Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2022 Jul 27;11:e76964. doi: 10.7554/eLife.76964

Exogenous capture accounts for fundamental differences between pro- and antisaccade performance

Allison T Goldstein 1, Terrence R Stanford 1, Emilio Salinas 1,
Editors: Daeyeol Lee2, Timothy E Behrens3
PMCID: PMC9328762  PMID: 35894379

Abstract

To generate the next eye movement, oculomotor circuits take into consideration the physical salience of objects in view and current behavioral goals, exogenous and endogenous influences, respectively. However, the interactions between exogenous and endogenous mechanisms and their dynamic contributions to target selection have been difficult to resolve because they evolve extremely rapidly. In a recent study (Salinas et al., 2019), we achieved the necessary temporal precision using an urgent variant of the antisaccade task wherein motor plans are initiated early and choice accuracy depends sharply on when exactly the visual cue information becomes available. Empirical and modeling results indicated that the exogenous signal arrives ∼80 ms after cue onset and rapidly accelerates the (incorrect) plan toward the cue, whereas the informed endogenous signal arrives ∼25 ms later to favor the (correct) plan away from the cue. Here, we scrutinize a key mechanistic hypothesis about this dynamic, that the exogenous and endogenous signals act at different times and independently of each other. We test quantitative model predictions by comparing the performance of human participants instructed to look toward a visual cue or away from it under high urgency. We find that, indeed, the exogenous response is largely impervious to task instructions; it simply flips its sign relative to the correct choice, and this largely explains the drastic differences in psychometric performance between the two tasks. Thus, saccadic choices are strongly dictated by the alignment between salience and behavioral goals.

Research organism: Human

Introduction

The oculomotor system of primates specifies a new target to look at every 200–250 ms. Two components of visuospatial attention contribute to the underlying selection process: exogenous mechanisms, which respond to the salience of the objects in view (typically dictated by physical properties such as size, luminance, or motion), and endogenous mechanisms, which prioritize those objects according to their relevance to current behavioral goals (Itti and Koch, 2001; Theeuwes, 2010; Wolfe and Horowitz, 2017).

The antisaccade task, in which the subject is required to look away from a lone visual cue, is ideal for examining this distinction because it decouples stimulus encoding and response preparation (Munoz and Everling, 2004). For a correct antisaccade to take place, endogenous, top-down influences must override the natural tendency to look to the cue. Indeed, in both imaging studies in humans (Brown et al., 2007; Anderson et al., 2008) and single-neuron recordings in nonhuman primates (Everling et al., 1998; Everling et al., 1999; Everling and Munoz, 2000), antisaccade performance is generally characterized as a conflict between volitional and sensory-driven, reflexive responses. The capacity to resolve such conflict is considered so fundamental, that reaction time (RT) and accuracy measurements in the antisaccade task are commonly used as clinical markers for cognitive dysfunction (Klein and Foerster, 2001; Klein et al., 2003; Hutton and Ettinger, 2006; Fielding et al., 2009; Johansson et al., 2022).

This work has established key qualitative distinctions between exogenous and endogenous mechanisms. However, resolving their dynamic interactions and specific, real-time contributions to the saccade selection process has been difficult because that requires temporal resolution far greater than can be achieved with traditional psychophysical tasks.

In a recent study (Salinas et al., 2019), we used an urgent version of the antisaccade task (Figure 1a) to dissociate the exogenous and endogenous contributions to performance with millisecond precision. In the urgent task design (Stanford and Salinas, 2021), the go signal that instructs the subject to respond is given before the visual cue that specifies the correct choice (an eye movement away from the cue, in this case). This way, motor plans are initiated early, so participants guess on some trials and make informed choices on others, but critically, each outcome depends fundamentally on how much time the participant has to see the cue before committing to a response, a quantity that we call the raw processing time (rPT). If choice accuracy is then plotted as a function of rPT, the result is a novel psychometric measure — the tachometric curve — that describes the participant’s perceptual performance on a moment-by-moment basis (Stanford and Salinas, 2021).

Figure 1. The urgent tasks.

(a) The compelled antisaccade (CAS) task. After a fixation period (500, 600, or 700 ms), the central fixation point disappears (Go), instructing the participant to look to the left or to the right within 425 ms. The cue is revealed later (Cue on, ±8°), after a time gap that varies unpredictably across trials (Gap, 0–350 ms). The correct response is an eye movement away from the cue, to the diametrically opposite location (Saccade, white arrow). (b) The compelled prosaccade (CPS) task. The sequence of events is the same as for the compelled antisaccade task, except that the correct response is an eye movement toward the cue. In all trials, the cue location and gap are selected randomly; the reaction time (RT) is measured between the onset of the go signal and the onset of the saccade; and the raw processing time (rPT) is measured between cue onset and saccade onset (calculated as RT − gap).

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. Nonurgent variants of the tasks.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

(a) The delayed antisaccade task. After a fixation period (500, 600, or 700 ms), the cue is revealed (Cue on, ±8°). After a delay period (Delay, 100 or 200 ms), the central fixation point disappears (Go), instructing the participant to make an eye movement within 425 ms. The correct response is to look away from the cue, to the diametrically opposite location (Saccade, white arrow). (b) The delayed prosaccade task. The sequence of events is the same as for the delayed antisaccade task, except that the correct response is an eye movement toward the cue. In all trials, the cue location and delay are selected randomly; the reaction time (RT) is measured between the onset of the go signal and the onset of the saccade; and the raw processing time (rPT) is measured between cue onset and saccade onset (calculated as Delay + RT).

In the urgent antisaccade experiment (Salinas et al., 2019), the tachometric curve revealed a unique feature: a range of processing times roughly between 90 and 130 ms in which performance plummeted to nearly 0% correct (Figure 2d). In other words, there was a range of conditions (i.e., states of motor preparation) indexed by processing time over which the cue onset would almost inevitably capture the saccade and lead to an error. Thereafter, for rPT ≳140 ms, accuracy increased steadily and reached nearly 100% correct.

Figure 2. Turning a model of antisaccade performance into one of prosaccade performance.

Figure 2.

(a–c) Three single antisaccade trials simulated with the CAS model. The cue is assumed to be on the left and the gap is 150 ms. Traces show motor plans rL toward the left (red, incorrect) and rR toward the right (black, correct) as functions of time. During the exogenous response interval (ERI, gray vertical shade), the plan toward the cue accelerates. After the ERI, the incorrect plan decelerates and the correct one accelerates. A saccade is triggered a short efferent delay after activity reaches threshold (dashed lines). Examples include a correct, short-rPT guess (a, rPT = 56 ms); an incorrect, captured saccade (b, rPT = 133 ms); and a correct, informed choice (c, rPT = 219 ms). (d) Simulated tachometric curves for the CAS (top, red) and CPS tasks (bottom, blue). The x and y axes correspond to raw processing time and fraction of correct choices, respectively. Gray shades indicate below-chance performance, where chance (white-gray border) is 50% correct. (e–g) Three single prosaccade trials with the same initial motor plans as in a–c but simulated with the CPS model. They include an incorrect, short-rPT guess (e, rPT = 56 ms); a correct, captured saccade (f, rPT = 133 ms); and a correct, informed choice (g, rPT = 149 ms). The pro- and antisaccade simulations differed only in the movement that was considered correct, which amounted to swapping the motor plans that were endogenously accelerated and decelerated.

We interpreted these data as the direct manifestation of the conflict between the early attentional pull toward the cue, which is salience-driven, involuntary, and transient, and the later intention to look away, which is task-dependent, voluntary, and sustained. This conflict is hardly detectable during active fixation, but becomes obvious when saccade plans are ongoing. As such, it was instantiated within a saccade competition model in which two opposing motor plans race against each other to trigger an eye movement to the right or to the left (Figure 2a–c). As elaborated below, these plans initially advance at randomly drawn rates, but afterward, time permitting, they are steered by perceptual information. The exogenous signal arrives early (∼80 ms after cue onset) and rapidly accelerates the plan toward the cue regardless of task instructions; the endogenous signal arrives (∼25 ms) later to accelerate the correct plan and decelerate the incorrect one as stipulated by the task. These dynamics replicated all aspects of the behavioral data in great detail.

Here, we leverage this quantitative framework to further test a fundamental conclusion derived from that study: that the exogenous modulation of ongoing motor plans is highly stereotyped and largely independent of behavioral context. We use the antisaccade model to predict the performance that should be observed during an urgent prosaccade task (Figure 1b) assuming that this independence hypothesis is correct, and we test the parameter-free predictions in human participants performing both urgent prosaccades and urgent antisaccades. In essence, the exogenous modulation should evolve with the same timecourse regardless of task instructions, and should favor the motor plan toward the cue in the same way whatever the state of development of the competing motor plans.

To anticipate the main findings, the differences between pro- and antisaccade performance are dramatic, but can be largely accounted for by a fixed, exogenously driven response (an exogenous response, for brevity) that is either aligned or misaligned with the endogenously defined goal.

Results

A quantitative framework for urgent anti- and prosaccade performance

In our previous study (Salinas et al., 2019), we developed a neurophysiologically feasible model that quantitatively replicated the rich psychophysical dataset obtained in the compelled antisaccade task (the CAS model). Now, with a minimal modification, we generate a model of the compelled prosaccade task (the CPS model). The modification corresponds to simply making the top-down, endogenous response favor a saccade to the cue instead of away from the cue — without changing anything else.

The CAS model consists of two variables, rL and rR, that represent oculomotor activity favoring saccades toward left and right locations (Figure 2a–c, red and black traces). The first one of these motor plans to exceed a fixed threshold level (Figure 2a–c, dashed lines) triggers a saccade, to the left if rL crosses the threshold first, or to the right if rR does. In each trial, rL and rR start increasing in response to the go signal, initially advancing with randomly drawn buildup rates. This ramping process may end in a random choice (i.e., a guess; Figure 2a) if the gap interval between the go and the cue onset is long and/or one of the initial buildup rates is high enough. Otherwise, time permitting, the arrival of the cue information modifies the ongoing motor plans in two ways.

