Skip to main content
Cambridge Open Access logoLink to Cambridge Open Access
. 2021 Jul 29;25(4):893–903. doi: 10.1017/S1368980021003098

Awareness of and reactions to health and environmental harms of red meat among parents in the United States

Anna H Grummon 1,2,*, Dina Goodman 3, Lindsay M Jaacks 4, Lindsey Smith Taillie 5,6, Christina A Chauvenet 7, Meg G Salvia 8, Eric B Rimm 8,9
PMCID: PMC8799779  NIHMSID: NIHMS1728444  PMID: 34321133

Abstract

Objective:

Evidence of the health and environmental harms of red meat is growing, yet little is known about which harms may be most impactful to include in meat reduction messages. This study examined which harms consumers are most aware of and which most discourage them from wanting to eat red meat.

Design:

Within-subjects randomised experiment. Participants responded to questions about their awareness of, and perceived discouragement in response to, eight health and eight environmental harms of red meat presented in random order. Discouragement was assessed on a 1-to-5 Likert-type scale.

Setting:

Online survey.

Participants:

544 US parents.

Results:

A minority of participants reported awareness that red meat contributes to health harms (ranging from 8 % awareness for prostate cancer to 28 % for heart disease) or environmental harms (ranging from 13 % for water shortages and deforestation to 22 % for climate change). Among specific harms, heart disease elicited the most discouragement (mean = 2·82 out of 5), followed by early death (mean = 2·79) and plants and animals going extinct (mean = 2·75), though most harms elicited similar discouragement (range of means, 2·60–2·82). In multivariable analyses, participants who were younger, identified as Black, identified as politically liberal, had higher general perceptions that red meat is bad for health and had higher usual red meat consumption reported being more discouraged from wanting to eat red meat in response to health and environmental harms (all P < 0·05).

Conclusions:

Messages about a variety of health and environmental harms of red meat could inform consumers and motivate reductions in red meat consumption.

Keywords: Meat, Environmental sustainability, Messages, Communication, Parents, Food labelling


High consumption of meat, particularly red and processed meat, increases risk of CVD, diabetes and some cancers(18). Further, red meat is a major contributor to environmental harms such as greenhouse gas emissions(1,913), air and water pollution(1,9), biodiversity loss(1,14) and deforestation(14,15). Reducing red meat consumption is therefore an important strategy for reducing chronic disease risk and mitigating environmental damage(16).

Despite growing recognition of the health and environmental harms of red meat, American adults consume an average of 284 g/week (about 0·6 pounds) of unprocessed red meat alone (i.e. not including processed red meats such as bacon), nearly three times the maximum level recommended for optimising human and planetary health(2). More than half of Americans say they are willing to eat less red meat(17). Yet red meat consumption is projected to increase over the next decade(18), perhaps in part because many Americans are unaware of the health and environmental harms of red meat(17,19). Given this willingness to change, coupled with lack of awareness about red meat’s impacts, educating consumers about the harms of red meat could reduce red meat consumption.

A growing body of research has shown that communicating about a products’ health harms, for example, through product warning labels and mass media campaigns, is an effective strategy for reducing unhealthy behaviours including cigarette smoking(20,21), alcohol consumption(22) and sugary drink consumption(2225). Similarly, a recent systematic review found that providing information about the health effects of meat consumption is an effective strategy for reducing intentions to eat meat as well as meat consumption(26). Emerging literature also suggests the promise of communicating about products’ environmental harms as a strategy for changing consumer behaviour. For example, a randomised experiment with undergraduate students in the UK found that sending students 2 weeks of daily messages about the environmental effects of meat production reduced students’ red and processed meat consumption compared with a no-message control group(27). What remains unknown is which specific health and environmental harms hold the most promise for motivating consumers to reduce their red meat intake. Identifying the specific harms that most discourage red meat intake is important because messaging campaigns may not be able to communicate about all harms (e.g. due to space constraints), and because prior studies of tobacco and sugary drink messages suggest advantages to shorter, simpler messages(2832). Also unknown is whether consumers’ reactions to health and environmental harms of red meat vary by demographic characteristics, information that could help to tailor messages to specific groups.

To inform communication efforts, we examined consumers’ responses to health and environmental harms of red meat in an experiment with US parents of young children. Parents are a critical group to study in dietary communication interventions because their behaviours influence both their own health and the dietary habits of their children(33). Parents of young children (i.e. under age five) are especially important, given that dietary habits in early childhood affect diet and health later in childhood and into adolescence(34,35). Moreover, US parents are nearly 40 % less likely than non-parents to have reduced their red meat intake compared with 3 years ago(19), suggesting red meat reduction campaigns may be especially beneficial for this group. Thus, the specific objectives of this study were to examine which health and environmental harms of red meat parents are aware of and which are most likely to discourage red meat consumption. Additionally, to provide insight on populations that might respond more strongly to messages about red meat’s harms, we examined demographic predictors of awareness of health and environmental harms and of the extent to which these harms discouraged participants from wanting to consume red meat.

Methods

Prior to data collection, we pre-registered the sample size, hypotheses and analysis plan (https://aspredicted.org/q5e9b.pdf). The only deviations from this plan were that we corrected for multiple comparisons using Bonferroni’s method rather than Tukey’s method because Tukey’s method cannot be applied to mixed models, and that we conducted two unplanned exploratory analyses, examining: (1) predictors of awareness of health and environmental harms and (2) predictors of harm-induced discouragement separately for health v. environmental harms.

Participants

In January 2020, we recruited a convenience sample of 544 US adults using the survey research firm Dynata as part of a study of parents’ responses to experimental stimuli and survey questions. Participants were eligible if they were aged 18 years or older and had a child aged 6 months to 5 years. Online convenience samples can provide efficient and generalisable findings for experiments like the one used in this study(36).

Procedures

Participants provided informed consent, completed an online survey programmed in Qualtrics and received incentives from Dynata (e.g. gift cards, reward points).

Measures

A flow of survey questions is shown in Fig. 1. First, participants answered questions about their usual red meat consumption(19,37) and general perceptions that red meat is bad for health and for the environment (e.g. ‘How bad or good for your health do you think eating red meat is?’). Next, they responded to questions about their awareness of, and discouragement in response to, specific health and environmental harms of red meat. The order of presentation of health and environmental harms was randomised such that half of participants answered questions about health harms first and half answered questions about environmental harms first.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Flow chart of survey questions

We assessed awareness of harms using a select-all-that-apply question adapted from previous studies(3840), ‘Before today, had you ever heard that eating red meat can contribute to the following harms?’ Then, we listed the eight health or eight environmental harms, displayed in random order. Participants could also select ‘I haven’t heard of red meat contributing to any of these harms before;’ this option was always displayed last.

Next, we assessed the extent to which each harm discouraged participants from wanting to eat red meat using an item adapted from previous studies(3840), ‘How much does knowing that eating red meat contributes to these harms discourage you from wanting to eat red meat?’ We assessed perceived discouragement because meta-analytic evidence indicates that perceived message effectiveness is predictive of actual message effectiveness(41). Participants rated perceived discouragement in response to each harm on a 5-point scale, from ‘Not at all’ (coded as 1) to ‘Very much’ (coded as 5).

We selected harms to present based on recent literature linking red meat production and consumption with health and environmental harms (Table 1). The eight health harms were type 2 diabetes(42,43), weight gain(4345), heart disease(43,46), stroke(47,48), colon cancer(43,49,50), prostate cancer(49), stomach cancer(49) and early death(43,51). The eight environmental harms were climate change(14,15), more greenhouse gases(1,912), water shortages(1,11,52), water pollution(1), air pollution(9), plants and animals going extinct(1,14), clearing of forests(14,15) and worse land quality(1,15,53). All participants rated their awareness and discouragement for each of the eight health harms and each of the eight environmental harms. Within each type of harm (health v. environmental), specific harms were presented in random order.

Table 1.

Health and environmental harms of red meat shown in experiment and supporting evidence

Red meat harms Supporting evidence
Health harms
Type 2 diabetes • Meta-analyses have found that both unprocessed and processed red meat intake are associated with increased risk of type 2 diabetes(3,72)
Weight gain • A systematic review(45) and a meta-analysis(44) found that red and processed meat intake is associated with weight gain and increased risk of overweight and obesity
Heart disease • Meta-analyses have found that red meat intake is associated with CVD risk factors(46) and increased risk of CHD and heart failure(73)
Stroke • Meta-analyses have found that red meat intake is associated with increased risk of stroke(47,73)
Colon cancer • Meta-analyses have found that red and processed meat intake is associated with increased risk of colorectal cancer(4,74,75). Additionally, the International Agency for Research on Cancer (IARC) classifies red meat as probably carcinogenic to humans(5)
Prostate cancer • A meta-analysis found that red meat intake is associated with increased risk of prostate cancer(5)
• A pooled analysis of fifteen prospective cohorts found that red and processed meat intake is associated with increased risk of advanced prostate cancer(76)
Stomach cancer • A meta-analysis found that red meat intake is associated with increased risk of gastric (i.e. stomach) cancer(77)
Early death • A meta-analysis found that red meat intake is associated with increased risk of all-cause mortality(78)
• A pooled analysis of two prospective cohorts found that red meat intake is associated with increased risk of all-cause mortality(79)
Environmental harms
Climate change • Systematic reviews of life cycle analyses indicate that production of red meat is a major contributor to greenhouse gas emissions(9,14), which are a key driver of climate change(80)
More greenhouse gases • Systematic reviews of life cycle analyses indicate that production of red meat is a major contributor to greenhouse gas emissions(9,14)
• Ruminant (e.g. cow, goat, sheep) production is a major contributor to methane emissions(80,81)
Water shortages • Production of red meat is a major contributor to water use(82) and water scarcity (i.e. the relative freshwater availability in a given region)(83)
Water pollution • Production of red meat is a major contributor to water pollution, including through leaching of fertilisers and pesticides used to grow animal feed(82) and through increases in eutrophication(9) (the process by which water becomes enriched with minerals and nutrients, stimulating algal blooms and other negative ecological effects)
Air pollution • Systematic reviews of life cycle analyses indicate that production of red meat is a major contributor to acidifying emissions (e.g. SO2, NH3 and NO x )(9)
Plants and animals going extinct • Meat production (particularly red meat) contributes to biodiversity loss through habitat destruction (e.g. when land is converted to use for feed production or animal grazing, or due to nutrient pollution)(1,84,85)
Clearing of forests • Red meat (particularly beef) production is a major contributor to deforestation (e.g. when forests are converted to pasture for cattle)(14,86)
Worse land quality • Red meat production contributes to land degradation via overgrazing, compaction and erosion(15,87)

Finally, the survey assessed standard demographics. Survey measures appear in online supplementary material, Supplemental Exhibit 1.

Analysis

First, we calculated the proportion of participants who reported they were aware of each harm and the mean discouragement ratings for each harm. We also calculated the proportion of participants who were aware of at least one harm, and the mean number of harms for which participants indicated awareness, both overall and separately for health and environmental harms. Next, we assessed whether likelihood of reporting awareness of harms was higher for health compared with environmental harms using mixed effects logistic regression, regressing awareness (coded as 0/1 for each harm) on an indicator variable for whether the harm was a health or environmental harm. These models treated the intercept as random to account for repeated measures within participants.

To shed light on the populations who were most aware of red meat’s harms, analyses also examined demographic predictors of the total number of harms for which participants indicated awareness (summed across all sixteen health and environment harms). These analyses used negative binomial regression with robust standard errors. We regressed the total number of harms for which participants indicated awareness on the following potential predictors: age, gender, race/ethnicity, educational attainment, income, political leaning and usual red meat consumption. We also used this approach to examine predictors of awareness separately for health harms v. environmental harms.

Next, analyses assessed whether health or environmental harms were more effective at discouraging participants from wanting to eat red meat. These analyses used a linear mixed model, regressing harm-induced discouragement ratings on an indicator variable for whether the harm was a health or environmental harm, treating the intercept as random. We then assessed the extent to which each specific harm elicited discouragement using a linear mixed model with indicator variables for each of the sixteen harms (excluding one as the referent), again treating the intercept as random. We used z-tests to conduct pairwise comparisons of predicted mean discouragement for each harm, applying Bonferroni’s method to adjust for multiple comparisons. These comparisons allowed us to determine which of the harms (if any) were more discouraging than the others while adjusting for repeated measures within participants.

To examine which population groups reported more discouragement in response to health and environmental harms of red meat, we also examined demographic predictors of average discouragement from wanting to eat red meat. First, we averaged discouragement ratings across all sixteen harms. We then used ordinary least squares linear regression to assess predictors of average discouragement. These analyses assessed the same demographic predictors as for awareness and additionally examined general perceptions that red meat is bad for health and for the environment. Exploratory analyses used the same approach to examine predictors of average harm-induced discouragement separately for health harms v. environmental harms.

Analyses were conducted in 2021 using Stata MP version 16.1 (StataCorp LLC).

Results

Participants’ average age was 33·8 (sd 8·0) years (range: 19–80). About two-thirds were White (69 %), 18 % were Latino(a), 8 % were another race/ethnicity and 5 % were Black (Table 2). Slightly more than half (57 %) of participants identified as female and 22 % had a high school education or less.

Table 2.

Participant characteristics, n 544 US parents of young children

Characteristic n %
Age in years
 18–25 91 17
 26–34 209 39
 35–44 204 38
 45+ 38 7
Gender
 Male 227 42
 Female 308 57
 Non-binary 1 0·2
Race/ethnicity
 Non-Hispanic White 373 69
 Non-Hispanic Black or African American 29 5
 Hispanic 97 18
 Non-Hispanic other 45 8
Education
 High school or less 120 22
 Some college 85 16
 College degree 224 41
 Graduate degree 114 21
Annual household income
 Less than $25 000 90 17
 $25 000–$49 999 106 19
 $50 000–$74 999 90 17
 $75 000–$99 999 115 21
 $100 000 or more 143 26
Political party
 Liberal 136 25
 Moderate 218 40
 Conservative 189 35
General perceptions of how good or bad red meat is for health
 Very bad 29 5
 Somewhat bad 78 14
 Neither good nor bad 221 41
 Somewhat good 135 25
 Very good 80 15
General perceptions of how good or bad red meat is to the environment
 Very bad 29 5
 Somewhat bad 82 15
 Neither good nor bad 272 50
 Somewhat good 88 16
 Very good 73 13
Usual red meat intake (servings/d)
 Mean 0·7
  sd 0·8

Missing data ranged from 0·0% to 1·5%.

For each of the sixteen harms, fewer than one-third of participants indicated awareness that red meat contributed to that harm (Table 3). About one-third (33 %) of participants were not aware of any of the sixteen harms; 46 % were not aware of any of the health harms, and 51 % were not aware of any of the environmental harms. The specific harms with the highest level of awareness in the sample were heart disease (28 % reported awareness), weight gain (27 %), climate change (22 %) and increased greenhouse gas emissions (21 %). Participants were least aware that red meat contributes to stomach cancer (11 %) and prostate cancer (8 %). In mixed effects logistic regression, participants were similarly likely to report awareness of harms regardless of harm topic (health v. environment, OR = 1·02, P = 0·77).

Table 3.

Awareness and discouragement of the health and environmental harms of red meat consumption, n 544 US parents of young children

Awareness Discouragement
Harm of red meat % Mean sd
Health harms
 Heart disease 28 2·82 1·46
 Weight gain 27 2·75 1·45
 Stroke 17 2·75 1·45
 Colon cancer 14 2·74 1·46
 Type 2 diabetes 13 2·68 1·44
 Early death 13 2·79 1·49
 Stomach cancer 11 2·72 1·44
 Prostate cancer 8 2·60 1·47
Environmental harms
 Climate change 22 2·70 1·44
 Greenhouse gas emissions 21 2·70 1·44
 Water pollution 18 2·69 1·42
 Air pollution 16 2·67 1·42
 Land quality 16 2·65 1·42
 Extinction of plants and animals 14 2·75 1·45
 Deforestation 13 2·68 1·43
 Water shortages 13 2·68 1·45

In multivariate analyses examining predictors of the number of harms for which participants reported awareness, participants aged 26–34 years reported awareness of about 0·3 fewer harms of red meat compared with those aged 18–25 years (B = –0·33, P = 0·029, Table 4). Participants who identified as female reported being aware of fewer harms than those who identified as male (B = –0·26, P = 0·023). Likewise, those who identified as politically moderate (B = –0·31, P = 0·013) or conservative (B = –0·34, P = 0·011) reported awareness of fewer harms than those who identified as liberal. Participants who identified as Black reported awareness of more harms than those identifying as White (B = 0·57, P = 0·006), but identifying as Latino(a) or as another race/ethnicity (compared with identifying as White) was not associated with awareness. Higher educational attainment and higher income were generally associated with being aware of more harms. Usual red meat consumption was not associated with being aware of more health and environmental harms of red meat (B = 0·04, P = 0·558).

Table 4.

Associations between participant characteristics and the total number of health and environmental harms of red meat for which participants reported awareness, n 544 US parents of young children

B se P
Age in years
 18–25 Reference
 26–34 −0·33 0·15 0·029
 35–44 −0·16 0·16 0·304
 45 or older −0·03 0·21 0·886
Female* −0·26 0·11 0·023
Race/ethnicity
 White Reference
 Black 0·57 0·21 0·006
 Latino(a) 0·07 0·14 0·622
 Other race/ethnicity 0·11 0·14 0·455
Education
 High school or less Reference
 Some college 0·29 0·21 0·176
 College degree 0·51 0·18 0·003
 Graduate degree 0·52 0·21 0·013
Annual household income
 Less than $25 000 Reference
 $25 000–$49 999 0·37 0·20 0·062
 $50 000–$74 999 0·42 0·19 0·028
 $75 000–$99 999 0·40 0·20 0·045
 $100 000 or more 0·41 0·20 0·046
Political leaning
 Liberal Reference
 Moderate −0·31 0·13 0·013
 Conservative −0·34 0·13 0·011
Red meat consumption, servings per day 0·04 0·06 0·558

Bs are unstandardised regression coefficients from negative binomial regressions, regressing the total number of health and environmental harms for which participants reported awareness on participant characteristics. Models estimated robust standard errors. Bold coefficients are statistically significant, P < 0·05.

*

Referent group was male. The one non-binary participant was excluded from analysis due to small cell size.

In analyses of awareness of health harms only, participants who identified as Black (compared with White) and those who had higher educational attainment reported awareness of a greater number of health harms. By contrast, participants who identified as politically moderate (compared with liberal) reported awareness of fewer harms (online supplementary material, Supplemental Table 1). In analyses of awareness of environmental harms only, being age 26–34 years (compared with 18–25), identifying as female (compared with male) and identifying as politically conservative (compared with liberal) were associated with awareness of fewer harms (online supplementary material, Supplemental Table 2). Higher education, higher income and higher usual red meat consumption were associated with awareness of more of the environmental harms of red meat.

In mixed effects regressions of harm-induced discouragement, health harms elicited slightly more discouragement than environmental harms, but the magnitude of the difference was small (mean discouragement 2·73 v. 2·69 on the 1–5 Likert scale; B = 0·04, P = 0·010). Harms with higher awareness generally elicited higher discouragement (Fig. 2). Among specific harms, heart disease elicited the highest mean discouragement (mean = 2·82 on the 1–5 Likert scale), followed by early death (mean = 2·79), plants and animals going extinct (mean = 2·75), stroke (mean = 2·75) and weight gain (mean = 2·75) (Table 3). Prostate cancer (mean = 2·60) and worse land quality (mean = 2·65) elicited the lowest discouragement. After adjusting for multiple comparisons, the only significant differences in discouragement between harms were that early death was more discouraging than prostate cancer (difference in predicted means = 0·20, adjusted P = 0·002), and heart disease was more discouraging than both prostate cancer (difference = 0·23, adjusted P < 0·001) and worsening land quality (difference = 0·17, adjusted P = 0·024).

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Health and environmental harms of red meat by discouragement and awareness, n 544 US parents of young children. Inline graphic, health harms; Inline graphic, environmental harms

In multivariate analyses examining predictors of average discouragement ratings, older participants generally reported lower discouragement than younger participants (Table 5). Participants who identified as Black reported higher levels of average discouragement compared with White participants (B = 0·56, P = 0·019). Those who identified as politically moderate (B = –0·40, P = 0·002) or conservative (B = –0·64, P < 0·001) were less discouraged by the health and environmental harms of red meat than those who identified as politically liberal. Participants who reported higher general perceptions that red meat is bad for health also reported higher average discouragement (B = 0·18, P = 0·006). By contrast, general perceptions that red meat is bad for the environment were not associated with average discouragement (B = 0·07, P = 0·329). Finally, participants who reported higher usual red meat consumption reported higher average discouragement (B = 0·36, P < 0·001). Gender, education, income and other race/ethnicities (i.e. Latino(a) and other race/ethnicity) were not associated with average discouragement (all Ps > 0·05). The pattern of results was similar when examining harm-induced discouragement separately for health v. environmental harms (online supplementary material, Supplemental Tables 3 and 4).

Table 5.

Associations between participant characteristics and average discouragement from wanting to eat red meat in response to health and environmental harms of red meat across, n 544 US parents of young children

B se P
Age in years
 18–25 Reference
 26–34 −0·40 0·16 0·011
 35–44 −0·41 0·16 0·013
 45 or older −0·26 0·24 0·272
Female* −0·04 0·12 0·724
Race/ethnicity
 White Reference
 Black 0·56 0·24 0·019
 Latino(a) 0·22 0·14 0·119
 Other race/ethnicity −0·20 0·19 0·281
Education
 High school or less Reference
 Some college 0·12 0·17 0·489
 College degree 0·29 0·16 0·063
 Graduate degree 0·32 0·20 0·109
Annual household income
 Less than $25 000 Reference
 $25 000–$49 999 0·09 0·18 0·607
 $50 000–$74 999 0·07 0·19 0·702
 $75 000–$99 999 0·32 0·20 0·113
 $100 000 or more 0·07 0·20 0·750
Political leaning
 Liberal Reference
 Moderate −0·40 0·13 0·002
 Conservative −0·64 0·14 < 0·001
General perceptions that red meat is bad for health 0·18 0·07 0·006
General perceptions that red meat is bad for the environment 0·07 0·07 0·329
Red meat consumption, servings per day 0·36 0·08 < 0·001

Bs are unstandardised regression coefficients from ordinary least squares regressions regressing participants’ average discouragement ratings (across all sixteen health and environmental harms) on participant characteristics. Bold coefficients are statistically significant, P < 0·05.

*

Referent group was male. The one non-binary participant was excluded from analysis due to small cell size.

Discussion

Results of this study suggest that messages describing the health and environmental harms of red meat could be a promising strategy for discouraging red meat consumption among US parents of young children. The majority of respondents were not yet aware of the specific health and environmental harms of red meat assessed in this study, and one-third were not aware of any of the sixteen harms examined. These results suggest a major opportunity to educate consumers and motivate positive behaviour change. Expectancy disconfirmation theory posits that when consumers receive negative information about a product that conflicts with their prior expectations (e.g. being informed about the harms of red meat when they had previously not known these harms), their attitudes towards the product will become more negative(5456). This theory would suggest that correcting consumers’ misperceptions about the health and environment risks of red meat could motivate them to reduce their red meat consumption. In line with this prediction, one study found that warning messages about the health harms of sugary drinks led to larger changes in parents’ attitudes and purchase intentions when the messages were displayed on beverages parents had perceived as healthier compared with beverages parents already understood to be unhealthy(57).

Regression analyses revealed that participants who were 26–34 years old (compared with 18–25 years), identified as female (compared with male) and identified as politically moderate or conservative (compared with liberal) reported awareness of fewer harms of red meat. By contrast, participants who identified as Black (compared with White) and those with higher educational attainment and higher income reported being aware of more harms. These results suggest that it may be beneficial to tailor awareness-raising campaigns to particular groups with lower awareness, such as parents who identify as female, are politically moderate or conservative or have lower educational attainment or income. However, given that the majority of participants were unaware that red meat contributes to the health and environmental harms assessed in this study, educational efforts are likely to benefit parents from all demographic groups.

Participants’ usual red meat consumption was not related to their awareness of red meat’s health harms, but higher red meat consumption did predict higher awareness of the environmental harms of red meat. The reason for this association is unclear. One explanation is that higher red meat consumers are more likely to pay attention to information about the environmental consequences of red meat consumption because this information is particularly relevant to them, but have not yet acted on their awareness by reducing their red meat consumption. Regardless of the explanation, this finding highlights that interventions would likely benefit from incorporating a variety of strategies to reduce red meat consumption, including increasing the accessibility, availability and attractiveness of non-meat options(58).

Several participant characteristics predicted higher discouragement from wanting to eat red meat in response to health and environmental harms. For example, consumers aged 18–25 years reported higher discouragement in response to environmental harms of red meat compared with those aged 26–34 and 35–44 years, perhaps because young adults have stronger interest in environmental sustainability and greater concern about climate change than older adults(5961). Younger adults also reported higher discouragement in response to health harms than older adults. This pattern of results differs somewhat from prior research finding that younger adults were less likely than older adults to report health reasons for not eating meat(62). Our results could potentially reflect a growing openness among young adults towards reducing their red meat consumption or consuming a plant-forward diet(63), regardless of the precise motivation for making dietary changes. Participants who reported higher red meat consumption also reported higher discouragement in response to health and environmental harms of red meat. This finding is encouraging, as it suggests that messages about the harms of red meat might have the greatest impact on those who stand to benefit the most from reducing their red meat intake. Additionally, participants who had stronger general perceptions that red meat is bad for health reported being more discouraged, on average, in response the specific health and environmental harms examined in this study. By contrast, general perceptions that red meat is bad for the environment were not associated with average discouragement ratings. These findings might suggest that strengthening the public’s general perception that red meat is bad for health could increase the public’s receptivity to messages about specific health or environmental harms. However, the observed associations between participants’ characteristics and their average discouragement ratings should be interpreted with caution because we cannot rule out the possibility that some demographic groups (such as young adults or those who hold stronger general perceptions that red meat is bad for health) might respond more strongly to any type of message presented in an online survey, even messages not about red meat. Studies that experimentally compare red meat messages to control messages are needed to establish whether characteristics like age, meat consumption, and general perceptions about red meat influence the effectiveness of red meat reduction messages on consumer behaviour.

Communication interventions such as product warnings and mass media campaigns that describe the health harms of cigarettes(20,21), alcohol(22) and sugary drinks(2225) have been shown to generate small but meaningful reductions in purchases and consumption of these products, suggesting that communicating about the harms of red meat could help curb red meat intake. We found that both health and environmental harms elicited similar levels of perceived discouragement. Likewise, a variety of health and environmental harms were similarly discouraging to consumers. These results suggest that message developers have many promising options for topics to address in meat reduction messages. The limited differences in mean discouragement observed between the specific harms also suggest that communication campaigns could easily rotate among these harms, a strategy that could help prevent messages from becoming ‘stale’ and losing efficacy over time(64).

The strengths of this study include the comprehensive set of health and environmental harms tested and the experimental comparison of how much each harm motivated participants to reduce their red meat consumption. Limitations include the use of a convenience sample of parents and the relatively young age distribution of the sample. Although prior studies indicate that online convenience samples can provide similar experimental results as probability samples(36,65,66), future research should confirm our findings with non-parents and with a wider range of ages. Additionally, although we did not query whether participants were vegetarian or vegan, about 13 % of our sample reported eating red meat less than 1 time per week during the past 30 d. Future studies may wish to examine awareness and discouragement specifically among non-vegans/vegetarians or among high red meat consumers, for whom messages may be most relevant. Another limitation is that we did not assess whether some consumers believe that red meat is beneficial for specific health and environmental outcomes; understanding whether these beliefs are widespread, and who is most likely to hold these beliefs, could help inform messaging campaigns. Additionally, this study focused on health and environmental harms because these are two of the key reasons that US adults report as motivating them to change their diet or reduce their meat consumption(19,67). However, consumers might also be motivated by other harms of red meat production. For example, a recent meta-analysis suggested that interventions appealing to animal welfare (e.g. by portraying farm animals) hold promise for reducing meat purchases and consumption(68). We also did not assess other potentially important aspects of message design, such as message framing(6971). Finally, while perceived message effectiveness is predictive of behaviour change(41), we did not assess behavioural outcomes. Future studies should experimentally evaluate the extent to which messages describing different types of harms of red meat reduce red meat purchases and consumption.

Conclusions

Reducing meat consumption is critical for mitigating climate change and reducing chronic disease burden(16). Our study suggests that communication interventions describing how red meat consumption affects both human and planetary health hold promise for informing US consumers and motivating reductions in red meat consumption.

Acknowledgements

Acknowledgements: The authors thank Marissa G. Hall for helpful feedback on survey measures. Financial support: This research was funded in whole, or in part, by the Wellcome Trust (216042/Z/19/Z). For the purpose of open access, the author has applied a CC BY public copyright licence to any Author Accepted Manuscript version arising from this submission. DG was supported by T32 HL 098048. MGS was supported by T32 DK 007703. Conflict of interest: There are no conflicts of interest. Authorship: A.H.G., L.M.J., C.A.C. and L.S.T. designed and conceptualised the study. A.H.G., D.G. and M.G.S. contributed to data curation. A.H.G. and D.G. analysed the data and created data visualisations. A.H.G. drafted the initial manuscript. E.B.R. provided study oversight. All authors provided critical feedback on the manuscript draft and approved the final manuscript. Ethics of human subject participation: This study was conducted according to the guidelines laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki and all procedures involving research study participants were approved by the Harvard Longwood Campus Institutional Review Board. Written informed consent was obtained from all subjects/patients.

Supplementary material

For supplementary material accompanying this paper visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S1368980021003098.

S1368980021003098sup001.docx (46.4KB, docx)

click here to view supplementary material

References

  • 1. Clark MA, Springmann M, Hill J et al. (2019) Multiple health and environmental impacts of foods. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 116, 23357. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Willett W, Rockström J, Loken B et al. (2019) Food in the Anthropocene: the EAT–Lancet Commission on healthy diets from sustainable food systems. Lancet 393, 447–492. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Pan A, Sun Q, Bernstein AM et al. (2011) Red meat consumption and risk of type 2 diabetes: 3 cohorts of US adults and an updated meta-analysis. Am J Clin Nutr 94, 1088–1096. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Vieira A, Abar L, Chan D et al. (2017) Foods and beverages and colorectal cancer risk: a systematic review and meta-analysis of cohort studies, an update of the evidence of the WCRF-AICR Continuous Update Project. Ann Oncol 28, 1788–1802. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Bouvard V, Loomis D, Guyton KZ et al. (2015) Carcinogenicity of consumption of red and processed meat. Lancet Oncol 16, 1599–1600. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Bernstein A, Sun Q, Hu F et al. (2010) Major dietary protein sources and risk of coronary heart disease in women. Circulation 122, 876–883. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Springmann M, Godfray HCJ, Rayner M et al. (2016) Analysis and valuation of the health and climate change cobenefits of dietary change. Proc Natl Acad Sci 113, 4146–4151. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Al-Shaar L, Satija A, Wang DD et al. (2020) Red meat intake and risk of coronary heart disease among US men: prospective cohort study. BMJ 371, m4141. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Poore J & Nemecek T (2018) Reducing food’s environmental impacts through producers and consumers. Science 360, 987–992. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Heller MC, Willits-Smith A, Meyer R et al. (2018) Greenhouse gas emissions and energy use associated with production of individual self-selected US diets. Environ Res Lett 13, 044004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Aleksandrowicz L, Green R, Joy EJ et al. (2016) The impacts of dietary change on greenhouse gas emissions, land use, water use, and health: a systematic review. PLoS One 11, e0165797. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Willits-Smith A, Aranda R, Heller MC et al. (2020) Addressing the carbon footprint, healthfulness, and costs of self-selected diets in the USA: a population-based cross-sectional study. Lancet Planet Health 4, e98–e106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Boehm R, Ver Ploeg M, Wilde PE et al. (2019) Greenhouse gas emissions, total food spending and diet quality by share of household food spending on red meat: results from a nationally representative sample of US households. Public Health Nutr 22, 1794–1806. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Gerber PJ, Steinfeld H, Henderson B et al. (2013) Tackling Climate Change Through Livestock: A Global Assessment of Emissions and Mitigation Opportunities. Rome, Italy: Food and Agriculture Organization of the United Nations (FAO). [Google Scholar]
  • 15. IPCC (2020) Summary for Policymakers. Climate Change and Land: An IPCC Special Report on Climate Change, Desertification, Land Degradation, Sustainable Land Management, Food Security, and Greenhouse Gas Fluxes in Terrestrial Ecosystems. Geneva, Switzerland: IPCC. [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Tilman D & Clark M (2014) Global diets link environmental sustainability and human health. Nature 515, 518–522. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Leiserowitz A, Ballew M, Rosenthal S et al. (2020) Climate Change and the American Diet. New Haven, CT: Yale University and Earth Day Network. [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Bentley J (2019) US Per Capita Availability of Red Meat, Poultry, and Seafood on the Rise. United States Department of Agriculture Amber Waves. https://www.ers.usda.gov/amber-waves/2019/december/us-per-capita-availability-of-red-meat-poultry-and-seafood-on-the-rise/ (accessed September 2020).
  • 19. Neff RA, Edwards D, Palmer A et al. (2018) Reducing meat consumption in the USA: a nationally representative survey of attitudes and behaviours. Public Health Nutr 21, 1835–1844. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Noar S, Hall M, Francis D et al. (2015) Pictorial cigarette pack warnings: a meta-analysis of experimental studies. Tob Control 25, 341–354. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Brewer NT, Hall MG, Noar SM et al. (2016) Effect of pictorial cigarette pack warnings on changes in smoking behavior: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Intern Med 176, 905–912. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Clarke N, Pechey E, Kosīte D et al. (2020) Impact of health warning labels on selection and consumption of food and alcohol products: systematic review with meta-analysis. Health Psychol Rev 1–24. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2020.1780147. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Grummon AH & Hall MG (2020) Sugary drink warnings: a meta-analysis of experimental studies. PLoS Med 17, e1003120. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. An R, Liu J, Liu R et al. (2020) Impact of sugar-sweetened beverage warning labels on consumer behaviors: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Am J Prev Med 60, 115–126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Schwartz MB, Schneider GE, Choi Y-Y et al. (2017) Association of a community campaign for better beverage choices with beverage purchases from supermarkets. JAMA Intern Med 177, 666–674. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Harguess JM, Crespo NC & Hong MY (2020) Strategies to reduce meat consumption: a systematic literature review of experimental studies. Appetite 144, 104478. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Wolstenholme E, Poortinga W & Whitmarsh L (2020) Two birds, one stone: the effectiveness of health and environmental messages to reduce meat consumption and encourage pro-environmental behavioral spillover. Front Psychol 11, 2596. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Noar SM, Kelley DE, Boynton MH et al. (2018) Identifying principles for effective messages about chemicals in cigarette smoke. Prev Med 106, 31–37. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Brewer NT, Jeong M, Hall MG et al. (2019) Impact of e-cigarette health warnings on motivation to vape and smoke. Tob Control 28, e64–e70. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Baig SA, Byron MJ, Boynton MH et al. (2017) Communicating about cigarette smoke constituents: an experimental comparison of two messaging strategies. J Behav Med 40, 352–359. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Grummon AH, Hall MG, Taillie LS et al. (2019) How should sugar-sweetened beverage health warnings be designed? A randomized experiment. Prev Med 121, 158–166. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Grummon AH, Hall MG, Mitchell CG et al. (2020) Reactions to messages about smoking, vaping and COVID-19: two national experiments. Tob Control 1–9. doi: 10.1136/tobaccocontrol-2020-055956. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Savage JS, Fisher JO & Birch LL (2007) Parental influence on eating behavior: conception to adolescence. J Law Med Ethics 35, 22–34. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Nicklaus S & Remy E (2013) Early origins of overeating: tracking between early food habits and later eating patterns. Curr Obes Rep 2, 179–184. [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Singh AS, Mulder C, Twisk JW et al. (2008) Tracking of childhood overweight into adulthood: a systematic review of the literature. Obes Rev 9, 474–488. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Jeong M, Zhang D, Morgan J et al. (2018) Similarities and differences in tobacco control research findings from convenience and probability samples. Ann Behav Med 53, 476–485. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Thompson FE, Midthune D, Kahle L et al. (2017) Development and evaluation of the National cancer Institute’s dietary screener questionnaire scoring algorithms. J Nutr 147, 1226–1233. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Rohde JA, Noar SM, Mendel JR et al. (2019) E-Cigarette health harm awareness and discouragement: implications for health communication. Nicotine Tob Res 22, 1131–1138. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Kelley DE, Boynton MH, Noar SM et al. (2018) Effective message elements for disclosures about chemicals in cigarette smoke. Nicotine Tob Res 20, 1047–1054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Carl A, Taillie L, Grummon AH et al. (2021) Awareness of and reactions to the health harms of sugary drinks: an online study of US parents. Appetite 164, 105234. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Noar SM, Barker J, Bell T et al. (2018) Does perceived message effectiveness predict the actual effectiveness of tobacco education messages? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Health Commun 35, 148–157. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Feskens EJ, Sluik D & van Woudenbergh GJ (2013) Meat consumption, diabetes, and its complications. Curr Diabetes Rep 13, 298–306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Dietary Guidelines Advisory Committee (2020) Scientific Report of the 2020 Dietary Guidelines Advisory Committee: Advisory report to the Secretary of Agriculture and the Secretary of Health and Human Services. Washington, DC: United States Department of Agriculture, Agricultural Research Service. [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Rouhani MH, Salehi-Abargouei A, Surkan PJ et al. (2014) Is there a relationship between red or processed meat intake and obesity? A systematic review and meta-analysis of observational studies. Obes Rev 15, 740–748. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Schlesinger S, Neuenschwander M, Schwedhelm C et al. (2019) Food groups and risk of overweight, obesity, and weight gain: a systematic review and dose-response meta-analysis of prospective studies. Adv Nutr 10, 205–218. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Guasch-Ferré M, Satija A, Blondin SA et al. (2019) Meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials of red meat consumption in comparison with various comparison diets on cardiovascular risk factors. Circulation 139, 1828–1845. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Kim K, Hyeon J, Lee SA et al. (2017) Role of total, red, processed, and white meat consumption in stroke incidence and mortality: a systematic review and meta-analysis of prospective cohort studies. J Am Heart Assoc 6, e005983. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Bernstein Adam M., Pan An, Rexrode Kathryn M et al. (2012) Dietary protein sources and the risk of stroke in men and women. Stroke 43, 637–644. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49. IARC Working Group on the Evaluation of Carcinogenic Risks to Humans (2018) Red Meat and Processed Meat. Lyon, France: International Agency for Research on Cancer. [Google Scholar]
  • 50. World Cancer Research Fund/American Institute for Cancer Research (2018) Meat, Fish and Dairy Products and the Risk of Cancer. London: World Cancer Research Fund/American Institute for Cancer Research. [Google Scholar]
  • 51. Etemadi A, Sinha R, Ward MH et al. (2017) Mortality from different causes associated with meat, heme iron, nitrates, and nitrites in the NIH-AARP Diet and Health Study: population based cohort study. BMJ 357, j1957. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52. Mekonnen MM & Hoekstra AY (2011) The Green, Blue and Grey Water Footprint of Farm Animals and Animal Products. Delft, The Netherlands: UNESCO-IHE Institute for Water Education. [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Foley JA, Ramankutty N, Brauman KA et al. (2011) Solutions for a cultivated planet. Nature 478, 337–342. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54. Van Raaij WF (1991) The Formation and Use of Expectations in Consumer Decision Making. Handbook of Consumer Behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. [Google Scholar]
  • 55. Burton S, Creyer E, Kees J et al. (2006) Attacking the obesity epidemic: the potential health benefits of providing nutrition information in restaurants. Am J Pub Health 96, 1669–1675. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56. Burton S, Howlett E & Tangari AH (2009) Food for thought: how will the nutrition labeling of quick service restaurant menu items influence consumers’ product evaluations, purchase intentions, and choices? J Retailing 85, 258–273. [Google Scholar]
  • 57. Moran AJ & Roberto CA (2018) Health warning labels correct parents’ misperceptions about sugary drink options. Am J Prev Med 55, e19–e27. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58. Ruby MB (2012) Vegetarianism. A blossoming field of study. Appetite 58, 141–150. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59. Saad L & Jones JM (2016) US concern about global warming at eight-year high. Gallup Politics, March 16, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  • 60. First Insight (2020) The State of Consumer Spending: Gen Z Shoppers Demand Sustainability Retail. Warrendale, PA: First Insight, Inc. [Google Scholar]
  • 61. Washington Post and Kaiser Family Foundation (2019) Washington Post-Kaiser Family Foundation Climate Change Survey, July 9–Aug. 5, 2019. The Washington Post. https://www.washingtonpost.com/context/washington-post-kaiser-family-foundation-climate-change-survey-july-9-aug-5-2019/601ed8ff-a7c6-4839-b57e-3f5eaa8ed09f/?itid=lk_inline_manual_4 (accessed May 2020).
  • 62. Pribis P, Pencak RC & Grajales T (2010) Beliefs and attitudes toward vegetarian lifestyle across generations. Nutrients 2, 523–531. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63. Pew Research Center (2016) Public Views about Americans’ Eating Habits. Pew Research Center. https://www.pewresearch.org/science/2016/12/01/public-views-about-americans-eating-habits/ (accessed August 2021). [Google Scholar]
  • 64. Hammond D (2011) Health warning messages on tobacco products: a review. Tob Control 20, 327–337. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65. Berinsky AJ, Huber GA & Lenz GS (2012) Evaluating online labor markets for experimental research: amazon.com’s Mechanical Turk. Political Anal 20, 351–368. [Google Scholar]
  • 66. Weinberg JD, Freese J & McElhattan D (2014) Comparing data characteristics and results of an online factorial survey between a population-based and a crowdsource-recruited sample. Sociol Sci 1, 292–310. [Google Scholar]
  • 67. YouGov (2020) YouGov: Preferred Diets, Fieldwork dates 3rd–6th January, 2020. YouGov. https://docs.google.com/viewer?url=https%3A%2F%2Fd25d2506sfb94s.cloudfront.net%2Fcumulus_uploads%2Fdocument%2Fuu7yxivmb6%2FResults%2520for%2520YouGov%2520RealTime%2520(Preferred%2520Diets)%25202%25201.6%2520(version%25201).xlsb%2520%2520%5BGroup%5D%5BAutoRecovered%5D.pdf (accessed August 2021).
  • 68. Mathur MB, Peacock J, Reichling DB et al. (2021) Interventions to reduce meat consumption by appealing to animal welfare: meta-analysis and evidence-based recommendations. Appetite 164, 105277. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69. Stea S & Pickering GJ (2019) Optimizing messaging to reduce red meat consumption. Environ Commun 13, 633–648. [Google Scholar]
  • 70. Duchene TN & Jackson LM (2019) Effects of motivation framing and content domain on intentions to eat plant- and animal-based foods. Soc Anim 27, 526–543. [Google Scholar]
  • 71. Bertolotti M, Chirchiglia G & Catellani P (2016) Promoting change in meat consumption among the elderly: factual and prefactual framing of health and well-being. Appetite 106, 37–47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72. Schwingshackl L, Hoffmann G, Lampousi A-M et al. (2017) Food groups and risk of type 2 diabetes mellitus: a systematic review and meta-analysis of prospective studies. Eur J Epidemiol 32, 363–375. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73. Bechthold A, Boeing H, Schwedhelm C et al. (2019) Food groups and risk of coronary heart disease, stroke and heart failure: a systematic review and dose-response meta-analysis of prospective studies. Crit Rev Food Sci Nutr 59, 1071–1090. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74. Zhao Z, Feng Q, Yin Z et al. (2017) Red and processed meat consumption and colorectal cancer risk: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Oncotarget 8, 83306. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75. Chan DSM, Lau R, Aune D et al. (2011) Red and processed meat and colorectal cancer incidence: meta-analysis of prospective studies. PLoS One 6, e20456. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76. Wu K, Spiegelman D, Hou T et al. (2016) Associations between unprocessed red and processed meat, poultry, seafood and egg intake and the risk of prostate cancer: a pooled analysis of 15 prospective cohort studies. Int J Cancer 138, 2368–2382. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77. Kim SR, Kim K, Lee SA et al. (2019) Effect of red, processed, and white meat consumption on the risk of gastric cancer: an overall and dose–response meta-analysis. Nutrients 11, 826. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78. Schwingshackl L, Schwedhelm C, Hoffmann G et al. (2017) Food groups and risk of all-cause mortality: a systematic review and meta-analysis of prospective studies. Am J Clin Nutr 105, 1462–1473. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79. Zheng Y, Li Y, Satija A et al. (2019) Association of changes in red meat consumption with total and cause specific mortality among US women and men: two prospective cohort studies. BMJ 365, l2110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80. Edenhofer O, Pichs-Madruga R, Sokona Y et al. (2014) IPCC, 2014: Summary for Policymakers. Climate Change 2014: Mitigation of Climate Change. Contribution of Working Group III to the Fifth Assessment Report of the Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 81. Stocker T (2014) Climate Change 2013: The Physical Science Basis: Working Group I Contribution to the Fifth Assessment Report of the Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 82. Gerbens-Leenes PW, Mekonnen MM & Hoekstra AY (2013) The water footprint of poultry, pork and beef: a comparative study in different countries and production systems. Water Resour Ind 1, 25–36. [Google Scholar]
  • 83. Heller MC, Willits-Smith A, Mahon T et al. (2021) Individual US diets show wide variation in water scarcity footprints. Nat Food 2, 255–263. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84. Machovina B, Feeley KJ & Ripple WJ (2015) Biodiversity conservation: the key is reducing meat consumption. Sci Total Environ 536, 419–431. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85. Williams DR, Clark M, Buchanan GM et al. (2021) Proactive conservation to prevent habitat losses to agricultural expansion. Nat Sustain 4, 314–322. [Google Scholar]
  • 86. Henders S, Persson UM & Kastner T (2015) Trading forests: land-use change and carbon emissions embodied in production and exports of forest-risk commodities. Environ Res Lett 10, 125012. [Google Scholar]
  • 87. Steinfeld H, Gerber P, Wassenaar T et al. (2006) Livestock’s Long Shadow: Environmental Issues and Options. Rome, Italy: Food & Agriculture Organization. [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

For supplementary material accompanying this paper visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S1368980021003098.

S1368980021003098sup001.docx (46.4KB, docx)

click here to view supplementary material


Articles from Public Health Nutrition are provided here courtesy of Cambridge University Press

RESOURCES