Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2022 Aug 2;11:e77462. doi: 10.7554/eLife.77462

Disruption in structural–functional network repertoire and time-resolved subcortical fronto-temporoparietal connectivity in disorders of consciousness

Rajanikant Panda 1,2, Aurore Thibaut 1,2, Ane Lopez-Gonzalez 3, Anira Escrichs 3, Mohamed Ali Bahri 4, Arjan Hillebrand 5, Gustavo Deco 3,6,7,8, Steven Laureys 1,2,9, Olivia Gosseries 1,2, Jitka Annen 1,2,†,, Prejaas Tewarie 5,10,†,
Editors: Muireann Irish11, Jonathan Roiser12
PMCID: PMC9385205  PMID: 35916363

Abstract

Understanding recovery of consciousness and elucidating its underlying mechanism is believed to be crucial in the field of basic neuroscience and medicine. Ideas such as the global neuronal workspace (GNW) and the mesocircuit theory hypothesize that failure of recovery in conscious states coincide with loss of connectivity between subcortical and frontoparietal areas, a loss of the repertoire of functional networks states and metastable brain activation. We adopted a time-resolved functional connectivity framework to explore these ideas and assessed the repertoire of functional network states as a potential marker of consciousness and its potential ability to tell apart patients in the unresponsive wakefulness syndrome (UWS) and minimally conscious state (MCS). In addition, the prediction of these functional network states by underlying hidden spatial patterns in the anatomical network, that is so-called eigenmodes, was supplemented as potential markers. By analysing time-resolved functional connectivity from functional MRI data, we demonstrated a reduction of metastability and functional network repertoire in UWS compared to MCS patients. This was expressed in terms of diminished dwell times and loss of nonstationarity in the default mode network and subcortical fronto-temporoparietal network in UWS compared to MCS patients. We further demonstrated that these findings co-occurred with a loss of dynamic interplay between structural eigenmodes and emerging time-resolved functional connectivity in UWS. These results are, amongst others, in support of the GNW theory and the mesocircuit hypothesis, underpinning the role of time-resolved thalamo-cortical connections and metastability in the recovery of consciousness.

Research organism: Human

Introduction

Diagnosis of the level of consciousness after coma due to severe brain injury is a well-known dilemma in the field of neurology and intensive care medicine. Coma after cardiac arrest or after traumatic brain injury may result in sustained altered states of consciousness. These patients with disorders of consciousness (DoC), irrespective of the aetiology, can be grouped into the unresponsive wakefulness syndrome (UWS) (Laureys et al., 2010), characterized by the presence of eye-opening and reflexive behaviours, and the minimally conscious state (MCS), characterized by consistent but fluctuant wilful conscious behaviours, such as command following or visual pursuit (Giacino et al., 2002; Schnakers et al., 2010). Recovery of consciousness is argued to emerge conjointly with restoration of resting-state functional brain networks (Edlow et al., 2021), which refers to patterns of neuronal interactions inferred by indirect (e.g. functional magnetic resonance imaging – fMRI) or direct (e.g. electro- and magneto-encephalography [EEG/MEG]) measurements. Analysis of these resting-state networks could potentially help in the diagnosis of patients with DoC and provide insight into the mechanisms that results in absence of recovery of consciousness in UWS.

Various resting-state networks that play an important role in the recovery of consciousness have been identified, among which the default mode network (DMN), fronto-parietal network (FPN), and the salience network are the most important (Amico et al., 2017; Heine et al., 2012). Recovery of the DMN in combination with recovery of the auditory network could for instance discriminate between MCS and UWS with a very high accuracy (~85%) (Demertzi et al., 2015). The mechanism of resting-state network restoration in DoC is yet unknown, however, thalamic activity and especially thalamo-cortical connectivity may be a driving force behind restorations of cortical network function that sustains conscious states (Fridman et al., 2014; Laureys et al., 2000). Previous work on resting-state networks in DoC have mainly focused on the ‘static’ picture of functional connectivity (Demertzi et al., 2013; Edlow et al., 2021; Giacino et al., 2014; Heine et al., 2012), that is connections are assessed over the entire duration of the (fMRI) recording and fluctuations in connectivity over time are ignored. However, the underlying dynamics of connectivity seem relevant for consciousness (Barttfeld et al., 2014; Luppi et al., 2019) and a static description may therefore be inadequate to provide mechanistic insight into failure of recovery of consciousness in DoC (Demertzi et al., 2019).

