Abstract
Here we attempted to define the relationship between: EEG activity, personality and coping during lockdown. We were in a unique situation since the COVID-19 outbreak interrupted our independent longitudinal study. We already collected a significant amount of data before lockdown. During lockdown, a subgroup of participants willingly continued their engagement in the study. These circumstances provided us with an opportunity to examine the relationship between personality/cognition and brain rhythms in individuals who continued their engagement during lockdown compared to control data collected well before pandemic. The testing consisted of a one-time assessment of personality dimensions and two sessions of EEG recording and deductive reasoning task. Participants were divided into groups based on the time they completed the second session: before or during the COVID-19 outbreak ‘Pre-pandemic Controls’ and ‘Pandemics’, respectively. The Pandemics were characterized by a higher extraversion and stronger connectivity, compared to Pre-pandemic Controls. Furthermore, the Pandemics improved their cognitive performance under long-term stress as compared to the Pre-Pandemic Controls matched for personality traits to the Pandemics. The Pandemics were also characterized by increased EEG connectivity during lockdown. We posit that stronger EEG connectivity and higher extraversion could act as a defense mechanism against stress-related deterioration of cognitive functions.
Subject terms: Cognitive neuroscience, Personality, Stress and resilience
Introduction
A personality trait is a stable psychological characteristic that influences an individual’s thoughts, feelings, and behavior1. Traits such as neuroticism and extraversion appear to be key factors that predict adherence to health measures. Individuals who score high on neuroticism are often worried about their health2 and are more likely to maintain healthy habits3,4. In contrast, extraverts seek social engagements and their neural networks are activated to a greater extent by external stimuli5,6.
Investigations tempting to define the relationship between brain activity and personality focused on resting-state and connectivity as the potential explanatory factors. Several studies have suggested an interrelation between personality traits and patterns of whole brain resting state functional connectivity7,8, although these results have been challenged (for review see9). By contrast, it is generally accepted that individual differences in cognitive performance are mediated by differences in dynamical neural systems and brain-wide interactions. Dynamic responses of resting-state networks affect memory performance10,11 and visual attention12–14. Importantly, both memory and attention influence adherence to COVID-19 pandemic regulations15 and vulnerability to stress16,17, a finding which could be related back to individual variations in brain connectivity. Indeed, several recent studies have found stress coping strategies are related to resting state functional connectivity18,19. Improved understanding about which inter-individual factors shape behavioral responses to perceived threats is crucial for predicting and developing relevant actions mitigating unwanted and risky behavior.
Here we attempted to define the relationship between EEG activity and coping during lockdown. We were in a unique situation since the COVID-19 outbreak interrupted our independent longitudinal EEG neurofeedback study. We had already collected a significant amount of control data well before lockdown. During lockdown, a subgroup of participants willingly continued their engagement in the study. This unique set of circumstances provided us with an opportunity to examine the relationship between personality/cognition and associated brain rhythms in individuals who continued their engagement during lockdown compared to control data collected well before pandemic.
Results
We compared personality dimensions, cognitive function, and EEG between subjects who continued their training during the COVID-19 pandemic and those who did not. The study design included: a “Pre-pandemic Controls” who completed two test sessions (EEG recording and cognitive tasks) before the COVID-19 outbreak. The “Pandemics” had their first session before the COVID-19 outbreak and the second during lock-down. Both groups completed psychological assessments before the pandemic, and a questionnaire about fear of COVID-19 during lockdown.
Characteristics and comparison of the Pre-pandemic Control vs Pandemic group
Fear of COVID-19 and Socioeconomic Status (SES)
The results of the fear of COVID-19 questionnaire are presented in Suppl. Inf. Table S1. A two-tailed Wilcoxon test did not show a difference between the fear medians (p = 0.76).
In our study, we also examined SES and found the dominant group of subjects were people with higher SES in both Pre-pandemic and Pandemic groups. Group statistics of SES are shown in Suppl. Inf. Table S2.
Personality dimensions
During Session-1 (before pandemic), we found a higher extraversion score (p < 0.05, Mann–Whitney U test) in the Pandemic group compared to the Pre-pandemic Control (Fig. 1A) and no differences for neuroticism scores (Fig. 1B).
Global connectivity index
Higher extraversion scores of the Pandemics, led us to speculate that biopsychological differences proposed by Eysenck6 might be detectable in EEG. We analyzed differences in connectivity using the Global Connectivity Index (GCI; see Fig. 2 legend), as similar measures appeared to be a better predictor of cognitive performance than detailed connectivity metrics20.
Analyses of the GCI for Session-1 using a two factor ANOVA for group (Pre-pandemic Control, n = 44 and Pandemic, n = 18) and EEG band (theta, alpha, beta-1, beta-2) revealed a significant effect of group (F = 497.59, p < 0.01) and EEG band (F = 71.74, p < 0.01) but no significance for interaction (F = 1.79, p = 0.15). Subsequent Tukey–Kramer post-hoc testing revealed significantly higher (p < 0.01) GCI values in the Pandemic group compared to controls for all EEG bands (Fig. 2).
