Skip to main content
PLOS One logoLink to PLOS One
. 2022 Jan 28;17(1):e0262916. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0262916

Dietary rescue of adult behavioral deficits in the Fmr1 knockout mouse

Suzanne O Nolan 1, Samantha L Hodges 2, Matthew S Binder 1, Gregory D Smith 2, James T Okoh 1, Taylor S Jefferson 1, Brianna Escobar 1, Joaquin N Lugo 1,2,*
Editor: Alexandra Kavushansky3
PMCID: PMC8797197  PMID: 35089938

Abstract

The current study aimed to further address important questions regarding the therapeutic efficacy of omega-3 fatty acids for various behavioral and neuroimmune aspects of the Fmr1 phenotype. To address these questions, our experimental design utilized two different omega-3 fatty acid administration timepoints, compared to both standard laboratory chow controls (“Standard”) and a diet controlling for the increase in fat content (“Control Fat”). In the first paradigm, post-weaning supplementation (after postnatal day 21) with the omega-3 fatty acid diet (“Omega-3”) reversed deficits in startle threshold, but not deficits in prepulse inhibition, and the effect on startle threshold was not specific to the Omega-3 diet. However, post-weaning supplementation with both experimental diets also impaired acquisition of a fear response, recall of the fear memory and contextual fear conditioning compared to the Standard diet. The post-weaning Omega-3 diet reduced hippocampal expression of IL-6 and this reduction of IL-6 was significantly associated with diminished performance in the fear conditioning task. In the perinatal experimental paradigm, the Omega-3 diet attenuated hyperactivity and acquisition of a fear response. Additionally, perinatal exposure to the Control Fat diet (similar to a “Western” diet) further diminished nonsocial anxiety in the Fmr1 knockout. This study provides significant evidence that dietary fatty acids throughout the lifespan can significantly impact the behavioral and neuroimmune phenotype of the Fmr1 knockout model.

Introduction

Fragile X syndrome (FXS) is a neurodevelopmental disorder resulting from a trinucleotide (CGG) repeat mutation in the fragile X mental retardation (FMR1) gene, which codes for the Fragile X mental retardation protein (FMRP). Mutations in FMR1 are one of the most prevalent genetic contributors to inherited intellectual disability [1] and Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) [2]. In clinical studies, individuals with FXS display a myriad of behavioral abnormalities, including significant cognitive dysfunction, autistic behaviors, and hyperactivity [3]. These phenotypes are often mirrored in the Fmr1 knockout mouse, which demonstrates significant learning impairments, hyperactivity, social impairments, and increased repetitive behavior, among other behavioral phenotypes [46]. Alongside these behavioral phenotypes, recent clinical investigations have also demonstrated significant evidence for the role of cytokine signaling in individuals with FXS [7, 8]. Previous studies have shown evidence of aberrant proinflammatory signaling as a key feature in both FXS and its comorbid condition, ASD, in both clinical populations and animal models [710].

Omega-3 fatty acids, which serve as potent anti-inflammatory dietary compounds, have been investigated for their potential to alleviate behavioral symptoms in clinical populations of both ASD [1115] and Rett syndrome [16]. Treatment with omega-3 fatty acids incorporates several potential mechanisms that may be relevant to the Fmr1 knockout. First, eicosapentaenoic acid (EPA) and docosahexaenoic acid (DHA), two of the primary omega-3 fatty acids, are significant components of the neuronal membrane, and their composition in this membrane regulates functioning of key transmembrane receptors and other proteins [17]. Previous work has demonstrated that even one membrane substitution of a single-bonded polyunsaturated fatty acid (PUFA) for a double-bonded PUFA exerts changes in membrane properties that in turn alter constitutive activity of integral membrane proteins, such as rhodopsin [18]. Mechanistically, increasing the bioavailability of long chain PUFAs may also enable important anti-inflammatory mechanisms [19]. These may be accomplished through a variety of methods, including interactions with G-protein coupled receptors or downstream production of small anti-inflammatory compounds [20]. There is also significant evidence for an influence of DHA on markers of synaptogenesis and expression of glutamate receptors [21]. Further, limited preclinical evidence finds that post-weaning treatment with omega-3 fatty acids resulted in improvements in novelty exploration, social interaction, and object recognition in association with reductions in neuroinflammatory signaling markers in the adult Fmr1 mouse model [10].

While the aforementioned evidence supports a role of omega-3s as a potential therapeutic for the Fmr1 knockout phenotype, it is unknown if administration of this intervention at an earlier timepoint would result in differential effects on the behavioral and neuroinflammatory phenotypes studied in the present model. Recent work has shown promising effects of perinatal administration of omega-3s in Fmr1 mutant rats, supporting its potential for rescuing behaviors in this model [22]. Mechanistically, accumulation of fatty acids is fundamental for the formation of neuronal membranes, particularly early in development [23]. Studies of fatty acid incorporation demonstrate that essential long chain PUFAs, such as DHA and EPA, are not absorbed and incorporated appropriately during development in children with ASD [24]. Moreover, increased bioavailability of fatty acids during early development can induce discernable phenotypic changes as early as the weaning point [25]. While no studies have examined the therapeutic potential of perinatal administration in the Fmr1 model, previous studies have supported the potential for prenatal omega-3 fatty acids in the reversal of autistic-like deficits in other ASD rodent models [2630]. Moreover, interventions during this timeframe provide a higher likelihood for maximal impact, given that administration of omega-3 fatty acids after diagnosis in clinical studies may be too late to improve outcomes [31].

The current study aimed to address questions regarding the therapeutic efficacy of omega-3 fatty acids for key aspects of the Fmr1 phenotype, such as whether any behavioral effects might be mediated through changes in inflammatory gene expression, whether timing matters, and how previous effects compare when referenced to a standard dietary control. It should be noted that the previous work utilized a dietary control to compare their behavioral findings against, which represents an increase in fat content from typical laboratory chows [10]. The behavioral measures were selected as they most appropriately reflect the FXS clinical phenotype [32], and were based on prior findings [5]. In the first experiment, we exposed male Fmr1 knockouts to dietary manipulations in the post-weaning period and examined various aspects of adult behavior and cytokine signaling markers. In the second experiment, we administered similar dietary manipulations during the perinatal period to investigate how perinatal dietary manipulations prior to weaning will influence these same behaviors and cytokine signaling markers during adulthood in male Fmr1 KO mice.

Materials and methods

Animals

All procedures were performed in accordance with Baylor University Institutional Care and Use Committee and the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals of the National Institutes of Health. The protocol was approved by the Baylor University Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (Protocol number: 744037–3). Male Fmr1+/+ and female Fmr1+/- FVB.129P2-Pde6b+Tyrc-ch Fmr1tm1Cgr/J (Jackson Labs Stock No: 004624) mice originally from Jackson Labs were housed at Baylor University and bred to produce male wildtype (WT) and Fmr1 knockout (KO) offspring. The colony was maintained on a 12-hour light/dark cycle (lights on at 7 am). On postnatal day (PD) 7, pups were separated from parents and a toe-clipping identification system was used to identify animals and the tissue was sent out for genotyping and preserved in 70% ethanol (Mouse Genotype, Escondido, CA, USA). Animals were housed in the Special Research Unit at the Baylor Science Building. All tested animals and their breeders had access to food and water ad libitum.

Only male mice were used for the present studies, as previous work from our lab did not find strong evidence of a phenotype in female mutants in many of the tasks included here [5]. However, other work from our lab has found evidence of robust sex-specific effects in ultrasonic vocalization behavior in this model [33, 34] and has explored the impact of these dietary manipulations on this behavior in other studies [35].

Experimental design

The studies were split into two main experimental paradigms (Fig 1). In the first experimental paradigm, male offspring were maintained on standard laboratory chow until weaning on PD21 (Harlan Teklab 2020X formulation). At PD21, animals were randomly assigned to one of three diet conditions: “Standard”, “Omega-3” and “Control Fat”. The latter two diets had identical lipid content (50g/kg). The Omega-3 condition received a diet enriched with fish oil and dyed with red food coloring to distinguish it from the control condition (Teklad Custom Diet TD.150384). To control for potential effects due to simply increasing fat content, the Control Fat diet contained olive and palm oils with no dyes (Teklad Custom Diet TD.1500385) (See Table 1 for Ingredients List). The dietary composition was based on levels previously used in this model [10]. For further information of the types of fatty acid chains included, see Table 2. Mixed-genotype littermates were housed in groups no larger than 5 animals, with each cage receiving the same diet assignment. No culling was performed in either feeding paradigm. Based on the methodologies of prior studies, animals were maintained on this diet and tested in the behavioral paradigm starting at PD90 [10]. Animals were maintained on the diet throughout the testing paradigm. We chose PD60 to start testing after the perinatal treatment because it was the earliest day that the mice would be considered a young adult. We chose PD90 for the postnatal treatment so the mice could have a longer exposure to the high omega-3 diet. The two time periods were chosen to maximize the effect of the diet. The behavior tests were ordered in a way that increased in invasiveness, to minimize the effect of training history on subsequent behavioral tests [36]. The final sample sizes were as follows: standard diet WT (n = 11), standard diet KO (n = 10), control fat WT (n = 17), control fat KO (n = 15), omega-3 WT (n = 19), omega-3 KO (n = 16). Due to equipment errors, data was not available for certainanimals for some behaviors, and final sample sizes for each behavioral test in this paradigm are delineated in Table 3.

Fig 1. Overview of the two feeding paradigms.

Fig 1

For the postnatal paradigm, the pups were weaned onto one of three dietary manipulations and maintained on this diet through the conclusion of the behavioral testing paradigm. For the perinatal paradigm, breeders were placed on the dietary manipulations for one week prior to pairing. Breeding pairs and litters were maintained on this diet until weaning on PD21, at which time male pups were weaned onto standard diet. All animals in this paradigm received standard diet from PD21 through the conclusion of testing.

Table 1. Diet ingredients for post-weaning paradigm.

Ingredients, g/kg Control for EPA and DHA Diet
(Control Fat)
Omega-3 EPA and DHA Diet
(Omega-3 Diet)
Casein 200.0 200.0
L-Cystine 3.0 3.0
Corn Starch 347.472 347.472
Maltodextrin 132.0 132.0
Sucrose 100.0 100.0
Canola Oil 13.0 10.0
Olive Oil 65.0 20.0
Palm Oil 42.0 10.0
Fish Oil - 80.0
Cellulose 50.0 50.0
Mineral Mix, AIN-93G 35.0 35.0
Vitamin Mix, AIN-93 10.0 10.0
Choline Bitartrate 2.5 2.5
TBHQ, antioxidant 0.028 0.028
Total 1000.0 1000.0

This table contains ingredients for Teklad Custom Diets TD.1500384 and TD.1500385.

Table 2. Fatty acid breakdown.

Fatty Acids (% of Total)
Control Fat Diet
Omega-3 Diet
SFA 27.9 26.8
MUFA 59.8 36.8
PUFA 12.2 36.4
14:00 0.4 5.7
16:00 23.6 16.5
16:01 0.7 7.9
18:00 3.2 2.8
18:01 59.0 26.8
18:02 10.7 5.2
18:03 1.5 2.2
18:04 0.0 2.3
20:5 (EPA) 0.0 10.7
22:6 (DHA) 0.0 7.2
n-3 1.5 22.6
n-6 10.7 6.7

Fatty acid breakdown for Teklad Custom Diets TD.1500384 and TD.1500385.

Table 3. Sample sizes for post-weaning paradigm.

Behavioral Test/Measure Standard Control Fat Omega-3 Total
WT KO WT KO WT KO
Elevated Plus Maze 9 10 14 16 17 16 82
Sensorimotor Gating Paradigms 11 9 16 16 19 16 87
Delay Fear Conditioning 11 10 15 15 19 16 86
PCR 6 5 6 6 6 6 35

Sample sizes for each behavioral test following exclusion for technical or scoring issues.

In the second experimental paradigm, breeders were placed on one of the three experimental diets one week prior to pairing (Fig 1). Both parents and offspring were maintained on the assigned diet throughout pregnancy, parturition and lactation. Many aspects of the experimental diets were the same from paradigm 1, however, there were some additional nutrients added for breeding suitability (for a list of ingredients, see Table 4). Animals were then weaned onto standard laboratory chow on PD21 and housed with mixed genotype littermates in groups of no larger than 5 mice. Male animals were then tested in the behavioral battery beginning at PD60. The final sample sizes were as follows: standard diet WT (n = 24), standard diet KO (n = 12), control fat WT (n = 16), control fat KO (n = 16), omega-3 WT (n = 14), omega-3 KO (n = 14). Due to equipment errors, some data was not available for all animals in some behaviors, and final sample sizes for each behavioral test for this paradigm are delineated in Table 5. Any additional data can be found in the S1 File.

Table 4. Diet ingredients for perinatal paradigm.

