Abstract
Postnatal development of hippocampal function has been reported in many mammalian species, including humans. To obtain synaptic evidence, we analyzed developmental changes in plasticity after an inhibitory avoidance task in rats. Learning performance was low in infants (postnatal 2 weeks) but clearly improved from the juvenile period (3–4 weeks) to adulthood (8 weeks). One hour after the training, we prepared brain slices and sequentially recorded miniature excitatory postsynaptic currents (mEPSCs) and inhibitory postsynaptic currents (mIPSCs) from the same hippocampal CA1 neuron. Although the training failed to affect the amplitude of either mEPSCs or mIPSCs at 2 weeks, it increased mEPSC, but not mIPSC, amplitude at 3 weeks. At 4 weeks, the training had increased the amplitude of both mEPSCs and mIPSCs, whereas mIPSC, but not mEPSC, amplitude was increased at 8 weeks. Because early-life physiological functions can affect performance, we also evaluated sensory–motor functions together with emotional state and found adequate sensory/motor functions from infancy to adulthood. Moreover, by analyzing performance of rats in multiple hippocampal-dependent tasks, we found that the developmental changes in the performance are task dependent. Taken together, these findings delineate a critical period for learning and plastic changes at hippocampal CA1 synapses.
Subject terms: Neurophysiology, Cognitive neuroscience, Synaptic plasticity
Introduction
The hippocampus plays a central role in the formation of episodic memory1, processing spatio-temporal information2,3 related to a specific event4,5. Because postnatal experience enhances the development of the hippocampus, the function is immature in infants6–10.
Miles11 first published evidence that human adults could not recall specific events from early childhood, which is termed infantile amnesia7–10. In rats, Campbell and Campbell6 showed rapid forgetting of contextual memory on postnatal day 18. In turn, the retention was clearly improved if the task was performed on postnatal days 38, 54, and 100. Postnatal-day-18 rats consistently showed this deficit6,12, with improvement in their performance observed after postnatal day 2312,13. However, the learning behavior should be evaluated precisely, because insufficient sensory or motor performance could affect task performance.
Synaptic contacts in hippocampal CA1 neurons continuously increase in the first several weeks of life in rats14–16. Spine density dramatically increases from postnatal days 7 to 28, especially from days 7 to 21, expanding by threefold17. Moreover, the magnitude of long-term potentiation (LTP), which is a cellular substrate for learning and memory, dramatically increases from postnatal days 10 to 20, with the increase persisting up to day 6018. A recent brain-wide analysis of synapses across the lifespan of rats further demonstrated that the molecular/morphological diversity of excitatory synapses was dramatically increased within the first postnatal month, especially in the hippocampus19.
Insertion of synaptic membrane AMPA receptors is a major mechanism underlying LTP expression in CA1 pyramidal neurons20,21. Regarding the causal relationship, hippocampal-dependent contextual learning not only requires synaptic delivery of AMPA receptors, but also strengthens GABAA receptor-mediated inhibitory synapses onto neurons22,23. Moreover, this learning strengthens both excitatory and inhibitory synapses in different ways in individual CA1 neurons, thus producing a broad variety of synaptic input across cells in juvenile males24,25. Using the same experimental protocol, here we evaluated a critical change in the performance together with the training-induced plastic change that occurs from infancy to adulthood.
Results
Rats were subjected to the inhibitory avoidance (IA) task (Fig. 1A). They crossed from a light box to a dark box, where an electric foot shock (1.6 mA, 2 s) was delivered. Half an hour after the task, we measured the latency in the illuminated box as contextual learning performance. Figure 1B shows the latency in the training session and the retrieval test. Two-way ANOVA revealed a significant interaction (F3,33 = 4.73, P = 0.0075) and a main effect of developmental stage (F1,33 = 55.22, P < 0.0001) and training (F3,33 = 3.922, P = 0.0169). The training failed to increase the latency at 2 weeks (Fig. 1B).
To analyze training-induced synaptic plasticity, we recorded miniature excitatory postsynaptic currents (mEPSCs) or miniature inhibitory postsynaptic current (mIPSCs) in the presence of 0.5 µM tetrodotoxin in the dorsal hippocampus (Fig. 2A). By changing the membrane potential, we sequentially recorded mEPSCs (at − 60 mV) and mIPSCs (at 0 mV) from the same neuron, as reported previously23. We confirmed that the mEPSC and mIPSC events were clearly blocked by bath treatment with an AMPA receptor blocker (CNQX) or a GABAA receptor blocker (bicuculline). The postsynaptic currents are thought to correspond to the response elicited by a single vesicle of glutamate or GABA26. In contrast, the number of synapses affects the frequency of events.
The strength of AMPA receptor-mediated excitatory input vs. GABAA receptor-mediated inhibitory input was measured in each neuron and plotted two dimensionally (amplitude in Fig. 2B, upper panel; frequency in Fig. 2C, upper panel). Regarding the mEPSCs, IA training significantly increased their mean amplitude at 3 and 4 weeks (3 weeks: t102 = − 2.17; P = 0.0323; 4 weeks: t92 = − 2.57; P = 0.0120, unpaired t-test; Fig. 2D, left) and their frequency at 3 and 4 weeks (3 weeks: t102 = − 2.05; P = 0.0435; 4 weeks: t92 = − 2.53; P = 0.0140, unpaired t-test; Fig. 2E, left). Conversely, the mEPSC frequency was significantly decreased at 2 weeks (t82 = 2.77; P = 0.0070, unpaired t-test; Fig. 2E, left). Regarding the mIPSCs, IA training significantly increased their mean amplitude at 4 and 8 weeks (4 weeks: t92 = − 3.06; P = 0.0029; 8 weeks: t103 = − 3.56; P = 0.0006, unpaired t-test; Fig. 2D, right) and their frequency at 3 and 4 weeks (3 weeks: t110 = − 2.56; P = 0.0112; 4 weeks: t92 = − 2.82; P = 0.0059, unpaired t-test; Fig. 2E, right). Conversely, mIPSC frequency was significantly decreased at 2 weeks (t82 = 3.01; P = 0.0036, unpaired t-test; Fig. 2E, right). We also prepared the cumulative distribution in Supplementary Fig. 1.
Here we examined training-induced synaptic plasticity at multiple postnatal ages. Further yoked controls exposed to a foot shock (unpaired) or the apparatus alone (walk through) are necessary to clarify whether the plasticity is induced by learning23.
To calculate the balance between excitatory/inhibitory (E/I) input, we divided the mean mEPSC amplitude by the mean mIPSC amplitude in individual neurons. Although the training did not change the E/I balance at 2 (P = 0.51), 3 (P = 0.32), and 4 (P = 0.30) weeks, it clearly decreased the balance at 8 weeks exclusively (P = 0.0016; Fig. 2F).
Based on the information theory of Shannon, we calculated the appearance probability of the mean amplitudes of mEPSCs and mIPSCs. First, we identified the distribution of the appearance probability in untrained control animals (Fig. 3A, left), followed by the analysis of the cell‐specific appearance probability of all recorded neurons individually (Fig. 3A, right). Each probability of a single neuron was calculated as the self‐entropy and plotted two‐dimensionally; e.g., a point with a high appearance probability (around the mean level of mEPSC and mIPSC amplitudes) indicated a low self‐entropy, whereas a point with a very rare probability (deviated from mEPSC and mIPSC amplitudes) indicated a high self‐entropy.
We used a two‐dimensional kernel analysis to visualize synaptic density (Fig. 3A, lower panels). IA training clearly diversified the amount of information per neuron, which was sustained. Regarding the mEPSC and mIPSC amplitude, the training significantly increased the self-entropy at 4 and 8 weeks, but not at 2 and 3 weeks (mEPSC, 2 weeks: t82 = − 0.53; P = 0.5915; 3 weeks: t102 = − 1.82; P = 0.0745; 4 weeks: t92 = − 2.43; P = 0.0186; 8 weeks: t103 = − 2.33; P = 0.0220, Fig. 3C, left; mIPSC, 2 weeks: t82 = − 0.39; P = 0.6989; 3 weeks: t102 = − 1.49; P = 0.1384; 4 weeks: t92 = − 3.49; P = 0.0008; 8 weeks: t103 = − 3.77; P = 0.0003, Fig. 3C, right).