Given the differences between the groups in GCI, we examined connectivity using mean PLVs for all pairs of electrodes (matrix 57X57) in all EEG bands. The resulting connectivity matrix can be used to assess whether GCI differences are local—spread over specific head areas or global. Additionally, the strength of EEG connectivity between frontal and parietal brain areas has recently been shown to be related to task performance2 and personality dimensions21. Student’s two-tailed t-test with Bonferroni correction confirmed higher PLVs in the Pandemic group for most electrode pairs across all frequency bands (theta: higher PLVs for 802 pairs, lower = 56, alpha: higher = 800, lower = 56, beta-1: higher = 798, lower = 59, beta-2: higher = 794, lower = 90). PLV differences for all four EEG bands are shown in Fig. 3.
Transitive reasoning task
Inter-individual patterns of brain activity have been proposed to contribute to differences in cognitive performance in tasks of attention22 and working memory23,24. Using a transitive reasoning task, which relies on both attention and memory, we next investigated whether connectivity differences between the two groups were associated with differences in cognitive function.
Participants were tested two times 8–12 weeks apart; Pre-pandemic Control group – before pandemic (Session-1 and 2); Pandemic group – before pandemic (Session-1) and during pandemic (Session-2). Accuracy and reaction times did not differ between the groups for Session-1 or Session-2 (Fig. 4). Friedman ANOVA, and subsequent post hoc analyses, revealed that in both groups accuracy significantly increased in the second vs. first session for moderate and difficult, but not easy, versions of the task. In the Pre-pandemic Control group reaction times were faster, in Session-2, for all versions of the task, whereas in the Pandemic group faster reaction times were found only for the easy task (Fig. 4, Suppl. Inf. Table S3).
Considering that Pre-pandemic Control and Pandemic groups differed on scores of extraversion and EEG connectivity we speculated that this could influence the differences in reaction time between Session-1 and 2.
Assessment of the impact of COVID-19 lockdown using Pre-pandemic controls matched for personality traits
We attempted to control for personality differences by selecting individuals from the Pre-pandemic Controls to match Pandemics subjects for personality traits, SES, sex and age. We retrospectively analyzed 18 participants and termed this group “Matched Controls” (see Suppl. Inf. Table S4 for details). Matched Controls and Pandemics had similar scores for fear of COVID-19 (median based on all questions for both groups: med = 11.5, Wilcoxon test p = 0.75) and similar scores in transitive reasoning task and GCI (see “Effect of pandemic threat on performance in a transitive reasoning task” and “Assessment of impact of pandemic threat on GCI and PLV” sections).
Effect of pandemic threat on performance in a transitive reasoning task
Comparison of Pandemics and Matched Controls revealed a significant difference (Friedman test) in accuracy for the difficult task variant, and in reaction times for the easy variant. Post-hoc analyses for sessions and groups did not find significant differences. Within group comparisons (Session-1 vs. Session-2) revealed improved accuracy and shorter reaction times for the Pandemics alone (Fig. 5). Thus, excluding the pandemic threat as a factor underlying these effects in the Pandemics (see Suppl. Inf. Table S5).
To exclude differences in EEG connectivity as a potential factor influencing reaction we compared GCI scores in Pandemics and Matched Controls. We found no group differences for Session-1 for any band (theta: p = 0.18; alpha: p = 0.18; beta-1: p = 0.27; beta-2: p = 0.14; one-way ANOVA, post-hoc Tukey–Kramer test) or in the fear of COVID-19 level (p = 0.76, Wilcoxon test). Thus, GCI and extraversion were excluded as potential factors accounting for the difference in cognitive performance.
Assessment of impact of pandemic threat on GCI and PLV
Repeated measures ANOVA was used to assess the effect of COVID threat on GCI (group*EEG band) for all EEG bands. We found significant differences for group and group*session interactions for all bands (theta: group factor F = 254.49, interaction F = 447.98; alpha: group factor F = 254.06, interaction F = 447.66; beta-1: group factor F = 260.38, interaction F = 457.26; beta-2: group factor F = 241.20, interaction F = 437.63; all significant at p < 0.01). Subsequent post-hoc tests showed no difference between the groups in Session-1 (p > 0.3), but significant differences in Session-2 for all bands. In Session-2 we observed increased GCI values for all bands in the Pandemics (theta: diff = 0.0178, alpha: diff = 0.0179, beta-1: diff = 0.0182, beta-2: diff = 0.0200; all significant at p < 0.01), while for Matched Controls GCI decreased (theta: diff = -0.0126, alpha: diff = -0.0126, beta-1: diff = -0.0121, beta-2: diff = -0.0165; all significant at p < 0.01).