Ingredients, g/kg Control for EPA and DHA Diet
(Control Fat)
Omega-3 EPA and DHA Diet
(Omega-3 Diet)
Casein 200.0 200.0
L-Cystine 3.0 3.0
Corn Starch 341.60 341.60
Maltodextrin 132.0 132.0
Sucrose 100.0 100.0
Canola Oil 13.0 10.0
Olive Oil 65.0 20.0
Palm Oil 42.0 10.0
Fish Oil - 80.0
Cellulose 50.0 50.0
Mineral Mix AIN-93G 35.0 35.0
Vitamin Mix AIN-93 10.0 10.0
Choline Bitartrate 2.5 2.5
Calcium Phosphate, dibasic 3.1 3.1
Calcium Carbonate 1.0 1.0
Magnesium Oxide 0.154 0.154
Cupric Carbonate 0.0038 0.0038
Ferric Citrate 0.2352 0.2352
Sodium Selenite (0.0455% in sucrose) 1.25 1.25
Vitamin K1, phylloquinone 0.0003 0.0003
Vitamin B12 0.025 0.025
(0.1% in mannitol)
TBHQ, antioxidant 0.028 0.028
Total 1000.0 1000.0

Ingredients for Teklad Custom Diets TD.160486 and TD.160487.

Table 5. Sample sizes for perinatal paradigm.

Behavioral Test/Measure Standard Control Fat Omega-3 Total
WT KO WT KO WT KO
Elevated Plus Maze 22 10 17 13 14 14 90
Sensorimotor Gating Paradigms 23 9 15 16 14 13 90
Delay Fear Conditioning 24 12 16 16 14 14 96
PCR 6 6 6 6 6 6 36

Sample sizes for each behavioral test following exclusion for technical or scoring issues.

Elevated plus maze

The elevated plus maze task was performed to evaluate changes in baseline anxiety levels [37, 38]. The testing room was lit by LED dimmable lamps (30 lux in the open arm) and the background noise remained at 60 dB. The testing arena consisted of four arms (30 x 5 cm) and a center platform (5 x 5 cm) positioned approximately 40 cm above the floor. Two opposing arms were enclosed by acrylic walls. Animals were recorded for 10 minutes and their movement was assessed via Ethovision XT video tracking software (Noldus, Netherlands). This program scored the frequency and duration of visits to the various arms and center platform, as well as distance moved during the entire task and average velocity over the testing window. The percent of time in the open arms was calculated by taking the total duration of time spent across all visits to the arms as a function of the total testing time (10 minutes). The testing apparatus was cleaned thoroughly with 30% isopropyl alcohol solution before and after each subject was tested. Experimenters were not present during the testing window. Final sample sizes for each paradigm are delineated in Tables 3 and 5.

Sensorimotor gating assessment

To determine changes in sensorimotor gating abilities in the Fmr1 KO mice, the sensorimotor gating assessment paradigm was implemented as previously described [39]. Briefly, the apparatus consisted of an acrylic hollow constraint tube, with varying degrees of restraint availability. This apparatus was mounted on a platform equipped to transduce startle response amplitude through the SR-Lab System (San Diego Instruments, San Diego, CA, USA). This paradigm consisted of three separate testing days. During all testing sessions, background levels were maintained at 68 dB. On the first day, the animals underwent a habituation session. During this, they had a 5-minute acclimation session, followed by 80 startle stimuli delivered at a fixed interval (15 s). The startle stimulus was a 40 ms, 120 dB noise burst, with a rise/fall time of less than 1 ms.

The next day, prepulse inhibition was assessed. Following a 5-minute habituation phase, animals received 20 presentations of a 40 ms, 120 dB stimulus. They were then presented with the prepulse phase of the trial, consisting of 90 trials. The first three trial types consisted of the 20 ms prepulse stimulus at three different decibel levels (70, 75 and 80 dB). The second three trial types then consisted of these three prepulse stimuli paired with the original startle stimulus. The prepulse stimulus was then preceded by the startle stimulus by 100 ms. These trials were organized randomly and spaced by a 15 s inter-trial interval.

One week following the prepulse session, the startle threshold session was conducted. Following the initial 5-minute habituation period, mice were presented with 99 trials of 11 trial types: no stimulus, and ten startle stimuli ranging from 75–120 dB at five dB intervals. These startle stimuli lasted 40 ms with a rise/fall of less than 1 ms. The order of these trials was pseudorandomized, such that 11 trials were presented as a block in a different order each time. Experimenters were not present during the testing window. Final sample sizes for both paradigms are delineated in Tables 3 and 5.

Delay fear conditioning

The delay fear conditioning paradigm was conducted, as it has been shown to be selective for amygdala-based fear memories [40]. On the first day of testing, following a 2 minute baseline period, the animals received 2 pairings of a 30-second 80-dB white noise stimulus (designated the CS) followed by a 0.7 mA shock stimulus (designated the US). Following each pairing, there was a 120 second inter-trial interval (ITI). The session lasted approximately 334 seconds.

On the second day, there were two testing sessions. During the first, the animal was placed in the shock-associated context and allowed to move freely for 300 seconds to evaluate freezing in the original context (contextual freezing behavior). Following a two-hour rest, the animal was placed in a new context for 360 seconds. The context was altered by the following manipulations: a clear acrylic square placed over the shock grid (novel tactile context), the shape of the arena altered by the insertion of an acrylic panel, shredded paper towels in the transfer cage and 1 mL of pure vanilla extract placed beneath the floor (novel olfactory context). During the first 3 minutes, we measured the freezing behavior of the mouse in a new environment. In the second 3 minutes, the CS was presented continuously for 3 minutes. During all sessions, freezing behavior was measured by an automated software (Colbourn Instruments, Allentown, PA, USA). Experimenters were not present during the testing window. Final sample sizes for this test for each experimental paradigm are delineated in Tables 3 and 5.

Quantitative real-time polymerase chain reaction

Following completion of behavioral testing, the brain was removed and rinsed in 1X phosphate buffer (PBS) solution (for exact timing, please refer to Fig 1). Using previously described methods [41], hippocampi were then rapidly dissected from each hemisphere and quickly rinsed in ice cold 1X PBS, before being frozen on dry ice and stored in microcentrifuge tubes at -80 ºC until processing. The left hippocampus was used for all PCR assays. Total RNA was isolated from samples according to protocols from the RNeasy kit Qiagen, Hilden, Germany). Subsequently, concentration and purity of isolated samples were measured using a NanoDrop ND-1000 Spectrophotometer (Thermo Scientific, NanoDrop Products, Wilmington, DE). Using the High-Capacity cDNA Reverse Transcription Kit (Applied Biosystems, Carlsbad, CA), extracted RNA was then reverse transcribed into complementary DNA. Using a QuantStudio 5 Real-Time PCR System (Applied Biosystems, Carlsbad, CA), mRNA expression was then determined by quantitative real-time polymerase chain reaction (qRT-PCR) using Taq-man probe and primer chemistry. Reactions were performed in triplicate in a 384 well plate for each sample, using the endogenous control gene (β-actin) for normalization. The expression levels of each target gene, BDNF, IL-6, TNF-α and IL-1β, was calculated by normalizing the quantified mRNA amount to β–actin using the 2-ΔΔCt method of quantitation. Relative gene expression of each group was calculated in relation to the Standard Diet WT group and used to test significance between groups.

Statistical analyses

All data were analyzed using GraphPad Software 7.0 (San Diego, CA) or IBM SPSS Statistics 23 (Aramonk, NY). Results were evaluated using a 2 x 3 (Genotype [WT, KO] x Diet [Standard, Control Fat, Omega-3]) analysis of variance (ANOVA) on each dependent variable for the specific test. Any tests that involved repeated measures were analyzed using a within- subjects factor, by repeated measures ANOVA (specified in the appropriate results section). Significant within-subjects interactions were followed up with individual one-way ANOVAs at each repeated measure. Significant interactions of genotype and diet were followed up with the use of a unique identifier for all groups (i.e. “Standard WT”) and subsequent analysis. If there were multiple significant main effects, multiple comparisons were conducted using Fisher’s LSD comparisons (Standard WT vs Standard KO, Standard WT vs Control Fat KO, Standard WT vs Omega-3 KO). For PCR, following a significant main effect, multiple comparisons were again conducted using Fisher’s LSD comparisons. All results have been summarized for convenience, and all statistical results including correlations between cytokine expression levels and behavioral measures, which were conducted using Pearson’s correlation statistic, are summarized in the S1 File. For all inferential statistics, the level of significance remained at p < 0.05.

Results

Perinatal but not post-weaning supplementation with a high omega-3 diet attenuated hyperactivity in the elevated plus maze in the Fmr1 knockout mouse, with no effect on anxiety

Hyperactivity is a significant clinical component of FXS [42, 43], and has been well-described in the Fmr1 KO [5, 4446]. Given the potential impact of hyperactivity on the findings of subsequent behavioral testing and the consistent appearance in the Fmr1 KO model, we assessed the impact of high omega-3 fatty acids on activity levels in the elevated plus maze. For the post-weaning paradigm, in the elevated plus maze, loss of Fmr1 did not influence distance moved, F(1, 76) = 1.57, p = 0.21 (Fig 2A). Post-weaning diet also did not influence distance moved, F(2, 76) = 0.32, p = 0.74. Moreover, there was no significant interaction of genotype and this diet, F(2, 76) = 2.75, p = 0.07.

Fig 2. Perinatal supplementation with a high omega-3 diet attenuated hyperactivity in the elevated plus maze in the Fmr1 knockout mouse, with no effect on anxiety.

Fig 2

A. Exposure to omega-3 fatty acids had no effect in the post-weaning paradigm as measured by distance moved in the elevated plus maze. B. A similar pattern was detected for velocity in the elevated plus maze. C. No effect of the experimental diets was seen in the post-weaning paradigm. D. Perinatal exposure to omega-3 fatty acids reversed Fmr1 knockout induced hyperactivity in the elevated plus maze, as measured by distance moved. E. Perinatal exposure to omega-3s also reversed hyperactivity as measured by velocity in the elevated plus maze. F. Loss of Fmr1 was associated with diminished anxiety in the elevated plus maze, though this effect was not ameliorated by exposure to omega-3 fatty acids. Rather, exposure to the control fat perinatal diet exacerbated this genotype effect. Data are expressed as mean ± SEM.* = p < 0.05, ** = p < 0.01, *** = p< 0.001 ANOVA. A designation of “b” indicates that this group differed from the “a” comparison group at the level of p < 0.05 in post-hoc analyses.

A similar pattern was detected for velocity. Alone, loss of Fmr1 did not significantly increase velocity, F (1, 76) = 0.88, p = 0.35 (Fig 2B) and post-weaning diet did not significantly influence velocity on its own, F(2, 76) = 0.99, p = 0.38. Moreover, there was no interaction between genotype and diet, F(2, 76) = 2.96, p = 0.06.

Previous studies conducted in this model have indicated that non social anxiety is significantly reduced following loss of Fmr1 [4]. To examine the influence of these dietary paradigms on anxiety, we assessed animals in the elevated plus maze, and recorded the percentage of time spent in open arms of the maze. For the post-weaning paradigm, loss of Fmr1 did not impact anxiety in this task, F(1, 76) = 0.003, p = 0.96, and diet was also not a significant factor, F(2, 76) = 2.03, p = 0.14 (Fig 2C). These variables also did not significantly interact, F(2, 76) = 1.54, p = 0.22. The results suggest that the reduced anxiety characteristic of the Fmr1 KO is not present when tested around PD90 in this model, unlike previous studies conducted at an earlier time points [5]. Furthermore, neither post-weaning dietary fatty acid manipulation affected the anxiety phenotype of either WT or KO animals.

For the perinatal paradigm, in the elevated plus maze, loss of Fmr1 resulted in hyperactivity when measuring distance moved, F(1, 84) = 21.04, p = 0.001 (Fig 2D). However, the perinatal diet did significantly influence distance moved, F(2, 84) = 4.26, p = 0.02. Post-hoc multiple comparisons with LSD indicated that animals, across genotypes, receiving omega-3 fatty acids had reduced distance moved and velocity when compared to both standard and control fat conditions, p < 0.05 (Fig 2D). A significant interaction was detected for distance moved, F(2, 84) = 3.35, p = 0.04, and a follow-up analysis with unique group identifiers (i.e. Standard Diet WT) was conducted according to our a priori hypotheses. Interestingly, the perinatal control fat diet significantly exacerbated hyperactivity in the KO (Control Fat KO vs Standard WT, p = 0.02). However, the perinatal control fat diet reduced hyperactivity in the wildtypes (Control Fat WT vs Standard WT, p = 0.004). This was similarly true for the omega-3 WT diet (Omega-3 WT vs Standard WT, p = 0.01). Further, the omega-3 KO group expressed levels of activity similar to the standard WT group, p = 0.62 (Fig 2D), suggesting this rescued the phenotype seen in the Standard KO.

