Key Points
Question
At what age do individuals with higher genetic risk of Alzheimer disease first show cognitive differences from individuals with lower genetic risk, and which of 32 cognitive measures show the earliest difference?
Findings
In this cross-sectional study of 405 050 individuals, higher genetic risk of Alzheimer disease significantly modified the association of age with 13 of 32 cognitive measures. Best-fitting models suggested that higher genetic risk of Alzheimer disease was associated with changes in cognitive scores of individuals older than 56 years for all 13 measures and older than 47 years for 9 measures.
Meaning
These findings suggest that by early midlife, subtle differences in cognitive measures may emerge among individuals with higher genetic risk of Alzheimer disease.
Abstract
Importance
Identifying the youngest age when Alzheimer disease (AD) influences cognition and the earliest affected cognitive domains will improve understanding of the natural history of AD and approaches to early diagnosis.
Objective
To evaluate the age at which cognitive differences between individuals with higher compared with lower genetic risk of AD are first apparent and which cognitive assessments show the earliest difference.
Design, Setting, and Participants
This cross-sectional study used data from UK Biobank participants of European genetic ancestry, aged 40 years or older, who contributed genotypic and cognitive test data from January 1, 2006, to December 31, 2015. Data analysis was performed from March 10, 2020, to January 4, 2022.
Exposure
The AD genetic risk score (GRS), which is a weighted sum of 23 single-nucleotide variations.
Main Outcomes and Measures
Seven cognitive tests were administered via touchscreen at in-person visits or online. Cognitive domains assessed included fluid intelligence, episodic memory, processing speed, executive functioning, and prospective memory. Multiple cognitive measures were derived from some tests, yielding 32 separate measures. Interactions between age and AD-GRS for each of the 32 cognitive measures were tested with linear regression using a Bonferroni-corrected P value threshold. For cognitive measures with significant evidence of age by AD-GRS interaction, the youngest age of interaction was assessed with new regression models, with nonlinear specification of age terms. Models with youngest age of interaction from 40 to 70 years, in 1-year increments, were compared, and the best-fitting model for each cognitive measure was chosen. Results across cognitive measures were compared to determine which cognitive indicators showed earliest AD-related change.
Results
A total of 405 050 participants (mean [SD] age, 57.1 [7.9] years; 54.1% female) were included. Sample sizes differed across cognitive tests (from 12 455 to 404 682 participants). The AD-GRS significantly modified the association with age on 13 measures derived from the pairs matching (range in difference in mean cognition per decade increase in age for 1-SD higher AD-GRS, 2.5%-11.5%), symbol digit substitution (range in difference in mean cognition per decade increase in age for 1-SD higher AD-GRS, 2.0%-5.8%), and numeric memory tests (difference in mean cognition per decade increase in age for 1-SD higher AD-GRS, 8.8%) (P = 1.56 × 10−3). Best-fitting models suggested that cognitive scores of individuals with a high vs low AD-GRS began to diverge by 56 years of age for all 13 measures and by 47 years of age for 9 measures.
Conclusions and Relevance
In this cross-sectional study, by early midlife, subtle differences in memory and attention were detectable among individuals with higher genetic risk of AD.
This cross-sectional study of UK Biobank participants older than 40 years evaluates the youngest age at which the genetic risk scores for Alzheimer disease are associated with cognitive outcomes.
Introduction
A diagnosis of Alzheimer disease (AD) is preceded by a decades-long process of accumulating cerebral pathology.1,2 However, neither the precise age when symptoms of disease-related pathology begin nor the earliest symptomatic manifestations have been established. Identification of the earliest indicators of AD would improve understanding of the course of disease development. Identification of cognitive domains most sensitive to early changes would help guide effective screening, prevention, and treatment.
Longitudinal study designs that measure midlife cognition and late-life AD are impractical for identifying the timing and cognitive domains of the earliest AD manifestations. Such studies would require decades of follow-up and could not distinguish cognitive reserve from early disease-related changes. Innovations using genetic information offer more practical study designs.3 With the use of a genetic risk score (GRS) that is associated with AD development in late life, it is possible to detect early symptoms of AD in existing midlife cohorts.4,5,6 The AD-GRS is determined before early-life phenotypes, but associations between the AD-GRS and AD symptoms emerge as individuals age. The earliest AD symptoms are subtle, requiring a large sample for detection. Most previous studies7,8,9,10,11,12,13,14 of genetics and cognition used populations older than 65 years. Some studies8 examining associations of genetics with overall cognition in middle-aged to older adults have found that effects are stronger after 65 years of age. Few large-scale studies of middle-aged to older adults have comprehensively examined interactions between age and AD genetic risk with multiple cognitive measures.
Although episodic memory changes are generally considered leading indicators of AD,15,16,17,18,19,20 subtle changes in other domains, such as semantic memory, processing speed, and executive functioning, may occur at the same age or earlier.20,21,22,23 The potential for symptoms to manifest in any of multiple cognitive domains can be evaluated in parallel using a hypothesis-free approach to rapidly screen numerous possible indicators of disease based on phenotypes associated with a GRS.24,25 We adapt this method to evaluate potential early cognitive indicators of AD.
Because the hallmark of AD is age-related emergence of cognitive deficits,26 we applied an hypothesis-free method to identify cognitive domains differentially associated with the combination of aging and an AD-GRS. Considering 32 cognitive function indicators covering heterogeneous cognitive domains, we evaluated the youngest age at which the AD-GRS was associated with cognitive outcomes and which cognitive assessments showed the earliest changes. By estimating models in the large UK Biobank study, we had excellent power to detect subtle associations.
