SUMMARY
Impaired synaptic neurotransmission may underly circuit alterations contributing to behavioral autism spectrum disorder (ASD) phenotypes. A critical component of impairments reported in somatosensory and prefrontal cortex of ASD mouse models are parvalbumin (PV)-expressing fast-spiking interneurons. However, it remains unknown whether PV interneurons mediating hippocampal networks crucial to navigation and memory processing are similarly impaired. Using PV-labeled transgenic mice, a battery of behavioral assays, in vitro patch-clamp electrophysiology, and in vivo 32-channel silicon probe local field potential recordings, we address this question in a Cntnap2-null mutant mouse model representing a human ASD risk factor gene. Cntnap2‒/‒ mice show a reduction in hippocampal PV interneuron density, reduced inhibitory input to CA1 pyramidal cells, deficits in spatial discrimination ability, and frequency-dependent circuit changes within the hippocampus, including alterations in gamma oscillations, sharp-wave ripples, and theta-gamma modulation. Our findings highlight hippocampal involvement in ASD and implicate interneurons as a potential therapeutical target.
Graphical abstract
In brief
Paterno et al. show that deletion of autism-associated Cntnap2 alters hippocampal PV+ interneuronal density and perisomatic inhibitory neurotransmission. Perturbed gamma oscillations and sharp wave ripples (SWP-Rs) in area CA1 of Cntnap2 KO mice associated with impaired spatial discrimination highlight the involvement of the hippocampus in the autism phenotype.
INTRODUCTION
The incidence of autism spectrum disorder (ASD), a heterogenous condition characterized by hyperactivity, deficits in social interaction, and repetitive patterns of behavior, has risen dramatically over the past five decades (Taylor et al., 2020). Initially reported as 1 in 10,000 cases, ASD incidence is now 2 in 100 cases (Kim et al., 2011; Baio et al., 2018; Schendel and Thorsteinsson, 2018). Genome-wide association studies on these patients indicate a genetic basis spanning hundreds of different risk genes (Abrahams et al., 2013; Iakoucheva et al., 2019). As a neurodevelopmental disorder diagnosed as early as 2 years of age (Hyman et al., 2020), ASD significantly impacts neural circuits across many brain regions including the cerebellum, somatosensory cortex, and prefrontal cortex (Golden et al., 2018). Within these brain regions, a common simplified model is that excitatory/ inhibitory (E/I) imbalance, presumably linked to GABAergic interneuron dysfunction, represents a neurophysiological hallmark of ASD (Rubenstein and Merzenich, 2003; Gogolla et al., 2009; Lee et al., 2017; Sohal and Rubenstein, 2019). Consistent with this hypothesis, postmortem human ASD studies describe: (1) reduced interneuron density (Ariza et al., 2018; Hashemi et al., 2018), (2) decreased GABA receptor subunit expression (Blatt et al., 2001; Guptill et al., 2007; Fatemi et al., 2010), and (3) decreased magnetic resonance (MR) spectroscopy-measured GABA levels (Harada et al., 2011). Recapitulating human genetic mutations associated with ASD, mouse models further support a GABAergic interneuron dysfunction hypothesis (Chao et al., 2010; Vogt et al., 2015; Selimbeyoglu et al., 2017) and specifically highlight alterations in parvalbumin (PV)-positive fast-spiking interneurons in Shank3 (Filice, 2016), Mecp2 (Ito-Ishida et al., 2015), and Cntnap2 (Peñagarikano et al., 2011; Vogt et al., 2018) mutant mice as well as an environmental-induced ASD model such as prenatal valproate exposure (Lauber et al., 2016).
In the hippocampus, a PV-positive interneuron subpopulation, which includes further subtypes such as basket cells, axoaxonic cells, and bistratified neurons (Que et al., 2021), primarily innervate the soma and proximal dendrites of excitatory pyramidal neurons (Miles et al., 1996). These neurons represent a subset (~25%) of the total interneuron population (Kosaka et al., 1987), derive from progenitor cells in a subpallial embryonic region designated the medial ganglionic eminence (MGE) (Gelman and Marín, 2010), and can make connections with up to 1,000 pyramidal cells (Bezaire and Soltesz, 2013; Li et al., 1992). Hippocampal PV-positive interneurons play a fundamental role in the generation and maintenance of network oscillations associated with learning and episodic memory (Cardin et al., 2009; Sohal et al., 2009). For example, hippocampal PV-positive cells resonating at gamma frequency (40–120 Hz) exert perisomatic inhibition onto CA1 pyramidal cells to control spike generation (Cobb et al., 1995; Miles et al., 1996) and spike timing (Fernández-Ruiz et al., 2017) necessary for the functional processes underlying memory formation or spatial navigation (Burgess, 2008; Hasselmo et al., 2002). Hippocampal PV-positive interneurons are also essential for timing and spatial pyramidal cell synchrony during sharp wave ripples (SWRs) thought to play a role during navigation and memory consolidation (Buzsáki, 2015). Indeed, numerous studies suggest that PV-positive basket cell activity is phase-locked to ripple cycles (Ylinen et al., 1995; Klausberger et al., 2003; Rácz et al., 2009; Varga et al., 2012), and more importantly, activation of these interneurons can induce coherent ensemble spiking in pyramidal cells (Stark et al., 2014). Critically, these studies suggest an essential role for a sub-class of interneurons (e.g., PV-expressing fast-spiking interneurons) in dynamically regulating the E/I balance during normal network processing. Given these findings, one would suspect that hippocampal PV-positive interneuron deficit could result in a significant disruption of hippocampal-dependent network activity. While inhibitory interneuron density deficits have been reported (Ariza et al., 2018; Hashemi et al., 2018), a contrasting immunohistochemistry study in hippocampal tissue from 5 ASD patients described increased PV+ interneuron density (Lawrence et al., 2010). Because a systematic evaluation of hippocampal interneuron function in human ASD patients is not feasible, we focused our efforts on an established mouse model of the disease.
Among identified human ASD risk genes, contactin-associated protein-like 2 (CNTNAP2), which encodes a synaptic cell adhesion molecule, is considered by the Simons Foundation Autism Research Initiative (SFARI) to be a strong ASD candidate. Indeed, a loss-of-function CNTNAP2 gene mutation was identified in a cohort of ASD patients (Strauss et al., 2006), and CNTNAP2 polymorphism has been associated with an increased risk of ASD (Arking et al., 2008). Cntnap2 expression in mouse MGE at embryonic day 13.5 (E13.5) suggests a role in development/maturation of somatostatin (SOM)- and parvalbumin (PV)-expressing interneurons (Gordon et al., 2016; Vogt et al., 2018). Indeed, null Cntnap2 mutant mice have a modest, but significant, reduction of PV-positive cells in somatosensory cortex, striatum, and hippocampus (Peñagarikano et al., 2011; Lauber et al., 2018; Vogt et al., 2018). These mice also replicate clinical phenotypes including epileptiform discharges, hyperactivity, social interaction deficits, and repetitive behavior (Peñagarikano et al., 2011). While considerable work has been done to examine the functional consequences of Cntnap2 mutation on cortical circuits and associated behaviors, much less is known about how Cntnap2 regulates hippocampal networks and hippocampal-dependent behaviors.
To investigate the role of Cntnap2 in the hippocampus, we performed a comprehensive series of anatomical, behavioral, ex vivo, and in vivo electrophysiological studies using adult Cntnap2 knockout (KO) mice. We found a reduction in PV-labeled interneuron density in area CA1 and diminished synaptic inhibition onto CA1 pyramidal cells. Using 32-channel silicon probes, we uncovered frequency-dependent circuit changes with specific alterations in gamma oscillations (65–90 Hz), sharp-wave ripples (150–250 Hz), and theta-gamma modulation in area CA1. At a behavioral level, Cntnap2 KO mice were characterized by hyperactivity and deficits in hippocampal-dependent spatial object recognition in addition to the already established behavioral phenotypes such as repetitive behavior and altered levels of anxiety. Taken together, these findings identify a role for Cntnap2, particularly in hippocampal PV+ interneurons, and provide insights into ASD-associated behaviors.
RESULTS
PV interneuron density is reduced in the hippocampus of Cntnap2 KO mice
Interneuron deficits have been reported in the prefrontal cortex of ASD patients (Ariza et al., 2018; Hashemi et al., 2018) and several genetic mouse models of ASD including Cntnap2 KO mice (Gogolla et al., 2009; Vogt et al., 2018). Additionally, in the Cntnap2 KO animal model, decreased PV+ interneuron density was described in striatum, somatosensory cortex, and hippocampus (Peñagarikano et al., 2011; Lauber et al., 2018); however, other studies failed to report differences in PV+ interneuron density in mPFC (Lazaro et al., 2019) or hippocampus (Lauber et al., 2018). To address this discrepancy in PV+ interneuron density in hippocampus, we crossed Cntnap2 mice with a transgenic Pvalb-tdTomato reporter line (Jackson laboratory #027395). In hippocampal sections from adult wild-type (WT; 6 coronal sections; n = 2 mice) mice, we first confirmed that 91% of Pvalb-tdTomato fluorescent cells co-labeled with an antibody-recognizing PV (Figure 1). Next, using a blinded stereological assessment of tdTomato-fluorescent cells, we confirmed a reduction in overall density of PV+ interneurons for Cntnap2 KO mice compared to age-matched WT controls (Figure 1B). Parsing these cell counts by hippocampal sub-region, we noted that reduced PV+ interneuron density was only significant in area CA1 (Figure 1C, left panel). These data indicate a role for Cntnap2 in development or maturation of PV-positive hippocampal interneurons.
Decreased inhibitory synaptic transmission in CA1 pyramidal neurons from Cntnap2 KO mice
Impaired perisomatic inhibition may be a common pathophysiological alteration in ASD, including Cntnap2 KO mice (Gogolla et al., 2009; Jurgensen and Castillo, 2015). Because PV+ interneurons play a key role in mediating perisomatic inhibition (Freund and Katona, 2007) and are reduced in the CA1 region of adult Cntnap2 KO mice (Figure 1), we examined inhibitory transmission in acute hippocampal slices using visualized whole-cell patch-clamp recording techniques. We measured spontaneous inhibitory postsynaptic currents (sIPSCs) in CA1 pyramidal cells as a standard measure of inhibition. Representative traces are shown in Figure 2A. We found decreased sIPSC amplitude and frequency (Figures 2B and 2C) in Cntnap2 KO mice compared to WT age-matched controls; no change in sIPSC kinetics were noted (Table S1).