The results for velocity were similar. Overall, loss of Fmr1 increased velocity, F(1, 84) = 14.41, p = 0.001 (Fig 2E). While no interaction was detected, F(2, 84) = 1.21, p = 0.30, exposure to the different perinatal dietary manipulations did indeed affect velocity, F(2, 84) = 7.52, p = 0.001. Post-hoc LSD comparisons indicated that the Omega-3 Diet (“b”) condition significantly lowered velocity, relative to both the Standard Diet (“a”) (p = 0.29), as well as the Control Fat (“a”) diet (p = 0.001), suggesting that this aspect of hyperactivity was also rescued.

For the perinatal paradigm, loss of Fmr1 resulted in decreased anxiety, F(1, 84) = 7.91, p = 0.01, as suggested by increased percentage of time spent in the open arms (Fig 2F). Increased time spent in the open arm in the Fmr1 KO was significantly exaggerated by exposure to the control fat perinatal diet, F(2, 84) = 8.01, p = 0.001. Post-hoc analyses indicated that the control fat perinatal diet (“b”) significantly increased the proportion of time spent in the open arms compared to both standard (“a”) and omega-3 perinatal diets (“a”), at the level of p < 0.05. However, the standard perinatal diet and omega-3 conditions produced similar effects (Fig 2F). Genotype and perinatal diet also did not significantly interact, F(2, 84) = 2.10, p = 0.13. These results suggest that the omega-3 fatty acid perinatal diet did not improve altered anxiety characteristic of the Fmr1 model. Moreover, the control fat perinatal diet, similar to a typical Western diet, further exacerbated this reduced anxiety phenotype.

Overall, similar to the post-weaning paradigm, exposure to omega-3 fatty acids, but not other types of fatty acids, attenuates hyperactivity in the adult Fmr1 KO measured via the elevated plus maze.

Post-weaning exposure to high fat diets reversed startle threshold, while neither paradigm reversed PPI deficits

To further examine the potential therapeutic efficacy of omega-3 fatty acids, we examined this behavior in a three-day paradigm similar to previously published methodology [39]. For the post-weaning paradigm, results for habituation to the startle stimulus (data found in S1 File) indicated no impact of loss of Fmr1 on the ability to habituate to the chamber, F (1, 81) = 0.001, p = 0.97, and this did not vary across the testing window, F(7, 567) = 0.76, p = 0.62. Post-weaning diet also did not impact habituation, F(2, 81) = 0.72, p = 0.49, and this effect did not change over time, F(14, 567) = 0.89, p = 0.57. The interaction of the different levels of genotype and post-weaning diet were not significant, both overall, Ft(2, 81) = 1.91, p = 0.16, or across time, F(14, 567) = 0.52, p = 0.92. Overall, this indicated that neither post-weaning diet nor genotype had a significant impact on habituation to the startle stimulus.

As expected, loss of Fmr1 resulted in overall exaggerated levels of prepulse inhibition (measured on Day 2), F(1, 81) = 6.15, p = 0.02 (Fig 3A). This effect was consistent across decibel levels, F(2, 162) = 2.57, p = 0.08. Because this effect was consistent across the testing window, data are shown in the figure summarized across decibel level. Post-weaning exposure to dietary manipulation did not significantly impact percent inhibition alone, F(2, 81) = 0.05, p = 0.95, or across the levels of decibel of prepulse stimulus, F (4, 162) = 0.15, p = 0.97. Similarly, the combination of genotype and post-weaning diet was not statistically significant depending on the levels of the stimulus, Fl(4, 162) = 0.15, p = 0.96. However, overall, the combination of these two factors, genotype and diet, did in fact differentially impact percent inhibition, regardless of the level of the stimulus, F(2, 81) = 3.70, p = 0.03. Post-hoc analyses using LSD testing indicated that in standard diet condition, loss of Fmr1 exaggerated PPI [(a) Standard WT vs (b) Standard KO, p = 0.002]. However, exposure to both the omega-3 [(a) Standard WT vs (b) Omega-3 KO, p = 0.04] and the control fat diet [(a) Standard WT vs (b) Control Fat KO, p = 0.05] did not differentially impact the Fmr1 KO PPI phenotype (Fig 3A). Altogether, these results support previous findings that loss of Fmr1 results in exaggerated prepulse inhibition, though this was unaffected by the two post-weaning dietary manipulations.

Fig 3. Post-weaning exposure to high fat diets reversed startle threshold, while neither paradigm impacted PPI deficits.

Fig 3

A. Exposure to both experimental diets did not reverse exaggerated PPI in the Fmr1 knockout in the post-weaning paradigm. B. Startle threshold was diminished in the Fmr1 knockout, and this was reversed by exposure to both of the post-weaning experimental diets. C. Perinatal exposure to omega-3 fatty acids, as well as the control fat diet, failed to reverse PPI deficits. D. Perinatal exposure to the experimental diets did not impact the startle threshold deficits seen in the Fmr1 knockout. Data are expressed as mean ± SEM. * = P < 0.05 ANOVA, ** = P < 0.01 ANOVA. A designation of “b” indicates that this group differed from the “a” comparison group at the level of p < 0.05 in post-hoc analyses.

Finally, startle threshold was assessed one week later. As expected, loss of Fmr1 did not impact the startle response overall, F (1, 81) = 1.72, p = 0.19 (Fig 3B). However, there was a significant interaction of genotype and decibel levels, F (10, 810) = 2.88, p = 0.002. Further investigation indicated that Fmr1 KOs had reduced startle responding during this testing window at higher stimulus levels (115 and 120 dB). Post-weaning diet exposure did not impact startle responding overall, F(2, 81) = 0.42, p = 0.66, and the slope of the startle threshold curve was not shifted, F (20, 810) = 0.98, p = 0.38. The overall combination of genotype and post-weaning diet also failed to reach significance, F (2, 81) = 0.65, p = 0.53. However, the interaction of these levels with post-weaning diet and genotype did significantly impact startle responding, F(20, 810) = 1.87, p = 0.01. Post-hoc analyses indicated that at 115 dB and 120 dB, Standard KO demonstrated significantly reduced startle responding compared to Standard WT, p = 0.05. This was ameliorated by post-weaning exposure to both experimental post-weaning diets at both 115 dB (Control Fat KO vs Standard WT, p = 0.97; Omega-3 KO vs Standard WT, p = 0.43) and 120 dB (Control Fat KO vs Standard WT, p = 0.94; Omega-3 KO vs Standard WT, p = 0.37) (Fig 3B). Together, these results suggest that increasing the fat content of the post-weaning diet can potentially improve reduced startle responding at higher decibels in the Fmr1 KO.

For the perinatal paradigm, loss of Fmr1 reduced responding to the startle stimulus overall, F (1, 84) = 5.73, p = 0.02, during the habituation session (data found in S6 Fig in S1 File) and this effect become more significant over the testing window, F (7, 588) = 2.01, p = 0.05. Moreover, perinatal diet did significantly impact overall responding to the startle stimulus, F(2, 84) = 3.49, p = 0.04. Further post-hoc LSD testing indicated that the omega-3 fatty acid perinatal diet increased startle responding, compared to the standard perinatal diet, at the level of p < 0.05, regardless of genotype. This effect was consistent across time, F(14, 588) = 1.59, p = 0.08. The control fat perinatal diet was not significantly different from either the standard or omega-3 perinatal diet. The unique combination of genotype and perinatal diet did not significantly impact startle responding during the habituation task, both overall, F (2, 84) = 0.35, p = 0.70, or across time, F (14, 588) = 0.54, p = 0.91.

When looking at the prepulse inhibition phase, loss of Fmr1 significantly increased percent inhibition overall, F(1, 84) = 7.74, p = 0.01 (Fig 3C). This sensitivity was also greater with increasing decibel levels, F (2, 168) = 5.32, p = 0.01. Results also indicated that perinatal diet significantly altered percent inhibition, regardless of genotype, F(2, 84) = 5.55, p = 0.01. Post-hoc analyses indicated that the control fat perinatal diet significant increased PPI compared to the standard perinatal diet and omega-3 perinatal diet, while the omega-3 perinatal diet was not significantly different from the standard perinatal diet, at the level of p < 0.05 (Fig 3C). This effect was not dependent on decibel levels, F (4, 168) = 1.93, p = 0.11. The combination of genotype and perinatal diet did not impact this behavior, both overall, F(2, 84) = 2.89, p = 0.06, and across decibel levels, F(4, 168) = 1.89, p = 0.11.

For the startle threshold test, loss of Fmr1 reduced startle responding at higher decibel levels, F(10, 840) = 2.14, p = 0.02 (Fig 3D). This was not indicative of overall lowered startle responding in this task, F (1, 84) = 0.02, p = 0.88. Perinatal diet also affected the startle threshold curve, both overall, F(2, 84) = 4.01, p = 0.02, and across the different levels, F(20, 840) = 2.89, p = 0.001. Overall, the combination of perinatal diet and genotype did not impact startle responding, F(2, 84) = 0.28, p = 0.76. However, it did alter the threshold of startle responding, F(20, 8840) = 2.78, p = 0.0001. Subsequent analyses indicated the interaction between genotype and perinatal diet was only significant at the level of 110 and 115 decibels. Further testing indicated in WT animals, the omega-3 perinatal diet increased startle responding, [Omega-3 WT vs Standard WT, p < 0.05; Omega-3 WT vs Standard KO, p < 0.05; Omega-3 KO vs Standard WT, p < 0.05] (Fig 3D). Moreover, perinatal control fat diet shifted both the WT and KO animals to an intermediate position that was not statistically different between either group. Together these findings suggest that neither experimental perinatal diet improved reduced startle responding following loss of Fmr1.

Post-weaning supplementation with high fat diets impairs training, contextual and cued recall, while perinatal supplementation improved training in the delay fear conditioning task

In previous studies of the Fmr1 KO, the delay fear conditioning paradigm demonstrates impaired acquisition of a fear response and impairments in cued recall [5]. Thus, we chose this paradigm to assess the ability of our treatment to improve this cognitive dysfunction. For the post-weaning paradigm, results for acquisition indicated no effect of loss of Fmr1 on overall freezing during the acquisition of a fear memory, F(1, 80) = 1.06, p = 0.31 (Fig 4A). This effect was consistent across time, F(4, 320) = 1.05, p = 0.38. Assignment to either one of the two experimental post-weaning diets did, however, reduce freezing behavior during acquisition, F(2, 80) = 11.38, p = 0.0001. Moreover, the magnitude of this effect was different across the testing window, F(8, 320) = 5.99, p = 0.0001. In fact, beginning at tone 1, both omega-3 and control fat post-weaning diets, across genotypes, displayed reduced levels of freezing, compared to standard post-weaning diet controls, at the level of p < 0.05, and the magnitude of the difference grew over time. However, this effect was also dependent on genotype, as the interaction term was significant, both overall, F(2, 80) = 3.18, p = 0.05, and across time, F(8, 320) = 2.15, p = 0.03. Subsequent analyses indicated that this interaction reached significance only at the first inter-trial interval, the second presentation of the tone, and the second inter-trial interval at the level of p < 0.05. Analysis using a unique grouping variable indicated that at ITI 1, tone 2 and ITI 2, all other groups displayed diminished freezing compared to the Standard WT group, at the level of p < 0.05 (Fig 4A). Altogether, these results suggest that post-weaning exposure to both experimental post-weaning diets reduced short-term learning of the association between the tone (CS) and shock (US) in this paradigm.

Fig 4. Post-weaning supplementation with high fat diets impaired training, contextual and cued recall, while perinatal supplementation improved training in the delay fear conditioning task.

Fig 4

A. Acquisition of a fear memory was impaired by exposure to both experimental post-weaning diets in wildtype animals, and this effect was not additive for the Fmr1 knockout. B. Contextual memory was impaired in both wildtype and knockout animals in the experimental post-weaning diet conditions. C. Cued recall was impaired by both post-weaning diets in wildtype and knockout animals, and this effect was not additive in the Fmr1 knockout. D. Perinatal omega-3 fatty acids improve training, while the control fat perinatal diet exhibits no effect. E. No effect of either perinatal diet or genotype was detected for contextual memory. F. No effect of either perinatal diet or genotype was detected for cued recall. Data are expressed as mean ± SEM. * = p < 0.05 ANOVA, ** = p < 0.01 ANOVA. A designation of “b” indicates that this group differed from the “a” comparison group at the level of p < 0.05 in post-hoc analyses.

The next day, conditioning to the context was measured via freezing across the 5 minute window. As expected, loss of Fmr1 did not impact freezing behavior overall, F(1, 80) = 1.22, p = 0.27 (Fig 4B). This effect was consistent across the testing window, F(4, 320) = 1.61, p = 0.17. Post-weaning diet did significantly impact freezing behavior to the conditioned context, F(2, 80) = 11.07, p = 0.0001. Subsequent analyses with LSD post-hoc multiple comparisons indicated that animals exposed to both omega-3 (“b”) and control fat post-weaning diets (“b”) displayed significantly reduced freezing behavior in the conditioned context compared to the standard post-weaning diet (“a”), p < 0.05 (Fig 4B). This effect was consistent across the testing window, F(8, 320) = 0.19, p = 0.83. The combination of genotype and post-weaning diet did not significantly impact freezing behavior, both overall, F(2, 80) = 1.63, p = 0.20, and across time, F(8, 320) = 1.37, p = 0.21. These results suggest that post-weaning exposure to the high-fat experimental diets reduces contextual fear learning.

