Abstract
Background:
Sensory function declines with age and may impact sexual function in older adults. Indeed, the sense of smell plays a uniquely strong role in sexual motivation. Therefore, olfactory dysfunction in older adults may be intimately linked to changes in sexual desire and satisfaction.
Aim:
To test whether impaired olfactory function is associated with decreased sexual activity and motivation in older adults.
Methods:
Cross-sectional analysis of a nationally representative sample of community-dwelling older U.S. adults from the National Social Life, Health, and Aging Project.
Outcomes:
Two modalities of olfactory function were measured (sensitivity to n-butanol and odor identification) via validated methods (Sniffin’ Sticks). Respondents answered survey questions about frequency of sexual thoughts (motivation) and sexual activity, and satisfaction with their most recent sexual relationship. A wide range of demographic, health, and social information were also collected.
Results:
Decreased olfactory function in older U.S. adults was associated with decreased sexual motivation (OR 0.93, p=0.03) and less emotional satisfaction with sex (OR 0.89, p=0.04), but not decreased frequency of sexual activity or physical pleasure, in analyses that were adjusted for age, gender, race, education, cognition, comorbidities, and depression.
Clinical Implications:
Olfactory dysfunction may affect sexuality in older adults. Potentially treatable causes of sensory loss should be addressed by clinicians to improve quality of life.
Strengths and Limitations:
These results rely on validated olfactory testing, detailed measures of sexual attitudes and behaviors, and extensive demographic, health, and social history in a nationally-representative sample of older U.S. adults. Due to the cross-sectional nature of these analyses, we cannot determine causality.
Conclusions:
Olfactory dysfunction in older U.S. adults is associated with decreased sexual motivation and emotional satisfaction, potentially due to evolutionary-conserved neurological links between olfaction and sexuality.
Keywords: Smell, sensory loss, aging, sexual activity, sexual motivation
INTRODUCTION
Sex remains a regular and important part of life for older adults1. However, frequency of sex declines with increasing age, as a result of both physical and psychosocial stressors1,2. The roles of factors such as chronic illness, social isolation, and relationship dissatisfaction in decreased sexual activity have been well studied1–6. However, relatively little is known about how sensory loss impacts sexuality in older adults, despite the high prevalence of sensory loss with aging7 and the central role the classical senses play in sexuality8–12.
We previously reported that global sensory impairment (a measurement of dysfunction across the five classical senses: vision, hearing, smell, taste, and touch) in older adults was associated with a decrease in sexual activity but no change in sexual motivation or sexual satisfaction13. This suggests that global sensory impairment presents a barrier to engaging in sex but does not necessarily decrease the importance of sexual expression and satisfaction in the lives of older adults. These data characterized a key connection between age-related impairment of sensory function and sexual activity in older adults. However, it is likely that different senses contribute to sexuality in different ways. For example, in one study both men and women reported smell as one of the most important stimuli when selecting a romantic partner, particularly among young women12. Indeed, olfaction has a strong, evolutionarily-conserved connection to the limbic system, which plays a critical role in processing emotions and sexual motivation14–19. Neurons in the olfactory bulb also project directly to the hypothalamus, another key mediator of sexual motivation20,21.
Olfactory loss has severe consequences for the quality of life of older adults. For example, smell loss is associated with social isolation and anhedonia, which are also major contributors to sexual dysfunction22,23. Thus, we hypothesized that olfactory impairment and sexual dysfunction are related in a manner that goes beyond difficulty initiating sex. Considering the tight neurological coupling of olfaction with emotional processing, we hypothesized that olfaction may play an important role in sexual satisfaction and motivation. To explore this important relationship, we tested the role of olfactory specifically as a sensory modality associated with sexual function.
In this study, we investigated the relationship between sexuality and olfactory function using data from the National Social Life, Health, and Aging Project (NSHAP), a nationally representative study of older U.S. adults24. Respondents answered survey questions about various facets of sexual behavior and motivation, and olfactory function was tested. Our previous work on global sensory impairment included only assessment of odor identification, which is the ability to associate a smell with a picture or word and requires central cognitive processing, memory, and language. Here we also measure olfactory threshold, which more directly probes peripheral and subcortical odor detection. These tests in tandem provide a robust assessment of both components of olfaction and an insight into the balance of different brain systems involved. We also use extensive data on physical, mental, and social well-being which were collected by in-home interview. These data allow us to probe the link between olfaction and sexual function.