Because decreased inhibition could result from a change in synaptic drive onto soma-innervating inhibitory neurons, we next measured sIPSCs (Figure 2D) and excitatory postsynaptic currents (EPSCs; Figure 2I) onto tdTomato-labeled PV+ interneurons in the stratum pyramidale (str. pyramidale) of area CA1. While a small, but statistically significant, change in sIPSC decay time was noted (Figure 2F), measures of amplitude, rise-time, inter-event interval, and frequency were unchanged (Figures 2E and 2L–2O), suggesting that the overall synaptic drive onto PV+ interneurons in adult Cntnap2 KO mice is normal. To test whether deletion of Cntnap2 effected intrinsic firing properties of PV+ interneurons, we measured resting membrane potential, input resistance, action potential threshold, frequency, amplitude, and half-width from td-Tomato-labeled cells; no changes were noted (Figure S3; Table S2). These results confirm an impairment of inhibitory neurotransmission in the CA1 region of hippocampus that is consistent with reduced PV+ interneuron density (Figure 1).
Hippocampal-dependent spatial memory performance is impaired in adult Cntnap2 KO mice
Given the essential contribution of hippocampus to memory and navigation (O’Keefe and Nadel, 1978), we next explored potential behavioral deficits in Cntnap2 KO mice using a hippocampal-dependent one-trial spatial object location task (SOR) (Figure 3A). As previously reported (Peñagarikano et al., 2011), Cntnap2 KO mice exhibit generalized hyperactivity, quantified as increased distance moved across all trials of the task (habituation, familiarization, and test; Figure 3B). Despite this hyperactivity, Cntnap2 KO mice showed similar object interaction times during familiarization compared to age-matched WT controls (Figure 3C). During test, however, Cntnap2 KO animals exhibit impaired discrimination ability between the object located in the new versus the old location (Figure 3D).
To determine whether Cntnap2 KO animals had a general deficit in object-context association, we implemented the object congruence task. During this task, also referred to as a ‘‘pattern separation task’’ (Figure 3E), Cntnap2 KO mice displayed overall hyperactivity during trial 1 (Figure 3F), but no differences were found in objects’ interaction time during trial 1, trial 2, or the test trial. The task performed showed that under both conditions, age-matched Cntnap2 KO and WT mice can discriminate which objects were not congruent with the current context during the test. These findings suggest that both Cntnap2 KO and WT mice spent more time exploring the incongruent object (or object previously presented in a different context), as shown in Figure 3G. Collectively, these results indicate that Cntnap2 KO mice exhibit a specific spatial memory deficit.
To provide a more comprehensive evaluation of potential autism-related behavioral deficits, mice were also evaluated in an elevated plus maze (Figures S1A–S1D) (Walf and Frye, 2007), a modified 3-chamber social test (Figures S1E–S1H) (Yang et al., 2011), self-grooming (Figures S1I–S1L) (Kalueff et al., 2016), and open field assays (Figures S1M–S1O) (Seibenhener and Wooten, 2015). Deficits in the elevated plus maze, self-grooming, and the first 5 min of the open field test are congruent with previously published autism-like behavioral phenotypes described for Cntnap2-null (Peñagarikano et al., 2011) and other mouse models of ASD (Silverman et al., 2010). These results further validated hyperactivity, repetitive behavior, and decreased levels of anxiety in Cntnap2 KO mice.
Gamma activity disruption in a layer-specific distribution in Cntnap2 KO during open field navigation
To investigate theta-gamma dynamics along the hippocampal axis, we recorded local field potentials (LFPs) from 32 channels arranged on a 4-shank silicon probe covering most layers of CA1, CA3, and DG (Figure S2) while the animal navigated an open field. Mice were exposed to different open field settings as shown in Figure 4A for 2 trials per day (25 min for each recording session). During each exposure, Cntnap2 KO mice were more active compared to WT in the 3 different contexts (Figure 4B; Figure S4), further confirming an autism-like phenotype and persistence of these behavioral features after electrode implantation.
To isolate our analysis to theta periods during open field navigation, we restricted LFP analysis to theta epochs while animals were walking/running. Both groups ran at a similar speed during open field navigation (control n = 6; median speed 9.6 cm/s; Cntnap2 KO n = 7; median speed 10.6 cm/s; unpaired t test with Welch correction p > 0.1). We first confirmed physiological features of the hippocampal anatomical layers using power and phase of theta rhythm along the hippocampal axis (Figure 4C). As expected (Buzsáki, 2002), we noticed a progressive theta phase shift along the axis, with maximal shift occurring in CA1 stratum lacunosum-moleculare (str. LM) in addition to the peak amplitude of the laminar theta power profile occurring in str. LM (Figure 4D). Cntnap2 KO mice showed a similar layer-specific profile. To compare power across conditions, we calculated relative power in theta, slow, and mid gamma frequency ranges. In area CA1, Cntnap2 KO mice exhibited a statistically significant decrease in theta (6–10 Hz) in str. radiatum, increased slow-gamma power (30–55 Hz) in str. LM, and a trending decrease in mid gamma power (65–90 Hz) in str. pyramidale and str. LM (Figure 4E). In the DG (str. moleculare), no differences in any frequency band examined (p > 0.1) (data not shown) were observed between control and Cntnap2 KO mice.
Cntnap2 KO exhibit a reduced mid gamma-theta phase modulation
In area CA1 of the hippocampus, theta cycles, the most prominent oscillatory activity, are thought to be temporal windows in which different gamma bands transmit inputs from upstream regions. In particular, slow gamma (30–50 Hz), mid gamma (60–100 Hz), and fast gamma (>100 Hz) occur at descending, peak, and trough of the theta cycle (Colgin et al., 2009; Lasztóczi and Klausberger, 2014; Schomburg et al., 2014; Fernández-Ruiz et al., 2017). To determine whether loss-of-function of Cntnap2 interrupts theta-gamma co-modulation, we next examined distribution of gamma oscillations over the phase of individual theta cycles (Figure 5A). As expected, for both Cntnap2 KO and WT mice, slow gamma, mid gamma, and fast gamma dominated the descending, peak, and trough of the theta cycle, respectively (Figure 5B, left panel). However, modulation of power for mid gamma was significantly decreased at the peak of the theta cycle for Cntnap2 KO mice (Figure 5B).
To identify the contribution of different CA1 layers to gamma band oscillations (Lasztóczi and Klausberger, 2014), we next implemented current source density (CSD) to spatially separate LFP signals. CSD signals isolated fast gamma from str. pyramidale, slow gamma from str. radiatum, and mid gamma from str. LM recordings, as shown in Figure 5C. Cntnap2 KO mice exhibited increased modulation in fast gamma in pyramidal layers and a statistically significant decreased modulation in mid gamma in str. LM (Figure 5D). No differences were found in slow gamma-theta phase modulation in str. radiatum.
To further investigate the theta-gamma interaction, we calculated power modulation of high (20–200 Hz) frequency spectral activity as a function of the phase of theta oscillations (6–12 Hz). Cross-frequency coupling assessed as theta-gamma modulation index (MI) was performed in the CSD signals. As expected, in both conditions, the highest MI found was between theta and fast gamma (>120 Hz) in str. pyramidale and between theta and mid gamma (65–90 Hz) in str. LM. However, Cntnap2 KO mice showed an increased MI between theta and fast gamma in CA1 pyramidal cell layers and between theta and slow gamma in str. radiatum and a decreased MI between theta and mid gamma in str. LM (Figure 6). Taken together, these findings suggest altered input processing in the different layers of hippocampal CA1 in Cntnap2 KO mice.
Decreased SWR power during immobility in home-cage recording
In freely moving animals, phasic inhibition plays a major role in shaping hippocampal SWRs in CA1 (Gan et al., 2017). We therefore investigated SWRs during extended periods of immobility during home-cage recordings (Figure 7A). We found a significant decrease in SWR incidence (Figure 7B) and oscillatory amplitude in Cntnap2 KO mice (number of SWR events: 13,502 and 14,956 in WT and Cntnap2 KO, respectively) (Figure 7C). Other features characterizing SWRs, such as duration, number of cycles per event, and frequency, were similar in both conditions (Figures 7D–7F) and consistent with reported benchmarks of SWR activity (Buzsáki, 2015). Bursting events, defined as doublets and triplets, were fewer compared to singlets in both conditions. However, Cntnap2 KO mice showed an increased number of singlets and a reduced number of doublets compared to age-matched WT controls (Figure 7G).
Given the known positive correlation between the excitatory drive provided by SPWs and the ripple’s oscillatory activity recorded in CA1 (Stark et al., 2014), we next investigated if the difference in ripple power was secondary to a different input drive. Specifically, we compared the magnitude of the sharp waves recorded in str. radiatum in both conditions and found no significant differences (Figure 7H). Furthermore, the correlation coefficient between SPW amplitude and ripple power was statistically significantly smaller for Cntnap2 KO mice compared to age-matched WT controls (Figure 7I). Together, these results suggest a localized alteration in CA1 pyramidal-to-interneuron interactions during ripple generation in Cntnap2 KO mice. Given the known epileptic phenotype in Cntnap2 KO mice with ages older than 8 months (Peñagarikano et al., 2011), we investigated the occurrence of interictal epileptiform discharge (IEDs) and pathological fast ripples (p-ripples) in the CA1 region in younger animals. Using established criteria for the detection of IEDs (Gelinas et al., 2016; Lévesque et al., 2021; see STAR Methods), we did not detect fast ripple events in either WT or Cntnap2 KO mice during these recording epochs. However, we did identify sporadic IED events in 1 out of 7 Cntnap2 KO animals (Figure S5).