The same pattern of connectivity differences was found for all pairs of electrode signals. Figure 6 shows PLV differences (Bonferroni corrected) calculated between the Pandemics and Matched Controls for the beta-2 band for Session-1 and 2 and between both sessions (Fig. SF1 in Suppl. Inf. shows differences for all investigated bands). The strengthening of connectivity measured for signals recorded from most pairs of electrodes was observed in the Pandemic group between Session-1 (before lockdown) and Session-2 (during lockdown) suggests interrelation with the threat of the pandemic.
Discussion
Here, we attempted to identify if a set of psychological and physiological traits characterized participants who willingly continued training during lockdown. To this end we compared Pandemics with a Pre-pandemic Controls and its subgroup—a Matched Controls paired for personality, sex, and age with the Pandemic group. We found that Pandemics were characterized by higher intensity of extraversion and stronger EEG connectivity but also stable results of cognitive task during lockdown. Notably, these groups did not differ on fear of COVID-19, and thus subjective evaluation of the perceived threat between the groups could not account for the difference we observed. As such, other biopsychological factors should explain the differences between the Pandemics and Control groups.
Stress caused by social isolation25,26 and fear can increase brain activation by rerouting mental resources and decreasing efficiency15, which in the long-term may lead to anxiety disorders27,28. The same stressors are associated with the current pandemic29, but the extent of their adverse effects appears to depend on personality traits30,31. Psychological investigations report that higher extraversion is associated with lower stress levels32–34. This relationship may arise from a reduced physiological response to stress in extraverts35,36, their tendency to appraise a situation as less threatening37, and positive reinterpretation38. Consequently, extraverts might be more optimistic when faced with threats and/or use social engagement as a stress-coping mechanism39. Indeed, our results showed that individuals who continued to participate in this study, despite the increased risk of COVID-19, had higher extraversion compared to the controls (comp. Figure 1). Considering that extraversion is associated with a drive for social interaction29,40,41 and stimulation42, social reward may have been sufficiently motivating in the Pandemic group to outweigh fear of COVID-19.
Other specific traits of the Pandemic group (compared to Pre-pandemic Controls) were higher GCI and stronger PLVs. To date, only a few studies have addressed patterns of EEG connectivity and stress/fear in healthy subjects16,43. In particular Alonso and colleagues43 showed that stress induced by cognitive task led to increased connectivity in the beta-2 band. This finding is consistent with our observations, but in our case the extent of increased connectivity spanned over all investigated bands probably due to higher and persistent stress caused by the pandemic threat.
Theoretical considerations indicate that strong EEG connectivity in the beta-2 band is highly stable44,45, less prone to disturbance44,46 and consequently results in less dynamic processing and behavior47,48. This lower reconfiguration capability is also in line with our previous investigation which showed that subjects with stronger PLVs exhibited less flexible connection patterns49. Negative correlations between the strength of EEG connectivity and performance in attention and memory tasks has been confirmed in ADHD patients50–52. These studies showed that strong parietal and occipital connectivity correlated with inattention type ADHD. In the context of the current study, the decision by Pandemics to continue engagement in the study, despite risk, could be reinforced by a lower reconfiguration capability of neuronal networks and subsequently, reduced capacity to adjust their behavior to current environmental conditions49. Thus, lower network reconfiguration capacity, combined with higher extraversion appear to be associated with the decision to continue participation in this study. Exposure to a threat and associated stress, could lead to the increased EEG connectivity we observed in the cognitive test session, which took place during lockdown [comp. 43]. By contrast, under control conditions (before COVID-19) we observed decreased PLV scores in the second session in both control groups.
Exposure to stress among Pandemics was associated with faster reaction times in a transitive reasoning task compared to Matched Controls (Fig. 5) and no change in reaction time, in its two most difficult variants, compared to Pre-pandemic Controls (Fig. 4). These seemingly contradictory observations may result from strong EEG connectivity specific for Pandemics and Matched Controls. Lower capability of behavioral adjustment caused by strong connectivity49 could result in worse performance of Matched Controls under control conditions. Only strong stimulation by pandemic threat allowed network reconfiguration of the Pandemic group leading to outperformance of Matched Controls. Paradoxically, such a mechanism might not necessarily be disadvantageous—in stressful situations strong EEG connectivity and low capability of behavioral adjustment may help to protect cognitive resources against deterioration and subsequent decrease of efficiency, although at the same time it could also result in less adherence to pandemic regulations.
Methods
Study design
The study included two groups recruited for an independent, ongoing neurofeedback experiment. Independent analysis yielded that neurofeedback training did not affect neither the cognitive abilities nor the EEG characteristics of the participants who had completed the experiment, and the other group who’s training period happened during lock-down had their two examinations (separated by two months) before awaited training and therefore changes in their behavioral performance were neither expected. None participants from either group reported COVID-19 contagion. Both groups completed psychological assessments and repeated testing of cognitive tasks and resting-state EEG (Session-1 and Session-2, two months apart; Suppl. Inf. Table S6). Participants from the first group who completed all examinations before the pandemic formed the Pre-pandemic Control group, while participants whose' second testing session fell within the lockdown period were qualified to the Pandemic group (in contrast to five subjects who decided to discontinue their engagement after the announcement of a lock-down). The Fear of COVID-19 questionnaire was administered to both groups mean = 55.53 (SD = 12.37) days after the announced lockdown (i.e., between April and May 2020).