Following a two-hour rest, animals were placed in a novel context and the level of conditioning to the stimulus was measured via freezing to the tone expression. Loss of Fmr1 did not alter freezing behavior across the testing window, F(1, 80) = 1.0, p = 0.76 (Fig 4C). This effect was consistent across time, F(1, 80) = 1.46, p = 0.23. However, post-weaning diet significantly impacted freezing overall during this window, F(2, 80) = 9.88, p = 0.0001. Further multiple-comparisons with LSD indicated that exposure to both omega-3 (“b”) and control fat post-weaning diets (“b”) significantly reduce freezing behavior overall compared to the standard post-weaning diet condition (“a”) (Fig 4C). This effect was consistent across the testing window, F(2, 80) = 1.46, p = 0.24. Because the effect on freezing was the same across the testing window, only freezing in response to the cue is shown in the figure. The combination of genotype and post-weaning diet did not significantly impact freezing behavior, both overall, F(2, 80) = 0.39, p = 0.68, and across time, F(2, 80) = 2.04, p = 0.14. These results suggest that post-weaning exposure to both experimental diets reduced cued recall of the fear memory. It should be noted that based on previous findings, we expected to see a reduction in freezing to the tone based on previous findings from our group [5], and this lack of effect in the Fmr1 knockout may be driven by the later (PD90+) testing time point.

For the perinatal paradigm, the results showed a different pattern. During the acquisition phase, loss of Fmr1 resulted in no change overall to freezing levels across the testing window, F(1, 90) = 3.40, p = 0.07 (Fig 4D). However, the interaction of genotype and time did significantly impact freezing behavior, F(4, 360) = 6.27, p = 0.001 (Fig 4D). As expected, post-hoc multiple comparisons indicated that loss of Fmr1 resulted in diminished freezing starting at the second iteration of the tone, at the level of p < 0.05. Perinatal diet significantly impacted freezing overall during this acquisition phase, F(2, 90) = 5.08, p = 0.01, as well as across time, F(8, 360) = 3.34, p = 0.001. Further multiple comparisons testing indicated that both omega-3 fatty acid (“b”) and control fat (“b”) perinatal diets significantly increased freezing behavior across the testing window, at the level of p < 0.05, and the magnitude of this effect increased over time (Fig 4D). There was also a trending interaction of genotype and perinatal diet, F(2, 90) = 2.77, p = 0.07, though this failed to reach significance. No significant three-way interaction was detected, F(8, 360) = 1.65, p = 0.11. Overall, these results suggested that perinatal exposure to both experimental diets improved acquisition of a fear response in the Fmr1 KO.

Results for contextual fear conditioning, averaged across the 5 minute testing window, indicated no overall effect of genotype, F(1, 90) = 0.47, p = 0.50 (Fig 4E). No overall effect of perinatal diet was detected, F(2, 90) = 0.87, p = 0.42. Moreover, the different perinatal diet manipulations did not interact with genotype, F(2, 90) = 0.77, p = 0.47.

Results for examining memory for the conditioned tone indicated firstly that loss of Fmr1 resulted in decreased freezing behavior across the test window, F(1, 90) = 10.30, p = 0.002 (Fig 4F). This reduced freezing was significantly greater in response to the tone, F(1, 90) = 11.63, p = 0.001 (Fig 4F). This decrement in freezing behavior was not, however, impacted by perinatal dietary manipulations, both overall, F(2, 90) = 0.66, p = 0.52, or across time, F(2, 90) = 0.62, p = 0.54. There was no significant between-subjects interaction of genotype and perinatal diet, F(2, 90) = 0.60, p = 0.52. No significant three-way interaction was detected, F(2, 90) = 0.13, p = 0.55. Overall, this suggests that the improvement in acquisition following perinatal exposure to these diets did not translate to cued recall the following day.

Perinatal and post-weaning exposure to high fat diets differentially impacts hippocampal proinflammatory cytokine and BDNF expression

Among the potential mechanisms potentially at play here, omega-3 fatty acids have been shown to normalize expression of cytokine signaling [10, 19]. Following the conclusion of behavioral testing, expression of various cytokines in whole hippocampal samples was assayed. For the post-weaning paradigm, no effect of genotype or post-weaning diet was detected for: BDNF [F(1, 29) = 0.04, p = 0.84; F(2, 29) = 0.39, p = 0.68; F(2, 29) = 1.07, p = 0.36] (Fig 5A) or IL-1β [F(1, 29) = 0.95, p = 0.34; F(2, 29) = 1.54, p = 0.23; F(2, 29) = 0.14, p = 0.87] (Fig 5B). However, similar to previously published studies in our lab, IL-6 [F(1, 29) = 4.57, p = 0.04] (Fig 5C) and TNF-α [F(1, 29) = 7.91, p = 0.01] (Fig 5D) were significantly reduced following loss of Fmr1 [47]. TNF-α was not significantly impacted by post-weaning exposure to any of the experimental post-weaning diets, either overall, F(2, 29) = 0.40, p = 0.67, or according to genotype, F(2, 29) = 0.67, p = 0.52. However, post-weaning dietary exposure to high levels of omega-3 fatty acids further reduced IL-6 expression, F(2, 29) = 4.29, p = 0.02. Post-hoc LSD multiple comparisons supported this conclusion, showing that exposure to the omega-3 post-weaning diet significantly reduced IL-6 expression relative to the standard post-weaning diet condition, p < 0.01. Moreover, this effect was consistent across genotypes, F(2, 29) = 0.46, p = 0.63.

Fig 5. Perinatal and post-weaning exposure to high fat diets differentially impacted hippocampal proinflammatory cytokine and BDNF expression.

Fig 5

A. Hippocampal expression of BDNF was not impacted by loss of Fmr1 or exposure to experimental post-weaning diets. B. Hippocampal IL-1β expression was not impacted by loss of Fmr1 or exposure to experimental post-weaning diets. C. Expression of IL-6 in the hippocampus was diminished in the Fmr1 knockout, and exposure to omega-3 fatty acids also reduced its expression. The control fat post-weaning diet had no effect on IL-6 expression. D. TNF-α expression was also reduced in the Fmr1 knockout with no effect of post-weaning diet. E. BDNF expression was reduced by both experimental perinatal diets. F. Similar to BDNF, IL-1β was reduced in the hippocampus of animals exposed to both experimental perinatal diets. G. Unlike the postnatal paradigm, no effect was detected for IL-6. H. No effects were detected for TNF-α. Data are expressed as mean ± SEM. * = p < 0.05 ANOVA, ** = p < 0.01 ANOVA. A designation of “b” indicates that this group differed from the “a” comparison group at the level of p < 0.05 in post-hoc analyses.

For the perinatal paradigm, a total of 36 samples (n = 6 per group) were used to assess hippocampal expression of proinflammatory cytokines and BDNF. No effect of genotype was detected for BDNF, F(1, 30) = 0.007, p = 0.93 (Fig 5E). However, exposure to both high fat perinatal diets reduced hippocampal expression of BDNF, F(2, 30) = 7.38, p = 0.003. Post-hoc LSD multiple comparisons indicated that both high fat perinatal diets reduced hippocampal BDNF expression [(a) Standard Diet vs (b) Omega-3, p = 0.004; (a) Standard Diet vs (b) Control Fat, p = 0.001] (Fig 5E). This effect was consistent across the levels of genotype, F(2, 30) = 0.33, p = 0.72. A similar pattern was detected for IL-1β. No effect of genotype was detected, F(1, 30) = 0.42, p = 0.52 (Fig 5F). However, exposure to the two perinatal dietary manipulations reduced expression of IL-1β, F(2, 30) = 8.03, p = 0.002. Post-hoc LSD multiple comparison supported this [(a) Standard Diet vs (b) Omega-3 Diet, p = 0.001; (a) Standard Diet vs (b) Control Fat Diet, p = 0.002] (Fig 5F). This effect was consistent across genotypes as well, F(2, 29) = 0.16, p = 0.85. Unlike the post-weaning paradigm, no effects were seen for either IL-6 [F(1, 30) = 0.04, p = 0.84; F(2, 30) = 2.62, p = 0.09; F(2, 29) = 0.02, p = 0.98] (Fig 5G) or TNF-α [F(1, 30) = 0.38, p = 0.54; F(2, 30) = 2.16, p = 0.13; F(2, 29) = 0.11, p = 0.89] (Fig 5H). Overall, these results suggest that perinatal exposure to both dietary manipulations reduced hippocampal expression of BDNF and IL-1β regardless of expression of Fmr1.

Reductions in proinflammatory cytokine and BDNF signaling are associated with fear conditioning performance

Given the nature of the experimental design used, it is difficult to determine if these changes in cytokine expression are related to the behavioral changes. Thus, we next aimed to determine if these changes were statistically associated with the behavioral phenotypes shown in the current study. For each correlation, animals from all diets were correlated across the measures of interest. These results can be found in S3 Table.

Given the decrements in acquisition seen following post-weaning administration of omega-3 fatty acids, we were most interested in whether the expression of IL-6 was associated with freezing behavior during the fear conditioning task. As expected, reductions in IL-6 expression were significantly associated with reductions in freezing behavior to the second presentation of the tone during the acquisition phase, r(35) = 0.40, p = 0.02 (Fig 6A). Reductions in IL-6 expression were also significantly associated with freezing behavior during the contextual phase of the paradigm, r(35) = 0.43, p = 0.01 (Fig 6B). However, this same pattern did not hold up for cued learning the second day, r(35) = 0.12, p = 0.49 (Fig 6C). No other cytokines measured were significantly correlated with these measures, suggesting this is specific to IL-6. These results are in line with the understanding that cued learning is considered amygdala-dependent, while contextual conditioning is hippocampal-dependent, and as such we would not expect to see a relationship with hippocampal expression [40].

Fig 6. Reductions in proinflammatory cytokine and BDNF signaling were associated with fear conditioning performance.

Fig 6

A. In the post-weaning supplementation group, reductions in hippocampal IL-6 expression were significantly associated with the reduced freezing behavior seen during the acquisition of delay fear conditioning, specifically during the second presentation of the tone. B. Similarly, reduced freezing behavior during the contextual phase was also significantly associated with IL-6 expression in the animals receiving the post-weaning intervention. C. The association between IL-6 and freezing during the cued recall phase in the post-weaning dietary intervention groups was not significant. D. Concurrent with improvements seen in acquisition of a fear memory, BDNF is significantly negatively associated with freezing during the second presentation of the tone in the cohort of animals receiving perinatal dietary manipulations. E. Similar results were found for IL-1β expression in animals receiving perinatal dietary manipulations. Data points represent individual subject scores.

Given the studies showing that cytokine signaling predicts hyperactivity, we also wanted to determine if these reductions in IL-6 or other individual differences in expression levels were significantly associated with our measures of activity levels in the elevated plus maze [48]. The only significant association was between hippocampal expression of TNFα and velocity during the task, r(35) = -.40, p < 0.05.

Similar to the post-weaning paradigm, correlations were conducted to ascertain if changes in cytokine expressions were related to any changes in behavior in animals receiving the perinatal dietary manipulations (summarized in S3 Table). Given the improvements in fear learning acquisition seen in the perinatal paradigm concurrent with reductions in BDNF and IL-1β expression, we also assessed if the expression of BDNF and IL-1β was similarly associated with freezing behavior. Indeed, results of the Pearson correlation indicated that there was a significant negative association between hippocampal BDNF expression and freezing to the second tone during the acquisition phase, r(35) = -.34, p = 0.05 (Fig 6D). Similar results were demonstrated for IL-1β expression in the hippocampus, r(35) = -.35, p = 0.04 (Fig 6E). However, these cytokine levels were not significantly related to any other time point during the fear conditioning protocol.

Similar to the post-weaning paradigm, we found it pertinent to examine whether these reductions in cytokine signaling in the hippocampus were related to changes in activity levels. However, results suggested that no cytokines measured were significantly related to either velocity or distance moved in the elevated plus maze.

Discussion

Hyperactivity is a significant clinical component of the FXS phenotype [4952]. Previous clinical studies demonstrated that treatment with omega-3 fatty acids during a similar window reduced hyperactivity in individuals with ASD [11]. The hyperactivity phenotype is a consistent facet of the Fmr1 phenotype [45, 46, 53] and previous animal studies have indicated that treatment with anti-inflammatory agents also reduced hyperactivity [54]. Based on these findings, we hypothesized that dietary supplementation would ameliorate hyperactivity. Indeed, the results of the current study demonstrated that perinatal exposure to omega-3 fatty acids attenuated hyperactivity shown in the standard diet KO animals, though this was specific to perinatal supplementation (summarized in Fig 7). This effect was specific to the type of fatty acid, as the control fat diet had no impact on activity. This effect was concurrent with reductions in hippocampal BDNF expression as assessed by RT-PCR. While BDNF is typically considered a beneficial neurotrophic factor that promotes neuronal survival, in the Fmr1-null brain, BDNF-TrkB signaling is upregulated and loss of BDNF improves hyperactivity and altered sensorimotor gating in the Fmr1 model [55, 56]. Altogether, the results of the present study find that perinatal supplementation with omega-3 fatty acids is sufficient to attenuate hyperactivity in the Fmr1 knockout, which is accompanied by changes in BDNF expression in the hippocampus.