The analysis of dynamic or time-resolved functional connectivity, as well as the relationship between the underlying anatomical connections and emergent time-resolved functional connectivity (Avena-Koenigsberger et al., 2018; Suárez et al., 2020), may be clinically relevant in patients with DoC. Previous studies have already explored the role of time-resolved functional connectivity in DoC (Del Pozo et al., 2021; Golkowski et al., 2021; Sanz Perl et al., 2021). A recent study demonstrated that network states with long-distance connections occurred less frequently over time in MCS compared to UWS patients (Demertzi et al., 2019), emphasizing disintegration of interactions across the cortex in unconscious states. However, network states reminiscent of the well-known resting-state networks were not retrieved. Cao et al. used two methods to extract time-varying networks, that is independent component analysis and hidden Markov modelling, and revealed clinically relevant differences in network state durations between patients with DoC patients and healthy subjects (Cao et al., 2019), while lacking comparative analysis between patients in MCS and in UWS. In another fMRI study, the authors focused on the posterior cingulate area and the DMN using a spatiotemporal point process analysis and demonstrated decreased occurrence of DMN-like patterns in UWS. Dynamic connectivity analysis has also recently been applied to EEG data, revealing a loss of network integration and increased network segregation in DoC patients (Rizkallah et al., 2019). Spatiotemporal properties of networks have been explored using whole brain modelling, which shows reduction in stability, heterogeneity, and information flow in loss of consciousness (Escrichs et al., 2021; López-González et al., 2021; Panda et al., 2021). Despite the importance of the previously published work, the role of the well-known resting-state networks and especially thalamo-cortical functional connections (Monti et al., 2015) within the context of time-resolved connectivity and DoC has so far not been fully explored.

Another important aspect in the context of the emergence or restoration of resting-state networks is the underlying structural network, as anatomical connectivity patterns influence the repertoire of possible functional network states (Deco et al., 2013). It is widely assumed that switching between functional network states is achieved by so-called metastability in the brain (Deco and Kringelbach, 2016), that is winnerless competitive dynamics. A promising and robust approach to analyse the relationship between structural and functional network states is the so-called eigenmode approach (Atasoy et al., 2016; Robinson et al., 2016; Tewarie et al., 2020). With this approach, spatial harmonic components or eigenmodes are extracted from the anatomical network. These eigenmodes can be considered as patterns of ‘hidden connectivity’ that come to expression at the level of functional networks. It has been postulated that eigenmodes form elementary building blocks for spatiotemporal brain dynamics (Aqil et al., 2021). There is evidence that the well-known resting-state networks can be explained by the activation of a small set of eigenmodes (Atasoy et al., 2018). It can be hypothesized that switching between functional network states, as can be observed in the metastable brain, is accompanied by fluctuations in the expression of eigenmodes Preti and Van De Ville, 2019; therefore, a potential loss of metastability in DoC could co-occur with loss of modulations in eigenmode expression (Barttfeld et al., 2014).

In this context, the aim of the current study was fourfold. First, we tested whether loss of metastability and resting-state network activity, derived from time-resolved estimates of functional connectivity, could differentiate between MCS and UWS patients, with a potential extraction of a spatiotemporal thalamo-cortical network state. Second, we analysed whether time-resolved connectivity could be explained by modulations in expression of eigenmodes in DoC, and third, whether potential differences in eigenmode expression in DoC patients would co-occur with a loss of metastability. Finally, we conceptually link the findings of altered spatiotemporal dynamics underlying activity observed in the brain of DoC patients with several consciousness theories to increase our understanding of the mechanism behind pathological states of unconsciousness.