In the final step, we aimed at assessment of the impact of the pandemic threat on cognitive and electrophysiological features of brain activity. To this end, we selected from Pre-pandemic Controls participants matching personality traits, sex, age, and socio-economic status (SES) of the Pandemics (the Matched Controls). Next we compared differences between Session-1 and Session-2 for the Pandemics and Matched Controls.
Participants
The procedures were approved by the Local Bioethics Committee at Nicolaus Copernicus University in Torun. All participants gave their written informed consent to participate in the experiment in accordance with the WMA Declaration of Helsinki – Ethical Principles for Medical Research Involving Human Subjects. All experiments were performed under all relevant guidelines and regulations.
We examined 62 healthy adults (34 women) recruited through announcements at local universities and work agencies. The exclusion criteria were based on neurological screening and questionnaires and included neurological disorders, brain injury, current use of analgesic medication, substance abuse or dependence, and mental disorders. All participants were right-handed and had a normal or corrected-to-normal vision. The mean ± standard deviation of their age was 26.8 ± 4.7 ranging between 23 and 46 years.
The groups and times of their testing are summarized in Supplementary materials in Suppl. Inf. Table S6.
Personality dimensions
Personality dimensions (i.e., extraversion—E and neuroticism—N) were assessed using the paper-and-pencil individually administered Polish version of the revised Eysenck Personality Questionnaire5.
The Polish version52 consisted of 106 dichotomous items (yes/no) to assess the three dimensions of personality (extraversion, neuroticism, and psychoticism in E, N, and P scales, respectively) and the tendency to lie or distort responses in a favorable direction (L Scale). We focused on extraversion and neuroticism (E and N Scales). The result of a given scale consisted of the sum of points obtained in response to the questions it comprised, i.e., 23 for E scale (e.g., “Do you enjoy meeting new people?”), and 24 for the N scale (e.g., “Would you call yourself a nervous person”). The Polish version of the EPQ-R was shown to possess good reliability (α = 0.62–0.72, 0.78-0.81, 0.86-0.88, and 0.76–84 for P, E, N, and L scales, assessed in the Polish standardization trials and the four-factor structure. The reliability of the E and N Scales in the current sample was also good (α = 0.71 and 0.74, for E and N scales, respectively).
The raw scores for E and N Scales were converted into standard scores using the following formula (the sten scale): (Z-score × 2) + 5.5, in relation to groups distinguished by sex and age52.
The fear of COVID-19 scale
The computerized, self-administered scale of Fear of COVID-19 (the FCV-19S;53; Polish translation: Gola, 2020) was used to assess the fear of coronavirus. The questionnaire consisted of seven items: 1. “I am most afraid of COVID-19”, 2. “It makes me uncomfortable to think about Corona”, 3. “My hands become clammy when I think about Corona”, 4. “I am afraid of losing my life because of Corona”, 5. “When watching news and stories about Corona on social media, I become nervous or anxious”, 6. “I cannot sleep because I’m worried about getting Corona”, 7. “My heart races or palpitates when I think about getting Corona”. Participants responded to each item on a five-point Likert scale (from “strongly disagree” to “strongly agree”). A total score (ranged from 7 to 35) was calculated by summing the scores obtained for all items.
The original version of the questionnaire was shown to present a good internal consistency (α = 0.82) and the one-factor structure (confirmatory factor analyses). Our sample also showed good reliability (Cronbach’s α of 0.839).
Transitive reasoning task
The laboratory task, which effectively evaluates the effect of threat on cognition, should engage cognitive functions that are vulnerable to anxiety-induced changes (such as working memory and attention). We chose a transitive reasoning test that requires simultaneous processing, maintenance, and manipulation of information, which requires effective attention and working memory capacities54.
The version used in the current study was described in detail by Chuderski55. Briefly, we used three pairs of Greek letters with greater than or less than symbols. They were displayed for 10 s in the center of the screen, for example: (Ψ > Ω) (Ω > ή) (έ < ή) describing the relationship between the putative values of four different letters. Participants had to deduce the order of the four elements. After 10 s of familiarization, three new pairs appeared on the screen below the three original pairs with only one of them correct. The correct answer should match the guessed arrangement of the original rule. Participants were allowed 10 s for the answer. The task consisted of three conditions: 1. The easiest: where the premises and the elements inside them were arranged linearly from left to right on the basis of "greatest" to "smallest" or vice versa with equal probability; 2. Medium: The same as the easiest but the order of one element was random (keeping the relationship within elements); 3. Difficult: The same as the easiest but the order of two elements was random (keeping the relationship within elements). The whole task consisted of 60 trials.