METHODS
The National Social Life, Health, and Aging Project (NSHAP)
NSHAP is an omnibus, nationally representative survey of older U.S. adults living at home, who were born between 1920 and 194725. In 2010–2011, interviewers from the National Opinion Research Center (NORC) conducted in-home interviews with adults who had been interviewed 5 years previously along with their cohabitating partners24. Interviews included assessment of demographic, social, psychological, and biological measures, including olfaction and sexuality as described below. Here, we included respondents who had complete olfactory testing and sexuality measures in 2010–11, a timepoint when the most comprehensive information was collected. Further details regarding NSHAP study design, data collection, and baseline characteristics of respondents are available elsewhere25–27. The study was approved by the Institutional Review Boards of the University of Chicago and NORC. All respondents provided written, informed consent.
Olfaction
Odor sensitivity to n-butanol and odor identification ability were measured in a randomly selected subset (approximately 2/3) of subjects in 2010–11 using the Olfactory Function Field Exam, as previously described28–30. The odor sensitivity task measures the ability to physically detect an odor, while the odor identification task includes the more cognitively challenging task of recognizing a common smell and identifying the correct name from a list. We include both measures of olfaction because physical detection and central processing and recognition involve separate neurological pathways, which can be decoupled with aging but are both necessary for a functional sense of smell28.
Briefly, for odor sensitivity, respondents were asked to detect different concentrations of n-butanol presented via Sniffin’ Stick odor pens, a validated measure28,29. Respondents who were able to detect 5 or 6 n-butanol concentrations were classified as normosmic, whereas respondents who detected 4 or fewer pens were classified as having olfactory dysfunction. The number of errors (range 0–6) was used in the model fitting. For odor identification, respondents were asked to identify each of 5 common odors presented using Sniffin’ Stick odor pens by choosing from a set of four word/picture choices; refusals were coded as incorrect. Respondents who identified 4–5 odors correctly were classified as normosmic, whereas respondents who identified 3 or fewer odors correctly were classified as having olfactory dysfunction, as in prior work30. The number of errors (range 0–5) was used in the analysis. Pens were obtained from Burghart Messtechnik (Wedel, Germany) and utilized according to the manufacturer’s instructions.
Sexuality
Sex or sexual activity was defined in the NSHAP survey as “any mutually voluntary activity with another person that involves sexual contact, whether or not intercourse or orgasm occurs”1. Multiple components of sexual motivation and behavior were measured as previously described31. Sexual motivation was assessed by asking respondents to rate the frequency of sexual thoughts with the responses “never,” “less than once a month,” “one to a few times a month,” “one to a few times a week,” “every day,” or “several times a day.” Respondents also rated their emotional satisfaction with their most recent sexual relationship with the responses: “not at all,” “slightly,” “moderately,” “very,” or “extremely.” Sexual behavior was measured by the frequency of sexual activity. Respondents answered how often they had had sex with their most recent partner with the responses: “none at all,” “once a month or less,” “2 to 3 times a month,” “once or twice a week,” “3 to 6 times a week,” or “once a day or more.” A second measure was how physically pleasurable they found their most recent sexual relationship with the responses: “not at all,” “slightly,” “moderately,” “very,” or “extremely.”
Demographic, Health, and Social Information
Our analyses adjusted for numerous potential factors that could influence the relationship between olfaction and sexuality, including age, gender, race/ethnicity, education, cognitive function, and depression. Age and gender have previously been associated with both olfactory function32,33 and sexual behavior34. Race (which affects olfactory function35) and Hispanic ethnicity were measured via self-report according to standard NIH questions, and respondents were classified as White, African American, or Hispanic (those who reported their race as “Black/African American” and answered “Yes” to Hispanic ethnicity were classified as African American). Those reporting their race as “American Indian or Alaskan Native,” “Asian,” or “Other” were combined into a single “Other” category due to low numbers. Socioeconomic status was measured by highest educational degree or certification earned. Cognitive function was measured using a validated version of the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) adapted for survey administration (MoCA-SA)36–38. Burden of medical comorbidity was quantified with an adaptation of the validated Charlson index39 using the medical history data that were collected in NSHAP40, scored from 0 to 11. This index includes key diseases that affect sexuality including diabetes, cancer, neurodegenerative disease, and incontinence41–44. Depression has been previously shown to be associated with olfactory dysfunction and sexuality45, and was measured with a version of the Center for the Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale modified for NSHAP46 (scored from 0 to 21; a score of 9 or more indicates frequent depressive symptoms).
Statistical analysis
NSHAP had a 74% weighted response rate in 2010–11, excellent for a probability sample. 1,981 older adults had complete data collected on odor sensitivity and sexuality measures, and 1,605 older adults had complete data collected on odor identification and sexuality measures. Analyses were performed using person-level weights, accounting for differential non-response and differential probability of selection. Design-based standard errors were calculated using the linearization method together with the strata and Primary Sampling Unit indicators provided with the dataset. Multivariable ordinal logistic regression models were used to evaluate the relationship of sexual parameters with olfactory function, adjusting for relevant covariates. Results are presented as odds ratios and 95% confidence intervals (CI). Wald tests were used to determine p-values. Statistical significance was set at p<0.05. All statistical analyses were conducted using Stata Version 14.0 (StataCorp LP, College Station, TX).
RESULTS
In a nationally-representative sample of 2,084 U.S. older adults, 71.5% had impaired n-butanol sensitivity and 22.4% had impaired odor identification ability (Table 1). Self-reports of sexual behavior, motivation, and satisfaction are presented in Table 2 and described in more detail elsewhere31
Table 1.
Characteristic | N (%) |
---|---|
Odor identification – number of errors | |
0 | 896 (45) |
1 | 661 (32) |
2 | 254 (11) |
3 | 146 (6) |
4 | 63 (3) |
5 | 64 (3) |
Odor sensitivity – number of errors | |
0 | 152 (8) |
1 | 388 (20) |
2 | 540 (25) |
3 | 386 (18) |
4 | 237 (12) |
5 | 185 (8) |
6 | 196 (9) |
Gender | |
Men | 985 (47) |
Women | 1099 (53) |
Age (years, mean ± SD) | 72.4±7.5 |
Race/ethnicity | |
White | 1493 (81) |
Black | 323 (10) |
Hispanic, non-Black | 212 (6) |
Other | 48 (2) |
Education | |
<High school | 412(16) |
High school graduate or equivalent | 534 (26) |
Some college | 608 (30) |
Bachelors or higher | 530 (27) |
Cognition (MoCA-SA, mean ± SD) | 14.0 ± 3.9 |
Comorbidity index | |
(NSHAP-modified Charlson index, mean ± SD) | 1.2 ± 1.5 |
Frequent depressive symptoms | 4.7 ± 4.3 |
(NSHAP-modified CESD, mean ± SD) |
Weighted % unless otherwise specified
Table 2:
Overall (%) | Men (%) | Women (%) | |
---|---|---|---|
How often do you think about sex? (Sexual motivation) | |||
Never | 480 (15) | 91 (6) | 398 (22) |
Less than once a month | 742 (23) | 193 (13) | 549 (31) |
One to a few times a month | 836 (26) | 376 (26) | 460 (26) |
One to a few times a week | 707 (22) | 430 (29) | 277 (16) |
Every day | 336 (11) | 279 (19) | 57 (3) |
Several times a day | 112 (4) | 93 (6) | 19 (1) |
How often did you have sex with your most recent partner? (Sexual behavior) | |||
None at all | 917 (38) | 450 (35) | 467 (40) |
Once a month or less | 548 (23) | 297 (23) | 251 (22) |
2–3 times a month | 467 (19) | 258 (20) | 209 (18) |
Once or twice a week | 372 (15) | 199 (16) | 173 (15) |
3–6 times a week | 94 (4) | 53 (4) | 41 (4) |
Once a day or more | 31 (1) | 15 (1) | 16 (1) |
How physically pleasurable did/do you find your relationship with your most recent partner to be? (Sexual behavior) | |||
Not at all | 130 (5) | 50 (4) | 80 (7) |
Slightly | 129 (5) | 54 (4) | 75 (6) |
Moderately | 473(19) | 197 (15) | 276 (23) |
Very | 975 (38) | 536 (40) | 439 (36) |
Extremely | 850 (33) | 505 (38) | 345 (28) |
How emotionally satisfying did/do you find your relationship with your most recent partner to be? (Emotional cognition) | |||
Not at all | 79 (3) | 36 (3) | 43 (3) |
Slightly | 131 (5) | 54 (4) | 77 (6) |
Moderately | 473(18) | 194 (14) | 279 (23) |
Very | 1069 (41) | 580 (42) | 489 (39) |
Extremely | 864 (33) | 512 (37) | 352 (28) |
How important a part of your life would you say that sex is? | |||
Not at all | 932 (33) | 256 (20) | 676 (44) |
Somewhat | 566 (20) | 249 (20) | 317 (21) |
Moderately | 649 (23) | 348 (27) | 301 (20) |
Very | 497 (18) | 316 (25) | 181 (12) |
Extremely | 158 (6) | 107 (8) | 51 (3) |
Older adults with worse odor sensitivity (greater number of errors) had less sexual motivation, indicated by less frequent thoughts about sex (OR 0.93, 95% CI 0.87–0.99) (Table 3). Women thought about sex less frequently than men (OR 0.15, 95% CI 0.12–0.18), as did respondents who were older, less educated, and had lower cognition scores. Nonetheless, the association between olfactory sensitivity and sexual motivation remained statistically significant after adjusting for these factors. We also asked whether these associations were stronger in older women (as they are in younger women12). Such a gender difference was not evident here among older adults (p=0.60 for the gender by olfactory sensitivity interaction).
Table 3:
Odds Ratio (95% CI) | p-value | |
---|---|---|
Odor sensitivity errors | 0.93 (0.87–0.99) | 0.03 |
Gender | (reference) | <0.001 |
Men | 0.15 (0.12–0.18) | |
Women | ||
Age | (reference) | <0.001 |
62 to 69 years old | 0.61 (0.48–0.77) | |
70 to 79 years old | 0.40 (0.29–0.56) | |
80 to 90 years old | ||
Race/Ethnicity | (reference) | 0.08 |
White | 0.86 (0.63–1.15) | |
Black | 0.82 (0.56–1.20) | |
Hispanic, non-Black | 0.43 (0.20–0.93) | |
Others | ||
Education Level | (reference) | <0.01 |
Less than high school | 1.67 (1.10–2.56) | |
High school graduate | 1.51 (1.02–2.25) | |
Some college | 2.22 (1.39–3.56) | |
Bachelors or higher | ||
Cognition | 1.06 (1.03–1.10) | <0.001 |
Comorbidity Index | 0.95 (0.89–1.02) | 0.13 |
Frequent Depressive Symptoms | (reference) | 0.25 |
Negative | 1.18 (0.92–1.51) | |
Positive |
A similar result was found with the more cognitively-demanding task of odor identification. Older adults with worse odor identification scores reported less emotional satisfaction with their most recent sexual partner (OR 0.89, 95% CI 0.81–0.99) (Table 4). Women reported less emotional satisfaction than men (OR 0.57, 95% CI 0.45–0.73), and respondents with frequent depressive symptoms reported less emotional satisfaction (OR 0.44, 95% CI 0.30–0.69). However, neither of these factors accounted for the association between odor identification and finding sex emotionally satisfying. The relationship also did not very by gender (p=0.44 for the gender by odor identification interaction).
Table 4:
Odds Ratio (95% CI) | p-value | |
---|---|---|
Odor identification errors | 0.89 (0.81–0.99) | 0.04 |
Gender | (reference) | <0.001 |
Men | 0.57 (0.45–0.73) | |
Women | ||
Age | (reference) | 0.66 |
62 to 69 years old | 0.88 (0.69–1.13) | |
70 to 79 years old | 0.89 (0.63–1.24) | |
80 to 90 years old | ||
Race/Ethnicity | (reference) | 0.09 |
White | 0.91 (0.62–1.35) | |
Black | 0.46 (0.25–0.84) | |
Hispanic, non-Black | 1.03 (0.56–1.91) | |
Others | ||
Education Level | (reference) | 0.83 |
Less than high school | 0.94 (0.55–1.63) | |
High school graduate | 1.06 (0.69–1.62) | |
Some college | 0.97 (0.59–1.60) | |
Bachelors or higher | ||
Cognition | 1.04 (0.99–1.09) | 0.13 |
Comorbidity Index | 1.04 (0.95–1.14) | 0.43 |
Frequent Depressive Symptoms | <0.001 | |
(reference) | ||
Negative | 0.44 (0.30–0.69) | |
Positive |
Interestingly, neither measure of sexual behavior was associated with olfactory function as measured by either odor identification or sensitivity. Specifically, the frequency of sex with the most recent partner was not associated with either odor sensitivity or odor identification ability (p=0.53 and p=0.80, respectively) in adjusted models. Women, white respondents, and those who were older reported less frequent sex in these models. Similarly, physical pleasure of the most recent sexual relationship was not significantly associated with either odor sensitivity (p=0.23) or odor identification (p=0.81).
DISCUSSION
Our data suggest that olfactory dysfunction affects sexual motivation and emotional satisfaction with sex, in contrast to global sensory impairment. Global sensory impairment, which encompasses dysfunction of multiple senses, has been associated with decreased frequency of sex but not change in frequency of thinking about sex or satisfaction with sexual relationships13. However, when we focus on olfaction exclusively, we find the exact opposite result: respondents with olfactory dysfunction were able to have sex with the same frequency and achieve physical satisfaction, but they thought about sex less and were less emotionally satisfied by it. This striking contrast suggests that while the other senses included in global sensory impairment may be more important for initiating and performing sexual acts, olfaction - perhaps due to its strong evolutionarily-conserved link with the limbic system and hypothalamus - is critical for the thoughts and feelings that drive it.
Herz and Cahill surveyed adults about the stimuli that affect their sexual responsivity, and showed that for both men and women, olfactory information is central to sexual attraction12. Our data support this finding and demonstrate that with increasing age, availability of these olfactory data may continue to drive sexual desire and quality of sexual experience. However, Herz and Cahill also found that olfaction is particularly salient to sexual attraction in young women compared to men, and this finding has been replicated for young women across different cultures47. We find that this gender difference is not present in older adults. One possibility that could explain this difference is that olfaction becomes relatively less important for women as they age (perhaps due to hormonal changes), or that it increases in importance for men as they age. This remains an open question to be investigated in future work.
Gudziol and colleagues showed that olfactory dysfunction was associated with decreased sexual appetite, but suggested that depression caused by the olfactory dysfunction was the reason for the decreased interest in sex45. In our analyses, we clarified this relationship by controlling for presence of depressive symptoms. While depression is related to both olfactory dysfunction22 and decreased interest in sex1,48, it does not mediate their association.
In this study, we also expand on previous work by including olfactory sensitivity, a measurement which requires less higher-order integration of executive function and memory than odor identification. The finding that both measurements are associated with sexual motivation and emotional satisfaction suggests that the strong connection between olfaction and sexuality may also exist at a subcortical level, not only in the realm of cognition and memory. This supports our hypothesis regarding the unique way in which evolutionarily-conserved olfactory connections to the limbic system shape sexual drives and responses.
Several limitations of this study should be taken into consideration. While our findings were statistically significant, the magnitude of the observed relationships was not overwhelming. There are likely a variety of factors that contribute to sexuality in older adults, and each may have a small effect, consistent with our results. The lower strength of our association may reflect the difficulty of teasing out all of inputs on sexuality; thus, chemosensory function may play a demonstrable but small effect. Future studies may address these possibilities. In this study we only examined olfactory function rather than the other classical senses. To test the hypothesis that olfaction affects sexuality in a manner that is unique among the senses, future work will be needed to directly compare how olfaction is related to sexuality versus the other individual classical senses. Additionally, our data are cross-sectional; longitudinal analyses would provide insights into mechanisms and causality. If the relationship between olfaction and sexual experience is not causal, an alternative explanation would be that good olfactory and sexual function indicates that their shared neuroanatomic connections remain resilient in aging. Indeed, olfaction is now viewed as a sensitive indicator of other domains of health, especially in the nervous system49,50. In this case, impaired olfaction may be a measurable sign of underlying neurological or cognitive decline, particularly in the brain regions that contribute to sexuality.
Given the demographic changes in our society, the consequences of both impaired olfaction and sexual dysfunction on quality of life are likely to grow. Currently, age-related olfactory dysfunction affects approximately 15 million older Americans and age-related sexual dysfunction affects over 40% of women and 30% of men33,51,52. Further investigation into how these systems are connected and whether treating olfactory loss may improve sexual function has great promise to enhance the quality of life of older adults.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
We thank all NSHAP respondents for their generous participation. Susie Kim and Alyssa Anneken provided logistical assistance. Jesse Siegel and Sunny Kung were supported by the Scholarship and Discovery Program at the Pritzker School of Medicine at The University of Chicago.
Funding: The National Social Life, Health, and Aging Project is supported by the National Institute on Aging and the NIH (R37AG030481; R01AG033903). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health. This project was also funded in part by the University of Chicago Pritzker School of Medicine.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
Conflict of Interest Disclosures: The authors declare no conflicts of interests.
REFERENCES
- 1.Lindau ST, Schumm LP, Laumann EO, Levinson W, O’Muircheartaigh CA, Waite LJ. A study of sexuality and health among older adults in the United States. N Engl J Med. 2007;357(8):762–774. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa067423 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Laumann EO, Waite LJ. Sexual dysfunction among older adults: prevalence and risk factors from a nationally representative U.S. probability sample of men and women 57–85 years of age. J Sex Med. 2008;5(10):2300–2311. doi: 10.1111/j.1743-6109.2008.00974.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Laumann EO, West S, Glasser D, Carson C, Rosen R, Kang J. Prevalence and Correlates of Erectile Dysfunction by Race and Ethnicity Among Men Aged 40 or Older in the United States: From the Male Attitudes Regarding Sexual Health Survey. The Journal of Sexual Medicine. 2007;4(1):57–65. doi: 10.1111/j.1743-6109.2006.00340.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Call V, Sprecher S, Schwartz P. The Incidence and Frequency of Marital Sex in a National Sample. Journal of Marriage and Family. 1995;57(3):639–652. doi: 10.2307/353919 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 5.DeLamater J. Sexual Expression in Later Life: A Review and Synthesis. The Journal of Sex Research. 2012;49(2–3):125–141. doi: 10.1080/00224499.2011.603168 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Malavige LS, Jayaratne SD, Kathriarachchi ST, Sivayogan S, Fernando DJ, Levy JC. Erectile dysfunction among men with diabetes is strongly associated with premature ejaculation and reduced libido. J Sex Med. 2008;5(9):2125–2134. doi: 10.1111/j.1743-6109.2008.00907.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Correia C, Lopez KJ, Wroblewski KE, et al. Global Sensory Impairment in Older Adults in the United States. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society. 2016;64(2):306–313. doi: 10.1111/jgs.13955 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Frohlich PF, Meston CM. Tactile Sensitivity in Women with Sexual Arousal Disorder. Arch Sex Behav. 2005;34(2):207–217. doi: 10.1007/s10508-005-1798-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Ren D, Tan K, Arriaga XB, Chan KQ. Sweet love: The effects of sweet taste experience on romantic perceptions. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships. 10.1177/0265407514554512 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Rowland DL, Greenleaf W, Mas M, Myers L, Davidson JM. Penile and finger sensory thresholds in young, aging, and diabetic males. Arch Sex Behav. 1989;18(1):1–12. doi: 10.1007/bf01579287 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Lehane CM, Dammeyer J, Hovaldt HB, Elsass P. Sexuality and Well-Being Among Couples Living with Acquired Deafblindness. Sex Disabil. 2017;35(2):135–146. doi: 10.1007/s11195-016-9470-8 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Herz RS, Cahill ED. Differential use of sensory information in sexual behavior as a function of gender. Hum Nat. 1997;8(3):275–286. doi: 10.1007/BF02912495 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Zhong S, Pinto JM, Wroblewski KE, McClintock MK. Sensory Dysfunction and Sexuality in the U.S. Population of Older Adults. J Sex Med. 2018;15(4):502–509. doi: 10.1016/j.jsxm.2018.01.021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Zald DH, Pardo JV. Emotion, olfaction, and the human amygdala: amygdala activation during aversive olfactory stimulation. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1997;94(8):4119–4124. doi: 10.1073/pnas.94.8.4119 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Buchanan TW, Tranel D, Adolphs R. A Specific Role for the Human Amygdala in Olfactory Memory. Learn Mem. 2003;10(5):319–325. doi: 10.1101/lm.62303 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Hummer TA, Phan KL, Kern DW, McClintock MK. A human chemosignal modulates frontolimbic activity and connectivity in response to emotional stimuli. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2017;75:15–25. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.09.023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Hamann S, Herman RA, Nolan CL, Wallen K. Men and women differ in amygdala response to visual sexual stimuli. Nat Neurosci. 2004;7(4):411–416. doi: 10.1038/nn1208 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Childress AR, Ehrman RN, Wang Z, et al. Prelude to Passion: Limbic Activation by “Unseen” Drug and Sexual Cues. PLOS ONE. 2008;3(1):e1506. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0001506 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Baird AD, Wilson SJ, Bladin PF, Saling MM, Reutens DC. Neurological control of human sexual behaviour: insights from lesion studies. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2007;78(10):1042–1049. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2006.107193 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Price JL, Slotnick BM, Revial MF. Olfactory projections to the hypothalamus. J Comp Neurol. 1991;306(3):447–461. doi: 10.1002/cne.903060309 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Brunetti M, Babiloni C, Ferretti A, et al. Hypothalamus, sexual arousal and psychosexual identity in human males: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Eur J Neurosci. 2008;27(11):2922–2927. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06241.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Sivam A, Wroblewski KE, Alkorta-Aranburu G, et al. Olfactory Dysfunction in Older Adults is Associated with Feelings of Depression and Loneliness. Chem Senses. 2016;41(4):293–299. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjv088 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Keller A, Malaspina D. Hidden consequences of olfactory dysfunction: a patient report series. BMC Ear Nose Throat Disord. 2013;13(1):8. doi: 10.1186/1472-6815-13-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Hayward M, Wallace R. Wave 2 of the National Social Life, Health, and Aging Project: An Overview. The journals of gerontology Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences. 2014;69 Suppl 2:S1–3. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbu126 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.O’Muircheartaigh C, Eckman S, Smith S. Statistical Design and Estimation for the National Social Life, Health, and Aging Project. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2009;64B(Suppl 1):i12–i19. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbp045 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Waite LJ, Cagney K, Cornwell, et al. National Social Life, Health, and Aging Project (NSHAP) Wave 2 and Partner Data Collection. 2014;(Ann Arbor, MI: Inter-university Consortium for Political and Social Research; ). 10.3886/ICPSR34921.v1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Suzman R. The National Social Life, Health, and Aging Project: An Introduction. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2009;64B(Suppl 1):i5–i11. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbp078 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Kern DW, Wroblewski KE, Schumm LP, Pinto JM, Chen RC, McClintock MK. Olfactory function in Wave 2 of the National Social Life, Health, and Aging Project. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2014;69 Suppl 2:S134–143. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbu093 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Kern DW, Wroblewski KE, Schumm LP, Pinto JM, McClintock MK. Field Survey Measures of Olfaction: The Olfactory Function Field Exam (OFFE). Field methods. 2014;26(4):421–434. doi: 10.1177/1525822X14547499 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Mueller C, Renner B. A new procedure for the short screening of olfactory function using five items from the “Sniffin’ Sticks” identification test kit. Am J Rhinol. 2006;20(1):113–116. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Galinsky AM, McClintock MK, Waite LJ. Sexuality and Physical Contact in National Social Life, Health, and Aging Project Wave 2. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2014;69(Suppl 2):S83–S98. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbu072 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Brämerson A, Johansson L, Ek L, Nordin S, Bende M. Prevalence of olfactory dysfunction: the skövde population-based study. Laryngoscope. 2004;114(4):733–737. doi: 10.1097/00005537-200404000-00026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Murphy C, Schubert CR, Cruickshanks KJ, Klein BEK, Klein R, Nondahl DM. Prevalence of olfactory impairment in older adults. JAMA. 2002;288(18):2307–2312. doi: 10.1001/jama.288.18.2307 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.George LK, Weiler SJ. Sexuality in middle and late life. The effects of age, cohort, and gender. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1981;38(8):919–923. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1981.01780330077008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Pinto JM, Schumm LP, Wroblewski KE, Kern DW, McClintock MK. Racial Disparities in Olfactory Loss Among Older Adults in the United States. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2014;69A(3):323–329. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glt063 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Kotwal AA, Schumm P, Kern DW, et al. Evaluation of a Brief Survey Instrument for Assessing Subtle Differences in Cognitive Function Among Older Adults. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2015;29(4):317–324. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0000000000000068 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Nasreddine ZS, Phillips NA, Bédirian V, et al. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2005;53(4):695–699. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Shega JW, Sunkara PD, Kotwal A, et al. Measuring cognition: the Chicago Cognitive Function Measure in the National Social Life, Health and Aging Project, Wave 2. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2014;69 Suppl 2:S166–176. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbu106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Charlson M, Szatrowski TP, Peterson J, Gold J. Validation of a combined comorbidity index. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology. 1994;47(11):1245–1251. doi: 10.1016/0895-4356(94)90129-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Vasilopoulos T, Kotwal A, Huisingh-Scheetz MJ, Waite LJ, McClintock MK, Dale W. Comorbidity and chronic conditions in the National Social Life, Health and Aging Project (NSHAP), Wave 2. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2014;69 Suppl 2:S154–165. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbu025 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Schiavi RC, Stimmel BB, Mandeli J, Rayfield EJ. Diabetes mellitus and male sexual function: a controlled study. Diabetologia. 1993;36(8):745–751. doi: 10.1007/BF00401146 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Derogatis LR, Kourlesis SM. An approach to evaluation of sexual problems in the cancer patient. CA: A Cancer Journal for Clinicians. 1981;31(1):46–50. doi: 10.3322/canjclin.31.1.46 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Davies HD, Newkirk LA, Pitts CB, et al. The impact of dementia and mild memory impairment (MMI) on intimacy and sexuality in spousal relationships. Int Psychogeriatr. 2010;22(4):618–628. doi: 10.1017/S1041610210000177 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Hansen BL. Lower Urinary Tract Symptoms (LUTS) and Sexual Function in Both Sexes. European Urology. 2004;46(2):229–234. doi: 10.1016/j.eururo.2004.04.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Gudziol V, Wolff-Stephan S, Aschenbrenner K, Joraschky P, Hummel T. Depression resulting from olfactory dysfunction is associated with reduced sexual appetite--a cross-sectional cohort study. J Sex Med. 2009;6(7):1924–1929. doi: 10.1111/j.1743-6109.2009.01266.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Payne C, Hedberg EC, Kozloski M, Dale W, McClintock MK. Using and interpreting mental health measures in the National Social Life, Health, and Aging Project. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2014;69 Suppl 2:S99–116. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbu100 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Havlicek J, Saxton TK, Roberts SC, et al. He sees, she smells? Male and female reports of sensory reliance in mate choice and non-mate choice contexts. Personality and Individual Differences. 2008;45(6):565–570. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2008.06.019 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Nicolosi A, Moreira ED, Villa M, Glasser DB. A population study of the association between sexual function, sexual satisfaction and depressive symptoms in men. J Affect Disord. 2004;82(2):235–243. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2003.12.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Pinto JM, Wroblewski KE, Kern DW, Schumm LP, McClintock MK. Olfactory Dysfunction Predicts 5-Year Mortality in Older Adults. PLOS ONE. 2014;9(10):e107541. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0107541 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Siegel JK, Wroblewski KE, McClintock MK, Pinto JM. Olfactory dysfunction persists after smoking cessation and signals increased cardiovascular risk. International Forum of Allergy & Rhinology. 2019;9(9):977–985. doi: 10.1002/alr.22357 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Laumann EO, Paik A, Rosen RC. Sexual dysfunction in the United States: prevalence and predictors. JAMA. 1999;281(6):537–544. doi: 10.1001/jama.281.6.537 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Adams DR, Wroblewski KE, Kern DW, et al. Factors Associated with Inaccurate Self-Reporting of Olfactory Dysfunction in Older US Adults. Chem Senses. 2017;42(3):223–231. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjw108 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]