Results

We included 34 healthy control subjects (HC, 39 [mean] ± 14 years [standard deviation], 20 males), 30 MCS (41 ± 13 years, 21 males) and 14 UWS patients (48 ± 16 years, 7 males). There was no difference in patients with MCS and UWS in terms of age (p > 0.05), gender (p > 0.05), time since injury (p > 0.05), and aetiology (p > 0.05). There was also no difference in age (p > 0.05) and gender (p > 0.05) between HC and DoC patients. Further details about the patient population are described in the methods and Supplementary file 1.

Metastability and time-resolved functional connectivity in patients with DoC

Time-resolved or dynamic connectivity for all subjects was extracted from the phase information of the fMRI data. We quantified a proxy measure for metastability defined as the standard deviation of the overall phase behaviour over time (i.e. the Kuramoto order parameter). This was followed by the extraction of spatiotemporal patterns using non-negative tensor factorization (NNTF) from phase connectivity data, corresponding to resting-state networks or network states (see Figure 1). Well-known resting-state networks as well as a residual component were used as initial conditions for spatial connectivity patterns for all network states to allow for stable convergence of the algorithm (i.e. DMN, FPN, visual network, sensorimotor network, salience network, and subcortical network; Finn et al., 2015). However, the NNTF algorithm allowed the spatial patterns of these network states to change in order to maximize the explained variance of the data. Temporal statistics from the network states were derived for every network state in terms of excursions from the median proxy for nonstationarity (Zalesky et al., 2014) and state duration (i.e. dwell time).

Figure 1. Overview of the analysis pipeline.

Figure 1.

We used the same Shen parcellation for diffusion-weighted MRI (DWI) and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data. Time-resolved functional connectivity was estimated using a metric for phase connectivity. A proxy measure for metastability was derived from the phase information. Time-resolved networks were subsequently extracted from the concatenated data from all subjects using non-negative tensor factorization (NNTF). Dwell times and nonstationarity (excursions from the median) were retrieved for each spatial pattern of functional connectivity (six resting-state networks and one ‘residual’ network). At the same time, time-resolved connectivity was predicted on a sample-by-sample basis based on a linear combination of eigenmodes (hidden patterns in the anatomical network). Measures were used for classification and feature ranking.

A reduction of metastability was found in DoC patients compared to HCs (Figure 2A). Lower metastability was observed in UWS patients in comparison to MCS patients (Figure 2A). Reduced metastability is expected to occur with loss of switching between resting-state networks and potentially with dwelling within a more limited subset of resting-state networks in DoC. The output of the NNTF algorithm resulted in spatial topographies of some of the well-known resting-state networks, that is the DMN, a separate posterior DMN around the precuneus, the visual network, the salience network, the FPN, and a network consisting of subcortical fronto-temporoparietal regions (Sub-FTPN) (Figure 2I–N, Supplementary file 1B). Note that these network states were not identical to the initial conditions, for example the subcortical network that was provided as initial condition to NNTF was incorporated with the frontoparietal network (Figure 2N) by the NNTF algorithm. This modulated subcortical fronto-temporoparietal network consists of the following brain regions: bilateral thalamus, caudate, right putamen, bilateral anterior and middle cingulate, inferior and middle frontal areas, supplementary motor cortex, middle and inferior temporal gyrus, right superior temporal, bilateral inferior parietal, and supramarginal gyrus. At the same time, the sensorimotor network that was provided as initial condition disappeared as state. Excursions from the median were lower for most networks (DMN, visual, Salience, Posterior DMN, and Sub-FTPN) in DoC compared to HC (Figure 2C–F, H). Significant loss of nonstationarity was also found in UWS compared to MCS for the DMN, FPN, and Sub-FTPN (Figure 2C, G, H). The NNTF also yielded a residual state, with a lack of spatial structure, accounting for the variance of connectivity data not explained by the resting-state networks. The residual component had longer dwell times for the decreasing levels of consciousness (Figure 2B). In addition, there were lower dwell times in DoC patients for a specific set of resting-state networks (Salience, Posterior DMN, FPN, and Sub-FTPN), and dwell time was shorter in UWS patients compared to MCS patients only in the Sub-FTPN network (see results in Figure 2—figure supplement 1). These findings of very short dwell times in the posterior DMN, FPN, and Sub-FPTN and long dwell time in the residual network can be considered as a contraction of the functional network repertoire in DoC, which is in agreement with a loss in metastability in these patients.