First, when the cue onset is detected by the oculomotor circuit, the motor competition is biased toward the cue location during a time period that we refer to as the exogenous response interval, or ERI (Figure 2a–c, gray vertical shades). Specifically, this means that, during the ERI, the motor plan toward the cue is first briefly halted and then accelerated (Figure 2b, c, red traces during gray interval), whereas the motor plan toward the opposite, or ‘anti’, location is halted throughout (Figure 2b, c, black traces during gray interval). These modulations of the ongoing motor activity constitute the involuntary, exogenous response.

Then, after the ERI ends, once the cue location has been interpreted in accordance to task rules as ‘opposite to the target’, the motor selection process is steered toward the correct choice. Specifically, the (erroneous) plan toward the cue is decelerated (Figure 2c, red trace after gray interval) and the (correct) plan away from the cue is accelerated (Figure 2c, black trace after gray interval). These modulations informing the correct choice constitute the top-down, endogenous response.

With these elements in place, the CAS model was able to reproduce the full range of psychophysical results obtained in the urgent antisaccade task, both on average and in individual participants (with parameter values adjusted accordingly; Salinas et al., 2019). In particular, it quantitatively matched the full RT distributions for correct and error trials (see Figure 8 and its supplements in Salinas et al., 2019). Moreover, the model simulations accurately replicated the tachometric curve (Figure 2d, top), the function relating choice accuracy to rPT, which is the key behavioral metric in such urgent tasks. In the rest of the paper, we consider the CAS model with parameters fitted to the pooled data from six participants ( see Table 1, high-luminance cue, in Salinas et al., 2019). The tachometric curve simulated for this case (Figure 2d, top) directly illustrates the conflicting interaction between exogenous and endogenous mechanisms that characterizes the CAS task: When the cue is seen for less than 90 ms or so (as in Figure 2a), the saccades are uninformed and the success rate stays near chance. When the cue is seen for more than 140 ms or so (as in Figure 2c), most saccades are informed and the success rate rises steadily above chance and toward 100% correct. But in between, performance drops to nearly 0% correct, as a large fraction of saccades are drawn to the wrong location. Those saccades are captured by the cue (as in Figure 2b).

To generate predictions about performance in prosaccade trials, we modified the CAS model so that the endogenous signal accelerated the motor plan toward the cue and decelerated the competing plan toward the anti location. That is, we simply swapped the target and distracter designations for the two locations without altering any parameter values. In this way, in the resulting CPS model the exogenous modulation during the ERI still boosts the motor plan toward the cue, just as it did originally (Figure 2f, compare to panel b). Crucially, though, those motor plans that are propelled past the threshold, which previously resulted in erroneous captured saccades, now correspond to correct choices. Furthermore, the informed endogenous signal now typically acts to reinforce the motor plan that is already ahead by the end of the ERI (Figure 2g, compare to panel c). All this results in a predicted tachometric curve for prosaccade performance for which the success rate grows steadily with rPT, rising very early and very steeply (Figure 2d, bottom).

Specific model predictions

The fundamental assumption behind the CPS model just described is that the exogenous response is entirely insensitive to behavioral context, so the endogenous signal reinforces the correct choice in exactly the same way in the pro and anti tasks — that is, the acceleration and deceleration terms simply swap their spatial assignments — regardless of the state of development of the motor alternatives. This is one extreme in the spectrum of possible outcomes; opposite to this ‘full-independence’ scenario is the possibility that the exogenous and endogenous modulations are much different in one task than in the other; and, of course, one or both of these mechanisms could also change modestly across conditions. Hence, the more general question is, to what degree are the exogenous and endogenous mechanisms sensitive to behavioral context? We use the CPS model as a benchmark because it instantiates a conceptually simple hypothesis (independence) to yield parameter-free, quantitative predictions — which, as it turns out, to a first approximation are fairly accurate.

The CAS and CPS models make two specific, clear-cut predictions. The first one is that the upswing in performance during prosaccade trials should follow the same timecourse as the initial downswing in performance during antisaccade trials (Figure 3a, b). In other words, the initial departure from chance should exhibit the same dependence on rPT but going in opposite directions, toward 100% versus toward 0% correct. This is a direct consequence of the exogenous response reinforcing the motor plan toward the cue with the same timing and intensity in the two tasks.

Figure 3. Model predictions.

Figure 3.

(a) Simulated tachometric curves for the pro- (blue trace) and antisaccade tasks (red trace). Same curves as in Figure 2d, but superimposed. (b) Same data as in a, but shown over a smaller rPT range and with the prosaccade curve inverted (blue trace) relative to the chance level. Note that the initial departure from chance follows the same timecourse for the two tasks (arrow). (c) Antisaccade tachometric curves conditioned on cue location. Traces are expected results if the participant consistently guesses in one direction and trials are split into two groups: with the cue on the preferred side (red trace) or with the cue on the nonpreferred side (black trace). Note that the rise toward asymptotic performance occurs later when the initial motor bias is in the direction of the cue. (d) As in c, but for the prosaccade task. Note that, in this case, the rise in performance is similar regardless of the initial bias.

The second prediction is about the rise toward asymptotic performance when there is a motor bias, that is, when the participant tends to guess more toward one side than the other. If such a bias exists, then during antisaccade trials in which the cue happens to appear on the preferred guessing side, the rise in success rate is expected to occur late (Figure 3c, red trace). In contrast, during antisaccade trials in which the cue happens to appear opposite to the preferred guessing side, the success rate should rise earlier (Figure 3c, black trace). This difference arises because the exogenous and endogenous modulations are always opposite, so it is more difficult for the endogenous signal to prevent a capture when the exogenous response and the motor bias reinforce each other. On the other hand, during prosaccade trials the rise in success rate is expected to be very similar regardless of where the cue appears relative to the guessing direction (Figure 3d). In this case, the exogenous and endogenous modulations are aligned and can easily overcome any bias in the initial motor plans, so the rise in success rate is always very steep.

These two predictions are complementary in that they probe bottom-up and top-down components of the selection process. The first one tests whether the sensory-driven exogenous response itself changes across tasks, whereas the second one tests whether the endogenously driven recovery depends on the state of development of the competing motor alternatives.

The exogenous response is highly insensitive to behavioral context

To test the model predictions, 10 participants (6 female, 4 male) were recruited. The experimental procedures were largely the same as in the earlier report (Salinas et al., 2019) and are summarized in Materials and methods. Any differences in approach are described there, along with analysis methods specific to this study.

All the participants performed pro and anti trials in two ways: in blocks of same-task trials, and in blocks of randomly interleaved pro- and antisaccades (Materials and methods). The experiment was designed this way because it seemed possible that the two conditions could lead to major differences in performance, via task-switching costs, for instance (Wylie and Allport, 2000; Monsell, 2003). However, the observed differences were, in fact, very small (Figure 4—figure supplement 1) and had no bearing on any of the subsequent analyses. Thus, in what follows, we consider all the trials of each task aggregated across blocked and interleaved conditions.

For the compelled antisaccade task, the pooled tachometric curve (Figure 4a, red trace), generated by combining the data across participants, replicated our earlier result (Salinas et al., 2019). It exhibited the characteristic drop in success rate due to potent oculomotor capture in the range between 90 and 140 ms, approximately, with the subsequent recovery reaching nearly 100% correct after ∼200 ms of cue viewing time. More importantly, for the compelled prosaccade task the shape of the pooled tachometric curve (Figure 4a, blue trace) was as predicted by the model. In this case, the success rate rose steadily, early, and very steeply.

Figure 4. Early departure from chance during pro- and antisaccade performance.

(a) Tachometric curves for pro- (blue trace, 24,638 trials) and antisaccade trials (red trace, 22,608 trials) combined over 10 participants. Shades indicate 95% confidence intervals (CIs) across trials. (b) Same data as in a, but shown over a smaller rPT range and with the prosaccade curve inverted (blue trace) relative to the chance level. The dotted line is the inverted prosaccade curve shifted by 3 ms along the x-axis. (c) Pro- and antisaccade curves for four individual participants. Same x and y axes as in b, and same format. (d) Optimal shift for each participant. The optimal shift (along the rPT axis) minimized the difference between the inverted pro curve and the anti curve for rPTs in the [55, 105] ms interval. The gray line marks the optimal shift (3 ms) for the pooled data in b. Error bars are 95% CIs. (e) Tachometric curves pooled from three participants tested with both the standard high-luminance cues (bright colors) and low-luminance cues (dark colors). The rPT range is truncated to better appreciate the luminance-driven right shift, which is similar for the pro and anti curves (for full range, see Figure 4—figure supplement 3).

Figure 4.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1. Performance in blocked versus interleaved trials.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1.

In all panels, colors mark data collected in blocks of same-task trials (green) or in blocks in which pro and anti trials were randomly interleaved (orange). (a) Tachometric curves for prosaccade trials pooled across participants. Data points show the fraction of correct choices for each rPT bin (bin width = 21 ms). Continuous lines are sigmoidal fits. The inset zooms into the rPT range over which performance departs sharply from chance. (b) Curve rise point, which is the rPT at which prosaccade performance is halfway between chance and asymptotic. Bars show values for the pooled tachometric curves in a, with 95% CIs (error bars, from bootstrap). Circles are individual participant data (p = 0.006 for difference between conditions, signed rank test). (c) Fraction correct at the tail of the rPT range (rPT ≥125 ms). Same format as in b. For the individual participant data, p = 0.002 (signed rank test). (d) As in a, but for antisaccade trials. (e) As in b, but for the drop point, which is the rPT at which antisaccade performance is halfway between chance and its minimum value. For the individual participant data, p = 0.2 (signed rank test). (f) As in c, but for antisaccade trials (rPT ≥190 ms). For the individual participant data, p = 0.6 (signed rank test).
Figure 4—figure supplement 2. Perceptual performance is largely invariant with respect to gap value.

Figure 4—figure supplement 2.