Fig 7. Summary of the main findings.

Fig 7

The results of the present study provide preliminary support for omega-3 fatty acids to ameliorate particular aspects of the Fmr1 KO phenotype, including hyperactivity and sensorimotor gating deficits. ↑ = increase in behavioral output; ↓ = decrease in behavioral output. For example, ↑ under KO activity level indicates hyperactivity.

Alterations in sensorimotor gating behaviors like startle responding are a significant clinical component for children with FXS, and these changes are also seen in the Fmr1 knockout [39, 42, 43]. Previous studies supported the possibility that this treatment would be effective, as shown in the ketamine model of schizophrenia, changes in prepulse inhibition phenotypes are amenable to treatment with omega-3 fatty acids [57]. Results from both the post-weaning and perinatal paradigm indicated that the exaggerated PPI shown in the KO mouse was not affected by exposure to either experimental diet, while reduced startle threshold was indeed ameliorated by post-weaning exposure to both omega-3 and control fat diets (summarized in Fig 7). Previous work has demonstrated that startle responding and inhibition of the startle response with a prepulse stimulus rely on separate circuitry and mechanisms, suggesting they may be differentially responsive to such treatments [58, 59]. More work is needed to determine the mechanism in how omega-3 fatty acids reversed deficits in startle responding.

The current study finds that the timing of the intervention also mattered for the impact on fear learning and memory (summarized in Fig 7). Clinical data demonstrates mixed findings regarding the efficacy of omega-3 fatty acids on aspects of cognitive functioning [6063]. Yet, given that in a previous study omega-3 supplementation attenuated deficits in hippocampal-dependent novel object recognition in the knockout, we expected that other aspects of nonspatial hippocampal-dependent memory might be similarly improved by this treatment [10]. Moreover, recent work also has found evidence that exposure to omega-3s can improve aspects of cognitive function following loss of Fmr1 [22]. Conversely, the present studies found that post-weaning exposure to the two experimental diets diminished freezing behavior during acquisition, thus inhibiting learning of the acquired response. The differential acquisition of fear conditioning presents difficulty in interpretation of the contextual conditioning and cued recall results, as when tested 24 hours later, post-weaning exposure to the experimental diets also reduced contextual fear conditioning and cued recall, regardless of genotype. The discrepancy between our findings and the impact on memory processes shown in the previous study may be due to the inclusion of the standard diet control group. In the present study, the control fat diet mirrors a typical Western diet, which has been demonstrated to have its own effects on behavior [64, 65]. Thus, the inclusion of the standard diet control renders direct comparisons between our study and the previous study difficult. However, the perinatal paradigm resulted in improved acquisition of a fear response for both diets. Previous results have indeed similarly shown that manipulation of fat content can induce large changes in developing an appropriate fear response [6568]. Overall, the present study adds to the knowledge that manipulation of fat content across the lifespan can significantly impact fear learning and memory.

A fundamental tenet of these experiments was that loss of Fmr1 would result in behavioral changes that mirror those previously seen. As hypothesized, we noted hyperactivity in the elevated plus maze, enhanced pre-pulse inhibition, a reduced startle threshold curve, and diminished fear learning. Moreover, no effect of loss of Fmr1 was noted on either social behavior or stereotypy (data found in S1 File). Given what our lab [5] and others [69] have shown previously, these results in the social partition task are expected. While the present mouse model is widely considered a model for studying ASD-like characteristics, social deficits similar to clinical reports are more commonly reported in rat models with loss of Fmr1 [22]. Moreover, the null results could be related to the testing paradigm used, given that significant social deficits previously reported were described in the three-chamber social task, while here we used the social partition task [46, 70]. The social phenotype in the present model depends on the test used [70]. Regarding the null findings for stereotypy behavior, previous work from our lab has indeed found increased rearing and stereotypy behaviors in the open field test [5], yet we did not find that result here. However, the most consistent test for detecting increased repetitive behaviors in this model has been self-grooming during social tasks and perseverance during the reversal learning portion of the Morris water maze task [71, 72]. Future studies should examine the potential impact of omega-3 fatty acids on these phenotypes to further evaluate its efficacy.

There were some other unexpected behavioral findings which may be explained by the shifts in behavioral time points across the two paradigms. For example, our results for the elevated plus maze indicated that loss of Fmr1 resulted in decreased anxiety only in the perinatal paradigm (around the age of PD60). However, when tested after PD90 (in the post-weaning paradigm), no effect of genotype was detected. The finding from the perinatal paradigm is congruent with our previous findings, demonstrating decreased anxiety around the same time point [5]. Similarly, no impairments in cued recall were detected in the post-weaning paradigm for the delay fear conditioning task, however we did see this effect in the perinatal paradigm. The timing again lines up with the previous study conducted in our lab, and suggests that these impairments may not hold the same pattern after 3 months of age [5]. While the reason for this is unclear, this lack of effect at a later timepoint could reflect a shift to a different expression of the fear response. Many factors, including sex and individual differences, have been shown to result in divergent expression of fear responses [73, 74]. Overall, much of the work in the Fmr1 knockout adult phenotype has focused on the early (~PD60) adult phenotype, and the results of the present study clearly indicate a need to include more time points to fully elucidate the role of Fmr1 in fear learning.

A second fundamental hypothesis of this study was that changes in behavior resulting from these dietary manipulations could be related to changes in inflammatory cytokine signaling, and the results of this study broadly supported this. First, our results confirmed that reduction of hippocampal IL-1β is associated with improvements in acquisition of a fear response. Next, our results expanded on previous studies from our lab, showing that post-weaning treatment with omega-3 fatty acids reduced IL-6 expression levels [9]. Prior to conducting the study, we had anticipated that a normalization of hippocampal IL-6 expression would coincide with improvements in hippocampal-dependent behaviors; however, the current study indicated that in conjunction with reduced IL-6 expression, post-weaning exposure to omega-3s was detrimental to hippocampal-dependent fear conditioning performance, but not our amygdala-dependent task. IL-6 signaling is important for many functions across the brain, including promotion of neuronal survival, protection against damage and modulation of neurotransmitter synthesis [75]. In knockout studies, IL-6 has been demonstrated to be necessary for adequate performance in the novel object recognition task, as well as water maze performance, further suggesting its importance in hippocampal function [76]. Taken as a whole, these results support that constitutive expression of IL-6, rather than overall reduction, is important for proper hippocampal functioning in the Fmr1 model. This is in line with new evidence demonstrating that IL-6 may only take on a negative role in the presence of other proinflammatory cytokines [77].

The current study also improves our understanding of how consumption of the “Western diet” influences behavior. The control fat dietary manipulation included in the present study mirrors the typical “Western diet” [78]. Previously, it had been surmised that the high-fat diet was problematic for aspects of neurological functioning, resulting in significant deficits in learning and memory [79, 80]. Indeed, here we demonstrated that post-weaning exposure to this diet exaggerated the reduced anxiety characteristic of the Fmr1 knockout. However, the results of the current study also complicate this thesis, finding that a post-weaning diet high in monounsaturated and saturated fats reverses deficits in startle responding. Instead of solely focusing on the negative aspects a higher fat diet, this study seems to contribute to a larger body of evidence that implicates impairments in fatty acid metabolism for the etiology of neurodevelopmental disorders [81]. Going forward, the importance of a nuanced understanding of the role of fatty acid metabolism in neural functioning is an important consideration.

Altogether, the current study provides strong evidence of omega-3 fatty acids shaping behavior in the Fmr1 knockout mouse, further preclinical and clinical research should be conducted to determine the translational relevance. For example, the efficacy in females, the exact mechanism and the appropriate dosages all need to be determined in mouse models before establishing the potential efficacy in humans. Previously, preclinical research has produced several appealing treatment options, yet clinical trials to date have been inconclusive regarding the long-term efficacy, safety, and tolerability in human populations. Similarly, much more work is needed to determine the potential of this dietary intervention as an alternative to pharmaceuticals for individuals with FXS. However, beyond coping with the burden of the disease itself, the importance of finding affordable treatment options cannot be overstated. Nearly half of all families of children with FXS report a significant financial burden of the disease on their family, as out of pocket expenses can account for over 5% of family income [82]. Moreover, medication or therapy can account for over 50% of these out-of-pocket expenses [82]. This financial burden can disproportionately affect low-income families, as low-income families with children with special health care needs are 11 times more likely than higher income families to have out-of-pocket medical expenditures exceeding 5% of income [83]. Given the rising cost of pharmaceuticals [84], dietary interventions present as a more cost-effective alternative, further improving the lives of individuals with Fragile X syndrome.

Supporting information

S1 File. Additional methods and results.

(DOCX)

S1 Table. ANOVA results for post-weaning paradigm.

(DOCX)

S2 Table. ANOVA results for prenatal paradigm.

(DOCX)

S3 Table. Cytokine associations with behavior for both paradigms.

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

We would like to acknowledge the use of equipment in the Molecular Biosciences Core.

Data Availability

The data are available on Figshare: https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.18321518.v1.