Figure 2. Metastability and time-resolved functional networks in disorders of consciousness (DoC).

(A) Metastability for all groups: healthy controls (HC), minimally conscious state (MCS), and unresponsive wakefulness state (UWS) patients. (B–H) Distributions of nonstationarity (excursions from the median) for the residuals and time-resolved networks. (I–N) Spatial patterns of time-resolved output networks. Abbreviations: DMN, default mode network; FPN, frontoparietal network; Sub-FTPN, subcortical fronto-temporoparietal network. *, ** and *** denote p < 0.05, p < 0.01 and p < 0.001, respectively (Mann–Whitney U tests). The colourbar indicates the strength of that specific area to the overall spatial pattern. Note, the Sub-FTPN appears as modified network from the initially assigned subcortical network. This Sub-FTPN consists of the following brain regions: bilateral thalamus, caudate, right putamen, bilateral anterior and middle cingulate, inferior and middle frontal areas, supplementary motor cortex, middle and inferior temporal gyrus, right superior temporal, bilateral inferior parietal, and supramarginal gyrus.

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. Dwell time of time-resolved functional networks in disorders of consciousness (DoC).

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

(A–G) Dwell time for all the networks for the three groups (healthy control [HC], minimally conscious state [MCS], and unresponsive wakefulness syndrome [UWS]). Dwell times for all states add up to one. Abbreviations: DMN, default mode network; FPN, frontoparietal network. *, ** and *** denote p < 0.05, p < 0.01 and p < 0.001, respectively.

Relationship between structural eigenmodes and time-resolved functional connectivity in DoC

As resting-state network activity can be explained by the activation of structural eigenmodes, we next analyse the role of fluctuations in eigenmode expression over time. In order to put our findings into context, we first analysed the relationship between static functional networks and structural networks, using the Pearson correlation between static functional connectivity and structural connectivity (SC) for the different groups (Figure 3A). These results show that functional connectivity in DoC patients shows more correspondence with the underlying SC as compared to HCs, as the relationship between structural and functional connectivity was stronger for decreasing levels of consciousness (Figure 3A).

Figure 3. Relationship between time-resolved connectivity and eigenmodes.

Figure 3.

(A) The prediction of static functional connectivity based on structural connectivity for all three groups (healthy control [HC], minimally conscious state [MCS], and unresponsive wakefulness syndrome [UWS]) in terms of the Pearson correlation coefficient. (B) The prediction of time-resolved functional connectivity based on eigenmodes. These distributions of eigenmode predictions are accompanied with predictions based on surrogate data. We further illustrate the level of fluctuations in eigenmode expression for all three groups (HC, MCS, and UWS) for dominant (reflecting network integration, C) and non-dominant eigenmodes (reflecting increasing network segregation, D) accompanied with results for surrogate data, ** and *** denote p < 0.01 and p < 0.001, respectively (Mann–Whitney U tests). (E–G) Metastability is strongly correlated to modulations in eigenmode expression within every group.

We next obtained the eigenmodes from the SC by extracting the eigenvectors of the graph Laplacian. These eigenmodes can be regarded as distinct spatial harmonics within the SC, where the first eigenmodes correspond to patterns with low spatial frequency and subsequent eigenmodes contain patterns with increasingly higher spatial frequencies. Given their spatial configuration, consecutive eigenmodes can be associated with increasing levels of segregation while the first eigenmodes can be linked with network integration. For every time point we predicted the extent to which phase connectivity could be explained by a weighted combination of the eigenmodes (Tewarie et al., 2020). Since phase connectivity can evolve over time, the weighting coefficients for the eigenmodes can modulate as well, resulting in fluctuations in the strength of the expressions of eigenmodes over time. For every eigenmode, we could then quantify the modulation strength (i.e. how much the eigenmode expression varied over time). In addition to the weighting coefficients, we also obtained the goodness-of-fit for the predictions of time-resolved functional connectivity.

The goodness-of-fit for the eigenmode predictions is displayed in Figure 3B, where we show the average correlation between eigenmode predicted FC and empirical FC for the three groups. Results show better predictions for HC and MCS compared to predictions for static FC (median and interquartile range of correlations HC static 0.18 ± 0.04, HC eigenmode 0.39 ± 0.09, Z = −7.1, p < 0.001, MCS static 0.2 ± 0.05, MCS eigenmode 0.35 ± 0.18, Z = −4.8, p < 0.001). In order to test whether these eigenmode predictions of time-varying connectivity could have been obtained by chance, we redid our analysis using surrogate BOLD data (see methods ‘Analysis steps’). Results show that eigenmode predictions for time-resolved connectivity from surrogate data performed significantly worse compared to genuine empirical data (for all comparisons with surrogate data p < 0.001; Figure 3B). We did not test whether the contribution of individual eigenmodes differed between groups since this would come with a serious multiple comparisons problem.

Instead, since SC appeared to correlate stronger with static FC in DoC compared to HC, we expected that eigenmode coefficients in DoC patients would hardly change over time, highlighting the observation of a ‘fixed’ structural–functional network relationship in DoC patients. To analyse this lack of change in the structural–functional network relationship over time in DoC patients, we quantified the modulation strength of the weighting coefficients over time (see methods ‘Analysis steps’). We performed this analysis separately for the dominant (1st to 107th eigenmode, first half) and non-dominant eigenmodes (108th to 214th eigenmode, second half). Results for the dominant eigenmodes show a clear reduction in modulation of the eigenmode weighting in DoC patients compared to HCs (Figure 3C), with a significantly lower modulation of eigenmode expression in UWS compared to MCS patients. This result could not be explained by chance, since the same results could not be obtained from surrogate data (Figure 3C). Note that no between group difference was obtained for non-dominant eigenmodes (Figure 3D).

We have so far shown a reduction in the modulation strength of eigenmode expressions in DoC patients compared to HC subjects, as well as a loss of metastability in DoC patients and dwelling of the brain in fewer network states in DoC patients. This poses the question of whether these two observations are related. In Figure 3E–G, we show that metastability is strongly correlated with modulations in eigenmode expression for the three groups. This underscores the notion that loss of dynamic modulations in functional network patterns due to a loss of metastability could indeed be related to a reduced modulation of eigenmode expression.