EEG recording and preprocessing
Four minutes resting-state eyes open EEG was recorded in both sessions with 128 Ag/AgCl electrodes (Quick Amp; Brain Products GmbH, extended 10–20 system, sampling rate of 1000 Hz), reference at FCz and ground at FPz electrode. The impedance of electrodes was below 10 KΩ. The preprocessing included 0.5–70 Hz filtering, baseline correction, exclusion of 1 s data segments containing artifacts, and independent component analysis (ICA). The identified eye movement and muscle artifacts components were removed (maximum 10% of all ICA components).
Connectivity analyses
Spontaneous brain activity of the large-scale EEG and fMRI networks can predict cognitive performance49,56,57 we seized the opportunity to investigate the effect of the threat on the EEG connectivity. As a proxy we used phase-locking value [PLV, 58]. PLV is used for connectivity estimation in EEG/MEG studies59–62. PLV does not depend on spectral power and is robust to noisy signals63. Zero-phase correlations present the PLV provide information enabling prediction of the task results, not possible otherwise64. Methods preserving zero-phase correlations (PLV and AEC) significantly relate to the results of fMRI analyses unlike others65.
To compute PLV in a given frequency range, we filtered the EEG data using a two-sided finite impulse response filter and then subjected them to a Hilbert transform for computation of the instantaneous amplitude and phase. Only the phase component was used for PLV computation. The PLVs were calculated for 1-s non-overlapping epochs. The number of epochs was limited to the shortest EEG resting-state signal which remained after preprocessing, i.e. to 140 epochs. For the purpose of analyses, epochs were group averaged yielding the same number of samples (140) independently of the group size. As a general measure of connectivity in the given frequency band, we averaged PLVs of all electrode pairs (Global Connectivity Index—GCI). Similar global measures of EEG connectivity were proposed as potential biomarkers of different abnormal brain states66,67.
Due to high correlations of the signals collected from closely located electrodes in a 128 cap set we used subset matching standard positions of the 64 electrode cap (AF7, AF3, AFz, AF4, AF8, F7, F5, F3, F1, Fz, F2, F4, F6, F8, FT7, FC5, FC3, FC1, FCz, FC2, FC4, FC6, FT8, T7, C5, C3, C1, Cz, C2, C4, C6, T8, TP7, CP5, BP3, CP1, CPz, CP2, CP4, CP6, TP8, P7, P5, P3, P1, Pz, P2, P4, P6, P8, PO7, PO3, POz, PO4, PO8, O1, Oz and O2) except for the 7 electrodes located near jaws and neck, usually most contaminated by muscle artifacts.
Assessment of the impact of pandemic threat on the cognitive and electrophysiological processes
The assessment was performed by comparing within-group differences between Session 2 and Session 1 in the Pandemic and Matched Control groups. The Matched Control group consisted of participants selected from the Pre-pandemic Control group that matched the Pandemic subjects in terms of personality dimensions, sex, age, and socioeconomic status.
Statistical methods
Data were tested for normality by the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test and checked for the presence of outliers. The values deviating from the mean for more than three standard deviations were removed. Comparison of the groups were conducted using two-tailed two-sample t-tests or ANOVA. In cases of small samples, the Man-Whitney test and Friedman ANOVA with Chi square post-hoc test, were used. All PLV analyses were performed on single trial data. Results were corrected for false positives using Bonferroni adjustment where appropriate unless otherwise stated. Analyses and figures were created using MATLAB 2020a (connectivity analyses), R 4.0.4 (personality dimensions and SES analyses) and JASP (behavioral statistics).
Supplementary Information
Acknowledgements
Supported by the National Centre for Research and Development; Grant POIR-01.01.01-00-178/15.
Author contributions
J.R. wrote the main manuscript text, analyzed EEG data and prepared figures J.D. wrote the fear of COVID-19 and personality dimensions text, analyzed fear of COVID-19 and psychological data and prepared figures U.M. analyzed data from cognitive tasks M.W. analyzed data from cognitive tasks and prepared figures A.P. wrote introduction and reviewed fear of COVID-19 and personality dimensions text I.A. prepared EEG data for analyses A.W. reviewed the manuscript.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary Information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41598-021-96718-5.