Funding Statement

This study received support in the form of funding from the National Institutes of Health (NS088776, awarded to JL) The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Turner G, Webb T, Wake S, Robinson H. Prevalence of fragile X syndrome. American Journal of Medical Genetics. 1996;64(1):196–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Kaufmann WE, Cortell R, Kau ASM, Bukelis I, Tierney E, Gray RM, et al. Autism spectrum disorder in fragile X syndrome: communication, social interaction, and specific behaviors. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part A. 2004;129A(3):225–34. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.a.30229 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.H RJ, S WE. Psychopathology in fragile X syndrome. The American journal of orthopsychiatry. 1989;59(1):142–52. doi: 10.1111/j.1939-0025.1989.tb01641.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Liu Z-H, Smith CB. Dissociation of social and nonsocial anxiety in a mouse model of fragile X syndrome. Neuroscience Letters. 2009;454(1):62–6. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2009.02.066 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Nolan SO, Reynolds CD, Smith GD, Holley AJ, Escobar B, Chandler MA, et al. Deletion of Fmr1 results in sex-specific changes in behavior. Brain and behavior. 2017;7(10):e00800. doi: 10.1002/brb3.800 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Veeraragavan S, Bui N, Perkins JR, Yuva-Paylor LA, Carpenter RL, Paylor R. Modulation of behavioral phenotypes by a muscarinic M1 antagonist in a mouse model of fragile X syndrome. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2011;217(1):143–51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Ashwood P, Krakowiak P, Hertz-Picciotto I, Hansen R, Pessah I, Van de Water J. Elevated plasma cytokines in autism spectrum disorders provide evidence of immune dysfunction and are associated with impaired behavioral outcome. Brain, Behavior and Immunity. 2011;25(1):40–5. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2010.08.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Ashwood P, Nguyen DV, Hessl D, Hagerman RJ, Tassone F. Plasma cytokine profiles in Fragile X subjects: Is there a role for cytokines in the pathogenesis? Brain, Behavior, and Immunity. 2010;24(6):898–902. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2010.01.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Hodges SL, Nolan SO, Taube JH, Lugo JN. Adult Fmr1 knockout mice present with deficiencies in hippocampal interleukin-6 and tumor necrosis factor-alpha expression. Neuroreport. 2017. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0000000000000905 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Pietropaolo S, Goubran MG, Joffre C, Aubert A, Lemaire-Mayo V, Crusio WE, et al. Dietary supplementation of omega-3 fatty acids rescues fragile X phenotypes in Fmr1-Ko mice. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2014;49:119–29. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.07.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Amminger GP, Berger GE, Schafer MR, Klier C, Friedrich MH, Feucht M. Omega-3 fatty acids supplementation in children with autism: a double-blind randomized, placebo-controlled pilot study. Biological psychiatry. 2007;61(4):551–3. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.05.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Yui K, Imataka G, Kawasaki Y, Yamada H. Down-regulation of a signaling mediator in association with lowered plasma arachidonic acid levels in individuals with autism spectrum disorders. Neuroscience Letters. 2016;610:223–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2015.11.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Parellada M, Llorente C, Calvo R, Gutierrez S, Lázaro L, Graell M, et al. Randomized trial of omega-3 for autism spectrum disorders: Effect on cell membrane composition and behavior. European Neuropsychopharmacology. 2017;27(12):1319–30. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2017.08.426 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Hartman RE, Patel D. Dietary Approaches to the Management of Autism Spectrum Disorders. Adv Neurobiol. 2020;24:547–71. doi: 10.1007/978-3-030-30402-7_19 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Ooi YP, Weng S-J, Kossowsky J, Gerger H, Sung M. Oxytocin and autism spectrum disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Pharmacopsychiatry. 2017;50(01):5–13. doi: 10.1055/s-0042-109400 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.De Felice C, Signorini C, Durand T, Ciccoli L, Leoncini S, D’Esposito M, et al. Partial rescue of Rett syndrome by ω-3 polyunsaturated fatty acids (PUFAs) oil. Genes & Nutrition. 2012;7(3):447–58. doi: 10.1007/s12263-012-0285-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Frisardi V, Panza F, Seripa D, Farooqui T, Farooqui AA. Glycerophospholipids and glycerophospholipid-derived lipid mediators: a complex meshwork in Alzheimer’s disease pathology. Progress in lipid research. 2011;50(4):313–30. doi: 10.1016/j.plipres.2011.06.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Eldho NV, Feller SE, Tristram-Nagle S, Polozov IV, Gawrisch K. Polyunsaturated docosahexaenoic vs docosapentaenoic acid: Differences in lipid matrix properties from the loss of one double bond. Journal of the American Chemical Society. 2003;125(21):6409–21. doi: 10.1021/ja029029o [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Kang JX, Weylandt KH. Modulation of inflammatory cytokines by omega-3 fatty acids. Sub-cellular biochemistry. 2008;49:133–43. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4020-8831-5_5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Oh DY, Talukdar S, Bae EJ, Imamura T, Morinaga H, Fan W, et al. GPR120 is an omega-3 fatty acid receptor mediating potent anti-inflammatory and insulin-sensitizing effects. Cell. 2010;142(5):687–98. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2010.07.041 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Cao D, Kevala K, Kim J, Moon H-S, Jun SB, Lovinger D, et al. Docosahexaenoic acid promotes hippocampal neuronal development and synaptic function. Journal of Neurochemistry. 2009;111(2):510–21. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2009.06335.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Schiavi S, Carbone E, Melancia F, Buzzelli V, Manduca A, Campolongo P, et al. Perinatal supplementation with omega-3 fatty acids corrects the aberrant social and cognitive traits observed in a genetic model of autism based on FMR1 deletion in rats. Nutr Neurosci. 2020:1–14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Yehuda S. Polyunsaturated fatty acids as putative cognitive enhancers. Medical Hypotheses. 2012;79(4):456–61. doi: 10.1016/j.mehy.2012.06.021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Vancassel S, Durand G, Barthelemy C, Lejeune B, Martineau J, Guilloteau D, et al. Plasma fatty acid levels in autistic children. Prostaglandins, leukotrienes, and essential fatty acids. 2001;65(1):1–7. doi: 10.1054/plef.2001.0281 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Bongiovanni KD, Depeters EJ, Van Eenennaam AL. Neonatal growth rate and development of mice raised on milk transgenically enriched with omega-3 fatty acids. Pediatric Research. 2007;62(4):412. doi: 10.1203/PDR.0b013e31813cbeea [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Fortunato JJ, da Rosa N, Martins Laurentino AO, Goulart M, Michalak C, Borges LP, et al. Effects of omega-3 fatty acids on stereotypical behavior and social interactions in Wistar rats prenatally exposed to lipopolysaccarides. Nutrition (Burbank, Los Angeles County, Calif). 2017;35:119–27. doi: 10.1016/j.nut.2016.10.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Garay PA, Hsiao EY, Patterson PH, McAllister AK. Maternal immune activation causes age- and region-specific changes in brain cytokines in offspring throughout development. Brain, Behavior, And Immunity. 2013;31:54–68. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2012.07.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Li Q, Leung YO, Zhou I, Ho LC, Kong W, Basil P, et al. Dietary supplementation with n-3 fatty acids from weaning limits brain biochemistry and behavioural changes elicited by prenatal exposure to maternal inflammation in the mouse model. Translational psychiatry. 2015;5:e641. doi: 10.1038/tp.2015.126 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Malkova NV, Yu CZ, Hsiao EY, Moore MJ, Patterson PH. Maternal immune activation yields offspring displaying mouse versions of the three core symptoms of autism. Brain, Behavior, And Immunity. 2012;26(4):607–16. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2012.01.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Matsui F, Hecht P, Yoshimoto K, Watanabe Y, Morimoto M, Fritsche K, et al. DHA Mitigates Autistic Behaviors Accompanied by Dopaminergic Change in a Gene/Prenatal Stress Mouse Model. Neuroscience. 2018;371:407–19. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.12.029 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Mankad D, Dupuis A, Smile S, Roberts W, Brian J, Lui T, et al. A randomized, placebo controlled trial of omega-3 fatty acids in the treatment of young children with autism. Molecular autism. 2015;6(1):1–11. doi: 10.1186/s13229-015-0010-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.H RJ, H PJ. Fragile X syndrome: diagnosis, treatment, and research. 3rd ed. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press; 2002. xii, 540 p. p. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Reynolds CD, Nolan SO, Jefferson T, Lugo JN. Sex-specific and genotype-specific differences in vocalization development in FMR1 knockout mice. Neuroreport. 2016;27(18):1331–5. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0000000000000701 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Nolan SO, Hodges SL, Lugo JN. High-throughput analysis of vocalizations reveals sex-specific changes in Fmr1 mutant pups. Genes, Brain and Behavior. 2019. doi: 10.1111/gbb.12611 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Nolan SO, Hodges SL, Okoh JT, Binder MS, Lugo JN. Prenatal High-Fat Diet Rescues Communication Deficits in Fmr1 Mutant Mice in a Sex-Specific Manner. Developmental Neuroscience. 2020;42(2):94–104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.McIlwain KL, Merriweather MY, Yuva-Paylor LA, Paylor R. The use of behavioral test batteries: Effects of training history. Physiology & Behavior. 2001;73(5):705–17. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(01)00528-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Pellow S, Chopin P, File SE, Briley M. Validation of open: closed arm entries in an elevated plus-maze as a measure of anxiety in the rat. Journal of Neuroscience Methods. 1985;14(3):149–67. doi: 10.1016/0165-0270(85)90031-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Walf AA, Frye CA. The use of the elevated plus maze as an assay of anxiety-related behavior in rodents. Nature Protocols. 2007;2(2):322–8. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2007.44 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Frankland PW, Wang Y, Rosner B, Shimizu T, Balleine BW, Dykens EM, et al. Sensorimotor gating abnormalities in young males with fragile X syndrome and Fmr1-knockout mice. Mol Psychiatry. 2004;9(4):417–25. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001432 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Raybuck JD, Lattal KM. Double dissociation of amygdala and hippocampal contributions to trace and delay fear conditioning. PLoS One. 2011;6(1):e15982. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0015982 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Lugo JN, Smith GD, Arbuckle EP, White J, Holley AJ, Floruta CM, et al. Deletion of PTEN produces autism-like behavioral deficits and alterations in synaptic proteins. Frontiers in Molecular Neuroscience. 2014;7:27. doi: 10.3389/fnmol.2014.00027 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Hagerman RJ. Fragile X syndrome. Molecular and clinical insights and treatment issues. West J Med. 1997;166(2):129–37. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Hagerman RJ, Jackson AW 3rd, Levitas A, Rimland B, Braden M. An analysis of autism in fifty males with the fragile X syndrome. Am J Med Genet. 1986;23(1–2):359–74. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.1320230128 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Baker, Piven J, Sato Y. Autism and tuberous sclerosis complex: prevalence and clinical features. Journal of autism and developmental disorders. 1998;28(4):279–85. doi: 10.1023/a:1026004501631 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Ding Q, Sethna F, Wang H. Behavioral analysis of male and female Fmr1 knockout mice on C57BL/6 background. Behavioural brain research. 2014;271:72–8. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.05.046 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Pietropaolo S, Guilleminot A, Martin B, D’Amato FR, Crusio WE. Genetic-background modulation of core and variable autistic-like symptoms in Fmr1 knock-out mice. PLoS One. 2011;6(2):e17073. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0017073 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Hodges SL, Nolan SO, Reynolds CD, Lugo JN. Spectral and temporal properties of calls reveal deficits in ultrasonic vocalizations of adult Fmr1 knockout mice. Behavioural brain research. 2017;332:50–8. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2017.05.052 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Han YM, Cheung WK, Wong CK, Sze SL, Cheng TW, Yeung MK, et al. Distinct cytokine and chemokine profiles in autism spectrum disorders. Frontiers in immunology. 2017;8:11. doi: 10.3389/fimmu.2017.00011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Baumgardner TL, Reiss AL, Freund LS, Abrams MT. Specification of the neurobehavioral phenotype in males with fragile X syndrome. Pediatrics. 1995;95(5):744–52. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Fryns JP, Jacobs J, Kleczkowska A, van den Berghe H. The psychological profile of the fragile X syndrome. Clinical genetics. 1984;25(2):131–4. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-0004.1984.tb00474.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.RJ H. Fragile X syndrome. Current Problems in Pediatrics. 1987;17(11):626–74. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Hatton DD, Hooper SR, Bailey DB, Skinner ML, Sullivan KM, Wheeler A. Problem behavior in boys with fragile X syndrome. American Journal Of Medical Genetics. 2002;108(2):105–16. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.10216 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Baker KB, Wray SP, Ritter R, Mason S, Lanthorn TH, Savelieva KV. Male and female Fmr1 knockout mice on C57 albino background exhibit spatial learning and memory impairments. Genes Brain Behav. 2010;9(6):562–74. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2010.00585.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Dansie LE, Phommahaxay K, Okusanya AG, Uwadia J, Huang M, Rotschafer SE, et al. Long-lasting effects of minocycline on behavior in young but not adult Fragile X mice. Neuroscience. 2013;246(Supplement C):186–98. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Louhivuori V, Vicario A, Uutela M, Rantamaki T, Louhivuori LM, Castren E, et al. BDNF and TrkB in neuronal differentiation of Fmr1-knockout mouse. Neurobiology of disease. 2011;41(2):469–80. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2010.10.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Uutela M, Lindholm J, Louhivuori V, Wei H, Louhivuori LM, Pertovaara A, et al. Reduction of BDNF expression in Fmr1 knockout mice worsens cognitive deficits but improves hyperactivity and sensorimotor deficits. Genes, Brain and Behavior. 2012;11(5):513–23. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2012.00784.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Zugno AI, Chipindo HL, Volpato AM, Budni J, Steckert AV, de Oliveira MB, et al. Omega-3 prevents behavior response and brain oxidative damage in the ketamine model of schizophrenia. Neuroscience. 2014;259:223–31. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.11.049 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Azzopardi E, Louttit AG, DeOliveira C, Laviolette SR, Schmid S. The role of cholinergic midbrain neurons in startle and prepulse inhibition. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2018;38(41):8798–808. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0984-18.2018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Ison JR, Taylor MK, Bowen GP, Schwarzkopf SB. Facilitation and inhibition of the acoustic startle reflex in the rat after a momentary increase in background noise level. Behavioral neuroscience. 1997;111(6):1335–52. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.111.6.1335 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Jackson PA, Deary ME, Reay JL, Scholey AB, Kennedy DO. No effect of 12 weeks’ supplementation with 1 g DHA-rich or EPA-rich fish oil on cognitive function or mood in healthy young adults aged 18–35 years. British Journal of Nutrition. 2011;107(8):1232–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.van de Rest O, Geleijnse JM, Kok FJ, van Staveren WA, Dullemeijer C, Olderikkert MG, et al. Effect of fish oil on cognitive performance in older subjects: a randomized, controlled trial. Neurology. 2008;71(6):430–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Lee LK, Shahar S, Chin AV, Yusoff NA. Docosahexaenoic acid-concentrated fish oil supplementation in subjects with mild cognitive impairment (MCI): a 12-month randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2013;225(3):605–12. doi: 10.1007/s00213-012-2848-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Mazereeuw G, Lanctôt KL, Chau SA, Swardfager W, Herrmann N. Effects of omega-3 fatty acids on cognitive performance: a meta-analysis. Neurobiology of Aging. 2012;33(7):1482.e17–.e29. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.12.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Williams L, Seki Y, Vuguin PM, Charron MJ. Animal models of in utero exposure to a high fat diet: A review. Biochimica et Biophysica Acta 2014;1842(3):507–19. doi: 10.1016/j.bbadis.2013.07.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Peleg-Raibstein D, Luca E, Wolfrum C. Maternal high-fat diet in mice programs emotional behavior in adulthood. Behavioural brain research. 2012;233(2):398–404. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2012.05.027 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Owada Y, Abdelwahab SA, Kitanaka N, Sakagami H, Takano H, Sugitani Y, et al. Altered emotional behavioral responses in mice lacking brain-type fatty acid-binding protein gene. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;24(1):175–87. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.04855.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Sasaki A, de Vega W, Sivanathan S, St-Cyr S, McGowan PO. Maternal high-fat diet alters anxiety behavior and glucocorticoid signaling in adolescent offspring. Neuroscience. 2014;272:92–101. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.04.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Sullivan EL, Riper KM, Lockard R, Valleau JC. Maternal high-fat diet programming of the neuroendocrine system and behavior. Hormones and Behavior. 2015;76:153–61. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2015.04.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Frankland PW, Wang Y, Rosner B, Shimizu T, Balleine BW, Dykens EM, et al. Sensorimotor gating abnormalities in young males with fragile X syndrome and Fmr1-knockout mice. Molecular Psychiatry. 2004;9(4):417–25. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001432 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Spencer CM, Alekseyenko O, Serysheva E, Yuva-Paylor LA, Paylor R. Altered anxiety-related and social behaviors in the Fmr1 knockout mouse model of fragile X syndrome. Genes Brain Behav. 2005;4(7):420–30. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2005.00123.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Nolan SO, Lugo JN. Reversal learning paradigm reveals deficits in cognitive flexibility in the Fmr1 knockout male mouse. F1000Res. 2018;7:711. doi: 10.12688/f1000research.14969.1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Bernardet M, Crusio WE. Fmr1 KO mice as a possible model of autistic features. Thescientificworldjo. 2006;6:1164–76. doi: 10.1100/tsw.2006.220 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Gruene TM, Flick K, Stefano A, Shea SD, Shansky RM. Sexually divergent expression of active and passive conditioned fear responses in rats. eLife. 2015;4. doi: 10.7554/eLife.11352 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Bush DEA, Sotres-Bayon F, LeDoux JE. Individual differences in fear: Isolating fear reactivity and fear recovery phenotypes. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 2007;20(4):413–22. doi: 10.1002/jts.20261 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Gadient RA, Otten UH. Interleukin-6 (IL-6)—a molecule with both beneficial and destructive potentials. Progress in neurobiology. 1997;52(5):379–90. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0082(97)00021-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Baier PC, May U, Scheller J, Rose-John S, Schiffelholz T. Impaired hippocampus-dependent and -independent learning in IL-6 deficient mice. Behavioral Brain Research,. 2009;200(1):192–6. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.01.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Raison CL, Knight JM, Pariante C. Interleukin (IL)-6: A good kid hanging out with bad friends (and why sauna is good for health). Brain, Behavior, And Immunity. 2018;73:1–2. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2018.06.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Monsanto SP, Hintze KJ, Ward RE, Larson DP, Lefevre M, Benninghoff AD. The new total Western diet for rodents does not induce an overweight phenotype or alter parameters of metabolic syndrome in mice. Nutrition Research. 2016;36(9):1031–44. doi: 10.1016/j.nutres.2016.06.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Cordner ZA, Tamashiro KLK. Effects of high-fat diet exposure on learning & memory. Physiology & Behavior. 2015;152:363–71. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Labouesse MA, Stadlbauer U, Langhans W, Meyer U. Chronic high fat diet consumption impairs sensorimotor gating in mice. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2013;38(11):2562–74. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.06.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Richardson AJ, Ross MA. Fatty acid metabolism in neurodevelopmental disorder: a new perspective on associations between attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, dyslexia, dyspraxia and the autistic spectrum. Prostaglandins, Leukotrienes and Essential Fatty Acids (PLEFA). 2000;63(1):1–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Ouyang L, Grosse S, Raspa M, Bailey D. Employment impact and financial burden for families of children with fragile X syndrome: findings from the National Fragile X Survey. Journal Of Intellectual Disability Research. 2010;54(10):918–28. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2010.01320.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Newacheck PW, Kim SE. A national profile of health care utilization and expenditures for children with special health care needs. Archives of pediatrics & adolescent medicine. 2005;159(1):10–7. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.159.1.10 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Lakdawalla DN. Economics of the pharmaceutical industry. Journal of Economic Literature. 2018;56(2):397–449. [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Alexandra Kavushansky

