Abstract
Background: Parkinson’s disease (PD) is a neurodegenerative disease in which the progressive loss of dopaminergic neurons (DA) leads to initially sporadic and eventually widespread damage of the nervous system resulting in significant musculoskeletal and cognitive deterioration. Loss of motor function alongside increasing cognitive impairment is part of the natural disease progression. Gait is often considered an automatic activity; however, walking is the result of a delicate balance of multiple systems which maintain the body’s center of mass over an ever-changing base of support. It is a complex motor behavior that requires components of attention and memory to prevent falls and injury. In addition, evidence points to the critical role of salient visual information to gait adaptability. There is a growing understanding that treatment for PD needs to address movement as it occurs naturally and walking needs to be practiced in more complex environments than traditional therapy has provided.
Methods
In this single-blinded randomized-controlled pilot study, an immersive treadmill training was piloted to determine feasibility and preliminary efficacy on gait and cognition in people with PD. Eighteen participants with Hoehn and Yahr stages I-III PD were randomized to either an intervention or a waitlist control group. Following baseline data collection, the intervention group trained for 30 min, three times/week for 4 weeks on a split belt treadmill combined with a first-person immersive video game targeting visuospatial skills and working memory. Assessment was repeated after 4 weeks of training for the experimental group and 1-month after baseline for the control group. Primary motor outcomes were captured with the APDM Opal sensors during 6 MWT, TUG, and TUG Cognitive. Secondary outcomes of cognition were measured with the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA), Verbal Fluency (Fruit, Vegetable, and Animal) and the Symbol Digit Modality Test (SDMT). Within subject differences were calculated using the Wilcoxon Signed Ranked Test and between subject comparisons were analyzed using the Mann Whitney U-test.
Results
This novel treadmill training program was well-tolerated with all participants in the intervention group completing 4 weeks of training three times a week without any adverse effects. After immersive cognitive motor training, the experimental group made clinically relevant improvements in gait speed and walking distance during the 6 MWT while members of the control group showed no change or decreased gait speed and walking distance over the 1-month trial. In addition, the experimental group demonstrated significant improvement for the TUG Cognitive (p = 0.05) and those changes were greater than the control group (between group p = 0.040). The experimental group also improved scores on MoCA (p = 0.007) and SDMT (p = 0.01) cognitive outcome measures while the control group did not.
Conclusion
The use of immersive gaming technology to engage specific areas of cognition related to gait is feasible in PD. The treadmill training program paired with a customized interactive video game improved walking velocity in addition to non-significant but consistent improvements in other gait measures and cognitive performance in participants with early to mid-stage PD.
Keywords: dual task, gaming, rehabilitation, gait, cognitive-motor interference, neurodegenerative disease, physical therapy
Introduction
Parkinson’s disease (PD) leads to initially sporadic and eventually widespread damage of the nervous system resulting in significant musculoskeletal and cognitive deterioration (Del Tredici and Braak, 2012). Although symptoms vary, difficulty walking is a cardinal motor symptom of the disease. Clinically significant gait dysfunction occurs at some point in all persons with PD and 85% of individuals develop impairments within 3 years of diagnosis (Kang et al., 2005). The gait of patients with PD is typically marked by reduced speed, shortened stride length, and longer double support phase (Morris et al., 1994; Ebersbach et al., 1999; Sofuwa et al., 2005). In addition, gait dynamics are characterized by exaggerated stride-to-stride variability (Blin et al., 1990; Hausdorff et al., 1998; Schaafsma et al., 2003; Nasreddine et al., 2005; Baltadjieva et al., 2006) reflecting a disturbance in gait rhythmicity and an inconsistency of the locomotor pattern, increasing fall risk (Nakamura et al., 1996; Hausdorff et al., 2001; Schaafsma et al., 2003; Hausdorff, 2005). Loss of mobility is associated with reductions in quality of life, activities of daily living, and productivity in PD (Pickering et al., 2007; Forsaa et al., 2008; Muslimovic et al., 2008; Rahman et al., 2008).
Walking is not automatic; it involves cognitive areas of attention and memory (Holtzer et al., 2012), which are modulated by the dopaminergic system and known to be compromised in PD (Pagonabarraga and Kulisevsky, 2012). In PD, attentional demands may exceed available resources in tasks that depend on internal cues (Brown and Marsden, 1988). Several of the hallmark deficits in PD are due to changes in the frontal-striatal circuits and involve executive defects in planning, initiation, monitoring of goal-directed behaviors and working-memory. Visuospatial and memory deficits more representative of posterior cortical functioning are also present in persons with PD even without corresponding dementia (Pagonabarraga and Kulisevsky, 2012). In an experiment where motor and cognitive tasks were performed independently and combined in a dual task paradigm, individuals with PD showed distinct striatal recruitment that was not seen in single task performance or in the control participants. Results suggest that individuals with PD may have specific impairments of the cortical-striatal circuitry related to segregation needed to allow independence of motor and cognitive functions during dual tasking (Nieuwhof et al., 2017). There is a growing understanding that treatment needs to address movement as it occurs naturally and walking needs to be practiced in more complex environments than traditional therapy has provided (Moseley et al., 2005). Previous research has shown individuals with PD have greater deficits in gait under dual-task conditions than their healthy peers. Gait velocity and variability increases while cognitive performance on standardized tests decreases under dual-task conditions (O’Shea et al., 2002; Lord et al., 2010; Plotnick et al., 2011). In their review of this literature, Kelly et al. (2012) point out that the frequency of multitasking and the resulting impairments creates a pressing need for therapeutic interventions that address dual-task gait deficits in PD.
The use of treadmills in gait rehabilitation in PD patients improves gait performance (Miyai et al., 2000; Pohl et al., 2003; Herman et al., 2007; Bello et al., 2008, 2010). The repetitive, regulated walking on a treadmill improves spatiotemporal gait parameters and those improvements are maintained in PD (Mehrholz et al., 2016). Importantly, walking on a treadmill allows patients to increase walk intensity (increasing time on task) in a safe manner. However, traditional treadmill walking has a number of limitations. Standard treadmill training has minimal real-world sensory information necessary for motor learning. Previous work in PD (Rochester et al., 2010) and stroke (Peters et al., 2015) have shown the importance of providing salient sensory information, including combined proprioceptive and visual cues, to create long-term improvement in gait. Visuospatial information, particularly that created by optic flow during walking, is specifically important in healthy, adaptable gait control (Bruce et al., 1996; Patla et al., 2004; Mukherjee et al., 2011; Chien et al., 2014). Similarly, in PD, visual control appears to be critical during simple (Azulay et al., 1999) and complex (Vitório et al., 2013) walking.
Another restriction of treadmills typically used for rehabilitation are that they change speed and inclination only after user input explicitly cueing the performer of an upcoming adjustment. Step length variability (left-right step length variation) is likewise limited by a single belt moving under the walker. A split-belt treadmill, comprised of two belts as opposed to the conventional one belt, allows for users to drive each foot independently of the other and allows clinicians to target left-right asymmetries. A training regimen that utilizes both a split belt treadmill and an external game controller that links an immersive visual experience to movement of the treadmill, may provide another option for training motor adaption with varying amounts of complexity while also providing the benefits of ecologically valid visual information. This pilot study examines the feasibility and preliminary efficacy of such a novel immersive treadmill training program to improve automaticity of gait by addressing both the movement deficits and cognitive components critical to walking and walking adaptation by providing continuous, salient sensorimotor information.
Materials and Methods
This study had approval from the Stony Brook University ethics committee (00646/https://clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT01917903) and all participants gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki prior to data collection. Testing took place in the Rehabilitation Research and Movement Performance Laboratory, School of Health Professions, Stony Brook University.
Participants
Twenty-two participants with PD were recruited for inclusion in this study. Inclusion criteria included: confirmed diagnosis by a movement disorder neurologist; 21–79 years of age, ability to walk 15 min with or without an assistive device, and ability to understand all study procedures and sign informed consent documents. Exclusion from the study was based on not meeting the inclusion criteria, secondary orthopedic or neurological injury or disease that affected gait or balance, visual or vestibular deficits that impair walking, and diagnosis of a cognitive impairment unrelated to PD. Randomization was performed using a random number generator for five bins of 6, 4, 4, 4, and 4 participants recruited (i.e., randomization occurred after recruitment of the initial 6, and each set of 4 participants afterward). One participant was over 80 years of age after initial consent and randomization to the experimental group but prior to completing data collection and was removed from further inclusion. Two participants withdrew following randomization to the waitlist group but prior to any data collection. One participant allocated to the experimental group demonstrated significant cognitive difficulties. After follow-up with neurology a secondary cognitive impairment was diagnosed. Training was completed but data was not included for this participant in analysis. Eighteen participants with PD, modified Hoehn and Yahr stages 1–3 (Goetz et al., 2004) randomly allocated to the experimental (N = 9) or waitlist control group (N = 9) completed all testing and are included in all data analyses. Demographic data for individual participants are provided in Table 1.
TABLE 1.
Participant ID | Sex | Age | H&Y stage | PD medications | Baseline gait velocity | Baseline MoCA |
E1 | F | 68 | 1 | Y | 0.93 | 27 |
E2 | M | 75 | 2 | Y | 0.96 | 26 |
E3 | M | 57 | 2 | Y | 1.02 | 27 |
E4 | F | 57 | 2 | Y | 0.99 | 29 |
E5 | M | 71 | 3 | Y | 0.95 | 23 |
E6 | M | 59 | 2 | N | 0.94 | 28 |
E7 | M | 64 | 1.5 | Y | 1.09 | 23 |
E8 | M | 48 | 2 | Y | 1.04 | 25 |
E9 | F | 70 | 2.5 | Y | 0.69 | 26 |
Mean (SD) | 6M/3F | 64 (9) | 2 (0.56) | 8/9 | 0.96 (0.11) | 26 (2) |
C1 | M | 78 | 2 | Y | 0.72 | 23 |
C2 | F | 69 | 2 | Y | 1.05 | 25 |
C3 | M | 73 | 3 | Y | 0.92 | 26 |
C4 | M | 79 | 2.5 | Y | 0.83 | 18 |
C5 | M | 65 | 1 | Y | 1.17 | 29 |
C6 | F | 72 | 2 | Y | 0.87 | 24 |
C7 | F | 72 | 1.5 | Y | 1.17 | 28 |
C8 | M | 66 | 2 | Y | 1.01 | 30 |
C9 | M | 67 | 1 | Y | 1.08 | 30 |
Mean (SD) | 6M/3F | 71 (5) | 1.9 (0.65) | 9/9 | 0.98 (0.16) | 26 (4) |
Sex, Age, Hoehn and Yahr (H&Y) stage, use of PD medication, baseline velocity, and baseline MoCA score for individual participants in each group. Participants in the Experimental group (E1–E9) and Control group (C1–C9) were equivalent at baseline (p > 0.05 for all comparisons). There was only one participant (E6) that was not taking medication (dopamine, dopamine agonists, anticholinergics, COMT or MAO inhibitors) for PD. Means and SD are in bold.
Procedures
All assessments and training were performed with participants ON medication. Following randomization, baseline data was collected by two assessors (CB and MK) blinded to group allocation. Participants completed standing and walking assessments while wearing APDM Opal (Portland, OR) body worn inertial sensors on both wrists, mid-chest, lumbar spine, and both feet. Motor tasks were completed in the same order for each session and included the 6-min walk test (6 MWT), Timed Up and Go (TUG) (Podsiadlo and Richardson, 1991) and TUG Cognitive (performing the TUG while subtracting 3 from a randomly selected number in the 90s) (TUG Cognitive) (Schumway-Cook et al., 2000). In addition, participants were administered the Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT) (Smith, 1982), Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA), and Tests of Verbal Fluency Fruits, Vegetables, and Animals (TVF_F, TVF_V, and TVF_A, respectively) (Battig and Montague, 1969). All measures were repeated at the end of 4 weeks of training for the experimental group and after 1-month of usual activity for the control group.
Participants in the experimental group completed 360 min of training (30 min per session, 3 times per week × 4 weeks) using a novel, immersive rehabilitation program developed to target gait and cognition. Executive function and visuospatial attention tasks were specifically included as they have been previously described as critical components of walking (Amboni et al., 2013). Training consisted of walking on a split-belt treadmill (Woodway, Inc., Waukesha, WI, United States) connected to a computer running a proprietary first-person gaming system written using C + coding1 and projected on a large screen to include the entire visual field of the participant while they walked on the treadmill. Game controls were positioned on a board positioned in front of the participant and affixed to the treadmill and included three large push buttons (Ablenet, Inc., Roseville, MN, United States) to control forward/reverse, right, and left turns in the game (see Figure 1). Maximum and minimum inclination of the treadmill and speed of each leg—adjusted independently to allow for individualized attention to gait symmetry—was set by the training clinicians (RM and LM). Treadmill speed was set initially using the participant’s baseline gait speed and then adjusted during subsequent sessions to maintain an optimal challenge (Guadagnoli and Lee, 2004) for each subject. Each session consisted of two 15-min games, with a rest as needed in-between, so that participants completed 30 min of training each session.
The experimental group trained three times a week for 4 weeks. An overhead harness was used for safety but did not provide any bodyweight support. The game involved walking through a virtual town to make as many deliveries as possible in 15 min. Participants self-initiated the game by pushing any of the control buttons. Throughout the sessions, participants were able to view their score as well as a clock counting down from 15 min. The level of game play became progressively more difficult each week as follows:
-
•
Week 1—Game interface included a map that showed the location of the participant (red dot), the target location (yellow dot), and the location of the thieves moving around the town (see Figure 1B).
-
•
Week 2—Game interface showed a map of the town but the location of the participant, target location, and location of the thieves was no longer included (see Figure 1C).
-
•
Week 3—The map was no longer shown, requiring the participant to remember the location of streets and houses previously visited as well as visual scan for thieves.
-
•
Week 4—The map is still not present, an additional thief is added, and the prices of the delivered item and the amount paid by the customer is more complex, requiring greater calculations (see Figure 1D).
The system recorded treadmill speed of each leg, minimum and maximum inclines, game scores, total deliveries made, and calculation errors for each session. These measures were not part of the efficacy analyses but will be investigated for clinical utility in the future.
Data Analyses
All APDM sensor data were processed using Mobility Lab software (Version 2). Statistical analyses were completed with SPSS Version 26 (IBM, Armonk NY). Demographic data was compared using independent t-tests assuming equal variance. We used a Wilcoxon Signed Rank test to examine differences in motor (gait speed, 6 MWT distance, TUG, TUG Cognitive) and cognitive (MoCA, SDMT, TVF) task performance from baseline to 1-month post-training (Experimental) or 1-month of usual activity (Control) within subjects. For between subject comparisons, the Mann Whitney U-tests was used. We present graphic representations of all individual data points and provide descriptions of differences in relation to clinically meaningful changes observed.
Results
Feasibility
All participants in the experimental group completed 4 weeks of training without any adverse events. There was 94.4% (34/36 trials) completion of the training trials with two sessions missed due to mechanical issues with the treadmill rather than any participant difficulties. Two of nine participants required seated rests of 5 min between the two 15-min games while the other seven chose short standing rest periods of less than 5 min. All 34 training sessions lasted less than 1 h. Demographic information is shown in Table 1. Participants in the experimental group were not significantly different than the control group for any of baseline measures.
Motor Outcomes
The within and between subject analysis did not show significant differences for any of the motor performance measures except the TUG Cognitive. However, distance and gait speed measured during the 6 MWT trended toward significance for the experimental group with eight of the nine participants increasing speed and walking further post-training (p = 0.086). Small and moderate clinically meaningful difference of 0.06 and 0.14 m/s, respectively (Hass et al., 2014), were used to compare the outcomes among the two groups. One month from baseline, six participants in the experimental group showed moderate gains and another two participants had small but meaningful improvement. In the control group, only two participants had clinically meaningful changes in velocity, both in the moderate range (see Figure 2A). Similarly, considering an increase of 30 m to represent a clinically important change in distance traveled during the 6 MWT (Bohannon and Crouch, 2016), seven of nine people in the experimental group improved and one other increased distance 28.5 m, approaching the minimally clinically important difference. Only two control participants exceeded a 30 m increase in walking distance (see Figure 2B). While the 6 MWT is explicitly linked to gait speed, it is important to demonstrate that the change in speed resulted in a meaningful change in distance walking over the duration of the test. Indeed, one person who did not quite meet the criteria for a moderately clinically meaningful change in velocity did increase walking distance by more than 30 m.
Comparison of the baseline to 1-month testing of the TUG and TUG Cognitive demonstrated that most participants in the experimental group could incorporate the increase in gait speed into functional tasks. Interestingly, it was the TUG Cognitive, an outcome measure that directly measures combined cognitive and motor performance, that showed statistically significant improvement within the experimental group (p = 0.05) but not the control group (p = 0.37) resulting in a between group difference on the Mann-Whitney U-test (p = 0.040). Indeed, six participants in the experimental group showed greater improvement than anyone in the control group (see Figures 2C,D).
Cognitive Outcomes
Although change in cognitive performance was not a main outcome in this pilot study, the comparison from baseline to post-training (experimental) and 1-month post usual activity (control) revealed significant differences in outcomes. While neither group showed significant improvements in any of the verbal fluency tests, the experimental group showed significant improvement in both the MoCA (p = 0.007) (Figure 3A) and SDMT (p = 0.01) (Figure 3B) with all 9 participants improving on the MoCA and 8 of 9 improving on the SDMT. In the control group, baseline to post change in the MoCA approached significance (p = 0.08) but with only 4 participants showing improved scores, 4 showing no change and the final participant showing decline. Similarly, control participants did not show overall improvement in the SDMT (p = 0.40) with 4 participants improving, 1 participant showing no change, and 4 participants with poorer performance after 1-month of usual activity (see Figure 3).
Discussion
Leveraging technology to treat PD is not new. Deep brain stimulation is a well-established tool to improve motor performance even when medication is not effective (Groiss et al., 2009). In addition, there is increasing literature demonstrating the benefits of wearable sensors for the assessment and management of PD symptoms (Rovini et al., 2017). However, the use of technology for rehabilitation has not always considered real-life environments during functional mobility training. The principle of ecological validity for rehabilitation is that gait training should not take place only under optimal conditions in a therapeutic gym; rather, walking should be practiced in increasingly complex and variable environments with appropriate sensory information (Hallett and Poewe, 2008). Previous work has demonstrated the potential of dual task training to improve gait in PD (Yang et al., 2019) based on known cognitive motor interaction deficits (Yogev et al., 2005). This pilot study demonstrated the feasibility and preliminary efficacy of an immersive training with complex cognitive engagement and multisensory information processing. Importantly, changes in motor behaviors reached clinically meaningful levels after twelve 30-min sessions, well within the scope of traditional clinical practice.
The use of a treadmill with clinician interface to train under an optimal challenge point framework (Guadagnoli and Lee, 2004) was chosen as it provided a clinically feasible set-up for rehabilitation. Previous work in PD has shown that gait training with a treadmill can improve stride length and foot clearance (Bello et al., 2014) and lead to clinically meaningful improvements in gait velocity (Mehrholz et al., 2016). We found similar improvements in gait velocity and corresponding walking distance over 6 min. Herman et al. (2007) found that progressive, intensive treadmill training over a 6-week period improved both motor skills and quality of life and that these improvements were maintained several weeks after training had ceased. The mechanism underlying the improvement from use of the treadmill is not confirmed but likely includes neuroplastic changes from the intensity of training from the large volume of stepping that can be practiced (Hirsch et al., 2016) and increased sensory cues from the movement of the treadmill (Bello et al., 2010). Interestingly, there has been some suggestion that the individual with PD most likely to benefit from treadmill training is someone with intact cognition (Earhart and Williams, 2012). In this study, cognition was explicitly targeted and shown to improve with 4-weeks of individualized treadmill training.
The use of a split-belt treadmill has been recommended specifically for PD because of the laterality common in presentation (Seuthe et al., 2019). In this study, training clinicians were able to independently regulate the speed of the right and left treadmill belts to work on gait symmetry even though it was not an outcome in our preliminary work. While multiple studies have shown that individuals with PD can adapt to split-belt walking, some work suggests no improvement in coordination post-training (Nanhoe-Mahabier et al., 2013) while other studies indicate that carryover to overground walking may be specific to whether the more impaired or less impaired leg is manipulated on the treadmill (Fasano et al., 2016). More work is needed in this area to determine the potential use of split-belt vs. traditional treadmill training in PD.
While motor symptoms are the hallmark of PD, the presence of cognitive deficits is a well-established non-motor impairment (Meireles and Massano, 2012) that worsens with disease duration (Dubois et al., 2007) and may appear prior to the onset of motor symptoms (Pont-Sunyer et al., 2015). Cognitive training in PD has also shown specific benefits for targeted areas of treatment, including global cognition, attention, and verbal memory (Orgeta et al., 2020). The vast majority of these studies isolated cognition from motor impairments but there is recent work addressing both movement and cognition through enriched environment (Wang et al., 2021) and dual task training (for review, see De Freitas TB MS et al., 2020). Similar to these studies, we found that individuals with PD were able to improve gait speed and functional gait (TUG and TUG Cognitive). However, we additionally showed that integrating targeted cognitive training, including executive control, visuospatial working memory, and attention, during treadmill training resulted in improved cognitive scores as well. Combining cognitive and motor training into a single intervention represents a new way of addressing the complex deficits in PD. First, the treadmill walking in and of itself may lead to exercise-induced neuroplastic changes in motor and cognitive processing (Petzinger et al., 2013). While aerobic capacity was not measured, training combined two bouts of 15-min walking at a challenging pace for a 30-min exercise session. Second, the use of an immersive, visually engaging treadmill allowed for repeated training of areas of cognition important to gait to occur simultaneously with walking. The different game levels each week was intentionally directed at maximizing the cognitive challenge. The effect was that this training addressed both the motor and non-motor symptoms of PD.
Interestingly, both the trainers and assessors noted clear phenotypic differences in participants as they performed the assessments and/or training. PD is a heterogenous disease often viewed as a movement disorder with three motor types: Tremor-dominant; akinetic-rigid or postural instability with gait difficulty; or indeterminate (Luo et al., 2019; Wojtala et al., 2019; Markello et al., 2021). The clinical spectrum of PD encompasses many non-motor domains like cognition and autonomic function. In addition to motor phenotypes, PD has been characterized by the severity of non-dopaminergic factors in combination with motor complications. Cluster analysis showed four distinct subtypes, each with multiple motor and non-motor characteristics with groups varying by symptom severity (van Rooden et al., 2011). Biomarker studies show different physiological changes between tremor- dominant PD and non-tremor-dominant PD (Marras and Chaudhuri, 2016), but clinically, individuals rarely remain in one subtype throughout their course of disease (Nutt, 2016). While we did not classify participants, it may have been that the consistent outlier in our findings (participant E3) represented a different subtype. The preliminary findings suggest that stratifying by subgroup may provide more precise treatment response information and be beneficial to rehabilitation treatment planning.
Limitations
This study was limited by the small number of participants in each group. Of the 22 participants recruited, 18 were included and completed all the testing. The withdrawal of two participants after randomization to the waitlist control group suggests that there is a desire for individuals with PD to engage in regular training with new technology. This pilot was structured to provide proof of concept and feasibility of an immersive cognitive motor training program and, therefore, was not powered to make conclusive statements about long-term efficacy. In addition, although the training progression was clinician-driven based on perceived challenge points, the study would have benefited from collection of participant perceptions of exertion (e.g., monitoring heartrate or use of a Borg scale during targeted sessions).
Acceptance of new technology is a critical component of implementation science. It is important to gather more information about the perceptions of the participants and clinicians who might use this training program in clinics. Further data analysis will include quality of life measures and focus groups with qualitative assessment of the experience of the participants who completed the four weeks of training.
Finally, this study utilized a split-belt treadmill to access the ability of the participants to maintain continuous therapeutic walking speeds on separate belts and to provide clinicians a further avenue of individualizing treatment. However, it is unclear from the current data set whether a split-belt treadmill provides any benefits over a standard treadmill and, importantly, as a split-belt treadmill is less clinically available it may make such training less feasible for implementation.
Data Availability Statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
Ethics Statement
The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Stony Brook University Institutional Review Board. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
Author Contributions
LM conceptualized the research project. LM, JL, CR, CB, MK, and RM executed. RM and LM designed and executed the statistical analysis. JL, CR, and LM organized and wrote all drafts of the manuscript. RM, CB, and MK did the review and edits. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher’s Note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Acknowledgments
We thank Guy Schwartz, MD and Carine Mauer, MD, for their assistance with recruitment for this study and Sharon Martino, PT, Ph.D. for assistance with training and recruitment.
Footnotes
Funding
LM received funding for this work from the Hartman Foundation for Parkinson’s Research (Award # 64249) and the Parkinson’s Alliance (Award # 87474).
References
- Amboni M., Barone P., Hausdorff J. (2013). Cognitive contributions to gait and falls: evidence and implications. Mov. Disord. 28 1520–1533. 10.1002/mds.25674 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Azulay J. P., Mesure S., Amblard B., Blin O., Sangla I., Pouget J. (1999). Visual control of locomotion in Parkinson’s disease. Brain 122 111–120. 10.1093/brain/122.1.111 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baltadjieva R., Giladi N., Gruendlinger L., Peretz C., Hausdorff J. M. (2006). Marked alterations in the gait timing and rhythmicity of patients with de novo Parkinson’s disease. Eur. J. Neurosci. 24 1815–1820. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.05033.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Battig W. F., Montague W. E. (1969). Category norms of verbal items in 56 categories a replication and extension of the Connecticut category norms. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 80 1–46. 10.1037/h0027577 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Bello O., Marquez G., Camblor M., Fernandez-Del-Olmo M. (2010). Mechanisms involved in treadmill walking improvements in Parkinson’s disease. Gait Posture 32 118–123. 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2010.04.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bello O., Sanchez J. A., Fernandez-del-Olmo M. (2008). Treadmill walking in Parkinson’s disease patients: adaptation and generalization effect. Mov. Disord. 23 1243–1249. 10.1002/mds.22069 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bello O., Sanchez J. A., Vazquez-Santos C., Fernandez-Del-Olmo M. (2014). Spatiotemporal parameters of gait during treadmill and overground walking in Parkinson’s disease. J. Parkinsons Dis. 4 33–36. 10.3233/JPD-130251 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blin O., Ferrandez A. M., Serratrice G. (1990). Quantitative analysis of gait in Parkinson patients: increased variability of stride length. J. Neurol. Sci. 98 91–97. 10.1016/0022-510x(90)90184-o [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bohannon R. W., Crouch R. (2016). Minimal clinically important difference for change in 6-minute walk test distance of adults with pathology: a systematic review. J. Eval. Clin. Pract. 23 377–381. 10.1111/jep.12629 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown R. G., Marsden C. D. (1988). Internal versus external cues and the control of attention in Parkinson’s disease. Brain 111 323–345. 10.1093/brain/111.2.323 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bruce V., Georgeson M. A., Green P. R. (1996). Visual Perception: Physiology, Psychology, and Ecology, 3rd Edn. Hove: Psychology Press. [Google Scholar]
- Chien J. H., Eikema D.-J. A., Mukherjee M., Stergiou N. (2014). Locomotor sensory organization test: a novel paradigm for the assessment of sensory contributions in gait. Ann. Biomed. Eng. 42 2512–2523. 10.1007/s10439-014-1112-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Freitas TB MS P. T., Leite Phw B. S., Doná F PhD P. T., Pompeu Je PhD P. T., Swarowsky A PhD P. T., Torriani-Pasin C PhD P. T. (2020). The effects of dual task gait and balance training in Parkinson’s disease: a systematic review. Physiother Theory Pract. 36 1088–1096. 10.1080/09593985.2018.1551455 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tredici K. D., Braak H. (2012). Lewy pathology and neurodegeneration in premotor Parkinson’s disease. Mov. Disord. 27 597–607. 10.1002/mds.24921 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dubois B., Burn D., Goetz C., Aarsland D., Brown R. G., Broe G. A., et al. (2007). Diagnostic procedures for Parkinson’s disease dementia: recommendations from the Movement Disorder Society task force. Mov. Disord. 22 2314–24. 10.1002/mds.21844 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Earhart G. M., Williams A. J. (2012). Treadmill training for individuals with Parkinson disease. Phys. Ther. 92 893–897. 10.2522/ptj.20110471 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ebersbach G., Sojer M., Valldeoriola F., Wissel J., Müller J., Tolosa E., et al. (1999). Comparative analysis of gait in Parkinson’s disease, cerebellar ataxia and subcortical arteriosclerotic encephalopathy. Brain 122 1349–1355. 10.1093/brain/122.7.1349 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fasano A., Schlenstedt C., Herzog J., Plotnik M., Rose F. E. M., Volkmann J., et al. (2016). Split-belt locomotion in Parkinson’s disease links asymmetry, dyscoordination and sequence effect. Gait Posture 48 6–12. 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2016.04.020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Forsaa E. B., Larsen J. P., Wentzel-Larsen T., Herlofson K., Alves G. (2008). Predictors and course of health-related quality of life in Parkinson’s disease. Mov. Disord. 23 1420–1427. 10.1002/mds.22121 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Groiss S. J., Wojtecki L., Südmeyer M., Schnitzler A. (2009). Deep Brain Stimulation in Parkinson’s Disease. Ther. Adv. Neurol. Disord. 2 20–28. 10.1177/1756285609339382 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guadagnoli M. A., Lee T. D. (2004). challenge point: a framework for conceptualizing the effects of various practice conditions in motor learning. J. Motor Behav. 36 212–224. 10.3200/jmbr.36.2.212-224 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hallett M., Poewe W. (2008). Therapeutics of Parkinson’s Disease and Other Movement Disorders. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley-Blackwell. [Google Scholar]
- Hass C. J., Bishop M., Moscovich M., Stegemöller E. L., Skinner J., Malaty I. A., et al. (2014). Defining the clinically meaningful difference in gait speed in persons with Parkinson disease. J. Neurol. Phys. Ther. 38 233–238. 10.1097/npt.0000000000000055 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hausdorff J. M. (2005). Gait variability: methods, modeling and meaning. J. NeuroEng. Rehabil. 2 19. 10.1186/1743-0003-2-19 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hausdorff J. M., Cudkowicz M. E., Firtion R., Wei J. Y., Goldberger A. L. (1998). Gait variability and basal ganglia disorders: Stride-to-stride variations of gait cycle timing in Parkinson’s disease and Huntington’s disease. Mov. Disord. 13 428–437. 10.1002/mds.870130310 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hausdorff J. M., Rios D. A., Edelberg H. K. (2001). Gait variability and fall risk in community-living older adults: a 1-year prospective study. Arch. Phy. Med. Rehabil. 82 1050–1056. 10.1053/apmr.2001.24893 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herman T., Giladi N., Gruendlinger L., Hausdorff J. M. (2007). Six weeks of intensive treadmill training improves gait and quality of life in patients with Parkinson’s disease: a pilot study. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 88 1154–1158. 10.1016/j.apmr.2007.05.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hirsch M. A., Iyer S. S., Sanjak M. (2016). Exercise-induced neuroplasticity in human Parkinson’s disease: WHAT is the evidence telling us? Parkinson. Relat. Disord. 22 S78–S81. 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2015.09.030 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holtzer R., Wang C., Verghese J. (2012). The relationship between attention and gait in aging: facts and fallacies. Motor Control. 16 64–80. 10.1123/mcj.16.1.64 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kang G. A., Bronstein J. M., Masterman D. L., Redelings M., Crum J. A., Ritz B. (2005). Clinical characteristics in early Parkinson’s disease in a central california population-based study. Mov. Disord. 20 1133–1142. 10.1002/mds.20513 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelly V. E., Eusterbrock A. J., Shumway-Cook A. (2012). A review of dual-task walking deficits in people with parkinson’s disease: motor and cognitive contributions, mechanisms, and clinical implications. Parkinsons Dis. 2012 918719. 10.1155/2012/918719 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lord S., Rochester L., Hetherington V., Allcock L. M., Burn D. (2010). Executive dysfunction and attention contribute to gait interference in “off” state Parkinson’s Disease. Gait Posture 31 169–174. 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2009.09.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luo L., Andrews H., Alcalay R. N., Poyraz F. C., Boehme A. K., Goldman J. G., et al. (2019). Motor phenotype classification in moderate to advanced PD in BioFIND study. Parkinson. Relat. Disord. 65 178–183. 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2019.06.017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Markello R. D., Shafiei G., Tremblay C., Postuma R. B., Dagher A., Misic B. (2021). Multimodal phenotypic axes of Parkinson’s disease. NPJ Parkinsons Dis. 7 6. 10.1038/s41531-020-00144-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marras C., Chaudhuri K. R. (2016). Nonmotor features of Parkinson’s disease subtypes. Mov. Disord. 31 1095–1102. 10.1002/mds.26510 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mehrholz J., Kugler J., Storch A., Pohl M., Hirsch K., Elsner B. (2016). Treadmill training for patients with Parkinson disease. An abridged version of a Cochrane Review. Eur. J. Phys. Rehabil. Med. 52 704–713. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meireles J., Massano J. (2012). Cognitive impairment and dementia in Parkinson’s disease: clinical features, diagnosis, and management. Front. Neurol. 3:88. 10.3389/fneur.2012.00088 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miyai I., Fujimoto Y., Ueda Y., Yamamoto H., Nozaki S., Saito T., et al. (2000). Treadmill training with body weight support: Its effect on Parkinson’s disease. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 81 849–852. 10.1053/apmr.2000.4439 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morris M. E., Iansek R., Matyas T. A., Summers J. J. (1994). The pathogenesis of gait hypokinesia in Parkinson’s disease. Brain 117 1169–1181. 10.1093/brain/117.5.1169 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moseley A. M., Stark A., Cameron I. D., Pollock A. (2005). Treadmill training and body weight support for walking after stroke. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 4 CD002840. 10.1002/14651858.CD002840 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mukherjee M., Siu K.-C., Katsavelis D., Fayad P., Stergiou N. (2011). The influence of visual perception of self-motion on locomotor adaptation to unilateral limb loading. J. Motor Behav. 43 101–111. 10.1080/00222895.2010.548420 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muslimovic D., Post B., Speelman J. D., Schmand B., de Haan R. J. (2008). Determinants of disability and quality of life in mild to moderate Parkinson disease. Neurology 70 2241–2247. 10.1212/01.wnl.0000313835.33830.80 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nakamura T., Meguro K., Sasaki H. (1996). Relationship between falls and stride length variability in senile dementia of the Alzheimer Type. Gerontology 42 108–113. 10.1159/000213780 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nanhoe-Mahabier W., Snijders A. H., Delval A., Weerdesteyn V., Duysens J., Overeem S., et al. (2013). Split-belt locomotion in Parkinson’s disease with and without freezing of gait. Neuroscience 236 110–116. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.01.038 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nasreddine Z. S., Phillips N. A., Bédirian V., Charbonneau S., Whitehead V., Collin I., et al. (2005). The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc. 53 695–699. 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nieuwhof F., Bloem B. R., Reelick M. F., Aarts E., Maidan I., Mirelman A., et al. (2017). Impaired dual tasking in Parkinson’s disease is associated with reduced focusing of cortico-striatal activity. Brain 140 1384–1398. 10.1093/brain/awx042 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nutt J. G. (2016). Motor subtype in Parkinson’s disease: different disorders or different stages of disease? Mov. Disord. 31 957–961. 10.1002/mds.26657 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Orgeta V., McDonald K. R., Poliakoff E., Hindle J. V., Clare L., Leroi I. (2020). Cognitive training interventions for dementia and mild cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 2 CD011961. 10.1002/14651858.CD011961 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Shea S., Morris M. E., Iansek R. (2002). Dual task interference during gait in people with Parkinson disease: effects of motor versus cognitive secondary tasks. Phys. Ther. 82 888–897. 10.1093/ptj/82.9.888 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pagonabarraga J., Kulisevsky J. (2012). Cognitive impairment and dementia in Parkinson’s disease. Neurobiol. Dis. 46 590–596. 10.1016/j.nbd.2012.03.029 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Patla A. E., Niechwiej E., Davies T. C. (2004). Obstacle avoidance during locomotion using haptic information in normally sighted humans. Exp. Brain Res. 155 173–185. 10.1007/s00221-003-1714-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peters S., Handy T. C., Lakhani B., Boyd L. A., Garland S. J. (2015). Motor and visuospatial attention and motor planning after stroke: considerations for the rehabilitation of standing balance and gait. Phys. Ther. 95 1423–1432. 10.2522/ptj.20140492 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petzinger G. M., Fisher B. E., McEwen S., Beeler J. A., Walsh J. P., Jakowec M. W. (2013). Exercise-enhanced neuroplasticity targeting motor and cognitive circuitry in Parkinson’s disease. Lancet Neurol. 12 716–726. 10.1016/S1474-4422(13)70123-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pickering R. M., Grimbergen Y. A. M., Rigney U., Ashburn A., Mazibrada G., Wood B., et al. (2007). A meta-analysis of six prospective studies of falling in Parkinson’s disease. Mov. Disord. 22 1892–1900. 10.1002/mds.21598 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Plotnick M., Giladi N., Dagan Y., Hausdorff J. M. (2011). Postural instability and fall risk in Parkinson’s disease: impaired dual tasking, pacing, and bilateral coordination of gait during the “ON” medication state. Exp. Brain Res. 210 529–538. 10.1007/s00221-011-2551-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Podsiadlo D., Richardson S. (1991). The timed “Up & Go”: a test of basic functional mobility for frail elderly persons. JAmGeriatrSoc. 39 142–148. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pohl M., Rockstroh G., Rückriem S., Mrass G., Mehrholz J. (2003). Immediate effects of speed-dependent treadmill training on gait parameters in early Parkinson’s disease. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 84 1760–1766. 10.1016/s0003-9993(03)00433-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pont-Sunyer C., Hotter A., Gaig C., Seppi K., Compta Y., Katzenschlager R., et al. (2015). The onset of nonmotor symptoms in Parkinson’s disease (the ONSET PD study). Mov. Disord. 30 229-–37. 10.1002/mds.26077 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rahman S., Griffin H. J., Quinn N. P., Jahanshahi M. (2008). Quality of life in Parkinson’s disease: the relative importance of the symptoms. Mov. Disord. 23 1428–1434. 10.1002/mds.21667 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rochester L., Baker K., Hetherington V., Jones D., Willems A.-M., Kwakkel G., et al. (2010). Evidence for motor learning in Parkinson’s disease: Acquisition, automaticity and retention of cued gait performance after training with external rhythmical cues. Brain Res. 1319 103–111. 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.01.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rovini E., Maremmani C., Cavallo F. (2017). How wearable sensors can support Parkinson’s disease diagnosis and treatment: a systematic review. Front. Neurosci. 11:555. 10.3389/fnins.2017.00555 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schaafsma J. D., Giladi N., Balash Y., Bartels A. L., Gurevich T., Hausdorff J. M. (2003). Gait dynamics in Parkinson’s disease: relationship to Parkinsonian features, falls and response to levodopa. J. Neurol. Sci. 212 47–53. 10.1016/s0022-510x(03)00104-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seuthe J., D’Cruz N., Ginis P., Weisser B., Berg D., Deuschl G., et al. (2019). Split-belt treadmill walking in patients with Parkinson’s disease: A systematic review. Gait Posture 69 187–194. 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2019.01.032 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schumway-Cook A., Brauer S., Woollacott M. (2000). Predicting the probability for falls in community-dwelling older adults using the timed up & go test. Phys. Ther. 80 896–903. 10.1093/ptj/80.9.896 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith A. (1982). Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT). Manual (Revised). Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services. [Google Scholar]
- Sofuwa O., Nieuwboer A., Desloovere K., Willems A.-M., Chavret F., Jonkers I. (2005). Quantitative gait analysis in Parkinson’s disease: comparison with a healthy control group. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 86 1007–1013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Rooden S. M., Colas F., Martínez-Martín P., Visser M., Verbaan D., Marinus J., et al. (2011). Clinical subtypes of Parkinson’s disease. Mov. Disord. 26 51–58. 10.1002/mds.23346 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vitório R., Lirani-Silva E., Barbieri F. A., Raile V., Stella F., Gobbi L. T. B. (2013). Influence of visual feedback sampling on obstacle crossing behavior in people with Parkinson’s disease. Gait Posture 38 330–334. 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2012.12.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang X., Chen L., Zhou H., Xu Y., Zhang H., Yang W., et al. (2021). Enriched rehabilitation improves gait disorder and cognitive function in Parkinson’s disease: a randomized clinical trial. Front. Neurosci. 15:733311. 10.3389/fnins.2021.733311 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wojtala J., Heber I. A., Neuser P., Heller J., Kalbe E., Rehberg S. P., et al. (2019). Cognitive decline in Parkinson’s disease: the impact of the motor phenotype on cognition. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 90 171–179. 10.1136/jnnp-2018-319008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang Y.-R., Cheng S.-J., Lee Y.-J., Liu Y.-C., Wang R.-Y. (2019). Cognitive and motor dual task gait training exerted specific training effects on dual task gait performance in individuals with Parkinson’s disease: a randomized controlled pilot study. PLoS One 14:e0218180. 10.1371/journal.pone.0218180 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yogev G., Giladi N., Peretz C., Springer S., Simon E. S., Hausdorff J. M. (2005). Dual tasking, gait rhythmicity, and Parkinson’s disease: which aspects of gait are attention demanding? Eur. J. Neurosci. 22 1248–1256. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04298.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.