1. Introduction
The neuropeptide oxytocin persists as a hot topic in neuroscience, with steadily increasing interest in both scientific literature [1] and popular culture during recent decades [2–5]. This attention is inspired not only by the fascinating workings of oxytocin itself, which span in breadth from underlying critical reproductive biology [6] to supporting high-level social behaviours [7–9], but also by the enormous potential of leveraging oxytocin as a therapeutic to enhance social cognition [10–13]. However, accompanying this excitement are well-justified critiques. These include the following: (i) our understanding of how oxytocin modulates social cognition is lacking in methodological rigour [14–17]; (ii) the effects of oxytocin treatment can be highly context-dependent [18,19]; (iii) we are still building a mechanistic understanding for how oxytocin impacts social behaviours at the neurobiological level [9,20–24]; (iv) developmental and life experience can drastically change the function of oxytocinergic systems [25–27]; and (v) we lack a single overarching theory to predict how oxytocin may modulate behaviour [28–31]. These critical examinations are fundamental for advancing our understanding of oxytocin, enabling the utilization of oxytocinergic mechanisms as a means to study the neurobiology of social behaviours [32,33] and as a curative tool to restore the deficits in social cognition observed across a variety of psychiatric disorders [34,35].
One critical perspective that has been lacking in the literature until recently is the examination of how oxytocin interacts with other neuromodulatory systems [36]. In the brain, no single region or neuromodulator is an island, entire of itself, and although the practical considerations of laboratory experiments present limits on what any single experiment can address, studying the manipulations of oxytocin in isolation may lead to an incomplete or incorrect understanding. Historical experiments [6,37–42] have laid the crucial and foundational bases for researchers today to move forward with the difficult, but necessary, tasks of examining the substrates and effects of oxytocin in increasingly naturalistic behavioural contexts [7,32,43–45] and from holistic perspectives [44]. Present incongruities in our understanding of oxytocin may be, in part, the result of experimental approaches that seek to isolate the oxytocin system as an experimental variable while ignoring the rest of the brain or body. This is not intended as a criticism of past experiments, which built our understanding of oxytocin from a uterine-contracting agent to our multi-faceted perspective today, but instead a proposition for future studies that seek to address as yet unanswered questions.
Indeed, the field is already successfully moving in the direction of examining oxytocin function under more naturalistic contexts and more holistically, aided by advancements in technology [46,47] and decades of critical introspection in the literature [14,15,17,48]. For example, recent research has identified supralinear enhancements of social gaze from combinatorial treatment of oxytocin and the opioid antagonist naloxone [49], demonstrating a mechanistic link between the oxytocinergic and opioidergic systems in the regulation of social attention. Moreover, in the mouse nucleus accumbens, the endogenous endocannabinoid anandamide binding at the CB1 receptors drives social reward, and blockade or selective activation of oxytocin neurons in the paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus can suppress or enhance this socially driven anandamide mobilization [50], connecting the endocannabinoid system with oxytocin. Likewise, the genetic deletion of presynaptic oxytocin receptors from the nucleus accumbens, removing the relevant serotonergic innervation to the nucleus accumbens, eliminates the rewarding aspects of social interaction in mice [51], suggesting a role for serotonin–oxytocin interactions in social behaviours. These and other mechanistic connections between oxytocin and other neuromodulators are beginning to demonstrate that many of the functions attributed to oxytocin are mediated through complex and sometimes powerful crosstalk between oxytocin and other neuromodulators. This exciting perspective will be crucial for future research examining not only oxytocin but also the neurobiology of social behaviour as a whole. Thus, the goal of this introduction, and this entire theme issue more broadly, is to highlight some of the known interactions between oxytocin and other neuromodulators and to provide a holistic perspective of oxytocin function for future studies.
2. Oxytocin and sex hormones
Oxytocin, and the closely related arginine-vasopressin, are the result of ancient gene duplication in vertebral evolution [52–55]. While contemporary neuroscience and psychology may emphasize the neuromodulatory role of oxytocin, the hormonal functions of oxytocin in reproductive behaviours [56], parturition [57], lactation [6] and early parent–infant interactions are undoubtedly the more primal functions for this complex non-apeptide. Given the role that oxytocin plays in reproductive biology, it is unsurprising that previous studies have linked oxytocin with other sex hormones in shaping behaviours. Intranasal administration of oxytocin, for example, will also increase blood plasma levels of testosterone in healthy men [58]. In males, intranasal oxytocin has been shown to blunt the correlation between testosterone reactivity and competitiveness [59]. Oxytocin administration can also modulate testosterone levels in fathers in a fashion that is correlated with father–child social behaviours such as social gaze and social touch [60].
In this theme issue, Bakermans-Kranenburg et al. [61] asks how hormonal levels fluctuate in men from pregnancy to after the birth of their firstborn child, and how oxytocin and other hormones could explain differences in the quality of their parenting. Both oxytocin and oestradiol remained stable from the pre- to post-natal periods, while vasopressin and testosterone declined. Interestingly, oxytocin by itself, or in relation to other hormones, was not related to paternal sensitivity. However, for fathers with high oestradiol, a higher level of testosterone was associated with lower sensitivity. Adding to this understanding, Jiang et al. [62] examined how both oxytocin and testosterone modulated the gaze of non-human primates when shown conspecific social and sexual images. Both oxytocin and testosterone increased the innate bias for gaze on female genitalia over female faces and promoted viewing of the forehead region where rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) display sexual skin. This modulation of stimulus preference indicates that both oxytocin and testosterone influence reproductive behaviours by possibly increasing the visual salience of sexual features. Similarly, Paletta et al. [63] review how interactions between oxytocin and other regulatory systems mediate social behaviours, with a particular emphasis on the female sex hormone oestrogen, importantly highlighting a link between oxytocin and the oestrogen receptors in shaping behaviour.
3. Oxytocin interactions with dopaminergic, serotonergic and opioidergic systems
Interactions between oxytocin and other neuromodulator or neurotransmitter systems remain broadly underappreciated in the literature. However, links between oxytocin and the dopaminergic system are arguably the most well understood [64]. The classic prairie vole (Microtus ochrogaster) model of pair-bonding has been demonstrated to be mediated not just by oxytocin receptors in the nucleus accumbens [37–39,41], but also by mesolimbic dopamine circuits in the reward centres of the brain to create a conditioned partner preference [39,65]. Exploring this relationship between oxytocin binding and dopaminergic circuits, Frehner et al. [66] in this issue examined a dense clustering of oxytocin receptors in the human dopaminergic substantia nigra pars compacta to test if variations in oxytocin receptor expression could identify individuals with autism. Postmortem human brain tissue specimens revealed that females with autism had significantly lower levels of oxytocin receptor expression than did males with autism or typically developing males and females. In situ hybridization to visualize and quantify oxytocin receptor mRNA found no differences between groups, suggesting that the difference in receptor expression was possibly the result of local dysregulation in oxytocin receptor protein translation or changes in the endocytosis and recycling rates.
The nucleus accumbens is a key site of oxytocin–dopamine interactions, as detailed thoughtfully in a review by Borie and colleagues [67] that explores how endocannabinoids, by interacting with oxytocin, modulate experience-dependent social behaviours. Here, the authors examine how oxytocin modulates glutamatergic signalling through the recruitment of endocannabinoids in the prairie vole nucleus accumbens and broadly review our understanding of the effects of oxytocin–endocannabinoid interactions on social behaviour, with an emphasis on how sex differences and life experiences may modulate these processes.
Examining the relationship that oxytocin has with the dopamine and serotonin systems in maternal behaviours, Grieb & Lonstein [68] in this issue shed light on how these processes regulate different aspects of caregiving and postpartum behaviours. Although oxytocin–dopamine interactions have been understood to motivate active caregiving behaviours, such as retrieval of pups, the authors highlight the underappreciated interactions between oxytocin and serotonin. These oxytocin–serotonin interactions, they argue, regulate many of the remaining dimensions of maternal care including nursing, anxiety-like behaviours and strategies for coping with stress.
Beyond the classical neurotransmitters, oxytocin also interacts with other neuromodulatory systems. One notable example is interactions between oxytocin and the opioid receptor system, observed both in vitro [69–73] and in social behavioural experiments [49]. Putnam & Chang [74] detail interactions between the oxytocinergic and opioidergic systems in shaping social behaviours, postulating a model to explain the supralinear effects arising from co-administering oxytocin and opioid blockade on social attention.
4. Interventional oxytocin
The promise of using oxytocin as a therapeutic intervention remains a tantalizing goal for the field of social neuroscience [12,75]. However, a robustly effective approach has not yet been identified [48,76,77]. The paper by Wei et al. [78] in this issue tackles this problem by suggesting a combinatorial approach to treat neuropsychiatric social impairment using both the oxytocin and endocannabinoid systems. Here, the authors detail the neurobiology of both the oxytocin and the endocannabinoid systems and explain how a multi-targeted treatment strategy may be best suited to modulate the multiple signalling processes underlying social cognition.
In an intervention-focused research article, Daughters et al. [79] compare the efficacy of oxytocin treatment against a validated emotion training program to test improvements in emotion recognition. Interestingly, they show that psychological intervention, but not intranasal oxytocin, was able to improve recognition specifically for angry expressions. This interesting finding highlights that behavioural or psychological treatments, with fewer caveats and risks, could sometimes exceed pharmacological interventions and should be considered carefully for future clinical trials involving oxytocin.
5. Oxytocin interactions beyond the neuron
A holistic understanding of oxytocin extends beyond specific neurotransmitters or brain regions but encompasses underappreciated aspects of neurobiology. A prime example of this is reviewed by Gonzalez and Hammock [80], who examine how oxytocin interacts with the microglia. Here, the authors detail how microglia have a bidirectional regulatory relationship with the oxytocin system and how these processes enhance experience-dependent circuits during sensitive periods to shape social behaviours. This perspective not only emphasizes the important and undervalued role that microglia play in shaping neural activity [81], but also how microglia–oxytocin interactions may critically impact our understanding of endogenous oxytocin function [82].
Similarly, a review piece by Carter & Kingsbury [83] offers a novel perspective with respect to how the oxytocin system played an important evolutionary role in managing oxidative stress and inflammation from the earth's oxygen-rich conditions. The resultant unique properties of the oxytocin system may have significantly enabled vertebrates to manage the consequences of oxygen-linked processes, such as inflammation or free radicals, while still supporting complex social behaviours.
At the neurobiological level, ageing also drastically impacts cognitive capacities. However, the link with neuropeptides is understudied. Polk and colleagues [84] examine the link between oxytocin and social cognition in ageing, finding that higher levels of plasma oxytocin were associated with lower accuracy in emotion identification, while plasma levels of arginine-vasopressin had no relation to emotion identification accuracy. These novel findings support the involvement of oxytocin in age-related neural processes and the possible interactions between oxytocin and basic cognitive capacities.
Oxytocin interactions also encompass broad life events, as expounded by Bales and Rodgers [85] in a review examining oxytocin interactions in partner loss. The authors survey what is known about the neuroendocrine mechanisms that regulate the emotional consequences of partner loss and specifically focus on interactions among oxytocin, corticotropin-releasing-hormone and the κ-opioid system.
6. Conclusion
The final paper of our theme issue by Leng et al. [86] takes a critical perspective on whether oxytocin is indeed a ‘social’ neuropeptide. This is a fitting final note to this Introduction since our perspective on oxytocin must constantly be reevaluated and challenged. Without disputing the significance of previous findings, we suggest that future studies should seek to examine the function of oxytocin not in isolation, but instead from a holistic perspective. The interactions between oxytocin and other neuromodulatory systems in the brain and body are not only crucial to understanding oxytocinergic function, but also the fundamental neural substrates of social behaviour. Understanding these connections will enable scientists and clinicians to better realize therapeutic interventions targeting the oxytocinergic system and will provide a window into the evolved process that shaped social functions in a wide array of animal species.
Data accessibility
This article has no additional data.
Authors' contributions
P.T.P.: writing—original draft and writing—review and editing; S.W.C.C.: writing—original draft and writing—review and editing.
Both authors gave final approval for publication and agreed to be held accountable for the work performed therein.
Conflict of interest declaration
This theme issue was put together by the Guest Editor team under supervision from the journal's Editorial staff, following the Royal Society's ethical codes and best-practice guidelines. The Guest Editor team invited contributions and handled the review process. Individual Guest Editors were not involved in assessing papers where they had a personal, professional or financial conflict of interest with the authors or the research described. Independent reviewers assessed all papers. Invitation to contribute did not guarantee inclusion. S.W.C.C. is one of the inventors who holds a US patent ‘Oxytocin and opioid antagonists for treatment of social dysfunction disorder’, Patent no. 11160843, Application no. 16/398,744, issue date of 11/02/2021.’
Funding
This work was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health (R01MH120081).
References
- 1.Leng G, Leng RI. 2021. Oxytocin: a citation network analysis of 10 000 papers. J. Neuroendocrinol. 33, e13014. ( 10.1111/jne.13014) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Cimons M. 2016. Your dog can make you feel better, and here's why. Washington Post, 19 September 2016. (See https://www.washingtonpost.com/national/health-science/your-dog-can-make-you-feel-better-and-heres-why/2016/09/19/fde4aeec-6a2a-11e6-8225-fbb8a6fc65bc_story.html).
- 3.Belluck P. 2013. Oxytocin found to stimulate social brain regions in children with autism. The New York Times, 2 December 2013. (See https://www.nytimes.com/2013/12/03/health/oxytocin-found-to-stimulate-brain-in-children-with-autism.html)
- 4.Wade N. 2011. Depth of the kindness hormone appears to know some bounds. The New York Times, 10 January 2011. (See https://www.amren.com/news/2011/01/depth_of_the_ki/)
- 5.Angier N. 2009. The biology behind the milk of human kindness. The New York Times, 23 November 2009. (See https://www.nytimes.com/2009/11/24/science/24angier.html?)
- 6.Kendrick KM, Keverne EB, Baldwin BA. 1987. Intracerebroventricular oxytocin stimulates maternal behaviour in the sheep. Neuroendocrinology 46, 56-61. ( 10.1159/000124796) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Auyeung B, et al. 2015. Oxytocin increases eye contact during a real-time, naturalistic social interaction in males with and without autism. Transl. Psychiatry 5, e507. ( 10.1038/tp.2014.146) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Piva M, Chang SWC. 2018. An integrated framework for the role of oxytocin in multistage social decision-making. Am. J. Primatol. 80, e22735. ( 10.1002/ajp.22735) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Froemke RC, Young LJ. 2021. Oxytocin, neural plasticity, and social behavior. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 44, 359-381. ( 10.1146/annurev-neuro-102320-102847) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Andari E, Duhamel JR, Zalla T, Herbrecht E, Leboyer M, Sirigu A. 2010. Promoting social behavior with oxytocin in high-functioning autism spectrum disorders. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 107, 4389-4394. ( 10.1073/pnas.0910249107) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Carter CS, et al. 2020. Is oxytocin ‘Nature's Medicine’? Pharmacol. Rev. 72, 829-861. ( 10.1124/pr.120.019398) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Young LJ, Barrett CE. 2015. Neuroscience. Can oxytocin treat autism? Science 347, 825-826. ( 10.1126/science.aaa8120) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Young LJ. 2015. Oxytocin, social cognition and psychiatry. Neuropsychopharmacology 40, 243-244. ( 10.1038/npp.2014.186) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Evans SL, Dal Monte O, Noble P, Averbeck BB. 2014. Intranasal oxytocin effects on social cognition: a critique. Brain Res. 1580, 69-77. ( 10.1016/j.brainres.2013.11.008) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Walum H, Waldman ID, Young LJ. 2016. Statistical and methodological considerations for the interpretation of intranasal oxytocin studies. Biol. Psychiatry 79, 251-257. ( 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.06.016) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Quintana DS. 2022. Towards better hypothesis tests in oxytocin research: evaluating the validity of auxiliary assumptions. Psychoneuroendocrinology 137, 105642. ( 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2021.105642) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Macchia A, Zebhauser PT, Salcedo S, Burum B, Gold E, Alonso-Alonso M, Pascual-Leone A, Gilbert D, Brem AK. 2022. Divergent effects of oxytocin on ‘mind-reading’ in healthy males. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 22, 112-122. ( 10.3758/s13415-021-00936-3) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Bartz JA, Zaki J, Bolger N, Ochsner KN. 2011. Social effects of oxytocin in humans: context and person matter. Trends Cogn. Sci. 15, 301-309. ( 10.1016/j.tics.2011.05.002) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.De Dreu CKW, Greer LL, Van Kleef GA, Shalvi S, Handgraaf MJJ. 2011. Oxytocin promotes human ethnocentrism. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 108, 1262-1266. ( 10.1073/pnas.1015316108) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Busnelli M, Chini B. 2018. Molecular basis of oxytocin receptor signalling in the brain: what we know and what we need to know. Curr. Top. Behav. Neurosci. 35, 3-29. ( 10.1007/7854_2017_6) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Bakos J, Srancikova A, Havranek T, Bacova Z. 2018. Molecular mechanisms of oxytocin signaling at the synaptic connection. Neural. Plast. 2018, 4864107. ( 10.1155/2018/4864107) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Lefevre A, Benusiglio D, Tang Y, Krabichler Q, Charlet A, Grinevich V. 2021. Oxytocinergic feedback circuitries: an anatomical basis for neuromodulation of social behaviors. Front. Neural Circuits 15, 688234. ( 10.3389/fncir.2021.688234) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Mitre M, Minder J, Morina EX, Chao MV, Froemke RC. 2018. Oxytocin modulation of neural circuits. Curr. Top. Behav. Neurosci. 35, 31-53. ( 10.1007/7854_2017_7) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Marlin BJ, Froemke RC. 2017. Oxytocin modulation of neural circuits for social behavior. Dev. Neurobiol. 77, 169-189. ( 10.1002/dneu.22452) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Bales KL, Perkeybile AM. 2012. Developmental experiences and the oxytocin receptor system. Horm. Behav. 61, 313-319. ( 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.12.013) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Carter CS, Boone EM, Pournajafi-Nazarloo H, Bales KL. 2009. Consequences of early experiences and exposure to oxytocin and vasopressin are sexually dimorphic. Dev. Neurosci. 31, 332-341. ( 10.1159/000216544) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Veenema AH. 2012. Toward understanding how early-life social experiences alter oxytocin- and vasopressin-regulated social behaviors. Horm. Behav. 61, 304-312. ( 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.12.002) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Nave G, Camerer C, McCullough M. 2015. Does oxytocin increase trust in humans? A critical review of research. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 10, 772-789. ( 10.1177/1745691615600138) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Shou Q, Yamada J, Nishina K, Matsunaga M, Kiyonari T, Takagishi H. 2022. Is oxytocin a trust hormone? Salivary oxytocin is associated with caution but not with general trust. PLoS ONE 17, e0267988. ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0267988) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Quintana DS, Guastella AJ. 2020. An allostatic theory of oxytocin. Trends Cogn. Sci. 24, 515-528. ( 10.1016/j.tics.2020.03.008) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Shamay-Tsoory SG, Abu-Akel A. 2016. The social salience hypothesis of oxytocin. Biol. Psychiatry 79, 194-202. ( 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.07.020) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Putnam PT, Young LJ, Gothard KM. 2018. Bridging the gap between rodents and humans: the role of non-human primates in oxytocin research. Am. J. Primatol. 80, e22756. ( 10.1002/ajp.22756) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Chang SWC. 2017. An emerging field of primate social neurophysiology: current developments. eNeuro 4, ENEURO.0295-17.2017. ( 10.1523/ENEURO.0295-17.2017) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Hoertnagl CM, Hofer A. 2014. Social cognition in serious mental illness. Curr. Opin. Psychiatry 27, 197-202. ( 10.1097/YCO.0000000000000055) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Santamaría-García H, et al. 2020. The role of social cognition skills and social determinants of health in predicting symptoms of mental illness. Transl. Psychiatry 10, 165. ( 10.1038/s41398-020-0852-4) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Fan S, Weinberg-Wolf H, Piva M, Dal Monte O, Chang SWC. 2020. Combinatorial oxytocin neuropharmacology in social cognition. Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed.) 24, 8-12. ( 10.1016/j.tics.2019.10.004) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Witt DM, Carter CS, Walton DM. 1990. Central and peripheral effects of oxytocin administration in prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster). Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 37, 63-69. ( 10.1016/0091-3057(90)90042-g) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Cho MM, DeVries AC, Williams JR, Carter CS. 1999. The effects of oxytocin and vasopressin on partner preferences in male and female prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster). Behav. Neurosci. 113, 1071-1079. ( 10.1037/0735-7044.113.5.1071) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Young LJ, Wang Z. 2004. The neurobiology of pair bonding. Nat. Neurosci. 7, 1048-1054. ( 10.1038/nn1327) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Oettl LL, et al. 2016. Oxytocin enhances social recognition by modulating cortical control of early olfactory processing. Neuron 90, 609-621. ( 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.03.033) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Olazábal DE, Young LJ. 2006. Oxytocin receptors in the nucleus accumbens facilitate ‘spontaneous’ maternal behavior in adult female prairie voles. Neuroscience 141, 559-568. ( 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.04.017) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Ferguson JN, Aldag JM, Insel TR, Young LJ. 2001. Oxytocin in the medial amygdala is essential for social recognition in the mouse. J. Neurosci. 21, 8278-8285. ( 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-20-08278.2001) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Chang SWC, Barter JW, Ebitz RB, Watson KK, Platt ML. 2012. Inhaled oxytocin amplifies both vicarious reinforcement and self reinforcement in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 109, 959-964. ( 10.1073/pnas.1114621109) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Fan S, Dal Monte O, Chang SWC. 2021. Levels of naturalism in social neuroscience research. iScience 24, 102702. ( 10.1016/j.isci.2021.102702) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Gothard KM, Mosher CP, Zimmerman PE, Putnam PT, Morrow JK, Fuglevand AJ. 2018. New perspectives on the neurophysiology of primate amygdala emerging from the study of naturalistic social behaviors. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Cogn. Sci. 9, e1449. ( 10.1002/wcs.1449) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Knobloch HS, et al. 2012. Evoked axonal oxytocin release in the central amygdala attenuates fear response. Neuron 73, 553-566. ( 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.11.030) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Ino D, Hibino H, Nishiyama M. 2021. A fluorescent sensor for the real-time measurement of extracellular oxytocin dynamics in the brain. bioRxiv. ( 10.1101/2021.07.30.454450) [DOI]
- 48.Erdozain AM, Peñagarikano O. 2019. Oxytocin as treatment for social cognition, not there yet. Front. Psychiatry 10, 930. ( 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00930) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Dal Monte O, Piva M, Anderson KM, Tringides M, Holmes AJ, Chang SWC. 2017. Oxytocin under opioid antagonism leads to supralinear enhancement of social attention. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 114, 5247-5252. ( 10.1073/pnas.1702725114) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Wei D, Lee D, Cox CD, Karsten CA, Peñagarikano O, Geschwind DH, Gall CM, Piomelli D. 2015. Endocannabinoid signaling mediates oxytocin-driven social reward. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 112, 14 084-14 089. ( 10.1073/pnas.1509795112) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Dölen G, Darvishzadeh A, Huang KW, Malenka RC. 2013. Social reward requires coordinated activity of nucleus accumbens oxytocin and serotonin. Nature 501, 179-184. ( 10.1038/nature12518) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Keverne EB, Curley JP. 2004. Vasopressin, oxytocin and social behaviour. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 14, 777-783. ( 10.1016/j.conb.2004.10.006) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Pedersen CA, Chang SWC, Williams CL. 2014. Evolutionary perspectives on the role of oxytocin in human social behavior, social cognition and psychopathology. Brain Res. 1580, 1-7. ( 10.1016/j.brainres.2014.07.033) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Knobloch HS, Grinevich V. 2014. Evolution of oxytocin pathways in the brain of vertebrates. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 8, 31. ( 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00031) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Carter CS. 2014. Oxytocin pathways and the evolution of human behavior. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 65, 17-39. ( 10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115110) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Carter CS. 1992. Oxytocin and sexual behavior. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 16, 131-144. ( 10.1016/S0149-7634(05)80176-9) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Zeeman GG, Khan-Dawood FS, Dawood MY. 1997. Oxytocin and its receptor in pregnancy and parturition: current concepts and clinical implications. Obstet. Gynecol. 89, 873-883. ( 10.1016/s0029-7844(97)00056-2) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Gossen A, Hahn A, Westphal L, Prinz S, Schultz RT, Gründer G, Spreckelmeyer KN. 2012. Oxytocin plasma concentrations after single intranasal oxytocin administration – a study in healthy men. Neuropeptides 46, 211-215. ( 10.1016/j.npep.2012.07.001) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Cherki BR, Winter E, Mankuta D, Israel S. 2021. Intranasal oxytocin, testosterone reactivity, and human competitiveness. Psychoneuroendocrinology 132, 105352. ( 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2021.105352) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Weisman O, Zagoory-Sharon O, Feldman R. 2014. Oxytocin administration, salivary testosterone, and father–infant social behavior. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 49, 47-52. ( 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2013.11.006) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Verhees MWFT, Lotz AM, Alyousefi-van Dijk K, van IJzendoorn MH. 2022. Is paternal oxytocin an oxymoron? Oxytocin, vasopressin, testosterone, oestradiol and cortisol in emerging fatherhood. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 377, 20210060. ( 10.1098/rstb.2021.0060) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Jiang Y, Sheng F, Belkaya N, Platt ML. 2022. Oxytocin and testosterone administration amplify viewing preferences for sexual images in male rhesus macaques. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 377, 20210133. ( 10.1098/rstb.2021.0133) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Paletta P, Bass N, Kavaliers M, Choleris E. 2022. The role of oxytocin in shaping complex social behaviours: possible interactions with other neuromodulators. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 377, 20210058. ( 10.1098/rstb.2021.0058) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Baskerville TA, Douglas AJ. 2010. Dopamine and oxytocin interactions underlying behaviors: potential contributions to behavioral disorders. CNS Neurosci. Ther. 16, e92-e123. ( 10.1111/j.1755-5949.2010.00154.x) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Liu Y, Wang ZX. 2003. Nucleus accumbens oxytocin and dopamine interact to regulate pair bond formation in female prairie voles. Neuroscience 121, 537-544. ( 10.1016/S0306-4522(03)00555-4) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Frehner SS, Dooley KT, Palumbo MC, Smith AL, Goodman MM, Bales KL, Freeman SM. 2022. Effect of sex and autism spectrum disorder on oxytocin receptor binding and mRNA expression in the dopaminergic pars compacta of the human substantia nigra. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 377, 20210118. ( 10.1098/rstb.2021.0118) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Borie AM, Young LJ, Liu RC. 2022. Sex-specific and social experience-dependent oxytocin–endocannabinoid interactions in the nucleus accumbens: implications for social behaviour. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 377, 20210057. ( 10.1098/rstb.2021.0057) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Grieb ZA, Lonstein JS. 2022. Oxytocin interactions with central dopamine and serotonin systems regulate different components of motherhood. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 377, 20210062. ( 10.1098/rstb.2021.0062) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Bicknell RJ, Leng G. 1982. Endogenous opiates regulate oxytocin but not vasopressin secretion from the neurohypophysis. Nature 298, 161-162. ( 10.1038/298161a0) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Bicknell RJ, Ingram CD, Leng G. 1983. Oxytocin release is inhibited by opiates from the neural lobe, not those from the intermediate lobe. Neurosci. Lett. 43, 227-230. ( 10.1016/0304-3940(83)90192-1) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Bicknell RJ, Leng G, Lincoln DW, Russell JA. 1988. Naloxone excites oxytocin neurones in the supraoptic nucleus of lactating rats after chronic morphine treatment. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 396, 297-317. ( 10.1113/jphysiol.1988.sp016963) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Douglas AJ, Johnstone LE, Neumann I, Leng G, Russell JA. 1994. Oxytocin neurones in the supraoptic nucleus (SON) are inhibited by endogenous opioids in late pregnant rats. Gene Ther. 1(Suppl. 1), S84. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Shibuki K, Leng G, Way S. 1988. Effects of naloxone and of intraperitoneal hypertonic saline upon oxytocin release and upon supraoptic neuronal activity. Neurosci. Lett. 88, 75-80. ( 10.1016/0304-3940(88)90318-7) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Putnam PT, Chang SWC. 2022. Interplay between the oxytocin and opioid systems in regulating social behaviour. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 377, 20210050. ( 10.1098/rstb.2021.0050) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Andari E, Hurlemann R, Young LJ. 2018. A precision medicine approach to oxytocin trials. Curr. Top. Behav. Neurosci. 35, 559-590. ( 10.1007/7854_2017_29) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Sikich L, et al. 2021. Intranasal oxytocin in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. N. Engl. J. Med. 385, 1462-1473. ( 10.1056/NEJMoa2103583) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Horta M, Pehlivanoglu D, Ebner NC. 2020. The role of intranasal oxytocin on social cognition: an integrative human lifespan approach. Curr. Behav. Neurosci. Rep. 7, 175-192. ( 10.1007/s40473-020-00214-5) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Wei D, Tsheringla S, McPartland JC, Allsop AZASA. 2022. Combinatorial approaches for treating neuropsychiatric social impairment. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 377, 20210051. ( 10.1098/rstb.2021.0051) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Daughters K, Rees DA, Hunnikin L, Wells A, Hall J, van Goozen S. 2022. Oxytocin administration versus emotion training in healthy males: considerations for future research. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 377, 20210056. ( 10.1098/rstb.2021.0056) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Gonzalez A, Hammock EAD. 2022. Oxytocin and microglia in the development of social behaviour. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 377, 20210059. ( 10.1098/rstb.2021.0059) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Badimon A, et al. 2020. Negative feedback control of neuronal activity by microglia. Nature 586, 417-423. ( 10.1038/s41586-020-2777-8) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Loth MK, Donaldson ZR. 2021. Oxytocin, dopamine, and opioid interactions underlying pair bonding: highlighting a potential role for microglia. Endocrinology 162, bqaa223. ( 10.1210/endocr/bqaa223) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Carter CS, Kingsbury MA. 2022. Oxytocin and oxygen: the evolution of a solution to the ‘stress of life’. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 377, 20210054. ( 10.1098/rstb.2021.0054) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Polk R, Horta M, Lin T, Porges E, Ojeda M, Nazarloo HP, Carter CS, Ebner NC. 2022. Evaluating the neuropeptide–social cognition link in ageing: the mediating role of basic cognitive skills. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 377, 20210048. ( 10.1098/rstb.2021.0048) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Bales KL, Rodgers FD. 2022. Interactions between the κ opioid system, corticotropin-releasing hormone and oxytocin in partner loss. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 377, 20210061. ( 10.1098/rstb.2021.0061) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Leng G, Leng RI, Ludwig M. 2022. Oxytocin—a social peptide? Deconstructing the evidence. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 377, 20210055. ( 10.1098/rstb.2021.0055) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
This article has no additional data.