Abstract
This paper reviews evidence demonstrating a bidirectional relationship between memory and eating in humans and rodents. In humans, amnesia is associated with impaired processing of hunger and satiety cues, disrupted memory of recent meals, and overconsumption. In healthy participants, meal-related memory limits subsequent ingestive behavior and obesity is associated with impaired memory and disturbances in the hippocampus. Evidence from rodents suggests that dorsal hippocampal neural activity contributes to the ability of meal-related memory to control future intake, that endocrine and neuropeptide systems act in the ventral hippocampus to provide cues regarding energy status and regulate learned aspects of eating, and that consumption of hypercaloric diets and obesity disrupt these processes. Collectively, this evidence indicates that diet-induced obesity may be caused and/or maintained, at least in part, by a vicious cycle wherein excess intake disrupts hippocampal functioning, which further increases intake. This perspective may advance our understanding of how the brain controls eating, the neural mechanisms that contribute to eating-related disorders, and identify how to treat diet-induced obesity.
Keywords: amnesia, appetite, cognition, diet, episodic memory, food intake, hippocampus, interoception
1. INTRODUCTION
To date, the scientific effort to understand how the brain controls eating has focused primarily on brain areas involved in hunger, fullness and pleasure. In contrast, a critical question in behavioral neuroscience that has been relatively poorly addressed is how top-down cognitive processes such as memory modulate energy intake. Evidence suggests that hippocampal-dependent episodic memory of recent eating (i.e., autobiographical memories of personal experiences that have “what”, “when”, and “where” components) inhibit subsequent eating behavior. Episodic memories of personal experiences can last for hours, days, weeks and years, whereas physiological signals generated by ingestion typically last for many hours (Shimy et al., 2020). As a result, memory of a recently eaten meal can serve as a powerful mechanism for controlling future eating behavior because it provides a record of recent intake that likely outlasts most physiological signals generated by a meal. Moreover, episodic memories can provide more detailed information about what, where and what has been eaten than can these physiological signals. The brain has evolved many mechanisms to promote food-seeking and consummatory behaviors; however, neural controls are also needed to inhibit these behaviors, for instance, when sufficient energy has been obtained, to avoid predation, and to promote reproduction. Factoring in memory of recent energy intake into decisions about when, what and how much to eat during the next eating episode promotes flexibility and allows animals to avoid aversive physiological effects of overconsumption or costs associated with unnecessary foraging. Using memory for recent eating to control future eating is also advantageous because prior knowledge about the physiological consequences of foods on the body can be taken into account. Moreover, being able to integrate physiological signals with memory of the meal may allow for improved prediction of the consequences of future consumption. This raises a critical question regarding how cues associated with energy status (e.g., ghrelin, leptin) interact with neural mechanisms involved in memory to control eating. These questions are important given evidence reviewed below suggesting that impaired hippocampal-dependent function causes overeating and weight gain and thus may contribute to the development and maintenance of obesity. Knowing how cognitive processes such as memory control intake will provide a more complete understanding of how the brain controls eating and will likely provide insights regarding the brain mechanisms that contribute to eating-related disorders, such as diet-induced obesity.
This paper will review evidence from human research showing that amnesia is associated with impaired processing of hunger and satiety cues, disrupted memory of recent meals, and overconsumption, and that meal-related memory influences decisions we make about what and how much to eat. Then, evidence from rodents suggesting that neural activity and synaptic plasticity in hippocampal neurons is critical during the postprandial period for limiting future intake will be reviewed, as well as findings showing that endocrine and neuropeptide systems act in the hippocampus to regulate learned aspects of eating. Finally, the ways in which overeating and obesity impair memory and hippocampal function in humans and rodents will be described. Throughout, we attempt to identify gaps in our understanding and future directions. Collectively, this review will present evidence that is consistent with the possibility that diet-induced obesity is caused and/or maintained, at least in part, by a vicious cycle wherein excess intake disrupts the functioning of brain areas critical for memory, which further increases intake (Clasen et al., 2020).
2. IMPACT OF LEARNING AND MEMORY ON INGESTION IN HUMANS
2.1. Evidence from neuropsychological studies
The first clues that memory for recent eating might be important in human appetite control came from observations of patients with amnesia. These patients, who are unable to remember what they have eaten, appear to have disturbances in appetite (Hebben et al., 1985; Higgs, Williamson, & Attwood, 2008; Rozin et al., 1998). Hebben and colleagues (1985) first noted that the famous amnesic patient H.M., who suffered memory loss following brain surgery to treat his epilepsy, hardly ever mentioned being hungry or thirsty, even when he had not eaten or drank anything for some time (Hebben, et al. 1985). To investigate whether his appreciation of internal states had been affected by the surgery, the researchers asked H.M. to rate his hunger and thirst on several occasions before and after eating. H.M. often reported feeling neither hungry nor full, even when he had just completed a meal. Indeed, on some occasions, he reported feeling less full after a meal than before. A “dinner” experiment was also conducted in which H.M. was offered a large amount of food to see if overconsumption might shift his appetite ratings. One evening, after he had finished his usual dinner, any evidence of that meal was removed, and within 1 minute, a second, identical meal was offered. H.M consumed all of the first meal and then went on to eat most of the second meal, but even so, his appetite ratings still did not approach satiety. When questioned 20 minutes later, he could not remember having eaten anything. In the same study, other patients with global amnesia were similarly asked to rate their appetite. In contrast to H.M., three out of four of these patients reported expected decreases in hunger and thirst after all meals. Based on this evidence, Hebben and colleagues (1985) concluded that because most of the other patients with global amnesia reported normal appetite, the disturbances exhibited by H.M. were unlikely to be attributed to his memory loss. However, the results of further neuropsychological studies have suggested that impaired memory is associated with a deficit in satiety. For example, Rozin and colleagues (1998) found that two profoundly amnesic patients ate multiple meals one after another as they were offered: one of the patients consumed two 3-course meals before rejecting an additional meal (Rozin et al., 1998). The explanation forwarded by the authors was that intake is usually constrained by a social norm that eating should stop after a meal has been consumed and because the amnesic patients cannot remember having just eaten, they eat beyond the bounds of what is considered “normal consumption”.
Higgs and colleagues reported similar multiple meal consumption by two amnesic patients and provided evidence that this is a specific effect because other aspects of eating were unaffected (Higgs, Williamson, Rotshtein, et al., 2008). Specifically, sensory-specific satiety (the decline in the pleasantness of food as it is eaten) was intact in these patients and they also responded similarly to control participants when asked to rate the pleasantness of yogurts that had increasing amounts of sugar added, suggesting that their appreciation of sweet taste was normal. The authors reasoned that amnesic patients may consume multiple meals because they are unable to use information about recent consumption encoded in memory to inhibit further consumption. However, as with any studies of neuropsychological patients, the results of these investigations remain open to interpretation: the patients had suffered damage to areas of the brain associated with memory (e.g., hippocampus) but also to other areas involved in emotional responding (e.g., amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex) and had a range of neuropsychological problems. Therefore, an important next step in investigating the involvement of memory for recent eating in appetite in humans was to conduct experimental studies of healthy volunteers.
2.2. Manipulating meal memories in healthy volunteers
2.2.1. Recall of the most recent eating episode
A series of experiments by Higgs and colleagues were the first to investigate the effects of recall of recent eating on subsequent intake in healthy volunteers (Higgs, 2002; Higgs, Williamson, & Attwood, 2008). The rationale behind these studies was that if memory for recent eating inhibits intake, then boosting such memories by asking participants to recall a recent meal should enhance this effect. In the first study, participants ate a pizza lunch in the laboratory and then returned 3 hours later to take part in a snack taste test (Higgs, 2002). Just before the taste test, participants were randomly allocated to either the experimental condition, in which they were asked to write down their thoughts on the lunch they had eaten earlier, or to a control condition in which they were asked to write down thoughts about anything they wished. All participants had consumed the same fixed amount of pizza at the lunch and so the only difference between the conditions was whether they were asked to recall that lunch. Immediately after completing the recall task, the participants were invited to taste and rate some cookies and intake of the cookies was covertly measured. The lunch recall group ate significantly fewer cookies than the control group. These results are consistent with the suggestion that memory of the most recent meal has an inhibitory effect on subsequent intake, but they do not provide evidence of specificity for the recall of that particular meal: it is possible that thinking about any eaten food would have had the same effect. To address this issue, a follow-up study was conducted that included an additional control group in which the participants were asked to recall lunch they had eaten the previous day (Higgs, 2002). This condition controlled for general demand characteristics associated with meal recall and provided a test of whether the effect is specific to recall of the most recently eaten meal. Intake of cookies in the two control groups did not differ, but the inhibitory effect of recalling lunch eaten that day on later intake was replicated.
To further explore the specific role of memory-related processes in the effect of meal recall, another study was conducted that used the same recall paradigm but varied the time between consumption of the lunch and the recall session (Higgs, Williamson, & Attwood, 2008). The rationale here was that if the effect of recalling the most recent meal is reliant on memory processes, no effect of recall would be expected soon after the lunch, because at that point the memory of the meal would still be strong and so additional recall would have little effect. On the other hand, if the effect is due to non-memory related processes, such as meal recall inducing guilt about the lunch that was eaten, then the effects of recall would be expected to be similar regardless of the delay. Participants were either allocated to a “lunch today” recall group or a “lunch yesterday” recall group. For each group, on one test day the recall session and taste test occurred 1 hour after lunch had been eaten and on the other test day the cookie taste test occurred 3 hours after lunch. Participants who recalled the lunch eaten that day 3 hours after lunch, but not 1 hour after lunch, ate fewer cookies at the taste test than did the participants who recalled lunch eaten the previous day. The delay-dependency of the effect is supportive of a memory-related explanation of the phenomenon. Nevertheless, participants in this and other meal recall studies were consuming cookies at the taste test and so it is possible that participants may have reduced their intake at the taste test when reminded of recent eating because the reminder triggered thoughts about dieting and limiting intake of high calorie food. However, this explanation seems unlikely because it has been reported that recall of recent eating also reduces intake of low-calorie plain popcorn and reduces the intake of both restrained and unrestrained eaters (Higgs, Williamson, & Attwood, 2008). Furthermore, the effect is not dependent upon the type of lunch that is recalled, because the inhibitory effect of meal recall is observed regardless of whether participants consume a fixed lunch in the laboratory or whether they eat a self-selected lunch in their usual environment (Higgs, 2002; Higgs, Williamson, & Attwood, 2008). Because the effect is not restricted to thinking about consumption of specific food(s) (e.g., pizza), it is unlikely to be underpinned by mechanisms related to consumption of what may be regarded as unhealthy or forbidden foods.
The inhibitory effect of recalling recent eating on subsequent consumption has now been replicated many times by several research groups and some of the boundary conditions have begun to be investigated and the underlying mechanisms further investigated. Three additional studies found that recall of lunch eaten earlier reduces subsequent consumption (Collins & Stafford, 2015; Szypula et al., 2020; Vartanian et al., 2016) and one study found that recall of a recently consumed beverage inhibited lunch consumption (Yeomans et al., 2017). Across all recall studies (including the earlier studies by Higgs and colleagues) no consistent effects have emerged of meal recall on either appetite or mood ratings, suggesting that changes in these variables do not underlie the decreased intake. It also appears unlikely that meal recall suppresses intake by enhancing general health consciousness, because recall of a recent exercise bout (which might reasonably be expected to cue health-related thoughts) does not affect subsequent snack intake (Vartanian et al., 2016).
Work by Szypula and colleagues (2020) has further extended knowledge about the effects of meal recall by highlighting the importance of moderating contextual factors. These authors replicated the inhibitory effect of meal recall on snack intake, but only when participants freely recalled the lunch and wrote these thoughts down (as in the original studies by Higgs and colleagues). When a guided recall procedure was used, no effect on intake was observed; in fact, the participants tended to eat more following the guided recall (Szypula et al., 2020). The reason for this finding is not clear but may be related to the specific nature of what is recalled. The guided recall procedure used by Szypula and colleagues (2020) focused on sensory/hedonic aspects of the meal, which could have shifted focus away from remembering the consumption episode to thinking about food more generally. In line with this idea, it has been found that exposure to food cues per se tends to increase rather than decrease consumption (Fedoroff et al., 1997). Future studies could investigate whether guided recall focused on food consumption, such as the associated bodily sensations, decreases subsequent intake. Szypula and colleagues (2020) also found no effect on intake when participants recalled the meal verbally in the presence of the experimenter. Again, it is unclear why this was the case, but (Collins & Stafford, 2015) observed that the inhibitory effect of meal recall was attenuated when participants engaged in a positive mood induction task immediately after recall. It is possible that distraction away from the meal recall either by verbal interaction with the experimenter or positive mood induction reduces the effectiveness of the recall on subsequent consumption.
In summary, recall of the most recent eating episode has been found to inhibit subsequent consumption and this effect appears to be robust. Data from these meal recall studies support the idea that information in memory about the last meal is factored into decisions about subsequent consumption. However, evidence from one paradigm is inevitably limited by specific aspects of that method. In the case of the meal recall paradigm, participants are explicitly asked to think about what they have recently eaten and so it remains possible that non-memory related consequences of this procedure underlie, at least in part, the inhibition of intake. Therefore, it is important to test the effects of different kinds of meal memory manipulations to examine generalizability of the results.
2.2.2. Disrupting meal memory encoding
An alternative approach to testing the idea that memory for recent eating inhibits later intake is to focus on encoding of the meal memory rather than on explicit recall. If memory for recent eating is important in appetite control, then manipulations that either impair or enhance the formation of meal memories at the time of consumption should affect later intake. One way meal memory encoding can be disrupted experimentally is to divert participants’ attention away from the food being consumed by asking them to engage in as secondary activity such as watching TV or playing a computer game while eating. Several studies have employed such a paradigm, and all have found that dividing attention between the meal and the secondary activity results in a poorer meal memory and greater snack intake at a later taste test (Higgs & Woodward, 2009; Mittal et al., 2011; Oldham-Cooper et al., 2011). For example, Higgs and Woodward (2009) found that participants who consumed their lunch while watching television ate more cookies at an afternoon tasting session than did participants who ate exactly the same lunch but without the television distraction. The participants who had the television lunch also rated their meal memory as much less vivid that then control participants. A similar study by Brunstrom and colleagues (Oldham-Cooper et al., 2011) found that playing a computer game while eating lunch was associated with greater afternoon snack intake and less accurate recall of the order in which they ate different food items at lunch.
This ability of distraction during eating to increase later intake has been found regardless of the whether participants watched a boring, sad or funny television program, suggesting that the effect cannot be easily explained by an effect of the distracting activity on mood (Mittal et al., 2011). One recent study found no relationship between meal memory and later snacking, but in this study (unlike in previous studies) participants were also able to consume more when distracted because the amount of food eaten was not controlled and so this complicates interpretation of the results (Francis et al., 2017). Two studies have manipulated the level of distraction during consumption and assessed effects on subsequent intake. In the first study, the level of engagement with a concurrent computer task was manipulated using a financial incentive: in one condition participants were rewarded with a small amount of money if they played the game well (high engagement condition) and in another comparable condition no financial was offered (low engagement condition). Both distraction conditions were compared with a “no computer playing” control condition. Recall for the serial order of lunch items and memory vividness of the lunch was reduced and subsequent consumption was greater in the high engagement compared to the control condition with the low engagement condition in between (Higgs, 2015). In the second study, participants were distracted by engaging in either a low or high perceptual load task while consuming a either a high or low calorie preload drink that was delivered directly into the mouth via an intra-oral infusion device (Morris et al., 2020). In the low perceptual load condition, ingestion of the high-calorie drink increased rated satiety and reduced consumption at a subsequent snack test, but in the high perceptual load condition participants did not reduce their satiety or intake after the high calorie load, suggesting that greater attentional load during consumption impaired later satiety responses. There was no effect of the attentional manipulation on memory for preload sensory characteristics (how creamy, sweet, and pleasant it was) and so further studies are required to assess the precise mechanisms underlying the effects of the attentional manipulation on satiety in this paradigm.
2.2.3. Enhancing meal memory encoding
The effect of enhancing meal memory encoding has been investigated across several studies but the picture that has emerged here is not clear. Four initial studies used a paradigm in which participants were directed to pay more attention to food as it is eaten with the idea that this should facilitate meal memory encoding and reduce later food intake (Higgs, 2015; Higgs & Donohoe, 2011; Robinson et al., 2014; Seguias & Tapper, 2018). Focusing on the sensory characteristics of food while eating (sometimes referred to as “mindful” or “attentive eating”) was associated with reduced intake at the next episode, but there was no clear evidence from these studies that the effect was related to enhanced memory of the previous eating episode. More recent investigations have found no effect of focusing on food as it is eaten on either subsequent consumption or meal memory (Tapper & Seguias, 2020; Whitelock, Gaglione, et al., 2019; Whitelock et al., 2018; Whitelock & Robinson, 2018).
Another study took a slightly different approach and attempted to enhance meal memory by asking participants to rehearse what was satisfying about the meal immediately after consumption and this study also failed to show an effect on memory or intake (Whitelock & Robinson, 2018). Two further studies manipulated the amount of oral processing of food as a means of enhancing attention to and sensory processing of food as it is eaten (Higgs & Jones, 2013; Hinton et al., 2020). Higgs and Jones reported that prolonged chewing of food at lunch led to reduced later snacking but was not related to remembered vividness of the meal (Higgs & Jones, 2013). Hinton and colleagues found that a slower eating rate increased rated fullness during the inter-meal interval and resulted in participants recalling that they ate a larger portion than did participants who ate at a faster eating rate, but there was no effect of lunch eating rate on subsequent snack intake (Hinton et al., 2020). Taken together, the data do not provide support for a clear relationship between slower eating, meal memory, and satiety.
The evidence that distraction during eating increases later intake and impairs meal memory appears robust, but the evidence that eating more “mindfully” decreases later intake and increases memory is not consistent. One reason for this might be that it is difficult to enhance memory for recently eaten food at the time of encoding due to ceiling effects. It has been argued that memory for eating may be more accurately remembered than other kinds of memories because of the importance of eating for survival (Seitz, Blaisdell, Polack, et al., 2019). In support of this idea, participants were much better at remembering the number of times they ate a candy than they were at remembering the number of times they placed a candy or a non-food object from a bowl to a another container (Seitz et al., 2021). Therefore, it may be easier to demonstrate that distracting participants during eating worsens memory of eating and leads to greater consumption than it is to demonstrate that enhancing memory of eating reduces later intake. Rather than attempting to enhance encoding, a more effective approach to studying the influence of episodic memory on later appetite might be to induce a false memory of intake. Brunstrom and colleagues developed a novel paradigm that created a mismatch between what participants actually ate and what they perceived they ate using a refilling soup bowl (Brunstrom et al., 2012). This set up dissociated the independent effects of episodic memory and gastric feedback on appetite: participants in one condition ate 300 ml of soup but were led to believe that they had consumed 500 ml of soup. Immediately after eating, hunger ratings were related to the physical amount that had been consumed, but later in the post meal period hunger was predicted by the remembered rather than the actual portion size. The effect of altering memory in this way on later food intake has yet to be investigated, but there is potential to use this approach to further investigate the episodic food memory in appetite.
2.3. Future directions
An important overriding question in relation to the role of memory for recent eating in appetite that has yet to be answered relates to the type of memory that is involved. Research to date suggests that the memory is specific to aspects of the most recent meal, which points to the importance of episodic-like memory. Episodic memories contain contextual information about an experienced event, such as what happened, and where and when it happened (Clayton & Dickinson, 1998). It is unclear at present what aspects of the most recent eating event that are encoded are used to inform later decisions about eating. Evidence from of the lack of effectiveness of guided recall of the taste and texture of the food, where it was eaten and with whom (Szypula et al., 2020) suggests that these elements may less of a role than knowledge of the satiating effects of the food and how much of it was consumed. There is some evidence that higher calorie foods are remembered better than lower calorie foods but this does not extend to better memory for the context in which those foods were consumed (Seitz, Blaisdell, & Tomiyama, 2019). Further work could be directed at guiding recall to specific aspects of the consumption experience such as the internal sensations felt by the participants.
The methods that have been used to assess accuracy or quality of memory for recent eating will need to be refined to provide a more definitive test of whether the experimental manipulations aimed at altering meal memory indeed do so and the extent to which any effects on appetite are mediated by memory processes. Many different measures have been used to date, including asking participants to rate the vividness of their memory, recall of the number of and serial order of items consumed, the number of episodic details, and remembered portion size. A few studies have tested whether meal memory manipulations affect later intake because they impair or enhance episodic memory for the meal (i.e., have tested for mediation effects), but the results have not been consistent. It is possible that these measures do not tap into the aspects of memory that are affected by meal memory manipulation and that underlie the effects on subsequent consumption. For example, if memory for internal sensation is important, then it would be necessary to measure more directly how accurately or vividly participants recall these sensations. Future studies should test for mediation effects and employ a range of measures to assess different aspects of meal memory.