Each panel shows several tachometric curves superimposed. Each curve was constructed by selecting, from the complete dataset pooled across participants, only the pro or anti trials with a given gap. Then, for each gap-specific dataset, the fraction of correct responses was calculated for each rPT bin (bin width = 21 ms, with bins shifting every 1 ms), as in other figures. (a) Tachometric curves from antisaccade trials. Colors correspond to gap values (in ms), as indicated by the legend. The continuous gray line is the curve obtained by combining all the data across gaps (same as in Figure 4a, red trace). (b) As in a, but for prosaccade trials. The continuous gray curve is the same as the blue trace in Figure 4a. In general, shorter gaps tend to sample longer rPTs, leading to fewer guesses and noisier results for rPT ≲90 ms. However, although the range of gap values spans nearly 350 ms, the dependence of success on rPT is remarkably consistent; the only noticeable systematic deviation is for gap = 10 ms, for which the conditions are barely within the urgent regime.
Figure 4—figure supplement 3. Individual results for the three participants who performed the luminance experiment.

Figure 4—figure supplement 3.

Each column corresponds to one participant. (a) Tachometric curves for antisaccade trials with a high- (bright trace) or a low-luminance cue (dark trace). Shaded error bands indicate 95% CIs from binomial statistics. Vertical gray lines mark the drop points for the two curves. (b) As in a, but for prosaccade trials. Vertical gray lines mark the rise points for the two curves. For this figure, rPT bins of 41 ms were used.

We stress that the tachometric curve is the main metric in urgent tasks because it characterizes performance independently of RT (Stanford et al., 2010; Salinas et al., 2019; Stanford and Salinas, 2021; Seideman et al., 2022) or gap (Figure 4—figure supplement 2). These variables explain very little once rPT has been taken into account.

To evaluate the first prediction quantitatively, we compared the downswing in antisaccade performance to the upswing in prosaccade performance (Figure 4b). First, the pro curve was inverted so that it decreased monotonically, and then a shift analysis was carried out to determine the optimal shift along the rPT axis that would maximize the overlap between the two curves (Materials and methods). Whereas according to the prediction the optimal time shift should be zero (Figure 3b, arrow), experimentally, we found that the optimal shift was, on average, 3 ms within a 95% confidence interval (CI) of [1, 5] ms (Figure 4b, d). That is, in prosaccade trials, the effect of the exogenous response manifested about 3 ms earlier than in antisaccade trials. This time difference was of comparable magnitude across individual participants (Figure 4c), which had optimal shifts in the [−1, 10] ms range (Figure 4d). Also, the result was very similar when green and magenta cues were assigned to pro and anti trials, respectively (participants 1–5; Materials and methods), and when the color assignments were flipped (participants 6–10). In general, the upswing in prosaccade performance had just a slightly earlier onset than the downswing in antisaccade performance.

To confirm these results, we ran a variant of the experiment in which the cue could be of high or low luminance (Materials and methods). We knew that, in the antisaccade task, less luminant cues produce capture at longer processing times (Salinas et al., 2019), so we reasoned that the rise in prosaccade performance should demonstrate the same temporal dependence — if it is driven by the same exogenous signal. The low luminance level was chosen to elicit a robust rightward shift of the anti tachometric curve (Salinas et al., 2019), and the high-luminance level was identical to that in the main experiment. In this case, the cue luminance (high or low) and task type (pro or anti) were randomly and independently chosen in each trial, and data were collected from three participants.

As expected, in antisaccade trials the decrease in cue luminance resulted in a rightward shift of the tachometric curve (Figure 4e, red curves), which was of 35 ms ([33, 38] ms 95% CI), as determined by an optimal-shift analysis. And critically, a comparable delay was observed in prosaccade trials (Figure 4e, blue curves), for which the lower luminance resulted in a shift of 30 ms ([26, 34] ms 95% CI). The results were qualitatively the same for the three participants (Figure 4—figure supplement 3). In conclusion, the data indicate that, although they are not quite identical, the exogenous signal that rapidly propels prosaccade performance above chance must be very similar to the one that drives antisaccade performance below chance.

Visuomotor dynamics revealed by motor biases

The second prediction derived from the model applies when participants exhibit a motor bias, that is, a preference for looking to one side more than the other. Fortuitously, we did not have to manipulate the experimental conditions to induce such a preference because participants adopted one spontaneously.

When we analyzed our participants’ performance as a function of their prior choices, we noticed a trend: their guesses in a given trial were generally toward the location where they should have responded in the previous trial (i.e., the prior target location). The psychometric manifestation of this tendency is a systematic deviation from chance observed when the trials are sorted according to the history of prior target locations. Specifically, the tachometric curve conditioned on a repeated target location (Figure 5a, green trace) demonstrated a positive offset (above chance) in the range of processing times for which participants must guess (rPT ≲80 ms), whereas the tachometric curve conditioned on a switch in target location (Figure 5a, magenta trace) demonstrated a negative offset (below chance) in the same range. This effect was cumulative; the more target repetitions, the larger the offsets (Figure 5, compare across columns). It was also observed in most participants (Figure 5—figure supplement 1) and in both pro and anti trials.

Figure 5. Tachometric curves conditioned on target-location history.

Each panel shows choice accuracy as a function of processing time when the target location in a given trial was the same as in previous trials (green curves) or when it was different (magenta curves). Note that the participants’ guesses (rPT ≲80 ms) tended to be toward the previous target location. (a–c) Performance in the compelled antisaccade task conditioned on the history of prior target locations going back 1, 2, or 3 trials (panels a, b, c, respectively). A and B labels stand for left or right target locations, and indicate patterns of repeats (AA, AAA, AAAA) or switches in location (AB, AAB, AAAB) preceding each choice. Shaded error bands indicate 95% CIs across trials. Continuous black curves are fits to the empirical data. Gray vertical shades indicate 95% CIs for the curve rise points. (d–f) As in a–c, but for the compelled prosaccade task. Results are for data pooled across participants.

Figure 5.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1. Effect of motor bias on antisaccade performance in individual participants.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1.

(a) Rise point of the tachometric curve for trials in which the target switched locations (y-axis) versus trials in which the target location was repeated (x-axis). Colors indicate results for trial histories going back 1 (green), 2 (orange), or 3 trials (purple). Data points are for individual participants (circles) and for trials pooled across participants (triangles; from the curves in Figure 5a–c). Gray lines indicate 95% CIs from bootstrap. (b) Rise point of the tachometric curve as a function of the chance level of the same curve, which corresponds to the probability correct for guesses (rPT ≲80 ms). Data points are for individual participants (filled circles), for trials pooled across participants (triangles; from the curves in Figure 5a–c), and from simulations of the CAS model (open blue circles; blue line is linear fit to the simulated data points). Each participant contributed three points from repeat trials (magenta) and three points from switch trials (green). Gray lines join data from the same participant in a given history condition (going back 1, 2, or 3 trials). Note that, in spite of the visible individual variability, there is a clear trend across participants for a negative correlation between rise point and chance level that is highly consistent with that predicted by the model.

Given this motor bias, we analyzed the tachometric curves conditioned on prior target location and measured the rise point in each case; for anti trials, this is the rPT at which the success rate is halfway between minimum and asymptotic (Figure 5a–c, gray vertical shades), and for pro trials, it is the rPT at which the success rate is halfway between chance and asymptotic (Materials and methods). We found that both aspects of the second model prediction were correct. In the antisaccade task, the rise toward asymptotic performance occurred late when the motor bias was aligned with the cue location (Figure 5a–c, magenta curves) and early when it was aligned with the anti, or target location (Figure 5a–c, green curves); whereas in the prosaccade task, the rise was essentially the same in the two cases (Figure 5d–f). It is notable that the effect in anti trials grew stronger as the bias became more extreme, and it is instructive to calculate by how much.

Consider the tachometric curves conditioned on one previous trial (Figure 5a). As the chance level went from 0.36 ([0.34, 0.39] 95% CI, AB trials) to 0.64 ([0.61, 0.66], AA trials), the rise point of the tachometric curve went from 154 ms ([149, 154] CI, AB trials) to 143 ms ([139, 144], AA trials). Taking the ratio of the differences gives a drop of approximately 39 ms when traversing the full chance range from 0 to 1; or equivalently, if the chance level increases by 0.1, the rise point of the curve should drop by about 3.9 ms. The reason why this number is of interest is that it can also be computed for the simulated data. To do this, we simply ran the model (as in Figure 2c) with different amounts of motor bias, and for each run we tracked the chance level and the rise point of the simulated tachometric curve (Materials and methods). According to this procedure, the predicted change in rise point over the full chance range was 40 ms ([36, 44] 95% CI; Figure 5—figure supplement 1b, blue line and circles), in excellent agreement with the experimental data. Although in this analysis there was considerable variance across individual participants (Figure 5—figure supplement 1), the model prediction was very much in line with the data pooled across participants (Figure 5—figure supplement 1b, compare triangles and blue circles).

Consistency in the strength of exogenous capture across participants

As expected, in the two tasks there were noticeable differences in performance between individual participants, and analysis of this variance provided further support for the two hypotheses underlying our model predictions.

For the antisaccade task, the tachometric curves of the participants were quite similar in their initial departure from chance, as saccades were increasingly captured by the cue, but they were much more variable during the later rise in success rate (Figure 6a). Across the individual curves, the drop point (the rPT for which the success rate is halfway between chance and the minimum; Materials and methods) ranged from 87 to 98 ms, whereas the rise point ranged from 133 to 203 ms. More importantly, the values for the drop point and the rise point were not significantly correlated (Figure 6c; p= 0.6, permutation test), consistent with the idea that, in each subject, the exogenous response that determines the former is independent of the endogenous process responsible for the latter.

Figure 6. Variations in the timing of exogenous and endogenous modulation across participants.

Figure 6.

(a) Tachometric curves in the compelled antisaccade task for all 10 participants. Dotted line indicates chance performance (0.5). (b) As in a, but for the compelled prosaccade task. (c) Comparison between drop point (x-axis; time of early, exogenously driven drop in performance) and rise point (y-axis; time of later, endogenously driven rise in performance) in antisaccade trials. Each circle represents one participant. Lines indicate 95% CIs (from bootstrap). The Pearson correlation of the data (ρ) and its significance (from permutation test) are indicated. The red triangle marks data from the pooled tachometric curve. (d) Comparison between drop point in antisaccade trials (x-axis; time of early, exogenously driven drop in performance) and rise point in prosaccade trials (y-axis; time of early rise in performance). Same format as in c. Note the positive correlation between the early responses in the two tasks.

For the prosaccade task, the rise point covered a range from 82 to 95 ms across participants (Figure 6b), similar to that of the drop point in the anti task. More importantly, however, these two sets of values were significantly correlated (Figure 6d; ρ= 0.73, p= 0.004, permutation test). An alternate analysis based on optimal shifts instead of rise and drop points produced nearly identical correlation numbers. In summary, then, participants that showed an earlier departure from chance in one task also showed an earlier departure from chance in the other, consistent with the notion that, for each individual, the exogenous capture is fundamentally the same in the two tasks.

Discussion

We used the contrast between urgent pro- and antisaccade performance to test the idea that exogenous and endogenous contributions to saccadic target selection are largely independent. The cue onset and the endogenously defined goal were spatially congruent in one task but incongruent in the other, and yet, in both cases we observed a similarly steep increase in the frequency of saccades made toward the cue. As a result, there were striking differences in the proportion and timing of the correct responses across conditions (pro versus anti), but this was very much as predicted by our race model.

Dependencies between exogenous and endogenous signals could have plausibly manifested in many ways. The exogenous response in anti trials could have been visibly attenuated, delayed, or less sensitive to luminance relative to that in pro trials (Figure 4); the exogenous capture in anti trials could have been stronger in interleaved as compared to blocked conditions (Figure 4—figure supplement 1); or it could have been much weaker than predicted by the model when the ongoing motor activity pointed away from the cue rather than toward it (Figure 5). A substantial difference in any of these cases would have meant that the exogenous response was subject to internal modulation. Discrepancies were, in fact, very slight, largely consistent with the independence assumption — but this was by no means a foregone conclusion. This is not to say that robust modulation of the exogenous response is impossible, but the results circumscribe considerably the conditions under which it may occur.

Exogenous capture connects numerous visuomotor phenomena

Given our modeling framework, the mechanistic intuition behind these results is straightforward: when saccade-related neural activity is ramping up, the cue onset can accelerate that activity past the threshold beyond which a saccade is inevitably produced — before top-down information can influence the choice. When the cue itself is not the target, as in the antisaccade task, this leads to so-called captured saccades, which are errors. But when the cue itself is the target, as in the prosaccade task, these same ‘captured’ saccades are correct. In either case, the eye movement is elicited very rapidly, after 75–100 ms of viewing time. The quantitative variations in this process across conditions were tiny, consistent with the hypothesis that the resulting bias on the motor selection process is associated with the detection of the cue and is largely independent of its behavioral relevance or meaning. Perhaps an appropriate descriptor for this effect, as it is thought to occur in oculomotor circuits, is ‘exogenous capture’.

Understood this way — as an involuntary and stereotypical bias on motor planning that occurs when a stimulus is detected by oculomotor circuits — exogenous capture potentially encompasses a wide range of oculomotor phenomena (Salinas and Stanford, 2021). These include (1) oculomotor capture itself, which is observed when saccades are overtly directed toward salient distracters (Theeuwes et al., 1998; Theeuwes et al., 1999); (2) express saccades, which are saccades triggered with extremely short latencies (Kalesnykas and Hallett, 1987; Paré and Munoz, 1996; Sparks et al., 2000); (3) exogenous attention, which improves perceptual performance at explicitly cued locations (Carrasco, 2011) and has neural correlates that are highly consistent with our framework (Bisley and Goldberg, 2003; Busse et al., 2008; Herrington and Assad, 2010); and (4) attentional capture, which is typically observed when saccades to a target take longer in the presence of salient distracters (Ruz and Lupiáñez, 2002; Theeuwes, 2010).

Exogenous capture is a potentially useful construct because it represents a specific hypothesis about the neural events that determine, or at least contribute significantly to, all of these behaviors. For example, the variations in RT that define attentional capture can be easily explained by our framework (Oor et al., 2021; Salinas and Stanford, 2021). In essence, when the distracter-driven acceleration of the cue-aligned plan is relatively weak (weaker than in Figure 2c), most saccades are ultimately correct, but their onset is still delayed relative to a control condition with a less salient or absent distracter. That is, there is reason to think that in attentional capture paradigms the exogenous bias is qualitatively identical to that in the model (Figure 2b, c, f, g) but weaker, so its overt manifestation (in the RT distribution) is more subtle.

A key consideration is that, during active fixation, motor plans are strongly suppressed, so establishing a quantitative link between visual transients, motor activity, and probability of success in a task is more difficult than under urgent conditions. When motor plans are already underway, exogenous capture becomes strong and highly reliable, and its impact on performance easier to discern across tasks (Salinas et al., 2019; Oor et al., 2021; Salinas and Stanford, 2021).

Work by Poth, 2021 showed that robust capture phenomena can be generated in urgent tasks that use button presses instead of saccades and impose very different cognitive demands on the choice process. Task conditions akin to pro and anti were distinguished not by different spatial rules, but rather by contextual features that could be congruent (e.g., >>>>>) or incongruent with a central stimulus (e.g., <<><<). The qualitative similarities with our pro- and antisaccade tasks are remarkable, and suggest two critical questions to pursue more broadly: how some visuomotor associations become more reflexive than others, and how established reflexive tendencies are overcome.

No clear target for inhibitory control

Success in the antisaccade task is typically conceptualized as the result of inhibitory control. That is, it is assumed that an inhibitory process suppresses the reflexive reaction of looking at the suddenly appearing visual cue (Munoz and Everling, 2004; Wiecki and Frank, 2013; Coe and Munoz, 2017). But, what exactly is supposed to be inhibited?

Several factors determine success in an antisaccade trial. On average, weaker preparatory activity, due to a late break of fixation or to low urgency to respond, leads to lower probability of capture, consistent with single-neuron recordings in the superior colliculus and the frontal eye field (Everling et al., 1999; Everling and Munoz, 2000). A developing motor plan that, by chance, is spatially incongruent with the cue is less likely to be captured than one that is congruent (Figure 5a–c and Figure 5—figure supplement 1b), also in agreement with neural recordings (Everling et al., 1998; Everling et al., 1999; Everling and Munoz, 2000). The intensity of the exogenous response is certainly critical, with less salient cues leading to later and less pronounced capture (Figure 4e and Figure 4—figure supplement 3; Salinas et al., 2019). But as far as we can tell, there is no mechanism for preventing the exogenous burst; at most it may be slightly modulated by task conditions (Figure 4 and Figure 4—figure supplement 1; Everling et al., 1998; Everling and Munoz, 2000; see also Gu et al., 2016; Salinas and Stanford, 2018, Salinas et al., 2019; Buonocore et al., 2017; Bompas et al., 2020; Salinas and Stanford, 2021). Finally, the probability of a successful antisaccade is higher when the endogenous modulation that steers the motor plans is stronger or arrives earlier. This signal does implicate inhibition because it must decelerate the incorrect motor plan, but according to modeling results, this applies equally to any urgent saccadic choice that is perceptually informed (Stanford et al., 2010; Shankar et al., 2011; Costello et al., 2013; Salinas and Stanford, 2013; Salinas and Stanford, 2018; Salinas and Stanford, 2021; Seideman et al., 2018; Stanford and Salinas, 2021), including prosaccades. Furthermore, endogenous inhibition does not act alone, it complements endogenous excitation that is necessary for accelerating the correct motor plan.

All this indicates that the role of inhibition in antisaccade performance is likely widespread and quite nuanced (see Aron, 2007). In summary, then, we found that the capture mechanism is the same in the urgent pro- and antisaccade tasks, and that it is the consequence of a robust exogenous burst of oculomotor activity that always occurs. Therefore, it remains to be determined whether antisaccade performance depends critically on a specific circuit element being inhibited, but the obvious candidate, the exogenous response itself, is not it.

Materials and methods

In general, methods were the same as in the preceding study (Salinas et al., 2019). Here, we summarize key elements of the approach and detail new procedures that were implemented specifically for this experiment.

Subjects and setup

Experimental subjects were 10 healthy human volunteers, 4 male and 6 female, with a median age of 28 and ranging between 22 and 63 years. They were recruited from the Wake Forest School of Medicine and Wake Forest University communities. All had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. All participants provided informed written consent before the experiment. All experimental procedures were conducted with the approval of the Institutional Review Board of Wake Forest School of Medicine (IRB00035241).

As in the preceding study (Salinas et al., 2019), the experiment took place in a semi-dark room. The participants sat on an adjustable chair, with their chin and forehead supported, and viewed stimuli presented on a VIEWPixx LED monitor (VPixx Technologies Inc, Saint Bruno, Quebec, Canada; 1920 × 1200 screen resolution, 120 Hz refresh rate, 12-bit color) at a distance of 57 cm. Eye position was recorded using an EyeLink 1000 infrared camera and tracking system (SR Research, Ottawa, Canada; 1000 Hz sampling rate). For this experiment, stimulus presentation and data collection were controlled using the Psychtoolbox 3.0 package (Brainard, 1997; Kleiner et al., 2007) and custom Matlab scripts (Goldstein et al., 2022).

Behavioral tasks

The sequence of events was the same in the pro- and antisaccade tasks, and is described in Figure 1. As in the earlier study (Salinas et al., 2019), the gap values used were −200, −100, 0, 75, 100, 125, 150, 175, 200, 250, and 350 ms, where negative numbers correspond to delays in nonurgent versions of the task (Figure 1—figure supplement 1); that is, compelled and easy, nonurgent trials were interleaved. In each trial, the gap value and cue location (−8° or 8°) were randomly sampled. The cue was a circle of 1.5° diameter. Auditory feedback (a beep) was provided at the end of the trial if the saccadic choice was made within the allowed RT window (425 ms); no sound was played if the limit was exceeded. No feedback was given about the correctness of the spatial choice. The intertrial interval was 350 ms.

Data were collected in blocks of 150 trials, with 2–3 min of rest allowed between blocks. For the main experiment, each participant completed 9 or 10 blocks of prosaccade trials, 9 or 10 blocks of antisaccade trials, and 18 blocks of pro and anti trials interleaved in equal proportions. For half of the participants (P1–P5), the cue was green (RGB vector [0 0.88 0]) in prosaccade trials and magenta (RGB vector [1 0.32 1]) in antisaccade trials. For the other half (P6–P10), the color assignments were swapped. The green and magenta stimuli had the same luminance (48 cd m−2), as determined by a spectrophotometer (i1 Pro 2 from X-Rite, Inc, Grand Rapids, MI). In blocks in which pro and anti trials were interleaved, the color of the fixation point was the same as that of the cue and indicated to the participant the type of trial that was coming up. In blocks of single-task trials, the fixation point was gray (RGB vector [0.25 0.25 0.25]).

Secondary experiment

Additional data were collected in a secondary experiment in which luminance varied across trials (Figure 4e). One of the previous volunteers and two newly recruited ones participated (all female). The sequence of events in the tasks was exactly the same as in the main experiment. In this case, however, in each trial, the task type (pro or anti) and cue luminance (high or low) were independently sampled, so the four possible combinations were randomly interleaved. The color of the fixation spot, which was always the same as that of the upcoming cue, indicated the correct response: green stimuli corresponded to prosaccades and magenta stimuli to antisaccades. The green and magenta cues of high luminance (48 cd m−2) were the same as in the main experiment. The green (RGB vector [0 0.0733 0]) and magenta (RGB vector [0.0833 0.0267 0.833]) cues of low luminance (0.24 cd m−2) were chosen so that significant rightward shifts of the tachometric curve would be generated (Salinas et al., 2019).

Data analysis

All results are based on the analysis of urgent trials (gap ≥0) only; easy, nonurgent trials (gap <0) were excluded. All data analyses were carried out using customized scripts written in Matlab (The MathWorks, Natick, MA), as detailed previously (Salinas et al., 2019).

The RT was always measured as the time elapsed between the go signal (fixation offset) and saccade onset (equal to the first time point after the go for which the eye velocity exceeded a threshold of 40°/s). Trials were scored as correct or incorrect based on the direction of the first saccade made after the go signal. Trials with fixation breaks (aborts) or primarily vertical saccades were excluded from analysis. Otherwise, completed trials were scored and included even if they exceeded the allotted time limit.

Processing times were computed as rPT = RT – gap, where all quantities are specific to each trial. To generate tachometric curves, trials were grouped into rPT bins that were shifted every 1 ms, and the numbers of correct and incorrect responses were tallied for each bin. From these, we calculated the fraction of correct responses in each bin and, using binomial statistics, CIs for it. The bin size was 21 ms in all cases except Figure 4—figure supplement 3, where it was set to 41 ms.

To quantify perceptual performance, each tachometric curve was fitted with a continuous analytical function. The fits served to determine key characteristic metrics from the empirical curves, which are inherently noisy. For the prosaccade task, the fitting curve was an increasing sigmoid

s(x)=B+AB1+exp(xCD) (1)

where C is the curve rise point, that is, the rPT at which the fraction correct is halfway between chance (equal to B) and asymptotic (equal to A). For the antisaccade task, the fitting curve was defined as

v(x)=max(sL(x),sR(x),0) (2)

where the maximum function, max(a,b,c), returns the largest of a, b, or c. Also, sL and sR are two sigmoidal curves given by

sL(x)=BLR+ALBLR1+exp(xCLDL) (3)
sR(x)=BLR+ARBLR1+exp(xCRDR) (4)

where sL tracks the left (decreasing) side of the tachometric curve and sR tracks the right (increasing) side. In this case, CL is the curve drop point, that is, the rPT at which the fraction correct is halfway between chance (equal to AL) and minimum (equal to BLR), whereas CR is the curve rise point, which in this case corresponds to the rPT at which the fraction correct is halfway between the minimum (BLR) and asymptotic (AR). Previously (Salinas et al., 2019), we referred to the antisaccade rise point as the curve centerpoint, but the new nomenclature is more clear and more suitable for comparing results between pro and anti conditions.

For any given tachometric curve, parameter values (A, B, C, etc.) were found that minimized the mean absolute error between the experimental data and the analytical curves using the Matlab function fminsearch. In most cases, chance levels were fixed at 50% correct by setting B= 0.5 and AL= 0.5, so these parameters did not vary during the minimization. However, when analyzing motor biases (Figure 5), these parameters were allowed to vary.

In this study, we focus on the drop point CL and the rise points C and CR. CIs for these quantities were obtained by bootstrapping (Davison and Hinkley, 2006; Hesterberg, 2014), as done previously (Salinas et al., 2019). This involved resampling the trial-wise data with replacement, refitting the resulting (resampled) tachometric curves, storing the new parameter values, and repeating the process many times to generate parameter distributions. Reported 95% CIs correspond to the 2.5 and 97.5 percentiles obtained from the bootstrapped distributions based on 1000–10,000 iterations.

In addition to comparisons between C, CL, and CR values, to evaluate differences in timing between two tachometric curves (as in Figure 4b–d), we also performed a time-shift analysis, which was as follows. First, one curve, f1(x), was designated as the reference and remained fixed. Then, transformed versions of the second curve, f2(x), were considered that would minimize the absolute difference between it and the reference curve over a range of x values (i.e., rPTs). Three transforming operations were considered: a shift along the y-axis (i.e., a change in baseline), a change in gain, and a shift along the x-axis. In practice, values of the parameters Δb, g, and Δx were found that minimized the error

E= | g(Δb+f2(x+Δx))f1(x) |  (5)

where the angle brackets indicate an average over all rPT values in the specified range. For identical tachometric curves f1 and f2 the optimal parameters would be Δb= 0, g= 1, and Δx= 0. Although the quantity of interest is the relative time shift between the curves, Δx, the gain factor and change in baseline are included to account for the fact that the two curves may saturate at somewhat different levels, or may be slightly offset in the vertical direction from each other, which would tend to overestimate the magnitude of the optimal shift, that is, the Δx for which the error is minimized. Notably, the effect of Δb and g was modest; results varied by approximately 2 ms when the procedure allowed variations in Δx only, in which case Δb and g were fixed at 0 and 1, respectively.

This shift analysis applies to curves f1 and f2 that have similar shapes. To compare the early departure from chance in pro- versus antisaccade curves, the prosaccade curve was first inverted (Figure 4b, c) relative to the chance level by applying the transformation f(x)1-f(x). Then the above procedure for determining Δx was carried out. CIs for Δx were again obtained by bootstrapping, that is, by resampling the data with replacement, recalculating the tachometric curves f1 and f2, and rerunning the entire minimization process. In this way, by iterating this resampling procedure many times, distributions of Δx values were generated.

Model simulations

Predictions were made with the CAS and CPS models as explained in Results. The CAS model is the exact same accelerated race-to-threshold model developed previously (for parameter values, see Table 1 in Salinas et al., 2019, high-luminance cue condition). The CAS model thus represents typical, or average, urgent antisaccade performance, as it replicates the psychometric results obtained by pooling all the trials from the six subjects that participated in the prior experiment. The Matlab scripts for simulating the CAS and CPS models differed by a single sign that determined whether the target location was equal to the cue location or opposite to it. Thus, for a given set of parameters, the exogenous and endogenous modulations produced by the two models were the same; the only thing that varied was which plan was accelerated and which decelerated endogenously. To generate predictions, the CPS model was run with the same parameter values as the CAS model.

In standard model simulations with no motor bias, the two initial buildup rates for the competing motor plans were drawn from a bivariate distribution and were assigned randomly to the cue and anti motor plans. Model runs in which a motor bias was included proceeded in the same way, with initial buildup rates drawn from the same bivariate distribution, but these were assigned to the cue and the anti plans with a biased probability. For example, to generate a bias of 0.1 in favor of the cue location, in each simulated trial the larger of the two buildup rates was assigned to the cue side with a 60% probability. Biases of arbitrary magnitude favoring one direction or the other were generated this way.

Acknowledgements

We thank Denise Anderson for technical and logistical assistance.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection, and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Emilio Salinas, Email: esalinas@wakehealth.edu.

Daeyeol Lee, Johns Hopkins University, United States.

Timothy E Behrens, University of Oxford, United Kingdom.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • National Eye Institute R01EY025172 to Terrence R Stanford, Emilio Salinas.

  • National Institute of Mental Health R21MH120784 to Terrence R Stanford, Emilio Salinas.

  • National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke T32NS073553 to Allison T Goldstein.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

No competing interests declared.

Reviewing editor, eLife.

Author contributions

Data curation, Software, Formal analysis, Investigation, Writing – review and editing.

Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Writing – original draft, Project administration, Writing – review and editing.

Conceptualization, Data curation, Software, Formal analysis, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Writing – original draft, Project administration, Writing – review and editing.

Ethics

All participants provided informed written consent before the experiment. All experimental procedures were conducted with the approval of the Institutional Review Board of Wake Forest School of Medicine (IRB00035241).

Additional files

Transparent reporting form

Data availability

The trial-by-trial behavioral data that support the findings of this study are publicly available from Zenodo, https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.6757621. Matlab scripts for reproducing analysis results are included as part of the shared data package.

The following dataset was generated:

Goldstein AT, Stanford TR, Salinas E. 2022. Dataset: Exogenous capture accounts for fundamental differences between prosaccade and antisaccade performance. Zenodo.

References

  1. Anderson EJ, Husain M, Sumner P. Human intraparietal sulcus (IPS) and competition between exogenous and endogenous saccade plans. NeuroImage. 2008;40:838–851. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.10.046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Aron AR. The neural basis of inhibition in cognitive control. The Neuroscientist. 2007;13:214–228. doi: 10.1177/1073858407299288. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bisley JW, Goldberg ME. Neuronal activity in the lateral intraparietal area and spatial attention. Science. 2003;299:81–86. doi: 10.1126/science.1077395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bompas A, Campbell AE, Sumner P. Cognitive control and automatic interference in mind and brain: A unified model of saccadic inhibition and countermanding. Psychological Review. 2020;127:524–561. doi: 10.1037/rev0000181. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Brainard DH. The psychophysics toolbox. Spatial Vision. 1997;10:433–436. doi: 10.1163/156856897X00357. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Brown MRG, Vilis T, Everling S. Frontoparietal activation with preparation for antisaccades. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2007;98:1751–1762. doi: 10.1152/jn.00460.2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Buonocore A, Purokayastha S, McIntosh RD. Saccade reorienting is facilitated by pausing the oculomotor program. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2017;29:2068–2080. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Busse L, Katzner S, Treue S. Temporal dynamics of neuronal modulation during exogenous and endogenous shifts of visual attention in macaque area MT. PNAS. 2008;105:16380–16385. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0707369105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Carrasco M. Visual attention: the past 25 years. Vision Research. 2011;51:1484–1525. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2011.04.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Coe BC, Munoz DP. Mechanisms of saccade suppression revealed in the anti-saccade task. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences. 2017;372:20160192. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2016.0192. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Costello MG, Zhu D, Salinas E, Stanford TR. Perceptual modulation of motor--but not visual--responses in the frontal eye field during an urgent-decision task. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33:16394–16408. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1899-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Davison AC, Hinkley D. Bootstrap Methods and Their Applications. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2006. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  13. Everling S, Dorris MC, Munoz DP. Reflex suppression in the anti-saccade task is dependent on prestimulus neural processes. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1998;80:1584–1589. doi: 10.1152/jn.1998.80.3.1584. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Everling S, Dorris MC, Klein RM, Munoz DP. Role of primate superior colliculus in preparation and execution of anti-saccades and pro-saccades. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1999;19:2740–2754. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-07-02740.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Everling S, Munoz DP. Neuronal correlates for preparatory set associated with pro-saccades and anti-saccades in the primate frontal eye field. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2000;20:387–400. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-01-00387.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Fielding J, Kilpatrick T, Millist L, White O. Antisaccade performance in patients with multiple sclerosis. Cortex; a Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior. 2009;45:900–903. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.02.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Goldstein AT, Stanford TR, Salinas E. Software for running the urgent pro- and antisaccade tasks. Zenodo. 2022 doi: 10.5281/zenodo.6784836. [DOI]
  18. Gu C, Wood DK, Gribble PL, Corneil BD. A trial-by-trial window into sensorimotor transformations in the human motor periphery. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2016;36:8273–8282. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0899-16.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Herrington TM, Assad JA. Temporal sequence of attentional modulation in the lateral intraparietal area and middle temporal area during rapid covert shifts of attention. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30:3287–3296. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6025-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Hesterberg TC. What teachers should know about the bootstrap: resampling in the undergraduate statistics curriculum. arXiv. 2014 doi: 10.1080/00031305.2015.1089789. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  21. Hutton SB, Ettinger U. The antisaccade task as a research tool in psychopathology: a critical review. Psychophysiology. 2006;43:302–313. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2006.00403.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Itti L, Koch C. Computational modelling of visual attention. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2001;2:194–203. doi: 10.1038/35058500. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Johansson ME, Cameron IGM, Van der Kolk NM, de Vries NM, Klimars E, Toni I, Bloem BR, Helmich RC. Aerobic exercise alters brain function and structure in parkinson’s disease: A randomized controlled trial. Annals of Neurology. 2022;91:203–216. doi: 10.1002/ana.26291. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Kalesnykas RP, Hallett PE. The differentiation of visually guided and anticipatory saccades in gap and overlap paradigms. Experimental Brain Research. 1987;68:115–121. doi: 10.1007/BF00255238. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Klein C, Foerster F. Development of prosaccade and antisaccade task performance in participants aged 6 to 26 years. Psychophysiology. 2001;38:179–189. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Klein CH, Raschke A, Brandenbusch A. Development of pro- and antisaccades in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and healthy controls. Psychophysiology. 2003;40:17–28. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Kleiner M, Brainard D, Pelli D. What’s New in Psychtoolbox-3? Perception. 2007;36:1–16. [Google Scholar]
  28. Monsell S. Task switching. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2003;7:134–140. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(03)00028-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Munoz DP, Everling S. Look away: the anti-saccade task and the voluntary control of eye movement. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2004;5:218–228. doi: 10.1038/nrn1345. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Oor EE, Salinas E, Stanford TR. Encoding of salience and informed choice colludes and conflicts across FEF neuronal types in the guidance of urgent saccadic decisions. Program Number P491. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts.2021. [Google Scholar]
  31. Paré M, Munoz DP. Saccadic reaction time in the monkey: advanced preparation of oculomotor programs is primarily responsible for express saccade occurrence. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1996;76:3666–3681. doi: 10.1152/jn.1996.76.6.3666. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Poth CH. Urgency forces stimulus-driven action by overcoming cognitive control. eLife. 2021;10:e73682. doi: 10.7554/eLife.73682. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Ruz M, Lupiáñez J. A review of attentional capture: On its automaticity and sensitivity to endogenous control. Psicológica. 2002;23:283–309. [Google Scholar]
  34. Salinas E, Stanford TR. The countermanding task revisited: fast stimulus detection is a key determinant of psychophysical performance. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33:5668–5685. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3977-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Salinas E, Stanford TR. Saccadic inhibition interrupts ongoing oculomotor activity to enable the rapid deployment of alternate movement plans. Scientific Reports. 2018;8:14163. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-32224-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Salinas E, Steinberg BR, Sussman LA, Fry SM, Hauser CK, Anderson DD, Stanford TR. Voluntary and involuntary contributions to perceptually guided saccadic choices resolved with millisecond precision. eLife. 2019;8:e46359. doi: 10.7554/eLife.46359. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Salinas E, Stanford TR. Under time pressure, the exogenous modulation of saccade plans is ubiquitous, intricate, and lawful. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2021;70:154–162. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2021.10.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Seideman JA, Stanford TR, Salinas E. Saccade metrics reflect decision-making dynamics during urgent choices. Nature Communications. 2018;9:2907. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-05319-w. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Seideman JA, Stanford TR, Salinas E. A conflict between spatial selection and evidence accumulation in area LIP. Nature Communications. 2022 doi: 10.1038/s41467-022-32209-z. In press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Shankar S, Massoglia DP, Zhu D, Costello MG, Stanford TR, Salinas E. Tracking the temporal evolution of a perceptual judgment using a compelled-response task. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:8406–8421. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1419-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Sparks D, Rohrer WH, Zhang Y. The role of the superior colliculus in saccade initiation: a study of express saccades and the gap effect. Vision Research. 2000;40:2763–2777. doi: 10.1016/s0042-6989(00)00133-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Stanford TR, Shankar S, Massoglia DP, Costello MG, Salinas E. Perceptual decision making in less than 30 milliseconds. Nature Neuroscience. 2010;13:379–385. doi: 10.1038/nn.2485. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Stanford TR, Salinas E. Urgent decision making: Resolving visuomotor interactions at high temporal resolution. Annual Review of Vision Science. 2021;7:323–348. doi: 10.1146/annurev-vision-100419-103842. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Theeuwes J, Kramer AF, Hahn S, Irwin DE. Our eyes do not always go where we want them to go: Capture of the eyes by new objects. Psychological Science. 1998;9:379–385. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00071. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  45. Theeuwes J, Kramer AF, Hahn S, Irwin DE, Zelinsky GJ. Influence of attentional capture on oculomotor control. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 1999;25:1595–1608. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.25.6.1595. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Theeuwes J. Top-down and bottom-up control of visual selection. Acta Psychologica. 2010;135:77–99. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.02.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Wiecki TV, Frank MJ. A computational model of inhibitory control in frontal cortex and basal ganglia. Psychological Review. 2013;120:329–355. doi: 10.1037/a0031542. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Wolfe JM, Horowitz TS. Five factors that guide attention in visual search. Nature Human Behaviour. 2017;1:0058. doi: 10.1038/s41562-017-0058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Wylie G, Allport A. Task switching and the measurement of “switch costs.”. Psychological Research. 2000;63:212–233. doi: 10.1007/s004269900003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Editor's evaluation

Daeyeol Lee 1

When subjects are instructed to produce saccades away from suddenly appearing visual targets under time pressure, early saccades tend to be directed incorrectly to the peripheral target, suggesting that exogenous and endogenous signals that are related to the target position and instruction, respectively, compete to control the motor responses. In this study, the authors provide further evidence for the independence of these two processes by showing that they can account for the temporal evolution of correct saccades regardless of the instruction, stimulus luminance or motor bias.

Decision letter

Editor: Daeyeol Lee1
Reviewed by: Daeyeol Lee2

Our editorial process produces two outputs: (i) public reviews designed to be posted alongside the preprint for the benefit of readers; (ii) feedback on the manuscript for the authors, including requests for revisions, shown below. We also include an acceptance summary that explains what the editors found interesting or important about the work.

Decision letter after peer review:

Thank you for submitting your article "Exogenous capture accounts for fundamental differences between prosaccade and antisaccade performance" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by 3 peer reviewers, including Daeyeol Lee as Reviewing Editor and Reviewer #1, and the evaluation has been overseen by Timothy Behrens as the Senior Editor.

The reviewers have discussed their reviews with one another, and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this to help you prepare a revised submission.

Essential revisions:

(1) For the trials with low luminance, the asymptotic level of accuracy remained quite low (Figure 4e). Is this because the results are truncated? It might be useful to show the results more completely.

(2) It seems odd that the initial motor bias for small rPT was absent for pro-saccade trials (Figure 5d). Is there any explanation?

(3) The Introduction is devoted almost entirely to describing one or two past studies by the current authors. There is substantial literature on anti-saccades in humans and NHP. The Introduction should orient the reader to this literature and set up the questions addressed in the current study. As it stands, the Introduction does little to frame the issues for a general audience, or even for readers who specialize in oculomotor behavior and neurophysiology.

Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

Line 1. "Oculomotor circuits generate eye movements based on the physical salience of objects and current behavioral goals, exogenous and endogenous influences, respectively." This is an interesting hypothesis, but it should not be stated as a matter of fact. Salience and behavioral goals are only some of the factors that influence eye movements.

Line 18. "The oculomotor system of primates chooses a new target to look at every 200-250 ms." Is there literature to support this? Are all eye movements the result of a "choice?"

Line 20. "Physical salience" is a questionable construction. Salience can be computed from physical properties of the stimulus, but that doesn't mean it is itself a physical property.

Line 28. Please change “commands” to “instructs”.

Line 34. Plotting accuracy as a function of reaction time is not a new concept. It's a conventional speed-accuracy trade-off. Inventing new terms for established ideas is not helpful. Chronometry or chronometric function is a conventional term for relating reaction time to performance, or, in general, for discussing the time dependence of mental processes. Unless a compelling case can be made for distinguishing "tachometry" from "chronometry," please use the latter.

Line 37. "Attentional vortex" seems like a poor word choice. Is there a rotational component to this phenomenon?

Line 46. It is remarkable that the term "race-to-threshold" could be used without a single reference to the vast literature dealing with this class of model. For example, Noorani and Carpenter 2011 adapted the LATER model to predict error rates v. reaction time for anti-saccades. Their model had 3 ramp-to-threshold processes, so it was basically a race model. Reviewed here: https://doi.org/10.3389/fnint.2014.00067

Line 55. This compound sentence is very difficult to parse.

Line 68. How are "compelled" movements related to "urgent" movements. Also, please change "compel" to "instruct" or something more appropriate. Human subjects can't be compelled.

Figure 2 a/e. It appears that the saccades in these examples would be initiated before the cue appears, i.e. they have a negative "raw processing time." Were such saccades actually observed in the human data? If not, what held subjects in check during the gap interval.

Figure 2d. If the height of the gray rectangles is different, can one assume that the proportion of pro:anti saccades was not 50:50? This isn't stated.

Behavioral task:

What was the criterion for classifying a trial as correct or incorrect? Was every trial scored as correct or incorrect, or were there other outcome categories, e.g. "no response" if the time limit was exceeded? If other outcomes were considered, how were these incorporated into the calculation of percent correct?

It would be nice to show the effect of gap duration on performance, both in terms of fraction correct and response time distributions.

Model:

It isn't clear how the cue is modeled or how it interacts with the endogenous preparatory signals. Is the cue represented by a square wave pulse? Some other function? Does it interact additively or multiplicatively? How exactly does it cause the endogenous signals to accelerate or decelerate?

Does the model account for reaction time distributions?

Line 408. Are the analytical functions in equations 1-4 derived from the race model? Why are fits shown in Figure 5, but not Figures3 and 4?

Are there any figures showing the race model fit to the empirical data?

Discussion:

Line 263. "We used the contrast between urgent pro- and antisaccade performance to test the idea that exogenous and endogenous contributions to saccadic target selection are largely independent." Given equal prior probabilities of A and B in an AB binary choice, is there any plausible scenario in which the endogenous and exogenous contributions would be inter-dependent? Can the authors provide an example?

Line 314. "exogenous response?" Did the authors mean "response to the exogenous cue?" It isn't clear that a response can ever be exogenous.

Line 308. The section that starts here undermines the conceptual clarity of the model. As this reviewer understands it, inhibition in the model serves to decelerate endogenous movement plans, as stated on line 323. This section packs in a lot of ideas that seem intended to clarify the idea of response inhibition, but it needs more work.

The "exogenous capture" described by the authors seems reminiscent of that reported by Bisley and Goldberg 2003.

How do the current results relate to the literature on counter-manding, which employs very similar race models.

Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

Lines 82-83: Is the "exogenous response interval" (ERI) in this manuscript the equivalent of the "attentional vortex" in Stanford et al., 2019 eLife?

Lines 319 and 322: Everling's last name is misspelt ("Evering") for his 2000 paper with Munoz.

Line 353: Were any of the subjects authors?

Line 400: I might have missed this in previous papers…is there evidence that the behavioral effects scale with gap duration for urgent gaps? I.e., does a gap of -200 ms produce larger effects than a gap of -100 and/or 0 ms?

eLife. 2022 Jul 27;11:e76964. doi: 10.7554/eLife.76964.sa2

Author response


Essential revisions:

(1) For the trials with low luminance, the asymptotic level of accuracy remained quite low (Figure 4e). Is this because the results are truncated? It might be useful to show the results more completely.

We truncated the results to highlight the precise bifurcation points of the high versus low luminance curves; however, indeed, truncating the curves cuts off the late rise in antisaccade performance. The full curves for these low luminance trials are shown above. The low luminance curves reach the same level of asymptotic accuracy as the high luminance curves, albeit ~40 ms later. The figure caption now notes the reason for using a limited rPT range in this case, and the full curves for the 3 participants in this experiment are now shown in a supplementary figure (Figure S3).

(2) It seems odd that the initial motor bias for small rPT was absent for pro-saccade trials (Figure 5d). Is there any explanation?

We do not know for sure, but there are two things to note about the motor biases. One is that they are quite idiosyncratic (see Figure S4); participants may demonstrate a strong bias in both tasks, in one of them, or in neither. In Figure 5, data are pooled across participants, so the individual extremes are hidden, but a couple of subjects did show strong biases in prosaccade trials in the condition of Figure 5d. So, the effect was definitely present in some cases. The second general observation is that prosaccades are perceptually easier than antisaccades. Participants can go along with the exogenous pull of the cue and, in effect, have more time to make a prosaccade than an antisaccade. Perhaps this is partly why motor biases were generally weaker and less consistent in the prosaccade task compared to the antisaccade. All this, however, is quite speculative.

(3) The Introduction is devoted almost entirely to describing one or two past studies by the current authors. There is substantial literature on anti-saccades in humans and NHP. The Introduction should orient the reader to this literature and set up the questions addressed in the current study. As it stands, the Introduction does little to frame the issues for a general audience, or even for readers who specialize in oculomotor behavior and neurophysiology.

This being a Research Advance, the Introduction had been crafted with just the essential context (which was provided by the earlier article), but the point is well taken; it becomes an easier read with a broader scope if this literature is acknowledged. The new second paragraph of the Introduction now highlights some of this important prior work, and puts ours in a wider context.

Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

Line 1. "Oculomotor circuits generate eye movements based on the physical salience of objects and current behavioral goals, exogenous and endogenous influences, respectively." This is an interesting hypothesis, but it should not be stated as a matter of fact. Salience and behavioral goals are only some of the factors that influence eye movements.

The sentence was edited to make it clear that exogenous and endogenous influences are not the only factors driving eye movements.

Line 18. "The oculomotor system of primates chooses a new target to look at every 200-250 ms." Is there literature to support this? Are all eye movements the result of a "choice?"

We’re not quite sure what is the objection to the term “choice” here, as it simply refers to the selection of one of many alternatives. We now say that “the oculomotor system specifies a new target to look at,” which is perhaps more neutral.

Line 20. "Physical salience" is a questionable construction. Salience can be computed from physical properties of the stimulus, but that doesn't mean it is itself a physical property.

Thanks for noting this. We now refer simply to “salience,” pointing out that it typically depends on key physical properties of the stimulus.

Line 28. Please change “commands” to “instructs”.

This has been changed.

Line 34. Plotting accuracy as a function of reaction time is not a new concept. It's a conventional speed-accuracy trade-off. Inventing new terms for established ideas is not helpful. Chronometry or chronometric function is a conventional term for relating reaction time to performance, or, in general, for discussing the time dependence of mental processes. Unless a compelling case can be made for distinguishing "tachometry" from "chronometry," please use the latter.

While the chronometric function is an established convention for relating RT to performance, it does not fit our analyses here. In general, RT and processing time are different, and more to the point, the curve relating performance to processing time has vastly different properties from that based on RT; in particular, it eliminates the speed-accuracy tradeoff to yield a behavioral metric that fundamentally reflects perceptual capacity only (Salinas et al., 2014, 2019; Salinas and Stanford, 2021; Stanford and Salinas, 2021). For that reason, a new naming convention needed to be established, and was done some time ago (Stanford et al., 2010).

Line 37. "Attentional vortex" seems like a poor word choice. Is there a rotational component to this phenomenon?

The mention to that term was eliminated.

Line 46. It is remarkable that the term "race-to-threshold" could be used without a single reference to the vast literature dealing with this class of model. For example, Noorani and Carpenter 2011 adapted the LATER model to predict error rates v. reaction time for anti-saccades. Their model had 3 ramp-to-threshold processes, so it was basically a race model. Reviewed here: https://doi.org/10.3389/fnint.2014.00067

The race is a useful analogy for the oculomotor mechanism that triggers saccades, but this is simply an experimental fact that applies to any saccadic choice. Prior antisaccade models, including the LATER implementation, are of little relevance to the work presented here because they do not consider the urgent regime. This situation is very different from the standard, non-urgent experiment. In any case, we now use the term “saccade competition model,” to avoid unintended implications.

Line 55. This compound sentence is very difficult to parse.

We tweaked the sentence, which we think is more clear now.

Line 68. How are "compelled" movements related to "urgent" movements. Also, please change "compel" to "instruct" or something more appropriate. Human subjects can't be compelled.

The “compelled” task naming convention has been previously published, dating back to Stanford et al., 2010, and in numerous subsequent publications. To compel someone is to pressure them into doing something they wouldn’t necessarily do spontaneously. We believe “compelled” is a highly appropriate term to use here because it conveys the somewhat unintuitive nature of the task, namely, that one must respond before knowing what the correct answer is, something that wouldn’t normally happen unless the right task conditions are in place.

Figure 2 a/e. It appears that the saccades in these examples would be initiated before the cue appears, i.e. they have a negative "raw processing time." Were such saccades actually observed in the human data? If not, what held subjects in check during the gap interval.

Indeed, saccades with negative raw processing times were observed in the human data. For long gap lengths, participants sometimes respond before the cue is revealed (as intended). Figures report only saccades with positive processing times because, while they do occur, such early guesses are infrequent and not particularly informative.

Figure 2d. If the height of the gray rectangles is different, can one assume that the proportion of pro:anti saccades was not 50:50? This isn't stated.

The proportion of pro:anti saccades was 50:50, as is now stated in the Methods. Note, however, that even if the proportion had been different, this would have had no effect on the chance level. The figure caption now specifies that chance was 50% in both tasks.

Behavioral task:

What was the criterion for classifying a trial as correct or incorrect? Was every trial scored as correct or incorrect, or were there other outcome categories, e.g. "no response" if the time limit was exceeded? If other outcomes were considered, how were these incorporated into the calculation of percent correct?

Correct versus incorrect saccades were scored based on the direction of the first saccade made after the go signal. Completed trials were scored even if they exceeded the time limit; to the participants, going past the time limit simply resulted in different feedback (no sound). Aborted trials, including those with fixation breaks or primarily vertical saccades, were excluded from analysis. These details are now included in the Methods.

It would be nice to show the effect of gap duration on performance, both in terms of fraction correct and response time distributions.

The effect of the gap has been documented at length in previous publications, and the bottom line is that, once processing time has been taken into account, the effect of the gap on performance is very small. This is now mentioned around line 188. We think elaborating too much on the gap would be rather distracting of the key findings, which focus on processing time. However, we appreciate that similar questions may occur to other readers (e.g., see the last comment from Reviewer #3), so we have added a supplementary figure that illustrates the strong invariance of the tachometric curve relative to gap sampling (Figure S3).

Model:

It isn't clear how the cue is modeled or how it interacts with the endogenous preparatory signals. Is the cue represented by a square wave pulse? Some other function? Does it interact additively or multiplicatively? How exactly does it cause the endogenous signals to accelerate or decelerate?

The model is described in detail in Salinas et al., (2019), and is very similar to prior instantiations designed to replicate urgent choice tasks (Salinas et al., 2010; Stanford et al., 2010; Shankar et al., 2011). For the purposes of this model, there is no function representing the cue itself. Rather, we consider the effect that the cue must eventually exert on the motor plans. We assume that the cue information arrives at the motor planning circuit (akin to, say, FEF) at a certain moment in time, at which point the plans start being modulated accordingly; so, thought of as a function of time, this would correspond to a step function representing the time at which the acceleration and deceleration turn on.

Does the model account for reaction time distributions?

Yes, it reproduces with remarkable detail the RT distributions for correct and incorrect trials, which have a very rich structure. This has been documented for similar urgent tasks (Salinas et al., 2010; Stanford et al., 2010; Shankar et al., 2011; Salinas and Stanford, 2013) and for the compelled antisaccade task in particular (Salinas et al., 2019). This is now mentioned around line 110.

Line 408. Are the analytical functions in equations 1-4 derived from the race model?

No, they are entirely independent. They are simply a means to quantify perceptual performance, i.e., to determine key characteristic metrics from the empirical tachometric curves, which are noisy. This is now stated in the Methods, just before the equations. Note, however, that the fits represent only some of the methods used for analysis of the data.

Why are fits shown in Figure 5, but not Figures3 and 4?

Figure 3 shows simulated tachometric curves. No fits are necessary to determine their characteristic metrics. In Figure 4, none of the key metrics derived from the fits are shown or discussed; that just wasn’t necessary. Analyses based on the fits produced consistent results, though.

Are there any figures showing the race model fit to the empirical data?

In Salinas et al., (2019), we did precisely this type of analysis on an individual participant basis for the CAS model. In the current study, the model fits are also good but they do not add anything critical. The key here is the qualitative difference between the pro- and antisaccade conditions, which is essentially independent of any parameter settings. So, in short, yes there are, but we think they would be more of a distraction than anything else.

Discussion:

Line 263. "We used the contrast between urgent pro- and antisaccade performance to test the idea that exogenous and endogenous contributions to saccadic target selection are largely independent." Given equal prior probabilities of A and B in an AB binary choice, is there any plausible scenario in which the endogenous and exogenous contributions would be inter-dependent? Can the authors provide an example?

Interactions between exogenous and endogenous contributions might have been expected based on several arguments, and could have manifested in many ways. The exogenous response in anti trials could have been strongly attenuated, strongly delayed, or much less sensitive to luminance relative to that for the pro, but the differences were actually minimal. The exogenous capture during anti trials could have been much stronger in interleaved as compared to blocked conditions (due to adaptation or switching effects). Similarly, the capture could have been much stronger when the ongoing motor activity was in the direction of the cue location rather than in the opposite direction (due to attention enhancing visual sensitivity). Any one of these effects would have meant that the exogenous response was, in some way, modulated by non-sensory factors, but in fact, the results were consistent with it being largely impervious to all such conditions. We are looking at a signal that is highly localized in time (rPT ~90-140 ms) and that hadn’t been examined in isolation before (separately from antisaccade errors at long rPTs), so this was by no means a foregone conclusion. The second paragraph of the Discussion now makes this point.

Line 314. "exogenous response?" Did the authors mean "response to the exogenous cue?" It isn't clear that a response can ever be exogenous.

A response (whether neuronal or behavioral) can be driven exogenously or endogenously. “Exogenously driven response” has been purposefully shortened to “exogenous response,” as is now noted in the last paragraph of the Introduction, line 78.

Line 308. The section that starts here undermines the conceptual clarity of the model. As this reviewer understands it, inhibition in the model serves to decelerate endogenous movement plans, as stated on line 323. This section packs in a lot of ideas that seem intended to clarify the idea of response inhibition, but it needs more work.

In a sense, the objective of the section is just the opposite! — to point out that the idea of response inhibition is somewhat ill-defined. More specifically, it is not at all clear what, if anything, is being inhibited during anti trials that isn’t during pro trials. This section was heavily revised, so hopefully the message is more evident now.

The "exogenous capture" described by the authors seems reminiscent of that reported by Bisley and Goldberg 2003.

Yes, we believe they are related. Bisley and Goldberg analyzed activity from visually responsive neurons in area LIP that are strong candidates for mediating the sorts of exogenously driven behavioral effects discussed here. We now refer to this work in the Discussion, line 320.

How do the current results relate to the literature on counter-manding, which employs very similar race models.

This issue is discussed at length in a previous analysis of the countermanding task (Salinas and Stanford, 2013), which shows that much of the phenomenology in that task can be understood in terms of processing time. There are likely important mechanistic similarities between the countermanding and antisaccade tasks (for example, compare our data to Bompas et al., 2020), however, the exogenous contribution in the former is harder to isolate because so much of the dynamics remains covert. For that reason, the parallels are somewhat speculative. However, we have included our countermanding paper in the references mentioned when discussing similar mechanisms, line 362.

Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

Lines 82-83: Is the "exogenous response interval" (ERI) in this manuscript the equivalent of the "attentional vortex" in Stanford et al., 2019 eLife?

The ERI and the attentional vortex are not the same, but they are closely related. Both here and in Salinas et al., (2019), the ERI is defined as the time during which the motor competition is biased in favor of the cue location — in the model — so it is essentially a model parameter. In contrast, the vortex was meant to delineate the range of rPTs where captures are highly likely based on the tachometric curve, so it is an empirical construct. However, there is a direct connection: the longer the ERI, the deeper and wider the vortex. Here, we have removed the term “attentional vortex,” as it wasn’t critical and may have rubbed some readers the wrong way.

Lines 319 and 322: Everling's last name is misspelt ("Evering") for his 2000 paper with Munoz.

Thanks for noting the typo. It has been fixed.

Line 353: Were any of the subjects authors?

No, none of the subjects were authors.

Line 400: I might have missed this in previous papers…is there evidence that the behavioral effects scale with gap duration for urgent gaps? I.e., does a gap of -200 ms produce larger effects than a gap of -100 and/or 0 ms?

Essentially no, and that is one cool property of the tachometric curve! In the urgent range, the dependence of accuracy on time (rPT) is largely the same regardless of the gap duration (only a tiny deviation is generally found as one approaches gap=0). Each gap samples one relatively limited part of the same tachometric curve, and when all the gap snapshots are combined, this curve is revealed to its full extent. We have examined the dependence on gap in different ways in previous papers, but here we have added a supplementary figure that most directly illustrates the answer to the question posed by the reviewer (Figure S3).

References

Bompas A, Campbell AE, Sumner P (2020) Cognitive control and automatic interference in mind and brain: A unified model of saccadic inhibition and countermanding. Psychol Rev 127:524-561.

Salinas E, Scerra VE, Hauser CK, Costello MG, Stanford TR (2014) Decoupling speed and accuracy in an urgent decision-making task reveals multiple contributions to their trade-off. Front Neurosci 8:85.

Shankar S, Massoglia DP, Zhu D, Costello MG, Stanford TR, Salinas E (2011) Tracking the temporal evolution of a perceptual judgment using a compelled-response task. J Neurosci 31:8406-8421.

Salinas E, Stanford TR (2013) The countermanding task revisited: fast stimulus detection is a key determinant of psychophysical performance. J Neurosci 33:5668.

Salinas E, Stanford TR (2021) Under time pressure, the exogenous modulation of saccade plans is ubiquitous, intricate, and lawful. Curr Opin Neurobiol 70:154-162.

Salinas E, Shankar S, Costello MG, Zhu D, Stanford TR (2010) Waiting is the hardest part: comparison of two computational strategies for performing a compelled-response rask. Front Comput Neurosci 4:153.

Salinas E, Steinberg BR, Sussman LA, Fry SM, Hauser CK, Anderson DD, Stanford TR (2019) Voluntary and involuntary contributions to perceptually guided saccadic choices resolved with millisecond precision. eLife 8.

Stanford TR, Salinas E (2021) Urgent decision making: resolving visuomotor interactions at high temporal resolution. Annu Rev Vis Sci 7:323-348.

Stanford TR, Shankar S, Massoglia DP, Costello MG, Salinas E (2010) Perceptual decision making in less than 30 milliseconds. Nature Neuroscience 13:379-385.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Data Citations

    1. Goldstein AT, Stanford TR, Salinas E. 2022. Dataset: Exogenous capture accounts for fundamental differences between prosaccade and antisaccade performance. Zenodo. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]

    Supplementary Materials

    Transparent reporting form

    Data Availability Statement

    The trial-by-trial behavioral data that support the findings of this study are publicly available from Zenodo, https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.6757621. Matlab scripts for reproducing analysis results are included as part of the shared data package.

    The following dataset was generated:

    Goldstein AT, Stanford TR, Salinas E. 2022. Dataset: Exogenous capture accounts for fundamental differences between prosaccade and antisaccade performance. Zenodo.


    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES