Abstract
Visual hallucinations are a common feature of Lewy body dementia. Previous studies have shown that visual hallucinations are highly specific in differentiating Lewy body dementia from Alzheimer’s disease dementia and Alzheimer–Lewy body mixed pathology cases. Computational models propose that impairment of visual and attentional networks is aetiologically key to the manifestation of visual hallucinations symptomatology. However, there is still a lack of experimental evidence on functional and structural brain network abnormalities associated with visual hallucinations in Lewy body dementia.
We used EEG source localization and network based statistics to assess differential topographical patterns in Lewy body dementia between 25 participants with visual hallucinations and 17 participants without hallucinations. Diffusion tensor imaging was used to assess structural connectivity between thalamus, basal forebrain and cortical regions belonging to the functionally affected network component in the hallucinating group, as assessed with network based statistics. The number of white matter streamlines within the cortex and between subcortical and cortical regions was compared between hallucinating and not hallucinating groups and correlated with average EEG source connectivity of the affected subnetwork. Moreover, modular organization of the EEG source network was obtained, compared between groups and tested for correlation with structural connectivity.
Network analysis showed that compared to non-hallucinating patients, those with hallucinations feature consistent weakened connectivity within the visual ventral network, and between this network and default mode and ventral attentional networks, but not between or within attentional networks. The occipital lobe was the most functionally disconnected region. Structural analysis yielded significantly affected white matter streamlines connecting the cortical regions to the nucleus basalis of Meynert and the thalamus in hallucinating compared to not hallucinating patients. The number of streamlines in the tract between the basal forebrain and the cortex correlated with cortical functional connectivity in non-hallucinating patients, while a correlation emerged for the white matter streamlines connecting the functionally affected cortical regions in the hallucinating group.
This study proposes, for the first time, differential functional networks between hallucinating and not hallucinating Lewy body dementia patients, and provides empirical evidence for existing models of visual hallucinations. Specifically, the outcome of the present study shows that the hallucinating condition is associated with functional network segregation in Lewy body dementia and supports the involvement of the cholinergic system as proposed in the current literature.
Keywords: graph, Parkinson, EEG, MRI, diffusion
Using EEG and diffusion tensor imaging, Mehraram et al. demonstrate that reduced structural cholinergic innervation as well as lower functional connectivity of the visual system may be key contributors to the manifestation of visual hallucinations in Lewy body dementia.
See Zarkali and Weil (https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awac170) for a scientific commentary on this article.
See Zarkali and Weil (https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awac170) for a scientific commentary on this article.
Introduction
Lewy body dementia (LBD) comprises dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB) and Parkinson’s disease dementia (PDD), which share similar pathology and clinical phenotype.1,2 Complex visual hallucinations (VH) are a clinical feature, occurring in about 80% of clinically diagnosed cases.3–6 Hallucinations are typically of people, animals and inanimate objects,7,8 tend to have short duration and may be triggered by impoverished visual conditions, such as low-lighting.9–11 VH were shown to have high specificity in discriminating LBD from Alzheimer’s disease and Alzheimer–Lewy body mixed pathology cases,12–15 as well as to be associated with poorer outcome and increased institutionalization in the patients.11,16 Hence, this is a topic of high interest in clinical research and an important treatment target.
A consistent picture of the pathophysiology of VH in LBD has not yet been defined. Clinical studies have suggested that generation of VH may be due to a dysfunction of multiple neurotransmitter systems, including the cholinergic system throughout visual and attentional brain networks.17–22 There are a number of existing models specific to Lewy body disorders. It has been suggested that a functional mismatch between the top-down [i.e. prefrontal cortex (PFC)—inferior-temporal cortex (IT) network] and bottom-up visual streams [i.e. occipital cortex (OC)—IT network] is associated with the pathological mechanisms generating VH.3,23,24 Evidence supporting this hypothesis includes PFC grey matter atrophy,25 hypometabolism within occipital and temporal areas,26,27 reduced activation over secondary visual areas28 and lower occipital GABA levels.29 The role of altered visual pathways in the VH phenotype was also proposed by other models, where VH are associated with defective visual and episodic memory and attentional mechanisms.18,30 Attentional impairment is also central in the model by Shine et al.21 who linked VH to a dysfunctional interaction between the dorsal attentional network, default mode network (DMN) and ventral attentional network (VAN); this hypothesis is supported by a functional MRI study showing reduced activation of areas within the dorsal attentional network and lower connectivity between VAN and DMN associated with lower grey matter within the insula.22 Moreover, other studies reported an over-activation of DMN associated with VH.31,32 However, these results contrast with Hepp et al.33 who failed to find any significant functional connectivity difference within the attentional networks of hallucinating and non-hallucinating Parkinson’s disease patients, although widespread reduced connectivity emerged in the first group compared to healthy controls. Outside of Lewy body specific models, but potentially still applicable, ffytche34 attributed a central role to hyper- or hypo- connectivity between visual and other brain regions at the time of manifestation of VH.
EEG is used in clinical research to assess communication dynamics within brain, by inferring pathological alterations of neuronal excitation and inhibition processes.35–37 Studies involving task-based experimental protocols reported a correlation between latency of event-related potential components and visual stimuli in LBD with VH (LBD-VH),38–41 while resting-state research showed lower dominant frequency in the power spectrum, lower beta (β)-band power and right temporal connectivity42,43 and higher delta (δ)-band and alpha (α)-band power44 compared to patients without hallucinations (LBD-NVH) and healthy controls. Previous studies proposed that the cholinergic system must have a primary role in the generation of VH-related functional brain alterations in LBD.17–22 This idea is supported by findings that include the association between integrity of the cholinergic system and EEG α-power,45 a restoration effect by cholinergic medication on EEG measurements returning towards normative values in patients diagnosed with DLB,46–48 the ability of the treatment to suppress VH49 and degeneration of the nucleus basalis of Meynert (NBM) in LBD-VH.50,51
Pathology-related network features can also be inferred by measuring structural brain properties. Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) is reportedly an effective method to assess the structural connectome in healthy and pathological condition,52,53 and in combination with EEG can overcome the limitations of measuring subcortical correlates and provide a comprehensive picture of the disease-related brain alterations.54–56 Recent studies have reported altered white matter properties associated with LBD-VH, including the aforementioned abnormal changes of the NBM,50 correlation between mean diffusivity and VH severity within the right thalamus57 and reduced widespread structural connectivity associated with VH.58,59 So far, no study has investigated the association between functional and structural correlates of LBD-VH using EEG and DTI in a combined fashion.
In the present work, we used EEG to assess cortical network alteration patterns associated with LBD-VH. We focused our analysis on the α-band network, as it has reportedly been associated with lower level visual and attentional processing60–65 and abnormal alteration of α-band rhythms has been reported in LBD against other forms of dementia and healthy condition.66,67 To investigate any involvement of the cholinergic system and the thalamic structure in VH-related EEG abnormalities, we used DTI to assess structural connectivity between the NBM, the thalamus and the cortical regions associated with VH in our EEG analysis, and tested its relationship with extracted EEG features. We included the thalamus due to its regulatory role on EEG oscillations68–71 and its reported involvement in the clinical phenotype of LBD.20,72,73 In line with existing models, we hypothesized that functional and structural connectivity within the visual network would be significantly affected by LBD-VH neuropathology (Fig. 1).
Materials and methods
Participants
Participant recruitment took place at the Newcastle Biomedical Research Centre, Newcastle University. Our sample comprised 25 LBD-VH patients (74 ± 6 years old, 11 DLB and 14 PDD) and 17 LBD-NVH patients (74 ± 6 years old, 7 DLB and 10 PDD). Groups are referred to as VH and NVH in the figures. Diagnoses were performed by two experienced clinicians according to the DLB consensus criteria77,78 and the diagnostic criteria for PDD.79 Demographic information on the participants is reported in Table 1. Global cognition was assessed through the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), and the Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale part III (UPDRS-III) was used to assess motor signs. To exclude patients with too-severe cognitive impairment,80 participants with a MMSE score <12 were excluded from the sample, resulting in the exclusion of one PDD patient with VH with MMSE = 8. Levodopa equivalent daily dose was estimated for patients on dopaminergic medication.81 To exclude any association between absence of VH and cholinergic medication in the LBD-NVH group, clinical histories of those patients were checked, and no record of VH either before or after the medication onset was reported. All patients did not have other neurological or psychiatric conditions apart from dementia and written informed consent was provided by themselves or their caregivers before recruitment. This study was approved by the Northumberland Tyne and Wear NHS Trust and Newcastle ethics committee.
Table 1.
Demographic information | LBD-VH (n = 25) | LBD-NVH (n = 17) | Statistics | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age | 73.52 | ±6.17 | 74.06 | ±6.18 | P = 0.949a |
Male/female | 24/1 | 13/4 | P = 0.055b (d.f. = 1) | ||
DLB/PDD | 11/14 | 7/10 | P = 0.856b (d.f. = 1) | ||
MMSE | 23.04 | ±3.78 | 24.88 | ±4.08 | P = 0.067a |
Duration of dementia, years | 2.1 | ±3.3 | 4.4 | ±5.6 | P = 0.253a |
NPI hall number | 1.56 | ±0.87 | 0 | 0 | – |
NPI hall frequency | 2.08 | ±1.04 | 0 | 0 | – |
NPI hall severity | 1.12 | ±0.33 | 0 | 0 | – |
NPI hall distress | 0.92 | ±1.22 | 0 | 0 | – |
NPI hall total (frequency × severity) | 2.44 | ±1.66 | 0 | 0 | – |
Complex VH (yes/no) | 21/4 | – | – | ||
Duration of VH, years | 2.2 | ±1.3 | – | – | |
UPDRS-III | 24.16 | ±14.27 | 24.59 | ±16.69 | P = 0.644a |
ACheI (yes/no) | 22/3 | 10/6c | P = 0.076b (d.f. = 2) | ||
LEDD | 540 | ±482 | 540 | ±497 | P = 0.500a |
Values in the table are reported as mean ± standard deviation. d.f. = degrees of freedom; LEDD = levodopa equivalent daily dose; UPDRS-III = Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale part III.
Unpaired Mann–Whitney U-test (one-tailed for MMSELBD-VH < MMSELBD-NVH).
χ2 test.
One PDD patient was on memantine.
Experimental protocol
EEG signals were recorded in an eyes-closed resting state with a Waveguard high-density cap (ANT Neuro, Netherlands) with 128 sintered Ag/AgCl electrodes distributed according to the 10-5 system.82 The recording sessions lasted 2 min and took place in a dimly lit room, where participants were asked to sit at rest, keep their eyes closed and remain awake. Recordings were digitized at 1024 Hz while keeping electrode impedance <5 kΩ. The recording system used a ground electrode attached to the right clavicle, and a reference electrode in position Fz.
EEG data preprocessing
Preprocessing of the EEG data was performed with the EEGLAB toolbox v.1483 on MATLAB v.9.2 (The MathWorks Inc., Natick, MA, USA, 2017). A Hamming windowed-sinc finite impulse response filter was applied with a range of 0.5 and 80 Hz, with a 50-Hz notch filter applied to remove power line noise. The obtained time-series were segmented in 2-s time epochs, and visually inspected to remove noisy or disconnected channels (respectively showing systematic bursts or flat activity over time) (number of removed channels: 16 ± 16) as well as epochs featuring sporadic artefactual activity including voltage bursts or isolated peaks (number of removed epochs: 9 ± 8). Independent component analysis (ICA) was performed using the InfoMax algorithm,84 with data dimensionality reduction through principal component analysis. The obtained components were visually inspected, and artefactual patterns were rejected according to established canonical classifications in the literature; these included eye blinks, heartbeat and muscular contractions (number of removed components: 36 ± 12).85,86 The preserved components were projected back to the time-series domain.
MRI: recording and preprocessing
MRI recordings were performed on a 3-T Philips Intera Achieva scanner with magnetization prepared rapid gradient echo sequence, sagittal acquisition, echo time 4.6 ms, repetition time 8.3 ms, inversion time 1250 ms, flip angle = 8°, SENSE factor = 2, in-plane field of view 240 × 240 mm2 with a slice thickness of 1.0 mm, yielding a voxel size of 1.0 × 1.0 × 1.0 mm3.45,87 Preprocessing and segmentation of acquired T1-weighted images were performed using FreeSurfer (v. 5.1, http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/).88,89 The automated processing pipeline included intensity non-uniformity correction, Talairach registration, removal of non-brain tissue (i.e. skull stripping), white matter and subcortical grey matter segmentation, tessellation of the grey–white matter boundary and surface deformation following grey matter–CSF intensity gradients for optimal placing of grey–white matter and grey matter–CSF borders. Modelling of the cortical surface was followed by surface inflation, transformation to spherical atlas and parcellation into 148 regions according to the Destrieux atlas.90 This parcellation was preferred due to its high resolution and inclusion of only cortical regions. Resulting images from each processing step were visually inspected and, where required, manually corrected to ensure accurate segmentation.91,92
DTI recordings were performed with a two-dimensional spin-echo, echo planar imaging diffusion-weighted sequence with 59 slices: repetition time = 6100 ms; echo time = 70 ms; flip angle = 90°; field of view = 270 × 270 mm; pixel size= 2.1 × 2.1 mm and slice thickness = 2.1 mm. Images were diffusion weighted along 64 uniformly distributed directions (diffusion contrast b = 1000 s·mm−2), and six acquisitions did not have any diffusion weight applied (b = 0 s·mm−2).93 DTI recordings were then corrected for eddy current distortion, movement and motion-induced signal dropout using the ‘eddy' package.94,95 Since different nuisance profiles may have potentially affected our analysis, we made sure that there was no difference in head movement between groups by comparing the average volumetric root mean squares of the head movement with a Mann–Whitney U-test, which yielded an insignificant outcome (U = 545, P = 0.858).
Cortical source localization
Cortical source reconstruction from EEG signals was obtained with the standardized low resolution brain electromagnetic tomography (sLORETA)96 implemented in the Brainstorm toolbox for MATLAB.97 Previous studies proved a higher accuracy of this method compared with other existing non-parametric methods in the literature,98 and its suitability for connectivity analysis.99 Due to unavailability of digitized sensor localization, we manually coregistered the EEG sensors distribution over the scalp for each participant’s MRI using the Brainstorm toolbox.100 We used the boundary element method as implemented in OpenMEEG101,102 to build the head models based on the individual T1-weighted MRI data. Noise covariance was set to an identity matrix, reconstruction of cortical sources was performed assuming a normal dipole orientation with respect to cortical surface, and the obtained source-domain time-series were averaged within each cortical region after flipping dipoles with opposite signs to match the main orientation. Validation of the implemented source localization pipeline was made using EEG data recorded during a task-based paradigm from subjects of the same cohort and is reported in the Supplementary material.
Weighted phase lag index and modularity
Connectivity between cortical sources was measured with the weighted phase lag index (WPLI),103 which is a measure of synchronization between any couple of signals across time. Its mathematical formulation is:
(1) |
where X is the cross-spectrum between the signals, Im(X) is its imaginary part, Δϕ is the phase difference between the signals, tk is the time step with k = 1, 2, …, N and sign is the signum function. Connectivity values span between 0 (lack of connectivity) and 1 (full connectivity). The WPLI is insensitive to almost-zero- and zero-lagging synchronization between signals, i.e. Δϕ ≈ / = 0, typically reflecting volume conduction, which is a common issue in EEG recordings.104 Connectivity was computed using functions included in the Fieldtrip toolbox.105 Specifically, EEG source time-series were first transformed into the frequency domain using Windowed Fourier Transform (3–10 cycles adaptive window width, 0.5 frequency step) and WPLI within the α-band (8–13.5 Hz) was computed. WPLI values were averaged across time and frequency bins, resulting in one connectivity matrix per subject. Weighted modularity (Qw) was also computed from each connectivity matrix as implemented in the Brain Connectivity Toolbox (BCT) for MATLAB,106 on the basis of the following formulation:107
(2) |
where mi is the module containing the i-node, wij is the connection weight between i-node and j-node, l is the number of edges, Ki is the i-node strength, and δ is the Kronecker delta function; strength of a i-node is defined as the sum of the weights of the edges connecting to that node.106,108 Higher values of Qw indicate stronger network segregation.
For completeness and exploratory purpose, these measures were also extracted and the subsequent analyses performed for the theta (4.6–7 Hz) and beta (15–20 Hz) frequency bands, as described in the Supplementary material.
EEG-network statistical analysis
For the statistical tests we used MATLAB and the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences v.24. Differential topographical network patterns between LBD-VH and LBD-NVH were obtained using the Network Based Statistics (NBS) toolbox.109 All connections were first tested against the null hypothesis that mean connectivity is equal between groups. A test statistic threshold (tth) was chosen, and those connections whose t-values overcame tth were candidates to form a significant network component. The size of the detected significant component, i.e. sum of individual connection t-values, was computed and data were permuted between groups. This process iterated 5000 times, and the largest component sizes were recorded. The family wise error rate-corrected P-value for each component was obtained by computing the ratio between number of iterations at which the largest component was of the same size of the current component or greater and total number of permutations. The choice of tth is arbitrary, and we chose to push the values towards the strictest thresholding to obtain as local differential topographies as possible. This resulted in performing two F-tests at, respectively, tth = 14 and tth = 15.4, controlling for the MMSE score; network differential components were deemed significant at P < 0.05, and were visualized with the BrainNet Viewer.110 We then computed the average connectivity strength across the NBS connections (WPLINBS) and compared it between groups with a Mann–Whitney U-test (P < 0.05).
We also computed the node strengths (K) within the NBS component and compared them between groups by performing a Wilks’ Lambda multivariate test (P < 0.05) followed by post hoc Mann–Whitney U-tests (P < 0.05, Holm–Bonferroni corrected for number of NBS nodes). K computation was preceded by dividing connection weights within the NBS component by the maximum WPLI value111 to attenuate any connectivity strength bias potentially affecting the network measure.
We compared the modularity between groups with analysis of covariance test corrected for MMSE (P < 0.05), while the modular distribution was obtained for both groups using routines implemented in the BCT. For each EEG network, the optimal community structure was obtained and each node was assigned to a module; an agreement matrix was obtained for each group, where each element indicated the frequency within the group for which every couple of nodes belonged to the same module; the elements of the agreement matrix were divided by the number of networks; a threshold was applied on the obtained matrix preserving probabilities above 64% and eventually, a consensus matrix was computed.112 The obtained modular distribution was visualized with the BrainNet Viewer by identifying each module with a colour. For each group, proportion of within-module NBS connections was computed as a ratio between number of within-module NBS connections and total number of NBS connections.
For topographical visualization, network nodes were defined as mass centroids across head-model vertices within each corresponding region. Coordinates of network nodes in the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) reference space are reported in Supplementary Table 1.
White matter fibres tractography and statistical analysis
To investigate whether any correlation exists between functional and structural network feature alterations associated with VH, DTI data were used to assess white matter connectivity within the functionally affected cortical regions (WMNBS) as well as between the cortex, the thalamus and the basal forebrain. A region of interest corresponding to the NBM (Ch4 cellular group) in the MNI space was generated with the Statistical Parametric Mapping Anatomy Toolbox for MATLAB,113 while the thalamus MNI region of interest was based on the Oxford thalamic connectivity atlas114 included in the FMRIB Software Library.115 To transform the subcortical region of interest to the subject space we used affine and non-linear transformations implemented in the Advanced Normalization Tools software.116 Cortical region of interest were defined as volume masks of the NBS-detected regions and transformed from the subject space to the diffusion space. To this purpose, a linear transformation matrix for each subject was generated with the FLIRT package117 using as origin and target respectively a brain-extracted T1 MRI image and a functional anisotropy map. The subcortical region of interest as defined in the MNI space are shown in Fig. 2.
The tractography pipeline was implemented with the FMRIB’s Diffusion Toolbox.115 We first assessed the local probability distribution of fibre direction at each voxel with automatic detection of number of fibres per voxel.118,119 A probabilistic tractography algorithm118,119 was then used to track white matter streamlines connecting the ROI. Connection probability between voxels was estimated as proportion of connecting fibres, over 5000 sampled fibres per voxel. For each network, the tractography resulted in a structural connectivity matrix of dimension n + 2 (seed masks) × n + 2 (target masks) streamlines, where n was the number of NBS nodes. Since streamline directionality could not be assessed, the connectivity matrices were symmetrized by replacing each (i,j)-element with the average between itself and the (j,i)-element.120
We tested whether connectivity of WMNBS, cortex-NBM and cortex-thalamus, i.e. the number of streamlines, was affected in LBD-VH compared to LBD-NVH (Mann–Whitney U-test, one-tailed, P < 0.05, three tests). We also investigated for any association between structural and functional alterations by performing Spearman rank correlation tests (P < 0.05, one-tailed) between the EEG features, i.e. WPLINBS (right-tailed) and Qw (left-tailed), and, respectively, the average structural connectivity of WMNBS and total number of white matter streamlines connecting the cortical regions to the thalamus and NBM, respectively, for both LBD-VH and LBD-NVH groups (six tests for each EEG metric).
Data availability
The data that support the findings of this study are available from the senior authors J.-P.T. and M.K., on reasonable request by qualified researchers.
Results
Demographic data
Demographic information and clinical score comparisons between groups are reported in Table 1. The participant cohort comprised DLB and PDD patients without VH as well as patients with distributed levels of VH frequency and severity, based on the Neuropsychiatric Inventory (NPI) subscale test for hallucinations; groups were matched for diagnosis, age and sex. MMSE was higher in the LBD-NVH group, with a trend towards significance, hence the NBS test and comparisons of modularity (Qw) between groups were corrected for MMSE score to exclude any confounding effect of cognition given that this is a risk factor for VH.8,9 Most participants (22 LBD-VH and 10 LBD-NVH) were on cholinergic medication and one LBD-NVH patient was on memantine; the percentage of patients on medication was not different across groups. UPDRS-III scores and Levodopa equivalent daily dose were not significantly different between groups.
EEG-network topographical differences
Results of the NBS analysis are shown in Fig. 3 and correspond to the altered subnetwork within the α-band between LBD-VH and LBD-NVH groups. Comparison between groups at tth = 14 yielded one significant component comprising 19 nodes and 18 edges (P = 0.032), which included areas belonging to the OC, left and right IT, the PFC, the middle-posterior cingulate and anterior insula. Comparison between groups of the average connectivity across the edges comprised in the NBS component (WPLINBS) and average strength across the network nodes within the NBS component (Kav) resulted in weaker connectivity in LBD-VH compared to LBD-NVH (Fig. 4). The subnetwork produced at tth = 15.4 comprised a lower number of connections with PFC, insula, cingulate, between OC and right IT and between right IT and the left intraparietal sulcus.
Before testing individual node strength differences between groups, we performed a multivariate test, which yielded a significant result (F(18, 23) = 2.46, P = 0.022). Eight out of 19 node strengths were significantly lower in LBD-VH compared to LBD-NVH as shown in Fig. 4, and were marked in yellow or red in Fig. 3, but only the regions comprising the right inferior-occipital gyrus and sulcus (P = 0.0014) and left middle occipital sulcus and lunatus sulcus (P = 0.0016) survived Holm–Bonferroni correction.
Modular organization of the network and the NBS-detected subnetwork are shown in Fig. 5. We found four modules in LBD-VH group, one including right and central-frontal regions and right temporal pole, one comprising left frontal, parietal and occipital regions and one comprising left frontal and temporal regions; one node in the left prefrontal lobe did not belong to any of the modules. The LBD-NVH network comprised three modules over the left hemisphere, the right prefrontal region and the right temporal-parietal-occipital areas; one further module only comprised three nodes within the left prefrontal region and one in the left temporal lobe. In the LBD-NVH group, 27.78% of NBS-detected edges were connecting nodes belonging to the same module, whilst in LBD-VH that was the case for 16.67%. Connections that connected within-module nodes in one group and between-module nodes in the other, or vice versa, included two edges connecting PFC to OC and cingulate, and two connections between the right temporal cortex and the right and left OC, respectively. Modularity (Qw) was significantly higher in LBD-VH compared with LBD-NVH (F(1, 39) = 6.06, P = 0.018).
As hypothesized, these results were specific to the α-band network, as also described in the Supplementary material.
Structural versus functional connectivity
Comparison of the number of white matter streamlines between groups produced a significant outcome for the NBM-cortex and thalamus-cortex tracts, but not for WMNBS. Specifically, connectivity between the thalamus and NBS-regions was significantly lower in LBD-VH compared to LBD-NVH (U = 457, P = 0.02), and the NBM-cortex streamlines were significantly less in LBD-VH, as shown in Fig. 6A (U = 433, P = 0.004); both tests survived Holm–Bonferroni correction. Significant correlations were found between WMNBS and WPLINBS in LBD-VH (ρ = 0.42, P = 0.019, not surviving Holm–Bonferroni correction), as well as between number of streamlines in the NBM-cortex tract and WPLINBS in the LBD-NVH group (ρ = 0.58, P = 0.008, surviving Holm–Bonferroni correction), but no correlation with EEG connectivity emerged for the thalamus-cortex tract in either group, nor between structural connectivity and Qw. WM-WPLINBS trends are shown in Fig. 6B.
Discussion
We investigated EEG source network abnormalities during the resting state and their correlation with structural features associated with LBD-VH. As hypothesized, we found that brain visual networks showed altered properties in the LBD-VH group compared to LBD-NVH, and we found an association with alteration of the cholinergic projections of the NBM.
The visual ventral network is disconnected in LBD-VH
As detected with NBS, functional connectivity within the α-band network between OC, IT and PFC was reduced in LBD-VH compared to LBD-NVH. The obtained differential topography agrees with the Perception and Attention Deficit model3 and the computational model by Tsukada and colleagues;23 according to these models, VH in LBD are driven by a mismatch between the top-down, i.e. PFC to IT, and bottom-up streams, i.e. OC to IT. Notably, connectivity with multiple areas within the OC was reduced in LBD-VH, in line with reported occipital functional changes in Lewy body disease hallucinators including lower GABA level29 and glucose metabolism.26,27,121 It is possible that OC functional alteration in LBD-VH propagates along the visual ventral network and affects the IT lobes, whose functional strength was also significantly reduced (P < 0.05), although this observation did not survive multiple comparison correction. PFC-IT edges were also weakened, probably reflecting dysfunctions of the top-down visual stream as predicted by the models.3,23 Specifically, PFC regions involved in the affected network comprised the left frontomarginal sulcus and gyrus, which is in the inferior part of the frontal pole, continuous to the orbital gyrus,90 limiting the frontopolar and orbital regions.122 According to existing visual perception models, a sparse version of visual information is projected from OC to PFC,74 through either dorsal visual network123 or the thalamus,124 so that the orbito-frontal cortex can generate a predictive template when this limited visual information stimulus is congruent with information stored in memory.75 This process rapidly takes place before the stimulus goes through the bottom-up system, i.e. from OC towards IT,125 which is where the two streams match and an internal image, or hallucination, is generated.74,126 Lateralization towards the left hemisphere is in line with reported evidence of role of the left PFC for the retrieval of internal imagery.127–129 The involvement of the frontal areas was also reported in another functional MRI study using dynamic casual modelling, where the authors could successfully simulate association between face recognition, activation of frontal areas and effective information flow along the visual ventral network.130 Our findings may be considered as empirical evidence of the PAD and similar models, suggesting that in LBD-VH the information flow stream is also affected in the resting state, possibly reflecting an alteration of trait liability to VH.
With the NBS analysis we also found reduced functional interhemispheric connectivity. Association between this phenomenon and auditory hallucinations was reported for schizophrenia131–133; this is the first time that a similar differential topographical pattern in LBD-VH has been reported. An EEG study showed that coherence between the left and the right occipitotemporal areas early in time is positively correlated with successful object recognition, with the authors suggesting that interhemispheric information flow during visual perception is probably ‘the first gate of an active attention system'.134 The NBS topography resembled an interhemispheric pattern between occipitotemporal areas, suggesting that early functional processes associated with active perception might be impaired in LBD-VH, and this also emerges in the resting state. From a pathological perspective, reduced node strength within the IT might be associated with higher burden of Lewy bodies within this region, which has previously been associated with VH.19
Interaction between attentional and visual networks is weakened in LBD-VH
We found that connectivity between visual areas and the right middle-posterior cingulate cortex, left anterior insula and left temporal superior sulcus, was weakened in LBD-VH. The middle-posterior cingulate is reportedly part of the DMN,135 while the anterior insula and temporal superior sulcus are part of the VAN.76,136,137 However, in the NBS component, no alteration emerged within or between these networks. This result is only partially consistent with the models proposing a role of attentional networks in the VH trait,18,21 which are supported by empirical evidence showing an over-activation of DMN and VAN together with a faulty engagement of dorsal attentional network associated with image misperception.22,31,32 We posit that this apparent contrast in results could stem from the use of a different methodological approach. In the supportive studies, regions of interest were chosen based on a prior hypothesis, and the experimental condition consisted in task-based paradigms; in contrast, we focused on affected connectivity patterns in the resting state, using a data-driven approach, i.e. NBS.109 We speculate that if any affected interaction between and within attentional and default mode networks in LBD-VH exists, it may not be as prominent as in the visual network and would not emerge with our strategy. On the other hand, our results indicate that the faulty interaction between attentional networks and DMN in LBD-VH may be mediated by the visual streams. Our findings are also consistent with previous research showing that grey matter of the anterior insula and hypometabolism within the middle cingulate correlate with the intensity of VH in Alzheimer’s disease dementia and Alzheimer–Lewy body mixed pathology condition.138
The functional network is more segregated in LBD-VH compared to LBD-NVH
Modular network organization has been shown to be crucial for visual perception processing139–141 as also described in computational models.74,75,125,142–144 We provide for the first time evidence of functional network segregation and modular disruption in LBD-VH associated with impairment of long-range connections. Although the latter has been reported previously when comparing DLB with Alzheimer’s disease dementia,2,67 no direct association with VH has been reported until now. On the basis of the NBS topographies and the persistence of affected connections to PFC, even when setting a stricter NBS-tth, we theorized that the affected top-down projections may be among the drivers of the network segregation in LBD-VH. Overall, our analysis showed that visual-related functional subnetwork properties are potentially informative for the correct diagnosis of LBD against Alzheimer’s disease dementia and mixed pathology cases.
Alteration of cholinergic pathways is associated with EEG-network abnormalities in LBD-VH
We found higher degeneration of white matter streamlines between the NBM and the NBS-detected regions in LBD-VH compared to LBD-NVH. A similar result was also reported in a study with Parkinson’s disease patients, where higher mean diffusivity was detected in hallucinating compared to non-hallucinating patients within the tracts connecting the NBM to occipital and parietal areas.50 The authors suggested that such projections might have a key role in the aetiology of VH in Parkinson’s disease. Interestingly, we also found a correlation between cortical functional connectivity and number of NBM-cortex white matter fibres in LBD-NVH group, but not in LBD-VH. Disrupted structure-function coupling has been reported in other conditions including bipolar disorder145 and migraine,146 but not in LBD-VH. Our results concur with the idea of a structure-function coupling as a physiological phenomenon,54,147–149 which is probably altered in pathological condition. Although both participant groups included LBD patients, the ones who did not develop VH might still present some intact physiological features, specifically associated with the cholinergic system. These might be more severely affected in LBD-VH, leading to the observed disrupted structural-functional correlation. In fact, we speculate that hallucinating and non-hallucinating condition are different phenotypes of the same disease, the first being driven by strategic cholinergic loss and the latter being more associated with a global cortical pathologic change.16,150
Structural connectivity between the thalamus and cortical regions of interest was also significantly affected in LBD-VH compared to LBD-NVH. This outcome concurs with a previous study where correlation between mean diffusivity within the right thalamic subregion and NPI hallucination score was reported in DLB.57 Degeneration of white matter tracts connecting the thalamus to the OC associated with VH and visual dysfunction were also reported in more recent studies.59,151 However, streamlines’ count within this tract did not correlate with any EEG measure; in contrast there was a positive correlation between WMNBS in the LBD-VH group and WPLINBS. We speculate that this latter finding may reflect the fact that VH-related pathological mechanisms may equally affect functional and structural network properties in the cortex in LBD patients.
Limitations
Our work presents some limitations.
Although the gender distribution was similar, we found a trend towards significance in the between-group comparison. We could not correct the results for gender, as the number of female participants was too low. Since there is no evidence in literature of gender effect on VH-related pathological and functional processes, it was assumed that results were not significantly affected by any gender imbalance.
Most patients in this study were taking cholinesterase inhibitors, which may have partially restored the EEG-network properties towards normative values in the LBD-VH group,47,48 making group differences less distinct. Nevertheless, we found significant differences between groups. With an exploratory purpose, we additionally performed the NBS also including the medication state as a nuisance covariate; as a result, we obtained a network component resembling the one detected within our main analysis, although with a lower number of connections, possibly due to the low statistical power originating from the low number of patients not on medication. This additional analysis is reported in Supplementary material.
A methodological limitation lies in the fact that both WPLI and white matter streamline count could not provide any information on directionality of the connections. Future studies will be needed to assess whether any VH-related alteration exists in the direction of information flow between brain regions.
Concerning the EEG-network analysis, NBS threshold remains an arbitrary choice.109 The purpose of our research was to investigate for localized differential network properties; hence we chose conservative tth values. However, it is possible that sensitivity of NBS may vary depending on the choice of lower thresholds, producing either larger network components or no significance at all.
A potential limitation of our analysis lies in the choice of the MMSE as a cognition-related behavioural score. This score reportedly shows low sensitivity in characterizing the cognitive phenotype of patient groups, although this is mainly the case in mild cognitive impairment.152 Our choice was aimed to include a measure of global cognition, as focusing on cognitive subdomains was beyond the scope of our work. To address any potential concern, we also performed the NBS analysis correcting for the Cambridge Cognition Examination subdomain scores, and we detected a subnetwork that resembled the one we reported in the Results section, confirming the robustness of our results. This additional analysis is reported in the Supplementary material.
Furthermore, we choose not to investigate subtypes of VH, grouping together patients with simple and complex hallucinations, with the first being only four. Surprisingly, WPLINBS within the LBD-VH group did not show any outlier, as shown in Fig. 4. However, previous work has suggested that simple and complex hallucinations may be associated with different pathological mechanisms and excluded simple hallucinations from the LBD-VH group.29,31,153,154 Due to our limited sample size, we could not investigate the extent to which simple hallucinations influence functional connectivity as compared to complex hallucinations.
Although we included the thalamus in our white matter fibre tracking, we focused on the cholinergic projections towards the cerebral cortex. The majority of cholinergic innervations towards the thalamus reportedly originate from the pedunculopontine tegmentum and latero-dorsal tegmentum;155,156 their possible association with visual-related alterations could be investigated in future studies.
Conclusion
In the present study, we showed that LBD features specific EEG functional network properties depending on whether patients have developed VH or not. For the first time, we demonstrate a differential topography between LBD-VH and LBD-NVH without any prior hypothesis, which agreed with models of VH. Visual processing streams and, specifically, the OC, were affected in LBD-VH compared to LBD-NVH, although an affected interaction between the ventral visual network and DMN/VAN also emerged. Modular organization of EEG source network within both top-down and bottom-up visual streams is more disrupted in LBD-VH. Our study also provides evidence of an association between disruption of the cholinergic system and functional connectivity abnormalities in LBD-VH. The results of our work validate EEG and the use of multiple recording modalities as effective tools to investigate pathological insights and treatment strategies associated with LBD and, specifically, VH.
Supplementary Material
Abbreviations
- DLB
dementia with Lewy bodies
- DMN
default mode network
- DTI
diffusion tensor imaging
- IT
inferior-temporal cortex
- K
node strength
- LBD
Lewy body dementia
- LBD-VH/NVH
Lewy body dementia with/without visual hallucinations
- MMSE
Mini-Mental State Examination
- NBM
nucleus basalis of Meynert
- NBS
network-based statistics
- NPI
Neuropsychiatric Inventory
- OC
occipital cortex
- PDD
Parkinson’s disease dementia
- PFC
prefrontal cortex
- VAN
ventral attentional network
- VH
visual hallucinations
- WPLI
weighted phase lag index
Contributor Information
Ramtin Mehraram, Experimental Oto-rhino-laryngology (ExpORL) Research Group, Department of Neurosciences, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium; NIHR Newcastle Biomedical Research Centre, Campus for Ageing and Vitality, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK; Translational and Clinical Research Institute, Newcastle University, Campus for Ageing and Vitality, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK; Interdisciplinary Computing and Complex BioSystems (ICOS) research group, School of Computing, Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK.
Luis R Peraza, IXICO Plc, London, UK.
Nicholas R E Murphy, Baylor College of Medicine, Menninger Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Houston, TX 77030, USA; The Menninger Clinic, Houston, TX 77035, USA; Michael E. DeBakey VA Medical Center, 2002 Holcombe Boulevard, Houston, TX 77030, USA.
Ruth A Cromarty, Translational and Clinical Research Institute, Newcastle University, Campus for Ageing and Vitality, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK.
Sara Graziadio, NIHR Newcastle in vitro Diagnostics Cooperative, Newcastle upon Tyne Hospitals NHS Foundation Trust, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK.
John T O’Brien, Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge School of Medicine, Cambridge, UK.
Alison Killen, Translational and Clinical Research Institute, Newcastle University, Campus for Ageing and Vitality, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK.
Sean J Colloby, Translational and Clinical Research Institute, Newcastle University, Campus for Ageing and Vitality, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK.
Michael Firbank, Translational and Clinical Research Institute, Newcastle University, Campus for Ageing and Vitality, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK.
Li Su, Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge School of Medicine, Cambridge, UK; Department of Neuroscience, The University of Sheffield, Sheffield, UK.
Daniel Collerton, Translational and Clinical Research Institute, Newcastle University, Campus for Ageing and Vitality, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK.
John Paul Taylor, Translational and Clinical Research Institute, Newcastle University, Campus for Ageing and Vitality, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK.
Marcus Kaiser, Interdisciplinary Computing and Complex BioSystems (ICOS) research group, School of Computing, Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK; NIHR Nottingham Biomedical Research Centre, School of Medicine, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, UK; Sir Peter Mansfield Imaging Centre, School of Medicine, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, UK; Department of Functional Neurosurgery, Ruijin Hospital, Shanghai Jiao Tong University School of Medicine, Shanghai, China.
Funding
The NIHR Newcastle Biomedical Research Centre (BRC) is a partnership between Newcastle upon Tyne Hospitals NHS Foundation Trust and Newcastle University, funded by the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR). The research was co-funded by the Medical Research Council (MR/T004347/1, MR/T004347/2) and the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Newcastle Biomedical Research Centre (BRC). J.O.B. is supported by the Cambridge NIHR Biomedical Research Centre and the Cambridge Centre for Parkinson’s Plus. L.S. is funded by Alzheimer’s Research UK (ARUK-SRF2017B-1). The views expressed are those of the author(s) and not necessarily those of the NIHR or the Department of Health and Social Care. The study—participant recruitment and data collection—was funded by an intermediate clinical Wellcome Trust Fellowship (WT088441MA) to J.-P.T.
Competing interests
The authors report no competing interests.
Supplementary material
Supplementary material is available at Brain online.
References
- 1. McKeith I. Dementia with Lewy bodies and Parkinson’s disease with dementia: Where two worlds collide. Pract Neurol. 2007;7:374–382. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Mehraram R, Kaiser M, Cromarty R, et al. . Weighted network measures reveal differences between dementia types: An EEG study. Hum Brain Mapp. 2019;41:1573–1590. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Collerton D, Perry E, McKeith I. Why people see things that are not there: A novel perception and attention deficit model for recurrent complex visual hallucinations. Behav Brain Sci. 2005;28:737–757. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. McKeith I. Dementia with Lewy bodies. In: Handbook of clinical neurology. Elsevier; 2007:531–548. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Mosimann UP, Collerton D, Dudley R, et al. . A semi-structured interview to assess visual hallucinations in older people. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2008;23:712–718. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Weil RS, Schrag AE, Warren JD, Crutch SJ, Lees AJ, Morris HR. Visual dysfunction in Parkinson’s disease. Brain. 2016;139:2827–2843. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Aarsland D, Ballard C, Larsen JP, McKeith I. A comparative study of psychiatric symptoms in dementia with Lewy bodies and Parkinson’s disease with and without dementia. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2001;16:528–536. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Burghaus L, Eggers C, Timmermann L, Fink GR, Diederich NJ. Hallucinations in neurodegenerative diseases. CNS Neurosci Ther. 2012;18:149–159. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Fénelon G, Mahieux F, Huon R, Ziégler M. Hallucinations in Parkinson’s disease: prevalence, phenomenology and risk factors. Brain. 2000;123:733–745. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Barnes J, David AS. Visual hallucinations in Parkinson’s disease: A review and phenomenological survey. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2001;70:727–733. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Holroyd S, Currie L, Wooten GF. Prospective study of hallucinations and delusions in Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2001;70:734–738. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Tiraboschi P, Salmon DP, Hansen LA, Hofstetter RC, Thal LJ, Corey-Bloom J. What best differentiates Lewy body from Alzheimer’s disease in early-stage dementia? Brain. 2006;129:729–735. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Toledo JB, Cairns NJ, Da X, et al. . Clinical and multimodal biomarker correlates of ADNI neuropathological findings. Acta Neuropathol Commun. 2013;1:65. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Yoshizawa H, Vonsattel JPG, Honig LS. Early neuropsychological discriminants for Lewy body disease: An autopsy series. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2013;84:1326–1330. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Jicha GA, Schmitt FA, Abner E, et al. . Prodromal clinical manifestations of neuropathologically confirmed Lewy body disease. Neurobiol Aging. 2010;31:1805–1813. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Ibarretxe-Bilbao N, Ramirez-Ruiz B, Junque C, et al. . Differential progression of brain atrophy in Parkinson’s disease with and without visual hallucinations. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2010;81:650–657. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Ballard C, Piggott M, Johnson M, et al. . Delusions associated with elevated muscarinic binding in dementia with Lewy bodies. Ann Neurol. 2000;48:868–876. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Diederich NJ, Goetz CG, Stebbins GT. Repeated visual hallucinations in Parkinson’s disease as disturbed external/internal perceptions: Focused review and a new integrative model. Mov Disord. 2005;20:130–140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Harding AJ, Broe GA, Halliday GM. Visual hallucinations in Lewy body disease relate to Lewy bodies in the temporal lobe. Brain. 2002;125:391–403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Onofrj M, Espay AJ, Bonanni L, Delli Pizzi S, Sensi SL. Hallucinations, somatic-functional disorders of PD-DLB as expressions of thalamic dysfunction. Mov Disord. 2019;34:1100–1111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Shine JM, Halliday GM, Naismith SL, Lewis SJ. Visual misperceptions and hallucinations in Parkinson’s disease: Dysfunction of attentional control networks? Mov Disord. 2011;26:2154–2159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Shine JM, Halliday GM, Gilat M, et al. . The role of dysfunctional attentional control networks in visual misperceptions in Parkinson’s disease. Hum Brain Mapp. 2014;35:2206–2219. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Tsukada H, Fujii H, Aihara K, Tsuda I. Computational model of visual hallucination in dementia with Lewy bodies. Neural Networks. 2015;62:73–82. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Benrimoh D, Parr T, Adams RA, Friston K. Hallucinations both in and out of context: An active inference account. PLoS ONE. 2019;14:e0212379. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Pezzoli S, Cagnin A, Antonini A, Venneri A. Frontal and subcortical contribution to visual hallucinations in dementia with Lewy bodies and Parkinson’s disease. Postgrad Med. 2019;131:509–522. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Gasca-Salas C, Clavero P, García-García D, Obeso JA, Rodríguez-Oroz MC. Significance of visual hallucinations and cerebral hypometabolism in the risk of dementia in Parkinson’s disease patients with mild cognitive impairment. Hum Brain Mapp. 2016;37:968–977. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Pasquier J, Michel BF, Brenot-Rossi I, Hassan-Sebbag N, Sauvan R, Gastaut JL. Value of (99 m)Tc-ECD SPET for the diagnosis of dementia with Lewy bodies. Eur J Nucl Med Mol Imaging. 2002;29:1342–1348. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Taylor J-P, Firbank MJ, He J, et al. . Visual cortex in dementia with Lewy bodies: Magnetic resonance imaging study. Br J Psychiatry. 2012;200:491–498. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Firbank MJ, Parikh J, Murphy N, et al. . Reduced occipital GABA in Parkinson disease with visual hallucinations. Neurology. 2018;91:e675–e685. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Barnes J, Boubert L, Harris J, Lee A, David AS. Reality monitoring and visual hallucinations in Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia. 2003;41:565–574. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Franciotti R, Delli Pizzi S, Perfetti B, et al. . Default mode network links to visual hallucinations: A comparison between Parkinson’s disease and multiple system atrophy. Mov Disord. 2015;30:1237–1247. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Yao N, Shek-Kwan Chang R, Cheung C, et al. . The default mode network is disrupted in Parkinson’s disease with visual hallucinations. Hum Brain Mapp. 2014;35:5658–5666. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Hepp DH, Foncke EMJ, Dubbelink KTEO, Berg WDJVD, Berendse HW, Schoonheim MM. Loss of functional connectivity in patients with Parkinson disease and visual hallucinations. Radiology. 2017;285:896–903. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Ffytche DH. The hodology of hallucinations. Cortex. 2008;44:1067–1083. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. de Haan W, Pijnenburg YA, Strijers RL, et al. . Functional neural network analysis in frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease using EEG and graph theory. BMC Neuroscience. 2009;10:101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Acharya UR, Sree SV, Swapna G, Martis RJ, Suri JS. Automated EEG analysis of epilepsy: A review. Knowl Based Syst. 2013;45:147–165. [Google Scholar]
- 37. Furlong S, Cohen JR, Hopfinger J, Snyder J, Robertson MM, Sheridan MA. Resting-state EEG connectivity in young children with ADHD. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2021;50:1–17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Kurita A, Murakami M, Takagi S, Matsushima M, Suzuki M. Visual hallucinations and altered visual information processing in Parkinson disease and dementia with Lewy bodies. Mov Disord. 2010;25:167–71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Kurita A, Nakamura M, Suzuki M, Mochio S, Inoue K. Visual and auditory event-related potential comparisons between Parkinson’s disease with dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Int Congr Series. 2005;1278:57–60. [Google Scholar]
- 40. Matsui H, Udaka F, Tamura A, et al. . The relation between visual hallucinations and visual evoked potential in Parkinson disease. Clin Neuropharmacol. 2005;28:79–82. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Chang Y-P, Yang Y-H, Lai C-L, Liou L-M. Event-related potentials in Parkinson’s disease patients with visual hallucination. Parkinsons Dis. 2016;2016:1863508. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Dauwan M, Linszen MMJ, Lemstra AW, Scheltens P, Stam CJ, Sommer IE. EEG-based neurophysiological indicators of hallucinations in Alzheimer’s disease: Comparison with dementia with Lewy bodies. Neurobiol Aging. 2018;67:75–83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Dauwan M, Hoff JI, Vriens EM, Hillebrand A, Stam CJ, Sommer IE. Aberrant resting-state oscillatory brain activity in Parkinson’s disease patients with visual hallucinations: An MEG source-space study. NeuroImage Clin. 2019;22:101752. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Babiloni C, Pascarelli MT, Lizio R, et al. . Abnormal cortical neural synchronization mechanisms in quiet wakefulness are related to motor deficits, cognitive symptoms, and visual hallucinations in Parkinson’s disease patients: An electroencephalographic study. Neurobiol Aging. 2020;91:88–111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Schumacher J, Thomas AJ, Peraza LR, et al. . EEG alpha reactivity and cholinergic system integrity in Lewy body dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Res Ther. 2020;12:46. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Kai T, Asai Y, Sakuma K, Koeda T, Nakashima K. Quantitative electroencephalogram analysis in dementia with Lewy bodies and Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurol Sci. 2005;237:89–95. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47. Agnoli A, Martucci N, Manna V, Conti L, Fioravanti M. Effect of cholinergic and anticholinergic drugs on short-term memory in Alzheimer’s dementia: A neuropsychological and computerized electroencephalographic study. Clin Neuropharmacol. 1983;6:311–323. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48. Balkan S, Yaras N, Mihci E, Dora B, Agar A, Yargiçoglu P. Effect of donepezil on EEG spectral analysis in Alzheimer’s disease. Acta Neurol Belg. 2003;103:164–169. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Perry EK, Perry RH. Acetylcholine and hallucinations: Disease-related compared to drug-induced alterations in human consciousness. Brain Cogn. 1995;28:240–258. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50. Hepp DH, Foncke EMJ, Berendse HW, et al. . Damaged fiber tracts of the nucleus basalis of Meynert in Parkinson’s disease patients with visual hallucinations. Sci Rep. 2017;7:10112–10112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51. Sakai K, Ikeda T, Ishida C, Komai K, Yamada M. Delusions and visual hallucinations in a patient with Parkinson’s disease with dementia showing pronounced Lewy body pathology in the nucleus basalis of Meynert. Neuropathology. 2019;39:319–323. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52. Alexander AL, Lee JE, Lazar M, Field AS. Diffusion tensor imaging of the brain. Neurotherapeutics. 2007;4:316–329. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53. Johansen-Berg H, Behrens TE. Diffusion MRI: From quantitative measurement to in vivo neuroanatomy: Academic Press; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- 54. Chu CJ, Tanaka N, Diaz J, et al. . EEG functional connectivity is partially predicted by underlying white matter connectivity. NeuroImage. 2015;108:23–33. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55. Duru AD, Duru DG, Yumerhodzha S, Bebek N. Analysis of correlation between white matter changes and functional responses in thalamic stroke: A DTI & EEG study. Brain Imaging Behav. 2016;10:424–436. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56. Nunez PL, Srinivasan R, Fields RD. EEG functional connectivity, axon delays and white matter disease. Clin Neurophysiol. 2015;126:110–120. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57. Pizzi S D, Maruotti V, Taylor J-P, et al. . Relevance of subcortical visual pathways disruption to visual symptoms in dementia with Lewy bodies. Cortex. 2014;59:12–21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58. Zarkali A, McColgan P, Ryten M, et al. . Differences in network controllability and regional gene expression underlie hallucinations in Parkinson’s disease. Brain. 2020;143:3435–3448. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59. Zarkali A, McColgan P, Leyland L-A, Lees AJ, Rees G, Weil RS. Fiber-specific white matter reductions in Parkinson hallucinations and visual dysfunction. Neurology. 2020;94:e1525–e1538. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60. Klimesch W, Doppelmayr M, Russegger H, Pachinger T, Schwaiger J. Induced alpha band power changes in the human EEG and attention. Neurosci Lett. 1998;244:73–76. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61. Mulholland T, Runnals S. Increased occurrence of EEG alpha during increased attention. J Psychol. 1962;54:317–330. [Google Scholar]
- 62. Benedek M, Schickel RJ, Jauk E, Fink A, Neubauer AC. Alpha power increases in right parietal cortex reflects focused internal attention. Neuropsychologia. 2014;56:393–400. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63. Wan L, Huang H, Schwab N, et al. . From eyes-closed to eyes-open: role of cholinergic projections in EC-to-EO alpha reactivity revealed by combining EEG and MRI. Hum Brain Mapp. 2019;40:566–577. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64. Chapman RM, Armington JC, Bragdon HR. A quantitative survey of kappa and alpha EEG activity. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1962;14:858–868. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65. Jensen O, Mazaheri A. Shaping functional architecture by oscillatory alpha activity: Gating by inhibition. Front Hum Neurosci. 2010;4:186. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66. Briel RC, McKeith IG, Barker WA, et al. . EEG findings in dementia with Lewy bodies and Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1999;66:401–403. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67. Peraza LR, Cromarty R, Kobeleva X, et al. . Electroencephalographic derived network differences in Lewy body dementia compared to Alzheimer’s disease patients. Sci Rep. 2018;8:4637. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68. Lopes da Silva FH, van Lierop THMT, Schrijer CF, Storm van Leeuwen W. Organization of thalamic and cortical alpha rhythms: Spectra and coherences. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1973;35:627–639. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69. Schürmann M, Demiralp T, Başar E, Başar Eroglu C. Electroencephalogram alpha (8–15 Hz) responses to visual stimuli in cat cortex, thalamus, and hippocampus: A distributed alpha network? Neurosci Lett. 2000;292:175–178. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70. Roberts J, Robinson P. Modeling distributed axonal delays in mean-field brain dynamics. Phys Rev E. 2008;78:051901. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71. Robinson P, Rennie C, Wright J, Bahramali H, Gordon E, Rowe D. Prediction of electroencephalographic spectra from neurophysiology. Phys Rev E. 2001;63:021903. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72. Delli Pizzi S, Franciotti R, Taylor JP, et al. . Thalamic involvement in fluctuating cognition in dementia with Lewy bodies: Magnetic resonance evidences. Cereb Cortex. 2015;25:3682–3689. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73. Schumacher J, Peraza LR, Firbank M, et al. . Dysfunctional brain dynamics and their origin in Lewy body dementia. Brain. 2019;142:1767–1782. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74. Bar M. A cortical mechanism for triggering top-down facilitation in visual object recognition. J Cogn Neurosci. 2003;15:600–609. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75. Chaumon M, Kveraga K, Barrett LF, Bar M. Visual predictions in the orbitofrontal cortex rely on associative content. Cereb Cortex. 2014;24:2899–2907. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76. Vossel S, Geng JJ, Fink GR. Dorsal and ventral attention systems: Distinct neural circuits but collaborative roles. Neuroscientist. 2014;20:150–159. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77. McKeith IG, Boeve BF, Dickson DW, et al. . Diagnosis and management of dementia with Lewy bodies: Fourth consensus report of the DLB Consortium. Neurology. 2017;89:88–100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78. McKeith IG, Dickson DW, Lowe J, et al. . Diagnosis and management of dementia with Lewy bodies: Third report of the DLB Consortium. Neurology. 2005;65:1863–1872. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79. Emre M, Aarsland D, Brown R, et al. . Clinical diagnostic criteria for dementia associated with Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord. 2007;22:1689–1707. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12:189–198. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81. Tomlinson CL, Stowe R, Patel S, Rick C, Gray R, Clarke CE. Systematic review of levodopa dose equivalency reporting in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord. 2010;25:2649–2653. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82. Oostenveld R, Praamstra P. The five percent electrode system for high-resolution EEG and ERP measurements. Clin Neurophysiol. 2001;112:713–719. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83. Delorme A, Makeig S. EEGLAB: An open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J Neurosci Methods. 2004;134:9–21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84. Bell AJ, Sejnowski TJ. An information-maximization approach to blind separation and blind deconvolution. Neural Comput. 1995;7:1129–1159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85. McMenamin BW, Shackman AJ, Maxwell JS, et al. . Validation of ICA-based myogenic artifact correction for scalp and source-localized EEG. NeuroImage. 2010;49:2416–2432. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86. Delorme A, Sejnowski T, Makeig S. Enhanced detection of artifacts in EEG data using higher-order statistics and independent component analysis. NeuroImage. 2007;34:1443–1449. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87. Peraza LR, Kaiser M, Firbank M, et al. . fMRI resting state networks and their association with cognitive fluctuations in dementia with Lewy bodies. NeuroImage Clin. 2014;4:558–565. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88. Dale AM, Fischl B, Sereno MI. Cortical surface-based analysis: I. segmentation and surface reconstruction. NeuroImage. 1999;9:179–194. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89. Fischl B, Dale AM. Measuring the thickness of the human cerebral cortex from magnetic resonance images. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2000;97:11050–11055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90. Destrieux C, Fischl B, Dale A, Halgren E. Automatic parcellation of human cortical gyri and sulci using standard anatomical nomenclature. NeuroImage. 2010;53:1–15. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91. Blanc F, Colloby SJ, Philippi N, et al. . Cortical thickness in dementia with Lewy bodies and Alzheimer’s disease: A comparison of prodromal and dementia stages. PLoS ONE. 2015;10:e0127396. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92. Colloby SJ, Firbank MJ, Vasudev A, Parry SW, Thomas AJ, O’Brien JT. Cortical thickness and VBM-DARTEL in late-life depression. J Affect Disord. 2011;133:158–164. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93. Firbank MJ, Blamire AM, Krishnan MS, et al. . Diffusion tensor imaging in dementia with Lewy bodies and Alzheimer’s disease. Psychiatry Res. 2007;155:135–145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94. Andersson JLR, Sotiropoulos SN. An integrated approach to correction for off-resonance effects and subject movement in diffusion MR imaging. NeuroImage. 2016;125:1063–1078. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95. Andersson JLR, Graham MS, Zsoldos E, Sotiropoulos SN. Incorporating outlier detection and replacement into a non-parametric framework for movement and distortion correction of diffusion MR images. NeuroImage. 2016;141:556–572. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96. Pascual-Marqui RD. Standardized low-resolution brain electromagnetic tomography (sLORETA): Technical details. Methods Find Exp Clin Pharmacol. 2002;24:5–12. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97. Tadel F, Baillet S, Mosher JC, Pantazis D, Leahy RM. Brainstorm: A user-friendly application for MEG/EEG analysis. Comput Intell Neurosci. 2011;2011:879716. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98. Grech R, Cassar T, Muscat J, et al. . Review on solving the inverse problem in EEG source analysis. J Neuroeng Rehabil. 2008;5:25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99. Hincapié A-S, Kujala J, Mattout J, et al. . The impact of MEG source reconstruction method on source-space connectivity estimation: A comparison between minimum-norm solution and beamforming. NeuroImage. 2017;156:29–42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100. Stropahl M, Bauer A-KR, Debener S, Bleichner MG. Source-modeling auditory processes of EEG data using EEGLAB and brainstorm. protocols. Front Neurosci. 2018;12:309. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101. Gramfort A, Papadopoulo T, Olivi E, Clerc M. OpenMEEG: Opensource software for quasistatic bioelectromagnetics. Biomed Eng Online. 2010;9:45. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102. Kybic J, Clerc M, Abboud T, Faugeras O, Keriven R, Papadopoulo T. A common formalism for the Integral formulations of the forward EEG problem. IEEE Trans Med Imaging. 2005;24:12–28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103. Vinck M, Oostenveld R, van Wingerden M, Battaglia F, Pennartz CM. An improved index of phase-synchronization for electrophysiological data in the presence of volume-conduction, noise and sample-size bias. NeuroImage. 2011;55:1548–1565. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104. Peraza LR, Asghar AUR, Green G, Halliday DM. Volume conduction effects in brain network inference from electroencephalographic recordings using phase lag index. J Neurosci Methods. 2012;207:189–199. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105. Oostenveld R, Fries P, Maris E, Schoffelen JM. FieldTrip: Open source software for advanced analysis of MEG, EEG, and invasive electrophysiological data. Comput Intell Neurosci. 2011;2011:156869. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106. Rubinov M, Sporns O. Complex network measures of brain connectivity: Uses and interpretations. NeuroImage. 2010;52:1059–1069. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107. Newman ME. Fast algorithm for detecting community structure in networks. Phys Rev E. 2004;69:066133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108. Opsahl T, Agneessens F, Skvoretz J. Node centrality in weighted networks: generalizing degree and shortest paths. Soc Netw. 2010;32:245–251. [Google Scholar]
- 109. Zalesky A, Fornito A, Bullmore ET. Network-based statistic: Identifying differences in brain networks. NeuroImage. 2010;53:1197–207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110. Xia M, Wang J, He Y. BrainNet viewer: A network visualization tool for human brain connectomics. PLoS ONE. 2013;8:e68910. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111. Onnela J-P, Saramäki J, Kertész J, Kaski K. Intensity and coherence of motifs in weighted complex networks. Phys Rev E. 2005;71:065103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112. Lancichinetti A, Fortunato S. Consensus clustering in complex networks. Sci Rep. 2012;2:336. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113. Eickhoff SB, Stephan KE, Mohlberg H, et al. . A new SPM toolbox for combining probabilistic cytoarchitectonic maps and functional imaging data. NeuroImage. 2005;25:1325–1335. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114. Behrens TEJ, Johansen-Berg H, Woolrich MW, et al. . Non-invasive mapping of connections between human thalamus and cortex using diffusion imaging. Nat Neurosci. 2003;6:750–757. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115. Jenkinson M, Beckmann CF, Behrens TE, Woolrich MW, Smith SM. FSL. NeuroImage. 2012;62:782–790. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116. Avants BB, Tustison N, Song G. Advanced normalization tools (ANTS). Insight J. 2009;2:1–35. [Google Scholar]
- 117. Jenkinson M, Bannister P, Brady M, Smith S. Improved optimization for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain images. NeuroImage. 2002;17:825–841. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118. Behrens TE, Woolrich MW, Jenkinson M, et al. . Characterization and propagation of uncertainty in diffusion-weighted MR imaging. Magn Reson Med. 2003;50:1077–1088. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 119. Behrens TE, Berg HJ, Jbabdi S, Rushworth MF, Woolrich MW. Probabilistic diffusion tractography with multiple fibre orientations: What can we gain? NeuroImage. 2007;34:144–155. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120. Cabral J, Hugues E, Sporns O, Deco G. Role of local network oscillations in resting-state functional connectivity. NeuroImage. 2011;57:130–139. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 121. Perneczky R, Drzezga A, Boecker H, Förstl H, Kurz A, Häussermann P. Cerebral metabolic dysfunction in patients with dementia with Lewy bodies and visual hallucinations. Dementia Geriatr Cognit Disord. 2008;25:531–538. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122. Luders HO. Textbook of epilepsy surgery: CRC Press; 2008. [Google Scholar]
- 123. Livingstone MS, Hubel DH. Psychophysical evidence for separate channels for the perception of form, color, movement, and depth. J Neurosci. 1987;7:3416–3468. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 124. Morris JS, Öhman A, Dolan RJ. A subcortical pathway to the right amygdala mediating “unseen” fear. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1999;96:1680–1685. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 125. Bar M, Kassam KS, Ghuman AS, et al. . Top-down facilitation of visual recognition. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2006;103:449–454. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 126. Kveraga K, Ghuman AS, Bar M. Top-down predictions in the cognitive brain. Brain Cogn. 2007;65:145–168. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 127. Lundstrom BN, Petersson KM, Andersson J, Johansson M, Fransson P, Ingvar M. Isolating the retrieval of imagined pictures during episodic memory: Activation of the left precuneus and left prefrontal cortex. NeuroImage. 2003;20:1934–1943. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 128. Nolde SF, Johnson MK, Raye CL. The role of prefrontal cortex during tests of episodic memory. Trends Cogn Sci. 1998;2:399–406. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 129. Fletcher PC, Henson RNA. Frontal lobes and human memory: Insights from functional neuroimaging. Brain. 2001;124:849–881. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 130. Summerfield C, Egner T, Greene M, Koechlin E, Mangels J, Hirsch J. Predictive codes for forthcoming perception in the frontal cortex. Science. 2006;314:1311–1314. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 131. Lang X, Wang L, Zhuo C-J, Jia F, Wang L-N, Wang C-L. Reduction of interhemispheric functional connectivity in sensorimotor and visual information processing pathways in schizophrenia. Chin Med J. 2016;129:2422–2426. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 132. Chang X, Xi YB, Cui LB, et al. . Distinct inter-hemispheric dysconnectivity in schizophrenia patients with and without auditory verbal hallucinations. Sci Rep. 2015;5:11218. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 133. Wigand M, Kubicki M, von Hohenberg CC, et al. . Auditory verbal hallucinations and the interhemispheric auditory pathway in chronic schizophrenia. World J Biol Psychiatry. 2015;16:31–44. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 134. Mima T, Oluwatimilehin T, Hiraoka T, Hallett M. Transient interhemispheric neuronal synchrony correlates with object recognition. J Neurosci. 2001;21:3942–3948. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 135. Raichle ME. The brain’s default mode network. Ann Rev Neurosci. 2015;38:433–447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 136. Jimenez AM, Lee J, Wynn JK, et al. . Abnormal ventral and dorsal attention network activity during single and dual target detection in schizophrenia. Front Psychol. 2016;7:323. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 137. Krall SC, Rottschy C, Oberwelland E, et al. . The role of the right temporoparietal junction in attention and social interaction as revealed by ALE meta-analysis. Brain Struct Funct. 2015;220:587–604. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 138. Blanc F, Noblet V, Philippi N, et al. . Right anterior insula: Core region of hallucinations in cognitive neurodegenerative diseases. PLoS ONE. 2014;9:e114774. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 139. Treisman AM, Kanwisher NG. Perceiving visually presented objects: Recognition, awareness, and modularity. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 1998;8:218–226. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 140. Zeki S, Bartels A. The autonomy of the visual systems and the modularity of conscious vision. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1998;353:1911–1914. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 141. Borowsky R, Loehr J, Kelland Friesen C, Kraushaar G, Kingstone A, Sarty G. Modularity and intersection of “what”, “where” and “how” processing of visual stimuli: a new method of FMRI localization. Brain Topogr. 2005;18:67–75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 142. O’Callaghan C, Kveraga K, Shine JM, Adams RB, Bar M. Predictions penetrate perception: Converging insights from brain, behaviour and disorder. Conscious Cogn. 2017;47:63–74. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 143. Gamond L, George N, Lemaréchal J-D, Hugueville L, Adam C, Tallon-Baudry C. Early influence of prior experience on face perception. NeuroImage. 2011;54:1415–1426. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 144. Chaumon M, Hasboun D, Baulac M, Adam C, Tallon-Baudry C. Unconscious contextual memory affects early responses in the anterior temporal lobe. Brain Res. 2009;1285:77–87. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 145. Zhang R, Shao R, Xu G, et al. . Aberrant brain structural–functional connectivity coupling in euthymic bipolar disorder. Hum Brain Mapp. 2019;40:3452–3463. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 146. Li K, Liu L, Yin Q, et al. . Abnormal rich club organization and impaired correlation between structural and functional connectivity in migraine sufferers. Brain Imaging Behav. 2017;11:526–540. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 147. Guye M, Parker GJM, Symms M, et al. . Combined functional MRI and tractography to demonstrate the connectivity of the human primary motor cortex in vivo. NeuroImage. 2003;19:1349–1360. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 148. Greicius MD, Supekar K, Menon V, Dougherty RF. Resting-state functional connectivity reflects structural connectivity in the default mode network. Cereb Cortex. 2008;19:72–78. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 149. Hindriks R, Woolrich M, Luckhoo H, et al. . Role of white-matter pathways in coordinating alpha oscillations in resting visual cortex. NeuroImage. 2015;106:328–339. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 150. Perry EK, Marshall E, Kerwin J, et al. . Evidence of a monoaminergic-cholinergic imbalance related to visual hallucinations in Lewy body dementia. J Neurochem. 1990;55:1454–1456. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 151. Zarkali A, McColgan P, Leyland L-A, Lees AJ, Weil RS. Visual dysfunction predicts cognitive impairment and white matter degeneration in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord. 2021;36:1191–1202. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 152. Arevalo-Rodriguez I, Smailagic N, Roqué-Figuls M, et al. . Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) for the early detection of dementia in people with mild cognitive impairment (MCI). Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2021;7:CD010783. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 153. Archibald NK, Clarke MP, Mosimann UP, Burn DJ. Visual symptoms in Parkinson’s disease and Parkinson’s disease dementia. Mov Disord. 2011;26:2387–2395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 154. Pizzi S D, Franciotti R, Tartaro A, et al. . Structural alteration of the dorsal visual network in DLB patients with visual hallucinations: A cortical thickness MRI study. PLoS ONE. 2014;9:e86624. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 155. Benarroch EE. Pedunculopontine nucleus: functional organization and clinical implications. Neurology. 2013;80:1148–1155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 156. Satoh K, Fibiger HC. Cholinergic neurons of the laterodorsal tegmental nucleus: efferent and afferent connections. J Comp Neurol. 1986;253:277–302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The data that support the findings of this study are available from the senior authors J.-P.T. and M.K., on reasonable request by qualified researchers.