Little is currently known about how individual differences play into the effects of memory for recent eating on appetite. Many of the studies to date have recruited homogenous populations such as women low in dietary restraint and inhibition, although both men and women have been found to reduce snacking after meal recall (Higgs, Williamson, & Attwood, 2008). Higgs and colleagues reported that participants who score high on a measure of dietary disinhibition (uncontrolled eating) did not reduce intake after meal recall (Higgs, Williamson, & Attwood, 2008), but Szypula and colleagues found that the inhibitory effect of meal recall on snack intake was not moderated by either dietary restraint or disinhibition (Szypula et al., 2020). They also found episodic memory ability was not a moderator of the meal recall effect, but the memory task that was used to categorize participants as high or low in memory ability may not have measured differences in the kind of memory that modulates the meal-recall effect. Studies with larger more diverse samples are required to confirm whether meal recall suppresses eating regardless of individual differences in memory ability, eating behavior traits and sex.
2.4. Interim Summary
There is converging evidence from studies using a range of methodological approaches that memory for recent eating plays a role in human appetite.
Studies of amnesic patients have shown that being unable to remember a recent eating episode is associated with overeating.
Experimental studies in healthy volunteers have found that impairing meal memory formation (e.g., via distraction during eating) is associated with increased later food intake whereas enhancing memory for recent eating (e.g., by recalling the most recent eating episode) decreases subsequent intake.
3. DORSAL HIPPOCAMPAL NEURONS LIKELY MEDIATE THE INHIBITORY EFFECTS OF MEAL-RELATED MEMORY ON FUTURE EATING BEHAVIOR IN RODENTS
It is well established that the hippocampus plays a pivotal role in memory in primates and rodents (Murray et al., 2018). In rodents, the hippocampus can be divided along the septo-temporal plane into dorsal (dHC) and ventral (vHC) regions (in primates posterior and anterior, respectively). dHC and vHC have different anatomical connections, cellular and circuit properties and patterns of gene expression that likely contribute to the different functions that they serve (Barkus et al., 2010; Bienkowski et al., 2018; Dong et al., 2009; Fanselow & Dong, 2010; Moser & Moser, 1998; Strange et al., 2014; Thompson et al., 2008). dHC appears to be primarily responsible for the cognitive functions associated with the hippocampus, namely spatial and episodic-like memories (Barbosa et al., 2012; Drieskens et al., 2017; Hunsaker et al., 2008; Panoz-Brown et al., 2018; Zhou et al., 2012). Decades of research using food-motivated tasks has shown that manipulations that interfere with dHC function impair food-related memory, such as the location of food sources (Layfield et al., 2020), the conditions under which food will be available (Crystal et al., 2013) and when food is expected to arrive (Tam et al., 2015).
Given the critical role of dHC in mediating episodic memories, including those involving food, research conducted by the Parent group has focused on testing the hypothesis that dHC neurons mediate the inhibitory effects of ingestion-related memory on future intake observed in humans (Reviewed in Section 2.2). The strategy used to test this hypothesis has been influenced substantially by an extensive body of research showing that manipulations given immediately after a learning event target the consolidation of the memory of that event. This evidence indicates that memory traces are initially labile and gain stability and permanence through consolidation, and that peripheral or central treatments given immediately after learning can enhance or impair the consolidation of the memory of that event (Izquierdo & McGaugh, 2000; McGaugh, 2000). Several studies have used posttraining manipulations to show that dHC neurons are involved in memory consolidation. Specifically, temporary disruption of dHC function during the period immediately following learning impairs retention performance when memory is tested later after the effects of the manipulations have worn off (Bonini et al., 2003; Jerusalinsky et al., 1992).
3.1. Postmeal dHC inhibition increases future intake
Inspired by the impact of posttraining interventions on memory consolidation, Parent and colleagues determined whether postmeal inhibition of dHC neurons, which would be expected to impair the consolidation of the memory of that meal, would increase subsequent intake. Initially, they tested the effects of dHC infusions of the GABAA agonist muscimol on eating behavior because dHC muscimol infusions reversibly hyperpolarize principal glutamatergic neurons (Majchrzak & Di Scala, 2000; Rombo et al., 2016) that play a central role in dHC-dependent memory (Baker & Kim, 2002; Izquierdo et al., 2008; Morris, 2013; Zamorano et al., 2018). Importantly, previous research has shown that posttraining dHC infusions of muscimol impair future retention performance, indicating that dHC is critical for the consolidation of the memory of that experience (Izquierdo et al., 1992; Oliveira et al., 2010). In support of a memory consolidation hypothesis, intra-dHC infusions of muscimol administered immediately following the end of a sucrose meal accelerates the initiation of the next meal, doubles the amount rats consume during that second meal, and increases the number of meals ingested during the period following the injection (Henderson et al., 2013).
This novel use of a postmeal treatment appears to be the first to show that dHC neurons inhibit future energy intake and suggests that dHC glutamatergic neurons are critical, although the effects of muscimol could include dHC interneurons that also express GABAA receptors (Glykys et al., 2007; Mann & Mody, 2010; Semyanov et al., 2003). An additional limitation of these results is that muscimol inhibits neural activity for several hours (Majchrzak & Di Scala, 2000; Martin, 1991; Martin & Ghez, 1999). Thus, it is not clear whether these postmeal manipulations increase intake by disrupting memory-based processes during the early postprandial period versus effects on dHC function during consumption of the second and subsequent sucrose meals. To resolve this, Parent and colleagues used an activity-guided optogenetic approach that permits neural inhibition with remarkable temporal precision and used a construct containing a calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II (CaMKII)α promoter that limits opsin expression to glutamatergic neurons while sparing inhibitory neurons, fibers of passage, and glia (Butler et al., 2016; Han & Boyden, 2007; Kohl et al., 2011; Stark et al., 2013; Stark et al., 2014; Weitz et al., 2015; Yizhar et al., 2011). They first confirmed that optogenetic inhibition of dHC glutamatergic neurons for 10 min inhibits neuronal firing in a temporally specific and steady manner, and critically, that neuronal activity returns immediately to baseline as soon as the photoinhibition is terminated (Hannapel et al., 2019). These temporal and anatomical features permitted the specific inhibition of dHC glutamatergic neurons for 10 min that was restricted to the period either before, during or after ingestion, and importantly, allowed for the measurement of ingestive behavior at a later time when the neurons were no longer inhibited. As in the case of the muscimol infusions, photoinhibition given for 10 min after the end of a sucrose or chow meal promoted the initiation of the next meal and augmented the amount consumed during the next meal when the neurons were no longer inhibited (Hannapel et al., 2019). The specificity of this effect was illustrated by the finding that inhibition given before or during the first meal did not affect the amount consumed during the first meal or the next one. The finding that inhibition given during intake did not affect the amount eaten during that meal is reminiscent of the findings in humans showing that disrupting episodic meal-related memory has a bigger effect on intake during the next eating episode than on intake of the meal being remembered (Robinson, Aveyard, et al., 2013).
An important characteristic of the effects of posttraining manipulations on memory is that the effects are time-dependent, such that the efficacy of posttraining treatments diminishes as the learning-treatment interval increases (McGaugh, 1966; McGaugh & Izquierdo, 2000). The period in which posttraining manipulations effectively influence future retention performance delineates the consolidation window for that particular experience. For example, temporarily inactivating dHC with tetrodotoxin (TTX) immediately after inhibitory avoidance training impairs memory tested 48 hours after acquisition; in contrast, dHC infusions of TTX given 6 hours after training have no effect (Lorenzini et al., 1996). Similarly, blocking dHC AMPA, NMDA, or metabotropic glutamate receptors after learning impairs retention in a time-dependent manner (Bonini et al., 2003; Jerusalinsky et al., 1992). To test the hypothesis that dHC neurons influence future intake through a process that requires meal-related memory consolidation, Parent and colleagues determined whether the effects of postmeal dHC photoinhibition are also time-dependent. Importantly, they showed that inactivation given 5 minutes after the end of a meal increases the amount consumed during the next bout, whereas inactivation given 80 minutes later does not impact subsequent intake (Briggs et al., 2021).
Thus, like posttraining manipulations, the effects of postmeal manipulations on future intake are time-dependent, which is consistent with the hypothesis that postmeal manipulations disrupt the consolidation of the memory of the meal. An alternative interpretation is that the early postprandial period is crucial because of the timing of interoceptive visceral satiety cues generated by the meal (Page et al., 2012; Waise et al., 2018). This is possible given that disturbances in hippocampal function impair the sensation and/or processing of ingestion-related interoceptive cues (reviewed in Sections 2 and 4). However, the possibility that hippocampal neural activity is critical during the early postprandial period because that is when critical food-related postingestive cues are available is not supported by the finding that dHC inactivation given after the completion of a saccharin meal promotes the initiation of the next saccharin meal and increases the amount consumed during that meal (Hannapel et al., 2019), and that the effects of inactivation given after the end of a saccharin meal are time-dependent (Briggs et al., 2021). In these experiments, animals are given limited exposure to saccharin to reduce the likelihood of any conditioned responses to the taste. Under such conditions, saccharin has minimal postingestive consequences (Foletto et al., 2016; Mook et al., 1980; Renwick, 1985, 1986; Sclafani & Nissenbaum, 1985) and unlike sucrose, saccharin meal size and meal frequency are controlled primarily by oropharyngeal satiety (Kushner & Mook, 1984; Mook et al., 1980; Mook et al., 1981; Renwick, 1985, 1986; Sclafani & Nissenbaum, 1985). Nonetheless, the possibility remains that dHC inhibition disrupts saccharin intake by interfering with the processing of any mechanical stimulation produced by the saccharin solution in the gut (Waise et al., 2018).
3.2. Ingestion increases biomarkers of synaptic plasticity in dHC
Collectively, the findings reviewed in the previous section demonstrate that dHC glutamatergic neural activity during the early postprandial period, when the memory of a meal is likely undergoing consolidation, is critical for dHC inhibition of future intake. If dHC neurons limit intake through a process that requires meal-related memory, then ingestion should increase events necessary for synaptic plasticity in dHC glutamatergic neurons during the early postprandial period because plasticity in these synapses is critical for memory formation (Ashby et al., 2021; Bartsch & Wulff, 2015). The most common forms of synaptic plasticity depend on glutamate NMDA receptor (NMDAR)-dependent increases in intracellular calcium that activate proteins and stimulate mRNA synthesis and protein translation that act collectively to increase glutamatergic AMPA receptor (AMPAR) function in postsynaptic cells, thereby increasing glutamatergic signaling and synaptic strength (Bengtson & Bading, 2012; Clark et al., 2015; Czerniawski et al., 2012; Kent et al., 2007; Maggio et al., 2015; Portero-Tresserra et al., 2014; Xia & Storm, 2012; Zhu et al., 2014). Posttranslational modifications of AMPAR function occur early in the induction of synaptic plasticity. For instance, long-term potentiation (LTP)-inducing stimuli and learning increase phosphorylation of serine 831 (pSer831) residues on AMPARs (Whitlock et al., 2006). Elevations in AMPAR pSer831, in turn, increase synaptic strength by augmenting AMPAR conductance and by promoting insertion of AMPARs in postsynaptic spines (Derkach et al., 1999; Hu et al., 2007; Kristensen et al., 2011). In support of the prediction that ingestion induces synaptic plasticity in dHC glutamatergic neurons, Parent and colleagues showed that consuming sucrose increases AMPAR pSer831 in dHC neurons (Ross et al., 2019), and of note, that the magnitude of the increase is comparable to that produced by learning (Whitlock et al., 2006). Also, as in the case of learning (Whitlock et al., 2006), the effect is selective such that ingestion activates pSer831 but not pSer845 (Ross et al., 2019). Learning events also stimulate the expression of activity-regulated cytoskeleton-associated protein (Arc) mRNA in dHC glutamatergic neurons (Descalzi et al., 2019; Guzowski, McNaughton, et al., 2001; Guzowski, Setlow, et al., 2001; Hudgins & Otto, 2019; Tripathi et al., 2020). Arc is considered a master regulator of synaptic plasticity that is downstream of many molecular signaling pathways, and critically, it is necessary for many types of synaptic plasticity and for memory consolidation (Bramham et al., 2008; Korb & Finkbeiner, 2011; Shepherd & Bear, 2011). As in the case of learning experiences, Parent and colleagues showed that ingestion of sucrose or saccharin solutions increase dHC Arc nuclear mRNA levels (Henderson et al., 2017; Henderson et al., 2016), which is significant because unlike other immediate early genes, Arc expression reflects synaptic plasticity rather than neuronal activity (Carpenter-Hyland et al., 2010; Fletcher et al., 2006; Guzowski et al., 2006). The Arc data support the hypothesis that consuming a meal induces glutamatergic-dependent synaptic plasticity in dHC given that Arc mRNA expression is NMDAR-dependent (Czerniawski et al., 2011; Link et al., 1995; Lyford et al., 1995; Steward et al., 1998) and is required for dHC-dependent synaptic plasticity and memory (Guzowski et al., 2000). Interestingly, chronic reductions in dHC NMDAR function interferes with the ability to form and remember an association between a taste and gastric discomfort, although whether the manipulation interferes with the ability to sense or process gastric discomfort is unclear (Chinnakkaruppan et al., 2014).
Evidence suggests that familiarity and repetition decrease dHC involvement in memory (Packard & McGaugh, 1996). Similarly, familiarity also decreases the ability of learning to increase dHC hippocampal expression of several molecules critical for memory, including Arc (Gardner et al., 2016; Guzowski et al., 2006; Guzowski, Setlow, et al., 2001; Kelly & Deadwyler, 2002, 2003), phosphorylated cAMP response element-binding protein (pCREB)(Moncada & Viola, 2006) and protein kinase M-ζ (Moncada & Viola, 2008). Thus, one prediction is that the ability of ingestion to activate the molecular processes underlying synaptic plasticity and memory formation should also decrease with repeated consumption in the same context. To test this prediction, rats were given access to a sucrose solution 10 min/day for 3, 5, or 10 days in the same context. The results showed that the amount of previous sucrose experience impacted the ability of sucrose ingestion to elevate pSer831 in dHC. Specifically, sucrose consumption increased dHC pSer831 in rats given 3 days of sucrose exposure, but not in rats given 5 or 10 days of exposure (Ross et al., 2019). As the amount of preceding sucrose experience increases there is a trend for a linear decrease in the amount of sucrose-induced dHC pSer831. This finding is consistent with previous findings showing that the ability of a stimulus to activate pSer831 depends on the history of synaptic activation (Lee et al., 2000) and that increased sucrose experience also appears to attenuate sucrose-induced increases in dHC Arc expression (Henderson et al., 2016). Together, the Arc and pSer831 results suggest that repeated consumption of the same meal may decrease the mnemonic demands associated with that meal, the amount of neural activity required to remember that meal and/or diminish the role of the dHC in remembering the last meal when the energy source is not novel and consumed habitually in the same context.
3.3. Future directions
Studies are needed to determine whether postmeal manipulations that increase subsequent intake do indeed impair memory of the preceding meal. The comparable effects of training and ingestion on dHC expression of biomarkers of synaptic plasticity and memory suggest that increased dHC pSer831 and Arc expression are molecular events underlying the memory of a meal. Additional experiments are needed, however, to determine whether these events are necessary for dHC control of future intake. The findings with saccharin may help identify the variables that influence the ability of ingestion to induced synaptic plasticity and by extrapolation, the memory of a meal. Specifically, the finding that saccharin ingestion induces Arc expression in dHC neurons (Henderson et al., 2016) and that inhibition of these neurons during the early postprandial period increases future saccharin intake (Briggs et al., 2021; Hannapel et al., 2019) suggest that the act of ingestion, orosensation, and/or mechanical stimulation of the gut are sufficient to engage meal-related synaptic plasticity and that calories are not necessary. Future experiments are needed to further identify what is being remembered in a meal, whether manipulations that augment dHC function decrease subsequent intake and the neural circuitry through which dHC neurons control ingestive behavior. To date, evidence suggests that dHC glutamatergic neurons limit intake via effects on neurons in the septal area (Azevedo et al., 2019), but whether these neurons are the same ones that are critical during the postprandial period remains to be determined. Another critical question is how cues associated with energy status interact with neural mechanisms involved in memory to control eating, which is the subject of the next section.
3.4. Interim Summary
dHC glutamatergic neurons appear to mediate the ability of meal-related memory to inhibit later intake observed in humans.
Ingestion activates molecular processes critical for plasticity and memory in dHC during the early postprandial period.
Neural activity during the early postprandial period is critical for dHC glutamatergic neurons to inhibit future intake.
dHC neurons limit homeostatic (i.e., intake of standard chow), hedonic (i.e., sucrose) and non-caloric (i.e., saccharin) feeding.
4. ENDOCRINE AND NEUROPEPTIDE SYSTEMS ACT IN THE VENTRAL HIPPOCAMPUS TO REGULATE LEARNED ASPECTS OF EATING BEHAVIOR
The hippocampus is critical for the processing of interoceptive cues that inform about energy status (e.g., “hunger” and “satiety” cues). This notion is supported by data discussed above (see Section 2.1) in which bilateral damage to the hippocampus in humans is associated with disrupted awareness of hunger and satiety status. A similar phenomenon is observed in rodents with selective lesions to the hippocampus, based on experiments utilizing the deprivation intensity discrimination procedure. In this procedure, rats are trained to discriminate between a high (e.g., 24 hours) and a low (e.g., 1 hour) level of food restriction as interoceptive predictive stimuli for either positive reinforcement or positive punishment (sucrose pellets or foot shock, respectively). While intact rats are capable of learning the deprivation intensity discrimination task, rats with selective hippocampal lesions are impaired in either acquisition of this type of learning (Davidson & Jarrard, 1993; Hock & Bunsey, 1998) or in retention of the discrimination when lesions occur after the acquisition phase (Davidson et al., 2010). Moreover, similar to patients with amnesia who accept and consume multiple consecutive meals, selective hippocampal lesions in rats leads to increased spontaneous meal frequency (Clifton et al., 1998), an outcome likely contributing to an overall increase in caloric intake and body weight gain (Davidson et al., 2009). Thus, findings from both humans and experimental rodent models indicate that the hippocampus is functionally linked with the processing of interoceptive cues that provide information about energy status.
An important yet unanswered question is whether the hippocampus is required for the sensory detection of interoceptive energy status signals vs. the interpretation of these cues via integration with mnemonic processes. The latter is more probable, as hindbrain processing alone is sufficient for basic satiation responses (based in part on data from the decerebrate rat model) (Grill & Hayes, 2012), and the hypothalamus has long been regarded as a key region for nutrient sensing and energy balance control (Timper & Bruning, 2017). Further, that the hippocampus interprets interoceptive cues based on previous learned experiences is consistent with various models of hippocampal function, including Richard Morris’s model that the hippocampus resolves the “predictable ambiguity” of stimuli (Morris, 2007). Regardless of whether or not the hippocampus is required for the sensory detection of energy status cues, which again is unlikely, it is nevertheless clear that the hippocampus serves a critical role in processing cues that fluctuate based on energy status to appropriately guide behavioral outcomes, including food seeking and eating. The locus of these interoceptive hunger and satiety signals and the precise signaling mechanisms through which they communicate with hippocampal neurons are incompletely understood.
Here, we argue that interoceptive energy status-relevant cues are communicated to the hippocampus, at least in part, via endocrine and neuropeptide systems that fluctuate either directly or indirectly with changes in energetic status. Hippocampal neurons indeed express receptors for a number of such signals, including three systems that we discuss in more depth below: ghrelin, leptin, and glucagon-like peptide-1 (GLP-1). Evidence is reviewed that these peptide systems communicate with hippocampal neurons, predominantly in the rodent vHC, to either directly or indirectly impact food-motivated behavior and/or eating behavior. Building off models proposed by Davidson, Kanoski, and colleagues (Davidson et al., 2019; Davidson et al., 2005; Kanoski & Grill, 2017), the collective data support a framework through which these endocrine and neuropeptide signals resolve the ambiguity of food-associated cues to influence various aspects of eating, including both appetitive responses and meal size control. While there are various energy balance-relevant peptide systems with receptor expression in the hippocampus that have been shown to be relevant for plasticity and memory function, such as insulin (Ferrario & Reagan, 2018), the subsequent subsections highlight systems where direct relevance to eating and/or food-motivated behaviors have been extensively investigated.
4.1. Ghrelin
Ghrelin is a stomach-derived 28-amino acid hormone that increases appetite and food intake via action on its seven transmembrane G-protein-coupled receptor, the type 1a growth hormone secretagogue receptor (GHSR1a) (Howard et al., 1996; Sun et al., 2004). Ghrelin levels are elevated during energy restriction and prior to anticipated eating (Blum et al., 2009; Davis et al., 2011; Drazen et al., 2006; Wren et al., 2001) and decrease following a meal (Ariyasu et al., 2001; Cummings et al., 2001; Nass et al., 2008). Importantly, the rise in ghrelin levels prior to anticipated eating exceeds that induced by energy restriction alone (Drazen et al., 2006), suggesting that its release is potently influenced by learned and other cognitive factors. Ghrelin’s impact on eating behavior is multifaceted, such that it influences both learned aspects of eating (e.g., conditioned appetitive responses; reviewed in (Hsu et al., 2016) as well as meal size control by interacting with vagally-mediated within-meal satiation signaling (Cabral et al., 2017; Cao et al., 2016; Levin et al., 2006; Suarez et al., 2020). GHSR1a is expressed in hippocampal neurons with particularly dense expression observed in vHC (Mani et al., 2014). Direct application of ghrelin to the vHC in rats approximately doubles food intake levels a few hours after injections by increasing both meal frequency and size, whereas ghrelin delivered to the dHC has no effect (Kanoski et al., 2013).
A role for vHC ghrelin signaling in learned aspects of eating behavior is directly supported by two key results. First, vHC ghrelin administration increases meal frequency in rats in response to auditory cues that were previously associated with palatable food access, but not in response to auditory cues not associated with food access (Kanoski et al., 2013). Second, while GHSR blockade in the vHC has no effect on consumption of either chow or a high-fat diet under free-feeding conditions, vHC GHSR blockade significantly reduces chow intake in meal-entrained rats when the antagonist is delivered immediately before an anticipated meal (Hsu et al., 2015). Both results are consistent with a framework in which vHC ghrelin signaling resolves the ambiguity of food-associated cues to stimulate eating; the former result exemplifying a response to external discrete cues, the latter a response based on temporal circadian cues. A more recent report builds on this work by revealing that vHC GHSR signaling increases meal size by diminishing the efficacy of various within-meal satiation signals via a descending hypothalamic (orexin neurons in the lateral hypothalamic area) to hindbrain (laterodorsal tegmental nucleus) pathway. This report also revealed that vHC ghrelin injections produce an interoceptive state that generalizes to 24-hour food restriction (in otherwise nonrestricted rats) in the deprivation intensity discrimination procedure described above (Suarez et al., 2020). This latter outcome is important, as it suggests that the extent to which a high level of food restriction can disambiguate food cues by increasing their capacity to drive an appetitive response may be based, in part, on GHSR1a action in the vHC. Collectively, these results are consistent with a model in which vHC ghrelin signaling stimulates both meal frequency and meal size by resolving the ambiguity of both external and internal food-associated cues; that is, by increasing the potency of various cues to contribute to either the initiation or continuation of eating.
4.2. Leptin
Leptin is a hormone produced principally from white adipocytes (Zhang et al., 1994). Leptin receptor (LepRb) signaling potently reduces food intake and body weight; whereas, downregulation of leptin signaling through mutation of leptin or LepRb results in hyperphagia and extreme obesity in both humans and rodents (Leshan et al., 2006). While the effects of CNS LepRb signaling on energy balance have largely focused on the hypothalamic arcuate nucleus (Myers et al., 2009); the caudal brainstem (Grill & Hayes, 2012; Hayes et al., 2010; Kanoski et al., 2012), and ventral tegmental area mesoaccumbens dopamine signaling (Fulton et al., 2006; Hommel et al., 2006), LepRb is also robustly expressed in the hippocampus (Scott et al., 2009). In opposition to ghrelin effects discussed above, activation of either dHC or vHC LepRbs produces modest but significant reductions in food intake 24 hours after injections, with more potent effects observed following vHC than dHC delivery (Kanoski et al., 2011).
In addition to reducing consumption of chow during free-feeding conditions, vHC leptin signaling also reduces conditioned appetitive behaviors. For example, vHC leptin administration reduces food seeking in an environment that has been associated with consuming a palatable meal (expression of conditioned place preference [CPP]). Further, vHC LepRb activation reduces the latency to obtain palatable food in an operant runway procedure and blocks memory consolidation for the spatial location of food (Kanoski et al., 2011). One interpretation of these results that is consistent with the framework introduced above is that hippocampal leptin signaling modulates memory formation and retrieval in a manner that actively reduces the saliency of food-related features in the environment in favor of nonfood features, resulting in reduced appetitive responding in the presence of environmental food-associated cues. In this regard, when leptin levels are endogenously high (during energy balance or surplus), this system may be signaling to the vHC (and likely other regions) to resolve the ambiguity of food cues by suppressing the memory of the reinforcement associated with these cues (eating), which in turn reduces food-motivated behavior in response to these cues.
4.3. GLP-1
GLP-1 is a hormone produced in the distal small intestines that also functions as a neuropeptide produced in the caudal brainstem. Circulating peripheral levels of GLP-1 increase during eating (Holst, 2007) and the caudal brainstem GLP-1 neurons are engaged by various factors, including stomach distension (Vrang et al., 2003), consuming food to satiation (Kreisler & Rinaman, 2016), and following either interoceptive or psychogenic stress (Holt et al., 2019; Rinaman, 1999). While both the peripheral and central GLP-1 systems are associated with reductions in energy intake, recent evidence indicates that these two populations of GLP-1-producing cells influence eating via independent mechanisms (Brierley et al., 2021). GLP-1Rs are expressed throughout the neuraxis and activation of central GLP-1Rs reduces food intake when agonists are applied to any one of various CNS regions, including (but not limited to) the hypothalamus (Lopez-Ferreras et al., 2018; McMahon & Wellman, 1998; Schick et al., 2003), caudal brainstem (Fortin et al., 2020; Hayes et al., 2011; Hayes et al., 2008; Kinzig et al., 2002), and mesolimbic dopaminergic circuitry (Alhadeff et al., 2012; Dickson et al., 2012; Dossat et al., 2011). GLP-1Rs are also expressed in hippocampal neurons, particularly in the vHC subregion (Merchenthaler et al., 1999). Given that GLP-1 neurons do not project to the hippocampus, the source of GLP-1 signaling to hippocampal neurons is unknown, either coming from periphery to brain signaling (blood-brain barrier transport), or from volume transmission signaling from GLP-1-producing brainstem neurons, likely via humoral-like release into the cerebrospinal fluid (Hsu et al., 2015; Noble et al., 2018). Regardless of the endogenous signaling pathway, activation of vHC GLP-1Rs potently reduces food intake in rats, with ~40–50% reduction in caloric intake 24 hours after injections based on reduced meal size with no effect on meal frequency (Hsu et al., 2015). These intake-reducing effects are physiologically relevant to normal eating, as blockade of vHC GLP-1Rs increases food intake under free-feeding conditions.
GLP-1 signaling also reduces the expression of learned food-motivated appetitive responses. First, vHC GLP-1R agonism reduces motivated lever-press responding for palatable food in a progressive ratio (PR) reinforcement schedule in which each subsequent reinforcer earned requires a higher number of lever presses than the previous reinforcer (Hsu et al., 2015). Second, vHC GLP-1R agonism (via downstream medial prefrontal cortex signaling) reduces impulsive operant responses for palatable food in the differential reinforcement of low rates of responding test (DRL), in which animals must refrain from pressing a lever for a subsequent reinforcer for at least 20 seconds to maximize the number of pellets earned in a session (Hsu et al., 2018). The rats were trained in DRL under ad libitum fed conditions, but were tested following 24-hour food restriction. Interestingly, increased vHC GLP-1R activation produced levels of impulsivity that mirrored levels observed at the end of training under food-sated conditions. This suggests that, in contrast to vHC ghrelin signaling replicating the effects of 24-hour food restriction in the deprivation intensity discrimination procedure, vHC GLP-1 signaling is replicating the effects of satiety on impulsive responding for food in the DRL task.
While vHC GLP-1 signaling reduced appetitive responses for palatable food in both the PR and DRL procedures, unlike vHC LepRb signaling, activation of GLP-1R in the vHC has no effect on CPP expression for a location previously associated with palatable food access. It is interesting to note that these former two tests (PR and DRL) involve testing under conditions that allow for periodic food consumption (albeit below levels to induce satiation). The CPP test, on the other hand, occurs under conditions without food access or consumption. Given that GLP-1 release, at least from the periphery, is prandial in nature (occurs during eating), it is tempting to speculate that vHC GLP-1 signaling, similar to vHC leptin signaling, reduces the effectiveness of conditioned food cues to promote appetitive responses, but unlike with leptin signaling, this capacity requires some nutrient consumption to actually occur. Within the framework of the model discussed within this section, the capacity of vHC GLP-1 signaling to reduce the ambiguity of food-associated cues (towards reducing their efficacy to promote further appetitive responses or eating) may interact with other signaling systems whose levels fluctuate during eating. One candidate system is insulin, as insulin receptors are robustly expressed in hippocampal neurons (Grillo et al., 2019), and GLP-1 signaling in the periphery promotes glucose-dependent insulin release from the pancreas (Holst, 2007).
4.4. Concluding framework and future directions
Data discussed above from rodent models identify a prominent role for vHC processing of energy balance-relevant endocrine and neuropeptide systems to influence both food-motivated responses as well as caloric consumption. Results were reviewed from experiments targeting either orexigenic (ghrelin) or anorexigenic (leptin, GLP-1) systems. Given the evidence reviewed herein and the findings that both humans and rodents with hippocampal damage are impaired in interpreting and utilizing hunger and satiety cues to appropriately guide behavior, we propose that the hippocampus, particularly the vHC subregion, processes energy balance-relevant cues, in part, through endocrine and neuropeptide ligand-receptor binding mechanisms. In some cases, activation of these systems appears to replicate the effects of energy restriction (vHC ghrelin signaling in the deprivation intensity discrimination task) or satiety (vHC GLP-1 signaling in the DRL impulsivity test) on learned food-motivated responses. We posit that these hippocampal signaling pathways, much like hunger and satiety, function to disambiguate food-associated cues to bias behavior towards (ghrelin) or away from (leptin, GLP-1) food-directed behavior and/or continued consumption.
We conclude this section by highlighting two underdeveloped areas for future investigation: [1] The examination of potential interactions between different peptide systems acting in hippocampal neurons to modulate food-directed responses. In addition to the possible GLP-1 and insulin hippocampus signaling interaction alluded to above, recent results show that vHC ghrelin injections at doses subthreshold for intake effects alone attenuate the food intake-reducing effects of peripheral GLP-1 analog administration (Suarez et al., 2020), suggesting possible opposing interactions between these systems. [2] Investigation into the role of feeding-relevant hypothalamic neuropeptide systems on hippocampal-mediated processing of energy status information. Indeed, the hypothalamic neuropeptide melanin-concentrating hormone (MCH) is influenced by energy status and acts in the vHC to increase impulsive responding for palatable food (without affecting consumption under free-feeding conditions) (Noble et al., 2019). Hippocampal neurons also express receptors for the hypothalamic neuropeptides, orexin (orexigenic) and oxytocin (anorexigenic), whose role in the hippocampus has been explored with regards to memory (Mavanji et al., 2017) and social behaviors (Lin et al., 2018), respectively, but not for potential roles in eating-relevant behaviors.
4.5. Interim Summary
Hippocampal lesions in rodents disrupts discrimination learning based on varying hunger and satiety states, suggesting that the hippocampus is functionally linked with the processing of interoceptive energy status cues.
Emerging evidence indicates that energy status-relevant cues are communicated to the hippocampus via endocrine and neuropeptide systems that fluctuate with changes in energetic status.
The vHC is particularly sensitive to energy status-relevant peptide signals.
Both orexigenic (ghrelin) and anorexigenic (leptin, glucagon-like peptide 1) peptide systems act in the vHC to modulate both eating and conditioned food-motivated behaviors.
5. OBESITY IS ASSOCIATED WITH IMPAIRED MEMORY AND DEFICITS IN MEMORY-RELATED NEURAL ACTIVITY IN HUMANS
Obesity is an epidemic in the Western world and is linked to overconsumption of energy dense food (Ng et al., 2014; Stevens et al., 2012). The role of learning and memory in the regulation of consumption is of significant import when placed in the context of mounting evidence that obesity is associated with impairments in cognition (Farruggia & Small, 2019; Smith et al., 2011). Of particular relevance is a consistent finding of reduced memory ability in individuals who are obese (Cheke et al., 2017; Cheke et al., 2016; Coppin et al., 2014; Dye et al., 2017; Loprinzi & Frith, 2018; Nguyen et al., 2014; Prickett et al., 2015; Spitznagel et al., 2015). Overweight and obesity are linked with memory deficits across the lifespan, with evidence for the association in children and adolescents (Bozkurt et al., 2017; Maayan et al., 2011), young, otherwise healthy adults (Cheke et al., 2016), and older age (Benito-León et al., 2013; Beydoun et al., 2008). In a relatively small (N=50) study of nondiabetic 18–35 year olds, Cheke and colleagues (2016) investigated whether performance on item, spatial, temporal or associative “what-where-when” memory varied as a function of BMI. They found a significant negative association between BMI and performance on all tasks, with no interaction between BMI and number of items to be remembered (memory difficulty) or reaction time. Individuals with higher BMI made a comparable number of errors in terms of “total forgetting” or “spatial imprecision” but differed significantly from those with lower BMI in terms of “binding” errors, in which correct elements were incorrectly combined into item-location-time conjunctions. These findings suggest that BMI was predictive of lower memory ability specifically, rather than of general deficits (which might be expected to produced increasing deficits as difficulty increases) or reduced motivation or attention (which might be expected to alter reaction times). One advantage of this study is that it provides some predictions about whether we might expect obesity to be associated with impaired performance in all aspects of episodic memory, (and therefore, likely consistent findings across different types of memory tasks (Cheke & Clayton, 2013, 2015) or whether the deficits may be more specific to particular memory demands. Based on Cheke and colleagues’ findings, it might be predicted that all memory tests (emphasizing item, spatial or temporal information) would be impaired, but that there may be a particular deficit in associative memory. However, this study has been criticized because the effect of BMI on associative memory dropped below significance once multiple demographic factors (age, sex, education) were controlled for, although this is hard to interpret given the limited power to control for multiple factors.
A further criticism of studies such as Cheke and colleagues (2016) pertains to the reliance on body mass index (BMI) as its measure of obesity. BMI is a standard metric, useful both because it is calculated from easy to obtain measures (height and weight) and because it has well defined cut-offs: Somebody is “obese” if they have a BMI above 30. However, BMI has a number of issues (Price et al., 2006; Stevens et al., 2008). In particular, this measure makes an equivalence between bodyweight and bodyfat, which can lead to multiple confounds (e.g., from muscle mass and pregnancy). Studies using only BMI also do not allow for exploration of which obesity-related factor is linked with memory deficits. Multiple conditions that are highly comorbid with obesity have the potential to impact cognitive function. In a review of multiple studies, (Farruggia & Small, 2019) conclude that the evidence supports both adiposity and insulin resistance, which is highly comorbid with obesity (Khaodhiar et al., 1999), having independent (but potentially interacting) influences.
A small number of studies have attempted to disentangle the relative influence of different obesity-related factors on memory and cognition. Hartanto and Yong (2018) investigated whether obesity as measured by BMI was a predictor of episodic memory performance when multiple factors were taken into account, and how this relationship compared against another measure of obesity- waist-hip ratio (WHR). In a sample of 4,206 participants from the MIDUS II cohort (Ryff & Lachman, 2010), they assessed BMI and WHR alongside a number of other variables relating to demographics, health and personality. They found that BMI was significantly negatively related to episodic memory (word list) performance when accounting for demographics, but not when health variables (including hypertension, diabetes and stroke) were included in the model. In contrast, WHR was negatively associated with memory performance regardless of which factors were included, suggesting that this measure is a more reliable predictor. Given that WHR is a more direct measure of visceral fat than overall BMI, this may speak to the relative influence of different types of bodyfat on cognitive function. A greater influence of visceral as compared to general or peripheral obesity on cognition is mechanistically credible, given that visceral fat is thought to be a major site of inflammatory cytokine production (Gil et al., 2011; Samaras et al., 2010). The role of inflammation in linking obesity and memory deficits is further supported by the findings of (Cook et al., 2017) who investigated the association between obesity and cognition in nondiabetic young women. They found that participants with obesity scored lower on a range of cognitive tasks including memory and that this result did not change when adjusted for depressive symptoms and physical activity. However, adjustment for inflammatory markers rendered the association nonsignificant, suggesting that this measure may mediate the impact of obesity.
Direct and indirect measures of adiposity have also been linked with both structural and functional changes in the brain, and particularly in areas linked with memory (Willette & Kapogiannis, 2015). Obesity is associated with reduced global brain volume (Beyer et al., 2019), incorporating both white matter and grey matter changes (Geha et al., 2017; Metzler-Baddeley et al., 2013), which seem to be modulated by age (Caunca et al., 2019). A recent study by Lynch and colleagues (2021) in children and adolescents found that age-corrected BMI was negatively associated with spatial memory and that this effect was partially mediated by reduced radial thickness in the left anterior hippocampus. This reflects a wider literature indicating that obesity and metabolic syndrome are associated with reductions in grey matter volume (GMV) in memory-relevant regions in childhood (Bauer et al., 2015; Chaddock et al., 2010; Mestre et al., 2017; Nouwen et al., 2017; Yau et al., 2014), although the picture may be less clear in adolescence (Lynch et al., 2021). This association continues throughout the lifespan into midlife (Debette et al., 2010) and older age (Metzler-Baddeley et al., 2013). Once again, the precise contribution of obesity, rather than comorbid conditions, is unclear. For example, patients with type 2 diabetes show reduced GMV in medial temporal and frontal regions (Moran et al., 2013), and plasma glucose and years of diabetes are positively associated with degree of brain atrophy (Tiehuis et al., 2008). The greater predictive value of WHR is supported by neural evidence from a recent study using samples from the UK Biobank (Hamer & Batty, 2019). However, here it was found that it was those who had both high BMI and high WHR who showed the most significantly reduced levels of GMV, compared to those who were high in only one or the other. While alterations to grey matter are pertinent to cognitive function, white matter changes may be particularly salient with regard to the role of memory in regulating consumption. For example, the fornix links the hippocampus with the hypothalamus and hypothalamic area (Kim et al., 2012; Poletti & Creswell, 1977) as well as areas involved in the hedonic control of consumption, such as the nucleus accumbens and orbitofrontal cortex (Blatt & Rosene, 1998). Fornix microstructure is associated with memory ability and is altered in older adults with higher BMI (Metzler-Baddeley et al., 2013; Stanek et al., 2011).
The obesity-related changes in brain structure are also reflected in function. Cheke and colleagues (2017) showed that higher BMI in young healthy adults was associated with reduced activity throughout the core memory network during tasks assessing episodic memory. In particular, in the hippocampus and angular gyrus, which are areas implicated in memory integration and precision (Richter et al., 2016; Yazar et al., 2014). The authors also found that there was a similar, yet distinct, pattern of group differences in brain activity if the participants were grouped by fasting plasma insulin rather than BMI, suggesting that both adiposity and insulin resistance might be contributing to neural changes, but that one did not directly explain the other.
5.1. Diet and cognition in humans
According to the vicious cycle hypothesis, excess intake disrupts the functioning of brain areas critical for memory, which further increases intake and contributes to the development and/or maintenance of diet-induced obesity (Clasen et al., 2020). However, while it is clear from the review above that obesity is indeed associated with memory impairments, it is not apparent whether poor diet must necessarily be associated with memory deficits via body fat accumulation and obesity (including associated comorbidities), or whether it is the diet itself that is detrimental. There is a substantial body of evidence linking quality of diet to cognition and brain health. However, the precise associations can be difficult to untangle in studies on humans: Experiments are rarely able to control participant diet, and thus directly measuring dietary nutrients often involves either blood sampling or supplementation, which can be difficult and expensive. Instead, most studies of the impact of diet on cognition make use of “food frequency questionnaires” (FFQs), which assess via self-report the relative abundance of particular indicator foods in a person’s diet during a particular period of time. However, the reliability of these questionnaires has been challenged (e.g. Archer et al., 2018) and it is pertinent to note that measures of dietary intake that rely on memory for what has been consumed may lead to issues of circularity when considering the impact of diet on memory, and particularly on memory for what has been consumed. Studies of “whole diets” or the frequency of particular foods (e.g., sugar-sweetened beverages or “junk food”) in the diet have identified a number of consistent patterns (Kim & Kang, 2017; Muñoz-García et al., 2020; Wiles et al., 2009). For example, “processed food” diets have been associated with poorer memory and increased risk of cognitive deficit (Akbaraly et al., 2009). Other studies have explored the importance of individual nutrients from levels estimated from FFQs. In particular, much focus has been placed upon the levels of dietary “bad” (saturated/trans) fats and “bad” (refined) sugars (Yeomans, 2017). These two nutrient groups are often studied together as a “high energy” or “Western Diet” (eg. Kanoski & Davidson, 2011). Doing so avoids many of the issues involved in trying to isolate the precise contribution of each nutrient (e.g., sugars or fats) to deficits, acknowledging that each may contribute and that it is potentially the combined impact of a diet high in both saturated fat and refined sugar that that is specifically deleterious (see for example, research suggesting ketogenic diets may be neuroprotective (e.g. Hallböök et al., 2012). Much of the evidence on the impact of diet on memory and cognition has been in the context of neurodegeneration (Eskelinen et al., 2008; Gustaw-Rothenberg, 2009; Morris, 2004; Okereke et al., 2012; Pugazhenthi et al., 2017) and age-related memory loss (Jacka et al., 2015). However, a growing number of studies of young, healthy adults have also demonstrated negative correlations between self-reported fat and sugar intake and performance on memory tests (Attuquayefio et al., 2016; Francis & Stevenson, 2011; Gibson et al., 2013).
The possible mechanisms by which an unhealthy diet may lead to deficits in memory overlap considerably with those posited for adiposity, with two of the leading candidates once again being insulin resistance and inflammation. Kheirouri and Alizadeh (Kheirouri & Alizadeh, 2019) conducted a systematic review into studies exploring the “inflammatory potential” of an individual’s diet and learning and memory outcomes, mostly using the ‘Dietary Inflammation Index’ (Shivappa et al., 2014): A higher DII score indicates a more pro-inflammatory diet, often higher in saturated fat and refined sugar. However, see (Lawrence, 2021) for an alternative view on pro-inflammatory fatty acids. The authors found that six of seven studies that assessed dementia risk reported that a higher DII was associated with increased likelihood of symptoms (Assmann et al., 2018; Frith et al., 2018; Gu et al., 2011; Hayden et al., 2017; Kesse-Guyot et al., 2017; Ozawa et al., 2017; Shin et al., 2018). Furthermore, three of these studies specifically showed negative associations between DII and measures of episodic memory (Assmann et al., 2018; Frith et al., 2018; Kesse-Guyot et al., 2017). While these findings point to an impact of inflammation on memory, it is not clear to what extend this is a direct result of the diet: The authors note that there were a number of other factors that differentiated individuals with high DII, including BMI.
One critique of cross-sectional, and even longitudinal studies of diet and cognition, is that they are correlational: unlike the rodent literature, it is difficult to determine whether dietary composition is a causal factor in memory deficits, or a result. There are a small number of studies that address this question. Two studies, both conducted in 2011, showed a negative influence of a short (5–7 day) exposure to experimental high fat diet on attention (Edwards et al., 2011; Holloway et al., 2011), but did not assess hippocampal-dependent memory. More recently, Attuquayefio and colleagues (2017) found a significant reduction in scores on the Hopkins Verbal Learning Task (HVLTR) in healthy adults who consumed a breakfast high in saturated fat and refined sugar for 4 days, compared with controls who consumed a low fat, low sugar breakfast matched for palatability. Finally, a study using twin-pairs from the NUtriGenomic Analysis in Twins Study found that 6-week exposure to a high-fat diet impaired memory consolidation in healthy adults (Schüler et al., 2018). As with much of the rodent literature, however, dietary intervention studies struggle with the issue of direct comparison: If diets are to be matched in calories, then reductions in one type of nutrient (e.g., fat) must be balanced by an increase in another (e.g., protein). As such, it is not always easy to isolate the relative impact of decreases in one nutrient from that of increasing the other. Notably, these dietary intervention studies were conducted for short periods, and thus participants are unlikely to have gained much, if any, weight during these interventions. Similarly, many of the studies in young adults reviewed above were able to dissociate the effects of diet from the potential confound of adiposity by using only normal weight volunteers (e.g. Attuquayefio et al., 2016; Francis & Stevenson, 2011). These findings thus suggest that unhealthy “Western” diets may be sufficient to produce memory deficits even in the absence of obesity. However, limiting the sample in this way ignores the potential for an interrelated or cumulative effect of multiple factors.
5.2. Memory for food
The evidence therefore suggests that there may be general issues with memory in obesity, and that multiple elements of the “obese syndrome”, including diet, may make independent contributions to this. These findings are significant given the importance of memory in regulating consumption. However, is there any reason to believe this may specifically impact memory for food? FMRI investigations indicate that individuals with obesity have unusual hippocampal responses to food cues, and that this is modulated by nutritive state (DelParigi et al., 2004; Geha et al., 2017; Leidy et al., 2011). In a recent study, Jones and colleagues (Jones et al., 2021) showed that lean, but not obese, participants showed greater hippocampal food-cue reactivity immediately following glucose consumption relative to water. The BMI group difference was driven by a significantly reduced level of hippocampal reactivity in individuals who were obese in the glucose condition. Reduced hippocampal response to food cues while in a post-consumptive state may suggest reduced contextualized processing of these stimuli. This would be significant both in terms of the suggested role of the hippocampus in integrating food stimuli with contextual information about current need state (e.g. Davidson et al., 2009), but also with the likelihood that these stimuli will be retained in memory.
Evidence for memory deficits have been shown both in studies using food stimuli (Cheke et al., 2017; Cheke et al., 2016) and nonfood stimuli (Cook et al., 2017), but few studies have explicitly compared the two. Leng and colleagues tested young women with either healthy or obese BMI on a word-list source memory task including both food and non-food stimuli. They found that item memory for non-food stimuli was impaired in the group with obesity, but that recognition of food words was better in the group with obesity than in lean controls. Source memory was, however, impaired for both types of stimuli (Leng et al., 2021). The authors suggest that this could reflect an attentional bias towards food cues in young women who are obese, something that has been previously reported in samples with obesity (Docteur et al., 2008; Hagan et al., 2020; Soetens & Braet, 2007). According to central/peripheral trade-off theory (Kensinger, 2009), attentional biases may improve item- while impairing source-memory as the salient item “captures” disproportionate attention, reducing encoding of contextual information. Such an account may suggest that during a meal, individuals with obesity may encode the food eaten, but may be less able to integrate this information with the contextual information that renders the memory useful for regulating consumption, particularly motivational need state and time of eating.
5.3. Future directions
In summary, obesity has been associated consistently with changes in both memory and the neural structures that underpin it. There appear to be multiple, potentially mutually exacerbating, mechanisms driving this association. Notably, a high fat, high sugar diet is sufficient to induce neural and cognitive changes itself, as well as via accumulation of bodyfat and comorbidities such as diabetes. Factors such as insulin resistance and inflammation are highly implicated by multiple lines of evidence. However, these associations are complex and can be difficult to isolate in human studies, and much can be learned from rodent models in this respect. Nonetheless, large sample longitudinal studies may be able to tease apart the relative contributions of different causal factors and should be a target of future research. In addition, the impact of short vs long term high fat diet requires further exploration: Evidence from mice suggests that 24 hours of a high fat diet may be sufficient to induce memory deficits that are then quickly reversible on return to a low fat regime (McLean et al., 2018). Is this also the case in humans, and if so, is it due to fast-acting deleterious effects, or, for example, the costs associated with the physiological transfer between primary fuel sources which may resolve with time (Cunnane et al., 2020)? The answers to these questions could have considerable implications for dietary guidance and intervention.
The combination of obesity-related changes to general and associative memory processes and alterations to food-cue reactivity may be particularly significant for understanding the factors that perpetuate obesity through dysregulated consumption. The next stage in this research will require a better understanding of how memories for consumption episodes are encoded and represented in the brain, and specifically how these representations are affected by obesity. It is not currently clear how the memory deficits demonstrated in obesity are manifested when it comes to remembered meals, and subsequent changes in consumption. Such explorations are vital in closing the gaps in the “vicious cycle.”
5.4. Interim Summary
Multiple elements of the “obese syndrome” are associated with memory deficits and brain changes in humans.
The relative contributions of adiposity and diet are difficult to isolate, and both may contribute independently.
Obesity may be associated with particular deficits in remembering contextual details of food events.
6. IMPACT OF OBESITY ON HIPPPOCAMPAL FUNCTION IN RODENTS
The evidence reviewed above indicates that cognitive functions mediated by the hippocampus limit eating behavior and that, in humans, obesity is associated with impaired cognitive and hippocampal function. The following section will review the impact of hypercaloric foods and obesity on hippocampal function in rodents and will summarize potential mechanisms through which obesity mediates these effects. Given that hippocampal dysfunction increases energy intake, this large body of evidence provides convincing support for the possibility that diet-induced obesity is caused and/or maintained at least in part by a vicious cycle wherein overeating disrupts hippocampal function, leading to further overeating (Clasen et al., 2020).
Extensive research had shown that hypercaloric diets and the associated obesity and metabolic complications impair dHC-dependent memory, including spatial memory in the water maze, radial arm maze, spontaneous alternation and object location tasks (Darling et al., 2013; Farr et al., 2008; Fu et al., 2017; Greenwood & Winocur, 1990; Hernández-Ramírez et al., 2021; Heyward et al., 2016; Li et al., 2002; McFadden et al., 2020; Ross et al., 2009; Ross et al., 2012; Stranahan et al., 2008; Valladolid-Acebes et al., 2012). The memory-impairing effects of high energy diets and obesity are accompanied by deficits in dHC glutamatergic metabolism, neurotransmission (Valladolid-Acebes et al., 2012), synaptic plasticity (Farr et al., 2008; Gerges et al., 2003; Hao et al., 2016; Karimi et al., 2015; Li et al., 2002; Porter et al., 2012; Stranahan et al., 2008), and neurogenesis (Bonds et al., 2020; Bracke et al., 2019; Lindqvist et al., 2006). Obesity likely interferes with synaptic plasticity by reducing hippocampal spine density (Stranahan et al., 2008) decreasing levels of critical molecules such Arc (Mateos et al., 2009), brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) (Stranahan et al., 2008; Wu et al., 2004), and CREB (Wu et al., 2004), by dysregulating ubiquitin-proteasome signaling and protein degradation (McFadden et al., 2020), and by promoting microglia-induced synaptic stripping (Hao et al., 2016). Some of the proximal mechanisms include peripheral elevations in triglycerides based on evidence showing that lowering triglycerides reverses memory deficits and that application of triglycerides impairs synaptic plasticity (Farr et al., 2008). Given that diets low in carbohydrates and ketogenic diets effectively reduce triglyceride levels, findings showing that lowering triglyceride levels reverses memory deficits is consistent with the possibility that this may be the mechanism through which low carbohydrate and ketogenic diets exert the cognitive improvements observed in human participants described in Section 5. Elevated adiposity also increases peripheral levels of pro-inflammatory cytokines and induces neuroinflammation (Bondan et al., 2019; Guo et al., 2020; Hao et al., 2016). Cytokines produced by adipose tissue, particularly interleukin1 (IL-1) and IL-1-mediated microglia activation in dHC have been implicated in the impact of obesity on hippocampal synaptic plasticity and memory (Erion et al., 2014; Guo et al., 2020; Hao et al., 2016). The effects of high energy diets and obesity on memory and plasticity are rescued by antioxidants, suggesting that some of their effects on the brain may also be mediated by oxidative stress (Karimi et al., 2013; Wu et al., 2004). Other contributing factors include hippocampal insulin resistance (Fu et al., 2017; Gladding et al., 2018) and epigenetic dysregulation of hippocampal genes critical for memory (Heyward et al., 2016).
In addition to impairing exteroceptive-based learning, synaptic plasticity, and other memory-associated neurobiological processes in the hippocampus, high energy diet consumption and/or obesity are also associated with impairments in hippocampal processing of interoceptive signals that inform about energy status. For example, in rats that were previously trained to discriminate between low and high levels of food restriction for appetitive reinforcement (the deprivation intensity discrimination task described in Section 4), switching from healthy chow to a high energy Western-style diet disrupted the capacity for the rats to discriminate between energetic states (Sample et al., 2016). Several findings suggest that this impaired interoceptive awareness associated with high energy diet consumption, analogous to that observed following selective hippocampal damage in rodents (Davidson et al., 2010), is based on disrupted hippocampal processing of endocrine and vagus nerve-mediated signals that fluctuate based on energy status and/or meal consumption. For example, ghrelin receptor activation in vHC neurons engages phosphoinositide 3-kinase (PI3K) subunit p85 and AKT (Ser473) intracellular signaling pathways in chow-fed rats, but not in rats maintained on a high energy diet for 4 weeks (Kanoski et al., 2013). Given that vHC ghrelin administration replicates the effects of 24 hour food restriction in otherwise nonrestricted rats in the deprivation intensity discrimination task (Suarez et al., 2020), this suggests that high energy diet consumption reduces the capacity of ghrelin to communicate interoceptive hunger status to vHC neurons.
Consuming a high energy diet also appears to reduce the capacity of anorexigenic interoceptive signals to engage hippocampal neurons. For example, leptin administration increases dHC expression of BDNF, a neurotrophin that promotes memory formation and neural plasticity, in control rats fed a healthy chow diet but not in rats fed a high energy diet for 2 weeks (Kanoski et al., 2014). Further, the within-meal satiation signal cholecystokinin (CCK) activates dHC neurons via a vagus nerve-mediated polysynaptic pathway (Suarez et al., 2018). While this response has not been directly evaluated in obese or high energy diet-fed rodents, it has been established that high energy diet maintenance reduces the capacity of peripheral CCK to activate neurons in the hindbrain nucleus tractus solitarius (Covasa et al., 2000), the first CNS relay connecting the vagus nerve signaling to the dHC (Suarez et al., 2018). Collectively, these findings suggest that consumption of a high energy Western-style diet blunts the capacity of both hunger-associated biological signals (ghrelin) and satiation-and satiety-associated biological signals (CCK, leptin) to communicate interoceptive energy status to both the ventral and dorsal subregions of the hippocampus.
6.1. Interim summary
Hypercaloric diets and the associated obesity and metabolic complications impair dHC-dependent memory.
The memory-impairing effects of high energy diets and obesity are accompanied by changes in dHC glutamatergic neuronal structure and synaptic plasticity.
Several mechanisms may collectively contribute to these alterations, including elevated triglycerides, peripheral and central inflammation, oxidative stress, hippocampal insulin resistance, and epigenetic dysregulation of hippocampal genes critical for memory.
Hypercaloric diets and the associated obesity and metabolic complications are also associated with impaired hippocampal processing of interoceptive signals that inform about energy status.
These deficits in interoception appear to be based on disrupted hippocampal processing of endocrine and vagus nerve-mediated hunger and satiety signals.
7. CONCLUDING REMARKS
In summary, the evidence reviewed above demonstrates that impaired memory and disrupted hippocampal functioning are associated with deficits in processing of hunger and satiety cues and overeating in both humans and rodents. Convincing evidence was presented indicating that endocrine and neuropeptide systems act primarily in vHC to provide hunger and satiety signals and regulate learned aspects of eating. Extensive findings show that meal-related memory limits future intake and this appears to be mediated, at least in part, by principal dHC neurons. The relationship between memory and eating is bidirectional such that overconsumption of high energy diets and obesity impairs interoceptive processing, memory and hippocampal function in both humans and rodents. These disturbances, in turn, would be expected to contribute to further excessive intake, thereby creating a vicious cycle that contributes to and/or perpetuates unhealthy weight gain.
Several outstanding questions remain. For instance, although obesity is clearly associated with disruptions in many hippocampal processes, there is no equivocal evidence showing that obesity actually interferes with hippocampal-mediated inhibition of energy intake. The relationship between dHC and vHC regulation of eating is also unclear. Although there are cases when dHC and vHC involvement in eating diverges (e.g., the ability of ghrelin to stimulate intake is mediated by vHC but not dHC (Kanoski et al., 2013); there are instances when there is overlap in dHC and vHC participation in interoceptive processing and eating. For example, while endocrine signals appear to predominantly engage vHC, there is also evidence indicating that meal-related satiation signals communicating from the gut to the brain via the vagus nerve engage dHC and promote memory function (Suarez et al., 2018). Similarly, as in the case of postmeal dHC inactivation, postmeal vHC inactivation also increases consumption (Hannapel et al., 2019; Hannapel et al., 2017). Finally, neural circuits that suppress energy intake in mice have been established from both the vHC (Sweeney & Yang, 2015) and dHC (Azevedo et al., 2019) to distinct septal nuclei (lateral and medial, respectively). Given that vHC is involved primarily in emotional and food-related memory and dHC in episodic and spatial memory, both dHC and vHC may contribute to the mnemonic representation of an eating episode and inhibit subsequent intake via both common and distinct mechanisms. Indeed, memory is mediated by multiple brain areas that each represent different information contained within the same experience (Gasbarri et al., 2014; White et al., 2013). While the focus here, and in the rodent literature more generally, is within the hippocampus, other areas within the “core memory network” may also contribute to the association between memory and eating. Future work should particularly explore those areas found to be altered in human obesity, such as the angular gyrus (Cheke et al., 2017).
Another question is whether this relationship between memory and eating can be used to identify treatments to promote weight loss. For example, it is possible that compounds that are used to treat memory problems could attenuate weight gain. The application of the results from experimental studies in humans on the role of memory for recent eating in appetite to the development of healthy eating/weight loss interventions is only beginning to be explored. A smartphone application aimed at enhancing food memories and encouraging “attentive” eating was found to be acceptable by users (Robinson, Higgs, et al., 2013), but an initial efficacy trial did not find any effects of using the application on energy intake or weight loss (Whitelock, Kersbergen, et al., 2019). An analysis of user experiences identified facilitators (e.g., ability to incorporate into routine) and barriers (e.g., older participants feeling that using the application in social settings was inappropriate) to application usage that could inform the design of future studies (Whitelock et al., 2020). Future research could also be aimed at identifying more effective ways of reliably enhancing memory for recent eating that could be used in healthy eating/weight loss interventions and investigating whether such interventions should be targeted at individuals with lower memory ability.
Finally, evidence from weight loss interventions suggest that the negative impact of overweight on memory may be reversible (Siervo et al., 2011). Bariatric surgery studies consistently show memory deficits in pre-operative patients (Alosco, Galioto, et al., 2014; Alosco, Spitznagel, et al., 2014; Gunstad et al., 2011; Miller et al., 2013; Walø-Syversen et al., 2021). Consistent with results from preclinical rodent models (Grayson et al., 2014), many of these studies have shown that verbal memory performance is significantly improved 12 weeks following gastric bypass surgery and that these benefits are maintained at 1- (Miller et al., 2013), 2- (Alosco, Spitznagel, et al., 2014) and 3-years (Alosco, Galioto, et al., 2014) postoperative. These studies also indicated that (after controlling for initial BMI) postoperative BMI was related to memory performance, suggesting that those who returned to a healthier weight were more likely to see memory improvement. It should be noted however, that even here the causality is not clear cut: There is some evidence to suggest that cognitive performance may predict outcome following surgery, potentially via ability to maintain the strict lifestyle and medication regime required (Elfhag & Rössner, 2005; Spitznagel et al., 2015). This may suggest that those within the surgery group that had initially better cognition would be more likely to have lost more weight. Weight-loss may also be associated with neural changes: Boraxbekk and colleagues (2015) found that 6 months on a restricted diet not only improved face-name associative memory, but also that decreases in waist circumferences were correlated with increased brain activity in the superior temporal gyrus and insula. Lastly, whether effects are permanent or reversible may depend on the key causal factor. In patients with diabetes, for example, large-scale trials of intense glycemic control have found no significant recovery of brain volume or cognition (Murray et al., 2017).
Highlights.
Meal-related memory inhibits eating in humans
Amnesia is associated with overeating and impaired interoception in humans
Research in rodents and humans implicates the hippocampus in these effects
Obesity disrupts memory and hippocampal functioning in rodents and humans
These effects of obesity may create a vicious cycle that perpetuates obesity
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
The preparation of this article was supported by NIH DK114700 (to MBP), BBSRC Grant UK BB/N008847/1 (to SH), and NIH DK123423 and DK104897 (to SEK). The funding sources did not play any role in the decision to submit the article for publication.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
REFERENCES
- Akbaraly TN, Singh-Manoux A, Marmot MG, & Brunner EJ (2009). Education attenuates the association between dietary patterns and cognition. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord, 27(2), 147–154. 10.1159/000199235 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alhadeff AL, Rupprecht LE, & Hayes MR (2012, Feb). GLP-1 Neurons in the Nucleus of the Solitary Tract Project Directly to the Ventral Tegmental Area and Nucleus Accumbens to Control for Food Intake. Endocrinology, 153(2), 647–658. 10.1210/en.2011-1443 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alosco ML, Galioto R, Spitznagel MB, Strain G, Devlin M, Cohen R, Crosby RD, Mitchell JE, & Gunstad J (2014). Cognitive function after bariatric surgery: evidence for improvement 3 years after surgery. The American Journal of Surgery, 207(6), 870–876. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alosco ML, Spitznagel MB, Strain G, Devlin M, Cohen R, Paul R, Crosby RD, Mitchell JE, & Gunstad J (2014). Improved memory function two years after bariatric surgery. Obesity, 22(1), 32–38. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Archer E, Marlow ML, & Lavie CJ (2018). Controversy and debate: Memory-Based Methods Paper 1: the fatal flaws of food frequency questionnaires and other memory-based dietary assessment methods. Journal of Clinical epidemiology, 104, 113–124. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ariyasu H, Takaya K, Tagami T, Ogawa Y, Hosoda K, Akamizu T, Suda M, Koh T, Natsui K, Toyooka S, Shirakami G, Usui T, Shimatsu A, Doi K, Hosoda H, Kojima M, Kangawa K, & Nakao K (2001, Oct). Stomach is a major source of circulating ghrelin, and feeding state determines plasma ghrelin-like immunoreactivity levels in humans. J Clin Endocrinol Metab, 86(10), 4753–4758. 10.1210/jcem.86.10.7885 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ashby DM, Floresco SB, Phillips AG, McGirr A, Seamans JK, & Wang YT (2021, 01). LTD is involved in the formation and maintenance of rat hippocampal CA1 place-cell fields. Nat Commun, 12(1), 100. 10.1038/s41467-020-20317-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Assmann KE, Adjibade M, Shivappa N, Hébert JR, Wirth MD, Touvier M, Akbaraly T, Hercberg S, Galan P, & Julia C (2018). The Inflammatory Potential of the Diet at Midlife is Associated with Later Healthy Aging in French Adults. The Journal of Nutrition, 148(3), 437–444. 10.1093/jn/nxx061 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Attuquayefio T, Stevenson RJ, Boakes RA, Oaten MJ, Yeomans MR, Mahmut M, & Francis HM (2016, Oct). A high-fat high-sugar diet predicts poorer hippocampal-related memory and a reduced ability to suppress wanting under satiety. J Exp Psychol Anim Learn Cogn, 42(4), 415–428. 10.1037/xan0000118 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Attuquayefio T, Stevenson RJ, Oaten MJ, & Francis HM (2017). A four-day Western-style dietary intervention causes reductions in hippocampal-dependent learning and memory and interoceptive sensitivity. PLoS One, 12(2), e0172645. 10.1371/journal.pone.0172645 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Azevedo EP, Pomeranz L, Cheng J, Schneeberger M, Vaughan R, Stern SA, Tan B, Doerig K, Greengard P, & Friedman JM (2019, 05). A Role of Drd2 Hippocampal Neurons in Context-Dependent Food Intake. Neuron, 102(4), 873–886.e875. 10.1016/j.neuron.2019.03.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baker KB, & Kim JJ (2002, 2002 Mar-Apr). Effects of stress and hippocampal NMDA receptor antagonism on recognition memory in rats. Learn Mem, 9(2), 58–65. 10.1101/lm.46102 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barbosa FF, Pontes IM, Ribeiro S, Ribeiro AM, & Silva RH (2012, Jun 15). Differential roles of the dorsal hippocampal regions in the acquisition of spatial and temporal aspects of episodic-like memory [Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. Behav Brain Res, 232(1), 269–277. 10.1016/j.bbr.2012.04.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barkus C, McHugh SB, Sprengel R, Seeburg PH, Rawlins JN, & Bannerman DM (2010, Jan 10). Hippocampal NMDA receptors and anxiety: at the interface between cognition and emotion. Eur J Pharmacol, 626(1), 49–56. 10.1016/j.ejphar.2009.10.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bartsch T, & Wulff P (2015, Nov 19). The hippocampus in aging and disease: From plasticity to vulnerability. Neuroscience, 309, 1–16. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.07.084 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bauer CC, Moreno B, Gonzalez-Santos L, Concha L, Barquera S, & Barrios FA (2015, Jul 3). Child overweight and obesity are associated with reduced executive cognitive performance and brain alterations: a magnetic resonance imaging study in Mexican children. Pediatr Obes. 10.1111/ijpo.241 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bengtson CP, & Bading H (2012). Nuclear calcium signaling. Adv Exp Med Biol, 970, 377–405. 10.1007/978-3-7091-0932-8_17 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Benito-León J, Mitchell A, Hernández-Gallego J, & Bermejo-Pareja F (2013). Obesity and impaired cognitive functioning in the elderly: a population-based cross-sectional study (NEDICES). European Journal of Neurology, 20(6), 899–e877. 10.1111/ene.12083 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beydoun MA, Beydoun H, & Wang Y (2008). Obesity and central obesity as risk factors for incident dementia and its subtypes: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Obesity Reviews, 9(3), 204–218. 10.1111/j.1467-789X.2008.00473.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beyer F, García-García I, Heinrich M, Schroeter ML, Sacher J, Luck T, Riedel-Heller SG, Stumvoll M, Villringer A, & Witte AV (2019). Neuroanatomical correlates of food addiction symptoms and body mass index in the general population. Hum Brain Mapp, 40(9), 2747–2758. 10.1002/hbm.24557 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bienkowski MS, Bowman I, Song MY, Gou L, Ard T, Cotter K, Zhu M, Benavidez NL, Yamashita S, Abu-Jaber J, Azam S, Lo D, Foster NN, Hintiryan H, & Dong HW (2018, Nov). Integration of gene expression and brain-wide connectivity reveals the multiscale organization of mouse hippocampal networks. Nat Neurosci, 21(11), 1628–1643. 10.1038/s41593-018-0241-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blatt GJ, & Rosene DL (1998). Organization of direct hippocampal efferent projections to the cerebral cortex of the rhesus monkey: projections from CA1, prosubiculum, and subiculum to the temporal lobe. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 392(1), 92–114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blum ID, Patterson Z, Khazall R, Lamont EW, Sleeman MW, Horvath TL, & Abizaid A (2009, Dec 1). Reduced anticipatory locomotor responses to scheduled meals in ghrelin receptor deficient mice. Neuroscience, 164(2), 351–359. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.08.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bondan EF, Cardoso CV, Martins MFM, & Otton R (2019). Memory impairments and increased GFAP expression in hippocampal astrocytes following hypercaloric diet in rats. Arq Neuropsiquiatr, 77(9), 601–608. 10.1590/0004-282X20190091 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bonds JA, Shetti A, Stephen TKL, Bonini MG, Minshall RD, & Lazarov O (2020, 10). Deficits in hippocampal neurogenesis in obesity-dependent and -independent type-2 diabetes mellitus mouse models. Sci Rep, 10(1), 16368. 10.1038/s41598-020-73401-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bonini JS, Rodrigues L, Kerr DS, Bevilaqua LR, Cammarota M, & Izquierdo I (2003, Mar). AMPA/kainate and group-I metabotropic receptor antagonists infused into different brain areas impair memory formation of inhibitory avoidance in rats. Behav Pharmacol, 14(2), 161–166. 10.1097/00008877-200303000-00008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boraxbekk CJ, Stomby A, Ryberg M, Lindahl B, Larsson C, Nyberg L, & Olsson T (2015). Diet-Induced Weight Loss Alters Functional Brain Responses during an Episodic Memory Task. Obes Facts, 8(4), 261–272. 10.1159/000437157 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bozkurt H, Özer S, Yılmaz R, Sönmezgöz E, Kazancı Ö, Erbaş O, & Demir O (2017). Assessment of neurocognitive functions in children and adolescents with obesity. Applied Neuropsychology: Child, 6(4), 262–268. 10.1080/21622965.2016.1150184 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bracke A, Domanska G, Bracke K, Harzsch S, van den Brandt J, Bröker B, & von Bohlen Und Halbach O (2019). Obesity Impairs Mobility and Adult Hippocampal Neurogenesis. J Exp Neurosci, 13, 1179069519883580. 10.1177/1179069519883580 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bramham CR, Worley PF, Moore MJ, & Guzowski JF (2008, Nov 12). The immediate early gene arc/arg3.1: regulation, mechanisms, and function. J Neurosci, 28(46), 11760–11767. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3864-08.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brierley DI, Holt MK, Singh A, de Araujo A, McDougle M, Vergara M, Afaghani MH, Lee SJ, Scott K, Maske C, Langhans W, Krause E, de Kloet A, Gribble FM, Reimann F, Rinaman L, de Lartigue G, & Trapp S (2021, Feb). Central and peripheral GLP-1 systems independently suppress eating. Nat Metab, 3(2), 258–273. 10.1038/s42255-021-00344-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Briggs SB, Ware CB, Sharma K, Davis SC, Lalumiere RT, & Parent MB (2021, Sep). Postmeal optogenetic inhibition of dorsal hippocampal principal neurons increases future intake in a time-dependent manner. Neurobiol Learn Mem, 183, 107478. 10.1016/j.nlm.2021.107478 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brunstrom JM, Burn JF, Sell NR, Collingwood JM, Rogers PJ, Wilkinson LL, Hinton EC, Maynard OM, & Ferriday D (2012). Episodic memory and appetite regulation in humans. PloS one, 7(12), e50707. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Butler JL, Mendonca PR, Robinson HP, & Paulsen O (2016, Apr 13). Intrinsic Cornu Ammonis Area 1 Theta-Nested Gamma Oscillations Induced by Optogenetic Theta Frequency Stimulation. J Neurosci, 36(15), 4155–4169. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3150-15.2016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cabral A, Cornejo MP, Fernandez G, De Francesco PN, Garcia-Romero G, Uriarte M, Zigman JM, Portiansky E, Reynaldo M, & Perello M (2017, May 1). Circulating Ghrelin Acts on GABA Neurons of the Area Postrema and Mediates Gastric Emptying in Male Mice. Endocrinology, 158(5), 1436–1449. 10.1210/en.2016-1815 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cao SG, Wu H, & Cai ZZ (2016, Mar). Dose-dependent effect of ghrelin on gastric emptying in rats and the related mechanism of action. Kaohsiung J Med Sci, 32(3), 113–117. 10.1016/j.kjms.2016.01.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carpenter-Hyland EP, Plummer TK, Vazdarjanova A, & Blake DT (2010, Aug 17). Arc expression and neuroplasticity in primary auditory cortex during initial learning are inversely related to neural activity. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 107(33), 14828–14832. 10.1073/pnas.1008604107 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caunca MR, Simonetto M, Alperin N, Elkind MS, Sacco RL, Wright CB, & Rundek T (2019). Measures of Adiposity and Alzheimer’s Disease-Related MRI Markers: The Northern Manhattan Study. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 70(4), 995–1004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chaddock L, Erickson KI, Prakash RS, Kim JS, Voss MW, Vanpatter M, Pontifex MB, Raine LB, Konkel A, Hillman CH, Cohen NJ, & Kramer AF (2010, Oct 28). A neuroimaging investigation of the association between aerobic fitness, hippocampal volume, and memory performance in preadolescent children. Brain Res, 1358, 172–183. 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.08.049 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cheke LG, Bonnici HM, Clayton NS, & Simons JS (2017, Jan 16). Obesity and insulin resistance are associated with reduced activity in core memory regions of the brain. Neuropsychologia, 96, 137–149. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.01.013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cheke LG, & Clayton NS (2013). Do different tests of episodic memory produce consistent results in human adults? Learning & Memory, 20(9), 491–498. 10.1101/lm.030502.113 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cheke LG, & Clayton NS (2015). The six blind men and the elephant: Are episodic memory tasks tests of different things or different tests of the same thing? J Exp Child Psychol. 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.03.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cheke LG, Simons JS, & Clayton NS (2016, Nov). Higher body mass index is associated with episodic memory deficits in young adults. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove), 69(11), 2305–2316. 10.1080/17470218.2015.1099163 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chinnakkaruppan A, Wintzer ME, McHugh TJ, & Rosenblum K (2014, Aug). Differential contribution of hippocampal subfields to components of associative taste learning. J Neurosci, 34(33), 11007–11015. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0956-14.2014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clark JK, Furgerson M, Crystal JD, Fechheimer M, Furukawa R, & Wagner JJ (2015, Nov). Alterations in synaptic plasticity coincide with deficits in spatial working memory in presymptomatic 3xTg-AD mice. Neurobiol Learn Mem, 125, 152–162. 10.1016/j.nlm.2015.09.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clasen MM, Riley AL, & Davidson TL (2020). Hippocampal-Dependent Inhibitory Learning and Memory Processes in the Control of Eating and Drug Taking. Curr Pharm Des, 26(20), 2334–2352. 10.2174/1381612826666200206091447 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clayton NS, & Dickinson A (1998). Episodic-like memory during cache recovery by scrub jays. Nature, 395(6699), 272–274. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clifton PG, Vickers SP, & Somerville EM (1998, Jun). Little and often: ingestive behavior patterns following hippocampal lesions in rats. Behavioral Neuroscience, 112(3), 502–511. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9676968 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Collins R, & Stafford LD (2015). Feeling happy and thinking about food. Counteractive effects of mood and memory on food consumption. Appetite, 84, 107–112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cook RL, O’Dwyer NJ, Donges CE, Parker HM, Cheng HL, Steinbeck KS, Cox EP, Franklin JL, Garg ML, & Rooney KB (2017). Relationship between obesity and cognitive function in young women: The food, mood and mind study. Journal of Obesity, 2017. 10.1155/2017/5923862 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coppin G, Nolan-Poupart S, Jones-Gotman M, & Small DM (2014). Working memory and reward association learning impairments in obesity. Neuropsychologia, 65, 146–155. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Covasa M, Grahn J, & Ritter RC (2000, Jan 29). High fat maintenance diet attenuates hindbrain neuronal response to CCK. Regul Pept, 86(1–3), 83–88. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10672906 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crystal JD, Alford WT, Zhou W, & Hohmann AG (2013, Mar). Source memory in the rat. Curr Biol, 23(5), 387–391. 10.1016/j.cub.2013.01.023 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cummings DE, Purnell JQ, Frayo RS, Schmidova K, Wisse BE, & Weigle DS (2001, Aug). A preprandial rise in plasma ghrelin levels suggests a role in meal initiation in humans [Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t Research Support, U.S. Gov’t, Non-P.H.S. Research Support, U.S. Gov’t, P.H.S.]. Diabetes, 50(8), 1714–1719. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11473029 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cunnane SC, Trushina E, Morland C, Prigione A, Casadesus G, Andrews ZB, Beal MF, Bergersen LH, Brinton RD, de la Monte S, Eckert A, Harvey J, Jeggo R, Jhamandas JH, Kann O, la Cour CM, Martin WF, Mithieux G, Moreira PI, Murphy MP, Nave KA, Nuriel T, Oliet SHR, Saudou F, Mattson MP, Swerdlow RH, & Millan MJ (2020, Sep). Brain energy rescue: an emerging therapeutic concept for neurodegenerative disorders of ageing. Nat Rev Drug Discov, 19(9), 609–633. 10.1038/s41573-020-0072-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Czerniawski J, Ree F, Chia C, & Otto T (2012, Jul). Dorsal versus ventral hippocampal contributions to trace and contextual conditioning: differential effects of regionally selective NMDA receptor antagonism on acquisition and expression. Hippocampus, 22(7), 1528–1539. 10.1002/hipo.20992 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Czerniawski J, Ree F, Chia C, Ramamoorthi K, Kumata Y, & Otto TA (2011, Aug 3). The importance of having Arc: expression of the immediate-early gene Arc is required for hippocampus-dependent fear conditioning and blocked by NMDA receptor antagonism. J Neurosci, 31(31), 11200–11207. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2211-11.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Darling JN, Ross AP, Bartness TJ, & Parent MB (2013, May). Predicting the effects of a high-energy diet on fatty liver and hippocampal-dependent memory in male rats. Obesity (Silver Spring), 21(5), 910–917. 10.1002/oby.20167 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davidson TL, Chan K, Jarrard LE, Kanoski SE, Clegg DJ, & Benoit SC (2009, Mar). Contributions of the hippocampus and medial prefrontal cortex to energy and body weight regulation. Hippocampus, 19(3), 235–252. 10.1002/hipo.20499 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davidson TL, & Jarrard LE (1993, Mar). A role for hippocampus in the utilization of hunger signals. Behavioral and neural biology, 59(2), 167–171. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davidson TL, Jones S, Roy M, & Stevenson RJ (2019). The Cognitive Control of Eating and Body Weight: It’s More Than What You “Think”. Front Psychol, 10, 62. 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00062 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davidson TL, Kanoski SE, Chan K, Clegg DJ, Benoit SC, & Jarrard LE (2010, Feb). Hippocampal lesions impair retention of discriminative responding based on energy state cues. Behavioral Neuroscience, 124(1), 97–105. https://doi.org/2010-01915-020 [pii] 10.1037/a0018402 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davidson TL, Kanoski SE, Walls EK, & Jarrard LE (2005, Dec 15). Memory inhibition and energy regulation. Physiology & Behavior, 86(5), 731–746. https://doi.org/S0031-9384(05)00401-4 [pii] 10.1016/j.physbeh.2005.09.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davis JF, Choi DL, Clegg DJ, & Benoit SC (2011, Apr 18). Signaling through the ghrelin receptor modulates hippocampal function and meal anticipation in mice. Physiol Behav, 103(1), 39–43. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2010.10.017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Debette S, Beiser A, Hoffmann U, Decarli C, O’Donnell CJ, Massaro JM, Au R, Himali JJ, Wolf PA, Fox CS, & Seshadri S (2010, Aug). Visceral fat is associated with lower brain volume in healthy middle-aged adults. Ann Neurol, 68(2), 136–144. 10.1002/ana.22062 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DelParigi A, Chen K, Salbe A, Hill J, Wing R, Reiman E, & Tataranni P (2004). Persistence of abnormal neural responses to a meal in postobese individuals. Int J Obs Relat Metab Disord, 28(3), 370–377. 10.1038/sj.ijo.0802558 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Derkach V, Barria A, & Soderling TR (1999, Mar 16). Ca2+/calmodulin-kinase II enhances channel conductance of alpha-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolepropionate type glutamate receptors. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 96(6), 3269–3274. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10077673 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Descalzi G, Gao V, Steinman MQ, Suzuki A, & Alberini CM (2019). Lactate from astrocytes fuels learning-induced mRNA translation in excitatory and inhibitory neurons. Commun Biol, 2, 247. 10.1038/s42003-019-0495-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dickson SL, Shirazi RH, Hansson C, Bergquist F, Nissbrandt H, & Skibicka KP (2012, Apr 4). The glucagon-like peptide 1 (GLP-1) analogue, exendin-4, decreases the rewarding value of food: a new role for mesolimbic GLP-1 receptors. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 32(14), 4812–4820. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6326-11.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Docteur A, Urdapilleta I, Defrance C, & Raison J (2008). Implicit and explicit memory bias for words related to food, shape and body parts in obese and normal weight females. Current Psychology Letters, 24(2, 2008). 10.4000/cpl.3983 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Dong HW, Swanson LW, Chen L, Fanselow MS, & Toga AW (2009, Jul 14). Genomic-anatomic evidence for distinct functional domains in hippocampal field CA1. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 106(28), 11794–11799. 10.1073/pnas.0812608106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dossat AM, Lilly N, Kay K, & Williams DL (2011, Oct 12). Glucagon-like peptide 1 receptors in nucleus accumbens affect food intake [Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural]. J Neurosci, 31(41), 14453–14457. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3262-11.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Drazen DL, Vahl TP, D’Alessio DA, Seeley RJ, & Woods SC (2006, Jan). Effects of a fixed meal pattern on ghrelin secretion: evidence for a learned response independent of nutrient status. Endocrinology, 147(1), 23–30. 10.1210/en.2005-0973 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Drieskens DC, Neves LR, Pugliane KC, de Souza IBMB, Lima Á, Salvadori MGDS, Ribeiro AM, Silva RH, & Barbosa FF (2017, Nov). CA1 inactivation impairs episodic-like memory in rats. Neurobiol Learn Mem, 145, 28–33. 10.1016/j.nlm.2017.08.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dye L, Boyle NB, Champ C, & Lawton C (2017). The relationship between obesity and cognitive health and decline. Proc Nutr Soc, 76(4), 443–454. 10.1017/S0029665117002014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Edwards LM, Murray AJ, Holloway CJ, Carter EE, Kemp GJ, Codreanu I, Brooker H, Tyler DJ, Robbins PA, & Clarke K (2011, Mar). Short-term consumption of a high-fat diet impairs whole-body efficiency and cognitive function in sedentary men. FASEB J, 25(3), 1088–1096. 10.1096/fj.10-171983 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elfhag K, & Rössner S (2005). Who succeeds in maintaining weight loss? A conceptual review of factors associated with weight loss maintenance and weight regain. Obs Rev, 6(1), 67–85. 10.1111/j.1467-789X.2005.00170.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Erion JR, Wosiski-Kuhn M, Dey A, Hao S, Davis CL, Pollock NK, & Stranahan AM (2014, Feb 12). Obesity elicits interleukin 1-mediated deficits in hippocampal synaptic plasticity. J Neurosci, 34(7), 2618–2631. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4200-13.2014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eskelinen MH, Ngandu T, Helkala EL, Tuomilehto J, Nissinen A, Soininen H, & Kivipelto M (2008). Fat intake at midlife and cognitive impairment later in life: a population-based CAIDE study. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry, 23(7), 741–747. 10.1002/gps.1969 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fanselow MS, & Dong HW (2010, Jan 14). Are the dorsal and ventral hippocampus functionally distinct structures? Neuron, 65(1), 7–19. 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.11.031 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Farr SA, Yamada KA, Butterfield DA, Abdul HM, Xu L, Miller NE, Banks WA, & Morley JE (2008, May). Obesity and hypertriglyceridemia produce cognitive impairment. Endocrinology, 149(5), 2628–2636. 10.1210/en.2007-1722 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Farruggia MC, & Small DM (2019). Effects of adiposity and metabolic dysfunction on cognition: A review. Physiol Behav, 208, 112578. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2019.112578 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fedoroff ID, Polivy J, & Herman CP (1997). The effect of pre-exposure to food cues on the eating behavior of restrained and unrestrained eaters. Appetite, 28(1), 33–47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferrario CR, & Reagan LP (2018, Jul 1). Insulin-mediated synaptic plasticity in the CNS: Anatomical, functional and temporal contexts. Neuropharmacology, 136(Pt B), 182–191. 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2017.12.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fletcher BR, Calhoun ME, Rapp PR, & Shapiro ML (2006, Feb 1). Fornix lesions decouple the induction of hippocampal arc transcription from behavior but not plasticity. J Neurosci, 26(5), 1507–1515. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4441-05.2006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foletto KC, Melo Batista BA, Neves AM, de Matos Feijo F, Ballard CR, Marques Ribeiro MF, & Bertoluci MC (2016, Jan 1). Sweet taste of saccharin induces weight gain without increasing caloric intake, not related to insulin-resistance in Wistar rats. Appetite, 96, 604–610. 10.1016/j.appet.2015.11.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fortin SM, Lipsky RK, Lhamo R, Chen J, Kim E, Borner T, Schmidt HD, & Hayes MR (2020, Mar 4). GABA neurons in the nucleus tractus solitarius express GLP-1 receptors and mediate anorectic effects of liraglutide in rats. Sci Transl Med, 12(533). 10.1126/scitranslmed.aay8071 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Francis HM, & Stevenson RJ (2011, Dec). Higher reported saturated fat and refined sugar intake is associated with reduced hippocampal-dependent memory and sensitivity to interoceptive signals. Behavioral Neuroscience, 125(6), 943–955. https://doi.org/2011-24000-001 [pii] 10.1037/a0025998 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Francis HM, Stevenson RJ, Oaten MJ, Mahmut MK, & Yeomans MR (2017). The Immediate and Delayed Effects of TV: Impacts of Gender and Processed-Food Intake History. Front Psychol, 8, 1616. 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01616 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frith E, Shivappa N, Mann JR, Hébert JR, Wirth MD, & Loprinzi PD (2018). Dietary inflammatory index and memory function: population-based national sample of elderly Americans. Br J Nutr, 119(5), 552. 10.1017/S0007114517003804 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fu Z, Wu J, Nesil T, Li MD, Aylor KW, & Liu Z (2017, 02). Long-term high-fat diet induces hippocampal microvascular insulin resistance and cognitive dysfunction. Am J Physiol Endocrinol Metab, 312(2), E89–E97. 10.1152/ajpendo.00297.2016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fulton S, Pissios P, Manchon RP, Stiles L, Frank L, Pothos EN, Maratos-Flier E, & Flier JS (2006, Sep 21). Leptin regulation of the mesoaccumbens dopamine pathway. Neuron, 51(6), 811–822. https://doi.org/S0896-6273(06)00686-6 [pii] 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.09.006 [doi] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gardner RS, Suarez DF, Robinson-Burton NK, Rudnicky CJ, Gulati A, Ascoli GA, & Dumas TC (2016, 05). Differential Arc expression in the hippocampus and striatum during the transition from attentive to automatic navigation on a plus maze. Neurobiol Learn Mem, 131, 36–45. 10.1016/j.nlm.2016.03.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gasbarri A, Pompili A, Packard MG, & Tomaz C (2014, Oct). Habit learning and memory in mammals: behavioral and neural characteristics. Neurobiol Learn Mem, 114, 198–208. 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.06.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Geha P, Cecchi G, Todd Constable R, Abdallah C, & Small DM (2017). Reorganization of brain connectivity in obesity. Hum Brain Mapp, 38(3), 1403–1420. 10.1002/hbm.23462 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gerges NZ, Aleisa AM, & Alkadhi KA (2003). Impaired long-term potentiation in obese zucker rats: possible involvement of presynaptic mechanism. Neuroscience, 120(2), 535–539. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12890522 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gibson EL, Barr S, & Jeanes YM (2013). Habitual fat intake predicts memory function in younger women. Front Hum Neurosci, 7, 838. 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00838 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gil A, Olza J, Gil-Campos M, Gomez-Llorente C, & Aguilera CM (2011). Is adipose tissue metabolically different at different sites? Int J Pediatr Obes, 6(sup1), 13–20. 10.3109/17477166.2011.604326 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gladding JM, Abbott KN, Antoniadis CP, Stuart A, & Begg DP (2018). The Effect of Intrahippocampal Insulin Infusion on Spatial Cognitive Function and Markers of Neuroinflammation in Diet-induced Obesity. Front Endocrinol (Lausanne), 9, 752. 10.3389/fendo.2018.00752 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glykys J, Peng Z, Chandra D, Homanics GE, Houser CR, & Mody I (2007, Jan). A new naturally occurring GABA(A) receptor subunit partnership with high sensitivity to ethanol. Nat Neurosci, 10(1), 40–48. 10.1038/nn1813 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grayson BE, Fitzgerald MF, Hakala-Finch AP, Ferris VM, Begg DP, Tong J, Woods SC, Seeley RJ, Davidson TL, & Benoit SC (2014, Mar). Improvements in hippocampal-dependent memory and microglial infiltration with calorie restriction and gastric bypass surgery, but not with vertical sleeve gastrectomy. International journal of obesity, 38(3), 349–356. 10.1038/ijo.2013.100 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greenwood CE, & Winocur G (1990, Jan). Learning and memory impairment in rats fed a high saturated fat diet. Behav Neural Biol, 53(1), 74–87. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/2302144 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grill HJ, & Hayes MR (2012, Sep 5). Hindbrain neurons as an essential hub in the neuroanatomically distributed control of energy balance. Cell metabolism, 16(3), 296–309. 10.1016/j.cmet.2012.06.015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grillo CA, Woodruff JL, Macht VA, & Reagan LP (2019, Aug). Insulin resistance and hippocampal dysfunction: Disentangling peripheral and brain causes from consequences. Exp Neurol, 318, 71–77. 10.1016/j.expneurol.2019.04.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gu Y, Nieves JW, Luchsinger JA, & Scarmeas N (2011). Dietary inflammation factor rating system and risk of Alzheimer disease in elders. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord, 25(2), 149. 10.1097/WAD.0b013e3181ff3c6a [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gunstad J, Strain G, Devlin MJ, Wing R, Cohen RA, Paul RH, Crosby RD, & Mitchell JE (2011, Jul-Aug). Improved memory function 12 weeks after bariatric surgery. Surg Obes Relat Dis, 7(4), 465–472. 10.1016/j.soard.2010.09.015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guo DH, Yamamoto M, Hernandez CM, Khodadadi H, Baban B, & Stranahan AM (2020, 04). Visceral adipose NLRP3 impairs cognition in obesity via IL-1R1 on CX3CR1+ cells. J Clin Invest, 130(4), 1961–1976. 10.1172/JCI126078 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gustaw-Rothenberg K (2009). Dietary patterns associated with Alzheimer’s disease: population based study. Int J Environ Res Public Health, 6(4), 1335–1340. 10.3390/ijerph6041335 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guzowski JF, Lyford GL, Stevenson GD, Houston FP, McGaugh JL, Worley PF, & Barnes CA (2000, Jun 1). Inhibition of activity-dependent arc protein expression in the rat hippocampus impairs the maintenance of long-term potentiation and the consolidation of long-term memory. J Neurosci, 20(11), 3993–4001. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10818134 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guzowski JF, McNaughton BL, Barnes CA, & Worley PF (2001, Oct). Imaging neural activity with temporal and cellular resolution using FISH. Curr Opin Neurobiol, 11(5), 579–584. 10.1016/s0959-4388(00)00252-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guzowski JF, Miyashita T, Chawla MK, Sanderson J, Maes LI, Houston FP, Lipa P, McNaughton BL, Worley PF, & Barnes CA (2006, Jan 24). Recent behavioral history modifies coupling between cell activity and Arc gene transcription in hippocampal CA1 neurons. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 103(4), 1077–1082. 10.1073/pnas.0505519103 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guzowski JF, Setlow B, Wagner EK, & McGaugh JL (2001, Jul). Experience-dependent gene expression in the rat hippocampus after spatial learning: a comparison of the immediate-early genes Arc, c-fos, and zif268. J Neurosci, 21(14), 5089–5098. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11438584 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hagan KE, Alasmar A, Exum A, Chinn B, & Forbush KT (2020). A systematic review and meta-analysis of attentional bias toward food in individuals with overweight and obesity. Appetite, 151, 104710. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hallböök T, Ji S, Maudsley S, & Martin B (2012). The effects of the ketogenic diet on behavior and cognition. Epilepsy research, 100(3), 304–309. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hamer M, & Batty GD (2019). Association of body mass index and waist-to-hip ratio with brain structure: UK Biobank study. Neurology, 92(6), e594–e600. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Han X, & Boyden ES (2007). Multiple-color optical activation, silencing, and desynchronization of neural activity, with single-spike temporal resolution. PLoS One, 2(3), e299. 10.1371/journal.pone.0000299 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hannapel R, Ramesh J, Ross A, LaLumiere RT, Roseberry AG, & Parent MB (2019, Jan-Feb). Postmeal Optogenetic Inhibition of Dorsal or Ventral Hippocampal Pyramidal Neurons Increases Future Intake. eNeuro, 6(1). 10.1523/ENEURO.0457-18.2018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hannapel RC, Henderson YH, Nalloor R, Vazdarjanova A, & Parent MB (2017, Jan 25). Ventral hippocampal neurons inhibit postprandial energy intake. Hippocampus. 10.1002/hipo.22692 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hao S, Dey A, Yu X, & Stranahan AM (2016, Jan). Dietary obesity reversibly induces synaptic stripping by microglia and impairs hippocampal plasticity. Brain Behav Immun, 51, 230–239. 10.1016/j.bbi.2015.08.023 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hartanto A, & Yong JC (2018). Measurement matters: Higher waist-to-hip ratio but not body mass index is associated with deficits in executive functions and episodic memory. PeerJ, 6, e5624. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hayden KM, Beavers DP, Steck SE, Hebert JR, Tabung FK, Shivappa N, Casanova R, Manson JE, Padula CB, & Salmoirago-Blotcher E (2017). The association between an inflammatory diet and global cognitive function and incident dementia in older women: The Women’s Health Initiative Memory Study. Alzheimers Dement, 13(11), 1187–1196. 10.1016/j.jalz.2017.04.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hayes MR, Leichner TM, Zhao S, Lee GS, Chowansky A, Zimmer D, De Jonghe BC, Kanoski SE, Grill HJ, & Bence KK (2011, Mar 2). Intracellular signals mediating the food intake-suppressive effects of hindbrain glucagon-like peptide-1 receptor activation [Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. Cell Metab, 13(3), 320–330. 10.1016/j.cmet.2011.02.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hayes MR, Skibicka KP, & Grill HJ (2008, Aug). Caudal brainstem processing is sufficient for behavioral, sympathetic, and parasympathetic responses driven by peripheral and hindbrain glucagon-like-peptide-1 receptor stimulation. Endocrinology, 149(8), 4059–4068. https://doi.org/en.2007-1743 [pii] 10.1210/en.2007-1743 [doi] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hayes MR, Skibicka KP, Leichner TM, Guarnieri DJ, DiLeone RJ, Bence KK, & Grill HJ (2010, Jan). Endogenous leptin signaling in the caudal nucleus tractus solitarius and area postrema is required for energy balance regulation. Cell metabolism, 11(1), 77–83. https://doi.org/S1550-4131(09)00335-0 [pii] 10.1016/j.cmet.2009.10.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hebben N, Corkin S, Eichenbaum H, & Shedlack K (1985). Diminished ability to interpret and report internal states after bilateral medial temporal resection: case HM. Behavioral neuroscience, 99(6), 1031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Henderson YO, Nalloor R, Vazdarjanova A, Murphy AZ, & Parent MB (2017, 05). Sex-dependent effects of early life inflammatory pain on sucrose intake and sucrose-associated hippocampal Arc expression in adult rats. Physiol Behav, 173, 1–8. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2017.01.025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Henderson YO, Nalloor R, Vazdarjanova A, & Parent MB (2016, Mar). Sweet orosensation induces Arc expression in dorsal hippocampal CA1 neurons in an experience-dependent manner. Hippocampus, 26(3), 405–413. 10.1002/hipo.22532 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Henderson YO, Smith GP, & Parent MB (2013, Jan). Hippocampal neurons inhibit meal onset. Hippocampus, 23(1), 100–107. 10.1002/hipo.22062 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hernández-Ramírez S, Osorio-Gómez D, Escobar ML, Rodríguez-Durán L, Velasco M, Bermúdez-Rattoni F, Hiriart M, & Guzmán-Ramos KR (2021, Feb). Catecholaminergic stimulation restores high-sucrose diet-induced hippocampal dysfunction. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 127, 105178. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2021.105178 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heyward FD, Gilliam D, Coleman MA, Gavin CF, Wang J, Kaas G, Trieu R, Lewis J, Moulden J, & Sweatt JD (2016, Jan 27). Obesity Weighs down Memory through a Mechanism Involving the Neuroepigenetic Dysregulation of Sirt1. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 36(4), 1324–1335. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1934-15.2016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Higgs S (2002). Memory for recent eating and its influence on subsequent food intake. Appetite, 39(2), 159–166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Higgs S (2015). Manipulations of attention during eating and their effects on later snack intake. Appetite, 92, 287–294. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Higgs S, & Donohoe JE (2011). Focusing on food during lunch enhances lunch memory and decreases later snack intake. Appetite, 57(1), 202–206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Higgs S, & Jones A (2013). Prolonged chewing at lunch decreases later snack intake. Appetite, 62, 91–95. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Higgs S, Williamson AC, & Attwood AS (2008). Recall of recent lunch and its effect on subsequent snack intake. Physiol Behav, 94(3), 454–462. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2008.02.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Higgs S, Williamson AC, Rotshtein P, & Humphreys GW (2008). Sensory-specific satiety is intact in amnesics who eat multiple meals. Psychological Science, 19(7), 623–628. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Higgs S, & Woodward M (2009). Television watching during lunch increases afternoon snack intake of young women. Appetite, 52(1), 39–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hinton EC, Leary SD, Comlek L, Rogers PJ, & Hamilton-Shield JP (2020). How full am I? The effect of rating fullness during eating on food intake, eating speed and relationship with satiety responsiveness. Appetite, 157, 104998. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hock BJ Jr., & Bunsey MD (1998, Sep 1). Differential effects of dorsal and ventral hippocampal lesions. J Neurosci, 18(17), 7027–7032. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-17-07027.1998 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holloway CJ, Cochlin LE, Emmanuel Y, Murray A, Codreanu I, Edwards LM, Szmigielski C, Tyler DJ, Knight NS, Saxby BK, Lambert B, Thompson C, Neubauer S, & Clarke K (2011). A high-fat diet impairs cardiac high-energy phosphate metabolism and cognitive function in healthy human subjects. Am J Clin Nutr, 93(4), 748–755. 10.3945/ajcn.110.002758 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holst JJ (2007, Oct). The physiology of glucagon-like peptide 1. Physiol Rev, 87(4), 1409–1439. https://doi.org/87/4/1409 [pii] 10.1152/physrev.00034.2006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holt MK, Richards JE, Cook DR, Brierley DI, Williams DL, Reimann F, Gribble FM, & Trapp S (2019, Jan). Preproglucagon Neurons in the Nucleus of the Solitary Tract Are the Main Source of Brain GLP-1, Mediate Stress-Induced Hypophagia, and Limit Unusually Large Intakes of Food. Diabetes, 68(1), 21–33. 10.2337/db18-0729 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hommel JD, Trinko R, Sears RM, Georgescu D, Liu ZW, Gao XB, Thurmon JJ, Marinelli M, & DiLeone RJ (2006, Sep 21). Leptin receptor signaling in midbrain dopamine neurons regulates feeding. Neuron, 51(6), 801–810. https://doi.org/S0896-6273(06)00645-3 [pii] 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.08.023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Howard AD, Feighner SD, Cully DF, Arena JP, Liberator PA, Rosenblum CI, Hamelin M, Hreniuk DL, Palyha OC, Anderson J, Paress PS, Diaz C, Chou M, Liu KK, McKee KK, Pong SS, Chaung LY, Elbrecht A, Dashkevicz M, Heavens R, Rigby M, Sirinathsinghji DJ, Dean DC, Melillo DG, Patchett AA, Nargund R, Griffin PR, DeMartino JA, Gupta SK, Schaeffer JM, Smith RG, & Van der Ploeg LH (1996, Aug 16). A receptor in pituitary and hypothalamus that functions in growth hormone release. Science, 273(5277), 974–977. 10.1126/science.273.5277.974 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hsu TM, Hahn JD, Konanur VR, Lam A, & Kanoski SE (2015, Jan). Hippocampal GLP-1 receptors influence food intake, meal size, and effort-based responding for food through volume transmission. Neuropsychopharmacology, 40(2), 327–337. 10.1038/npp.2014.175 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hsu TM, Noble EE, Liu CM, Cortella AM, Konanur VR, Suarez AN, Reiner DJ, Hahn JD, Hayes MR, & Kanoski SE (2018, Jul). A hippocampus to prefrontal cortex neural pathway inhibits food motivation through glucagon-like peptide-1 signaling. Mol Psychiatry, 23(7), 1555–1565. 10.1038/mp.2017.91 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hsu TM, Suarez AN, & Kanoski SE (2016, Aug 1). Ghrelin: A link between memory and ingestive behavior. Physiol Behav, 162, 10–17. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2016.03.039 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hu H, Real E, Takamiya K, Kang MG, Ledoux J, Huganir RL, & Malinow R (2007, Oct 5). Emotion enhances learning via norepinephrine regulation of AMPA-receptor trafficking. Cell, 131(1), 160–173. 10.1016/j.cell.2007.09.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hudgins C, & Otto T (2019, 05). Hippocampal Arc protein expression and conditioned fear. Neurobiol Learn Mem, 161, 175–191. 10.1016/j.nlm.2019.04.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hunsaker MR, Lee B, & Kesner RP (2008, Apr). Evaluating the temporal context of episodic memory: the role of CA3 and CA1. Behav Brain Res, 188(2), 310–315. 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.11.015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Izquierdo I, Bevilaqua LR, Rossato JI, da Silva WC, Bonini J, Medina JH, & Cammarota M (2008, Oct). The molecular cascades of long-term potentiation underlie memory consolidation of one-trial avoidance in the CA1 region of the dorsal hippocampus, but not in the basolateral amygdala or the neocortex. Neurotox Res, 14(2–3), 273–294. 10.1007/BF03033816 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Izquierdo I, da Cunha C, Rosat R, Jerusalinsky D, Ferreira MB, & Medina JH (1992, Jul). Neurotransmitter receptors involved in post-training memory processing by the amygdala, medial septum, and hippocampus of the rat. Behav Neural Biol, 58(1), 16–26. 10.1016/0163-1047(92)90847-w [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Izquierdo I, & McGaugh JL (2000, Nov). Behavioural pharmacology and its contribution to the molecular basis of memory consolidation. Behav Pharmacol, 11(7–8), 517–534. 10.1097/00008877-200011000-00001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jacka FN, Cherbuin N, Anstey KJ, Sachdev P, & Butterworth P (2015). Western diet is associated with a smaller hippocampus: a longitudinal investigation. BMC Medicine, 13(1), 1–8. 10.1186/s12916-015-0461-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jerusalinsky D, Ferreira MB, Walz R, Da Silva RC, Bianchin M, Ruschel AC, Zanatta MS, Medina JH, & Izquierdo I (1992, Jul). Amnesia by post-training infusion of glutamate receptor antagonists into the amygdala, hippocampus, and entorhinal cortex. Behav Neural Biol, 58(1), 76–80. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/1417675 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jones S, Luo S, Dorton HM, Angelo B, Yunker AG, Monterosso JR, & Page KA (2021). Evidence of a Role for the Hippocampus in Food-Cue Processing and the Association with Body Weight and Dietary Added Sugar. Obesity (Silver Spring), 29(2), 370–378. 10.1002/oby.23085 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kanoski SE, & Davidson TL (2011, Apr 18). Western diet consumption and cognitive impairment: links to hippocampal dysfunction and obesity. Physiology & Behavior, 103(1), 59–68. https://doi.org/S0031-9384(10)00450-6 [pii] 10.1016/j.physbeh.2010.12.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kanoski SE, Fortin SM, Ricks KM, & Grill HJ (2013, May 1). Ghrelin Signaling in the Ventral Hippocampus Stimulates Learned and Motivational Aspects of Feeding via PI3K-Akt Signaling. Biological psychiatry, 73(9), 915–923. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.07.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kanoski SE, & Grill HJ (2017, May 1). Hippocampus Contributions to Food Intake Control: Mnemonic, Neuroanatomical, and Endocrine Mechanisms. Biol Psychiatry, 81(9), 748–756. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.09.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kanoski SE, Hayes MR, Greenwald HS, Fortin SM, Gianessi CA, Gilbert JR, & Grill HJ (2011, Aug). Hippocampal leptin signaling reduces food intake and modulates food-related memory processing. Neuropsychopharmacology, 36(9), 1859–1870. 10.1038/npp.2011.70npp201170 [pii] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kanoski SE, T.M. H, & Pennell S (2014). Obesity, Western Diet Intake, and Cognitive Impairmen. In Meester WD (Ed.), Omega-3 Fatty Acids in Brain and Neurological Health. Elsevier. 10.1016/B978-0-12-410527-0.00005-3 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Kanoski SE, Zhao S, Guarnieri DJ, Dileone RJ, Yan J, De Jonghe BC, Bence KK, Hayes MR, & Grill HJ (2012, Aug). Endogenous leptin receptor signaling in the medial nucleus tractus solitarius affects meal size and potentiates intestinal satiation signals. Am J Physiol Endocrinol Metab, 303(4), E496–503. 10.1152/ajpendo.00205.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karimi SA, Komaki A, Salehi I, Sarihi A, & Shahidi S (2015, Apr). Role of group II metabotropic glutamate receptors (mGluR2/3) blockade on long-term potentiation in the dentate gyrus region of hippocampus in rats fed with high-fat diet. Neurochem Res, 40(4), 811–817. 10.1007/s11064-015-1531-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karimi SA, Salehi I, Komaki A, Sarihi A, Zarei M, & Shahidi S (2013, Nov 20). Effect of high-fat diet and antioxidants on hippocampal long-term potentiation in rats: an in vivo study. Brain Res, 1539, 1–6. 10.1016/j.brainres.2013.09.029 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelly MP, & Deadwyler SA (2002). Acquisition of a novel behavior induces higher levels of Arc mRNA than does overtrained performance. Neuroscience, 110(4), 617–626. 10.1016/s0306-4522(01)00605-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelly MP, & Deadwyler SA (2003, Jul 23). Experience-dependent regulation of the immediate-early gene arc differs across brain regions. J Neurosci, 23(16), 6443–6451. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12878684 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kensinger EA (2009). Remembering the Details: Effects of Emotion. Emot Rev, 1(2), 99–113. 10.1177/1754073908100432 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kent K, Hess K, Tonegawa S, & Small SA (2007). CA3 NMDA receptors are required for experience-dependent shifts in hippocampal activity. Hippocampus, 17(10), 1003–1011. 10.1002/hipo.20332 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kesse-Guyot E, Assmann KE, Andreeva VA, Touvier M, Neufcourt L, Shivappa N, Hébert JR, Wirth MD, Hercberg S, & Galan P (2017). Long-term association between the dietary inflammatory index and cognitive functioning: findings from the SU. VI. MAX study. Eur J Nutr, 56(4), 1647–1655. 10.1007/s00394-016-1211-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Khaodhiar L, McCowen KC, & Blackburn GL (1999). Obesity and its comorbid conditions. Clin Cornerstone, 2(3), 17–31. 10.1016/s1098-3597(99)90002-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kheirouri S, & Alizadeh M (2019). Dietary Inflammatory Potential and the Risk of Incident Depression in Adults: A Systematic Review. Adv Nutr, 10(1), 9–18. 10.1093/advances/nmy100 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim D-H, Woods SC, & Seeley RJ (2012). Hypothalamic Akt/PKB signaling in regulation of food intake. Front Biosci (Schol Ed), 4(3), 953. 10.2741/s311 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim JY, & Kang SW (2017). Relationships between dietary intake and cognitive function in healthy Korean children and adolescents. J Lifestyle Med, 7(1), 10. 10.15280/jlm.2017.7.1.10 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kinzig KP, D’Alessio DA, & Seeley RJ (2002, Dec 1). The diverse roles of specific GLP-1 receptors in the control of food intake and the response to visceral illness. J Neurosci, 22(23), 10470–10476. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-23-10470.2002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kohl MM, Shipton OA, Deacon RM, Rawlins JN, Deisseroth K, & Paulsen O (2011, Nov). Hemisphere-specific optogenetic stimulation reveals left-right asymmetry of hippocampal plasticity. Nat Neurosci, 14(11), 1413–1415. 10.1038/nn.2915 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Korb E, & Finkbeiner S (2011, Nov). Arc in synaptic plasticity: from gene to behavior. Trends Neurosci, 34(11), 591–598. 10.1016/j.tins.2011.08.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kreisler AD, & Rinaman L (2016, May 15). Hindbrain glucagon-like peptide-1 neurons track intake volume and contribute to injection stress-induced hypophagia in meal-entrained rats. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol, 310(10), R906–916. 10.1152/ajpregu.00243.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kristensen AS, Jenkins MA, Banke TG, Schousboe A, Makino Y, Johnson RC, Huganir R, & Traynelis SF (2011, Jun). Mechanism of Ca2+/calmodulin-dependent kinase II regulation of AMPA receptor gating. Nat Neurosci, 14(6), 727–735. 10.1038/nn.2804 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kushner LR, & Mook DG (1984, Nov). Behavioral correlates of oral and postingestive satiety in the rat. Physiol Behav, 33(5), 713–718. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/6522491 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lawrence GD (2021, Jun 1). Perspective: The Saturated Fat-Unsaturated Oil Dilemma: Relations of Dietary Fatty Acids and Serum Cholesterol, Atherosclerosis, Inflammation, Cancer, and All-Cause Mortality. Adv Nutr, 12(3), 647–656. 10.1093/advances/nmab013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Layfield D, Sidell N, Abdullahi A, & Newman EL (2020, 02). Dorsal hippocampus not always necessary in a radial arm maze delayed win-shift task. Hippocampus, 30(2), 121–129. 10.1002/hipo.23141 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee HK, Barbarosie M, Kameyama K, Bear MF, & Huganir RL (2000, Jun 22). Regulation of distinct AMPA receptor phosphorylation sites during bidirectional synaptic plasticity. Nature, 405(6789), 955–959. 10.1038/35016089 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leidy HJ, Lepping RJ, Savage CR, & Harris CT (2011). Neural responses to visual food stimuli after a normal vs. higher protein breakfast in breakfast-skipping teens: a pilot fMRI study. Obesity (Silver Spring), 19(10), 2019–2025. 10.1038/oby.2011.108 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leng X, Xiao M, Bian Z, Zhang Y, Shi P, & Chen H (2021). Episodic memory for food and non-food cues in females with obesity. Eat Behav, 40, 101472. 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2020.101472 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leshan RL, Bjornholm M, Munzberg H, & Myers MG Jr. (2006, Aug). Leptin receptor signaling and action in the central nervous system [Review]. Obesity, 14 Suppl 5, 208S–212S. 10.1038/oby.2006.310 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levin F, Edholm T, Schmidt PT, Gryback P, Jacobsson H, Degerblad M, Hoybye C, Holst JJ, Rehfeld JF, Hellstrom PM, & Naslund E (2006, Sep). Ghrelin stimulates gastric emptying and hunger in normal-weight humans. J Clin Endocrinol Metab, 91(9), 3296–3302. 10.1210/jc.2005-2638 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li XL, Aou S, Oomura Y, Hori N, Fukunaga K, & Hori T (2002). Impairment of long-term potentiation and spatial memory in leptin receptor-deficient rodents. Neuroscience, 113(3), 607–615. https://doi.org/S0306452202001628 [pii] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lin YT, Hsieh TY, Tsai TC, Chen CC, Huang CC, & Hsu KS (2018, Jan 31). Conditional Deletion of Hippocampal CA2/CA3a Oxytocin Receptors Impairs the Persistence of Long-Term Social Recognition Memory in Mice. J Neurosci, 38(5), 1218–1231. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1896-17.2017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lindqvist A, Mohapel P, Bouter B, Frielingsdorf H, Pizzo D, Brundin P, & Erlanson-Albertsson C (2006, Dec). High-fat diet impairs hippocampal neurogenesis in male rats. European Journal of Neurology, 13(12), 1385–1388. 10.1111/j.1468-1331.2006.01500.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Link W, Konietzko U, Kauselmann G, Krug M, Schwanke B, Frey U, & Kuhl D (1995, Jun 6). Somatodendritic expression of an immediate early gene is regulated by synaptic activity. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 92(12), 5734–5738. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/7777577 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lopez-Ferreras L, Richard JE, Noble EE, Eerola K, Anderberg RH, Olandersson K, Taing L, Kanoski SE, Hayes MR, & Skibicka KP (2018, May). Lateral hypothalamic GLP-1 receptors are critical for the control of food reinforcement, ingestive behavior and body weight. Mol Psychiatry, 23(5), 1157–1168. 10.1038/mp.2017.187 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Loprinzi PD, & Frith E (2018). Obesity and episodic memory function. J Physiol Sci, 68(4), 321–331. 10.1007/s12576-018-0612-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lorenzini CA, Baldi E, Bucherelli C, Sacchetti B, & Tassoni G (1996, Aug 19). Role of dorsal hippocampus in acquisition, consolidation and retrieval of rat’s passive avoidance response: a tetrodotoxin functional inactivation study. Brain Res, 730(1–2), 32–39. 10.1016/0006-8993(96)00427-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lyford GL, Yamagata K, Kaufmann WE, Barnes CA, Sanders LK, Copeland NG, Gilbert DJ, Jenkins NA, Lanahan AA, & Worley PF (1995, Feb). Arc, a growth factor and activity-regulated gene, encodes a novel cytoskeleton-associated protein that is enriched in neuronal dendrites. Neuron, 14(2), 433–445. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/7857651 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lynch KM, Page KA, Shi Y, Xiang AH, Toga AW, & Clark KA (2021). The effect of body mass index on hippocampal morphology and memory performance in late childhood and adolescence. Hippocampus, 31(2), 189–200. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maayan L, Hoogendoorn C, Sweat V, & Convit A (2011, Jul). Disinhibited eating in obese adolescents is associated with orbitofrontal volume reductions and executive dysfunction. Obesity (Silver Spring), 19(7), 1382–1387. 10.1038/oby.2011.15 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maggio N, Shavit Stein E, & Segal M (2015, Nov). Ischemic LTP: NMDA-dependency and dorso/ventral distribution within the hippocampus. Hippocampus, 25(11), 1465–1471. 10.1002/hipo.22467 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Majchrzak M, & Di Scala G (2000). GABA and muscimol as reversible inactivation tools in learning and memory. Neural Plast, 7(1–2), 19–29. 10.1155/NP.2000.19 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mani BK, Walker AK, Lopez Soto EJ, Raingo J, Lee CE, Perello M, Andrews ZB, & Zigman JM (2014, Nov 1). Neuroanatomical characterization of a growth hormone secretagogue receptor-green fluorescent protein reporter mouse. J Comp Neurol, 522(16), 3644–3666. 10.1002/cne.23627 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mann EO, & Mody I (2010, Feb). Control of hippocampal gamma oscillation frequency by tonic inhibition and excitation of interneurons. Nat Neurosci, 13(2), 205–212. 10.1038/nn.2464 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martin JH (1991, Jun 24). Autoradiographic estimation of the extent of reversible inactivation produced by microinjection of lidocaine and muscimol in the rat [Research Support, U.S. Gov’t, P.H.S.]. Neurosci Lett, 127(2), 160–164. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/1881625 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martin JH, & Ghez C (1999, Jan). Pharmacological inactivation in the analysis of the central control of movement. J Neurosci Methods, 86(2), 145–159. 10.1016/s0165-0270(98)00163-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mateos L, Akterin S, Gil-Bea FJ, Spulber S, Rahman A, Björkhem I, Schultzberg M, Flores-Morales A, & Cedazo-Mínguez A (2009, Jan). Activity-regulated cytoskeleton-associated protein in rodent brain is down-regulated by high fat diet in vivo and by 27-hydroxycholesterol in vitro. Brain Pathol, 19(1), 69–80. 10.1111/j.1750-3639.2008.00174.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mavanji V, Butterick TA, Duffy CM, Nixon JP, Billington CJ, & Kotz CM (2017, Dec). Orexin/hypocretin treatment restores hippocampal-dependent memory in orexin-deficient mice. Neurobiol Learn Mem, 146, 21–30. 10.1016/j.nlm.2017.10.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McFadden T, Musaus M, Nelsen JL, Martin K, Jones N, Smith P, Kugler H, & Jarome TJ (2020, 09). Dysregulation of protein degradation in the hippocampus is associated with impaired spatial memory during the development of obesity. Behav Brain Res, 393, 112787. 10.1016/j.bbr.2020.112787 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGaugh JL (1966, Sep 16). Time-dependent processes in memory storage. Science, 153(3742), 1351–1358. 10.1126/science.153.3742.1351 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGaugh JL (2000, Jan 14). Memory--a century of consolidation [Research Support, U.S. Gov’t, P.H.S. Review]. Science, 287(5451), 248–251. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10634773 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGaugh JL, & Izquierdo I (2000, Jun). The contribution of pharmacology to research on the mechanisms of memory formation. Trends Pharmacol Sci, 21(6), 208–210. 10.1016/s0165-6147(00)01473-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McLean FH, Grant C, Morris AC, Horgan GW, Polanski AJ, Allan K, Campbell FM, Langston RF, & Williams LM (2018). Rapid and reversible impairment of episodic memory by a high-fat diet in mice. Sci Rep, 8(1), 1–9. 10.1038/s41598-018-30265-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McMahon LR, & Wellman PJ (1998, Jan). PVN infusion of GLP-1-(7–36) amide suppresses feeding but does not induce aversion or alter locomotion in rats. Am J Physiol, 274(1 Pt 2), R23–29. 10.1152/ajpregu.1998.274.1.R23 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Merchenthaler I, Lane M, & Shughrue P (1999, Jan 11). Distribution of pre-pro-glucagon and glucagon-like peptide-1 receptor messenger RNAs in the rat central nervous system. J Comp Neurol, 403(2), 261–280. [pii] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mestre ZL, Bischoff-Grethe A, Eichen DM, Wierenga CE, Strong D, & Boutelle KN (2017). Hippocampal atrophy and altered brain responses to pleasant tastes among obese compared with healthy weight children. Int J Obes (Lond), 41(10), 1496–1502. 10.1038/ijo.2017.130 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Metzler-Baddeley C, Baddeley RJ, Jones DK, Aggleton JP, & O’Sullivan MJ (2013). Individual differences in fornix microstructure and body mass index. PLoS One, 8(3), e59849. 10.1371/journal.pone.0059849 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller LA, Crosby RD, Galioto R, Strain G, Devlin MJ, Wing R, Cohen RA, Paul RH, Mitchell JE, & Gunstad J (2013). Bariatric surgery patients exhibit improved memory function 12 months postoperatively. Obes Surg, 23(10), 1527–1535. 10.1007/s11695-013-0970-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mittal D, Stevenson RJ, Oaten MJ, & Miller LA (2011). Snacking while watching TV impairs food recall and promotes food intake on a later TV free test meal. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 25(6), 871–877. 10.1002/acp.1760 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Moncada D, & Viola H (2006, Jul). Phosphorylation state of CREB in the rat hippocampus: a molecular switch between spatial novelty and spatial familiarity? Neurobiol Learn Mem, 86(1), 9–18. 10.1016/j.nlm.2005.12.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moncada D, & Viola H (2008, Nov). PKMzeta inactivation induces spatial familiarity. Learn Mem, 15(11), 810–814. 10.1101/lm.1139508 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mook DG, Bryner CA, Rainey DL, & Wall CL (1980). Release of feeding by the sweet tasted in rats: oropharyngeal satiety. Appetite, 1, 299–315. [Google Scholar]
- Mook DG, Kushner LR, & Kushner BD (1981). Release of Feeding by the Sweet Taste in Rats: The Specificity of Oral Satiety. Appetite, 2(4), 267–280. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moran C, Phan TG, Chen J, Blizzard L, Beare R, Venn A, Münch G, Wood AG, Forbes J, & Greenaway TM (2013). Brain atrophy in type 2 diabetes: regional distribution and influence on cognition. Diabetes Care, 36(12), 4036–4042. 10.2337/dc13-0143 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morris J, Vi CT, Obrist M, Forster S, & Yeomans MR (2020). Ingested but not perceived: response to satiety cues disrupted by perceptual load. Appetite, 155, 104813. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morris MC (2004). Diet and Alzheimer’s disease: what the evidence shows. Medscape General Medicine, 6(1), 48. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morris R (2007). Theories of Hippocampal Function. In Anderson P, Morris R, Amaral D, Bliss T, & O’Keefe J (Eds.), The Hippocampus Book (pp. 872). Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Morris RG (2013, Nov). NMDA receptors and memory encoding. Neuropharmacology, 74, 32–40. 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2013.04.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moser MB, & Moser EI (1998). Functional differentiation in the hippocampus. Hippocampus, 8(6), 608–619. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muñoz-García MI, Martínez-González MA, Martín-Moreno JM, Razquin C, Cervantes S, Guillén-Grima F, & Toledo E (2020). Sugar-sweetened and artificially-sweetened beverages and changes in cognitive function in the SUN project. Nutr Neurosci, 23(12), 946–954. 10.1080/1028415X.2019.1580919 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murray AM, Hsu F-C, Williamson JD, Bryan RN, Gerstein HC, Sullivan MD, Miller ME, Leng I, Lovato LL, & Launer LJ (2017). ACCORDION MIND: results of the observational extension of the ACCORD MIND randomised trial. Diabetologia, 60(1), 69–80. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murray EA, Wise SP, & Graham KS (2018, Jul 27). Representational specializations of the hippocampus in phylogenetic perspective. Neurosci Lett, 680, 4–12. 10.1016/j.neulet.2017.04.065 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Myers MG Jr., Munzberg H, Leinninger GM, & Leshan RL (2009, Feb). The geometry of leptin action in the brain: more complicated than a simple ARC [Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t Review]. Cell Metab, 9(2), 117–123. 10.1016/j.cmet.2008.12.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nass R, Farhy LS, Liu J, Prudom CE, Johnson ML, Veldhuis P, Pezzoli SS, Oliveri MC, Gaylinn BD, Geysen HM, & Thorner MO (2008, May). Evidence for acyl-ghrelin modulation of growth hormone release in the fed state. J Clin Endocrinol Metab, 93(5), 1988–1994. 10.1210/jc.2007-2234 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ng M, Fleming T, Robinson M, Thomson B, Graetz N, Margono C, Mullany EC, Biryukov S, Abbafati C, Abera SF, Abraham JP, Abu-Rmeileh NM, Achoki T, AlBuhairan FS, Alemu ZA, Alfonso R, Ali MK, Ali R, Guzman NA, Ammar W, Anwari P, Banerjee A, Barquera S, Basu S, Bennett DA, Bhutta Z, Blore J, Cabral N, Nonato IC, Chang JC, Chowdhury R, Courville KJ, Criqui MH, Cundiff DK, Dabhadkar KC, Dandona L, Davis A, Dayama A, Dharmaratne SD, Ding EL, Durrani AM, Esteghamati A, Farzadfar F, Fay DF, Feigin VL, Flaxman A, Forouzanfar MH, Goto A, Green MA, Gupta R, Hafezi-Nejad N, Hankey GJ, Harewood HC, Havmoeller R, Hay S, Hernandez L, Husseini A, Idrisov BT, Ikeda N, Islami F, Jahangir E, Jassal SK, Jee SH, Jeffreys M, Jonas JB, Kabagambe EK, Khalifa SE, Kengne AP, Khader YS, Khang YH, Kim D, Kimokoti RW, Kinge JM, Kokubo Y, Kosen S, Kwan G, Lai T, Leinsalu M, Li Y, Liang X, Liu S, Logroscino G, Lotufo PA, Lu Y, Ma J, Mainoo NK, Mensah GA, Merriman TR, Mokdad AH, Moschandreas J, Naghavi M, Naheed A, Nand D, Narayan KM, Nelson EL, Neuhouser ML, Nisar MI, Ohkubo T, Oti SO, Pedroza A, Prabhakaran D, Roy N, Sampson U, Seo H, Sepanlou SG, Shibuya K, Shiri R, Shiue I, Singh GM, Singh JA, Skirbekk V, Stapelberg NJ, Sturua L, Sykes BL, Tobias M, Tran BX, Trasande L, Toyoshima H, van de Vijver S, Vasankari TJ, Veerman JL, Velasquez-Melendez G, Vlassov VV, Vollset SE, Vos T, Wang C, Wang X, Weiderpass E, Werdecker A, Wright JL, Yang YC, Yatsuya H, Yoon J, Yoon SJ, Zhao Y, Zhou M, Zhu S, Lopez AD, Murray CJ, & Gakidou E (2014, Aug). Global, regional, and national prevalence of overweight and obesity in children and adults during 1980–2013: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2013. Lancet, 384(9945), 766–781. 10.1016/S0140-6736(14)60460-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nguyen JC, Killcross AS, & Jenkins TA (2014). Obesity and cognitive decline: role of inflammation and vascular changes. Front Neurosci, 8, 375. 10.3389/fnins.2014.00375 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Noble EE, Hahn JD, Konanur VR, Hsu TM, Page SJ, Cortella AM, Liu CM, Song MY, Suarez AN, Szujewski CC, Rider D, Clarke JE, Darvas M, Appleyard SM, & Kanoski SE (2018, Jul 3). Control of Feeding Behavior by Cerebral Ventricular Volume Transmission of Melanin-Concentrating Hormone. Cell Metab, 28(1), 55–68 e57. 10.1016/j.cmet.2018.05.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Noble EE, Wang Z, Liu CM, Davis EA, Suarez AN, Stein LM, Tsan L, Terrill SJ, Hsu TM, Jung AH, Raycraft LM, Hahn JD, Darvas M, Cortella AM, Schier LA, Johnson AW, Hayes MR, Holschneider DP, & Kanoski SE (2019, Oct 29). Hypothalamus-hippocampus circuitry regulates impulsivity via melanin-concentrating hormone. Nat Commun, 10(1), 4923. 10.1038/s41467-019-12895-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nouwen A, Chambers A, Chechlacz M, Higgs S, Blissett J, Barrett TG, & Allen HA (2017). Microstructural abnormalities in white and gray matter in obese adolescents with and without type 2 diabetes. NeuroImage: Clinical, 16, 43–51. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Okereke OI, Rosner BA, Kim DH, Kang JH, Cook NR, Manson JE, Buring JE, Willett WC, & Grodstein F (2012). Dietary fat types and 4-year cognitive change in community-dwelling older women. Ann Neurol, 72(1), 124–134. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oldham-Cooper RE, Hardman CA, Nicoll CE, Rogers PJ, & Brunstrom JM (2011). Playing a computer game during lunch affects fullness, memory for lunch, and later snack intake. The American journal of clinical nutrition, 93(2), 308–313. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oliveira AM, Hawk JD, Abel T, & Havekes R (2010, Mar). Post-training reversible inactivation of the hippocampus enhances novel object recognition memory [Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. Learn Mem, 17(3), 155–160. 10.1101/lm.1625310 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ozawa M, Shipley M, Kivimaki M, Singh-Manoux A, & Brunner EJ (2017). Dietary pattern, inflammation and cognitive decline: the Whitehall II prospective cohort study. Clin Nutr, 36(2), 506–512. 10.1016/j.clnu.2016.01.013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Packard MG, & McGaugh JL (1996, Jan). Inactivation of hippocampus or caudate nucleus with lidocaine differentially affects expression of place and response learning. Neurobiol Learn Mem, 65(1), 65–72. 10.1006/nlme.1996.0007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Page AJ, Symonds E, Peiris M, Blackshaw LA, & Young RL (2012, Jul). Peripheral neural targets in obesity. Br J Pharmacol, 166(5), 1537–1558. 10.1111/j.1476-5381.2012.01951.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Panoz-Brown D, Iyer V, Carey LM, Sluka CM, Rajic G, Kestenman J, Gentry M, Brotheridge S, Somekh I, Corbin HE, Tucker KG, Almeida B, Hex SB, Garcia KD, Hohmann AG, & Crystal JD (2018, 05). Replay of Episodic Memories in the Rat. Curr Biol, 28(10), 1628–1634.e1627. 10.1016/j.cub.2018.04.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poletti CE, & Creswell G (1977). Fornix system efferent projections in the squirrel monkey: an experimental degeneration study. J Comp Neurol, 175(1), 101–127. 10.1002/cne.901750107 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Porter D, Faivre E, Flatt PR, Hölscher C, & Gault VA (2012, May). Actions of incretin metabolites on locomotor activity, cognitive function and in vivo hippocampal synaptic plasticity in high fat fed mice. Peptides, 35(1), 1–8. 10.1016/j.peptides.2012.03.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Portero-Tresserra M, Del Olmo N, Marti-Nicolovius M, Guillazo-Blanch G, & Vale-Martinez A (2014, Nov). D-cycloserine prevents relational memory deficits and suppression of long-term potentiation induced by scopolamine in the hippocampus. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol, 24(11), 1798–1807. 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2014.10.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Price GM, Uauy R, Breeze E, Bulpitt CJ, & Fletcher AE (2006). Weight, shape, and mortality risk in older persons: elevated waist-hip ratio, not high body mass index, is associated with a greater risk of death. Am J Clin Nutr, 84(2), 449–460. 10.1093/ajcn/84.1.449 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prickett C, Brennan L, & Stolwyk R (2015, Mar-Apr). Examining the relationship between obesity and cognitive function: a systematic literature review. Obes Res Clin Pract, 9(2), 93–113. 10.1016/j.orcp.2014.05.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pugazhenthi S, Qin L, & Reddy PH (2017). Common neurodegenerative pathways in obesity, diabetes, and Alzheimer’s disease. Biochim Biophys Acta Mol Basis Dis, 1863(5), 1037–1045. 10.1016/j.bbadis.2016.04.017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Renwick AG (1985, Apr-May). The disposition of saccharin in animals and man--a review. Food Chem Toxicol, 23(4–5), 429–435. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&dopt=Citation&list_uids=3891556 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Renwick AG (1986, Oct-Nov). The metabolism of intense sweeteners. Xenobiotica, 16(10–11), 1057–1071. 10.3109/00498258609038983 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Richter FR, Cooper RA, Bays PM, & Simons JS (2016, Oct 25). Distinct neural mechanisms underlie the success, precision, and vividness of episodic memory. Elife, 5. 10.7554/eLife.18260 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rinaman L (1999, Aug). Interoceptive stress activates glucagon-like peptide-1 neurons that project to the hypothalamus. Am J Physiol, 277(2), R582–590. 10.1152/ajpregu.1999.277.2.R582 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson E, Aveyard P, Daley A, Jolly K, Lewis A, Lycett D, & Higgs S (2013, Apr). Eating attentively: a systematic review and meta-analysis of the effect of food intake memory and awareness on eating. Am J Clin Nutr, 97(4), 728–742. 10.3945/ajcn.112.045245 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson E, Higgs S, Daley AJ, Jolly K, Lycett D, Lewis A, & Aveyard P (2013). Development and feasibility testing of a smart phone based attentive eating intervention. BMC Public Health, 13(1), 1–7. 10.1186/1471-2458-13-639 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson E, Kersbergen I, & Higgs S (2014). Eating ‘attentively’ reduces later energy consumption in overweight and obese females. Br J Nutr, 112(4), 657–661. 10.1017/S000711451400141X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rombo DM, Dias RB, Duarte ST, Ribeiro JA, Lamsa KP, & Sebastiao AM (2016, Mar). Adenosine A1 Receptor Suppresses Tonic GABAA Receptor Currents in Hippocampal Pyramidal Cells and in a Defined Subpopulation of Interneurons. Cereb Cortex, 26(3), 1081–1095. 10.1093/cercor/bhu288 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ross A, Barnett N, Faulkner A, Hannapel R, & Parent MB (2019, Feb 1). Sucrose ingestion induces glutamate AMPA receptor phosphorylation in dorsal hippocampal neurons: Increased sucrose experience prevents this effect. Behav Brain Res, 359, 792–798. 10.1016/j.bbr.2018.07.031 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ross AP, Bartness TJ, Mielke JG, & Parent MB (2009, Oct). A high fructose diet impairs spatial memory in male rats. Neurobiol Learn Mem, 92(3), 410–416. 10.1016/j.nlm.2009.05.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ross AP, Bruggeman EC, Kasumu AW, Mielke JG, & Parent MB (2012, May 15). Non-alcoholic fatty liver disease impairs hippocampal-dependent memory in male rats. Physiol Behav, 106(2), 133–141. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2012.01.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rozin P, Dow S, Moscovitch M, & Rajaram S (1998). What causes humans to begin and end a meal? A role for memory for what has been eaten, as evidenced by a study of multiple meal eating in amnesic patients. Psychological Science, 9(5), 392–396. 10.1111/1467-9280.00073 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ryff C, & Lachman ME (2010). National survey of midlife development in the United States (MIDUS II): Cognitive Project, 2004–2006. ICPSR25281-v1. Ann Arbor: Inter-university Consortium for Political and Social Research. [Google Scholar]
- Samaras K, Botelho NK, Chisholm DJ, & Lord RV (2010). Subcutaneous and visceral adipose tissue gene expression of serum adipokines that predict type 2 diabetes. Obesity, 18(5), 884–889. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sample CH, Jones S, Hargrave SL, Jarrard LE, & Davidson TL (2016, Oct 01). Western diet and the weakening of the interoceptive stimulus control of appetitive behavior. Behav Brain Res, 312, 219–230. 10.1016/j.bbr.2016.06.020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schick RR, Zimmermann JP, vorm Walde T, & Schusdziarra V (2003, Jun). Peptides that regulate food intake: glucagon-like peptide 1-(7–36) amide acts at lateral and medial hypothalamic sites to suppress feeding in rats. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol, 284(6), R1427–1435. 10.1152/ajpregu.00479.2002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schüler R, Seebeck N, Osterhoff MA, Witte V, Flöel A, Busjahn A, Jais A, Brüning JC, Frahnow T, & Kabisch S (2018). VEGF and GLUT1 are highly heritable, inversely correlated and affected by dietary fat intake: consequences for cognitive function in humans. Molec Metab, 11, 129–136. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sclafani A, & Nissenbaum JW (1985, Jun). On the role of the mouth and gut in the control of saccharin and sugar intake: a reexamination of the sham-feeding preparation. Brain Res Bull, 14(6), 569–576. 10.1016/0361-9230(85)90106-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scott MM, Lachey JL, Sternson SM, Lee CE, Elias CF, Friedman JM, & Elmquist JK (2009, Jun 10). Leptin targets in the mouse brain [Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural]. J Comp Neurol, 514(5), 518–532. 10.1002/cne.22025 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seguias L, & Tapper K (2018). The effect of mindful eating on subsequent intake of a high calorie snack. Appetite, 121, 93–100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seitz BM, Blaisdell A, & Tomiyama AJ (2019). Calories Count: Memory of Eating Is Evolutionarily Special.
- Seitz BM, Blaisdell AP, Polack CP, & Miller RR (2019). The role of biological significance in human learning and memory. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 32. [Google Scholar]
- Seitz BM, Blaisdell AP, & Tomiyama AJ (2021). Calories count: Memory of eating is evolutionarily special. Journal of Memory and Language, 117, 104192. [Google Scholar]
- Semyanov A, Walker MC, & Kullmann DM (2003, May). GABA uptake regulates cortical excitability via cell type-specific tonic inhibition. Nat Neurosci, 6(5), 484–490. 10.1038/nn1043 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shepherd JD, & Bear MF (2011, Mar). New views of Arc, a master regulator of synaptic plasticity. Nat Neurosci, 14(3), 279–284. 10.1038/nn.2708 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shimy KJ, Feldman HA, Klein GL, Bielak L, Ebbeling CB, & Ludwig DS (2020, Jul 1). Effects of Dietary Carbohydrate Content on Circulating Metabolic Fuel Availability in the Postprandial State. J Endocr Soc, 4(7), bvaa062. 10.1210/jendso/bvaa062 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shin D, Kwon SC, Kim MH, Lee KW, Choi SY, Shivappa N, Hébert JR, & Chung H-K (2018). Inflammatory potential of diet is associated with cognitive function in an older adult Korean population. Nutrition, 55, 56–62. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shivappa N, Steck SE, Hurley TG, Hussey JR, & Hébert JR (2014). Designing and developing a literature-derived, population-based dietary inflammatory index. Public Health Nutr, 17(8), 1689–1696. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Siervo M, Arnold R, Wells J, Tagliabue A, Colantuoni A, Albanese E, Brayne C, & Stephan B (2011). Intentional weight loss in overweight and obese individuals and cognitive function: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Obes Rev, 12(11), 968–983. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith E, Hay P, Campbell L, & Trollor JN (2011, Sep). A review of the association between obesity and cognitive function across the lifespan: implications for novel approaches to prevention and treatment. Obes Rev, 12(9), 740–755. 10.1111/j.1467-789X.2011.00920.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Soetens B, & Braet C (2007). Information processing of food cues in overweight and normal weight adolescents. Br J Health Psychol, 12(2), 285–304. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spitznagel MB, Hawkins M, Alosco M, Galioto R, Garcia S, Miller L, & Gunstad J (2015). Neurocognitive Effects of Obesity and Bariatric Surgery. Eur Eat Disord Rev, 23(6), 488–495. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stanek KM, Grieve SM, Brickman AM, Korgaonkar MS, Paul RH, Cohen RA, & Gunstad JJ (2011). Obesity is associated with reduced white matter integrity in otherwise healthy adults. Obesity, 19(3), 500–504. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stark E, Eichler R, Roux L, Fujisawa S, Rotstein HG, & Buzsaki G (2013, Dec 4). Inhibition-induced theta resonance in cortical circuits. Neuron, 80(5), 1263–1276. 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.09.033 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stark E, Roux L, Eichler R, Senzai Y, Royer S, & Buzsaki G (2014, Jul 16). Pyramidal cell-interneuron interactions underlie hippocampal ripple oscillations. Neuron, 83(2), 467–480. 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.06.023 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stevens GA, Singh GM, Lu Y, Danaei G, Lin JK, Finucane MM, Bahalim AN, McIntire RK, Gutierrez HR, Cowan M, Paciorek CJ, Farzadfar F, Riley L, Ezzati M, & Index), G. B. o. M. R. F. o. C. D. C. G. B. M. (2012, Nov). National, regional, and global trends in adult overweight and obesity prevalences. Popul Health Metr, 10(1), 22. 10.1186/1478-7954-10-22 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stevens J, McClain J, & Truesdale K (2008). Selection of measures in epidemiologic studies of the consequences of obesity. Int J Obes (Lond), 32(3), S60–S66. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steward O, Wallace CS, Lyford GL, & Worley PF (1998, Oct). Synaptic activation causes the mRNA for the IEG Arc to localize selectively near activated postsynaptic sites on dendrites. Neuron, 21(4), 741–751. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9808461 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stranahan AM, Norman ED, Lee K, Cutler RG, Telljohann RS, Egan JM, & Mattson MP (2008). Diet-induced insulin resistance impairs hippocampal synaptic plasticity and cognition in middle-aged rats. Hippocampus, 18(11), 1085–1088. 10.1002/hipo.20470 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strange BA, Witter MP, Lein ES, & Moser EI (2014, Oct). Functional organization of the hippocampal longitudinal axis. Nat Rev Neurosci, 15(10), 655–669. 10.1038/nrn3785 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Suarez AN, Hsu TM, Liu CM, Noble EE, Cortella AM, Nakamoto EM, Hahn JD, de Lartigue G, & Kanoski SE (2018, Jun 5). Gut vagal sensory signaling regulates hippocampus function through multi-order pathways. Nat Commun, 9(1), 2181. 10.1038/s41467-018-04639-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Suarez AN, Liu CM, Cortella AM, Noble EE, & Kanoski SE (2020, Jun 1). Ghrelin and Orexin Interact to Increase Meal Size Through a Descending Hippocampus to Hindbrain Signaling Pathway. Biol Psychiatry, 87(11), 1001–1011. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2019.10.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sun Y, Wang P, Zheng H, & Smith RG (2004, Mar 30). Ghrelin stimulation of growth hormone release and appetite is mediated through the growth hormone secretagogue receptor. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 101(13), 4679–4684. 10.1073/pnas.0305930101 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sweeney P, & Yang Y (2015, Dec 15). An excitatory ventral hippocampus to lateral septum circuit that suppresses feeding. Nat Commun, 6, 10188. 10.1038/ncomms10188 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Szypula J, Ahern A, & Cheke L (2020). The role of memory ability, depth and mode of recall in the impact of memory on later consumption. Appetite, 149, 104628. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tam SK, Jennings DJ, & Bonardi C (2015, Apr). Effects of dorsal hippocampal damage on conditioning and conditioned-response timing: A pooled analysis. Hippocampus, 25(4), 444–459. 10.1002/hipo.22381 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tapper K, & Seguias L (2020). The effects of mindful eating on food consumption over a half-day period. Appetite, 145, 104495. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thompson CL, Pathak SD, Jeromin A, Ng LL, MacPherson CR, Mortrud MT, Cusick A, Riley ZL, Sunkin SM, Bernard A, Puchalski RB, Gage FH, Jones AR, Bajic VB, Hawrylycz MJ, & Lein ES (2008, Dec 26). Genomic anatomy of the hippocampus. Neuron, 60(6), 1010–1021. 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.12.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tiehuis AM, Van der Graaf Y, Visseren FL, Vincken KL, Biessels GJ, Appelman AP, Kappelle LJ, & Mali WP (2008). Diabetes increases atrophy and vascular lesions on brain MRI in patients with symptomatic arterial disease. Stroke, 39(5), 1600–1603. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Timper K, & Bruning JC (2017, Jun 1). Hypothalamic circuits regulating appetite and energy homeostasis: pathways to obesity. Dis Model Mech, 10(6), 679–689. 10.1242/dmm.026609 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tripathi S, Verma A, & Jha SK (2020). Training on an Appetitive Trace-Conditioning Task Increases Adult Hippocampal Neurogenesis and the Expression of Arc, Erk and CREB Proteins in the Dorsal Hippocampus. Front Cell Neurosci, 14, 89. 10.3389/fncel.2020.00089 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Valladolid-Acebes I, Merino B, Principato A, Fole A, Barbas C, Lorenzo MP, Garcia A, Del Olmo N, Ruiz-Gayo M, & Cano V (2012, Feb 15). High-fat diets induce changes in hippocampal glutamate metabolism and neurotransmission. Am J Physiol Endocrinol Metab, 302(4), E396–402. 10.1152/ajpendo.00343.2011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vartanian LR, Chen WH, Reily NM, & Castel AD (2016). The parallel impact of episodic memory and episodic future thinking on food intake. Appetite, 101, 31–36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vrang N, Phifer CB, Corkern MM, & Berthoud HR (2003, Aug). Gastric distension induces c-Fos in medullary GLP-1/2-containing neurons. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol, 285(2), R470–478. 10.1152/ajpregu.00732.2002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Waise TMZ, Dranse HJ, & Lam TKT (2018, 10). The metabolic role of vagal afferent innervation. Nat Rev Gastroenterol Hepatol, 15(10), 625–636. 10.1038/s41575-018-0062-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walø-Syversen G, Kvalem IL, Kristinsson J, Eribe IL, Rø Ø, Brunborg C, & Lindvall Dahlgren C (2021). Preoperative inhibitory control predicts weight loss 1 year after bariatric surgery. European Eating Disorders Review, 29(1), 123–132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weitz AJ, Fang Z, Lee HJ, Fisher RS, Smith WC, Choy M, Liu J, Lin P, Rosenberg M, & Lee JH (2015, Feb 15). Optogenetic fMRI reveals distinct, frequency-dependent networks recruited by dorsal and intermediate hippocampus stimulations. Neuroimage, 107, 229–241. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.10.039 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- White NM, Packard MG, & McDonald RJ (2013, Dec). Dissociation of memory systems: The story unfolds. Behav Neurosci, 127(6), 813–834. 10.1037/a0034859 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whitelock V, Gaglione A, Davies-Owen J, & Robinson E (2019). Focused attention during eating enhanced memory for meal satiety but did not reduce later snack intake in men: A randomised within-subjects laboratory experiment. Appetite, 136, 124–129. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whitelock V, Higgs S, Brunstrom JM, Halford JC, & Robinson E (2018). No effect of focused attention whilst eating on later snack food intake: Two laboratory experiments. Appetite, 128, 188–196. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whitelock V, Kersbergen I, Higgs S, Aveyard P, Halford JC, & Robinson E (2019). A smartphone based attentive eating intervention for energy intake and weight loss: results from a randomised controlled trial. BMC Public Health, 19(1), 1–11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whitelock V, Kersbergen I, Higgs S, Aveyard P, Halford JC, & Robinson E (2020). User Experiences of a Smartphone-Based Attentive Eating App and Their Association With Diet and Weight Loss Outcomes: Thematic and Exploratory Analyses From a Randomized Controlled Trial. JMIR mHealth and uHealth, 8(10), e16780. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whitelock V, & Robinson E (2018). Remembered meal satisfaction, satiety, and later snack food intake: a laboratory study. Nutrients, 10(12), 1883. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whitlock JR, Heynen AJ, Shuler MG, & Bear MF (2006, Aug 25). Learning induces long-term potentiation in the hippocampus. Science, 313(5790), 1093–1097. https://doi.org/313/5790/1093 [pii] 10.1126/science.1128134 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wiles NJ, Northstone K, Emmett P, & Lewis G (2009). ‘Junk food’diet and childhood behavioural problems: results from the ALSPAC cohort. Eur J Clin Nutr, 63(4), 491–498. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Willette AA, & Kapogiannis D (2015, Mar). Does the brain shrink as the waist expands? Ageing Res Rev, 20, 86–97. 10.1016/j.arr.2014.03.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wren AM, Seal LJ, Cohen MA, Brynes AE, Frost GS, Murphy KG, Dhillo WS, Ghatei MA, & Bloom SR (2001, Dec). Ghrelin enhances appetite and increases food intake in humans. J Clin Endocrinol Metab, 86(12), 5992. 10.1210/jcem.86.12.8111 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wu A, Ying Z, & Gomez-Pinilla F (2004, Apr). The interplay between oxidative stress and brain-derived neurotrophic factor modulates the outcome of a saturated fat diet on synaptic plasticity and cognition. Eur J Neurosci, 19(7), 1699–1707. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03246.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xia Z, & Storm DR (2012, Sep). Role of signal transduction crosstalk between adenylyl cyclase and MAP kinase in hippocampus-dependent memory. Learn Mem, 19(9), 369–374. 10.1101/lm.027128.112 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yau PL, Kang EH, Javier DC, & Convit A (2014). Preliminary evidence of cognitive and brain abnormalities in uncomplicated adolescent obesity. Obesity, 22(8), 1865–1871. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yazar Y, Bergstrom ZM, & Simons JS (2014). Continuous theta burst stimulation of angular gyrus reduces subjective recollection. PLoS One, 9(10), e110414. 10.1371/journal.pone.0110414 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yeomans MR (2017, Nov). Adverse effects of consuming high fat-sugar diets on cognition: implications for understanding obesity. Proc Nutr Soc, 76(4), 455–465. 10.1017/S0029665117000805 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yeomans MR, Milton MR, & Chambers L (2017). Additive effects of sensory-enhanced satiety and memory for recent eating on appetite. Appetite, 117, 335–341. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yizhar O, Fenno LE, Davidson TJ, Mogri M, & Deisseroth K (2011, Jul 14). Optogenetics in neural systems. Neuron, 71(1), 9–34. 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.06.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zamorano C, Fernández-Albert J, Storm DR, Carné X, & Sindreu C (2018, 02). Memory Retrieval Re-Activates Erk1/2 Signaling in the Same Set of CA1 Neurons Recruited During Conditioning. Neuroscience, 370, 101–111. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.03.034 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang Y, Proenca R, Maffei M, Barone M, Leopold L, & Friedman JM (1994, Dec 1). Positional cloning of the mouse obese gene and its human homologue.[see comment][erratum appears in Nature 1995 Mar 30;374(6521):479]. Nature, 372(6505), 425–432. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhou W, Hohmann AG, & Crystal JD (2012, Jun). Rats answer an unexpected question after incidental encoding. Curr Biol, 22(12), 1149–1153. 10.1016/j.cub.2012.04.040 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhu H, Pleil KE, Urban DJ, Moy SS, Kash TL, & Roth BL (2014, Jul). Chemogenetic inactivation of ventral hippocampal glutamatergic neurons disrupts consolidation of contextual fear memory. Neuropsychopharmacology, 39(8), 1880–1892. 10.1038/npp.2014.35 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]