For the mEPSC and mIPSC frequency, we identified the distribution of appearance probability in untrained controls (Fig. 3B, left), followed by the analysis of the appearance probability of all recorded neurons individually. We found cell‐specific self‐entropy in all recorded neurons, with self‐entropy varying from cell to cell (Fig. 3B, upper panels).
A two‐dimensional kernel analysis allowed the visualization of synaptic density (Fig. 3B, lower panels). IA training diversified the amount of information per neuron, which was sustained. In the mEPSC frequency, IA training significantly increased the self-entropy at 3 and 4 weeks (3 weeks: t102 = − 2.60; P = 0.0116; 4 weeks: t92 = − 2.69; P = 0.0094, Fig. 3D, left). In the mIPSC frequency, the training significantly increased the self-entropy at 4 and 8 weeks (4 weeks: t92 = − 3.49; P = 0.0008; 8 weeks: t103 = − 3.77; P = 0.0003, Fig. 3D, right).
To analyze presynaptic plasticity, we examined the paired-pulse ratio after the training (Fig. 4A). At the excitatory synapses in the apical dendrite, IA training significantly increased the paired-pulse ratio at 3 and 4 weeks, whereas the ratio was decreased at 2 weeks (2 weeks: t62 = 2.36; P = 0.0224; 3 weeks: t62 = − 2.36; P = 0.0214; 4 weeks: t75 = − 2.79; P = 0.0066, Fig. 4B). At the synapses in the basal dendrite, the training decreased the paired-pulse ratio at 2 weeks (t51 = 4.61; P < 0.0001, Fig. 4C). These results suggest that the training increased presynaptic glutamate release at 2 weeks, but decreased it at 3 and 4 weeks.
At the inhibitory synapses in the apical dendrite, the training significantly decreased the paired-pulse ratio at 4 weeks (t65 = 2.03; P = 0.0468, unpaired t-test; Fig. 4B). At the synapses in the basal dendrite, the training significantly increased the ratio at 4 and 8 weeks (4 weeks: t64 = − 2.21; P = 0.0335; 8 weeks: t54 = − 2.09; P = 0.0410, unpaired t-test; Fig. 4C).
Early-life physiological functions may affect latency in the IA task. To rule out this possibility, we evaluated several physiological functions using the visual placing response, open field, light–dark box, flinch/jump, incline, and rotor rod tests. In the visual placing test, one-way ANOVA showed no significant main effect of development (F3,27 = 0.51, P = 0.68), indicating the presence of basic visual function at 2 weeks.
To evaluate anxiety and innate curiosity about a novel environment, we used the open field and the light–dark box test (Fig. 5A–C). One-way ANOVA of the open field test results showed a significant main effect associated with the center-circle latency (F3,28 = 3.81, P = 0.0224), and a post-hoc analysis showed that the latency in the center circle of the open field was shorter at 3, 4 and 8 weeks than it was at 2 weeks (Fig. 5A). Conversely, regarding the traveled distance test, one-way ANOVA showed a significant main effect of traveled distance (F3,27 = 19.397, P < 0.0001), and a post-hoc analysis showed that the traveled distance in the open field was longer at 3, 4 and 8 weeks than it was at 2 weeks (vs. 3 weeks: P < 0.0001; vs. 4 weeks: P < 0.0001; vs. 8 weeks: P = 0.0002; Fig. 5B). In the light–dark box test, one-way ANOVA showed a significant main effect of development (F3,29 = 5.97, P = 0.0031), and the time in a novel environment was longer at 8 weeks than it was at 2 and 3 weeks (Fig. 5C).
Pain sensitivity was evaluated using the flinch/jump test. One-way ANOVA showed no significant main effect of development (flinch: F3,30 = 1.47, P = 0.24; Fig. 5D; and jump: F3,30 = 0.94, P = 0.4351; Fig. 5E), showing sufficient pain sensitivity at 2 weeks. The current threshold of vocalization at 3 weeks was more sensitive to foot shock than it was at 2 weeks (F3,30 = 4.73, P = 0.0089; 2 vs. 3 weeks: P = 0.0037 in post-hoc analysis; Fig. 5F).
To evaluate social behavior, we used the social interaction test (Fig. 5G). One-way ANOVA showed a significant main effect of development (F3,29 = 8.41, P = 0.0005), and the sniffing time of the social target was shorter at 3 and 8 weeks than it was at 2 weeks. By comparing the touching time of a social target with an empty cage, we evaluated social preference. The touching time of social target was longer at 4 and 8 weeks, but not at 2 and 3 weeks (4 weeks: P = 0.0006; 8 weeks: P < 0.0001, paired t-test; Fig. 5H).
We further evaluated motor functions and skill learning performance. In the hanging wire test, one-way ANOVA showed a significant main effect (F3,30 = 13.140, P < 0.0001), and a post-hoc analysis showed that the latency to fall was shorter at 3, 4 and 8 weeks than it was at 2 weeks (3 weeks: P = 0.0002; 4 weeks: P < 0.0001; 8 weeks: P = 0.0001). In the incline test, one-way ANOVA showed a significant main effect (F3,28 = 18.137, P < 0.0001), and a post-hoc analysis showed that the angle to fall was smaller at 8 weeks than it was at 2 weeks (Fig. 5I). In the rotor rod test (Fig. 5J), we measured the latency to falling from the rotating rod, with longer latency considered to indicate better motor performance. For graphic expression, the mean latency at 1st trial was normalized to find the developmental changes. Two-way ANOVA revealed a significant interaction (F57,475 = 2.39, P = 0.0015) and a main effect of developmental stage (F3,475 = 36.27, P < 0.0001) and trial (F19,475 = 7.76, P < 0.0001). Compared with the overall latency at 2 weeks, post-hoc analysis showed that the latency was longer at 3, 4 and 8 weeks (Fig. 5J). By comparing the latency at first trial, we observed significant improvement of the performance at final trials on day 1 (10th trial) and day 2 (20th trial) at 4 and 8 weeks but not 2 and 3 weeks.
To analyze further the retention of contextual memory, we compared the performance in the retrieval test at 24 h after the training session in contextual fear conditioning. In contextual fear conditioning, two-way ANOVA revealed a significant interaction (F3,25 = 46.48, P < 0.0001) and a main effect of learning (F1,25 = 264.28, P < 0.0001) and developmental stage (F3,25 = 22.41, P < 0.0001; Fig. 6A). A significant extension of freezing time was observed after 3 weeks of age (3 weeks: P < 0.0001; 4 weeks: P < 0.0001; 8 weeks: P = 0.0001 in post-hoc analysis), which provided further evidence of the existence of a critical period.
To evaluate cognitive functions, we used the object recognition, novel object placement, and social recognition tasks. Prior to the tasks, all groups of rats were checked equal left/right exploration in the sample phase (see Supplementary Tables 1 and 2). In the object recognition task, the training increased the touch time of a novel object at 3, 4 and 8 weeks, but not at 2 weeks (3 weeks: t6 = − 2.48; P = 0.0476; 4 weeks: t7 = 3.20; P = 0.0150, 8 weeks: t7 = 3.48; P = 0.0102, paired t-test; Fig. 6B). In the novel object placement task, the training significantly increased the touch time of the exchanged objects only at 8 weeks (t7 = 4.35; P = 0.0033, paired t-test; Fig. 6C). In the social recognition task, the training increased the touch time of the novel rat at 3, 4 and 8 weeks, but not at 2 weeks (3 weeks: t6 = 3.44; P = 0.0138; 4 weeks: t9 = 2.27; P = 0.0491, 8 weeks: t15 = 2.40; P = 0.0296, paired t-test; Fig. 6D). Thus, neither the novelty nor the changed location of the objects was identified at 2 weeks.
Spatial working memory was evaluated using the Y-maze. One-way ANOVA showed a significant main effect of development (F3,28 = 4.18, P = 0.0158), and the alternation rate was higher at 8 weeks than it was at 2 weeks (Fig. 6E). The total number of arm entries did not change during development (F3,28 = 1.51, P = 0.237; Fig. 6F).
Discussion
Developmental changes in memory, sensory, and motor functions
Here, we examined the developmental changes in training-induced synaptic plasticity. At 2 weeks postnatally (postnatal day 16), but not at 3 and 8 weeks (days 22–56), rats could not maintain a memory of IA learning, suggesting an undeveloped contextual memory function in infant rats (Fig. 1 and Ref.10). Although rats at 2 weeks showed basic sensory/motor functions, they did not avoid aversive pain by becoming immobilized in the light box. Vision arises around postnatal days 13 and 1427, and sufficient pain sensitivity developed before postnatal day 16 in rats (Fig. 5D–F). Regarding motor functions, the incline test showed lower grip performance at 8 weeks (Fig. 5I), but rats at 4 and 8 weeks can learn motor skill in the rotor rod test (Fig. 5J). Moreover, the move into the dark box during IA training session and the immobilization in the dark box in the light–dark box test suggests sufficient motor function for choosing to stay or move (Figs. 1B, 5C). Regarding the emotional state, however, the open field test showed the longest center-circle time at 2 weeks (Fig. 5A). At 8 weeks, rats seemed to be more exploratory (Fig. 5B,C), but also more cautious (Fig. 5A), than they were at 2 weeks. Although the unanxious emotional state observed at 2 weeks could partly have reduced IA latency, the memory deficit may not be attributed to the development of sensory/motor functions.
Thus, since the physiological functions of animals change significantly during development, postnatal age of the animal should be carefully considered when using various rodent models.
Training-induced plasticity at excitatory synapses
Contextual learning requires plasticity at CA1 synapses at 4 weeks22–25; however, a developmental change in learning-dependent synaptic plasticity has not been reported. IA training increased the mEPSC amplitude at 3 and 4 weeks, but not at 2 and 8 weeks (Fig. 2D), indicating the development of training-induced synaptic plasticity at excitatory synapses. Regarding the other evidence of development, naive AMPA receptor densities increased dramatically (184%) during postnatal days 0–10 and were stable during postnatal days 20–3028. Moreover, the magnitude of the late-phase LTP at postnatal days 12–15 was almost half that at days 19–3529, while early-phase LTP magnitude increased at postnatal days 15–2030,31.
In rats at 4 weeks, we previously showed a training-induced increase in the postsynaptic number of AMPA receptor channels without a change in the cation current per single channel in the CA125. Moreover, regarding a causal relationship, we previously demonstrated that bilateral gene expression of GluA1-containing AMPA receptor delivery blockers in the CA1 neurons impairs IA learning22, suggesting that newly delivered GluA1-containing AMPA receptors contribute to the formation of contextual memory.
The paired-pulse analysis performed here further revealed presynaptic plasticity at excitatory synapses. In rats at 3 and 4 postnatal weeks, the paired-pulse ratio increased after IA training, indicating a decrease in presynaptic glutamate release (Fig. 4B). Because the mEPSC frequency is used as an indicator of evoked release32–34 or the number of functional synapses35,36, postsynaptic plasticity may have contributed to the increase in mEPSC frequency observed at 3 and 4 weeks. In our study of the temporal dynamics, presynaptic glutamate release increased at 5 min but decreased at 60 min24,37. The late decrease might have been associated with feedback via presynaptic NMDA receptors38.
Unexpectedly, training failed to strengthen the excitatory synapses at 8 weeks (Fig. 2D,E). Since IA training is known to induce both synaptic depression and potentiation in adult rats39, it is possible that the synapse-dependent occurrence of depression or potentiation counteracted the effect at 8 weeks. The number of synapses40 and AMPA receptors28 reach a maximum value at 4 weeks and decline slightly at 8 weeks, suggesting that synaptic elimination plays a role in the refinement of network connections40. In adult animals, therefore, it is possible that the decrease in E/I balance at 8 weeks of age increased contrast in the firing activity of engram cells, and genetic manipulation to visualize CA1 engram cells may be necessary to detect the spine-specific plasticity after training41,42.
Even without engram cell tagging, a 5-min exposure to an emotional experience promotes mean AMPA receptor-mediated current in CA1 neurons of adult mice43. We also found a 10-min exposure to an emotional experience, such as restraint stress or contact with a reproductive female, clearly strengthened AMPA receptor-mediated excitatory currents in CA1 pyramidal neurons of adult rats5. The result suggesting extent of synaptic plasticity and the created diversity depend on the recent experience in adult animals. It is possible, therefore, that the brief foot shock administered in the IA task may not be sufficient to arouse emotion increasing the mean mEPSC amplitude/frequency at 8 weeks (Fig. 2D,E).
Training-induced plasticity at inhibitory synapses
IA training also increased the mean mIPSC amplitudes at 4–8 weeks, suggesting a postsynaptic strengthening at GABAA receptor-mediated synapses23,24. Because the task also increased mIPSC frequency at 3 and 4 weeks, it may activate silent GABAA synapses, to increase the number of synapses. A genetic deficiency in the GABAA receptor β3 subunit and the prevention of GABAA receptor-mediated plasticity in the CA1 both impair contextual learning23,44, suggesting a causal relationship between GABAA receptor delivery and learning. Optogenetic manipulation of CA1 neurons has further revealed a timing-specific causal relationship: the inactivation of dendrite-targeting CA1 interneurons during aversive stimuli is sufficient to prevent fear learning45. Thus, IA training may trigger GABAA receptor-mediated plasticity to encode contextual memory from the juvenile to adult periods.
We recently found rapid phosphorylation of the Ser408–409 GABAA receptor β3 subunit immediately after training24. In cultured neurons, Ser408–409 phosphorylation increases both the amplitude and frequency of mIPSCs by blocking clathrin-dependent endocytosis of the synaptic receptors46. Because local interference in Ser408–409 phosphorylation of the bilateral CA1 clearly blocked learning- and GABAA receptor-mediated plasticity at CA1 synapses24, IA training may lead to rapid phosphorylation of Ser408–409 of the GABAA β3 subunit, to encode the episodic experience.
Decrease in excitatory/inhibitory balance in adulthood
Although the training failed to increase the mEPSC amplitude at 8 weeks of age (Fig. 2), the animals successfully maintained the memory of the task (Fig. 1). As for spatial learning, the mIPSC frequency increased with the water maze training47, while the mIPSC amplitude increased with the novel object recognition task5. This suggests that the pattern of synaptic plasticity differs depending on the type of hippocampal learning.
Because mIPSC amplitude consistently increased at 4–8 weeks of age, the training decreased the excitatory/inhibitory (E/I) balance only at 8 weeks. A decrease in E/I balance is consistently observed in other regions in adult rodents (somatosensory cortex48; amygdala49. In the prefrontal cortex, the mature GABAergic system of adult animals enables an enhanced inhibitory control, whereas juveniles show a higher E/I balance because of an immature GABAergic system50,51. Moreover, optogenetic elevation, but not reduction, of the cellular E/I balance within the mouse medial prefrontal cortex impairs cellular information processing52. Although the role of the cellular E/I balance remains unknown in CA1 pyramidal neurons, the decrease of the cellular E/I balance by the mature GABAergic system may play an important role in the creation of synaptic diversity for memory processing.
Shunting inhibition occurs when the reversal potential of the synapse is approximately equal to the resting membrane potential53. This occurs when the inhibitory current is carried by Cl- ions or by a particular combination of K+ and Na+ or Ca2+ ions. This type of inhibitory input is silent: it does not change the membrane potential directly, but it can reduce the depolarization54 and the mEPSC frequency55,56. Although the contribution of shunting inhibition is still unclear, strengthened GABAA receptor-mediated mIPSCs may affect both mEPSC frequency and the E/I balance.
Critical period and infant-specific synaptic depression
Long-term depression (LTD) has been investigated in neonatal-to-juvenile CA1 neurons for decades57–60. LTD induction peaks during days 12–20, and is less effective in mature animals61. In addition, at the inhibitory synapses, GABAergic synapses on CA1 pyramidal neurons induce LTD over days 14–21, which involves the GABAB receptor-dependent suppression of adenylyl cyclase62. Because the training decreased the frequencies of both mEPSCs and mIPSCs at 2 weeks, but increased them at 3 and 4 weeks (Fig. 2E), the training may decrease the number of functional synapses at 2 weeks, but increase them at 3 and 4 weeks.
The training decreased the mEPSC and mIPSC frequencies at 2 weeks exclusively (Fig. 2E), but increased presynaptic glutamate release without changes in GABA release. Although the underlying mechanism is unclear at inhibitory synapses, activation of AMPA receptors by presynaptic electric stimulation is known to induce silencing in infants, but not juvenile rats, decreasing mEPSC frequency, but not amplitude63,64. Since the decrease in mEPSC frequency reflects synaptic silencing at the excitatory synapses65, it may provide the first evidence of training-induced synaptic silencing in infants.
As rats at 8 weeks exhibited difficulties in retrieving a memory that was trained at 2 weeks6, critical changes in the plasticity response might play a key role in infantile amnesia. The rapid forgetting of contextual memory in infants may be closely associated with the GABAergic system in early development66,67. Regarding a causal relationship, bilateral microinjection of a GABAA receptor inverse agonist (FG-7142) into the hippocampus recovered the rapid forgetting of contextual memory on postnatal day 1866, whereas the microinjection of a GABAA receptor agonist (midazolam) impaired the retrieval of forgotten memory67. These results suggest that tonic GABAA receptor-mediated inhibition in infants may cause the rapid forgetting of contextual memory.
The development of performance is task-dependent
By analyzing the performance of other hippocampal-dependent tasks (Fig. 6), we identified the task-dependent period of amnesia. Rats learned the IA, fear conditioning, and object recognition tasks after 3 weeks of age; in contrast, they learned the novel object placement task after 8 weeks of age. In the novel object placement task using two objects, rats successfully performed the associative spatial recognition task at postnatal day 30, but not at day 2468. Because rats can perform spatial tasks after postnatal days 26–2769, the association of place and spatial memory between objects seemed to require a longer developmental period than did the object memory alone.
The number of place cells increases during development3. Less than half of CA1 pyramidal neurons have formed in the place field (place-specific firings) at 2 weeks of age in rats, whereas the population has increased by up to 90% in adult animals. Because the targeted disruption of the place field impairs memory-guided spatial behavior70, this increase may be closely associated with the development of spontaneous alternation in the Y-maze71 (Fig. 6E,F). Moreover, approximately 75% of dorsal CA1 neurons not only process their own location, but also express the location of the other male rat in the same cage72, suggesting the existence of a large population of junction-place cells when two adult male rats are housed in the same cage. Although the postnatal change in the population is completely unknown, the development of social behaviors (Figs. 5G,H, 6D) suggest a possible critical period to functionalize the junction-place cells.
Materials and methods
Animals
Male Sprague Dawley rats were used in this study (Table 1). After weaning, same-sex groups of 2–3 rats were housed in opaque plastic cages lined with wood chips (length 25 cm; width 40 cm; height 25 cm) at a constant temperature of 23 °C ± 1 °C under a constant cycle of light and dark (lights on: 8:00 a.m. to 8:00 p.m.). We counted the parturition day as postnatal day 1, and litters were reduced to 10 pups per cage on day 2. The pups were weaned on postnatal day 20 or 21 and housed in group cages with free access to food and water. The post-weaning rats were individually housed at least 24 h prior to the experiments, to avoid any episodic experience. Food (MF, Oriental Yeast Co. Ltd, Tokyo Japan) and tap water were available ad libitum in all experimental periods. All animal housing and surgical procedures followed the guidelines of the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of Yamaguchi University. All experimental procedures were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of Yamaguchi University (Approval No. 04-S02). the These guidelines comply with the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals published by the National Institutes of Health (NIH Publication No. 85-23, revised 1996). The study reported in accordance with ARRIVE guidelines.
Table 1.
Postnatal weeks | Postnatal days | Body weight (g) | Number of rats |
---|---|---|---|
2 | 16.0 ± 0.1 | 34.8 ± 0.6 | 79 |
3 | 21.5 ± 0.1 | 53.0 ± 0.9 | 55 |
4 | 28.5 ± 0.1 | 102.5 ± 2.1 | 67 |
8 | 56.2 ± 0.2 | 306.0 ± 5.5 | 58 |
Data are the means ± SEM.
The IA training apparatus (length 33 cm; width 58 cm; height 33 cm) was a two-chambered box consisting of a lit safe side and a dark shock side separated by a trap door (Fig. 1A22–24. For training, rats were placed in the lit side of the box, facing the corner opposite the door. After the trap door was opened, the rats could enter the dark box at will. The latency before entering the novel dark box was measured as a behavioral parameter (latency before IA learning, Fig. 1B). Soon after the animals entered the dark side, we closed the door and applied a scrambled electrical foot shock (1.6 mA, 2 s) via electrified steel rods placed in the floor of the box. The rats were kept in the dark compartment for 10 s before being returned to their home cage. Untrained control rats were not moved from their home cages.
Thirty minutes after the procedure described above, the rats were placed in the lit side of the box. The latency before entering the dark box was measured as an indicator of learning performance (latency after IA learning, Fig. 1B).
Electrophysiological recording of slice-patch clamping
We have previously reported the detailed technical protocol of the slice-patch clamp technique used for analyzing training-induced synaptic plasticity, with a short demonstration movie73. Briefly, 1 h after the delivery of the paired foot shock, rats were anesthetized with pentobarbital and acute brain slices were prepared22,23,25. We used naive rats as the untrained group, all of which were injected with the same dose of anesthesia in their home cage. For the whole-cell recordings74, the brains were quickly perfused with ice-cold dissection buffer (25.0 mM NaHCO3, 1.25 mM NaH2PO4, 2.5 mM KCl, 0.5 mM CaCl2, 7.0 mM MgCl2, 25.0 mM glucose, 90 mM choline chloride, 11.6 mM ascorbic acid, and 3.1 mM pyruvic acid) and gassed with 5% CO2/95% O2. Coronal sections (target CA1 area: AP, − 3.8 mm, DV, 2.5 mm, LM, ± 2.0 mm75 were prepared (350 μm, Leica vibratome, VT-1200) in dissection buffer and transferred to physiological solution (22–25 °C; 114.6 mM NaCl, 2.5 mM KCl, 26 mM NaHCO3, 1 mM NaH2PO4, 10 mM glucose, 4 mM MgCl2, and 4 mM CaCl2, pH 7.4) gassed with 5% CO2/95% O2. The recording chamber was perfused with physiological solution at 22–25 °C.
Although we followed the physiological solution and temperature for many plasticity studies22,23,76,77, behaving animals may show different excitatory/inhibitory currents in in vivo brain. In fact, the mEPSC responses are known to depend on Mg2+ and Ca2+ concentrations in the extracellular fluid78, as well as the temperature79.
Patch recording pipettes (4–7 MΩ) were filled with intracellular solution (127.5 mM cesium methanesulfonate, 7.5 mM CsCl, 10 mM HEPES, 2.5 mM MgCl2, 4 mM Na2ATP, 0.4 mM Na3GTP, 10 mM sodium phosphocreatine, and 0.6 mM EGTA at pH 7.25). Whole-cell recordings were obtained from CA1 pyramidal neurons from the rat hippocampus using an Axopatch 700 A amplifier (Axon Instruments). The whole-cell patch-clamp data were collected using a Clampex 10.4 instrument, and the data were analyzed using the Clampfit 10.4 software (Axon Instruments).
Miniature postsynaptic current recordings
We have previously reported the detailed technical protocol of the miniature postsynaptic current recording23–25. mEPSCs are thought to correspond to the responses elicited by the presynaptic release of a single vesicle of glutamate. In contrast, mIPSCs are thought to correspond to GABA. Increased mEPSC and mIPSC amplitudes reflect the strengthening of postsynaptic transmission, whereas increased event frequency reflects an increased number of functional synapses or presynaptic release probability.
For the miniature recordings, we added a Na+ channel blocker (0.5 μM tetrodotoxin) to the physiological solution. The mEPSCs (− 60 mV holding potential) and mIPSCs (0 mV holding potential) were recorded sequentially for 5 min in the same CA1 neuron. The miniature events were detected using the Clampfit 10.4 software (Axon Instruments), and the events above 10 pA were used in the analysis. We recorded for at least 5 min, to determine the event frequency of mEPSCs or mIPSCs. The amplitudes of the events were averaged to obtain the mean amplitude. Bath application of an AMPA receptor blocker (CNQX, 10 μM) or GABAA receptor blocker (bicuculline methiodide, 10 μM) consistently blocked the mEPSC or mIPSC events, respectively.
Paired-pulse stimulation
We have previously reported the detailed technical protocol of the paired-pulse stimulation24. To analyze presynaptic plasticity at excitatory synapses, we added 0.1 mM picrotoxin and 4 μM 2-chloroadenosine to the physiological solution and performed paired-pulse stimulation at − 60 mV. To analyze presynaptic plasticity at inhibitory synapses, we added 10 μM CNQX to the perfusate and performed paired-pulse stimulation at 0 mV. To evaluate the paired-pulse ratio from the EPSC or IPSC average, 50–100 sweeps were recorded with paired stimuli at 100-ms intervals. We placed the stimulation electrode in either the stratum oriens (basal) or stratum radiatum (apical), to record evoked somatic currents. The ratio of the second amplitude to the first amplitude was calculated as the paired-pulse ratio23,80.
Self-entropy analysis
Based on the Shannon entropy, we quantified the synaptic diversity by measuring the population differences in mEPSC and mIPSC amplitude or frequency compared with untrained rats24,25. We used a standard spreadsheet software (Excel 2010, Microsoft Co., Redmond, WA, USA) to calculate the self-entropy per neuron. First, we obtained four miniature parameters (i.e., mean mEPSC amplitude, mean mIPSC amplitude, mean mEPSC frequency, and mean mIPSC frequency) in individual CA1 pyramidal neurons. Subsequently, we determined the distribution of the appearance probability of four miniature parameters separately using one-dimensional kernel density analysis. The geometric/topographic features of the appearance probability were calculated using a kernel density analysis. Let X1, X2,…, Xn denote a sample of size n from real observations. The kernel density estimates of P at the point x is given by the following equation:
where K is a smooth function called the Gaussian kernel function and h > 0 is the smoothing bandwidth that controls the amount of smoothing. We chose Silverman’s reference bandwidth or Silverman’s rule of thumb81,82, which is given by the following equation:
where A = min (standard deviation, interquartile range/1.34). By normalizing the integral value in untrained controls, we identified the distribution of the appearance probability at any point. Subsequently, we calculated the appearance probability at selected points. All data points for probability in untrained and trained rats were converted to self-entropy (bits) using the Shannon entropy concept, as defined in the Information Theory83.
To perform calculations using the spreadsheet software, the data for four miniature parameters were summarized in four different sheets, and we obtained the bandwidth (h) of individual parameters in the untrained group using the following formula: [= 0.9 STDEV (neuron 1, neuron 2,… neuron N)/COUNT (neuron 1, neuron 2,… neuron N)(1/5)]. Then, using the data from the untrained group, we calculated the distribution of the appearance probability as follows:
The probability distribution of the first datum for a parameter (neuron 1) was calculated using the formula [= EXP (− (((data of neuron 1 − any point)/h)2/2))/SQRT (2 × PI())].
Moreover, the probability distribution of the second datum for the parameter (neuron 2) was calculated using the formula [= EXP (− (((data of neuron 2 − any point)/h)2/2))/SQRT(2 × PI())].
Similarly, the probability distribution of the N datum for the parameter (neuron N) was calculated using the formula [= EXP (− (((data of neuron N − any point)/h)2/2))/SQRT(2 × PI())].
All probability distributions from neurons 1 to N were summed, and the integral value was normalized to 1.
Based on the probability distribution, we calculated the individual appearance probability of all recorded neurons. Subsequently, the appearance probability of the neuron was converted to the self-entropy using Shannon’s formula (= − LOG [appearance probability of the neuron, 2]) (Fig. 3A,B). For graphic expression, the distribution was visualized two-dimensionally in the R software environment (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria) (Figs. 2B,C and 3A,B).
Behavioral test battery
Behavioral tests were performed in the following sequence: open field, object recognition, social preference, social recognition task, social interaction, novel object placement task, light–dark box test, visual placing response test, hanging wire test, Y-maze spontaneous alternation, contextual fear conditioning, and flinch and jump. We used different sampling rats in social preference, social recognition task and social interaction, and different object in object recognition and novel object placement task. The between tests interval was at least 60 min. Rats were habituated to the testing room 30 min prior to testing and the apparatus was cleaned with 70% ethanol between each trial. An additional group of animals was used in the incline and the rotor rod tests.
Visual placing response test
To evaluate the sense of sight, we administered the visual placing response test84,85. In this test, the rat was suspended by its tail and then lowered toward a black foam plate placed on the front side of its head, without any contact to the vibrissae. Normally, when the head of a rat is lowered to near the edge of the plastic plate, the animal turns its head and trunk and extends its forelimbs to place them on the plate. The success ratio was calibrated to whether the rat successfully placed its forelimbs on the plate86.
Open field test
To evaluate emotional state and spontaneous locomotor activity, we used the open field test (Fig. 5A,B). The center area (diameter 36 cm) and the peripheral area (diameter 60 cm) of the gray circle floor were lined. After the audio-visual recording, we measured the time spent in the center area and the distance traveled over 5 min9.
Light–dark box test
To evaluate anxiety and the exploration of a novel environment, we administered the light–dark box test87–89. The light–dark box (length 48 cm; width 20 cm; height 23 cm) was constructed of light and dark compartments separated by a sliding door (width 7 cm; height 8 cm). The rats were placed in the center of the dark box and allowed to explore for 5 min. Then, the sliding door was opened, and they explored both boxes for 5 min (Fig. 5C).
Flinch and jump test
To evaluate pain sensibility, we performed the flinch and jump test90. Rats were placed individually in the fear-conditioning chamber (Fig. 5D–F). The conditioning chamber (length 25 cm; width 31 cm; height 42 cm) was constructed of clear Plexiglass on the top, front, and back. The floor had 18 stainless steel bars (4 mm in diameter; 15-mm spacing), to deliver the scrambled shocks produced by a stimulator (LE100-26 Shocker, Panlab, Cornellà, Spain). After a 3-min period of habituation to the test box, shock titrations continued to increase in a stepwise manner (0.05 mA increments; range 0.05–0.6 mA). In this way, the “flinch” and “jump” thresholds (in mA) were defined for each rat. The interval between shocks was 2 min, and each animal was tested only once at each intensity. The behavior of each rat was recorded through a front digital video recording camera. The “flinch” threshold was defined as the lowest shock intensity that elicited a detectable response. The “jump” threshold was defined as the lowest shock intensity that elicited the simultaneous removal of at least three paws (including both hind paws) from the grid91. The “vocalization” threshold was defined as the lowest shock intensity that led to a detectable audible vocalization in response to shock stimuli.
Social interaction test
To evaluate the social interaction with an unfamiliar partner (stranger; same strain, sex, and age), we used the social interaction test92. Prior to the task, we habituated the rats to an empty open field arena (diameter 45 cm; height 45 cm) for 5 min. Five minutes later, the rats were placed in the center of the arena again. One minute later, we placed an unfamiliar partner in the center of the arena, to assess social interaction for 5 min (Fig. 5G). Sniffing behavior was defined as the animal directing its nose toward or touching the partner.
Social preference test
To evaluate social preference, we used a U-field two-choice box93–95. The U-field box consisted of two symmetrical rectangular fields that were defined by partitioning an open field (length 45 cm; width 45 cm; height 45 cm) with a wall (length 20 cm; height 45 cm). Prior to the task, we habituated the rats to the U-field two-choice box containing two circular wire empty cages (8 cm in diameter) for 5 min. Five minutes later, the rat was placed in the center of the box again and allowed to freely explore a wire cage containing a social target (stranger; same strain, sex, and age) or an empty wire cage for 5 min (Fig. 5H).
To assess social approach, we measured the time spent touching the social target or empty wire cage in a test phase. Touching behavior was defined as the animal directing its nose toward or its forelimbs touching the wire cage. The apparatus was cleaned with 70% alcohol and air-dried prior to each trial.
Hanging wire test
To evaluate basic motor function, we administered the hanging wire test96. Rats were placed on a meshed wire, and the wire was turned upside down. We measured the latency until the rats fell to the cage floor lined with soft wood chips.
Incline test
We tested the ability of the rats to balance on an angled plywood, which is easy for nails to get caught on, containing a moveable plate with angle adjustment from 0° to 90° (Fig. 5I). The rat was placed perpendicularly and the inclination between the inclined plane and the horizontal plane was increased gradually, until the rat could no longer remain on the table for 5 s. The angle was recorded as the maximum value97.
Rotor rod task
To evaluate change in motor skills, we conducted the rotor rod test (ENV577; Med Associates Inc., St. Albans, VT, USA). The rat was trained for two consecutive days and allowed 10 attempts with a 30 s inter-trial interval for each test (Fig. 5J). The rotor rod was set to increase from 4 to 40 rpm over 5 min, and the duration of rod-riding was recorded. We recorded the average latency to falling from the rotating rod, and considered longer latency as indicative of better motor performance74.
Contextual fear conditioning
To evaluate the longer retention of contextual memory, we administered contextual fear conditioning using the conditioning chamber described above (Fig. 6A). Under audio-visual recording (IXY3, Canon Inc, Tokyo, Japan), rats were allowed to explore for 3 min. Then, as the aversive unconditioned stimulus, we delivered foot shocks three times (0.8 mA, 2 s duration). Subsequently, the rats were allowed to recover for 30 s in the conditioning chamber and returned to their home cage. Twenty-four hours later, the rats were again placed in the conditioning chamber, and spontaneous behaviors were monitored for a 5-min period. To assess conditioning, we measured the time spent freezing per every 30 s of the testing period. The time spent freezing in the chamber was considered the measure of contextual learning. Freezing behavior was defined as cessation of all but respiratory movements9.
Object recognition task
In the habituation phase, rats were placed in the center of an empty open field box (length 45 cm; width 45 cm; height 45 cm) and allowed to explore the box for 5 min. In the sample phase, we placed two identical objects in the box (Fig. 6B). We placed the rats in the center of the open field box again and allowed them to explore for 5 min. Five minutes after the sample phase, we exchanged one of the familiar objects with a new object. In the testing phase, we placed the rats in the center of the open field box and allowed them to explore for 5 min. The apparatus was cleaned with 70% alcohol and air-dried prior to the commencement of each trial for each rat. To assess novel object memory, we measured the time spent touching a novel/familiar object during the test phase. Touching behavior was defined as the animal directing its nose toward or its forelimbs touching the object and the minimum touching time for object was set as a criterion of at least one second. Any other touching behavior, such as resting against the object, was not considered as touching98.
Novel object placement task
Prior to the task, the rats were habituated to the empty open field box for 5 min. In the sample phase, we placed four different objects (A, B, C, and D) in the corners of the arena, respectively (Fig. 6C). The rats were then placed in the center of the arena and allowed to explore for 5 min. During the 5-min delay period, all of the objects were cleaned with alcohol, to remove olfactory cues. In the test phase, the positions of two of the four objects were exchanged, and the rats were allowed to explore for 5 min. The time spent touching the exchanged objects was compared with the time spent touching the unexchanged objects98. We chose the exchanged objects randomly and the minimum touching time for object was set as a criterion of at least one second.
Social recognition task
Prior to the task, the rats were habituated to the U-field two-choice box for 5 min. Then, in the sample phase, each rat was placed in the center of the box and allowed to explore an unfamiliar target (same strain, age, and sex) placed in one side (Fig. 6D). Five minutes later, the rat was placed in the box again and allowed to freely explore the same target (familiar) or a stranger (novel) for 5 min. To assess social recognition memory, we measured the time spent touching a familiar or novel social target in test phase. Touching behavior was defined as the animal directing its nose toward or its forelimbs touching the object.
Y-maze spontaneous alternation
To evaluate spatial working memory, we used a Y-maze apparatus (Fig. 6E,F). The maze consisted of three arms made of gray plastic joined in the middle to form a “Y” shape (MY-10, Shinfactory, Japan). The walls of the arms had an outside slope of 76° (12 cm high), allowing the rat to see distal spatial landmarks. There were no intermaze cues inside the arms.
Prior to the experiment, the rats were allowed to explore the maze for 5 min. Then, 24 h after the habituation, the rats were placed in one arm again and their spontaneous behavior was recorded for 5 min. By analyzing the number and sequence of arms entered, we calculated the score as the number of alternations divided by the total alternations22.
Statistical analysis
We used unpaired t-tests to analyze the data for mEPSCs, mIPSCs, and self-entropy. Because the self-entropy data had large variations within the group, we performed log (1 + x) transformation prior to the analysis99. To analyze the recognition memory tasks (object recognition, novel object placement, social preference, and social recognition tasks), we used paired t-tests to compare the time spent in touching novel and familiar targets in test phase. To analyze other behavioral tasks (visual, open field, light–dark box, pain threshold, social interaction, hanging wire, incline and Y-maze spontaneous alternation tests), we used a one-way factorial ANOVA in which the between-group factors were the postnatal weeks. To analyze the developmental change in the IA, contextual freezing, and rotor rod tasks, we used a two-way ANOVA with repeated measures; the between-group factors were the postnatal weeks and within-group factors were trials. Significance was set at P < 0.05.
Supplementary Information
Acknowledgements
This project was supported by Grants-in-Aid for Scientific Research B (D.M.), Scientific Research C (Y.S. and D.M.), and Scientific Research in Innovative Areas (D.M.), from the Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science, and Technology of Japan (Grant nos. 20K07276, 19H03402 and 16H05129). The authors would like to thank Enago (www.enago.jp) for the English language review.
Author contributions
D.M. and Y.S. wrote the manuscript. Y.S., D.Y., A.S., G. M., and H. K. performed the experiments. D.M. designed the study. Y.S. analyzed data for both statistics and self-entropy.
Data availability
The entirety of raw data from this study is available from the authors upon request.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Contributor Information
Yuya Sakimoto, Email: ysaki@yamaguchi-u.ac.jp.
Dai Mitsushima, Email: mitsu@yamaguchi-u.ac.jp.
Supplementary Information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41598-022-10453-z.
References
- 1.Scoville WB, Milner B. Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry. 1957;20:11–21. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.20.1.11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Mitsushima D, Takase K, Funabashi T, Kimura F. Gonadal steroids maintain 24-h acetylcholine release in the hippocampus: organizational and activational effects in behaving rats. J. Neurosci. 2009;29:3808–3815. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5301-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Wills TJ, Cacucci F, Burgess N, O’Keefe J. Development of the hippocampal cognitive map in preweanling rats. Science. 2010;328:1573–1576. doi: 10.1126/science.1188224. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Gelbard-Sagiv H, Mukamel R, Harel M, Malach R, Fried I. Internally generated reactivation of single neurons in human hippocampus during free recall. Science. 2008;322:96–101. doi: 10.1126/science.1164685. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Ishikawa J, Tomokage T, Mitsushima D. A possible coding for experience: Ripple-like events and synaptic diversity. BioRxiv. 2019 doi: 10.1101/2019.12.30.891259. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Campbell BA, Campbell EH. Retention and extinction of learned fear in infant and adult rats. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 1962;55:1–8. doi: 10.1037/h0049182. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Campbell BA, Spear NE. Ontogeny of memory. Psychol. Rev. 1972;79:215–236. doi: 10.1037/h0032690. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Josselyn SA, Frankland PW. Infantile amnesia: A neurogenic hypothesis. Learn. Mem. 2012;19:423–433. doi: 10.1101/lm.021311.110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Takase K, Sakimoto Y, Kimura F, Mitsushima D. Developmental trajectory of contextual learning and 24-h acetylcholine release in the hippocampus. Sci. Rep. 2014;4:3738. doi: 10.1038/srep03738. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Travaglia A, Bisaz R, Sweet ES, Blitzer RD, Alberini CM. Infantile amnesia reflects a developmental critical period for hippocampal learning. Nat. Neurosci. 2016;19:1225–1233. doi: 10.1038/nn.4348. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Miles C. A study of individual psychology. Am. J. Psychol. 1893;6:534–558. doi: 10.2307/1411191. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Pugh CR, Rudy JW. A developmental analysis of contextual fear conditioning. Dev. Psychobiol. 1996;29:87–100. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-2302(199603)29:2<87::AID-DEV1>3.0.CO;2-H. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Alberini CM, Travaglia A. Infantile amnesia: A critical period of learning to learn and remember. J. Neurosci. 2017;37:5783–5795. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0324-17.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Baudry M, Arst DS, Lynch G. Increased [3H] glutamate receptor binding in aged rats. Brain Res. 1981;223:195–198. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(81)90823-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Baudry M, Arst DS, Oliver M, Lynch G. Development of glutamate binding sites and their regulation by calcium in rat hippocampus. Brain Res. 1981;227:37–48. doi: 10.1016/0165-3806(81)90092-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Steward O, Falk PM. Selective localization of polyribosomes and developing synapses in the hippocampus and dentate gyrus. J. Comp. Neurol. 1991;314:545–557. doi: 10.1002/cne.903140311. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Collin C, Miyaguchi K, Segal M. Dendritic spine density and LTP induction in cultured hippocampal slices. J. Neurophysiol. 1997;77:1614–1623. doi: 10.1152/jn.1997.77.3.1614. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Hussain RJ, Carpenter DO. Development of synaptic responses and plasticity at the SC-CA1 and MF-CA3 synapses in rat hippocampus. Cell Mol. Neurobiol. 2001;21:357–368. doi: 10.1023/A:1012602105208. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Cizeron M, et al. A brain-wide atlas of synapses across the mouse lifespan. Science. 2020;369:270–275. doi: 10.1126/science.aba3163. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Malinow R, Malenka RC. AMPA receptor trafcking and synaptic plasticity. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2002;25:103–126. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.25.112701.142758. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Derkach VA, Oh MG, Guire ES, Soderling TR. Regulatory mechanisms of AMP receptors in synaptic plasticity. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2007;8:101–113. doi: 10.1038/nrn2055. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Mitsushima D, Ishihara K, Sano A, Kessels HW, Takahashi T. Contextual learning requires synaptic AMPA receptor delivery in the hippocampus. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2011;108:12503–12508. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1104558108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Mitsushima D, Sano A, Takahashi T. A cholinergic trigger drives learning-induced plasticity at hippocampal synapses. Nat. Commun. 2013;4:2760. doi: 10.1038/ncomms3760. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Sakimoto Y, Kida H, Mitsushima D. Temporal dynamics of learning-promoted synaptic diversity in CA1 pyramidal neurons. FASEB J. 2019;33:14382–14393. doi: 10.1096/fj.201801893RRR. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Sakimoto Y, Mizuno J, Kida H, Kamiya Y, Ono Y, Mitsushima D. Learning promotes subfield-specific synaptic diversity in hippocampal CA1 neurons. Cereb. Cortex. 2019;29:2183–2195. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhz022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Pinheiro PS, Mulle C. Presynaptic glutamate receptors: Physiological functions and mechanisms of action. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2008;9:423–436. doi: 10.1038/nrn2379. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Schneider T, Prezewlocki R. Behavioral alterations in rats prenatally exposed to valproic acid: animal model of autism. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2005;30:80–89. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300518. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Behuet S, et al. Developmental changes of glutamate and GABA receptor densities in wistar rats. Front. Neuroanat. 2019;13:100. doi: 10.3389/fnana.2019.00100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Cao G, Harris KM. Developmental regulation of the late phase of long-term potentiation (L-LTP) and metaplasticity in hippocampal area CA1 of the rat. J. Neurophsiol. 2012;107:902–912. doi: 10.1152/jn.00780.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Teyler TJ, Perkins AT, 4th, Harris KM. The development of long-term potentiation in hippocampus and neocortex. Neuropsychologia. 1989;27:31–39. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(89)90088-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Dumas TC. Postnatal alterations in induction threshold and expression magnitude of long-term potentiation and long-term depression at hippocampal synapses. Hippocampus. 2012;22:188–199. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20881. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Bolshakov VY, Golan H, Kandel ER, Siegelbaum SA. Recruitment of new sites of synaptic transmission during the cAMP-dependent late phase of LTP at CA3-CA1 synapses in the hippocampus. Neuron. 1997;19:635–651. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80377-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Prange O, Murphy TH. Correlation of miniature synaptic activity and evoked release probability in cultures of cortical neurons. J. Neurosci. 1999;19:6427–6438. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-15-06427.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Liao D, Scannevin RH, Huganir R. Activation of silent synapses by rapid activity-dependent synaptic recruitment of AMPA receptors. J. Neurosci. 2001;21:6008–6017. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-16-06008.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Isaac JT, Nicoll RA, Malenka RC. Evidence for silent synapses: Implications for the expression of LTP. Neuron. 1995;15:427–434. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(95)90046-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Wierenga CJ, Ibata K, Turrigiano GG. Postsynaptic expression of homeostatic plasticity at neocortical synapses. J. Neurosci. 2005;25:2895–2905. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5217-04.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Paw-Min-Thein-Oo, Sakimoto Y, Kida H, Mitsushima D. Proximodistal heterogeneity in learning-promoted pathway-specific plasticity at dorsal CA1 synapses. Neuroscience. 2020;437:184–195. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2020.04.040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Padamsey Z, Tong R, Emptage N. Glutamate is required for depression but not potentiation of long-term presynaptic function. Elife. 2017;6:e29688. doi: 10.7554/eLife.29688. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Whitlock JR, Heynen AJ, Shuler MG, Bear MF. Learning induces long-term potentiation in the hippocampus. Science. 2006;313:1093–1097. doi: 10.1126/science.1128134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Lohmann C, Kessels HW. The developmental stage of synaptic plasticity. J. Physiol. 2014;592:13–31. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2012.235119. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Matsuo N, Reijmers L, Mayford M. Spine-type-specific recruitment of newly synthesized AMPA receptors with learning. Science. 2008;319:1104–1107. doi: 10.1126/science.1149967. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Choi JH, et al. Interregional synaptic maps among engram cells underlie memory formation. Science. 2018;360:430–435. doi: 10.1126/science.aas9204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Hu H, et al. Emotion enhances learning via norepinephrine regulation of AMPA-receptor trafficking. Cell. 2007;131:160–173. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2007.09.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.DeLorey TM, et al. Mice lacking the β3 subunit of the GABAA receptor have the epilepsy phenotype and many of the behavioral characteristics of angelman syndrome. J. Neurosci. 1998;18:8505–8514. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-20-08505.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Lovett-Barron M, et al. Dendritic inhibition in the hippocampus supports fear learning. Science. 2014;343:857–863. doi: 10.1126/science.1247485. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Kittler JT, et al. Phospho-dependent binding of the clathrin AP2 adaptor complex to GABAA receptors regulates the efficacy of inhibitory synaptic transmission. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2005;102:14871–14876. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0506653102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Cui Y, et al. Neurofibromin regulation of ERK signaling modulates GABA release and learning. Cell. 2008;135:549–560. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2008.09.060. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Han K, et al. Excitation-inhibition imbalance leads to alteration of neuronal coherence and neurovascular coupling under acute stress. J. Neurosci. 2020;40:9148–9162. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1553-20.2020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Wang GY, Zhu ZM, Cui S, Wang JH. Glucocorticoid induces incoordination between glutamatergic and GABAergic neurons in the amygdala. PLoS One. 2016;11:e0166535. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0166535. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Selemon LD. A role for synaptic plasticity in the adolescent development of executive function. Transl. Psychiatry. 2013;3:e238. doi: 10.1038/tp.2013.7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Page CE, Alexander J, Shepard R, Coutellier L. Npas4 deficiency interacts with adolescent stress to disrupt prefrontal GABAergic maturation and adult cognitive flexibility. Genes Brain Behav. 2018;17:e12459. doi: 10.1111/gbb.12459. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Yizhar O, et al. Neocortical excitation/inhibition balance in information processing and social dysfunction. Nature. 2011;477:171–178. doi: 10.1038/nature10360. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Qian N, Sejnowski TJ. When is an inhibitory synapse effective? Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 1990;87:8145–8149. doi: 10.1073/pnas.87.20.8145. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Koch C, Poggio T, Torre V. Nonlinear interactions in a dendritic tree: Localization, timing, and role in information processing. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 1983;80:2799–2802. doi: 10.1073/pnas.80.9.2799. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Garcia-Bereguiain MA, et al. In vivo synaptic scaling is mediated by GluA2-lacking AMPA receptors in the embryonic spinal cord. J. Neurosci. 2013;33:6791–6799. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4025-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Qui Z, et al. The Rett syndrome protein MeCP2 regulates synaptic scaling. J. Neurosci. 2012;32:989–994. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0175-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Dudek SM, Bear MF. Homosynaptic long-term depression in area CA1 of hippocampus and effects of N-methyl-d-aspartate receptor blockade. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 1992;89:4364–4367. doi: 10.1073/pnas.89.10.4363. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Mulkey RM, Malenka RC. Mechanisms underlying induction of homosynaptic long-term depression in area CA1 of the hippocampus. Neuron. 1992;9:967–975. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(92)90248-C. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Mulkey RM, Herron CE, Malenka RC. An essential role for protein phosphatases in hippocampal long-term depression. Science. 1993;261:1051–1055. doi: 10.1126/science.8394601. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Bolshakov VY, Siegelbaum SA. Postsynaptic induction and presynaptic expression of hippocampal long-term depression. Science. 1994;264:1148–1152. doi: 10.1126/science.7909958. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Kemp N, McQueen J, Faulkes S, Bashir ZI. Different forms of LTD in the CA1 region of the hippocampus: Role of age and stimulus protocol. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2000;12:360–366. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2000.00903.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Jappy D, Valiullina F, Draguhn A, Rozov A. GABAA receptor-dependent long-term depression at hippocampal synapses between CB1-positiveinterneurons and CA1 pyramidal cells. Front. Cell Neurosci. 2016;10:4. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2016.00004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Wasling P, Stranderg J, Hanse E. AMPA receptor activation causes silencing of AMPA receptor-mediated synaptic transmission in the developing hippocampus. PLoS One. 2012;7:e34474. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0034474. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Xiao MY, Wasling P, Hanse E, Gustafsson B. Creation of AMPA-silent synapses in the neonatal hippocampus. Nat. Neurosci. 2004;7:236–243. doi: 10.1038/nn1196. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Mateos JM, et al. Synaptic modifications at the CA3-CA1 synapses after chronic AMPA receptor blockade in rat hippocampal slices. J. Physiol. 2007;15:129–138. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2006.120550. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Kim JH, McNally GP, Richardson R. Recovery of fear memories in rats: Role of gamma-amino butyric acid (GABA) in infantile amnesia. Behav. Neurosci. 2006;120:40–48. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.120.1.40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Kim JH, Richardson R. Immediate post-reminder injection of gamma-amino butyric acid (GABA) agonist midazolam attenuates reactivation of forgotten fear in the infant rat. Behav. Neurosci. 2007;121:1328–1332. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.121.6.1328. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Ainge JA, Langston RF. Ontogeny of neural circuits underlying spatial memory in the rat. Front. Neural. Circuits. 2012;6:8. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2012.00008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Akers KG, Candelaria-Cook FT, Rice JP, Johnson TE, Hamiton DA. Delayed development of place navigation compared to directional responding in young rats. Behav. Neurosci. 2009;123:267–275. doi: 10.1037/a0014594. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Robinson NTM, et al. Targeted activation of hippocampal place cells drives memory-guided spatial behavior. Cell. 2020;183:1586–1599. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2020.09.061. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Douglas RJ, Peterson JJ, Douglas DP. The ontogeny of a hippocampus-dependent response in two rodent species. Behav. Biol. 1973;8:27–37. doi: 10.1016/S0091-6773(73)80003-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Danjo T, Toyoizumi T, Fujisawa S. Spatial representations of self and other in the hippocampus. Science. 2018;359:213–218. doi: 10.1126/science.aao3898. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Kida H, Sakimoto Y, Mitsushima D. Slice patch clamp technique for analyzing learning-induced plasticity. J. Vis. Exp. 2017;129:e55876. doi: 10.3791/55876. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Kida DH, et al. Motor training promotes both synaptic and intrinsic plasticity of layer II/III pyramidal neurons in the primary motor cortex. Cereb. Cortex. 2016;26:3494–3507. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw134. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Paxinos, G., & Watson, C. The Rat Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates, 6th ed. (2006). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 76.Huang YY, Colino A, Selig DK, Malenka RC. The influence of prior synaptic activity on the induction of long-term potentiation. Science. 1992;255:730–733. doi: 10.1126/science.1346729. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Shi S, Hayashi Y, Esteban JA, Malinow R. Subunit-specific rules governing AMPA receptor trafficking to synapses in hippocampal pyramidal neurons. Cell. 2001;105:331–343. doi: 10.1016/S0092-8674(01)00321-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Mangan PS, Kapur J. Factors underlying bursting behavior in a network of cultured hippocampal neurons exposed to zero magnesium. J. Neurophysiol. 2004;91:946–957. doi: 10.1152/jn.00547.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Van Hook MJ. Temperature effects on synaptic transmission and neuronal function in visual thalamus. PLoS One. 2020;15:e0232451. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0232451. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Rogers CJ, Hunter BE. Chronic ethanol treatment reduces inhibition in CA1 of the rat hippocampus. Brain Res. Bull. 1992;28:587–592. doi: 10.1016/0361-9230(92)90107-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Silverman BW. Density Estimation for Statistics and Data Analysis. Monographs on Statistics and Applied Probability. Chapman and Hall; 1986. [Google Scholar]
- 82.Sheather SJ. Density estimation. Stat. Sci. 2004;19:588–597. doi: 10.1214/088342304000000297. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Shannon CE. A mathematical theory of communication. Bell. Syst. Tech. J. 1948;27:379–423. doi: 10.1002/j.1538-7305.1948.tb01338.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Pinto LH, Enroth-Cugell C. Tests of the mouse visual system. Mamm. Genome. 2000;11:531–536. doi: 10.1007/s003350010102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Metz GA, Schwab ME. Behavioral characterization in a comprehensive mouse test battery reveals motor and sensory impairments in growth-associated protein-43 null mutant mice. Neuroscience. 2004;129:563–574. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.07.053. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Sooksawate T, et al. Viral vector mediated selective and reversible blockade of the pathway for visual orienting in mice. Front. Neural Circuits. 2013;7(162):2013. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2013.00162.eCollection. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Carlile EL, Shirachi DY, Quock RM. An anxiolytic-like effect of hyperbaric oxygen in the mouse light/dark exploration test. Life Sci. 2012;90:267–271. doi: 10.1016/j.lfs.2011.11.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Arrant AE, Schramm-Sapyta NL, Kuhn CM. Use of the light/dark test for anxietyin adult and adolescent male rats. Behav. Brain Res. 2013;256:119–127. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2013.05.035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Chen Z, Wei H, Pertovaara A, Wang J, Carlson S. Anxiety- and activity-related effects of paracetamol on healthy and neuropathic rats. Pharmacol. Res. Perspect. 2018;6:e00367. doi: 10.1002/prp2.367. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Lehner AM, et al. The relationship between pain sensitivity and conditioned fear response in rats. Acta Neurobiol. Exp. (Wars) 2010;70:56–66. doi: 10.55782/ane-2010-1774. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Abada YS, Nguyen HP, Ellenbroek B, Schreiber R. Reversal learning and associative memory impairments in a BACHD rat model for Huntington disease. PLoS One. 2013;8:e71633. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0071633. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Vengeliene V, et al. Towards trans-diagnostic mechanisms in psychiatry: Neurobehabioral profile of rats with a loss-of-function point mutation in the dopamine transporter gene. Dis. Model Mech. 2017;10:451–461. doi: 10.1242/dmm.027623. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Park JY, et al. Implementation of a two-dimensional behavior matrix to distinguish individuals with differential depression states in a rodent model of depression. Exp. Neurobiol. 2014;23:215–223. doi: 10.5607/en.2014.23.3.215. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Lee Y, Kim H, Han PL. Striatal inhibition of MeCP2 or TSC1 produces sociability deficits and repetitive behaviors. Exp. Neurobiol. 2018;27:539–549. doi: 10.5607/en.2018.27.6.539. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Lee Y, et al. Excessive D1 dopamine receptor activation in the dorsal striatum promotes autistic-like behaviors. Mol. Neurobiol. 2018;55:5658–5671. doi: 10.1007/s12035-017-0770-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Bowen MT, et al. Oxytocin prevents ethanol actions at δ subunit-containing GABAA receptors and attenuates ethanol-induced motor impairment in rats. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 2015;112:3104–3109. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1416900112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Sakamoto I, et al. The protective effect and mechanism of COA-Cl in acute phase after spinal cord injury. Neurosci. Res. 2021;170:114–121. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2020.10.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Barker GR, Bird F, Alexander V, Warburton EC. Recognition memory for objects, place, and temporal order: A disconnection analysis of the role of the medial prefrontal cortex and perirhinal cortex. J. Neurosci. 2007;27:2948–2957. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5289-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Mitsushima D, Hei D, Terasawa E. γ-Aminobutyric acid is an inhibitory neurotransmitter restricting the release of luteinizing-hormone releasing hormone before the onset of puberty. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 1994;91:395–399. doi: 10.1073/pnas.91.1.395. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The entirety of raw data from this study is available from the authors upon request.