Methods
Study Setting and Participants
The UK Biobank is an ongoing cohort study, described in detail elsewhere.27 More than 500 000 individuals aged 40 to 69 years enrolled from January 1, 2006, to December 31, 2010, providing biological samples and survey responses. Online or in-person cognitive assessments were fielded for some or all of the study participants. Given the challenge of participation in the UK Biobank study, the prevalence of mild cognitive impairment and dementia is low.28,29 Ethical approval for UK Biobank data collection was obtained from the National Health Service National Research Ethics Service; all participants provided written informed consent. Analyses for the current cross-sectional study were based on fully deidentified data with no access to identifiers and therefore deemed not human subjects research by the University of California, San Francisco Institutional Review Board. This study follows the Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) reporting guideline for cross-sectional studies.30
From 497 087 UK Biobank participants 40 years or older at baseline, we excluded participants with 1 or more of the following: missing genetic information (n = 15 210 [3.1%]), non-European genetic ancestry (n = 78 494 [15.8%]), or no completed cognitive tests (n = 946 [0.002%]). Our final eligible sample included 405 050 participants. Cognitive assessments were not all conducted for all respondents, and our analytic sample ranged from 12 455 to 404 682 participants across the cognitive tests (Table 1), with data collected from January 1, 2006, to December 31, 2015. Data analysis was performed from March 10, 2020, to January 4, 2022.
Table 1. Sample Composition.
Variable | No. of participants | Age, mean (SD), ya | Female, % | AD-GRS, mean (SD) | AD-GRS range |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Fluid intelligence (logic and reasoning) | |||||
No. correct in person | 128 550 | 57.5 (7.8) | 53.8 | 0.012 (0.996) | 0-13 |
No. attempted in person | 128 550 | 57.5 (7.8) | 53.8 | 0.012 (0.996) | 1-13 |
No. correct online | 102 873 | 62.8 (7.5) | 55.4 | 0.002 (0.989) | 0-14 |
Numeric memory (short-term memory capacity and attention) | |||||
No. correct in person | 41 811 | 57.2 (7.9) | 54.2 | 0.011 (0.996) | 2-12 |
No. correct online | 92 625 | 62.6 (7.5) | 55.4 | 0.001 (0.989) | 2-11 |
Pairs matching (short-term memory and attention) | |||||
No. correct in person (round 1) | 404 682 | 57.1 (7.9) | 54.1 | 0.019 (1.001) | 0-8 |
No. correct in person (round 2) | 404 682 | 57.1 (7.9) | 54.1 | 0.019 (1.001) | 0-6 |
No. incorrect in person (round 1) | 404 682 | 57.1 (7.9) | 54.1 | 0.019 (1.001) | 0-146 |
No. incorrect in person (round 2) | 404 682 | 57.1 (7.9) | 54.1 | 0.019 (1.001) | 0-146 |
Time to complete in person (round 1) | 396 627 | 57 (7.9) | 54.1 | 0.018 (1) | 40-13 751 |
Time to complete in person (round 2) | 395 763 | 57 (7.9) | 54.2 | 0.018 (1) | 91-13 751 |
No. correct online (round 1) | 98 753 | 62.8 (7.5) | 55.5 | 0.003 (0.99) | 0-8 |
No. incorrect online (round 1) | 98 753 | 62.8 (7.5) | 55.5 | 0.003 (0.99) | 0-50 |
No. incorrect online (round 2) | 12 567 | 60.4 (7.6) | 55 | −0.003 (0.989) | 0-33 |
Time to complete online (round 1) | 95 178 | 62.6 (7.5) | 55.7 | 0.001 (0.988) | 2195-99 988 |
Time to complete online (round 2) | 12 455 | 60.3 (7.6) | 54.9 | −0.004 (0.988) | 10851-99 988 |
Prospective memory | |||||
First attempt correct in person | 131 255 | 57.5 (7.8) | 53.8 | 0.013 (0.997) | 0-1 |
First or second attempt correct in person | 131 255 | 57.5 (7.8) | 53.8 | 0.013 (0.997) | 0-1 |
Reaction time | |||||
Mean time | 402 528 | 57.1 (7.9) | 54.1 | 0.018 (1.001) | 63-2000 |
Symbol digit substitution (processing speed) | |||||
No. attempted in person | 13 986 | 63.8 (7.3) | 50.9 | 0.006 (0.984) | 1-105 |
No. correct in person | 13 986 | 63.8 (7.3) | 50.9 | 0.006 (0.984) | 0-37 |
No. correct online | 98 692 | 62.8 (7.5) | 55.4 | 0.002 (0.99) | 0-103 |
No. attempted online | 98 692 | 62.8 (7.5) | 55.4 | 0.002 (0.99) | 1-109 |
Time to complete 10 substitutions online | 75 997 | 62.6 (7.5) | 54.8 | −0.001 (0.985) | 12 146-152 295 |
Trail-making (processing speed and/or executive functioning) | |||||
Time to complete numeric trail in person | 13 958 | 63.8 (7.3) | 50.8 | 0.007 (0.984) | 94-2557 |
No. of errors in numeric trail in person | 14 093 | 63.8 (7.3) | 50.9 | 0.007 (0.985) | 0-147 |
Time to complete alphanumeric trail in person | 13 638 | 63.7 (7.3) | 50.8 | 0.005 (0.984) | 160-5768 |
No. of errors in alphanumeric trail in person | 14 093 | 63.8 (7.3) | 50.9 | 0.007 (0.985) | 0-165 |
Time to complete numeric trail online | 86 861 | 62.5 (7.5) | 54.5 | −0.002 (0.987) | 13.666-733.97 |
Time to complete alphanumeric trail online | 86 859 | 62.5 (7.5) | 54.5 | −0.002 (0.987) | 20.556-746.531 |
No. of errors in numeric trail online | 22 029 | 63.2 (7.5) | 59.7 | 0.019 (0.998) | 1-536 |
No. of errors in alphanumeric trail online | 35 462 | 63.6 (7.4) | 57.2 | 0.007 (0.997) | 1-320 |
Full sample | |||||
Total | 404 050 | 57.1 (7.9) | 54.1 | 0.029 (1) | NA |
Abbreviations: AD, Alzheimer disease; GRS, genetic risk score; NA, not applicable.
For each cognitive test, the age represents the age at testing. For the full sample, age represents the age at recruitment.
Genotyping and AD Genetic Risk Scores
UK Biobank samples were genotyped in batches of approximately 4700 with 2 assays (UK BiLEVE array and UK Biobank Axiom array) using genotyping and quality control methods detailed elsewhere.31,32 We calculated AD-GRSs using single-nucleotide variations (SNVs) identified in a meta-analysis of genome-wide association studies of AD.33 The AD-GRS is a weighted sum of 23 SNVs that has been validated as being associated with dementia outcomes in prior work.4,5,6,34 For the goal of this study, it was essential that the AD-GRS is associated with dementia. Exploratory evaluations demonstrated that the AD-GRS is associated with an AD diagnosis in the UK Biobank (eAppendix 1 and eTables 1 and 2 in the Supplement). In quality control checks, we found that the AD-GRS was more strongly associated with cognition based on verbal reasoning than a newer GRS that included additional loci from a 2019 International Genomics of Alzheimer Project meta-analysis.35 Higher AD-GRS scores correspond to higher risk of developing AD. For analysis, we standardized the AD-GRS by centering at the full sample’s mean and dividing by its SD
Our AD-GRS included weights for apolipoprotein E (APOE) ε4 alleles. In sensitivity analyses, we used an alternative AD-GRS calculated without SNVs from the APOE region. This alternative AD-GRS included 21 SNVs (eAppendix 2 and eTable 3 in the Supplement). In addition, we used the count of APOE ε4 alleles in place of the AD-GRS measure to assess the impact of APOE. All AD-GRSs were created using PLINK, version 1.9.36,37
Cognitive Measures (Phenotypes)
Cognitive measurements were conducted in person at UK Biobank assessment centers and through online follow-up. We considered only cognitive measures available for at least 10 000 individuals. Domains assessed using 7 instruments included fluid intelligence, episodic memory, processing speed, executive functioning, and prospective memory. Assessments as fielded in the UK Biobank are detailed in eAppendix 3 in the Supplement. Multiple measures were derived from some tests, including total score, component or round scores, completion status for the entire test or rounds of the test, and duration to complete a test or its components or rounds (eAppendix 2 and eTable 4 in the Supplement). This process resulted in 32 variables, all of which were coded such that larger positive values correspond to better performance.
When measures were obtained at multiple visits, we used the earliest available measure for each test to maximize sample sizes. Online and in-person versions of the same instrument were treated as distinct phenotypes because, even when measures tap the same underlying construct, 1 mode of administration may be more sensitive to early changes. Some participants completed both the in-person and online versions of an instrument.
Covariates
In addition to phenotype and GRS data, our analyses considered age at cognitive assessment, self-reported sex, genotyping assay (a binary indicator for whether the UK BiLEVE or UK Biobank Axiom array was used), assessment center (when applicable), practice effect (online assessments only; an indicator of whether the participant previously completed the assessment center version of the test0), and 10 genetic ancestry principal components provided by the UK Biobank to account for population stratification in the sample.
Statistical Analysis
Our primary interests were to identify which cognitive assessments show population differences by AD-GRS at the youngest age and estimate the youngest age of differences in those outcomes. We first estimated regression models with the interaction of age and the AD-GRS for each of the 32 cognitive assessments to select a smaller set of cognitive phenotypes for more detailed evaluation. We fit models of cognitive score (Y) as a function of age, z-scored AD-GRS (ADGRSz), their interaction, and covariates Wi using linear regression (identity link) for continuous or ordinal phenotype variables and logistic regression (logit link) for binary phenotype variables: Y ∼ Link (b0 + b1 Age + b2 ADGRSz + b3 ADGRSz × Age + ∑ ibiWi).
We estimated the association between age and each cognitive score for people with an average AD-GRS (ie, b2 [the age main term]), the difference in the age slope associated with a 1-SD higher AD-GRS (ie, b3 [the interaction of age and ADGRSz]), and the percentage increase in the rate of change with age associated with a 1-SD higher AD-GRS compared with someone with an average AD-GRS (ie, 100 × b3/b2). Our primary coefficient of interest, b3, provides an estimate of whether the association of the AD-GRS with the cognitive phenotype is stronger for people of older age. For each coefficient, we report point estimates and 95% CIs. Our code is available at GitHub.38
If the interaction of age and the AD-GRS was statistically significant for the cognitive measure at a Bonferroni-corrected threshold (P < .05/32 = 1.56 × 10−3), we next fit models to detect the age at which the AD-GRS was associated with changes in the cognitive measure using a novel application of a standard cross-validation technique for best-fit model choice. To detect the youngest age at which divergence occurred (ie, the age at which the population average cognitive measures begin to separate based on level of AD-GRS), a cognitive score (Y) was fit to a quadratic function of age (t, centered at 40 years), a threshold function to detect age of divergence, and covariates Wi: Y ∼ Link (b0 + b1 t + b2 t2 + b3 ADGRSz × I(t > tthreshold) × (t − t > tthreshold)3 + ∑ ibiWi).
The threshold function was operationalized as an interaction between an indicator function I(t>tthreshold) for age above an hypothesized minimum age of divergence (tthreshold), age above the threshold cubed, and ADGRSz. This specification allowed modeled mean cognitive scores to smoothly diverge based on level of AD-GRS beginning at the specified threshold age. Below the threshold age, the association between AD-GRS and cognition (conditional on covariates) is constrained to zero so that the association between centered age and the outcome is governed by b1t + b2t2. Above the threshold, the cognitive score is allowed to diverge smoothly from b1t + b2t2 by adding a third-order term b3ADGRSz × (t − tthreshold)3 (eAppendix 4 in the Supplement).
We evaluated alternative hypothesized ages of 40 to 70 years for the threshold. Models with different thresholds were compared based on the mean squared prediction error in a 10-fold cross-validation,39 and we selected the age threshold from the model with the minimum mean squared prediction error (eFigure 1 in the Supplement). Within a given covariate stratum, this approach indicates that, below the selected age threshold, the mean cognitive measures in the data are best represented by a single value at a given age. However, above the threshold value, the data are better represented by AD-GRS–specific mean cognitive measures at each age. The range of threshold values evaluated included the full range of participant ages in the sample for the cognitive measure. Thus, a selected threshold at the lowest tested value indicates that the data are better represented by AD-GRS–specific curves across ages, whereas a selected threshold at the upper end of the range indicates that the data are better represented by a single curve across ages regardless of AD-GRS. For demonstration, we then used this model to simulate and plot anticipated average cognitive measures across age at the median values of covariates, comparing higher (95th percentile) and lower (fifth percentile) AD-GRSs.
In sensitivity analyses, all models were replicated using the alternative AD-GRS that omitted SNVs in the APOE region. In addition, to test whether results were sensitive to our modeling assumptions, we repeated the analyses using a more rigid linear age term with a quadratic divergence term above the threshold and a more flexible cubic age term with a fourth-order divergence term above the threshold (eAppendix 5 in the Supplement).
Results
A total of 405 050 participants (mean [SD] age, 57.1 [7.9] years; 54.1% female) were included (Table 1). Of the 32 cognitive outcomes evaluated using a Bonferroni-corrected P value threshold for statistical significance (P = 1.56 × 10−3), there was evidence that the AD-GRS modified the association of age with 13 measures derived from the pairs matching (range in difference in mean cognition per decade increase in age for 1-SD higher AD-GRS, 2.5%-11.5%), symbol digit substitution (range in difference in mean cognition per decade increase in age for 1-SD higher AD-GRS, 2.0%-5.8%), and numeric memory tests (difference in mean cognition per decade increase in age for 1-SD higher AD-GRS, 8.8%) (Table 2; eTable 5 in the Supplement). Among these measures, the difference in mean cognition per decade increase in age associated with a 1-SD higher AD-GRS was greatest for the number correct in round 1 of the online pairs matching task, with an 11.5% increase in age-related differences per 1-SD higher AD-GRS.
Table 2. Association of AD-GRS at 40 Years of Age and Modification of Age Slope by AD-GRS for Each Cognitive Assessment With a Statistically Significant Age by AD-GRS Interaction in Linear Regression Models.
Variable | Age slope per decade for person with mean GRS (95% CI)a | Difference in age slope for person with 1-SD higher GRS (95% CI)b | Difference in mean cognition per decade increase in age for 1-SD higher AD-GRS, %c |
---|---|---|---|
Pairs matching (short-term memory and attention) | |||
No. correct online (round 1) | −0.124 (−0.132 to −0.115) | −0.014 (−0.023 to −0.006) | 11.5 |
No. correct in person (round 1) | −0.091 (−0.095 to −0.087) | −0.007 (−0.011 to −0.003) | 7.9 |
No. in person correct (round 2) | −0.093 (−0.097 to −0.089) | −0.009 (−0.013 to −0.005) | 9.4 |
Time to complete in person (round 1) | −0.207 (−0.211 to −0.203) | −0.011 (−0.015 to −0.007) | 5.3 |
No. incorrect in person (round 1) | −0.144 (−0.148 to −0.140) | −0.006 (−0.010 to −0.002) | 4.4 |
Time to complete online (round 1) | −0.440 (−0.448 to −0.432) | −0.015 (−0.023 to −0.007) | 3.5 |
Time to complete in person (round 2) | −0.312 (−0.315 to −0.308) | −0.008 (−0.012 to −0.004) | 2.5 |
Numeric memory (short-term memory and attention) | |||
No. correct online | −0.180 (−0.189 to −0.172) | −0.016 (−0.025 to −0.007) | 8.8 |
Symbol digit substitution (processing speed) | |||
No. correct in person | −0.599 (−0.620 to −0.579) | −0.035 (−0.056 to −0.014) | 5.8 |
No. attempted in person | −0.621 (−0.642 to −0.601) | −0.036 (−0.056 to −0.015) | 5.7 |
Time to complete 10 substitutions online | −0.521 (−0.531 to −0.513) | −0.024 (−0.033 to −0.015) | 4.5 |
No. correct online | −0.588 (−0.594 to −0.579) | −0.014 (−0.022 to −0.007) | 2.4 |
No. attempted online | −0.617 (−0.624 to −0.610) | −0.012 (−0.020 to −0.005) | 2.0 |
Abbreviations: AD, Alzheimer disease; GRS, genetic risk score.
Coefficient on age term (b2) in eEquation 1 in eAppendix 6 in the Supplement.
Coefficient on AD-GRSz × age term (b3) in eEquation 1 in eAppendix 6 in the Supplement.
(100 × b3/b2) using the coefficients from eEquation 1 in eAppendix 6 in the Supplement.
From these 13 measures for which AD-GRS modified the association with age, we fit nonlinear models to estimate the youngest age of divergence in cognitive test performance between people with higher (95th percentile) vs lower (fifth percentile) AD-GRS (Figure 1 and Figure 2; eTable 6, and eFigures 2 and 3 in the Supplement). There was evidence of divergence at the youngest testable age for 8 of the 13 measures. The measures with the youngest observed age of divergence were for number correct in round 1 (age slope per decade for person with mean GRS in the corresponding linear model, −0.091; 95% CI, −0.095 to −0.087) and time to complete round 2 of the in-person pairs matching test (age slope per decade for person with mean GRS in the corresponding linear model, −0.312; 95% CI, −0.315 to −0.308). For those measures, the divergence occurred by age 40 years; because 40 years is the minimum enrollment age for the UK Biobank, no more specificity was possible. Best-fitting models suggested that cognitive scores of individuals with high vs low AD-GRSs began to diverge by 56 years of age for all 13 measures and by 47 years of age for 9 measures.
In sensitivity analyses, models using APOE ε4 allele count showed similar patterns to the main analysis (eAppendixes 6 and 7 and eTables 7-10 in the Supplement), and models using the AD-GRSs that omitted APOE ε4 had attenuated modification of age associations compared with the main analysis (eAppendix 8 and eTables 11 and 12 in the Supplement). Use of more flexible functional forms led to similar or earlier ages of divergence in most cases (eAppendix 8, eTables 13-15, and eFigures 4 and 5 in the Supplement).
Discussion
Using a large UK Biobank sample, we found that people with higher AD genetic risk began to manifest subtle changes in 3 cognitive tests (pairs matching, symbol digit substitution, and numeric memory) by early middle age. Several measures from these tests suggested changes began before 45 years of age. Some measures appeared to diverge before the minimum age of study participants with available data, so we could not pinpoint the age of divergence. These results suggest early changes in cognitive domains of attention, short-term memory, and processing speed among participants at higher risk of developing clinical AD in later life based on genetic risk.
Sensitivity analyses excluding APOE showed slightly older ages of divergence, suggesting that, as expected, APOE ε4 allele carriers experience earlier cognitive decline. Our findings are consistent with prior work1,15,16,17,18,21,40,41,42,43 in early- and late-onset AD, suggesting that cognitive and physiologic changes associated with AD begin at least 15 years before diagnosis—the mean age of dementia diagnosis for White UK residents is 82 years.44 Our study advances understanding of the natural history of late-onset AD by showing that, in a generally healthy, community-dwelling sample, those at high genetic risk of developing AD in late life performed worse on several cognitive measures in midlife.
Several studies7,8 have examined AD-GRS or APOE in middle-aged to older adults. Although some studies45,46,47have found associations to be stronger in older adults, few studies7,48 have found significant interactions with age in adjusted analyses, including in a prior UK Biobank study7 of approximately 100 000 participants. Davies et al8 found a significant age association for APOE and general cognitive function after 65 years of age; however, outcomes were based on a meta-analysis8 of different cohorts generally of older ages (mean cohort aged 55-80 years). Our work adds to this literature in important ways. We used a large sample size, which allows for relatively stronger power to detect effects. We focused on identifying the youngest ages at which APOE or the AD-GRS was associated with differences in cognitive tests by 40 to 70 years of age. We examined 30 or more cognitive measures and prioritized those most strongly associated with AD-GRS. Finally, we compared the estimated associations between an AD-GRS with APOE, AD-GRS without APOE, and APOE ε4 allele alone.
Functional brain differences of APOE ε4 carriers compared with noncarriers have been observed among people in their 20s.49,50 A previous study51 found that reduced cognition in midlife or even earlier was associated with increased risk of AD. However, studies of midlife cognitive assessments and subsequent late-onset AD52 have not been able to distinguish whether cognitive assessments are associated with AD because they indicate cognitive reserve or because they are early manifestations of disease.53 For instance, cognitive test scores as early as 11 years of age may be associated with diagnosis of late-onset AD,54 but this association was more likely because early-life cognitive scores provide cognitive reserve or delay onset or diagnosis. Our study design avoided this ambiguity in interpretation because we focused on age-related differences rather than level of cognitive performance. Because we focused on detecting age-related differences (ie, the interaction of AD-GRS and age) and because the genetic measure of AD risk in late life is not influenced by early-life exposures, the measures that begin to diverge in midlife must be early symptoms of disease rather than indicators of cognitive reserve or variables that simply delay diagnosis of AD.
Episodic memory is often considered the earliest indicator of preclinical AD,15,16,17,18,19,20 but changes to category fluency, naming, executive functioning,23 and visuoconstruction abilities55 may also occur early,40 and multidomain approaches may be especially sensitive to preclinical AD.15 Our results are consistent with previous research56,57 that suggests that processing speed and short-term memory are particularly sensitive early indicators of cognitive decline. The findings of early cognitive changes are consistent with some prior biomarker research41,58 that documented changes in cerebral spinal fluid and neuroimaging markers by late midlife. The current study adds to these findings by showing early differences in processing speed and short-term memory or attention in those at high genetic risk for AD.
Our findings suggest that biological processes underlying AD may begin to exert clinical effects decades earlier than the age at which clinical trials commonly enroll patients with AD; for example, the Finnish Geriatric Intervention Study to Prevent Cognitive Impairment and Disability (FINGER) trial59 enrolled participants aged 60 to 77 years, and the Advanced Cognitive Training for Independent and Vital Elderly (ACTIVE) trial60 enrolled participants older than 65 years. Early enrollment has been proposed as a way forward after disappointing results of recent trials,61 but our findings suggest that prevention may need to start 25 or more years before likely age at onset.
Strengths and Limitations
A strength of this study design is the identification of very early symptoms of AD while avoiding the ambiguity between factors that influence cognitive reserve or ease of diagnosis and those that reflect early symptoms. This design is a feasible approach to evaluate the earliest manifestations of AD without requiring long follow-up. By using cross-validation to choose the threshold age at which the GRS begins to influence cognition data adaptively, our modeling approach provides an estimate of the age at which AD begins to exert influence on each phenotype. In addition, the hypothesis-free approach allows comparison of multiple cognitive indicators to identify the most important early indicators of AD.
This study also has some limitations. First, this study was cross-sectional and interpreted differences in mean cognitive scores between older and younger people and level of AD-GRS. Such cross-sectional age differences represent the combination of biological aging, cohort differences, and any differential selection by age. Measurement error in the cognitive tests also makes it more difficult to detect age-associated changes in test performance. Because there is some evidence of a healthy volunteer effect in the UK Biobank that becomes more pronounced with age,29,62 future work examining longitudinal cognitive changes may be informative. Second, we restricted analysis to individuals with European ancestry—the predominant population of previous AD-GRS studies—and our results may not reflect patterns in other populations. Third, the UK Biobank cognitive test battery was not comprehensive and omitted measures that may be sensitive to early cognitive changes in AD, such as tests of episodic memory that capture long-delay recall and recognition,63,64 and measures of verbal fluency,23 language,65,66 and working memory67 or other approaches.68 With more sensitive measures, we may have detected even earlier changes. Fourth, caution in interpreting test differences is merited because the samples differed in size and composition. With a large sample size, some cognitive measures may have had an earlier detectable age of divergence. Fifth, although useful for improving understanding of the natural history of AD, the mean differences in cognition by AD-GRS are too small to be clinically relevant at younger ages. Sixth, selective survival or selective study participation may have introduced bias. Previous research using the same GRS in the UK Biobank with smoking as a negative control suggested that survival bias is minimal; this AD-GRS did not modify the age association with smoking despite the strong association between smoking and survival.69 Although participation in the UK Biobank might be differential for individuals with high AD-GRSs, selection bias would likely attenuate our findings because including more impaired higher AD-GRS cases would improve power to detect early differences by AD-GRS.
Conclusions
This cross-sectional study adds to the increasing evidence that cognitive outcomes associated with AD genes may begin in early midlife. The results indicate that multiple cognitive changes as early as 40 years of age may be relevant to AD development for some individuals. Research on the biological changes underlying the early cognitive symptoms is needed. Hypothesis-free approaches using genetic profiles have the potential to identify and compare early indicators of AD and other diseases with long preclinical periods.
References
- 1.Bateman RJ, Xiong C, Benzinger TLS, et al. ; Dominantly Inherited Alzheimer Network . Clinical and biomarker changes in dominantly inherited Alzheimer’s disease. N Engl J Med. 2012;367(9):795-804. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa1202753 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Jack CR Jr, Knopman DS, Jagust WJ, et al. Tracking pathophysiological processes in Alzheimer’s disease: an updated hypothetical model of dynamic biomarkers. Lancet Neurol. 2013;12(2):207-216. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(12)70291-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Davey Smith G, Hemani G. Mendelian randomization: genetic anchors for causal inference in epidemiological studies. Hum Mol Genet. 2014;23(R1):R89-R98. doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddu328 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Filshtein TJ, Brenowitz WD, Mayeda ER, et al. Reserve and Alzheimer’s disease genetic risk: effects on hospitalization and mortality. Alzheimers Dement. 2019;15(7):907-916. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2019.04.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Brenowitz WD, Filshtein TJ, Yaffe K, et al. Association of genetic risk for Alzheimer disease and hearing impairment. Neurology. 2020;95(16):e2225-e2234. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000010709 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Leng Y, Ackley SF, Glymour MM, Yaffe K, Brenowitz WD. Genetic risk of Alzheimer’s disease and sleep duration in non-demented elders. Ann Neurol. 2021;89(1):177-181. doi: 10.1002/ana.25910 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Lyall DM, Ward J, Ritchie SJ, et al. Alzheimer disease genetic risk factor APOE e4 and cognitive abilities in 111,739 UK Biobank participants. Age Ageing. 2016;45(4):511-517. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afw068 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Davies G, Armstrong N, Bis JC, et al. ; Generation Scotland . Genetic contributions to variation in general cognitive function: a meta-analysis of genome-wide association studies in the CHARGE consortium (N=53949). Mol Psychiatry. 2015;20(2):183-192. doi: 10.1038/mp.2014.188 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Powell DS, Kuo PL, Qureshi R, et al. The relationship of APOE ε4, race, and sex on the age of onset and risk of dementia. Front Neurol. 2021;12:735036. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2021.735036 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Marioni RE, Campbell A, Scotland G, Hayward C, Porteous DJ, Deary IJ. Differential effects of the APOE e4 allele on different domains of cognitive ability across the life-course. Eur J Hum Genet. 2016;24(6):919-923. doi: 10.1038/ejhg.2015.210 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Rawle MJ, Davis D, Bendayan R, Wong A, Kuh D, Richards M. Apolipoprotein-E (Apoe) ε4 and cognitive decline over the adult life course. Transl Psychiatry. 2018;8(1):18. doi: 10.1038/s41398-017-0064-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Caselli RJ, Dueck AC, Osborne D, et al. Longitudinal modeling of age-related memory decline and the APOE ε4 effect. N Engl J Med. 2009;361(3):255-263. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa0809437 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Wisdom NM, Callahan JL, Hawkins KA. The effects of apolipoprotein E on non-impaired cognitive functioning: a meta-analysis. Neurobiol Aging. 2011;32(1):63-74. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2009.02.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Bunce D, Bielak AAM, Anstey KJ, Cherbuin N, Batterham PJ, Easteal S. APOE genotype and cognitive change in young, middle-aged, and older adults living in the community. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2014;69(4):379-386. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glt103 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Riley KP, Jicha GA, Davis D, et al. Prediction of preclinical Alzheimer’s disease: longitudinal rates of change in cognition. J Alzheimers Dis. 2011;25(4):707-717. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2011-102133 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Bilgel M, An Y, Lang A, et al. Trajectories of Alzheimer disease–related cognitive measures in a longitudinal sample. Alzheimers Dement. 2014;10(6):735-742.e4. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2014.04.520 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Mistridis P, Krumm S, Monsch AU, Berres M, Taylor KI. The 12 years preceding mild cognitive impairment due to Alzheimer’s disease: the temporal emergence of cognitive decline. J Alzheimers Dis. 2015;48(4):1095-1107. doi: 10.3233/JAD-150137 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Bilgel M, Koscik RL, An Y, et al. Temporal order of Alzheimer’s disease–related cognitive marker changes in BLSA and WRAP longitudinal studies. J Alzheimers Dis. 2017;59(4):1335-1347. doi: 10.3233/JAD-170448 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Mortamais M, Ash JA, Harrison J, et al. Detecting cognitive changes in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease: a review of its feasibility. Alzheimers Dement. 2017;13(4):468-492. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2016.06.2365 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Belleville S, Fouquet C, Hudon C, Zomahoun HTV, Croteau J; Consortium for the Early Identification of Alzheimer’s Disease–Quebec . Neuropsychological measures that predict progression from mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer’s type dementia in older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neuropsychol Rev. 2017;27(4):328-353. doi: 10.1007/s11065-017-9361-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Amieva H, Le Goff M, Millet X, et al. Prodromal Alzheimer’s disease: successive emergence of the clinical symptoms. Ann Neurol. 2008;64(5):492-498. doi: 10.1002/ana.21509 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Schindler SE, Jasielec MS, Weng H, et al. Neuropsychological measures that detect early impairment and decline in preclinical Alzheimer disease. Neurobiol Aging. 2017;56:25-32. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2017.04.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Jutten RJ, Sikkes SAM, Amariglio RE, et al. ; Alzheimer Disease Neuroimaging Initiative; National Alzheimer’s Coordinating Center, the Harvard Aging Brain Study, and the Alzheimer Dementia Cohort . Identifying sensitive measures of cognitive decline at different clinical stages of Alzheimer’s disease. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2021;27(5):426-438. doi: 10.1017/S1355617720000934 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Denny JC, Ritchie MD, Basford MA, et al. PheWAS: demonstrating the feasibility of a phenome-wide scan to discover gene-disease associations. Bioinformatics. 2010;26(9):1205-1210. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btq126 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Millard LAC, Davies NM, Tilling K, Gaunt TR, Davey Smith G. Searching for the causal effects of body mass index in over 300 000 participants in UK Biobank, using Mendelian randomization. PLoS Genet. 2019;15(2):e1007951. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1007951 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.McKhann GM, Knopman DS, Chertkow H, et al. The diagnosis of dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2011;7(3):263-269. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Sudlow C, Gallacher J, Allen N, et al. UK biobank: an open access resource for identifying the causes of a wide range of complex diseases of middle and old age. PLoS Med. 2015;12(3):e1001779. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1001779 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Batty GD, Gale CR, Kivimäki M, Deary IJ, Bell S. Comparison of risk factor associations in UK Biobank against representative, general population based studies with conventional response rates: prospective cohort study and individual participant meta-analysis. BMJ. 2020;368:m131. doi: 10.1136/bmj.m131 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Fry A, Littlejohns TJ, Sudlow C, et al. Comparison of sociodemographic and health-related characteristics of UK Biobank participants with those of the general population. Am J Epidemiol. 2017;186(9):1026-1034. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwx246 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.von Elm E, Altman DG, Egger M, Pocock SJ, Gøtzsche PC, Vandenbroucke JP; STROBE Initiative . The Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) statement: guidelines for reporting observational studies. Epidemiology. 2007;18(6):800-804. doi: 10.1097/EDE.0b013e3181577654 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Genetic data. UK Biobank. Accessed September 13, 2020. https://www.ukbiobank.ac.uk/scientists-3/genetic-data/
- 32.Genotyping and quality control of UK Biobank, a large-scale, extensively phenotyped prospective resource. Accessed September 13, 2020. https://biobank.ctsu.ox.ac.uk/crystal/crystal/docs/genotyping_qc.pdf
- 33.Lambert JC, Ibrahim-Verbaas CA, Harold D, et al. ; European Alzheimer’s Disease Initiative (EADI); Genetic and Environmental Risk in Alzheimer’s Disease; Alzheimer’s Disease Genetic Consortium; Cohorts for Heart and Aging Research in Genomic Epidemiology . Meta-analysis of 74,046 individuals identifies 11 new susceptibility loci for Alzheimer’s disease. Nat Genet. 2013;45(12):1452-1458. doi: 10.1038/ng.2802 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Brenowitz WD, Zimmerman SC, Filshtein TJ, et al. Extension of mendelian randomization to identify earliest manifestations of Alzheimer disease: association of genetic risk score for Alzheimer disease with lower body mass index by age 50 years. Am J Epidemiol. 2021;190(10):2163-2171. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwab103 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Kunkle BW, Grenier-Boley B, Sims R, et al. ; Alzheimer Disease Genetics Consortium (ADGC); European Alzheimer’s Disease Initiative (EADI); Cohorts for Heart and Aging Research in Genomic Epidemiology Consortium (CHARGE); Genetic and Environmental Risk in AD/Defining Genetic, Polygenic and Environmental Risk for Alzheimer’s Disease Consortium (GERAD/PERADES) . Genetic meta-analysis of diagnosed Alzheimer’s disease identifies new risk loci and implicates Aβ, tau, immunity and lipid processing. Nat Genet. 2019;51(3):414-430. doi: 10.1038/s41588-019-0358-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Chang CC, Chow CC, Tellier LC, Vattikuti S, Purcell SM, Lee JJ. Second-generation PLINK: rising to the challenge of larger and richer datasets. Gigascience. 2015;4:7. doi: 10.1186/s13742-015-0047-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.PLINK 1.9. Accessed May 21, 2021. https://www.cog-genomics.org/plink/1.9
- 38.Scott Zimmerman. adgrs_cog. GitHub . Accessed March 3, 2020. https://github.com/ScottZimmerman/adgrs_cog
- 39.Hastie T, Tibshirani R, Friedman J. Elements of Statistical Learning: Data Mining, Inference, and Prediction. 2nd ed. Springer Science+Business Media LLC; 2009. Accessed September 13, 2020. https://web.stanford.edu/~hastie/ElemStatLearn/
- 40.Rajan KB, Wilson RS, Weuve J, Barnes LL, Evans DA. Cognitive impairment 18 years before clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer disease dementia. Neurology. 2015;85(10):898-904. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000001774 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Coupé P, Manjón JV, Lanuza E, Catheline G. Lifespan changes of the human brain in Alzheimer’s disease. Sci Rep. 2019;9(1):3998. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-39809-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.McDade E, Wang G, Gordon BA, et al. ; Dominantly Inherited Alzheimer Network . Longitudinal cognitive and biomarker changes in dominantly inherited Alzheimer disease. Neurology. 2018;91(14):e1295-e1306. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000006277 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Sperling R, Mormino E, Johnson K. The evolution of preclinical Alzheimer’s disease: implications for prevention trials. Neuron. 2014;84(3):608-622. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.10.038 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Mukadam N, Lewis G, Mueller C, Werbeloff N, Stewart R, Livingston G. Ethnic differences in cognition and age in people diagnosed with dementia: a study of electronic health records in two large mental healthcare providers. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2019;34(3):504-510. doi: 10.1002/gps.5046 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Reas ET, Laughlin GA, Bergstrom J, Kritz-Silverstein D, Barrett-Connor E, McEvoy LK. Effects of APOE on cognitive aging in community-dwelling older adults. Neuropsychology. 2019;33(3):406-416. doi: 10.1037/neu0000501 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Ge T, Sabuncu MR, Smoller JW, Sperling RA, Mormino EC; Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative . Dissociable influences of APOE ε4 and polygenic risk of AD dementia on amyloid and cognition. Neurology. 2018;90(18):e1605-e1612. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000005415 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Mielke MM, Machulda MM, Hagen CE, et al. Influence of amyloid and APOE on cognitive performance in a late middle-aged cohort. Alzheimers Dement. 2016;12(3):281-291. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2015.09.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Dik MG, Jonker C, Bouter LM, Geerlings MI, van Kamp GJ, Deeg DJ. APOE-epsilon4 is associated with memory decline in cognitively impaired elderly. Neurology. 2000;54(7):1492-1497. doi: 10.1212/WNL.54.7.1492 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Filippini N, MacIntosh BJ, Hough MG, et al. Distinct patterns of brain activity in young carriers of the APOE-ε4 allele. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009;106(17):7209-7214. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0811879106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Hodgetts CJ, Shine JP, Williams H, et al. Increased posterior default mode network activity and structural connectivity in young adult APOE-ε4 carriers: a multimodal imaging investigation. Neurobiol Aging. 2019;73:82-91. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2018.08.026 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Ritchie K, Ritchie CW, Yaffe K, Skoog I, Scarmeas N. Is late-onset Alzheimer’s disease really a disease of midlife? Alzheimers Dement (N Y). 2015;1(2):122-130. doi: 10.1016/j.trci.2015.06.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Satizabal CL, Beiser AS, Chouraki V, Chêne G, Dufouil C, Seshadri S. Incidence of dementia over three decades in the Framingham Heart Study. N Engl J Med. 2016;374(6):523-532. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa1504327 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Elias MF, Beiser A, Wolf PA, Au R, White RF, D’Agostino RB. The preclinical phase of Alzheimer disease: a 22-year prospective study of the Framingham Cohort. Arch Neurol. 2000;57(6):808-813. doi: 10.1001/archneur.57.6.808 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Russ TC, Hannah J, Batty GD, Booth CC, Deary IJ, Starr JM. Childhood cognitive ability and incident dementia: the 1932 Scottish Mental Survey cohort into their tenth decade. Epidemiology. 2017;28(3):361-364. doi: 10.1097/EDE.0000000000000626 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Rentz DM, Papp KV, Mayblyum DV, et al. Association of digital clock drawing with PET amyloid and tau pathology in normal older adults. Neurology. 2021;96(14):e1844-e1854. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000011697 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Wilson RS, Leurgans SE, Boyle PA, Bennett DA. Cognitive decline in prodromal Alzheimer disease and mild cognitive impairment. Arch Neurol. 2011;68(3):351-356. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2011.31 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Hughes ML, Agrigoroaei S, Jeon M, Bruzzese M, Lachman ME. Change in cognitive performance from midlife into old age: findings from the Midlife in the United States (MIDUS) study. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2018;24(8):805-820. doi: 10.1017/S1355617718000425 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Sutphen CL, Jasielec MS, Shah AR, et al. Longitudinal cerebrospinal fluid biomarker changes in preclinical Alzheimer disease during middle age. JAMA Neurol. 2015;72(9):1029-1042. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2015.1285 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Ngandu T, Lehtisalo J, Levälahti E, et al. Recruitment and baseline characteristics of participants in the Finnish Geriatric Intervention Study to Prevent Cognitive Impairment and Disability (FINGER): a randomized controlled lifestyle trial. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2014;11(9):9345-9360. doi: 10.3390/ijerph110909345 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Jobe JB, Smith DM, Ball K, et al. ACTIVE: a cognitive intervention trial to promote independence in older adults. Control Clin Trials. 2001;22(4):453-479. doi: 10.1016/S0197-2456(01)00139-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Rafii MS, Aisen PS. Alzheimer’s disease clinical trials: moving toward successful prevention. CNS Drugs. 2019;33(2):99-106. doi: 10.1007/s40263-018-0598-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Cornelis MC, Wang Y, Holland T, Agarwal P, Weintraub S, Morris MC. Age and cognitive decline in the UK Biobank. PLoS One. 2019;14(3):e0213948. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0213948 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Delis D, Kramer J, Kaplan E, Ober B. The California Verbal Learning Test: Research Edition, Adult Version. Psychological Corporation; 1987. [Google Scholar]
- 64.Rey A. L’Examen Clinique en Psychologie. Presses Universitaues de France; 1964. [Google Scholar]
- 65.Stasenko A, Jacobs DM, Salmon DP, Gollan TH. The Multilingual Naming Test (MINT) as a measure of picture naming ability in Alzheimer’s disease. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2019;25(8):821-833. doi: 10.1017/S1355617719000560 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Kaplan EF, Goodglass H, Weintraub S. The Boston Naming Test. 2nd ed. Lea & Febiger; 1983. [Google Scholar]
- 67.Kurt P, Yener G, Oguz M. Impaired digit span can predict further cognitive decline in older people with subjective memory complaint: a preliminary result. Aging Ment Health. 2011;15(3):364-369. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2010.536133 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Lindbergh CA, Walker N, La Joie R, et al. ; Hillblom Aging Network . Worth the wait: delayed recall after 1 week predicts cognitive and medial temporal lobe trajectories in older adults. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2021;27(4):382-388. doi: 10.1017/S1355617720001009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Brenowitz WD, Zimmerman SC, Filshtein TJ, et al. Using a genetic risk score to estimate the earliest age of Alzheimer’s disease-related physiologic change in body mass index. medRxiv. Preprint posted online December 4, 2019. doi: 10.1101/19013441 [DOI]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.