folding can't account for the current results.

3. The statistical model on repeated measurements: in the present work, there are lots of repeated measurements recorded (e.g., Figure 1, across angular distance and meridian). It is a need of clear and comprehensive description on the statistical methods to be reported in the method part.

4. Measurement reliability: this is a fundamental concept of individual differences, which the present work is based on to assess the link between brain, behavior and genetics. The reliability levels of these measurements should be reported due to the importance of understanding the correlational outcomes. For example, In Figure 3, a surprisingly high correlation was reported (r = 0.96). How we interpret this correlation in terms of the psychometric theory of individual differences. Again, how this correlation was derived from such a setting on the repeated measurements.

Some materials for measurement reliability are listed below:

1. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/31253883/

2. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/33439234/

3. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/33685310/

4. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/33685291/

5. The cortical surface representation is described as an "amplification" of asymmetries that are present in the retina. Looking at Figure 5, however, it doesn't appear that a multiplicative scaling of the cone or midget RF functions would fit the cortical data. The cortical asymmetries are certainly larger, but they are of a different form with eccentricity. This might be worth acknowledging, and perhaps considering that perceptual measures as a function of eccentricity and polar angle could deepen the correspondence with the cortical data.

Also, as the authors noted too, "behavioral pattern may vary with task". It would be helpful in general if the relationship between the present cortical magnification finding and behavioral results could be discussed with further details.

6. ICC: should be non-negative. In Figure 4, the negative ICCs appeared for DZ twins for some polar angle widths. Please clarify the reason.

Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

1. For the results of MZ, DZ twins analysis, I would suggest to provide more information about the included twin participants, such as the numbers of twins, their age ranges, etc.

2. Figure symbols can perhaps be made larger, and avoid using colours that are a little too light.

Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

1. The introduction contains the statement "This eccentricity effect is mediated by a far greater density of photoreceptors and retinal ganglion cells (RGCs) for the fovea than periphery". This should be edited to indicate the CONE photoreceptors.

2. In Figure 2, I understand the motivation for offsetting points on the x-axis to allow them to be more visible, but I found myself wanting to compare slopes, and points of cross-over of the fit lines, and this was frustrated by the x-axis shift.

eLife. 2021 Aug 3;10:e67685. doi: 10.7554/eLife.67685.sa2

Author response


Essential revisions:

1. Clarify the relationship with previous findings.

Firstly, the present results demonstrate that 45 deg angles are relatively under-represented on the cortical surface, but without a concomitant decline in perceptual performance (Figure 3). While a prior study demonstrated radial asymmetries in cortical magnification (Silva 2018), that prior study did not have the power and resolution to show the clear reduction in surface area around 45 deg that is shown here. This result is one of the more novel findings of the current study, but is not discussed. It is noted that in the Barbot (2021) paper, acuity was tested with a grating that was oriented at 45 deg. Could this property of the stimulus interact with the radial orientation bias that has been shown in perception and cortical response (e.g., Sasaki 2006).

The cortical surface areas corresponding to the 45° angles reported in the original submission were indeed puzzling and suggestive of some bias in the data or method. We have performed a substantial re-analysis of the results using a now-published dataset of manually-drawn V1, V2, and V3 boundaries (Benson et al., 2021; DOI: 10.1101/2020.12.30.424856); our method section was also substantially updated to accommodate this dataset and some new calculations. Specifically, there are two main changes in the method. First, we now use hand-drawn boundaries of the lower, upper, and horizontal meridians, and of several iso-eccentricity contours, to identify sectors of the V1 map, rather relying on the boundaries found by an automated atlas fit. Second, we now employ a new and more robust method to carve up these sectors into fine-grained regions. The manually labeled boundaries have high inter-rater reliability and their inclusion eliminates any biases that could have derived from the automated boundary-finding method we had previously employed. This re-analysis leaves our previous findings intact and largely unchanged, and it eliminates the apparent mystery of the surface area of the 45° angle, which is no longer under-represented on cortex relative to behavior (Figure 3).

Regarding the Barbot et al. study, the 45 deg orientation was chosen so that it would not contaminate the psychophysical measures at the vertical and the horizontal meridians, as discriminability would be better along the vertical meridian for orientations off vertical and along the horizontal meridian for orientations off horizontal; it is possible that the performance at the intercardinal locations is better than if they had used 0° or 90° orientation.

Secondly, although the results of twins are very impressive and novel from previous findings such as Silva et al., 2018, we suggest that these twins results should be more thoroughly discussed. For example, the correlations in the MZ twins may not be an independent source of information. That is, could a conclusion be drawn from the correlation in the MZ twins that there is a genetic influence specifically for radial asymmetry of the visual cortex? Instead, there may be genetic influences upon the general shape, folding, and functional organization of the cortex as a whole, of which the visual cortex is just one part. It would be informative, for example, if the correlation in MZ twins for visual cortex radial asymmetry is GREATER than the correlation that is observed for any other cortical property (Chen 2013). While we understand that perceptual data are not available for the twin cohort, it may be helpful to describe any relevant prior work on perceptual variation in twin populations (if such measurements exist).

Our intention in analyzing the asymmetry correlations between twins was not to suggest that there is a genetic influence limited only to asymmetry, and we have reworded the discussion to further clarify this point. For context, we have added reports of correlations between other cortical properties [Figure 4].

Thirdly, the Silva 2018 paper included a more "fine scale" analysis of cortical magnification as a function of polar angle (Figure 4B). The error bars in this prior report are an order of magnitude larger than in the current measurements, but it would be helpful to have an evaluation of the degree to which the current measures agree with this prior work.

We have expanded our discussion of this paper in the text and have included a supplemental comparison of their data (as digitized from their Figure 4) with our data (Supplementary file 3, P34).

Fourthly, These is an interesting way of discussing the present findings of the human vision system by looking them at the level of the global brain system (e.g., connectomics), for example, how these vision-related heritable features are related to or implicated for their connectome-level findings (Yang et al., 2016, https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/26891986)?

We have expanded the Discussion and have included text regarding the previous findings of connectome-level heritability in the visual cortex.

Lastly but not least, Credit HCP data use: Please visit https://www.humanconnectome.org/study/hcp-young-adult/document/hcp-citations

We thank the reviewers for catching this oversight and have included the relevant text in the Acknowledgements.

2. There is the obvious confound that the horizontal meridian is represented in the depth of a sulcus, while the vertical meridian is represented close to the gyral crowns. It is recommended to add some consideration in the methods or discussion of why cortical folding can't account for the current results.

In the original submission we reported only the surface areas of the midgray surface (i.e., the halfway point between pial and white surfaces) as a way to minimize bias of the cortical curvature that might arise on the pial surface (where the gyral crown is expected to have a larger surface area than the sulcal valley) or the white surface (where the opposite is expected). We have now included Figure 2S1 as a supplement to Figure 2 a re-analysis of the data using both the white and pial surface areas as well as the midgray surface area. Whereas surface areas and their ratios vary numerically depending on which surface is used for analysis, the main trends hold for all 3 analyses (white, midgray, pial): there is more surface area for the horizontal than vertical and for the lower vertical than upper vertical. Unsurprisingly, this re-analysis substantially affects the HVA, which depends on the gyral and sulcal surface areas, but only slightly the VMA, which depends only on gyral surface areas. We have also added text in the Results to address this topic.

3. The statistical model on repeated measurements: in the present work, there are lots of repeated measurements recorded (e.g., Figure 1, across angular distance and meridian). It is a need of clear and comprehensive description on the statistical methods to be reported in the method part.

The data referenced in Figure 1, and, in fact, all psychophysical data we analyzed, are from previous publications in which these details were reported, including analysis using linear mixed models. We have now duplicated the relevant details from these publications in the Methods along with relevant reports of the inter-rater reliability of the V1-V2 boundaries on which the surface area calculations were based. This subsection of the Methods is now titled Statistical Analysis and Measurement Reliability [P21–22].

4. Measurement reliability: this is a fundamental concept of individual differences, which the present work is based on to assess the link between brain, behavior and genetics. The reliability levels of these measurements should be reported due to the importance of understanding the correlational outcomes. For example, In Figure 3, a surprisingly high correlation was reported (r = 0.96). How we interpret this correlation in terms of the psychometric theory of individual differences. Again, how this correlation was derived from such a setting on the repeated measurements.

Some materials for measurement reliability are listed below:

1. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/31253883/

2. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/33439234/

3. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/33685310/

4. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/33685291/

We have added a section on Statistical Analysis and Measurement Reliability in the Methods section to address the topic of reliability [P21–22]. Additionally, we note that the correlation from Figure 3 is a correlation of mean values across subjects using different subject groups for the x and y axes and thus should not be interpreted as a finding about individual differences. We have clarified this fact in the text.

5. The cortical surface representation is described as an "amplification" of asymmetries that are present in the retina. Looking at Figure 5, however, it doesn't appear that a multiplicative scaling of the cone or midget RF functions would fit the cortical data. The cortical asymmetries are certainly larger, but they are of a different form with eccentricity. This might be worth acknowledging, and perhaps considering that perceptual measures as a function of eccentricity and polar angle could deepen the correspondence with the cortical data.

In this instance we used the word “amplification” loosely to mean that the asymmetries in cortex were consistently higher than the asymmetries in the retina, not in the mathematical sense of a multiplicative scale factor. We have now clarified this in the text, and we have expanded the discussion of this point.

Also, as the authors noted too, "behavioral pattern may vary with task". It would be helpful in general if the relationship between the present cortical magnification finding and behavioral results could be discussed with further details.

We have elaborated on this point in the Results section.

6. ICC: should be non-negative. In Figure 4, the negative ICCs appeared for DZ twins for some polar angle widths. Please clarify the reason.

We have clarified our use of an unbiased estimate of the ICC in the Methods and have provided the formulae for our calculations [Equations 1–2; P20].

Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

1. For the results of MZ, DZ twins analysis, I would suggest to provide more information about the included twin participants, such as the numbers of twins, their age ranges, etc.

We have expanded our discussion of the twin results substantially and have

provided relevant demographic information.

2. Figure symbols can perhaps be made larger, and avoid using colours that are a little too light.

For Figure 4, we have now increased the marker size. For Figure 2, we plot error bars without any markers. The reason is that the error bars are small, and symbols would obscure the error bars. Nonetheless, because the data points are connected by lines, the values are easy to see and the different series are distinct from one another. We have spent substantial effort configuring the color palette and layout of the figures and have received frequent positive feedback about their appearance. All figures are friendly to readers with common forms of color-blindness, and all data are plotted in vector-formats in order to enable readers to resize the figures freely. Accordingly, we are disinclined to fiddle with their appearance without a specific problem of clarity to address.

However, we have published a Jupyter notebook, included in this project’s original GitHub and OSF repositories, which contains all code for generating the figures as well as documentation and additional details. In particular, the first code cell in this notebook contains a configuration dictionary for the plot colors used in the figures. We invite readers with different aesthetic preferences to edit this dictionary then re-evaluate the relevant sections of the notebook in order to obtain plots with whatever styling they prefer.

Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

1. The introduction contains the statement "This eccentricity effect is mediated by a far greater density of photoreceptors and retinal ganglion cells (RGCs) for the fovea than periphery". This should be edited to indicate the CONE photoreceptors.

We have fixed this throughout the manuscript.

2. In Figure 2, I understand the motivation for offsetting points on the x-axis to allow them to be more visible, but I found myself wanting to compare slopes, and points of cross-over of the fit lines, and this was frustrated by the x-axis shift.

We have fixed this (Figure 2).

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Data Citations

    1. Benson NC, Kupers ER, Barbot A, Carrasco M, Winawer J. 2020. Visual Performance Fields. Open Science Framework. [DOI]
    2. Benson NC. 2018. The Human Connectome Project 7 Tesla Retinotopy Dataset. Open Science Framework. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
    3. Van Essen DC. 2012. The Human Connectome Project Young Adult Dataset, 1200 Subject Release. Amazon S3. s3://hcp-openaccess/HCP_1200
    4. Curcio CA. 1990. Human photoreceptor topography. Available as part of the ISETBIO toolbox. github.com/isetbio/isetbio
    5. Benson NC. 2021. Variability of V1, V2, and V3 in a Large Sample of Human Observers. Open Science Framework. [DOI]

    Supplementary Materials

    Transparent reporting form

    Data Availability Statement

    All data analyzed in this project was previously available from other sources. The results of all analyses and the source code required to produce them, as well as documentation, are publicly available at an Open Science Framework website (https://osf.io/5gprz/; DOI:10.17605/OSF.IO/5GPRZ) with the exception of analyses that rely on restricted data from the HCP, in which case code that will reproduce the analyses given the restricted data is provided. This paper’s Github repository can be found at https://github.com/noahbenson/performance-fields/ (copy archived at swh:1:rev:81daa65b3bc69a085acab9508610f853f6a7dcb0Benson, 2021a).

    All source code and data have been permanently archived on the Open Science Framework with https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/5GPRZ.

    The following dataset was generated:

    Benson NC, Kupers ER, Barbot A, Carrasco M, Winawer J. 2020. Visual Performance Fields. Open Science Framework.

    The following previously published datasets were used:

    Benson NC. 2018. The Human Connectome Project 7 Tesla Retinotopy Dataset. Open Science Framework.

    Van Essen DC. 2012. The Human Connectome Project Young Adult Dataset, 1200 Subject Release. Amazon S3. s3://hcp-openaccess/HCP_1200

    Curcio CA. 1990. Human photoreceptor topography. Available as part of the ISETBIO toolbox. github.com/isetbio/isetbio

    Benson NC. 2021. Variability of V1, V2, and V3 in a Large Sample of Human Observers. Open Science Framework.


    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES