Abstract
The Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (CERQ) is an assessment tool to evaluate cognitive emotion regulation strategies. The main objective of this study is to provide new empirical evidence about the validity and reliability of the CERQ via a sample of 271 Spanish adolescents (136 female, 135 male) aged from 15 to 18 years (M = 15.7, SD = 0.76). The analytical process was carried out in two phases. A confirmatory factor analysis was performed on the polychoric correlation matrix between items. Four possible alternative models were contrasted: two models with nine factors and two models with two second-order factors and nine first-order factors, with 36 and 27 items, respectively. The model with nine correlated factors and 27 items obtained the best indices of overall fit. Subsequently, the reliability of the measurements was estimated on this model. The results reaffirm the validity of the 27-item version of the CERQ over the original 36-item structure. The findings also confirm that the CERQ is a reliable instrument for the evaluation of emotion regulation strategies in adolescents.
Keywords: adolescents, Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire, confirmatory factor analysis, psychometric properties, Spanish validation
1. Introduction
Adolescence is considered as a transition process from childhood to adulthood. It is a stage of life in which major changes take place over a short period of time [1]. This process consists of three interrelated levels [2]: biological (puberty), psychological, and social. The changes are a consequence of the developmental mechanisms and cultural demands of a specific social context, which includes defining dimensions such as social values. Due to the need to quickly adapt to these changes, various psychopathological symptoms often occur that directly affect the well-being and functioning of adolescents [3,4,5]. Numerous studies have shown that 32% of the child and adolescent population suffer from some kind of anxiety disorder generated by the presence of a stressor, such as lack of self-confidence, low self-esteem, or frustration due to poor academic performance [6,7,8,9,10]. These studies suggest that between 30% and 50% of these cases are associated to another type of disorder, such as aggressive behaviour, eating disorders, obsessive compulsive disorder, etc. [11,12,13,14]. In addition, 80% of adolescents suffering from these conditions do not receive any treatment or professional care [15].
In many cases, these symptoms are linked to the inability of individuals to recognise their emotions or to inadequate emotion regulation [16]. Emotions are an essential aspect of daily life and can promote or impair a person’s health and play a significant role in different cognitive processes. Therefore, the capacity to regulate or modulate emotional states is key to maintaining good mental health [17]. Emotion regulation is defined as the use of mechanisms, skills, and strategies with the goal of maintaining, increasing, or suppressing an existing emotional state [18]. Emotion regulation implies being aware of the state of one’s emotions and using strategies to manage mood [19,20]. Different studies have demonstrated that when people are unable to effectively regulate their emotions, distress may persist over time and may lead to different psychopathological symptoms and health problems in general [21,22,23,24]. Furthermore, gender has also been considered in many studies; females tend to present higher anxiety-related symptoms and a wider variety of mental disorders than males [6,21,25,26,27,28,29].
The growing scientific interest in the personal and interpersonal processes and skills that facilitate emotion regulation has been highlighted by the increase in publications on these topics over the last 30 years [30,31,32]. A key factor for research in this area is the availability of instruments that allow accurate assessment of the emotion regulation mechanisms.
One of the most commonly used models for understanding emotion regulation processes is that of Garnefski et al. [19], which states that emotions evoked by a negative event can be regulated by cognitive processing. Thus, the model postulates that people can use nine emotion regulation strategies: Blaming others (blaming another person for the occurrence of negative incidents or events), Self-blame (blaming oneself for the experience), Rumination (systematically thinking about the feelings and thoughts associated with the negative event), Catastrophizing (explicitly emphasising the terror of what you have experienced), Putting into perspective (pushing aside the severity of the event in comparison to other events), Positive reappraisal (creating a positive appraisal of the event in terms of personal growth), Refocus on planning (thinking about what steps to take and how to handle the negative event), Positive refocusing (thinking about joyful and pleasant issues instead of thinking about the actual event), and Acceptance (accepting what you experienced and resigning yourself to what has happened).
In addition, these strategies have frequently been classified into two dimensions: adaptive cognitive strategies of emotion regulation (Putting into Perspective, Acceptance, Positive Refocusing, Positive Reappraisal, and Refocus on Planning) and maladaptive cognitive strategies of emotion regulation (Rumination, Catastrophizing, Self-blame, and Blaming others).
To evaluate these nine strategies, Garnefski et al. [19] proposed the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (CERQ), which is the first and only emotion regulation assessment instrument that separates cognitive regulation strategies from behavioural strategies [33]. It is one of the most widely used instruments for the assessment of emotion regulation and has shown its usefulness in different fields, such as clinical [34] or educational [35,36]. For example, Giménez et al. [34] indicate that poor use of emotion regulation strategies is associated with greater perceived distress and the possibility of developing psychopathological symptoms. In this sense, the CERQ is a suitable instrument to study cognitive emotion regulation strategies and to understand their influence on subjective well-being and mental health.
The CERQ is a self-report instrument consisting of 36 items distributed according to the nine strategies of the model (four items for each dimension). Garnefski et al. [19] reported that the nine factors together explained 64.6% of the variance with communalities ranging between 0.46 and 0.73. Subsequent studies confirmed the nine-dimensional structure [37,38] and verified the validity and reliability of the scale in different populations [39,40]. The instrument was also adapted in different countries such as France [41], China [42], and Spain [43], where the nine-dimensional model showed a good fit to the data. The internal consistency indices (Cronbach’s alpha) found in these and other investigations [44] typically range between 0.60 and 0.90 points.
However, some studies show discrepancies with these results. For example, McKinnon et al. [45] reported that a five-factor model provided the best fit in an adult clinical population. In relation to the Spanish population, Carvajal et al. [33] found that reducing the scale to 27 items improved model fit. Holgado-Tello et al. [46] supported this 27-item version of the CERQ. Also in the Spanish population, Domínguez-Sánchez et al. [43] reported that an alternative model integrating the nine dimensions into two second-order factors (adaptive and maladaptive strategies) showed appropriate global fit indices.
The CERQ has also been adapted for children [37], and this adaptation has received a considerable interest in various studies, [5,47]. However, even though Garnefski et al. [19] indicated that the CERQ is designed as a self-report questionnaire that can be administered from 12 years of age, its properties have not yet been sufficiently explored in the Spanish-speaking adolescent population. Given the importance of cognitive emotion regulation, the CERQ could constitute a conceptually sound instrument for assessing underlying cognitive coping processes in adolescence. Therefore, the main objective of this study is to provide new empirical evidence on the validity and reliability of the CERQ in a sample of Spanish adolescents.
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
A total of 271 Spanish compulsory secondary school students (Educación Secundaria Obligatoria, ESO) participated in this study (136 females 50.18%, 135 males 49.82%). Ages ranged from 15 to 18 years (M = 15.7, SD = 0.76). The participants attended two different secondary schools in the city of Valladolid, Spain. Non-probability convenience sampling was employed as, after contacting different schools in urban areas, a working relationship was established with the schools with the highest level of educational innovation.
2.2. Instruments
The Spanish adaptation of the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (CERQ-S; [43] was employed. This tool consists of 36 items distributed into nine subscales related to emotion regulation strategies. This questionnaire has shown to be reliable and valid in prior studies with both Spanish [32,43] and international populations [37,44,48,49,50].
The 36 items refer to the nine cognitive strategies of emotion regulation: Rumination, Catastrophizing, Self-blame, Blaming others, Putting into Perspective, Acceptance, Positive Refocusing, Positive Reappraisal, and Refocus on Planning [37]. Each of these subscales is made up of four items in the 36-item tool and three items in the 27-item tool.
High scores in each of the subscales indicate greater use of that strategy. Scores are obtained from participants’ responses about their use of each strategy on a Likert scale (1 = almost never; 5 = almost always). The internal consistency of each subscale oscillated between alphas of 0.68 and 0.89 in the original validation [19]. Scores obtained in the Spanish adaptation [43] were similar (between 0.61 and 0.89).
2.3. Procedure
In all the procedures, the ethical standards of the institutions, the criteria of the National Research Committee [Comité Nacional de Investigación], and the international criteria of the APA [51] and the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki [52] (as well as their subsequent amendments or similar ethical rules), were observed. Within these ethical principles for research involving human subjects, the confidentiality of the data and the pursuit of the benefit of the participants are ensured. Furthermore, as this was a study with adolescents, their parents or legal guardians gave prior informed consent for their children to participate and to be assessed.
The evaluation protocol was administered in group sessions with 20 to 30 students who were divided into their academic year groups within their classrooms. An examiner was always present to clarify any doubt that might arise.
2.4. Data Analysis
The analytical process was carried out in two phases. In phase 1, a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was performed on the polychoric correlation matrix between items [53] after verifying the fit of the data for the factor analysis using the Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin test (KMO) and Bartlett’s sphericity test (KMO = 0.75; Bartlett’s sphericity test, χ² (630) = 1676.94; p < 0.001). Following some suggestions in previous literature concerning the Spanish population [33,43,46], four models were estimated: two models with nine correlated factors with 36 and 27 items, (models 1 and 2, respectively), and two models with two second-order factors (models 3 and 4, respectively) combining five first-order factors (related to adaptative strategies of emotion regulation) and four first-order factors (related to maladaptive strategies of emotion regulation), with 36 and 27 items, respectively.
In the second phase, the reliability of the measures (internal consistency, reliability of the individual indicators, construct reliability, and observational error) was estimated using the model with nine correlated factors and 27 items (model 3). All models were estimated via diagonally least weighted squares on the polychoric correlation matrix using R 3.6.3 software [54] and the lavaan package [55]. All models were estimated using weighted least squares (WLSMV).
The goodness of fit was contrasted using the comparative fit index (CFI), the Tucker-Lewis index (TLI), and the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA). It is considered that CFI and TLI indices greater than 0.90 indicate acceptable fit, while a value higher than 0.95 is considered good [56]. In the case of the RMSEA, values of 0.05 or less are considered good and less than 0.08 is considered acceptable [56,57]. Moreover, following the recommendations of Chen [58] and Cheung and Rensvold [59], increases of less than 0.010 in CFI and TLI and decreases of less than 0.015 in RMSEA suggest no relevant changes in the fit of one model compared with the following more restrictive model.
3. Results
The results of the CFA allowed comparison of the goodness of fit of four alternative models: two with nine factors and two with two second-order factors and nine first-order factors, with 36 and 27 items respectively. The result was extremely favourable for model 3 with nine correlated factors and 27 items. As can be observed in Table 1, the improvement in the fit of model 3 over the other models proved conclusive. Figure 1 shows the graphical representation of the measurement model.
Table 1.
Model | χ² (df) | p(χ²) | RMSEA | RMSEA 90% CI | SRMR | CFI | TLI |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1 | 1024.68 (558) | <0.001 | 0.066 | [0.050–0.071] | 0.083 | 0.912 | 0.901 |
2 | 1487.14 (584) | <0.001 | 0.076 | [0.071–0.080] | 0.099 | 0.830 | 0.817 |
3 | 356.74 (288) | =0.004 | 0.030 | [0.018–0.039] | 0.064 | 0.981 | 0.977 |
4 | 597.68 (314) | <0.001 | 0.058 | [0.051–0.065] | 0.083 | 0.922 | 0.912 |
Note. Model 1 = 36 items and 9 correlated factors; Model 2 = 36 items, 2 s-order factors and 9 first-order factors; Model 3 = 27 items and 9 correlated factors; Model 4 = 27 items, 2 s-order factors and 9 first-order factors. RMSEA = root mean square error of approximation; CFI = comparative fit index; TLI = Tucker–Lewis index; df = degrees of freedom; CI = confidence interval.
Global alpha ordinal reliability values [60] of α = 0.83 and McDonald’s omega values [61,62] of ω = 0.82 were obtained. Both values are adequate. Good internal consistency indices were obtained for adaptive strategies (α1 = 0.87; ω1 = 0.9) and for maladaptive strategies (α2 = 0.82; ω2 = 0.89).
The composite reliability (CR) analysis of each latent variable provides an indicator of the reliability of the construct [63]. In all cases, CR was higher than 0.70 (CR1 = 0.78; CR2 = 0.84; CR3 = 0.79; CR4 = 0.71; CR5 = 0.74; CR6 = 0.71; CR7 = 0.79; CR8 = 0.81; and CR9 = 0.74). Therefore, it can be concluded that the indicators of the nine factors are a reliable measurement of the construct. Average variance extracted (AVE) showed values higher than or extremely close to 0.5 in all cases (AVE1 = 0.55; AVE2 = 0.64, AVE3 = 0.47, AVE4 = 0.47, AVE5 = 0.51; AVE6 = 0.44, AVE7 = 0.44, AVE8 = 0.58, and AVE9 = 0.48). Thus, it can be concluded that a substantial amount of the variance of the indicators is explained by the construct compared with the error of the measurement. All these indicators constitute evidence of reliability in the operationalisation of the nine latent variables of the scale.
The reliability of each indicator can be checked using the R2 values, which indicate the proportion of variance of each indicator that explains the latent variable (high R2 values indicate that the indicator is reliable). The most reliable indicator for the acceptance factor was item 11 (I think that I have to accept the situation; R2 = 0.88). The most reliable indicator for the positive refocusing factor was item 31 (I think of pleasant things that have nothing to do with it; R2 = 0.757). The most reliable indicator for the positive reappraisal factor was item 31 I look for the positive sides to the matter (R2 = 0.639). The item 29 I feel that others are responsible for what has happened proved to be the most reliable indicator for the blaming others factor (R2 = 0.735). The item 1 I feel that I am the one to blame for it proved to be the most reliable indicator for the self-blame factor (R2 = 0.694).
Regarding convergent validity (e.g., the indicators of each latent variable have a high shared variance), Table 2 and Table 3 indicate that (a) the factor loadings of all of the indicators proved significant; (b) all but three indicators (I often think about how I feel about what I have experienced—λ = 0.493; I often think that what I have experienced is the worst that can happen to a person—λ = 0.435; I think about how to change the situation—λ = 0.478) higher than 0.5; and (c) the AVE of the saturations of the items in each factor are all those which are higher or extremely close to 0.5.
Table 2.
Latent Factor | Indicator | B | SE | Z | p-Value | Beta |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Self-blame | I feel that I am the one to blame for it | 0.831 | 0.058 | 14.275 | 0 | 0.833 |
Self-blame | I feel that I am the one who is responsible for what has happened | 0.747 | 0.056 | 13.313 | 0 | 0.722 |
Self-blame | I think that basically the cause must lie within myself | 0.640 | 0.050 | 12.808 | 0 | 0.670 |
Blaming others | I feel that others are to blame for it | 0.759 | 0.059 | 12.789 | 0 | 0.802 |
Blaming others | I feel that others are responsible for what has happened | 0.872 | 0.068 | 12.919 | 0 | 0.858 |
Blaming others | I feel that basically the cause lies with others | 0.739 | 0.060 | 12.405 | 0 | 0.738 |
Rumination | I often think about how I feel about what I have experienced | 0.583 | 0.052 | 11.194 | 0 | 0.493 |
Rumination | I am preoccupied with what I think and feel about what I have experienced | 0.776 | 0.054 | 14.273 | 0 | 0.734 |
Rumination | I dwell upon the feelings the situation has evoked in me | 0.947 | 0.064 | 14.745 | 0 | 0.793 |
Catastrophising | I keep thinking about how terrible it is what I have experienced | 0.931 | 0.057 | 16.198 | 0 | 0.778 |
Catastrophising | I often think that what I have experienced is the worst that can happen to a person | 0.468 | 0.042 | 11.077 | 0 | 0.435 |
Catastrophising | I continually think how horrible the situation has been | 0.957 | 0.059 | 16.231 | 0 | 0.783 |
Note. B = Estimate Factor Loadings; SE = Standard Error; Beta = Standardized Factor Loadings.
Table 3.
Latent Factor | Indicator | B | SE | Z | p-Value | Beta |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Acceptance | I think that I have to accept that this has happened | 0.532 | 0.050 | 10.647 | 0 | 0.501 |
Acceptance | I think that I have to accept the situation | 0.899 | 0.064 | 14.145 | 0 | 0.940 |
Acceptance | I think that I must learn to live with it | 0.669 | 0.052 | 12.805 | 0 | 0.626 |
Perspective | I think that other people go through much worse experiences | 0.723 | 0.053 | 13.730 | 0 | 0.594 |
Perspective | I think that it hasn’t been too bad compared to other things | 0.747 | 0.050 | 14.849 | 0 | 0.704 |
Perspective | I tell myself that there are worse things in life | 0.802 | 0.055 | 14.680 | 0 | 0.695 |
Positive reappraisal | I think that I can become a stronger person as a result of what has happened | 0.721 | 0.047 | 15.403 | 0 | 0.630 |
Positive reappraisal | I think that the situation also has its positive sides | 0.617 | 0.045 | 13.829 | 0 | 0.532 |
Positive reappraisal | I look for the positive sides to the matter | 0.906 | 0.054 | 16.643 | 0 | 0.799 |
Positive refocusing | I think of pleasant things that have nothing to do with it | 0.852 | 0.051 | 16.585 | 0 | 0.696 |
Positive refocusing | I think of pleasant things that have nothing to do with it | 0.818 | 0.048 | 16.989 | 0 | 0.723 |
Positive refocusing | I think of pleasant things that have nothing to do with it | 0.997 | 0.055 | 18.180 | 0 | 0.870 |
Planning | I think about how I can best cope with the situation | 0.684 | 0.050 | 13.586 | 0 | 0.717 |
Planning | I think about how to change the situation | 0.445 | 0.041 | 10.960 | 0 | 0.478 |
Planning | I think of a plan of what I can do best | 0.686 | 0.052 | 13.124 | 0 | 0.635 |
Note. B = Estimate Factor Loadings; SE = Standard Error; Beta = Standardized Factor Loadings.
The evidence of discriminant validity demonstrates that each of the constructs analysed is unique and different from other constructs. To test discriminant validity, four approaches were used [53]. First, the correlation between each pair of factors was set to 1, and the fit of the resulting models was compared to the fit of the original model of nine correlated factors and 27 items. The results showed that this model was significantly superior to the models in which the correlation between each pair of factors was set to 1 (Table A1). Second, the confidence interval test [64] demonstrated that the confidence interval of the correlations between the factors does not contain 1 (Table 4). Third, it was verified that the HTMT ratio [65] of the correlations between the indicators of different factors (heterotrait-heteromethod correlations–HT) and between the correlations of the indicators of the same factor (monotrait-heteromethod correlations–MT) is less than 0.9 (F1–F2, HT/MT = 0.771; F1–F3, HT/MT = 0.693; F2–F3, HT/MT = 0.835).
Table 4.
F2 (SE) | F3 (SE) | F4 (SE) | F5 (SE) | F6 (SE) | F7 (SE) | F8 (SE) | F9 (SE) | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
F1 | 0.025 (0.038) | 0.502 (0.051) | 0.530 (0.052) | −0.068 (0.045) | 0.022 (0.045) | −0.163 (0.047) | −0.191 (0.04) | −0.061 (0.047) |
F2 | 0.109 (0.038) | 0.401 (0.044) | −0.031 (0.039) | 0.194 (0.043) | 0.077 (0.04) | 0.206 (0.037) | 0.084 (0.045) | |
F3 | 0.662 (0.059) | 0.092 (0.047) | 0.207 (0.05) | −0.027 (0.049) | 0.047 (0.042) | 0.141 (0.053) | ||
F4 | −0.234 (0.047) | −0.072 (0.048) | −0.343 (0.051) | −0.165 (0.043) | −0.214 (0.053) | |||
F5 | 0.443 (0.055) | 0.432 (0.056) | 0.330 (0.044) | 0.465 (0.059) | ||||
F6 | 0.716 (0.068) | 0.485 (0.051) | 0.637 (0.072) | |||||
F7 | 0.627 (0.054) | 0.820 (0.081) | ||||||
F8 | 0.478 (0.057) |
Note. F1 = Self-blame; F2 = Blaming others; F3 = Rumination; F4 = Catastrophizing; F5 = Acceptance; F6 = Perspective; F7 = Positive reappraisal; F8 = Positive refocusing; F9 = Planning.
Fourth, Fornell and Larcker’s criterion [63] states that AVE from each factor higher than the square of the correlations between each pair of factors can be considered as evidence of discriminant validity. This criterion is fulfilled in all but two cases (Table 5). Specifically, AVE of factors 6 (Perspective, AVE = 0.444) and 7 (Reappraisal, AVE = 0.440) was lower than the coefficient of determination between them both (ρ2 = 0.513). Likewise, AVE of factors 7 (Reappraisal, AVE = 0.440) and 9 (Planning, AVE = 0.482) was lower than the coefficient of determination between them both (ρ2 = 0.673).
Table 5.
F1 | F2 | F3 | F4 | F5 | F6 | F7 | F8 | F9 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Self-blame | 0.554 | ||||||||
Blaming others | 0.001 | 0.641 | |||||||
Rumination | 0.252 | 0.012 | 0.470 | ||||||
Catastrophizing | 0.28 | 0.16 | 0.438 | 0.469 | |||||
Acceptance | 0.005 | 0.001 | 0.009 | 0.055 | 0.509 | ||||
Perspective | 0 | 0.038 | 0.043 | 0.005 | 0.196 | 0.444 | |||
Reappraisal | 0.027 | 0.006 | 0.001 | 0.117 | 0.187 | 0.513 | 0.440 | ||
Refocusing | 0.036 | 0.042 | 0.002 | 0.027 | 0.109 | 0.235 | 0.393 | 0.588 | |
Planning | 0.004 | 0.007 | 0.02 | 0.046 | 0.216 | 0.406 | 0.673 | 0.229 | 0.482 |
Note. F2 = Blaming others; F3 = Rumination; F4 = Catastrophizing; F5 = Acceptance; F6 = Perspective; F7 = Positive reappraisal; F8 = Positive refocusing; F9 = Planning; AVE in diagonal (in italics), coefficient of determination (ρ2) below.
4. Discussion
The main objective of this study is to provide new empirical evidence on the validity and reliability of the CERQ [19,31] in a sample of Spanish adolescents. The CERQ is an emotion regulation assessment tool that allows discrimination between cognitive and behavioural strategies [33]. Prior studies have shown empirical evidence of the psychometric properties of the CERQ in the Spanish population [32,33,43,46,66,67]. However, less attention has been paid to its properties in the adolescent population. For this reason, the psychometric properties of the CERQ were analysed in a sample of students from the city of Valladolid (Spain).
According to some proposals in the literature, four models were estimated through CFA. The results obtained suggest the reduction of the scale to 27 items (model 3) distributed in nine factors (strategies). The nine-dimensional structure coincides with that originally proposed by Garnefski et al. [19]. However, the number of items suggested in the results differs from the original instrument and rather supports the proposal of Carvajal et al. [33]. In this sense, these authors recommend adapting the Spanish version to a shorter version called CERQ-S-27 and another even shorter version of 18 factors (ERQ-short), which has already been validated in other countries [38,48,50,68,69,70,71,72]. Given that the sample used in the present study is also Spanish, it is reasonable that the results regarding the number of items are more similar to previous studies carried out in this country.
Regarding the grouping into functional/adaptive and dysfunctional/maladaptive strategies, the results suggest the importance of maintaining the multidimensionality of the CERQ. Thus, it is recommended not to merge the scores of the different strategies as reported in other studies [36,39,49]. However, the fit of model 4 (27 items with two second-order factors and nine first-order factors) has also demonstrated appropriate global fit indices which should be considered in future research. Although it is better to address the scales separately, using them as a whole may have interesting implications in certain specific contexts, as already pointed out by Domínguez-Sánchez et al. [43] and D’Augerot et al. [36].
The results suggest that the nine subscales of the CERQ present adequate internal consistency, sufficient reliability and adequate construct reliability. Furthermore, sufficient evidence of convergent validity and discriminant validity was obtained. However, discriminant validity points to the possible convenience of merging some factors (Perspective-Appraisal-Planning), but this would require a much larger sample and a theory to support this hypothesis.
In short, these results confirm the need for further research on the dimensionality of the CERQ and cognitive emotion regulation strategies. Psychometric properties were generally similar to those of the original Spanish adaptation, confirming the validity of the short 27-item version. This conclusion has been reached not only in studies with national sampling, but also in other international research, such as the Italian [73] or German [74] adaptations.
Finally, the results of the present study should be interpreted in light of certain limitations. First, the sample was non-probabilistic, limiting the generalizability of the results. It would be of great interest to employ a random sampling method to be able to generalise the results. Second, the exclusive use of self-report methods also imply the possibility of assessment bias [75]. Third, because there are few studies on the CERQ functioning in adolescent populations, comparison of the findings was limited. Finally, although the number of participants was sufficient to perform the CFA and achieve the objective of the study, a larger sample would improve the accuracy of the results and give them greater scope. Moreover, a larger number of participants would enable additional analyses such as a gender factorial invariance analysis. As females tend to present a higher prevalence of anxiety-related symptoms and a wider variety of mental disorders than males [6,21,25,26,27,28,29,34,35], examining validity and reliability of the CERQ across gender would be of great interest. It is expected that future studies may address this limitation.
5. Conclusions
Despite these issues, the present study represents a significant contribution, since the findings support the use of the CERQ in the adolescent population, as well as its usefulness, validity, and reliability. Due to the implications of cognitive emotion regulation mechanisms on the quality of life of individuals, future studies should further explore this and other instruments that assess these processes. In this way, professionals would have appropriate instruments to assess adolescents’ coping strategies and, consequently, propose interventions to help them manage stressful situations. It would also be possible to investigate the relationship and influence of these strategies on psychopathological symptoms in order to improve the overall health of adolescents [76].
The ability to regulate one’s emotions not only affects the emotions a person experiences at a specific time, but also how they feel and express them [18]. Emotion regulation plays a fundamental role in both cognitive and psychological functioning within the field of mental health. However, despite the wide range of research on emotion regulation at different stages of life, only a small proportion of this research focuses on adolescence [77]. As previously mentioned, adolescence is characterised by physical, psychological, and social changes, which can lead to emotion dysregulation. All these changes affect not only emotions and psychopathology, but also other aspects, such as academics [49]. Therefore, it is of great importance to further investigate this ability at this developmental stage.
Acknowledgments
This research has been supported by the Spanish Government (Ministry of Education and Vocational Training) and by a doctoral grant (Formación de Profesorado Universitario, FPU 17/01994) awarded to the first author (E.B.).
Appendix A
Table A1.
Model | χ² (df) | Δχ² (Δdf) | p(Δχ²) | RMSEA | ΔRMSEA | CFI | ΔCFI | TLI | ΔTLI |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Model 3 | 356.74 (288) | - | - | 0.030 | - | 0.981 | - | 0.977 | - |
1 (F1–F2) | 520.80 (289) | 164.06 (1) | <0.001 | 0.055 | 0.025 | 0.936 | −0.045 | 0.922 | −0.055 |
2 (F1–F3) | 406.09 (289) | 49.35 (1) | <0.001 | 0.039 | 0.009 | 0.968 | −0.013 | 0.961 | −0.016 |
3 (F1–F4) | 400.19 (289) | 43.45 (1) | <0.001 | 0.038 | 0.008 | 0.969 | −0.012 | 0.963 | −0.014 |
4 (F1–F5) | - | - | - | - | - | - | - | - | - |
5 (F1–F6) | 482.68 (289) | 125.93 (1) | <0.001 | 0.050 | 0.02 | 0.946 | −0.035 | 0.935 | −0.042 |
6 (F1–F7) | - | - | - | - | - | - | - | - | - |
7 (F1–F8) | - | - | - | - | - | - | - | - | - |
8 (F1–F9) | - | - | - | - | - | - | - | - | - |
9 (F2–F3) | 499.59 (289) | 145.85 (1) | <0.001 | 0.052 | 0.022 | 0.942 | −0.039 | 0.929 | −0.048 |
10 (F2–F4) | 434.12 (289) | 77.37 (1) | <0.001 | 0.043 | 0.013 | 0.960 | −0.021 | 0.951 | −0.026 |
11 (F2–F5) | - | - | - | - | - | - | - | - | - |
12 (F2–F6) | 466.89 (289) | 110.14 (1) | <0.001 | 0.048 | 0.018 | 0.951 | −0.03 | 0.940 | −0.037 |
13 (F2–F7) | 480.45 (289) | 123.71 (1) | <0.001 | 0.050 | 0.02 | 0.947 | −0.034 | 0.936 | −0.041 |
14 (F2–F8) | 535.64 (289) | 178.9 (1) | <0.001 | 0.056 | 0.026 | 0.932 | −0.049 | 0.917 | −0.06 |
15 (F2–F9) | 436.69 (289) | 79.95 (1) | <0.001 | 0.044 | 0.014 | 0.959 | −0.022 | 0.950 | −0.027 |
16 (F3–F4) | 377.96 (289) | 21.22 (1) | <0.001 | 0.034 | 0.004 | 0.975 | −0.006 | 0.970 | −0.007 |
17 (F3–F5) | 485.04 (289) | 128.3 (1) | <0.001 | 0.050 | 0.02 | 0.946 | −0.035 | 0.934 | −0.043 |
18 (F3–F6) | 457.76 (289) | 101.02 (1) | <0.001 | 0.047 | 0.017 | 0.953 | −0.028 | 0.943 | −0.034 |
19 (F3–F7) | - | - | - | - | - | - | - | - | - |
20 (F3–F8) | 504.25 (289) | 147.51 (1) | <0.001 | 0.053 | 0.023 | 0.941 | −0.04 | 0.928 | −0.049 |
21 (F3–F9) | 432.94 (289) | 76.19 (1) | <0.001 | 0.043 | 0.013 | 0.960 | −0.021 | 0.952 | −0.025 |
22 (F4–F5) | - | - | - | - | - | - | - | - | - |
23 (F4–F6) | - | - | - | - | - | - | - | - | - |
24 (F4–F7) | - | - | - | - | - | - | - | - | - |
25 (F4–F8) | - | - | - | - | - | - | - | - | - |
26 (F4–F9) | - | - | - | - | - | - | - | - | - |
27 (F5–F6) | 406.52 (289) | 49.77 (1) | <0.001 | 0.039 | 0.009 | 0.968 | −0.013 | 0.961 | −0.016 |
28 (F5–F7) | 408.05 (289) | 51.30 (1) | <0.001 | 0.039 | 0.009 | 0.967 | −0.014 | 0.960 | −0.017 |
29 (F5–F8) | 446.91 (289) | 90.16 (1) | <0.001 | 0.045 | 0.015 | 0.956 | −0.025 | 0.947 | −0.023 |
30 (F5–F9) | 394.29 (289) | 37.54 (1) | <0.001 | 0.037 | 0.007 | 0.971 | −0.010 | 0.965 | −0.012 |
31 (F6–F7) | 369.06 (289) | 12.32 (1) | <0.001 | 0.032 | 0.002 | 0.978 | −0.003 | 0.973 | −0.004 |
32 (F6–F8) | 408.90 (289) | 52.15 (1) | <0.001 | 0.039 | 0.009 | 0.967 | −0.014 | 0.960 | −0.017 |
33 (F6–F9) | 372.65 (289) | 15.91 (1) | <0.001 | 0.033 | 0.003 | 0.977 | −0.004 | 0.972 | −0.005 |
34 (F7–F8) | 385.30 (289) | 28.55 (1) | <0.001 | 0.035 | 0.005 | 0.973 | −0.008 | 0.968 | −0.009 |
35 (F7–F9) | 360.62 (289) | 3.87 (1) | =0.049 | 0.030 | 0 | 0.980 | −0.001 | 0.976 | −0.001 |
36 (F8–F9) | 394.56 (289) | 37.81 (1) | <0.001 | 0.037 | 0.007 | 0.971 | 0.010 | 0.965 | 0.012 |
Note. RMSEA = root mean square error of approximation; CFI = comparative fit index; TLI = Tucker–Lewis index; df = degrees of freedom; CI = confidence interval; Δ (CFI, TLI, RMSEA) = changes in fit with respect to model 3; F1 = Self-blame; F2 = Blaming others; F3 = Rumination; F4 = Catastrophizing; F5 = Acceptance; F6 = Perspective; F7 = Positive reappraisal; F8 = Positive refocusing; F9 = Planning.
Author Contributions
Conceptualization, E.B., M.d.-V. and M.J.I.; Formal analysis, J.R.-M.; Investigation, E.B. and M.d.-V.; Methodology, J.R.-M.; Supervision, M.J.I.; Validation, M.J.I.; Writing—original draft, E.B., J.R.-M. and M.d.-V.; Writing—review & editing, E.B., J.R.-M., M.d.-V. and M.J.I. All authors contributed equally to this work. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
Funding
This research has received no external funding.
Institutional Review Board Statement
The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and has been approved by the Bioethics Committee of the University of Valladolid (code PI 21-2447).
Informed Consent Statement
Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.
Data Availability Statement
The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author. The data are not publicly available due to privacy reasons.
Conflicts of Interest
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
Footnotes
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Díaz Santos C., Santos Vallín L. La ansiedad en la adolescencia. RqR Enfermería Comunitaria (Rev. SEAPA) 2018;6:7–10. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Antona A., Madrid J., Aláez M. Adolescencia y salud. [(accessed on 15 January 2022)];Pap. Del Psicólogo. 2003 23 Available online: http://www.papelesdelpsicologo.es/resumen?pii=1054. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Copeland W.E., Angold A., Shanahan L., Costello E.J. Longitudinal Patterns of Anxiety From Childhood to Adulthood: The Great Smoky Mountains Study. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2014;53:21–33. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2013.09.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Del Hernández-Pozo M.R., Ramírez-Guerrero N., López-Cárdenas S.V., Macías-Martínez D. Relación entre ansiedad, desempeño y riesgo de deserción en aspirantes a bachillerato. Psychol. Av. Discip. 2015;9:45–57. doi: 10.21500/19002386.992. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Orgilés M., Méndez X., Espada J.P., Carballo J.L., Piqueras J.A. Síntomas de trastornos de ansiedad en niños y adolescentes: Diferencias en función de la edad y el sexo en una muestra comunitaria. Rev. Psiquiatr. Salud Ment. 2012;5:115–120. doi: 10.1016/j.rpsm.2012.01.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Aláez M., Martínez-Arias R., Rodríguez-Sutil C. Prevalencia de trastornos psicológicos en niños y adolescentes, su rela-ción con la edad y el género. Psicothema. 2000;12:525–532. [Google Scholar]
- 7.American Psychiatric Association . Guía de Consulta de los Criterios Diagnósticos del DSM-5: Spanish Edition of the Desk Reference to the Diagnostic Criteria from DSM-5. American Psychiatric Association; Washington, DC, USA: 2014. [Google Scholar]
- 8.World Health Organization The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders: Clinical Descriptions and Diagnostic Guidelines. Meditor. 1992. [(accessed on 10 December 2021)]. Available online: https://apps.who.int/iris/handle/10665/40510.
- 9.Tayeh P., Agámez P.M., Chaskel R. Trastornos de ansiedad en la infancia y adolescencia. Soc. Colomb. Pe-Diatría. 2013;15:6–17. [Google Scholar]
- 10.Wehry A.M., Beesdo-Baum K., Hennelly M.M., Connolly S.D., Strawn J.R. Assessment and Treatment of Anxiety Disorders in Children and Adolescents. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 2015;17:52. doi: 10.1007/s11920-015-0591-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Crespo-Eguílaz N., Narbona J. Procedural learning disorder: Neuropsychological characteristics. Rev. Neurol. 2009;49:409. doi: 10.33588/rn.4908.2009079. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Egger H.L., Angold A. Common emotional and behavioral disorders in preschool children: Presentation, nosology, and epidemiology. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 2006;47:313–337. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01618.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Penosa P. Doctoral Thesis. University Miguel Hernández; Elche, Spain: 2017. [(accessed on 13 December 2021)]. Ansiedad en Preescolares: Evaluación y Factores Familiares. Available online: http://dspace.umh.es/ [Google Scholar]
- 14.Rapee R.M. Trastornos de ansiedad en niños y adolescentes: Naturaleza, desarrollo, tratamiento y prevención. In: Irarrázaval M., editor. Manual de Salud Mental Infantil y Adolescente de la IACAPAP. International Association for Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Allied Professions; Geneva, Switzerland: 2016. [(accessed on 10 November 2021)]. pp. 1–22. Available online: http://iacapap.org/ [Google Scholar]
- 15.Busman R., Bubrick J., Howard J., Anderson D. Understanding Anxiety in Children and Teens. 2018. [(accessed on 10 November 2021)]. 2018 Children’s Mental Health Report [PDF File] Available online: http://www.infocoponline.es/
- 16.Aldao A., Nolen-Hoeksema S., Schweizer S. Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2010;30:217–237. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Compas B.E., Jaser S.S., Bettis A.H., Watson K.H., Gruhn M.A., Dunbar J.P., Williams E., Thigpen J.C. Coping, emotion regulation, and psychopathology in childhood and adolescence: A meta-analysis and narrative review. Psychol. Bull. 2017;143:939–991. doi: 10.1037/bul0000110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Gross J.J., Muñoz R.F. Emotion regulation and mental health. Clin. Psychol. Sci. Pr. 1995;2:151–164. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2850.1995.tb00036.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Garnefski N., Kraaij V., Spinhoven P. Negative life events, cognitive emotion regulation and emotional problems. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2001;30:1311–1327. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00113-6. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Koole S.L. The psychology of emotion regulation: An integrative review. Cogn. Emot. 2009;23:4–41. doi: 10.1080/02699930802619031. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Del Valle M.V., Betegón E., Irurtia M.J. Efecto del uso de estrategias cognitivas de regulación emocional sobre la ansiedad en adolescentes españoles. Suma Psicológica. 2018;25:153–161. doi: 10.14349/sumapsi.2018.v25.n2.7. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Erk S., Kiefer M., Grothe J., Wunderlich A.P., Spitzer M., Walter H. Emotional context modulates subsequent memory effect. Neuroimage. 2003;18:439–447. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(02)00015-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Poh M.-Z., Swenson N.C., Picard R.W. A Wearable Sensor for Unobtrusive, Long-Term Assessment of Electrodermal Activity. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. 2010;57:1243–1252. doi: 10.1109/tbme.2009.2038487. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Pylypow J., Quinn D., Duncan D., Balbuena L. A Measure of Emotional Regulation and Irritability in Children and Adolescents: The Clinical Evaluation of Emotional Regulation—9. J. Atten. Disord. 2020;24:2002–2011. doi: 10.1177/1087054717737162. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Battaglia M., Touchette E., Garon-Carrier G., Dionne G., Cote S.M., Vitaro F., Tremblay R.E., Boivin M. Distinct trajectories of separation anxiety in the preschool years: Persistence at school entry and early-life associated factors. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 2015;57:39–46. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12424. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Caballo V.E., Piqueras J.A., Antona C., Irurtia M.J., Salazar I.C., Bas-Sarmiento P., Salavera C. La autoestima y su relación con la ansiedad social y las habilidades sociales. Psicol. Conduct. 2018;26:23–53. [Google Scholar]
- 27.Irurtia M.J., Caballo V.E., Ovejero A. Trastorno de ansiedad provocado por sustancias psicoactivas. Psicol. Con-Ductual. 2009;17:155–168. [Google Scholar]
- 28.McLean C.P., Asnaani A., Litz B.T., Hofmann S.G. Gender differences in anxiety disorders: Prevalence, course of illness, comorbidity and burden of illness. J. Psychiatr. Res. 2011;45:1027–1035. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2011.03.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Nolen-Hoeksema S. Emotion Regulation and Psychopathology: The Role of Gender. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2012;8:161–187. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032511-143109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Domínguez-Lara S.A. Influencia de las estrategias cognitivas de regulación emocional sobre la ansiedad y depresión en universitarios: Análisis preliminar. Salud Uninorte. 2017;33:315–321. doi: 10.14482/sun.33.3.10891. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Garnefsky N., Kraaij V., Spinhoven P. Manual for the Use of Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire. DATEC; Leiderdorp, The Netherlands: 2002. [Google Scholar]
- 32.Rodríguez-Sabiote C., Ibáñez-Cubillas P., López-Rodríguez S., Álvarez-Rodríguez J. Psychometric Properties of the Spanish Version of the Cognitive Emotional Regulation Difficulties Questionnaire (CERQ) in Higher Education Students in Times of COVID-19. Front. Psychol. 2021;12:695147. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.695147. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Carvajal B.P., Molina-Martínez M.Á., Fernández-Fernández V., Paniagua-Granados T., Lasa-Aristu A., Luque-Reca O. Psychometric properties of the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (CERQ) in Spanish older adults. Aging Ment. Health. 2021;26:413–422. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2020.1870207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Giménez S., Cuevas A., Navarro A.F. Análisis de la regulación emocional cognitiva en personal de enfermería. [(accessed on 10 September 2021)];Discov. Med. 2019 3:27–36. Available online: https://revdiscovermedicine.com/index.php/inicio/article/view/179. [Google Scholar]
- 35.Canedo C.M., Andrés M.L., Canet-Juric L., Rubiales J. Influencia de las estrategias cognitivas de regulación emocional en el bienestar subjetivo y psicológico en estudiantes universitarios. Liberabit. 2019;25:25–40. doi: 10.24265/liberabit.2019.v25n1.03. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 36.D’Augerot M.I., Bayo M., González R., Bakker L. Estrategias cognitivas de regulación emocional adaptativas y desadaptativas en adolescentes argentinos. Anu. Investig. 2018;25:413–421. [Google Scholar]
- 37.Garnefski N., Kraaij V. The Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire: Psychometric features and prospective relationships with depression and anxiety in adults. Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 2007;23:141–149. doi: 10.1027/1015-5759.23.3.141. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Ireland M.J., Clough B.A., Day J. The cognitive emotion regulation questionnaire: Factorial, convergent, and criterion validity analyses of the full and short versions. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2017;110:90–95. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2017.01.035. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Feliu-Soler A., Reche-Camba E., Borràs X., Pérez-Aranda A., Andrés-Rodríguez L., Peñarrubia-María M.T., Navarro-Gil M., García-Campayo J., Bellón J.A., Luciano J.V. Psychometric Properties of the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (CERQ) in Patients with Fibromyalgia Syndrome. Front. Psychol. 2017;8:2075. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02075. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Megreya A.M., Latzman R.D., Al-Attiyah A.A., AlRashidi M. The Robustness of the Nine-Factor Structure of the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire across Four Arabic-Speaking Middle Eastern Countries. J. Cross-Cult. Psychol. 2016;47:875–890. doi: 10.1177/0022022116644785. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Jermann F., Van Der Linden M., D’Acremont M., Zermatten A. Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (CERQ) Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 2006;22:126–131. doi: 10.1027/1015-5759.22.2.126. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Zhu X., Auerbach R., Yao S., Abela J.R.Z., Xiao J., Tong X. Psychometric properties of the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire: Chinese version. Cogn. Emot. 2008;22:288–307. doi: 10.1080/02699930701369035. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Domínguez-Sánchez F.J., Lasa-Aristu A., Amor P.J., Holgado-Tello F.P. Psychometric Properties of the Spanish Version of the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire. Assessment. 2011;20:253–261. doi: 10.1177/1073191110397274. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Lara S.A.D., Medrano L.A. Invarianza factorial del Cognitive Emotional Regulation Questionarie (CERQ) en universitarios limeños y cordobeses. Univ. Psychol. 2016;15:89–98. doi: 10.11144/javeriana.upsy15-1.ifce. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 45.McKinnon A., Kuyken W., Hayes R., Werner-Seidler A., Watson P., Dalgleish T., Schweizer S. The psychometric properties of the cognitive emotion regulation questionnaire (CERQ) in a clinical sample of adults with recurrent depression. J. Affect. Disord. 2020;276:212–219. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2020.06.061. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Holgado-Tello F.P., Amor P.J., Lasa-Aristu A., Domínguez-Sánchez F.J., Delgado B. Two new brief versions of the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire and its relationships with depression and anxiety. An. Psicol. 2018;34:458–464. doi: 10.6018/analesps.34.3.306531. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Lemos V., Valega M., Serppe M. Cognitive Emotional Regulation Questionnaire: A Factorial Validation Study in Spanish for Children (CERQ-k) Int. J. Psychol. Res. 2021;14:61–81. doi: 10.21500/20112084.5234. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Cakmak A.F., Cevik E.I. Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire: Development of Turkish version of 18-item short form. Afr. J. Bus. Manag. 2010;4:2097–2102. [Google Scholar]
- 49.Medrano L.A., Moretti L., Ortiz Á., Pereno G.L. Validación del Cuestionario de Regulación Emocional en universitarios de Córdoba, Argentina. Psykhe (Santiago) 2013;22:83–96. doi: 10.7764/psykhe.22.1.473. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Navarro-Loli J., Dominguez-Lara S., Medrano L.A. Estructura Interna del Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (CERQ-18) en una Muestra de Adolescentes Peruanos. Rev. Iberoam. Diagnóstico Y Evaluación E Avaliação Psicológica. 2020;54:165–178. doi: 10.21865/RIDEP54.1.13. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 51.American Psychological Association . Ethical Principles of Psychologists and Code of Conduct. American Psychological Association; Washington, DC, USA: 2010. [Google Scholar]
- 52.World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki—Ethical Principles for Medical Research Involving Human Subjects. [(accessed on 3 December 2021)]. Available online: www.wma.net/policiespost/wma-declaration-of-helsinki-ethical-principles-formedical-research-involving-human-subjects/
- 53.Hair J.F., Black W.C., Babin B.J., Anderson R.E. Multivariate Data Analysis. 7th ed. Prentice Hall; New York, NY, USA: 2010. [Google Scholar]
- 54.R Core Team . R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Foundation for Statistical Computing; Vienna, Austria: 2018. [(accessed on 10 September 2021)]. Available online: https://www.R-project.org/ [Google Scholar]
- 55.Rosseel Y. lavaan: AnRPackage for Structural Equation Modeling. J. Stat. Softw. 2012;48:1–36. doi: 10.18637/jss.v048.i02. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Hu L.T., Bentler P.M. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Equ. Model. Multidiscip. J. 1999;6:1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Browne M.W., Cudeck R. Alternative ways of assessing model fit. Sage Focus Ed. 1993;154:136. doi: 10.1177/0049124192021002005. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Chen F.F. Sensitivity of Goodness of Fit Indexes to Lack of Measurement Invariance. Struct. Equ. Model. Multidiscip. J. 2007;14:464–504. doi: 10.1080/10705510701301834. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Cheung G.W., Rensvold R.B. Evaluating Goodness-of-Fit Indexes for Testing Measurement Invariance. Struct. Equ. Model. Multidiscip. J. 2002;9:233–255. doi: 10.1207/S15328007SEM0902_5. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Gadermann A.M., Guhn M., Zumbo D. Estimating ordinal reliability for Likert-type and ordinal item response data: A conceptual, empirical, and practical guide. Pract. Assess. Res. Eval. 2012;17:3. doi: 10.7275/n560-j767. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 61.McDonald R.P. Test Theory: A Unified Treatment. Psychology Press; New York, NY, USA: 2013. [Google Scholar]
- 62.Revelle W., Zinbarg R.E. Coefficients Alpha, Beta, Omega, and the glb: Comments on Sijtsma. Psychometrika. 2009;74:145–154. doi: 10.1007/s11336-008-9102-z. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Fornell C., Larcker D.F. Evaluating Structural Equation Models with Unobservable Variables and Measurement Error. J. Mark. Res. 1981;18:39–50. doi: 10.1177/002224378101800104. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Anderson J.C., Gerbing D.W. Structural equation modeling in practice: A review and recommended two-step approach. Psychol. Bull. 1988;103:411–423. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.103.3.411. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Henseler J., Ringle C.M., Sarstedt M. A new criterion for assessing discriminant validity in variance-based structural equation modeling. J. Acad. Market. Sci. 2015;43:115–135. doi: 10.1007/s11747-014-0403-8. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Chamizo Nieto M.T., Rey Peña L., Sánchez Álvarez N. Validation of the Spanish version of the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire in adolescents. Psicothema. 2020;32:153–159. doi: 10.7334/psicothema2019.156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Orgilés M., Morales A., Fernández-Martínez I., Ortigosa-Quiles J.M., Espada J.P. Spanish adaptation and psychometric properties of the child version of the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire. PLoS ONE. 2018;13:e0201656. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0201656. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Dominguez-Lara S., Merino-Soto C. Cognitive Emotional Regulation Questionnaire-18 en Universitarios: Evidencias de Validez Convergente y Discriminante. Rev. Iberoam. Diagnóstico Evaluación Avaliação Psicológica. 2018;47:171–184. doi: 10.21865/RIDEP47.2.12. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Castro A.P., Nunes M.J., Pimenteira R.L. Evidence of validity, reliability and psychometric parameters of the items of the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire-Short (CERQ-Short) Psico USF. 2020;25:547–559. doi: 10.1590/1413-82712020250312. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Santos A.C., Simões C., Daniel J.R., Arriaga P. Portuguese validation of the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire short version in youth: Validity, reliability and invariance across gender and age. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 2021:1–16. doi: 10.1080/17405629.2021.2011201. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Propheta I., van Zyl C.J. Measuring cognitive emotion regulation in South Africa using the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire-short form. Afr. J. Psychol. Assess. 2019;1:1–6. doi: 10.4102/ajopa.v1i0.9. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Garnefski N., Kraaij V. Cognitive emotion regulation questionnaire – development of a short 18-item version (CERQ-short) Pers. Individ. Differ. 2006;41:1045–1053. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2006.04.010. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Balzarotti S., Biassoni F., Villani D., Prunas A., Velotti P. Individual differences in cognitive emotion regulation: Implications for subjective and psychological well-being. J. Happiness Stud. 2016;17:125–143. doi: 10.1007/s10902-014-9587-3. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Görgen S.M., Loch N., Hiller W., Witthöft M. Kognitive Emotionsregulation und Psychopathologie: Erste teststatistische Untersuchung des deutschen Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (CERQ) in einer klinischen Stichprobe. Z. Für Psychiatr. Psychol. Psychother. 2015;63:255–265. doi: 10.1024/1661-4747/a000248. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Del Valle M.V., Zamora E.V., Grave L., Merlo E., Maidana J., Urquijo S. El rol de las estrategias cognitivas de regulación emocional en la depresión: Estudio en población universitaria. Rev. Investig. Científica Psicol. 2021;18:7–25. [Google Scholar]
- 76.Gutiérrez Trigo J.M., Márquez R. Relación de las estrategias de afrontamiento cognitivas con sintomatología de ansiedad y depresión. Apunt. Psicol. 2020;38:35–41. [Google Scholar]
- 77.Navarro J., Vara M.D., Cebolla A., Baños R.M. Validación psicométrica del cuestionario de regulación emocional (ERQ-CA) en población adolescente española. Rev. Psicol. Clínica Niños Adolesc. 2018;5:9–15. doi: 10.21134/rpcna.2018.05.1.1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author. The data are not publicly available due to privacy reasons.