23 Jul 2021

PONE-D-21-20764

Dietary rescue of adult behavioral deficits in the Fmr1 knockout mouse

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Lugo,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Sep 05 2021 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Alexandra Kavushansky, PhD

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at 

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and 

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. We note that the grant information you provided in the ‘Funding Information’ and ‘Financial Disclosure’ sections do not match. 

When you resubmit, please ensure that you provide the correct grant numbers for the awards you received for your study in the ‘Funding Information’ section.

3. Thank you for stating the following financial disclosure: 

"The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript."

At this time, please address the following queries:

a) Please clarify the sources of funding (financial or material support) for your study. List the grants or organizations that supported your study, including funding received from your institution. 

b) State what role the funders took in the study. If the funders had no role in your study, please state: “The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.”

c) If any authors received a salary from any of your funders, please state which authors and which funders.

d) If you did not receive any funding for this study, please state: “The authors received no specific funding for this work.”

Please include your amended statements within your cover letter; we will change the online submission form on your behalf.

4. We note that you have included the phrase “data not shown” in your manuscript. Unfortunately, this does not meet our data sharing requirements. PLOS does not permit references to inaccessible data. We require that authors provide all relevant data within the paper, Supporting Information files, or in an acceptable, public repository. Please add a citation to support this phrase or upload the data that corresponds with these findings to a stable repository (such as Figshare or Dryad) and provide and URLs, DOIs, or accession numbers that may be used to access these data. Or, if the data are not a core part of the research being presented in your study, we ask that you remove the phrase that refers to these data.

5. Please upload a copy of Figure 8, to which you refer in your text on page xx. If the figure is no longer to be included as part of the submission please remove all reference to it within the text.

6. Please include a copy of Table 8 which you refer to in your text on page 34.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: Re: PONE-D-21-20764

Nolan et al describe the effects of dietary rescue of omega-3 fatty acids at two different time points in the Fmr1 KO mouse. It is of interest, but there are some issues with the communication of the findings.

In the Introduction, first paragraph, ‘signaling in the present population’- what present population? Do the authors mean in a clinical population? In the next paragraph, the omega-3 effects in ASD could use some additional citations, including more recent ones, to reflect the mixed nature of the findings, and also could point out that intervention once the diagnosis is established might be too late for maximal impact, further justifying the prenatal administration explored in the present study. In the prenatal use of omega-3 in ASD models (refs 21-24), might also mention Matsui et al (Neuroscience 2018; 371:407-419), as it also examined prenatal vs postnatal-only omega-3. In the final paragraph, might expand the context and explain for the reader ‘and how previous effects compare when referenced to a standard dietary control’, as readers might not already be aware of the nature of the control diets in the previous work.

Methods- For Tables 1, 2, and 4, can the Standard Diet contents also be shown? The second study mentions that all tested rodents were males. The first study does not specify. Were only males used for each study? Might discuss for the reader why that is done. Also, should explain for the reader why it was decided to test study 1 mice at 90 days but study 2 mice at 60 days, particularly as this appeared to have quite an impact on the results. In the second paragraph of Delay Fear Conditioning, ‘placed in the familiar context’ on the second line- isn’t this more specific- ‘shock-associated contest’? Not just familiar?

Results- Would be helpful to find a way to indicate within Figure 2C more of the various individual significant findings within the Figure. There are several places in the Results where the text is quite unclear regarding which diet timeframe is being discussed. For the startle and PPI results, second paragraph, ‘Diet did not significantly impact percent inhibition alone’- which diet timeframe? Also please clarify ‘However, overall, the combination of these to factors differentially impacted percent inhibition’- which two factors (among genotype, diet, and level of stimulus)? Similarly, Figure 3B legend does not specify the diet timeframe. Figure 3C legend does not mention the significant finding illustrated by a and b in the Figure itself. Later, grammar ‘these interaction of these levels’. Paragraph 5 of this section it is again not specified the timing of the diet discussed for these PPI results. In the discussion of the elevated plus maze, this is the first time that the lack of effect of the Fmr1 KO on behavior is mentioned in the context of the PD90 timing- this should be mentioned with the earlier negative findings for effects of Fmr1 KO as well. It is mentioned in this section that the control fat diet resembles the typical Western diet- this probably deserves more mention in the Discussion or Introduction for its potential salience. Figure 5 legend for 5C- ‘Cued recall was impaired in wildtype and knockout animals’- do the authors mean ‘impaired by diet’ here? Figure 6 legend, again, need to be clear about the timing of the diets in the text of the legend. I could not find Table 8. Be clear what animals are being used for the cytokine and BDNF signaling for association with fear conditioning?- all diets? Also, for ‘may instead reflect baseline differences in this model’- please explain- since there is the co-factor of all of of the dietary interventions, I’m not clear how this conclusion was made. Figure 7 again needs clarity on diet timing in the text of the legend. Next to last paragraph of Results, ‘Similar to the post-weaning paradigm’- seems the end of the sentence should make some reference to this being the preweaning paradigm.

Reviewer #2: Comments to the authors

The present article aimed to elucidate the therapeutic efficacy of omega-3 fatty acids for various behavioral and neuro-immune aspects of the Fmr1 phenotype in the mouse. Overall, I find the study interesting as omega-3 supplementation and its relation with inflammation have been described, but further research is still needed to understand the precise mechanisms and therapeutic potential of the supplementation. Moreover, the timing of the treatment is essential to understand the potential of omega-3 supplementation in neurodevelopmental psychopathologies such as autism and fragile X syndrome. I appreciate the large amount of data presented (also in supplementary information) and I find the whole manuscript interesting. However, some conceptual errors and misleading information are present; moreover, in the manuscript, the authors refer to figure 8 that I cannot find in the figure lists. For this reason, I recommend major revision.

Introduction

- The introduction presents the topic clearly, and the aim of the study is supported by the appropriate background. However, when referred to the work of Pietropaolo and co-workers the authors state that "it is unknown if administration of this intervention at an earlier time-point would result in differential effects on the behavioral and neuroinflammatory phenotypes studied". On this purpose, the authors should take into account the work of Schiavi et al. 2020 (PMID: 32912100) which assess the effect of omega-3 perinatal supplementation in fmr1 mutant rats. Here, the timing of the dietary administration is similar to the “prenatal paradigm” described by the authors and thus support the authors' findings. I suggest to discuss this study also in the discussion section to corroborate the present findings.

Methods

- As dietary supplementation in the prenatal paradigm begin with parents and ends at weaning, is more appropriate to call this period "perinatal", a time interval that includes pregnancy, parturition, and lactation, differently from the “prenatal” period that refers to the interval of time before parturition.

- The authors stated that breeders were placed on one of the three experimental diets one week before pairing. I wondered if there is a specific reason for that instead of starting the dietary manipulation from gestational day 0 (the day of conception). Moreover, from the methods section is not clear how the authors calculate the gestational day 0 (plug?) reported in figure 1 (G0). This aspect should be detailed in the text. If G0 was not calculated, the authors should remove from figure 1 "G0" and specify that the dietary manipulation begins at the moment of breeding. I recommend revising the figure to avoid misleading interpretations of the prenatal/perinatal paradigm.

- From the methods section, is not clear if the authors culled the litter or not. Culling of rodent litters in the early postnatal period (generally postnatal day 1) is a standard practice in many laboratories, to reduce the litter size-induced variability in the growth and development of pups during the postnatal period and thus increase the sensitivity of the statistical analyses used to detect treatment-related effects (Agnish and Keller, 1997). Please add information about litter size. On the contrary, if litters were not culled, please specify this in the methods section.

- In the elevated plus-maze description, the parameter analyzed should be listed (Distance Moved, Velocity, and % Time in Open Arms); moreover, the method used to calculate the % Time in Open Arms should be described.

Results

- The EPM task is principally performed to evaluate anxiety in rodents and in a second time it could be used to analyze locomotor activity. Thus, devote a single paragraph to the locomotor activity detected from the EPM could be confusing, for this reason, I suggest merging the two paragraphs relative to the EPM task and describe the results concerning anxiety and the locomotor activity together.

Discussion

- In this section, the authors state that omega-3 fatty acids are a promising alternative to pharmaceuticals for individuals with FXS. This is an overstatement that, in my opinion, needs to be mitigated, considering that clinical data on this issue are still lacking.

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: Yes: David Q. Beversdorf, MD

Reviewer #2: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2022 Jan 28;17(1):e0262916. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0262916.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


21 Oct 2021

We would first like to thank the reviewers for their time and input into this manuscript. We believe we have addressed their comments. We put the original comments in black text below, followed by our rebuttal that is bulleted and in blue text. We believe the manuscript is much improved through their input.

Reviewer #1: Re: PONE-D-21-20764

Nolan et al describe the effects of dietary rescue of omega-3 fatty acids at two different time points in the Fmr1 KO mouse. It is of interest, but there are some issues with the communication of the findings.

In the Introduction, first paragraph, ‘signaling in the present population’- what present population? Do the authors mean in a clinical population?

• We agree that this was confusing, and this has been clarified in the text to reflect that we were referring to the clinical population with FXS.

In the next paragraph, the omega-3 effects in ASD could use some additional citations, including more recent ones, to reflect the mixed nature of the findings, and also could point out that intervention once the diagnosis is established might be too late for maximal impact, further justifying the prenatal administration explored in the present study. In the prenatal use of omega-3 in ASD models (refs 21-24), might also mention Matsui et al (Neuroscience 2018; 371:407-419), as it also examined prenatal vs postnatal-only omega-3.

• We thank the reviewer for these thoughtful inclusions and have expanded this section to include this reference and further discussion of why the prenatal administration is critical.

In the final paragraph, might expand the context and explain for the reader ‘and how previous effects compare when referenced to a standard dietary control’, as readers might not already be aware of the nature of the control diets in the previous work.

• We agree that this should be highlighted and as such have included further context in the final paragraph of the introduction.

Methods- For Tables 1, 2, and 4, can the Standard Diet contents also be shown?

• Because this is a proprietary diet formulation, we were unable to get a full ingredients list from the provider, but the methods have been amended to include the specific chow being fed to the Standard Diet group.

The second study mentions that all tested rodents were males. The first study does not specify. Were only males used for each study? Might discuss for the reader why that is done.

• A short explanation of the reason for not including females was added to the methods section. We agree that future studies should include these, but it was not feasible for the present study.

Also, should explain for the reader why it was decided to test study 1 mice at 90 days but study 2 mice at 60 days, particularly as this appeared to have quite an impact on the results.

• We agree with the reviewer that on the surface it is odd that we chose different days for testing. We chose day 60 after the perinatal treatment because it was the earliest day that the mice would be considered a young adult. We chose PD 90 for the postnatal treatment that we the mice could have a longer exposure to the high omega-3 diet. The time periods were chosen to maximize the effect of the diet.

In the second paragraph of Delay Fear Conditioning, ‘placed in the familiar context’ on the second line- isn’t this more specific- ‘shock-associated contest’? Not just familiar?

• We have adjusted this language in the methods.

Results- Would be helpful to find a way to indicate within Figure 2C more of the various individual significant findings within the Figure.

• This figure has been updated in accordance with this comment.

There are several places in the Results where the text is quite unclear regarding which diet timeframe is being discussed. For the startle and PPI results, second paragraph, ‘Diet did not significantly impact percent inhibition alone’- which diet timeframe?

• We agree that this is confusing and have made adjustments throughout the manuscript to mention the specific diet timeframe being evaluated.

Also please clarify ‘However, overall, the combination of these two factors differentially impacted percent inhibition’- which two factors (among genotype, diet, and level of stimulus)?

• We have clarified this in the text.

Similarly, Figure 3B legend does not specify the diet timeframe. Figure 3C legend does not mention the significant finding illustrated by a and b in the Figure itself.

• We agree that the diet timeframe was confusing and have made adjustments accordingly. The a and b designation here is used to illuminate main effects and is discussed in the text discussing this figure.

Later, grammar ‘these interaction of these levels’.

• This has been amended in the manuscript.

Paragraph 5 of this section it is again not specified the timing of the diet discussed for these PPI results.

• We agree that the diet timeframe was confusing and have made adjustments accordingly.

In the discussion of the elevated plus maze, this is the first time that the lack of effect of the Fmr1 KO on behavior is mentioned in the context of the PD90 timing- this should be mentioned with the earlier negative findings for effects of Fmr1 KO as well.

• We have added some discussion into the results concerning the fear conditioning data where we also found an age-related discrepancy. Additionally, there is further discussion in the discussion section regarding these specific instances.

It is mentioned in this section that the control fat diet resembles the typical Western diet- this probably deserves more mention in the Discussion or Introduction for its potential salience.

• A paragraph was added to the end of the discussion about the potential implications of the findings here regarding the findings concerning the Western-like diet.

Figure 5 legend for 5C- ‘Cued recall was impaired in wildtype and knockout animals’- do the authors mean ‘impaired by diet’ here?

• We agree that this description was confusing and have made adjustments accordingly.

Figure 6 legend, again, need to be clear about the timing of the diets in the text of the legend.

• We agree that the diet timeframe was confusing and have made adjustments accordingly.

I could not find Table 8.

• We apologize for this omission and this has been rectified. This table was unfortunately still called Table 8 in the text but should instead refer there to Table S3!

Be clear what animals are being used for the cytokine and BDNF signaling for association with fear conditioning?- all diets?

• We apologize for the lack of clarity – a sentence has been added to the beginning of the section of results concerning the correlations to reflect that animals from all diets were included in these analyses.

Also, for ‘may instead reflect baseline differences in this model’- please explain- since there is the co-factor of all of the dietary interventions, I’m not clear how this conclusion was made.

• We agree that this conclusion was erroneous and have removed this statement from the results section.

Figure 7 again needs clarity on diet timing in the text of the legend.

• This has been amended in the text of that legend.

Next to last paragraph of Results, ‘Similar to the post-weaning paradigm’- seems the end of the sentence should make some reference to this being the preweaning paradigm.

• Phrasing has been added to make this clearer.

Reviewer #2: Comments to the authors

The present article aimed to elucidate the therapeutic efficacy of omega-3 fatty acids for various behavioral and neuro-immune aspects of the Fmr1 phenotype in the mouse. Overall, I find the study interesting as omega-3 supplementation and its relation with inflammation have been described, but further research is still needed to understand the precise mechanisms and therapeutic potential of the supplementation. Moreover, the timing of the treatment is essential to understand the potential of omega-3 supplementation in neurodevelopmental psychopathologies such as autism and fragile X syndrome. I appreciate the large amount of data presented (also in supplementary information) and I find the whole manuscript interesting. However, some conceptual errors and misleading information are present; moreover, in the manuscript, the authors refer to figure 8 that I cannot find in the figure lists. For this reason, I recommend major revision.

Introduction

The introduction presents the topic clearly, and the aim of the study is supported by the appropriate background. However, when referred to the work of Pietropaolo and co-workers the authors state that "it is unknown if administration of this intervention at an earlier time-point would result in differential effects on the behavioral and neuroinflammatory phenotypes studied". On this purpose, the authors should take into account the work of Schiavi et al. 2020 (PMID: 32912100) which assess the effect of omega-3 perinatal supplementation in fmr1 mutant rats. Here, the timing of the dietary administration is similar to the “prenatal paradigm” described by the authors and thus support the authors' findings. I suggest to discuss this study also in the discussion section to corroborate the present findings.

• We agree that more references could improve readability and have as such included this reference as well as others, and further mention in the discussion.

Methods

As dietary supplementation in the prenatal paradigm begin with parents and ends at weaning, is more appropriate to call this period "perinatal", a time interval that includes pregnancy, parturition, and lactation, differently from the “prenatal” period that refers to the interval of time before parturition.

• We agree that perinatal may be a more appropriate label for the timeframe chosen and have updated the manuscript, supplemental information and figures from prenatal to perinatal.

The authors stated that breeders were placed on one of the three experimental diets one week before pairing. I wondered if there is a specific reason for that instead of starting the dietary manipulation from gestational day 0 (the day of conception). Moreover, from the methods section is not clear how the authors calculate the gestational day 0 (plug?) reported in figure 1 (G0). This aspect should be detailed in the text. If G0 was not calculated, the authors should remove from figure 1 "G0" and specify that the dietary manipulation begins at the moment of breeding. I recommend revising the figure to avoid misleading interpretations of the prenatal/perinatal paradigm.

• We have updated the manuscript and figure 1, removing mention of G0, to make this part clearer. As the figure legend states and text state, the experimental diets were given starting 1 week prior to breeder pairing.

From the methods section, is not clear if the authors culled the litter or not. Culling of rodent litters in the early postnatal period (generally postnatal day 1) is a standard practice in many laboratories, to reduce the litter size-induced variability in the growth and development of pups during the postnatal period and thus increase the sensitivity of the statistical analyses used to detect treatment-related effects (Agnish and Keller, 1997). Please add information about litter size. On the contrary, if litters were not culled, please specify this in the methods section.

• We have updated the methods section to reflect that no litters were culled in either paradigm.

In the elevated plus-maze description, the parameter analyzed should be listed (Distance Moved, Velocity, and % Time in Open Arms); moreover, the method used to calculate the % Time in Open Arms should be described.

• We have updated the methods section with further description of these parameters and how they were calculated.

Results

The EPM task is principally performed to evaluate anxiety in rodents and in a second time it could be used to analyze locomotor activity. Thus, devote a single paragraph to the locomotor activity detected from the EPM could be confusing, for this reason, I suggest merging the two paragraphs relative to the EPM task and describe the results concerning anxiety and the locomotor activity together.

• We agree that this could be confusing and thus merged together both Figure 2 and Figure 4 as well as their respective results sections.

Discussion

In this section, the authors state that omega-3 fatty acids are a promising alternative to pharmaceuticals for individuals with FXS. This is an overstatement that, in my opinion, needs to be mitigated, considering that clinical data on this issue are still lacking.

• We have mitigated this statement and rephrased it into further evidence of a need for more clinical research.

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.docx

Decision Letter 1

Alexandra Kavushansky

21 Dec 2021

PONE-D-21-20764R1Dietary rescue of adult behavioral deficits in the Fmr1 knockout mousePLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Lugo,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Feb 04 2022 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Alexandra Kavushansky, PhD

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

Please review your reference list to ensure that it is complete and correct. If you have cited papers that have been retracted, please include the rationale for doing so in the manuscript text, or remove these references and replace them with relevant current references. Any changes to the reference list should be mentioned in the rebuttal letter that accompanies your revised manuscript. If you need to cite a retracted article, indicate the article’s retracted status in the References list and also include a citation and full reference for the retraction notice.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: Re: PONE-D-21-20764R1

Nolan et al describe the effects of dietary rescue of omega-3 fatty acids at two different time points in the Fmr1 KO mouse, which they have resubmitted. It is of interest, but there still a couple of remaining issues with the communication of the findings. They have addressed the remainder of my comments.

Methods- new text at the end of the first section- ‘Only male mice were used for the present studies, as previous work from our lab did not 129 find strong evidence of a phenotype in many of the tasks included here’- do the authors mean to add ‘…in females’ here? Otherwise, please clarify

Results- For the startle and PPI results, second paragraph, ‘Diet did not significantly impact percent inhibition alone’- which diet timeframe? This detail is still missing

Discussion- Paragraph 4- The lack of effect of the mutation on social behavior and stereotypy is fleetingly mentioned- should briefly place this in context of what is expected. Finally, the last paragraph should present more awareness of how much needs to be done before this can be translated clinically.

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: Yes: David Q. Beversdorf, MD

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2022 Jan 28;17(1):e0262916. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0262916.r004

Author response to Decision Letter 1


3 Jan 2022

Reviewer #1: Re: PONE-D-21-20764R1

Nolan et al describe the effects of dietary rescue of omega-3 fatty acids at two different time points in the Fmr1 KO mouse, which they have resubmitted. It is of interest, but there still a couple of remaining issues with the communication of the findings. They have addressed the remainder of my comments.

Methods- new text at the end of the first section- ‘Only male mice were used for the present studies, as previous work from our lab did not 129 find strong evidence of a phenotype in many of the tasks included here’- do the authors mean to add ‘…in females’ here? Otherwise, please clarify.

We addressed this comment by noting that we did not find strong evidence of a phenotype in female mutants. Thank you for catching this exclusion!

Results- For the startle and PPI results, second paragraph, ‘Diet did not significantly impact percent inhibition alone’- which diet timeframe? This detail is still missing

This timeframe has been clarifying in the appropriate text.

Discussion- Paragraph 4- The lack of effect of the mutation on social behavior and stereotypy is fleetingly mentioned- should briefly place this in context of what is expected.

We expanded this portion of the discussion to note that based on previous findings in both our labs and others that this phenotype is not typically noted, though is more common in rat models.

Finally, the last paragraph should present more awareness of how much needs to be done before this can be translated clinically.

We expanded this paragraph to note that though many treatments have been posited by preclinical research, their efficacy in humans is still not clear and this treatment deserves the same scrutiny despite the promising results demonstrated here.

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.docx

Decision Letter 2

Alexandra Kavushansky

10 Jan 2022

Dietary rescue of adult behavioral deficits in the Fmr1 knockout mouse

PONE-D-21-20764R2

Dear Dr. Lugo,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Alexandra Kavushansky, PhD

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: The authors have done an excellent job addressing the remaining comments. Looking forward to seeing the paper published!

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: Yes: Dr. David Q Beversdorf

Acceptance letter

Alexandra Kavushansky

20 Jan 2022

PONE-D-21-20764R2

Dietary rescue of adult behavioral deficits in the Fmr1 knockout mouse

Dear Dr. Lugo:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Alexandra Kavushansky

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 File. Additional methods and results.

    (DOCX)

    S1 Table. ANOVA results for post-weaning paradigm.

    (DOCX)

    S2 Table. ANOVA results for prenatal paradigm.

    (DOCX)

    S3 Table. Cytokine associations with behavior for both paradigms.

    (DOCX)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.docx

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    The data are available on Figshare: https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.18321518.v1.


    Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES