Skip to main content
Sleep logoLink to Sleep
. 2022 Mar 7;45(5):zsac053. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsac053

Neurotransmitters of sleep and wakefulness in flatworms

Shauni E T Omond 1,, Matthew W Hale 2, John A Lesku 1,
PMCID: PMC9216492  PMID: 35554581

Abstract

Study Objectives

Sleep is a prominent behavioral and biochemical state observed in all animals studied, including platyhelminth flatworms. Investigations into the biochemical mechanisms associated with sleep—and wakefulness—are important for understanding how these states are regulated and how that regulation changed with the evolution of new types of animals. Unfortunately, beyond a handful of vertebrates, such studies on invertebrates are rare.

Methods

We investigated the effect of seven neurotransmitters, and one pharmacological compound, that modulate either sleep or wakefulness in mammals, on flatworms (Girardia tigrina). Flatworms were exposed via ingestion and diffusion to four neurotransmitters that promote wakefulness in vertebrates (acetylcholine, dopamine, glutamate, histamine), and three that induce sleep (adenosine, GABA, serotonin) along with the H1 histamine receptor antagonist pyrilamine. Compounds were administered over concentrations spanning three to five orders of magnitude. Flatworms were then transferred to fresh water and video recorded for analysis.

Results

Dopamine and histamine decreased the time spent inactive and increased distance traveled, consistent with their wake-promoting effect in vertebrates and fruit flies; pyrilamine increased restfulness and GABA showed a nonsignificant trend towards promoting restfulness in a dose-dependent manner, in agreement with their sleep-inducing effect in vertebrates, fruit flies, and Hydra. Similar to Hydra, acetylcholine, glutamate, and serotonin, but also adenosine, had no apparent effect on flatworm behavior.

Conclusions

These data demonstrate the potential of neurotransmitters to regulate sleep and wakefulness in flatworms and highlight the conserved action of some neurotransmitters across species.

Keywords: dopamine, GABA, Girardia, histamine, nematode, platyhelminth, pyrilamine


Statement of Significance.

Although sleep, as a behavior, is widespread across the animal kingdom, whether the physiology and regulatory machinery associated with sleep and wakefulness, are also conserved remains unclear. We exposed flatworms (n = 504) to seven neurotransmitters, and one pharmacological compound, that regulate either sleep or wakefulness in mammals. The flatworm (phylum Platyhelminthes) are simple animals that lack circulatory, respiratory, and endocrine systems found in other bilaterally symmetric animals, and yet they sleep and possess a bilobal brain. We found that GABA showed a nonsignificant trend to induce sleep in flatworms, and histamine and dopamine promoted wakefulness. We conclude by casting our results into a phylogenetic framework to trace evolutionary similarities, and differences, in neurotransmitter function across distantly related types of animals.

Introduction

Sleep and wakefulness are two behavioral and physiological states exhibited by most, and perhaps all, animals [1]. These two states are characterized by variant responses to external stimuli. For instance, awake animals detect and respond to salient environmental cues readily, whereas sleeping animals show reduced responsiveness to stimulation. Sleep is also characterized by restfulness and homeostatic regulation. These states have been extensively studied in mammals and other vertebrates [2–5], and to a lesser extent invertebrates, including arthropods [6] and mollusks [7], roundworms [8], flatworms [9], jellyfish [10], and Hydra [11].

In vertebrates, sleep and wake are modulated by conserved neurotransmitter systems. Neurotransmitters such as acetylcholine, dopamine, glutamate, and histamine promote wakefulness [12–14]; others, including adenosine, serotonin, and ƴ-aminobutyric acid (GABA), promote sleep [12,14]. How neurotransmitters regulate sleep and wake among invertebrates is comparatively lacking. What is known from these lesser-studied groups comes from a handful of species. The fruit fly (Drosophila melanogaster) shows that many of these neurotransmitters produce the same behavioral output as observed in vertebrates [15,16]. There is less similarity between the behavioral responses of vertebrates and cnidarians, the most basal group of animals with a nervous system. GABA, melatonin (a hormone that promotes sleep in diurnal animals), and pyrilamine (a sleep-promoting H1 histamine receptor antagonist) likewise induce sleep in the cnidarian, Hydra [10,11] Acetylcholine, glutamate, histamine, and serotonin, conversely, have no effect on Hydra behavior [11]. Interestingly, dopamine was found to be sleep-promoting in Hydra, rather than wake-promoting, as observed in all other taxa studied [11,17], demonstrating that at least some neurotransmitters are evolutionarily labile.

The relative lack of comparative data on the behavioral response of invertebrates to neurotransmitters has been the main impediment to understanding how neurotransmitter systems have changed over evolutionary time. Additional data, particularly on unstudied animal phyla, are needed to ascertain which elements of the sleep/wake regulatory machinery are evolutionarily conserved, and which are derived. To this end, we introduce the platyhelminth flatworm.

Although flatworms sleep [9], we know nothing about how neurotransmitters regulate sleep and waking states in platyhelminths. Flatworms are simple invertebrates that lack circulatory, respiratory, and endocrine systems [18]. This simplicity is useful for dissecting the effect of neurotransmitters, which in some taxa, have non-neural biological targets. Several studies on neurotransmitter systems have been conducted on free-living and parasitic flatworms, but on isolated cells or muscle fibers, or without consideration of circadian timing [19–30]. While looking at these effects at a cellular level may be helpful, whole animal behavioral assays are needed to obtain the clearest picture of the effects on the individual [11,31,32]. Here, we add new insight into the effect of seven common neurotransmitters, and one pharmacological compound, on sleep and wakefulness in flatworms.

Methods

Animals

Free-living flatworms (Girardia tigrina) were wild-caught by Southern Biological (Melbourne, Australia) and housed at La Trobe University (n = 504). There they were kept in a temperature-controlled room at 14 ± 1°C and entrained to a 12:12 light:dark cycle with lights-off at 20:00 h. Each flatworm was exposed to only one concentration of only one compound to avoid any lingering effects of a compound (see Supplementary Table S1 for a complete list of sample sizes). Flatworms were initially group-housed (approx. 50 animals per container) in glazed ceramic bowls 15 cm in diameter filled with spring water. Animals were offered hard-boiled egg yolk twice weekly for 120 min; postfeeding they were transferred to a clean bowl with fresh water. Flatworms were fasted prior to experimental recording to ensure they were motivated to eat the neurotransmitter-imbued egg yolk; several fasting durations had been piloted with 14 days yielding good intake. Flatworms, as simple ectothermic invertebrates, have low energy requirements and do not need to eat nearly as frequently as many other animals.

A custom-built activity monitoring system was used to illuminate, and video record, the testing arenas [9]. Briefly, the system consisted of two box frames with a roof and two walls. Within each box, there was a single camera (A602f; Basler AG, Ahrensburg, Germany) placed above a testing area, with dim LEDs (64 ± 2 lux) placed upon the sides of the walls to simulate the attenuation of sunlight that occurs in a pond environment in which the flatworms are found in the wild. The camera had four aluminum arms that extended to the edges of the testing area below; the end of each arm was fitted with infrared LEDs (950 nm). The infrared lights provided constant illumination to the cameras to ensure no difference in image quality between light and dark, and flatworms do not respond to infrared light [33].

Compounds

All compounds were obtained from Sigma-Aldrich Pty Ltd (Castle Hill, Australia) and reconstituted from powder. Neurotransmitters that promote wakefulness in mammals are acetylcholine (Cat. A6625), dopamine (Cat. H8502), glutamate (Cat. G1251), and histamine (Cat. H7125); and compounds that promote sleep are adenosine (Cat. 0.1890), ɣ-aminobutyric acid (GABA, Cat. A2129), pyrilamine (Cat. P5514), and serotonin (Cat. 14927).

Compounds were mixed into a 3,000 μM solution and serially diluted to four concentrations (i.e., 300 μM, 30 μM, 3 μM, and 0.3 μM), which were used to infuse egg yolk with a specific compound (Figure 1). Five hundred μL of compound was added to 950 μL of egg yolk and 50 μL of food coloring; the addition of food coloring allowed us to visually determine which flatworms had eaten and then exclude those that had not (Figure 2). The resulting mixture was pipetted into Eppendorf tubes, vortexed, and heated at 60–70°C in a water bath until solid. Histamine, stored at –20°C, was made into a solution and thawed to make the egg yolk. Solutions and infused egg yolks were made fresh for each trial such that there was only one freeze-thaw cycle. The animals were exposed to the infused egg yolk for 40 minutes prior to the experiment.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Neurotransmitter preparation (top) and experimental design (bottom): the flatworms were fasted for 14 days, and then presented with the drug-infused egg yolk, and immersed in drug-treated water, for 40 minutes prior to the experiment (“soak and yolk”). Animals were transferred to spring water for the duration of the recording (40 minutes); the initial 10 minutes were excluded from analysis owing to acclimation. Created with BioRender.com.

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Photograph of a flatworm that has not ingested the compound-imbued egg yolk (top), and an animal that has consumed the black-dyed, drug-rich, egg yolk and, therefore, exhibits a visible change in color (bottom).

The 3,000 μM stock solution was also diluted to 1,000 μM, 100 μM, 10 μM, 1 μM, and 0.1 μM, to be used as a bath for the animals immediately prior to the experiment (Figure 1). Fifteen mL of these weaker solutions were used as a bath to match the concentration of the egg yolk (mentioned above). This ensured that there was no osmotic gradient and the dosage of the compound remained constant throughout the 40 minutes. High concentrations of serotonin (1,000 µM) and pyrilamine (100 μM, 1,000 µM) produced adverse behavioral reactions and were excluded from analysis; low samples sizes excluded 1,000 µM of histamine and glutamate from analysis.

Recording

Animals were transferred to small wells filled with 2 mL of spring water and behavior was recorded for 40 minutes (Figure 1); the first 10 minutes of the trial was excluded owing to an acute increase in movement whenever the animals are moved to the new environment. The pharmaceutical compounds that promote wakefulness in mammals (acetylcholine, dopamine, histamine) were administered to the flatworms during the first half of the day (between 08:30–11:30 h); purportedly sleep-promoting neurotransmitters (adenosine, GABA, pyrilamine, serotonin) were tested during the first half of the night (between 20:30–23:30 h). Glutamate was tested as sleep-promoting in flatworms, given ostensibly sleep-promoting effects in fruit flies [15,34]. Time frames were so chosen, because flatworms are strongly nocturnal [9], such that wake-promoting compounds were presented at the time-of-day when flatworms would otherwise be asleep. With this hypothesis-testing experimental design, we were able to determine whether each chemical similarly effected flatworm and vertebrate behavior, but only in one-half of the 24-h day.

Analysis

Video recordings were analyzed using EthoVision XT 10 (Noldus Information Technology, Wageningen, The Netherlands) to track the amount (%) and level (distance traveled) of activity. Statistical analyses were conducted using GraphPad Prism (version 9.0 for Windows, GraphPad Software, La Jolla, USA). Based on our previous work with flatworms demonstrating that rest/activity cycles reflect sleep/wake differences [9], we used inactivity as a proxy for sleep and activity as a marker for wake.

We confirmed that data conformed to the assumptions of parametric statistical tests using a Kolmogorov-Smirnov test. When data was non-normal, we tried to achieve normality using a log10 and square root transformation. In the distance traveled data, acetylcholine and pyrilamine were square root transformed, and glutamate was log10 transformed. For the % inactive data, only acetylcholine was normal and was analyzed using a one-way ANOVA with Dunnett’s posthoc comparisons; all other neurotransmitters could not be normalized and were analyzed using a Kruskal-Wallis with Dunn’s posthoc comparisons. For the distance traveled data, we used a one-way ANOVA with Dunnett’s posthoc comparisons for dopamine and glutamate, and a Kruskal-Wallis with Dunn’s posthoc comparisons for acetylcholine, histamine, serotonin, adenosine, GABA, and pyrilamine. Statistical outliers were identified by Grubb’s iterative test. Although data for some compounds was transformed for analysis, only untransformed data is presented in the figures to facilitate interpretation.

Results

Two neurotransmitters caused the flatworms to move in a dose-dependent manner. While acetylcholine did not reduce the amount of inactivity (F5,76 = 0.660, p = .656, Figure 3A), both dopamine (H = 20.880, df = 5, p < .001, Figure 3B) and histamine (H = 11.852, df = 4, p = .018, Figure 3C) decreased inactivity, notably at the highest concentrations tested. When the behavioral response was quantified as distance traveled, again dopamine (F5,41 = 7.009, p < .001, Figure 4B) and histamine (H = 14.550, df = 4, p = .012, Figure 4C), but not acetylcholine (F5,70 = 1.368, p = .247, Figure 4A), showed increased movement. The absence of an effect of acetylcholine could be due to greater variance observed in the control group.

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

Percent of inactivity over 30 minutes per concentration of: (A) acetylcholine, (B) dopamine, and (C) histamine—all expected to decrease inactivity; and (D) glutamate, (E) serotonin, (F) adenosine, (G) GABA, and (H) pyrilamine—all predicted to increase inactivity. Asterisks denote significance (p < .05). The figure was plotted using the Tukey method by which the bottom and top edge of each box reflect first and third quartiles, respectively; the band inside the box is the median; the whiskers reflect minimum and maximum values, datapoints are statistical outliers not identified in Grubb’s iterative test.

Figure 4.

Figure 4.

Distance traveled (cm) over 30 minutes per concentration of: (A) acetylcholine, (B) dopamine, and (C) histamine—all expected to increase distance traveled; and (D) glutamate, (E) serotonin, (F) adenosine, (G) GABA, and (H) pyrilamine—all predicted to decrease distance traveled. Asterisks denote significance (p < .05). Tukey’s method was employed whereby the bottom and top edge of each box reflect first and third quartiles, respectively; the band inside the box is the median; the whiskers reflect minimum and maximum values, datapoints are statistical outliers not identified in Grubb’s iterative test.

Of the five compounds expected to induce restfulness, results were neurotransmitter-specific. Glutamate (H = 4.516, df = 4, p = .341, Figure 3D) and serotonin (H = 3.347, df = 4, p = .482, Figure 3E) had no effect on inactivity. Adenosine showed an overall effect (H = 17.75, df = 5, p = .003, Figure 3F), but no significant pairwise differences. GABA displayed a nonsignificant trend to increase restfulness in a dose-dependent manner (H = 10.40, df = 5, p = .065, Figure 3G). Pyrilamine also increased inactivity (H = 7.708, df = 3, p = .052, Figure 3H). Effects of these four neurotransmitters were less apparent when looking at the distance traveled: glutamate (F5,63 = 0.988, p = .433, Figure 4D), serotonin (H = 1.094, df = 4, p = .895, Figure 4E), and adenosine (H = 9.976, df = 5, p = .076, Figure 4F) had no effect on movement. The dose-dependent mean decline in distance traveled under GABA was nonsignificant (H = 7.581, df = 5, p = .181, Figure 4G). Although pyrilamine had increased inactivity, it did not reduce how far the flatworms moved (F3,42 = 1.577, p = .209, Figure 4H).

Discussion

Evolutionarily conserved neurotransmitter systems regulate sleep and wakefulness in animals (Figure 5). Briefly, dopamine and histamine promote wakefulness in mammals [14] and fruit flies [35,36], and stimulate movement in flatworms. Thus, the increased distance traveled, and decreased inactivity, observed in flatworms exposed to dopamine and histamine likely reflects an increase in wakefulness. Owing to this stimulating effect of histamine, it is not surprising that the histamine antagonist pyrilamine, which is broadly sleep-promoting in other taxa [10,37,38], increased inactivity—and presumably sleep—in flatworms. The only neurotransmitter hinted to induce inactivity in flatworms was GABA. GABA stimulates sleep in all species studied from mammals and other vertebrates to the simplest invertebrates [11,14,15,25,39], suggesting that the mean increase in inactivity we observed in GABA-treated flatworms may reflect an increase in sleep. The great evolutionary longevity of GABA as a somnogenic neurotransmitter suggests that GABA may have been the first neurotransmitter to regulate sleep in animals, whose sleep-promoting effect has been retained over evolutionary time. Acetylcholine did not elicit wakefulness, nor did glutamate, serotonin, or adenosine elicit sleep in flatworms. The congruity of these findings with similar observations in Hydra [11], suggest that these neurotransmitters attained their state-regulating roles only with the appearance of more complex invertebrates (e.g., ecdysozoans and perhaps nonplatyhelminth lophotrochozoans) and vertebrates (see below). Overall, it appears that more complex animals (mice, zebrafish) have a greater complement of sleep- and wake-regulating neurotransmitters than simple (Hydra) and secondarily simplified (flatworms) animals.

Figure 5.

Figure 5.

Summary of the effects of wake- (yellow) and sleep- (blue) promoting neurotransmitters across diverse animals that include mice, zebrafish, fruit flies, flatworms, and Hydra. Colored circles indicate behavioral responses; gray circles denote the absence of a behavioral response; and the absence of a circle itself means that compound has yet to be tested for that taxon and/or that the receptor has not been identified. The blue-within-yellow circle for dopamine-exposed zebrafish reflects the added complexity that receptor type matters for the behavioral output. Along the top of the panel is a phylogenetic tree showing evolutionary relatedness among the five species. Simple (Hydra) and secondarily simplified (flatworms) animals have fewer active neurotransmitters than more complex animals (mice, zebrafish, fruit flies). Only GABA maintains a somnogenic effect across all taxa studied. Created with BioRender.com. For Color, please refer the online images.

Evolutionary position of flatworms

Before casting our results into the broader literature further, it is important to understand the phylogenetic position of flatworms and their evolutionary relationship with other groups of animals. Historically, platyhelminth flatworms were considered to be the earliest branching lineage of bilaterally symmetric animals [40]. This conclusion was based (in part) on the observation that these simple animals lack circulatory, respiratory, and endocrine systems found in other bilaterally symmetric animals [41]. However, more recent studies have challenged this view and place the platyhelminths as the sister to annelids and mollusks in the superphylum Lophotrochozoa [42,43]. Sister to the Lophotrochozoa is another superphylum called Ecdysozoa (or molting animals) that notably includes arthropods and nematodes. Under this phylogeny of invertebrates, the simplified physiology of flatworms is now thought to reflect evolutionary losses rather than the retention of a “primitive” basal condition (Figure 5). This secondary simplification makes the behavioral and biochemical patterns identified here even more salient. For instance, the retention of GABA as the sole neurotransmitter regulating sleep in flatworms, suggests a fundamental functional importance.

Wake-promoting neurotransmitters in flatworms

Dopamine is a (largely) wake-promoting neurotransmitter found across diverse animals, notably mammals and fruit flies [44] (Figure 5). The behavioral response to dopamine is more complex in zebrafish. Using a variety of dopamine D1-receptor agonists, Rihel et al. [17] found evidence for a sedation effect of dopamine in zebrafish, while D1 agonists tested on mammals are arousing. Given these complexities, understanding the evolutionary relatedness between the eight zebrafish dopamine receptors to the five mammalian receptors would provide added insight into how dopamine regulates sleep and/or wakefulness in these vertebrates. Dopamine appears to induce sleep in Hydra [11]; however, which dopamine receptors mediate the effect, and any homology with dopamine receptors in vertebrates, is unknown. Dopamine promotes wakefulness in flatworms. This observation agrees with work demonstrating the presence of dopamine receptors in the central nervous system of flatworms [27,45], whose distribution overlaps with that of serotonergic neurons [20,25]. Taken together, dopamine appears to be wake-promoting in flatworms, fruit flies, variably in zebrafish, and in mammals.

Histamine has been found in the central nervous system of mammals and zebrafish [38] where it plays a role in regulating several body functions, including wakefulness [46]. Histamine is also present in the nervous system of invertebrates, including cockroaches, fruit flies, locusts, and (platyhelminth) cestodes [47–50], and in the photoreceptor cells of the flatworm eyespots (ocelli) [20]. Among invertebrates, studies on the biochemical regulation of sleep and wakefulness have shown that histamine keeps flies and flatworms awake [24,25]. Conversely, the absence of a stimulating effect on Hydra might suggest histamine adopted a wake-promoting role only after the appearance of bilaterally symmetric animals (Figure 5).

Sleep-promoting compounds in flatworms

Pyrilamine is a H1 histamine receptor antagonist. Given the wake-promoting effects of histamine, perhaps it is unsurprising to find widespread sleep-promoting effects of pyrilamine across diverse and distantly related animals. Pyrilamine induces sleep in zebrafish [38] and mammals, at least at some dosages [37,46]. The response of fruit flies to pyrilamine has yet to be studied, but the upside-down jellyfish (Cassiopeia spp.) pulsed with a longer, more variable, inter-pulse interval indicative of sleep when exposed to pyrilamine [10]. Similarly, flatworms showed an increase in restfulness when administered pyrilamine, providing additional evidence that histamine is wake-promoting.

GABA is expressed throughout the mammalian brain and is one of the main neurotransmitters regulating sleep via GABAergic neurons [51]. GABA is thought to be of major importance to flatworms as well owing to high endogenous concentrations and widespread distribution of receptors throughout the nervous system [19]. More broadly, GABA is the only neurotransmitter found to have a consistent, somnogenic effect across all animals studied, including vertebrates [32,37] and diverse and distantly related invertebrates [6,11]. Thus, GABA appears to have held an evolutionarily conserved role in the biochemical regulation of sleep (Figure 5). Interestingly, Keenan et al. [52] investigated the effect of GABA on electrical activity in the nerve cords of another flatworm, Notoplana acticola. Using evoked responses, GABA reversibly suppressed neuronal activity. This GABA-mediated neuronal silence might reflect GABA-induced sleep, given other invertebrates show reduced neuronal activity during sleep, including honey bees (Apis mellifera) [53] and fruit flies [54,55]. Therefore, GABA-mediated neuronal quiescence and the mean increase in restfulness we observed in GABA-treated flatworms, suggests that GABA is involved in regulating sleep in flatworms. Nonetheless, it is important to verify this conclusion using other measures of brain activity, such as local field potentials or calcium imaging, in flatworms [20].

Neurotransmitters that did not yield a behavioral response in flatworms

Acetylcholine was expected to promote wakefulness in flatworms, as it does in mammals [14] and fruit flies [55], but surprisingly did not influence either the distance traveled by flatworms or the amount of inactivity. Acetylcholine has been found within the nervous system of flatworms where it can trigger either muscle contraction or relaxation in a dose-dependent manner in both parasitic and free-living flatworms [24,25,27,29,30]. The absence of a movement-inducing effect here could be artifactual, owing to unexplained increased variance in the control group for this neurotransmitter. This increased vehicle variance cannot be explained by the time-of-day, as dopamine and histamine show much more conserved vehicles; nor by time-of-year, because acetylcholine data was collected at the time as other compounds that showed unremarkable variation in their control. Consequently, there is value in investigating the role of acetylcholine further in the lives of flatworms.

When we designed this study, we thought that glutamate should induce sleep in flatworms, based on an ostensibly sleep-promoting role in fruit flies [15,34]. As such, we exposed flatworms to glutamate during the night when they would otherwise be awake. However, we since read that glutamate is wake-active in vertebrates and fruit flies alike [56]. While glutamate has been shown to trigger muscular contractions in flatworms [20,24,25], there is no evidence of increased or decreased activity during the waking period in the current study. The results found here are similar to those seen in Hydra where no effect of glutamate was found regardless of photoperiod [11].

In vertebrates, there are several adenosine receptor subtypes involved in inducing sleep [12,57–62]. Drosophila pose a contrast here, where the Drosophila homolog of the mammalian A2A receptor (responsible for sleep in mammals), dAdoR, does not regulate sleep-wake behaviors [58]. Not only did flatworms not respond to adenosine, but caffeine (an adenosine antagonist) has no effect on the locomotion of flatworms [63]. There have been no studies looking at the effects of adenosine in cnidarians.

Although serotonin promotes sleep in vertebrates and fruit flies [16,64,65], flatworms and Hydra show no response to serotonin. Despite the absence of an effect in flatworms, serotonin is found throughout the central- and peripheral nervous system in platyhelminths [19,21,22]. Itoh & Igarashi [21] showed that circulating levels of serotonin are lower during the day (when flatworms are predominantly asleep [9]), than during the night. In the current study, there was no evidence that serotonin elicited either an increase or decrease in activity, perhaps concurring with the hypothesis that serotonin is mainly involved in timekeeping and regeneration [21]. While other studies demonstrate that serotonin can increase and decrease locomotion depending on the concentration, no tests have been conducted in the context of circadian rhythms [23–25].

To study sleep, particularly in invertebrates, looking at the whole animal and specific behaviors is important [11,66]. A limitation of this approach relates to the interpretation of an absence of a response. And so, it is equally important to understand the distribution, and types, of receptors, and their homologies across species. Notwithstanding, GABA, dopamine, and histamine have been important neurochemical messengers since (at least) the appearance of the centralized nervous system. The conserved function of these neurotransmitters regulating sleep/wake states shows promise for further advances in sleep research.

Supplementary Material

zsac053_suppl_Supplementary_Table_S1

Acknowledgments

We offer special thanks to Dr Robert Barker and Alicia Dimovski for several stimulating conversations that greatly strengthened this study and the subsequent manuscript.

Disclosure Statement

Financial disclosure: This work was supported by the Australian Research Council (DP170101003) and the Defence Science Institute.

Non-financial disclosure: We declare that this work has no non-financial disclosures or conflicts of interest.

References

  • 1. Ungurean G, et al. Evolution and plasticity of sleep. Curr Opin Physiol. 2020;15:111–119. [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Tobler I. Phylogeny of sleep regulation. In: Dement WC, Rother T, Kryger MH, eds. Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine. Philadelphia: Elsevier Saunders; 2005: 77–90. [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Lesku JA, et al. Avian sleep. Curr Biol. 2014;24:R12–R14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Libourel PA, et al. Partial homologies between sleep states in lizards, mammals, and birds suggest a complex evolution of sleep states in amniotes. PLoS Biol. 2018;16:e2005982. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Rattenborg NC, et al. Sleep in nonmammalian vertebrates. In: Kryger MH, ed. Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine. 7th ed.Philadelphia: Elsevier; 2022. [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Yap MHW, et al. Oscillatory brain activity in spontaneous and induced sleep stages in flies. Nat Commun. 2017;8:1815. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Medeiros SL de S, et al. Cyclic alternation of quiet and active sleep states in the octopus. iScience. 2021;24:102223. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Raizen DM, et al. Lethargus is a Caenorhabditis elegans sleep-like state. Nature. 2008;451:569–572. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Omond S, et al. Inactivity is nycthemeral, endogenously generated, homeostatically regulated, and melatonin modulated in a free-living platyhelminth flatworm. Sleep. 2017;40:zsx124. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Nath RD, et al. The jellyfish Cassiopea exhibits a sleep-like state. Curr Biol. 2017;27:2984–2990. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Kanaya HJ, et al. A sleep-like state in Hydra unravels conserved sleep mechanisms during the evolutionary development of the central nervous system. Sci Adv. 2020;6:eabb9415. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Landolt HP. Sleep homeostasis: a role for adenosine in humans? Biochem Pharmacol. 2008;75:2070–2079. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Siegel JM. The neurotransmitters of sleep. J Clin Psychiatry. 2004;65(Suppl 16):4–7. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Stenberg D. Neuroanatomy and neurochemistry of sleep. Cell Mol Life Sci. 2007;64:1187–1204. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Joiner WJ. Unraveling the evolutionary determinants of sleep. Curr Biol. 2016;26:R1073–R1087. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Yuan Q, et al. A sleep-promoting role for the Drosophila serotonin receptor 1A. Curr Biol. 2006;16:1051–1062. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Rihel J, et al. Zebrafish behavioral profiling links drugs to biological targets and rest/wake regulation. Science. 2010;327:348–351. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Sarnat HB, et al. The brain of the planarian as the ancestor of the human brain. Can J Neurol Sci. 1985;12:296–302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Eriksson KS, et al. Gamma-aminobutyric acid in the nervous system of a planarian. J Comp Neurol. 1994;345:528–536. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Halton DW. Functional morphology of the platyhelminth nervous system. Parasitology. 1996;113:S47–S72. [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Itoh MT, et al. Circadian rhythm of serotonin levels in planarians. NeuroReport. 2000;11:473–476. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Welsh JH, et al. Monoamine-containing neurons in planaria. J Comp Neurol. 1970;138:103–116. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Farrell MS, et al. Behavioral characterization of serotonergic activation in the flatworm planaria. Behav Pharmacol. 2008;19:177–182. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. McVeigh P, et al. Flatworm neurobiology in the postgenomic era. In: Byrne JH, ed. The Oxford Handbook of Invertebrate Neurobiology. New York: Oxford University Press; 2019: 218–244. [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Ribeiro P, et al. Classical transmitters and their receptors in flatworms. Parasitology. 2005;131:S19–S40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Buttarelli FR, et al. Neuropharmacology and behavior in planarians: translations to mammals. Comp Biochem Physiol C Toxicol Pharmacol. 2008;147:399–408. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Buttarelli FR, et al. Acetylcholine/dopamine interaction in planaria. Comp Biochem Physiol C Toxicol Pharmacol. 2000;125:225–231. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Cebrià F, et al. The expression of neural-specific genes reveals the structural and molecular complexity of the planarian central nervous system. Mech Dev. 2002; 116:199–204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Moneypenny CG, et al. Physiological effects of FMRFamide-related peptides and classical transmitters on dispersed muscle fibres of the turbellarian, Procerodes littoralis. Parasitology. 2001;122:447–455. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Blair KL, et al. Physiological and pharmacological properties of muscle cells isolated from the flatworm Bdelloura candida (Tricladia). Parasitology. 1994;109:325–335. [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Gustafsson MKS. Cestode neurotransmitters. Parasitol Today. 1985;1:72–75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Rihel J, et al. Behavioral screening for neuroactive drugs in zebrafish. Dev Neurobiol 2012;72:373–385. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Paskin TR, et al. Planarian phototactic assay reveals differential behavioral responses based on wavelength. PLoS One. 2014;9:e114708. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Guo F, et al. Circadian neuron feedback controls the Drosophila sleep-activity profile. Nature. 2016;536:292–297. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Ueno T, et al. Identification of a dopamine pathway that regulates sleep and arousal in Drosophila. Nat Neurosci. 2012;15:1516–1523. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Oh Y, et al. Histamine-HisCl1 receptor axis regulates wake-promoting signals in Drosophila melanogaster. PLoS One. 2013;8:e68269. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Easton A, et al. Sex differences in mouse behavior following pyrilamine treatment: role of histamine 1 receptors in arousal. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2004;79:563–572. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Sundvik M, et al. The histaminergic system regulates wakefulness and orexin/hypocretin neuron development via histamine receptor H1 in zebrafish. FASEB J. 2011;25:4338–4347. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Zhdanova IV. Sleep and its regulation in zebrafish. Rev Neurosci. 2011;22:27–36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Zrzavý J, et al. Phylogeny of the metazoa based on morphological and 18S ribosomal DNA evidence. Cladistics. 1998;14:249–285. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Reuter M, et al. The flatworm nervous system: pattern and phylogeny. In: Breidbah O, Kutsch W, eds. The Nervous System of Invertebrates: An Evolutionary and Comparative Approach. Basel: Birkhäuser; 1995: 25–59. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Kocot KM. On 20 years of Lophotrochozoa. Org Divers Evol. 2016;16:329–343. [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Laumer CE, et al. Revisiting metazoan phylogeny with genomic sampling of all phyla. Proc R Soc B. 2019;286:20190831. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Kume K, et al. Dopamine is a regulator of arousal in the fruit fly. J Neurosci. 2005;25:7377–7384. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Venturini G, et al. A pharmacological study of dopaminergic receptors in planaria. Neuropharmacology. 1989;28:1377–1382. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Thakkar MM. Histamine in the regulation of wakefulness. Sleep Med Rev. 2011;15:65–74. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Elias MS, et al. Histamine in the insect nervous system: distribution, synthesis and metabolism. J Neurochem. 1983;41:562–568. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Pirvola U, et al. Distribution of histamine in the cockroach brain and visual system: an immunocytochemical and biochemical study. J Comp Neurol. 1988;276:514–526. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Stenesen D, et al. The carcinine transporter CarT is required in Drosophila photoreceptor neurons to sustain histamine recycling. eLife. 2015;4:e10972. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Sukhdeo MVK, et al. Hymenolepis diminuta: behavioral effects of 5-hydroxytryptamine, acetylcholine, histamine and somatostatin. J Parasitol. 1984;70:682–688. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51. Winsky-Sommerer R. Role of GABAA receptors in the physiology and pharmacology of sleep. Eur J Neurosci. 2009;29:1779–1794. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52. Keenan L, et al. Primitive nervous systems: action of aminergic drugs and blocking agents on activity in the ventral nerve cord of the flatworm Notoplana acticola. J Neurobiol. 1979;10:397–407. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Kaiser W, et al. Neuronal correlates of sleep, wakefulness and arousal in a diurnal insect. Nature. 1983;301:707–709. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54. Nitz DA, et al. Electrophysiological correlates of rest and activity in Drosophila melanogaster. Curr Biol. 2002;12:1934–1940. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55. Bushey D, et al. Sleep- and wake-dependent changes in neuronal activity and reactivity demonstrated in fly neurons using in vivo calcium imaging. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2015;112:4785–4790. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56. Dai X, et al. Molecular resolution of a behavioral paradox: sleep and arousal are regulated by distinct acetylcholine receptors in different neuronal types in Drosophila. Sleep. 2021;44:zsab017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57. Zimmerman JE, et al. Glutamate is a wake-active neurotransmitter in Drosophila melanogaster. Sleep. 2017;40:zsw046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58. Huang ZL, et al. Adenosine A2A, but not A1, receptors mediate the arousal effect of caffeine. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8:858–859. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59. Basheer R, et al. Adenosine and sleep-wake regulation. Prog Neurobiol. 2004;73:379–396. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60. Wu MN, et al. The effects of caffeine on sleep in Drosophila require PKA activity, but not the adenosine receptor. J Neurosci. 2009;29:11029–11037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61. Svenningsson P, et al. Biphasic changes in locomotor behavior and in expression of MRNA for NGFI-A and NGFI-B in rat striatum following acute caffeine administration. J Neurosci. 1995;75:7612–7624. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62. Maximino C, et al. Adenosine A1, but not A2, receptor blockade increases anxiety and arousal in zebrafish. Basic Clin Pharmacol Toxicol. 2011;109:203–207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63. Moustakas D, et al. Guarana provides additional stimulation over caffeine alone in the planarian model. PLoS One. 2015;10:e0123310. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64. Lee DA, et al. Neuropeptide VF neurons promote sleep via the serotonergic raphe. eLife. 2020;9:e54491. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65. Oikonomou G, et al. The serotonergic raphe promote sleep in zebrafish and mice. Neuron. 2019;103:686–701. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66. Klein BA, et al. Sleep deprivation impairs precision of waggle dance signaling in honey bees. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2010;107:22705–22709. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

zsac053_suppl_Supplementary_Table_S1

RESOURCES