DISCUSSION
ASD is a common neurodevelopmental disease involving different brain regions and a spectrum of behavioral deficits (Silverman et al., 2010; Golden et al., 2018). Emerging evidence from mouse models points to a role for interneuron-mediated functional deficits underlying these complex behaviors (Vogt et al., 2015; Paterno et al., 2020; Filice et al., 2020). For example, one recent study using in vivo LFP recordings in the medial prefrontal cortex revealed a behavioral deficit correlated with reduced phase-locking to interneuron-mediated delta and theta oscillations (Lazaro et al., 2019). Although Cntnap2 is expressed in adult hippocampus and embryonic pallial sub-regions where hippocampal interneurons originate, much less is known about these interneurons in ASD. Our study provides functional evidence of reduced PV+ interneuron-mediated perisomatic inhibition onto CA1 pyramidal neurons, decreased theta-nested mid gamma activity, decreased SWR power, and an overall behavioral deficit in spatial memory in the Cntnap2 KO animal model. This hippocampal-specific microcircuit dysfunction may be amenable to hippocampal-specific therapeutic strategies for ASD
Here, we used Cntnap2 KO mice, which is a preclinical animal model with several advantages. First, it is a well-established mouse model that recapitulates key features of ASD (Peñagarikano et al., 2011). Second, previous studies suggested PV-positive interneuron dysfunction in this model (Peñagarikano et al., 2011; Vogt et al., 2018; Lauber et al., 2018). Third, using a rodent model allows us to leverage a substantial body of work linking known in vivo electrophysiological properties to underlying hippocampal cellular/network functions (Colgin, 2020). For example, gamma oscillations, SWRs, and theta-gamma cross-frequency coupling correlate to very specific and interneuron-dependent hippocampal network activities; that these findings were present in control animals but altered in Cntnap2 KO mice both validated our approach and provided a network-level interpretation of our results.
One of our major findings is that hippocampal gamma activity is altered in Cntnap2 KO mice. Interestingly, these gamma alterations were highly specific in both frequency and the hippocampal sub-layer. In general, gamma oscillations vary in frequency and relation to theta cycles across CA1 hippocampal sub-layers. In str. pyramidale, for example, fast gamma (>100 Hz) is the prominent oscillatory activity recorded with highest power at the trough of the theta cycle, corresponding to the highest probability of pyramidal cell spiking (Lasztóczi and Klausberger, 2014; Schomburg et al., 2014). Str. radiatum primarily receives input from CA3, and slow gamma oscillatory activity has been suggested to link these two regions (Csicsvari et al., 2003; Colgin et al., 2009). Finally, str. LM receives strong glutamatergic input from pyramidal cells located in the medial entorhinal cortex (Suh et al., 2011; Yamamoto et al., 2014), where mid gamma (60–90 Hz) are thought to be generated and transmitted to the hippocampus through the temporo-ammonic pathway (Colgin et al., 2009; Lasztóczi and Klausberger, 2014; Schomburg et al., 2014; Fernández-Ruiz et al., 2017). Using CSD analyses to isolate gamma oscillations from individual hippocampal layers, it was possible to infer input/output and local network activity of the underlying hippocampal circuitry. Hippocampal interneurons appear to be critical to maintain this layer-specific gamma activity as well as hippocampal input/output in general (Colgin, 2016). In particular, PV+ interneurons are primarily responsible for pyramidal cell perisomatic inhibition, whereas SOM-positive cells are primarily involved in controlling distal inhibition onto pyramidal cells (Royer et al., 2012; Fernández-Ruiz et al., 2017). Thus, the balance of inhibition from PV and SOM interneurons is critical in controlling and selecting hippocampal input from upstream regions. By altering PV interneuron density and activity, ASD animals experience a derangement of this balance of inhibition, resulting in abnormal neural oscillations as well as hippocampal input selections, which is not a consequence of a gross difference in animal behavior, given a similar running speed in both conditions.
Consistent with this, acute hippocampal slices from young adult Cntnap2 KO mice exhibited reduced perisomatic inhibition onto pyramidal cells associated with reduced inhibitory current frequency and amplitude, as well as a reduction in PV-positive interneurons. Given these PV-specific deficits, we expected to find a reduction in mid gamma power and theta-gamma modulation in str. LM during navigation, which was confirmed in Figures 4, 5, and 6. In addition, and perhaps a bit surprisingly, we found increased theta-fast gamma modulation in the pyramidal cell layer that can also be explained, at least in part, by a reduction in PV-positive interneuronal activity, given that PV-positive interneurons have a role in controlling the phase timing of (place) cells in this region. Together, these findings are consistent with reduced perisomatic inhibition in area CA1 leading to an alteration of the proximal and distal dendritic inhibition balance, thereby attenuating entorhinal cortex (EC) input in favor of CA3 input. Of course, reduced input from upstream regions such as entorhinal cortex (Brun et al., 2008) cannot be excluded from consideration as a mechanism here, and future studies will be necessary using simultaneous recordings from both regions during navigation to carefully evaluate this possibility.
The other major finding in our work is a reduction in hippocampal SWR power. SWRs play a critical role in learning and consolidation of hippocampal-dependent memories and in the decision-making process (Joo and Frank, 2018). Depolarization of CA1 pyramidal cells following a recurrent excitatory network of the CA3 area drives the onset of fast activity (ripple event) in pyramidal cell layers. The content of the event is a sequential activity of pyramidal neurons, which is dependent on previous experience (Carr et al., 2012), and PV-positive interneurons play a critical role in ensemble pyramidal cells during SWR events (Stark et al., 2014). In Cntnap2 KO mice, we found a reduced ripple power, which is considered a measure of pyramidal cell synchrony (Csicsvari et al., 2000; Schomburg et al., 2012), while all the other parameters analyzed were normal, including the degree of depolarization input. These results highlight a local network alteration likely responsible for altering the spike content of ripple events in Cntnap2 KO animals. The implication of altered ripple activity in ASD is not immediately clear; however, the fact that ASD is associated with a reduction in ripple power suggests that the ‘‘content’’ of the output message of hippocampus to cortex is abnormal in ASD (Siapas and Wilson, 1998).
An additional finding is a reduced discrimination ability in the spatial recognition task in Cntnap2 KO mice. Spatial object recognition memory is a hippocampal-dependent memory task (Broadbent et al., 2004) in which the CA1 hippocampal region has been shown to be involved in detecting novelty in the spatial environment when one object is moved in a different location (Larkin et al., 2014). Reduced discriminatory ability in these animals further highlights a hippocampal-dependent behavioral phenotype correlated with specific spatial information. Indeed, Cntnap2 KO mice were able to discriminate when tested in different type of tasks such as the context-dependent task. These animals also showed an autism-like behavioral phenotype previously described as hyperactivity, repetitive behaviors, and altered levels of anxiety. In our study, we did not detect any differences in social interaction using a modified 3-chamber social task, and even if some studies have showed an altered social interaction (Peñagarikano et al., 2011, 2015; Selimbeyoglu et al., 2017), others were not able to detect it (Scott et al., 2019). Additional studies combining different social task protocols to precisely detect which component of the social interaction is impaired in Cntnap2 KO mice, and at what precise stage of development, are warranted (as suggested in Rein et al., 2020). An additional caveat is that alterations in interneuron-mediated inhibition for Cntnap2 KO mice were evaluated at a single adult epoch in development, and it is possible that these deficits could lead to additional homeostatic alterations in circuit function (Turrigiano, 2011; Howard et al., 2014; Wu et al., 2020). Further, although a decreased number of PV+ interneurons in the hippocampal region (with a concomitant reduction in synaptic inhibition) is one plausible explanation for the altered oscillatory activity reported in this study, additional explanations are possible. For example, changes in unitary connections between inhibitory neurons and pyramidal cells, or changes in release probability at inhibitory synapses, could contribute to the observed alterations in hippocampal oscillations. In addition, PV+ interneurons include a subpopulation of interneurons such as basket, axoaxonic, and bistratified neurons with different morphological features (Que et al., 2021) and potentially distinct sub-roles in the overall modulation of network dynamics (Forro et al., 2015). As information emerges on the functional roles of distinct subpopulations of PV+ interneurons (and the experimental tools necessary to selectively manipulate these subpopulations), it will be interesting to study these possibilities in further detail.
In summary, our results show that ASD animals exhibited reduced PV+ density (Figure 1) and perisomatic inhibition (Figure 2), resulting in altered hippocampal gamma (Figures 4, 5, and 6) and SWR (Figure 7) activity, which is likely associated with a hippocampal-dependent deficit in spatial memory performance (Figure 3). Collectively, these findings suggest an involvement of the hippocampal memory system in autism phenotypes. Recent clinical studies have also implicated memory impairment as a common finding in patients with ASD (Crane and Goddard, 2008; Loth et al., 2011; Goddard et al., 2014; Robinson et al., 2017; Cooper and Simons, 2019), including deficits in object location recognition memory (Ring et al., 2015). For example, Cooper et al., (2017) investigated the neurophysiological basis of memory in ASD using an fMRI study while subjects were performing an episodic memory task and found a diminished retrieval performance associated with significantly reduced hippocampal connectivity during episodic memory recollection. Our data represent a detailed description of hippocampal-dependent activity and alterations in memory processing associated with a null Cntnap2 mutation, and further studies focused on hippocampal dependent function are warranted in ASD.
Limitations of the study
Some possible limitations should be considered when interpreting our results. First, although interpretations discussed here are concordant with a decreased number of soma-targeting hippocampal PV+ interneurons and reduced inhibition onto CA1 pyramidal neurons, functional alterations in interneuron sub-populations such as the dendrite innervating SOM+ interneurons would also influence circuit and behavior phenotypes. Although not the focus of these studies, examination of the involvement of SOM+ as well as other interneuron sub-populations are warranted in these animals. Second, interneuron function is not fixed, and compensatory PV+ interneuron alterations at different developmental ages are possible in Cntnap2 KO mice. Third, the theta-gamma alteration observed in str. LM during navigation could be interpreted as a local neuronal network alteration or a consequence of an alteration in some downstream hippocampal region such as entorhinal cortex or a combination of both. Future studies to address these latter issues might benefit from a multi-regional recoding approach.
STAR★METHODS
RESOURCE AVAILABILITY
Lead contact
Further information and requests for resources should be directed to and will be fulfilled by the lead contact, Rosalia Paterno (Rosalia. paterno@ucsf.edu).
Materials availability
This study did not generate new unique reagents.
Data and code availability
All data reported in this paper will be shared by the lead contact upon request. This paper does not report original code.
Any additional information required to reanalyze the data reported in this paper is available from the lead contact upon request.
EXPERIMENTAL MODEL AND SUBJECT DETAILS
All experiments were performed on male adult mice (≥2 months old) maintained in a C57B/6 background. Specifically, rodent genotypes Cntnap2 KO (Jackson laboratory; 017482), C57BL/6J (Jackson laboratory; 000664), and Parvalbumin tdt (+/‒) (Jackson laboratory; 027395) were used for all experiments. All animals were maintained on a 12-hour light/12-hour dark cycle with no food or water restrictions. All procedures involving animals followed guidelines of the National Institutes of Health and were approved by Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at University of California, San Francisco (#AN181254–02B).
METHOD DETAILS
Behavioral analysis
Behavioral tests were performed in male adult (P90–120) mutant and age-matched control littermates. All tests were performed during the light phase of the light/dark cycle by a trained operator blind to genotype. The order of testing was (1) open field exploration, (2) self-grooming behavior, (3) object congruence task, (4) elevated plus maze (EPM) task, and (5) modified three-chamber social task. An additional cohort of animals performed a spatial object recognition task. These tasks are further described in the following sections.
Open field
To assess locomotor activity, we used an open field consisting of a white acrylic glass arena (40 cm x 40 cm) under uniform dim lighting. Individual mice were placed in the center of the arena and their activity was recorded for 25 min. The total distance traveled, and the animal position were automatically calculated using Ethovision software tracking activity (Noldus).
Spatial object recognition task
All animals were moved into the testing room for one hour to acclimate under normal lighting conditions and handled for 5 min each day. Animals were habituated to a square testing apparatus (40 cm x 40 cm) with visual cues present on the walls for a 30-min period (habituation phase) (Bevins and Besheer, 2006). Items in the recording room were maintained constant across the experiment and served as distal cues. Each behavioral experiment consisted of 2 different phases – familiarization (F) and test (T) with a rest period between each phase. During familiarization, animals were allowed to explore for a 10-min period two identical novel objects located in 2 different quadrants of the apparatus. Between familiarization and test, animals were placed back in the home cage for 2 h and the arena and objects were cleaned to avoid any olfactory cues. During testing, one object was left in the previous location (unmoved-object) and the other object was placed in a previously empty quadrant (moved-object). The test phase lasted 5 min, during which time the animals explored the new apparatus setting. We restricted our analysis to the first 3 min of the test phase to avoid habituation (Dix and Aggleton, 1999). All experiments were videotaped with a camera placed above and analyzed post-acquisition by an operator blinded to genotype. The videos were processed using Ethovision software 11.5 (Noldus), which sampled video of the task to identify the animal’s body position at 12 frames/sec and the object exploration was manually identified as the animals approached the object within 3 cm with the nose directed to the object and in an active sniffing behavior. To measure recognition memory (Sivakumaran et al., 2018), we calculated discrimination index (DI) from the time of interaction with the moved object (Tmo) and the time of interaction with the unmoved object (Tuo) as follows:
Object congruence task
All animals were moved into the testing room for 1 hr to acclimate under normal lighting conditions and handled for 5 min each day. Experiments were carried out inside sound attenuating shells equipped with two 5W lamps. Two different contexts (40 cm x 40 cm) were used. Context A was made of a smooth white acrylic box and Context B had a grid made of green label tape and each context was cleaned with a different type of odorized cleaner. Two sets of objects were built (Object A and Object B) using Lego Megablocks each with a different shape and color/contrast; both set of objects contained an identical odor (20 μL vanilla extract place on the tip of a cotton swab) inserted inside each object. The task consisted of three 10-min trials with a period of home cage resting between each trial. Specifically, during trial 1, animals were exposed to one context containing two identical paired objects (i.e., Context A with Objects A); during trial 2, animals were exposed to another context (never encountered before, i.e., Context B with Objects B) and during test, a context was randomly chosen (Context A or Context B) containing one object A and one Object B. During test, objects were then defined as congruent (object previously shown with the same context i.e., Object A in Context A) or incongruent (object incongruent with the current context, i.e., Object B in Context A). Home cage resting time was 30-min between trial 1 and trial 2 and 2-hr between trial 2 and test. All experiments were videotaped with a camera placed above and analyzed post-acquisition by an operator blinded to genotype. The videos were processed using Ethovision software 11.5 (Noldus), which sampled video of the task to identify the animal’s body position at 12 frames/sec and the object exploration was manually identified as the animals approached the object within 3 cm and the nose directed to the object and in an active sniffing behavior.
Elevated plus maze task
The plus maze consisted of 4-arms elevated 50-cm from the floor with 2-closed arms with walls and 2-open arms (with no walls) connected by a junction part. All animals were placed in the junction part of the maze and animal position was recorded for 10 min using a tracking system that automatically calculates time spent in each arm, distance traveled, and number of entries in each arm.
Self-grooming behavior
All animals were moved into the testing room for 1 hr to acclimate under normal lighting conditions and handled for 5 min each day. They were then placed individually in a standard mouse cage (13 × 32 × 18 cm) without bedding and recorded for 20 min with a camera placed 1.5 m away on the side of the cage. Videos were then processed post-acquisition by an operator blinded to genotype. Self-grooming was scored for 10-min periods as a measure for grooming in all body parts after 10-min habituation period in the home cage. Number and duration of each episode was reported. If the time between consecutive events was less than 5 s, the events were combined and reported as single event.
Modified three-chamber social task
Social interaction was assessed using a modified-three chamber task. The task utilized a Plexiglas box composed of two interconnected chambers with one plastic cup in each chamber and consisted of two 10-min phases: Habituation (H) and Test (T). During Habituation (H), animals were allowed to explore the two chambers containing the two-empty cups for a 10-min period. During Test (T), animals were allowed to explore the two chambers, containing one empty cup in one chamber and one cup with an unfamiliar mouse in the other chamber. All experiments were videotaped with a camera placed above the chambers and analyzed post-acquisition by an operator blinded to genotype. Videos were processed using Ethovision software 11.5 (Noldus), which sampled video of the task to identify the animal’s body position at 12 frames/sec. The time and distance each animal spent in each chamber was calculated and interaction time with the empty or social cup was manually identified as animals approached the cup within 3 cm with nose directed to the object and in an active sniffing behavior.
Hippocampal slice preparation
Horizontal hippocampal-entorhinal cortex slices were prepared from Cntnap2 KO mice and age-matched WT sibling controls. Following deep anesthesia with isoflurane, mice were euthanized by decapitation. The brain was rapidly dissected and immersed in ice-cold oxygenated (95% O2/ 5% CO2) high-sucrose artificial cerebrospinal solution (sACSF) containing (in mM) 220 sucrose, 3 KCl, 26 NaHCO3, 10 dextrose, 2 MgSO4, 1.25 NaH2PO4 and 1 CaCl2. 320 mm thickness slices were made using a VT 1000S microtome (Leica) and immediately after transferred to an incubation chamber with artificial cerebrospinal fluid (ACSF) containing (in mM) 124 NaCl, 26 NaHCO3, 10 dextrose, 3 KCl, 1 CaCl2, 2 MgSO4, and 1.25 NaH2PO4, and maintained at 35°C for 30 min, and thereafter at room temperature for at least 40 min before recording. Ex vivo recordings were performed in slices from P90–110 WT and Cntnap2 KO mice, or P60–90 WT PV-tdt (+/‒) and Cntnap2 KO PVtdt (+/‒) mice.
Patch-clamp recording
Slices were transferred individually to the recording chamber, which was continuously perfused with oxygenated ACSF (2–3 ml/min at 28–30°C). Whole-cell patch-clamp recordings were performed from hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons or fluorescent-labeled parvalbumin-positive (PV+) interneurons, using an infrared differential interference contrast (IR-DIC) video-microscopy system with an epifluorescent microscope (Olympus BX50-WI) and water-immersion 40x objective. PV+ interneurons were identified by expression of a red fluorescent protein tdTomato. Patch micropipettes (3–6 MΩ) were pulled from borosilicate glass using a computer-controlled micropipette puller (P-1000, Sutter Instrument) and filled with the appropriate internal solution. To measure inhibitory postsynaptic currents (IPSC), internal solution contained (in mM) 117.5 Cs-Gluconate, 11 CsCl2, 1 MgCl2, 10 HEPES, 11 EGTA, 2 Na2ATP, 0.5 Na2GTP and 1.25 QX-314 (285–290 mOsm, pH 7.2). Recordings were performed at a holding potential of 0 mV. To measure excitatory postsynaptic currents (EPSC), internal solution contained (in mM) 135 CsCl2, 10 NaCl, 2 MgCl2, 10 HEPES, 10 EGTA, 2 Na2ATP and 0.2 Na2GTP (285–290 mOsm, pH 7.2). Recordings were performed at a holding potential of ‒65 mV. Intrinsic membrane properties were recorded in current-clamp mode with internal solution containing (in mM) 120 K-gluconate, 10 KCl 1 MgCl2, 0.025 CaCl2, 10 HEPES, 0.2 EGTA, 2 Na2ATP, 0.2 Na2GTP (285–290 mOsm, pH 7.2). Recordings were performed at a holding potential of ‒65 mV, and electrophysiological properties were measured in response to 250-ms current step injections delivered in 10 pA increments from ‒150 to +150 pA. Data were recorded using a Multiclamp 700B amplifier (Molecular Devices) and monitored with pClamp 10.3 software (Molecular Devices). Whole-cell voltage-clamp data were low-pass filtered at 1 kHz and digitally sampled at 10 kHz using a Digidata 1550A (Molecular Devices). Each IPSC event were manually selected using Mini Analysis 6.0.7 (Synaptosoft Software), and a mean of 100 individual events were analyzed for each cell.
Surgery for in vivo recordings
Six control and seven Cntnap2 KO male mice between 90–120 days of age were implanted with chronic silicon probe arrays (Vandecasteele et al., 2012) comprised of four-shanks spaced 400 mm apart with eight electrodes sites in each shank spaced 200 mm apart (A4×8 5mm 200–400-703 CM32; from NeuroNexus Technologies). Before implantation, the back of the shank was covered with DiI (lipophilic fluorescent dye - Thermo Fischer Scientific) to subsequently track electrode locations. Adult mice (3–4 months old; 28–30 g) were anesthetized with Ketamine/Xylazine mixture injected intraperitoneally and maintained with isoflurane 0.8 – 1.2% through a nose cone mounted on a mouse stereotaxic frame (Kopf Instruments, USA). The scalp was reflected with a single incision, disinfected with 3% hydrogen peroxide and the periosteum scraped from the scalp. All the following procedures were conducted under 0.5–5x magnification. Skull screws (Machine Screw #000–120× 1/16’’ Flat Head, Component Supply Company) were placed in the scalp to anchor and support the implant, and two additional screws (Fine Science Tools) were placed in the cerebellum for the reference and ground wires. The entire structure was additionally secured with dental adhesive cement (C&B- Metabond, Parkell). A rectangular craniotomy was then performed on the exposed skull, and the dura was opened. Once the brain parenchyma was exposed, the probe was lowered with a micromanipulator (Kopf Instruments) 2.5 mm beneath the skull surface to target the dorsal hippocampus at the following coordinates from bregma AP: – 2.2 mm and ML: 1.3 ‒1.5 mm. The reference and ground wires were then wrapped around the screws previously placed in the cerebellum area. The exposed cortex and electrodes shanks were covered with Kwik-Sil silicone elastomer. The remaining structure was secured with dental cement (Stoelting Co) to ensure headcap stability overtime. Mice were treated with buprenorphine 0.05 – 0.1 mg/kg and ketofen 5 mg/kg for the following two-three days or until no signs of pain were detected. The animals were given at least one-week recovery time before recording.
Electrode localization
To identify electrode localization, both physiologic and histological techniques were used (Bragin et al., 1995; Csicsvari et al., 1999; Montgomery et al., 2008; Vandecasteele et al., 2012). Specifically, mice were deeply anesthetized with a mixture of Ketamine/Xylazine and perfused transcardially with saline followed by 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA; Electron Microscopy Science). Brains were post-fixed with PFA 4% and 50 μm thick coronal sections were cut with a VT 1000S vibratome (Leica Microsystems Inc., Buffalo Grove, IL). Right hemibrains (containing the electrodes) were used for DAPI (PureBlu DAPI Nuclear Staining Dye, BioRad) staining to anatomically identify the labeled-electrodes tracks. Stained sections were then examined with a Nikon microscope. In addition, local field potential (LFP), sharp wave ripple (SWR) events (150–250 Hz) were used to functionally identify electrode location. SWRs were identified in the CA1 pyramidal cell layer and the concomitant sharp wave was used to locate electrodes in the deep layers (Csicsvari et al., 1999). Subsequently, SWR-comodulogram was calculated (Gillespie et al., 2016) and dentate spikes were detected in a sliding-window (Bragin et al., 1995). The final electrode positions were found by using (1) the histological sections to determine the vertical axis of the arrays position, (2) SWRs to determine electrodes in area CA1 and (3) dentate spikes to identify electrodes in dentate gyrus layers.
In vivo behavioral task and analysis
After the one-week post-operative recovery-time was completed, mice were handled for 3-min/day for the duration of the experiment and allowed to habituate to the recording room and the recording apparatus with home cage recordings for 1–1.5 h/day for 5–7 consecutive days. Afterward, they were exposed to a novel/familiar context paradigm. In detail, we placed mice in different boxes with identical dimensions (40 × 40 cm). In each different box, hereafter referred to as different ‘contexts’ for the remainder of the paper, the base and walls varied by color and material type. In addition, we placed unique spatial cues (printed shapes) on the walls. Thus, a unique context was defined by the color and texture of the walls and base, as well as the spatial cues on different faces of the walls. Using this method, we created three different contexts, which were consistent across mice. In the task, animals were first transferred to the recording room in their home cage and allowed to acclimate to the room for 1h before recording. Following this, animals were recorded for 30-min in the home cage. They then were placed in the first context for 25 min, followed by another 30 min within the home cage, followed by another 25 min in the same context, and finally another 15 min in the home cage. This cycle was repeated on three consecutive days, one for each unique context. Thus, on each day the context was changed such that the animal never encountered the same context on different recording days. Of note, all experiments were run during the light cycle from 1:00 to 7:00 PM PST.
Recording and data analysis
Animal position and running speed were tracked and recorded during the task by a digital video camera (NeuroMotive Camera, BlackRock Microsystem) which recognized the mouse head-mount LED and sampled the head position at 50 frames/sec. We validated this automatic tracking algorithm using Ethovision software 11.5 (Noldus), which sampled video of the task to identify the animal’s body position at 25 frames/sec.
Electrophysiological recordings were continuously acquired using a Multichannel Acquisition Cerebus System and a Cereplex mu headstage (16-bit resolution; BlackRock Microsystems, Salt Lake City, UT) at 32 KHz. Raw signal was first visually inspected and electrodes with noise were excluded from further analysis. Using custom-written MATLAB-based software (The MathWorks, MA, USA), LFPs were low pass filtered with a cut-off frequency of 40% of the Nyquist rate (413.6 Hz), and then downsampled to 1034 Hz.
Sharp wave ripple detection and analysis
The detection of sharp wave ripples (SWRs) was restricted to ‘immobility periods’ during home cage recording defined as speed less than 1.5 cm/sec, duration more than 20 s and low-theta periods. We found all such immobility periods during the home cage recordings from day 3 to 5, to ensure that mice were habituated to the recording setting and room. The LFP from all electrodes during the immobility periods was then bandpass filtered from 150–250 Hz. The baseline mean and standard deviation of these power values were used to define ripple-candidate events as periods when the smoothed envelope in the filtered data was greater than 3 standard deviations (SD) above baseline for at least 15 ms (Cheng and Frank, 2008). The beginning and end of these events were defined as the period of time in which the power surrounding the candidate ripple events was above one SD; events closer than 40 ms were combined.
Sharp waves (SPWs) were detected within these immobility periods in the channels located in CA1- stratum radiatum. The LFP signal was bandpass filtered at 4–50 Hz and events in the range 20–400 ms exceeding 2.5 SD above baseline were included as candidate SPWs (Fernández-Ruiz et al., 2019). SWRs events were then defined as periods in which both an SPW in CA1- stratum radiatum and a ripple in CA1- stratum pyramidale were found. SWR bursts were defined as periods when more than one event was detected in a 200 ms time window; these bursts were classified as doublets and triplets if there were two or three events, respectively (Yamamoto and Tonegawa, 2017). To detect fast ripples, LFPs from electrodes located in stratum pyramidale during periods of immobility were bandpass filtered from 250–600 Hz. Putative fast ripples were identified as periods in which the smoothed envelope in the filtered data was greater than 5 Standard Deviations (SD) above baseline. Events were required to have at least six cycles of oscillations (i.e., six peaks) with each peak having an amplitude greater than 5 SD of the bandpass LFP filter (Lévesque et al., 2011). To confirm that these fast ripple events were distinct from noise artifact, peak power in frequencies greater than 200 Hz had to be larger than peak power in the frequency range 75–125 Hz for each event (Ewell et al., 2019). Each individual identified fast ripple event was visually inspected and misidentified events were removed from further analysis.
Interictal epileptiform discharge (IEDs) were detected during immobility periods previously selected (see above) in the channel located in CA1-stratum pyramidale. LFP signal was first bandpass filtered at 60–80 Hz and IED events were identified when the smoothed envelope in the filtered data was greater than 5 SD above baseline and the event with an unfiltered envelope was greater than 5 SD above baseline (Gelinas et al., 2016; Lévesque et al., 2021). Using these criteria, detected IEDs exhibited waveform features similar to those shown previously (Buzsáki et al., 1991); representative IED examples are shown in Figure S5.
Spectral analysis
Theta epochs were detected automatically using a delta (1–4 Hz) and theta (6–10 Hz) ratio followed by manual adjustment with visual inspection of the LFP and power spectrum. These epochs were restricted to epochs in which the animal was walking/running in the open field. Those epochs were then, convolved with a bank of 100 Morlet wavelet filters (Addison, 2017), linearly spaced from 2 to 100 Hz to generate an instantaneous power and phase. The calculated instantaneous power was then averaged across time to obtain an estimate of the power spectrum for each epoch. To normalize power spectra across epochs, we calculated relative power in which each frequency was expressed as percentage of the total spectrum.
To analyze LFP power as a function of individual theta cycles (‘cycle-by-cycle’ analysis), we first extracted theta phase from the LFP recorded in the pyramidal cell layer. We bandpass filtered the signal between 1–40 Hz and linearly interpolated phase between minima and maxima in each theta cycle using a zero-crossing of theta filtered (6–10 Hz) signal (Belluscio et al., 2012). Theta peaks corresponded to 0° and 360° and theta troughs corresponded to 180° and 540° of theta cycles recorded in pyramidal cell layer. The theta phase of the pyramidal layer was used as reference for all analyses reported in the paper. To calculate gamma power in each theta cycle, we applied 65 Morlet wavelets (as previously described) linearly spaced between 20–150 Hz, and the resulting power amplitude values were z-scored across all the cycles in the theta epochs for each recording session. Theta phase was divided into 16 bins and the z-scored wavelet amplitude was averaged for each bin to obtain the distribution of gamma amplitude by theta phase.
To correct for volume conduction of the LFPs recorded across layers, cycle-by-cycle analysis was also conducted using current source density (CSD) signals. Specifically, CSD signals were calculated as follows,
where LFP(n) represents the LFP signal from the electrode of interest; LFP(n-1) and LFP(n+1) refer signals from electrodes above and below the electrode of interest, respectively, and distance refers to the distance between electrodes in millimeters. To test whether high frequency activity varied as a function of the theta cycle, three gamma sub-bands were averaged across each theta phase bin as follow: slow gamma (25–55 Hz), mid gamma (65–90 Hz) and fast gamma (> 100 Hz).
To measure cross-frequency coupling between theta and gamma activity, we calculated a modulation index (MI) (Tort et al., 2010). Briefly, we calculated the relationship between LFP phase in the range 6–12 Hz, and LFP amplitude in 5 Hz non-overlapping windows from 20 to 200 Hz during theta epochs. To ensure that each event had enough length for the low frequency calculation, we restricted the analysis to theta epochs longer than 1.5 s and performed the calculation on the CSD signal previously extracted.
Immunohistochemistry, imaging and quantification
Adult (P90–120) WT and Cntnap2 KO mice were deeply anesthetized with a mixture of Ketamine/Xylazine and perfused transcardially with saline followed by 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA; Electron Microscopy Science). Brains were post-fixed with PFA 4% and 50 mm thick coronal sections were cut with a VT 1000S vibratome (Leica Microsystems Inc., Buffalo Grove, IL). Primary antibody used: anti-parvalbumin (Sigma-Aldrich; P3088; 1:500) and RFP (Rockland; 600–401-379; 1:1000). Secondary antibody used: Alexa 488 and Alexa 594 (Invitrogen; A11001; A11012; 1:1000). Images were acquired at 1024 pixels resolution using a Nikon confocal microscope. Quantification analysis was performed using NIS-Elements (Nikon software) in fluorescent label sections (50 μm) imaged with 4X objective. All cells that expressed the marker were counted in every sixth coronal section (300 μm apart). To quantify the PV-positive density in the hippocampus, 3 sections for the dorsal hippocampus were analyzed for each animal and the value averaged to calculate a mean cell density (cell/mm2).
Graphical abstract
The graphical abstract was prepared using the BioRender software (BioRender.com).
QUANTIFICATION AND STATISTICAL ANALYSIS
All analyses were performed using MATLAB (MathWorks) and PRISM (GraphPad). We compared effects across session for each of the two animal conditions (wild-type versus KO). Based upon normality, we used parametric or non-parametric tests accordingly as specified in each analysis. For multi-comparison correction, post hoc testing was done using the Tukey or Sidak correction, as indicated in each analysis. All plots with error bars are reported as mean ± SEMs.
Supplementary Material
REAGENT or RESOURCE | SOURCE | IDENTIFIER |
---|---|---|
Antibodies | ||
| ||
Mouse anti-PV | Millipore-Sigma | Cat#P3088; RRID: AB_477329 |
Rabbit anti-RFP | Rockland | Cat#600–401-379; RRID: AB_2209751 |
Alexa Fluor488 goat anti-mouse IgG | Thermo Fisher | Cat#A-11001; RRID: AB_2534069 |
Alexa Fluor594 goat anti-rabbit IgG | Thermo Fisher | Cat#A-11012; RRID: AB_141359 |
| ||
Chemicals, peptides, and recombinant proteins | ||
| ||
DAPI | BioRad | Cat#1351303 |
Dil | Thermo Fisher | Cat#D282 |
Paraformaldehyde Aqueous Solution 16% | Electronic Microscopy Sciences | Cat#: RT15710-S |
Sucrose | Fisher Scientific | Cat#S3–500 |
Potassium Chloride | Millipore Sigma | Cat#PX1405 |
Calcium Chloride Dihydrate | Fisher Scientific | Cat#BP510–100 |
Magnesium Sulfate Heptahydrate | Fisher Scientific | Cat#BP213–1 |
Dextrose | Fisher Scientific | Cat#D15–500 |
Sodium Bicarbonate | Fisher Scientific | Cat#BP328–500 |
Sodium Phosphate Monobasic Monohydrate | Fisher Scientific | Cat#BP330–500 |
Sodium Chloride | Fisher Scientific | Cat#BP358–212 |
HEPES | Fisher Scientific | Cat#AC172572500 |
EGTA | Millipore Sigma | Cat#324626 |
QX-314 | Millipore Sigma | Cat#552233 |
Magnesium Chloride | Fisher Scientific | Cat#BP214–500 |
Cesium chloride | Millipore Sigma | Cat#C3011 |
Adenosine 5′-triphosphate disodium salt hydrate | Millipore Sigma | Cat#A26209 |
Guanosine 5′-triphosphate sodium salt hydrate | Millipore Sigma | Cat#G8877 |
| ||
Deposited data | ||
| ||
Raw and analyzed data | This Paper | Available upon request |
MATLAB Code | This Paper | Available upon request |
| ||
Experimental models: Organisms/strains | ||
| ||
Mouse: C57BL/6J | Jackson laboratory | JAX: 000664 |
Mouse: Cntnap2 knock-out | Jackson laboratory | JAX: 017482 |
Mouse: Pvalb-tdTomato | Jackson laboratory | JAX: 027395 |
| ||
Software and algorithms | ||
| ||
Adobe Illustrator CS6 | Adobe Systems | https://www.adobe.com/products/illustrator.html |
MATLAB 2017b | MathWorks | https://www.mathworks.com; RRID:SCR_001622 |
EthoVision XT | Noldus | https://www.noldus.com/animal-behavior-research/ |
Mini Analysis 6.0.7 | Synaptosoft | http://www.synaptosoft.com/MiniAnalysis/ |
PRISM | Graphpad | https://www.graphpad.com/ |
NIS-Elements | Nikon Software | https://www.microscope.healthcare.nikon.com/en_EU/products/software/nis-elements |
BioRender | BioRender.com | N/A |
| ||
Other | ||
| ||
Stereotaxic frame | Kopf Instruments | N/A |
Vibratome | Leica Biosystems Inc. | Cat#VT1000S |
Multichannel Acquisition Cerebus System | BlackRock Microsystems | N/A |
Silicon probes: 32-channel; 200 mm inter-spacing; 703 mm2 electrode area | NeuroNexus Technologies | CM32: A4×8 5mm 200–400-703 |
Highlights.
Reduced number of PV-positive interneurons in hippocampal area CA1 of Cntnap2 KO mice
Reduced frequency and amplitude of IPSCs onto CA1 pyramidal neurons of Cntnap2 KO mice
Hippocampal alteration in gamma oscillations and sharp wave ripples in Cntnap2 KO mice
Impaired spatial discrimination ability in Cntnap2 KO mice
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
The authors would like to thank Matthew Dinday for his general assistance in planning experiments and maintaining rodent colonies and Colleen Carpenter and Ivan Soltesz for their helpful comments on earlier versions of this research. This work was supported by NIH/NINDS grant R37 NS-071785–07 to S.C.B.
Footnotes
SUPPLEMENTAL INFORMATION
Supplemental information can be found online at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2021.109970.
DECLARATION OF INTERESTS
The authors declare no competing interests.
BIBLIOGRAPHY
- Abrahams BS, Arking DE, Campbell DB, Mefford HC, Morrow EM, Weiss LA, Menashe I, Wadkins T, Banerjee-Basu S, and Packer A (2013). SFARI Gene 2.0: a community-driven knowledgebase for the autism spectrum disorders (ASDs). Mol. Autism 4, 36. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Addison PS (2017). The Illustrated Wavelet Transform Handbook: Introductory Theory and Applications in Science (Engineering, Medicine and Finance; ). [Google Scholar]
- Ariza J, Rogers H, Hashemi E, Noctor SC, and Martínez-Cerdeño V (2018). The Number of Chandelier and Basket Cells Are Differentially Decreased in Prefrontal Cortex in Autism. Cereb. Cortex 28, 411–420. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arking DE, Cutler DJ, Brune CW, Teslovich TM, West K, Ikeda M, Rea A, Guy M, Lin S, Cook EH, and Chakravarti A (2008). A common genetic variant in the neurexin superfamily member CNTNAP2 increases familial risk of autism. Am. J. Hum. Genet 82, 160–164. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baio J, Wiggins L, Christensen DL, Maenner MJ, Daniels J, Warren Z, Kurzius-Spencer M, Zahorodny W, Rosenberg CR, White T, et al. (2018). Prevalence of Autism Spectrum Disorder Among Children Aged 8 Years ‒ Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network, 11 Sites, United States, 2014. Surveillance Summaries 67, 1–23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Belluscio MA, Mizuseki K, Schmidt R, Kempter R, and Buzsáki G (2012). Cross-frequency phase-phase coupling between θ and γ oscillations in the hippocampus. J. Neurosci 32, 423–435. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bevins RA, and Besheer J (2006). Object recognition in rats and mice: a one-trial non-matching-to-sample learning task to study ‘recognition memory’. Nat. Protoc 1, 1306–1311. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bezaire MJ, and Soltesz I (2013). Quantitative assessment of CA1 local circuits: knowledge base for interneuron-pyramidal cell connectivity. Hippocampus 23, 751–785. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blatt GJ, Fitzgerald CM, Guptill JT, Booker AB, Kemper TL, and Bauman ML (2001). Density and Distribution of Hippocampal Neurotransmitter Receptors in Autism: An Autoradiographic Study. J. Autism Dev. Disord 7, 537–543. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bragin A, Jandó G, Nádasdy Z, van Landeghem M, and Buzsáki G (1995). Dentate EEG spikes and associated interneuronal population bursts in the hippocampal hilar region of the rat. J. Neurophysiol 73, 1691–1705. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Broadbent NJ, Squire LR, and Clark RE (2004). Spatial memory, recognition memory, and the hippocampus. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 101, 14515– 14520. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brun VH, Leutgeb S, Wu H-Q, Schwarcz R, Witter MP, Moser EI, and Moser M-B (2008). Impaired spatial representation in CA1 after lesion of direct input from entorhinal cortex. Neuron 57, 290–302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burgess N (2008). Spatial cognition and the brain. Ann. N Y Acad. Sci 1124, 77–97. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buzsáki G (2002). Theta oscillations in the hippocampus. Neuron 33, 325–340. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buzsáki G (2015). Hippocampal sharp wave-ripple: A cognitive biomarker for episodic memory and planning. Hippocampus 25, 1073–1188. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buzsáki G, Hsu M, Slamka C, Gage FH, and Horváth Z (1991). Emergence and propagation of interictal spikes in the subcortically denervated hippocampus. Hippocampus 1, 163–180. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cardin JA, Carlén M, Meletis K, Knoblich U, Zhang F, Deisseroth K, Tsai L-H, and Moore CI (2009). Driving fast-spiking cells induces gamma rhythm and controls sensory responses. Nature 459, 663–667. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carr MF, Karlsson MP, and Frank LM (2012). Transient slow gamma synchrony underlies hippocampal memory replay. Neuron 75, 700–713. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chao H-T, Chen H, Samaco RC, Xue M, Chahrour M, Yoo J, Neul JL, Gong S, Lu H-C, Heintz N, et al. (2010). Dysfunction in GABA signalling mediates autism-like stereotypies and Rett syndrome phenotypes. Nature 468, 263–269. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cheng S, and Frank LM (2008). New experiences enhance coordinated neural activity in the hippocampus. Neuron 57, 303–313. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cobb SR, Buhl EH, Halasy K, Paulsen O, and Somogyi P (1995). Synchronization of neuronal activity in hippocampus by individual GABAergic interneurons. Nature 378, 75–78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Colgin LL (2016). Rhythms of the hippocampal network. Nat. Rev. Neurosci 17, 239–249. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Colgin LL (2020). Five Decades of Hippocampal Place Cells and EEG Rhythms in Behaving Rats. J. Neurosci 40, 54–60. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Colgin LL, Denninger T, Fyhn M, Hafting T, Bonnevie T, Jensen O, Moser M-B, and Moser EI (2009). Frequency of gamma oscillations routes flow of information in the hippocampus. Nature 462, 353–357. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cooper RA, and Simons JS (2019). Exploring the neurocognitive basis of episodic recollection in autism. Psychon. Bull. Rev 26, 163–181. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cooper RA, Richter FR, Bays PM, Plaisted-Grant KC, Baron-Cohen S, and Simons JS (2017). Reduced Hippocampal Functional Connectivity During Episodic Memory Retrieval in Autism. Cereb. Cortex 27, 888–902. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crane L, and Goddard L (2008). Episodic and semantic autobiographical memory in adults with autism spectrum disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord 38, 498–506. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Csicsvari J, Hirase H, Czurkó A, Mamiya A, and Buzsáki G (1999). Oscillatory coupling of hippocampal pyramidal cells and interneurons in the behaving Rat. J. Neurosci 19, 274–287. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Csicsvari J, Hirase H, Mamiya A, and Buzsáki G (2000). Ensemble patterns of hippocampal CA3-CA1 neurons during sharp wave-associated population events. Neuron 28, 585–594. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Csicsvari J, Jamieson B, Wise KD, and Buzsáki G (2003). Mechanisms of gamma oscillations in the hippocampus of the behaving rat. Neuron 37, 311–322. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dix SL, and Aggleton JP (1999). Extending the spontaneous preference test of recognition: evidence of object-location and object-context recognition. Behav. Brain Res 99, 191–200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ewell LA, Fischer KB, Leibold C, Leutgeb S, and Leutgeb JK (2019). The impact of pathological high-frequency oscillations on hippocampal network activity in rats with chronic epilepsy. eLife 8, e42148. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fatemi SH, Reutiman TJ, Folsom TD, Rooney RJ, Patel DH, and Thuras PD (2010). mRNA and protein levels for GABAAalpha4, α5, β1 and GABABR1 receptors are altered in brains from subjects with autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord 40, 743–750. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fernández-Ruiz A, Oliva A, Nagy GA, Maurer AP, Berényi A, and Buzsáki G (2017). Entorhinal-CA3 Dual-Input Control of Spike Timing in the Hippocampus by Theta-Gamma Coupling. Neuron 93, 1213–1226.e5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fernández-Ruiz A, Oliva A, Fermino de Oliveira E, Rocha-Almeida F, Tingley D, and Buzsáki G (2019). Long-duration hippocampal sharp wave ripples improve memory. Science 364, 1082–1086. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Filice F (2016). Reduction in parvalbumin expression not loss of the parvalbumin-expressing GABA interneuron subpopulation in genetic parvalbumin and shank mouse models of autism. Molecular Brain 9, 10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Filice F, Janickova L, Henzi T, Bilella A, and Schwaller B (2020). The Parvalbumin Hypothesis of Autism Spectrum Disorder. Front. Cell. Neurosci 14, 577525. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Forro T, Valenti O, Lasztoczi B, and Klausberger T (2015). Temporal organization of GABAergic interneurons in the intermediate CA1 hippocampus during network oscillations. Cereb. Cortex 25, 1228–1240. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freund TF, and Katona I (2007). Perisomatic inhibition. Neuron 56, 33–42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gan J, Weng SM, Pernía-Andrade AJ, Csicsvari J, and Jonas P (2017). Phase-Locked Inhibition, but Not Excitation, Underlies Hippocampal Ripple Oscillations in Awake Mice In Vivo. Neuron 93, 308–314. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gelinas JN, Khodagholy D, Thesen T, Devinsky O, and Buzsáki G (2016). Interictal epileptiform discharges induce hippocampal-cortical coupling in temporal lobe epilepsy. Nat. Med 22, 641–648. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gelman DM, and Marín O (2010). Generation of interneuron diversity in the mouse cerebral cortex. Eur. J. Neurosci 31, 2136–2141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gillespie AK, Jones EA, Lin Y-H, Karlsson MP, Kay K, Yoon SY, Tong LM, Nova P, Carr JS, Frank LM, and Huang Y (2016). Apolipoprotein E4 Causes Age-Dependent Disruption of Slow Gamma Oscillations during Hippocampal Sharp-Wave Ripples. Neuron 90, 740–751. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goddard L, Dritschel B, Robinson S, and Howlin P (2014). Development of autobiographical memory in children with autism spectrum disorders: deficits, gains, and predictors of performance. Dev. Psychopathol 26, 215–228. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gogolla N, Leblanc JJ, Quast KB, Südhof TC, Fagiolini M, and Hensch TK (2009). Common circuit defect of excitatory-inhibitory balance in mouse models of autism. J. Neurodev. Disord 1, 172–181. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Golden CE, Buxbaum JD, and De Rubeis S (2018). Disrupted circuits in mouse models of autism spectrum disorder and intellectual disability. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol 48, 106–112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gordon A, Salomon D, Barak N, Pen Y, Tsoory M, Kimchi T, and Peles E (2016). Expression of Cntnap2 (Caspr2) in multiple levels of sensory systems. Mol. Cell. Neurosci 70, 42–53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guptill JT, Booker AB, Gibbs TT, Kemper TL, Bauman ML, and Blatt GJ (2007). [3H]-flunitrazepam-labeled benzodiazepine binding sites in the hippocampal formation in autism: a multiple concentration autoradiographic study. J. Autism Dev. Disord 37, 911–920. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harada M, Taki MM, Nose A, Kubo H, Mori K, Nishitani H, and Matsuda T (2011). Non-invasive evaluation of the GABAergic/glutamatergic system in autistic patients observed by MEGA-editing proton MR spectroscopy using a clinical 3 tesla instrument. J. Autism Dev. Disord 41, 447–454. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hashemi E, Ariza J, Rogers H, Noctor SC, and Martínez-Cerdeño V (2018). The Number of Parvalbumin-Expressing Interneurons Is Decreased in the Prefrontal Cortex in Autism. Cereb. Cortex 28, 690. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hasselmo ME, Bodelón C, and Wyble BP (2002). A proposed function for hippocampal theta rhythm: separate phases of encoding and retrieval enhance reversal of prior learning. Neural Comput 14, 793–817. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Howard MA, Rubenstein JLR, and Baraban SC (2014). Bidirectional homeostatic plasticity induced by interneuron cell death and transplantation in vivo. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 111, 492–497. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hyman SL, Levy SE, and Myers SM; COUNCIL ON CHILDREN WITH DISABILITIES, SECTION ON DEVELOPMENTAL AND BEHAVIORAL PEDIATRICS (2020). Identification, Evaluation, and Management of Children With Autism Spectrum Disorder. Pediatrics 145, e20193447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iakoucheva LM, Muotri AR, and Sebat J (2019). Getting to the Cores of Autism. Cell 178, 1287–1298. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ito-Ishida A, Ure K, Chen H, Swann JW, and Zoghbi HY (2015). Loss of MeCP2 in Parvalbumin-and Somatostatin-Expressing Neurons in Mice Leads to Distinct Rett Syndrome-like Phenotypes. Neuron 88, 651–658. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Joo HR, and Frank LM (2018). The hippocampal sharp wave-ripple in memory retrieval for immediate use and consolidation. Nat. Rev. Neurosci 19, 744–757. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jurgensen S, and Castillo PE (2015). Selective Dysregulation of Hippocampal Inhibition in the Mouse Lacking Autism Candidate Gene CNTNAP2. J. Neurosci 35, 14681–14687. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kalueff AV, Stewart AM, Song C, Berridge KC, Graybiel AM, and Fentress JC (2016). Neurobiology of rodent self-grooming and its value for translational neuroscience. Nat. Rev. Neurosci 17, 45–59. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim YS, Leventhal BL, Koh Y-J, Fombonne E, Laska E, Lim E-C, Cheon K-A, Kim S-J, Kim Y-K, Lee H, et al. (2011). Prevalence of autism spectrum disorders in a total population sample. Am. J. Psychiatry 168, 904–912. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klausberger T, Magill PJ, Márton LF, Roberts JDB, Cobden PM, Buzsáki G, and Somogyi P (2003). Brain-state- and cell-type-specific firing of hippocampal interneurons in vivo. Nature 421, 844–848. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kosaka T, Katsumaru H, Hama K, Wu J-Y, and Heizmann CW (1987). GABAergic neurons containing the Ca2+-binding protein parvalbumin in the rat hippocampus and dentate gyrus. Brain Res 419, 119–130. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Larkin MC, Lykken C, Tye LD, Wickelgren JG, and Frank LM (2014). Hippocampal output area CA1 broadcasts a generalized novelty signal during an object-place recognition task. Hippocampus 24, 773–783. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lasztóczi B, and Klausberger T (2014). Layer-specific GABAergic control of distinct gamma oscillations in the CA1 hippocampus. Neuron 81, 1126–1139. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lauber E, Filice F, and Schwaller B (2016). Prenatal Valproate Exposure Differentially Affects Parvalbumin-Expressing Neurons and Related Circuits in the Cortex and Striatum of Mice. Front. Mol. Neurosci 9, 150. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lauber E, Filice F, and Schwaller B (2018). Dysregulation of Parvalbumin Expression in the Cntnap2−/− Mouse Model of Autism Spectrum Disorder. Front. Mol. Neurosci 11, 262. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lawrence YA, Kemper TL, Bauman ML, and Blatt GJ (2010). Parvalbumin-, calbindin-, and calretinin-immunoreactive hippocampal interneuron density in autism. Acta Neurol. Scand 121, 99–108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lazaro MT, Taxidis J, Shuman T, Bachmutsky I, Ikrar T, Santos R, Marcello GM, Mylavarapu A, Chandra S, Foreman A, et al. (2019). Reduced Prefrontal Synaptic Connectivity and Disturbed Oscillatory Population Dynamics in the CNTNAP2 Model of Autism. Cell Rep 27, 2567–2578.e6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee E, Lee J, and Kim E (2017). Excitation/Inhibition Imbalance in Animal Models of Autism Spectrum Disorders. Biol. Psychiatry 81, 838–847. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lévesque M, Bortel A, Gotman J, and Avoli M (2011). High-frequency (80–500 Hz) oscillations and epileptogenesis in temporal lobe epilepsy. Neurobiol. Dis 42, 231–241. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lévesque M, Macey-Dare ADB, Wang S, and Avoli M (2021). Evolution of interictal spiking during the latent period in a mouse model of mesial temporal lobe epilepsy. Curr. Res. Neurobiol 2, 100008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li X-G, Somogyi P, Tepper JM, and Buzsáki G (1992). Axonal and dendritic arborization of an intracellularly labeled chandelier cell in the CA1 region of rat hippocampus. Exp. Brain Res 90, 519–525. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Loth E, Gómez JC, and Happé F (2011). Do high-functioning people with autism spectrum disorder spontaneously use event knowledge to selectively attend to and remember context-relevant aspects in scenes? J. Autism Dev. Disord 41, 945–961. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miles R, Tóth K, Gulyás AI, Hájos N, and Freund TF (1996). Differences between somatic and dendritic inhibition in the hippocampus. Neuron 16, 815–823. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Montgomery SM, Sirota A, and Buzsáki G (2008). Theta and gamma coordination of hippocampal networks during waking and rapid eye movement sleep. J. Neurosci 28, 6731–6741. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Keefe J, and Nadel L (1978). The hippocampus as a cognitive map (Clarendon Press; Oxford University Press; ). [Google Scholar]
- Paterno R, Casalia M, and Baraban SC (2020). Interneuron deficits in neurodevelopmental disorders: Implications for disease pathology and interneuron-based therapies. Eur. J. Paediatr. Neurol 24, 81–88. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peñagarikano O, Abrahams BS, Herman EI, Winden KD, Gdalyahu A, Dong H, Sonnenblick LI, Gruver R, Almajano J, Bragin A, et al. (2011). Absence of CNTNAP2 leads to epilepsy, neuronal migration abnormalities, and core autism-related deficits. Cell 147, 235–246. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peñagarikano O, Lázaro MT, Lu X-H, Gordon A, Dong H, Lam HA, Peles E, Maidment NT, Murphy NP, Yang XW, et al. (2015). Exogenous and evoked oxytocin restores social behavior in the Cntnap2 mouse model of autism. Sci. Transl. Med 7, 271ra8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Que L, Lukacsovich D, Luo W, and Földy C (2021). Transcriptional and morphological profiling of parvalbumin interneuron subpopulations in the mouse hippocampus. Nat. Commun 12, 108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rácz A, Ponomarenko AA, Fuchs EC, and Monyer H (2009). Augmented hippocampal ripple oscillations in mice with reduced fast excitation onto parvalbumin-positive cells. J. Neurosci 29, 2563–2568. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rein B, Ma K, and Yan Z (2020). A standardized social preference protocol for measuring social deficits in mouse models of autism. Nat. Protoc 15, 3464–3477. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ring M, Gaigg SB, and Bowler DM (2015). Object-location memory in adults with autism spectrum disorder. Autism Res 8, 609–619. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson S, Howlin P, and Russell A (2017). Personality traits, autobiographical memory and knowledge of self and others: A comparative study in young people with autism spectrum disorder. Autism 21, 357–367. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Royer S, Zemelman BV, Losonczy A, Kim J, Chance F, Magee JC, and Buzsáki G (2012). Control of timing, rate and bursts of hippocampal place cells by dendritic and somatic inhibition. Nat. Neurosci 15, 769–775. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rubenstein JLR, and Merzenich MM (2003). Model of autism: increased ratio of excitation/inhibition in key neural systems. Genes Brain Behav 2, 255–267. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schendel DE, and Thorsteinsson E (2018). Cumulative Incidence of Autism Into Adulthood for Birth Cohorts in Denmark, 1980–2012. JAMA 320, 1811–1813. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schomburg EW, Anastassiou CA, Buzsáki G, and Koch C (2012). The spiking component of oscillatory extracellular potentials in the rat hippocampus. J. Neurosci 32, 11798–11811. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schomburg EW, Fernández-Ruiz A, Mizuseki K, Berényi A, Anastassiou CA, Koch C, and Buzsáki G (2014). Theta phase segregation of input-specific gamma patterns in entorhinal-hippocampal networks. Neuron 84, 470–485. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scott R, Sánchez-Aguilera A, van Elst K, Lim L, Dehorter N, Bae SE, Bartolini G, Peles E, Kas MJH, Bruining H, and Marín O (2019). Loss of Cntnap2 Causes Axonal Excitability Deficits, Developmental Delay in Cortical Myelination, and Abnormal Stereotyped Motor Behavior. Cereb. Cortex 29, 586–597. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seibenhener ML, and Wooten MC (2015). Use of the Open Field Maze to measure locomotor and anxiety-like behavior in mice. J. Vis. Exp 96, e52434. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Selimbeyoglu A, Kim CK, Inoue M, Lee SY, Hong ASO, Kauvar I, Ramakrishnan C, Fenno LE, Davidson TJ, Wright M, and Deisseroth K (2017). Modulation of prefrontal cortex excitation/inhibition balance rescues social behavior in CNTNAP2-deficient mice. Sci. Transl. Med 9, eaah6733. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Siapas AG, and Wilson MA (1998). Coordinated interactions between hippocampal ripples and cortical spindles during slow-wave sleep. Neuron 21, 1123–1128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silverman JL, Yang M, Lord C, and Crawley JN (2010). Behavioural phenotyping assays for mouse models of autism. Nat. Rev. Neurosci 11, 490–502. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sivakumaran MH, Mackenzie AK, Callan IR, Ainge JA, and O’Connor AR (2018). The Discrimination Ratio derived from Novel Object Recognition tasks as a Measure of Recognition Memory Sensitivity, not Bias. Sci. Rep 8, 11579. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sohal VS, and Rubenstein JLR (2019). Excitation-inhibition balance as a framework for investigating mechanisms in neuropsychiatric disorders. Mol. Psychiatry 24, 1248–1257. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sohal VS, Zhang F, Yizhar O, and Deisseroth K (2009). Parvalbumin neurons and gamma rhythms enhance cortical circuit performance. Nature 459, 698–702. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stark E, Roux L, Eichler R, Senzai Y, Royer S, and Buzsáki G (2014). Pyramidal cell-interneuron interactions underlie hippocampal ripple oscillations. Neuron 83, 467–480. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strauss KA, Puffenberger EG, Huentelman MJ, Gottlieb S, Dobrin SE, Parod JM, Stephan DA, and Morton DH (2006). Recessive symptomatic focal epilepsy and mutant contactin-associated protein-like 2. N. Engl. J. Med 354, 1370–1377. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Suh J, Rivest AJ, Nakashiba T, Tominaga T, and Tonegawa S (2011). Entorhinal cortex layer III input to the hippocampus is crucial for temporal association memory. Science 334, 1415–1420. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor MJ, Rosenqvist MA, Larsson H, Gillberg C, D’Onofrio BM, Lichtenstein P, and Lundström S (2020). Etiology of Autism Spectrum Disorders and Autistic Traits Over Time. JAMA Psychiatry 77, 936–943. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tort ABL, Komorowski R, Eichenbaum H, and Kopell N (2010). Measuring phase-amplitude coupling between neuronal oscillations of different frequencies. J. Neurophysiol 104, 1195–1210. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turrigiano G (2011). Too many cooks? Intrinsic and synaptic homeostatic mechanisms in cortical circuit refinement. Annu. Rev. Neurosci 34, 89–103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vandecasteele M, M, S., Royer S, Belluscio M, Berényi A, Diba K, Fujisawa S, Grosmark A, Mao D, Mizuseki K, et al. (2012). Large-scale recording of neurons by movable silicon probes in behaving rodents. J. Vis. Exp 61, e3568. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Varga C, Golshani P, and Soltesz I (2012). Frequency-invariant temporal ordering of interneuronal discharges during hippocampal oscillations in awake mice. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 109, E2726–E2734. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vogt D, Cho KKA, Lee AT, Sohal VS, and Rubenstein JLR (2015). The parvalbumin/somatostatin ratio is increased in Pten mutant mice and by human PTEN ASD alleles. Cell Rep 11, 944–956. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vogt D, Cho KKA, Shelton SM, Paul A, Huang ZJ, Sohal VS, and Rubenstein JLR (2018). Mouse Cntnap2 and Human CNTNAP2 ASD Alleles Cell Autonomously Regulate PV+ Cortical Interneurons. Cereb. Cortex 28, 3868–3879. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walf AA, and Frye CA (2007). The use of the elevated plus maze as an assay of anxiety-related behavior in rodents. Nat. Protoc 2, 322–328. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wu YK, Hengen KB, Turrigiano GG, and Gjorgjieva J (2020). Homeostatic mechanisms regulate distinct aspects of cortical circuit dynamics. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 117, 24514–24525. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yamamoto J, and Tonegawa S (2017). Direct Medial Entorhinal Cortex Input to Hippocampal CA1 Is Crucial for Extended Quiet Awake Replay. Neuron 96, 217–227.e4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yamamoto J, Suh J, Takeuchi D, and Tonegawa S (2014). Successful execution of working memory linked to synchronized high-frequency gamma oscillations. Cell 157, 845–857. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang M, Silverman JL, and Crawley JN (2011). Automated three-chambered social approach task for mice. Curr. Protoc. Neurosci Chapter 8, 26. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ylinen A, Bragin A, Nádasdy Z, Jandó G, Szabó I, Sik A, and Buzsáki G (1995). Sharp wave-associated high-frequency oscillation (200 Hz) in the intact hippocampus: network and intracellular mechanisms. J. Neurosci 15, 30–46. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
All data reported in this paper will be shared by the lead contact upon request. This paper does not report original code.
Any additional information required to reanalyze the data reported in this paper is available from the lead contact upon request.