References
- 1.Cervone DC, Pervin LA. Trait theory: the five-factor model; applications and evaluation of trait approaches to personality. In: Cervone D, Pervin LA, editors. Personality: Theory and Research. 12. Wiley; 2013. pp. 261–300. [Google Scholar]
- 2.van Dijk SDM, Hanssen D, Naarding P, Lucassen P, Comijs H, Oude Voshaar R. Big five personality traits and medically unexplained symptoms in later life. Eur. Psychiatry. 2016;38:23–30. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2016.05.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Meshram S, Gattani D, Shewale A, Bodele S. Association of personality traits with oral health status: a cross-sectional study. Int. J. Indian Psychol. 2012 doi: 10.25215/0402.167. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Shanker RK, Mohamed M, Hegde S, Kumar M. Influence of personality traits on gingival health. J. Indian Soc. Periodontol. 2013;17(1):58–62. doi: 10.4103/0972-124X.107476. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Eysenck HJ, Eysenck SBG. Manual of the Eysenck Personality Scales (EPS Adult) Including EPQ-Revised (EPQ-R), EPQ-R Short Scale, Impulsiveness (IVE) Questionnaire. Hodder and Stoughton Educational; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- 6.Eysenck HJ. Personality and extra-sensory perception. J. Soc. Psychical Res. 1967;44(732):55–71. [Google Scholar]
- 7.Harmon-Jones E, Gable PA. On the role of asymmetric frontal cortical activity in approach and withdrawal motivation: an updated review of the evidence. Psychophysiology. 2020;55:e12879. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12879. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Vecchio A, De Pascalis V. EEG resting asymmetries and frequency oscillations in approach/avoidance personality traits: a systematic review. Symmetry. 2020;12(10):1712. doi: 10.3390/sym12101712. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Braun U, Schäfer A, Walter H, Erk S, Romanczuk-Seiferth N, Haddad L, Schweiger JI, Grimm O, Heinz A, Tost H, Meyer-Lindenberg A, Bassett DS. Dynamic reconfiguration of frontal brain networks during executive cognition in humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2015;112(37):11678–11683. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1422487112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Kim D, Jeong W, Kim JS, Chung CK. Single-trial EEG connectivity of default mode network before and during encoding predicts subsequent memory outcome. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 2020;14:591675. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2020.591675. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Rouhinen S, Siebenhühner F, Palva JM, Palva S. Spectral and anatomical patterns of large-scale synchronization predict human attentional capacity. Cereb. Cortex. 2020;30:5293–5308. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhaa110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Madhyastha TM, Askren MK, Boord P, Grabowski TJ. Dynamic connectivity at rest predicts attention task performance. Brain Connect. 2015 doi: 10.1089/brain.2014.0248. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Spadone S, Penna DS, Sestieri C, Betti V, Tosoni A, Perrucci MG, Romani GL, Corbetta M. Reorganization of RSNs during attention. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2015;112(26):8112–8117. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1415439112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Fellman D, Ritakallio L, Waris O, Jylkkä J, Laine M. Beginning of the pandemic: COVID-19-elicited anxiety as a predictor of working memory performance. Front. Psychol. 2020;11:576466. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.576466. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Qin SZ, Hermans EJ, van Marle HJF, Fernandez LG. Acute psychological stress reduces working memory-related activity in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Biol. Psychiatry. 2009;66:25–32. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.03.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Nair N, Hegarty JP, II, Ferguson BJ, Hecht PM, Tilley M, Christ SE, Beversdorf DQ. Effects of stress on functional connectivity during problem solving. Neuroimage. 2020;208:116407. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116407. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Santarnecchi E, Polizzotto GG, Rossi ANR. Efficiency of weak brain connections support general cognitive functioning. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2014;35(9):1065–9471. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22495. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Haartsen R, Jones EJH, Orekhova EV, et al. Functional EEG connectivity in infants associates with later restricted and repetitive behaviours in autism; a replication study. Transl. Psychiatry. 2019;9:66. doi: 10.1038/s41398-019-0380-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.van Marle HJF, Hermans EJ, Qin S, Fernández G. Enhanced resting-state connectivity of amygdala in the immediate aftermath of acute psychological stress. Neuroimage. 2010;53:348–354. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.070. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Zhao W, Palmer CE, Thompson WK, Chaarani B, Garavan HP, Casey BJ, Jernigan TL, Dale AM, Fan CC. Individual differences in cognitive performance are better predicted by global rather than localized BOLD activity patterns across the cortex. Cereb. Cortex. 2021;31(3):1478–1488. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhaa290. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Klados MA, Konstantinidi P, Dacosta-Aguayo R, Kostaridou VD, Vinciarelli A, Zervakis M. Automatic recognition of personality profiles using EEG functional connectivity during emotional processing. Brain Sci. 2020;10(5):278. doi: 10.3390/brainsci10050278. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Kelly SP, Gomez-Ramirez M, Foxe JJ. Spatial attention modulates initial afferent activity in human primary visual cortex. Cereb. Cortex. 2008;18(11):2629–2636. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Hampson M, Driesen NR, Skudlarski P, Gore JC, Constable RT. Brain connectivity related to working memory performance. J. Neurosci. 2006;26(51):13338–13343. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3408-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Gordon EM, Stollstorff M, Vaidya CJ. Using spatial multiple regression to identify intrinsic connectivity networks involved in working memory performance. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2012;33:1536–1552. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21306. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Jaremka LM, Andridge RR, Fagundes CP, Alfano CM, Povoski SP, Lipari AM, Agnese DM, Arnold MW, Farrar WB, Yee LD, Carson III WE, Bekaii-Saab T, Martin Jr. EW, Schmidt CR, Kiecolt-Glaser JK, Pain depression, and fatigue: Loneliness as a longitudinal risk factor. Health Psychol. 2014;33(9):948–957. doi: 10.1037/a0034012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Cacioppo JT, Hawkley LC, Norman GJ, Berntson GG. Social isolation. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2011;1231(1):17–22. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06028.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Mathews A, MacLeod C. Cognitive vulnerability to emotional disorders. Ann. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2005;1:167–195. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.1.102803.143916. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Rodriguez BF, Bruce SE, Pagano ME, Keller MB. Relationships among psychosocial functioning, diagnostic comorbidity, and the recurrence of generalized anxiety disorder, panic disorder, and major depression. J. Anxiety Disorders. 2005;19(7):752–766. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2004.10.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Liu S, Lithopoulos A, Zhang C-Q, Garcia-Barrera MA, Rhodes RE. Personality and perceived stress during COVID-19 pandemic: testing the mediating role of perceived threat and efficacy. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2021;168:110351. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2020.110351. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Abdelrahman M. Personality traits, risk perception, and protective behaviors of Arab residents of Qatar during the COVID-19 pandemic. Int. J. Ment. Health Addiction. 2020 doi: 10.1007/s11469-020-00352-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Zettler I, Schild C, Lilleholt L, Kroencke L, Utesch T, Moshagen M, Böhm R, Back MD, Geukes K. The role of personality in COVID-19 related perceptions, evaluations, and behaviors: Findings across five samples, nine traits, and 17 criteria. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 2020 doi: 10.31234/osf.io/pkm2a. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Grant S, Langan-Fox J. Occupational stress, coping and strain: the combined/interactive effect of the big five traits. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2006;41:719–732. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2006.03.008. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Bouchard G, Guilllemette A, Landry-Leger N. Situational and dispositional coping: an examination of their relation to personality. Cognitive appraisals, and psychological distress. Eur. J. Personal. 2004;18:221–238. doi: 10.1002/per.512. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Langford C, Glendon AI. Effects of neuroticism, extraversion, circadian type and age on reported driver stress. Work Stress. 2002;16(4):316–334. doi: 10.1080/0267837021000059019. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Jonassaint CR, Why YP, Bishop GD, Tong EM, Diong SM, Enkelmann HC, Khader M, Ang J. The effects of neuroticism and Extraversion on cardiovascular reactivity during a mental and an emotional stress task. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2009;74(3):274–279. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2009.09.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Xing M, Tadayonnejad R, MacNamara A, Ajilore O, DiGangi J, Phan KL, Leow A, Klumpp H. Resting-state theta band connectivity and graph analysis in generalized social anxiety disorder. NeuroImage: Clin. 2017;13:24–32. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2016.11.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Hemenover SH, Dienstbier RA. Prediction of stress appraisals from mastery, extraversion, neuroticism, and general appraisal tendencies. Motiv. Emotion. 1996;20:299–317. doi: 10.1007/BF02856520. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Afshar H, Roohafza HR, Keshteli AH, Mazaheri M, Feizi A, Adibi P. The association of personality traits and coping styles according to stress level. J. Res. Med. Sci. 2015;20(4):353–358. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Volk AA, Brazil KJ, Franklin-Luther P, Dane AV, Vaillancourt T. The influence of demographics and personality on COVID-19 coping in young adults. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2021;168:110398. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2020.110398. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Wijngaards I, Sisouw de Zilwa SCM, Burger MJ. Extraversion moderates the relationship between the stringency of COVID-19 protective measures and depressive symptoms. Front. Psychol. 2020;11:568907. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.568907. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Lee RM, Dean BL, Jung K-R. Social connectedness, extraversion, and subjective well-being: testing a mediation model. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2008;45(5):414–419. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2008.05.017. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Swickert RJ, Rosentreter CJ, Hittner JB, Mushrush JE. Extraversion, social support processes, and stress. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2008;32(5):877–891. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(01)00093-9. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Alonso JF, Romero S, Ballester MR, Antonijoan RM, Mañanas MA. Stress assessment based on EEG univariate features and functional connectivity measures. Physiol. Meas. 2015;36:1351–1365. doi: 10.1088/0967-3334/36/7/1351. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Ermentrout GB, Kopell N. Oscillator death in systems of coupled neural oscillators. SIAM J. Appl. Math. 1990;50:125–146. doi: 10.1137/0150009. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Kopell N, Ermentrout B, Whittington MA, Traub R. Gamma rhythms and beta rhythms have different synchronization properties. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2000;97:1867–1872. doi: 10.1073/pnas.97.4.1867. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Chandrasekaran L, Achuthan S, Canavier CC. Stability of two cluster solutions in pulse coupled networks. J. Comput. Neurosci. 2010;30:427–445. doi: 10.1007/s10827-010-0268-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Zappasodi F, Olejarczyk E, Marzetti L, Assenza G, Pizzella V, Tecchio F. Fractal dimension of EEG activity senses neuronal impairment in acute stroke. PLoS ONE. 2014;9:1–8. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0100199. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Goldberger AL, Amaral LA, Hausdorff JM, Ivanov PCh, Peng CK, Stanley HE. Fractal dynamics in physiology: alterations with disease and aging. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2002;99:2466–2472. doi: 10.1073/pnas.012579499. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Rogala J, Kublik E, Krauz R, Wrobel A. Resting-state EEG activity predicts frontoparietal network reconfiguration and improved attentional performance. Sci. Rep. 2020;10:5064. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-61866-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Kiiski H, Rueda-Delgado LM, Bennett M, Knight R, Rai L, Roddy D, Grogan K, Bramham J, Kelly C, Whelan R. Functional EEG connectivity is a neuromarker for adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder symptoms. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2020;131(1):330–342. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2019.08.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Rodrak S, Wongsawat Y EEG (2013) Brain mapping and brain connectivity index for subtypes classification of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder children during the eye-opened period. In: 2013 35th Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (EMBC), Osaka, pp. 7400–7403. https://doi:10.1109/EMBC.2013.6611268 [DOI] [PubMed]
- 52.Jaworowska A. Kwestionariusze osobowości Eysencka. EPQ-R. EPQ-R w wersji skróconej. Polskie normalizacje [Eysenck's Personality Questionnaires. EPQ-R. EPQ-R Short Scale. A Polish standardisation], Pracownia Testów Psychologicznych Polskiego Towarzystwa Psychologicznego, Warszawa. 2012.
- 53.Ahorsu DK, Lin C-Y, Imani V, Saffari M, Griffith MD, Pakpour AH. The fear of COVID-19 scale: development and initial validation. Int. J. Mental Health Addict. 2020 doi: 10.1007/s11469-020-00270-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Brzezicka A, Kamiński M, Kamiński J, et al. Information transfer during a transitive reasoning task. Brain Topogr. 2011;24:1–8. doi: 10.1007/s10548-010-0158-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Chuderski A. Even a single trivial binding of information is critical for fluid intelligence. Intelligence. 2019;77:101396. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2019.101396. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Mennes M, Stiers P, Lagae L, et al. Antenatal maternal anxiety modulates the BOLD response in 20-year-old men during endogenous cognitive control. Brain Imaging Behav. 2020;14:830–846. doi: 10.1007/s11682-018-0027-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Sala-Llonch R, Arenaza-Urquijo EM, Valls-Pedret C, Vidal-Piñeiro D, Bargalló N, Junqué C, Bartrés-Faz D. Dynamic functional reorganizations and relationship with working memory performance in healthy aging. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2012;6:152. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00152. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Lachaux J-P, Rodriguez E, Martinerie J, Varela FJ. Measuring phase synchrony in brain signals. Hum. Brain Mapp. 1999;8:194–208. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1999)8:4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Bosch V, der Nederlanden CM, Joanisse MF, Grahn JA. Music as a scaffold for listening to speech: better neural phase-locking to song than speech. Neuroimage. 2020;214:116767. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116767. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Gong A, Liu J, Lu L, Wu G, Jiang C, Fu Y. Characteristic differences between the brain networks of high-level shooting athletes and non-athletes calculated using the phase-locking value algorithm. Biomed. Signal Process. Control. 2019;51:128–137. doi: 10.1016/j.bspc.2019.02.009. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Neubauer AC, Fink A. Intelligence and neural efficiency: measures of brain activation versus measures of functional connectivity in the brain. Intelligence. 2019;37(2):223–229. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2008.10.008. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Varela F, Lachaux J-P, Rodriguez E, Martinerie J. The brainweb: Phase synchronization and large-scale integration. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2001;2:229–239. doi: 10.1038/35067550. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Cohen MX. Effects of time lag and frequency matching on phase-based connectivity. J. Neurosci. Methods. 2015;250:137–146. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2014.09.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Jian W, Chen M, McFarland DJ. EEG based zero-phase phase-locking value (PLV) and effects of spatial filtering during actual movement. Brain Res. Bull. 2017;130:156–164. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2017.01.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Rizkallah J, Amoud H, Fraschini M, Wendling F, Hassan M. Exploring the correlation between M/EEG source–space and fMRI networks at rest. Brain Topogr. 2020;33:151–160. doi: 10.1007/s10548-020-00753-w. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Haartsen R, Jones EJH, Orekhova EV, et al. Functional EEG connectivity in infants associates with later restricted and repetitive behaviours in autism; a replication study. Transl. Psychiatry. 2019;9:66. doi: 10.1038/s41398-019-0380-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Koenig T, Lehmann D, Saito N, Kuginuki T, Kinoshita T, Koukkou M. Decreased functional connectivity of EEG theta-frequency activity in first-episode, neuroleptic-naive patients with schizophrenia: preliminary results. Schizophrenia Res. 2001;50(1–2):55–60. doi: 10.1016/S0920-9964(00)00154-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.