Discussion

Differentiation between MCS and UWS is key for adequate diagnosis and prognosis in DoC patients as it is connected to medical-ethical end of life decisions. Use of imaging characteristics allows testing of hypotheses about causes of delayed, or failure of, recovery of consciousness. Here, we used state-of-the-art techniques to quantify time-varying functional connectivity, metastability, and the relationship between the underlying anatomical network and time-resolved functional connections. We demonstrated that these advanced techniques were sensitive to detect clinically relevant differences for the diagnosis of MCS and UWS patients. More specifically, we first demonstrated that UWS patients showed reduced metastability, and spend less time in states outside the natural equilibrium state that would favour cerebral processing in a cooperative and coordinated manner to support consciousness. This is accompanied by shorter state durations that the brain spends in the subcortical fronto-temporoparietal configuration in UWS. A loss of nonstationarity was observed in several resting-state networks (i.e. DMN, frontoparietal, and subcortical fronto-temporoparietal) in UWS compared to MCS patients. Finally, we showed that functional brain networks are more ‘fixed’ to the underlying anatomical connections and are less subject to spatial reconfigurations over time in UWS compared to MCS patients. The extent to which these spatial reconfigurations occurred (i.e. expressed as modulations in eigenmode expression) correlated strongly to metastability.

Our results are in agreement with several hypothesis and theories for the emergence of consciousness, of which most share the importance of thalamo-cortical connectivity for consciousness (Blumenfeld, 2021; Dehaene et al., 2011; Schiff, 2010). The mesocircuit hypothesis states that deafferentation between the frontal cortex and subcortical regions is crucial in explaining the failure of recovery of consciousness (Schiff, 2010). One of the most novel findings in the current work is the generation of the subcortical fronto-temporoparietal network. Although subcortical connections were, among others, used as initial conditions for the decomposition of the time-varying functional connectivity patterns into resting-state networks, incorporation with fronto-temporoparietal connections emerged from the data-driven NNTF algorithm. Another observation confirmed that this NNTF approach extracted DoC-relevant networks, namely that the sensorimotor network disappeared after optimization of spatial network patterns. This latter result is in line with the fact that somatosensory cortices are not directly involved in the emergence of consciousness, based on current theories (Naccache, 2018). In addition, we found that the subcortical fronto-temporoparietal network showed shorter dwell times in DoC patients compared to HC subjects, with even shorter state durations in UWS compared to MCS patients. Finally, this network also demonstrated a loss of nonstationarity in UWS compared to MCS patients. However, it should be noted that the subcortical fronto-temporoparietal network was not the only network with loss of time-resolved network characteristics; other resting-state networks also showed loss of nonstationarity, such as the DMN and FPN. However, a combination of shorter dwell times and loss of nonstationarity was only found for the subcortical fronto-temporoparietal network.

The mesocircuit hypothesis postulates that increased subcortical (specifically thalamic nuclei) inhibition and reduced excitatory output from the thalamus to the cortex (frontal, parietal, temporal, and occipital) leads to weak neural activity and hampers consciousness in DoC patients (Fridman et al., 2014; Giacino et al., 2014). Although our method does not allow to assess excitatory and inhibitory activity, it may be tempting to interpret that the subcortical fronto-temporoparietal network may play a crucial role in orchestrating global network interactions and dwell times. Hence, this subnetwork may be instrumental for the observed loss of nonstationarity in the other subnetworks. Although the importance of functional connections between the thalamus and frontal cortex has been emphasized by the mesocircuit hypothesis, and shown to relate to consciousness in hypothesis-driven functional (e.g., Crone et al., 2018; Fridman et al., 2014; Monti et al., 2015) and structural (e.g., Annen et al., 2016; Weng et al., 2017) neuroimaging studies, this is the first demonstration of the ability of a (semi)data-driven approach to identify this sub-network in the context of time-resolved functional connectivity. Most previous data-driven approaches have been unable to extract such a network (Cao et al., 2019; Demertzi et al., 2019; Rizkallah et al., 2019). Our findings also support the global neuronal workspace (GNW) theory (Dehaene et al., 2011), which emphasizes the importance of long-distance, feedforward and recurrent functional connections, large-scale reverberant networks, and metastable brain states in the emergence and recovery of consciousness (Mashour et al., 2020). Specifically, the GNW postulates that conscious perception should rely on two neural circuits. First, the feedforward circuit encompasses the visual and sensory areas and propagates sensory input to higher order integration areas. Second, in order for sensory input to be accessible to consciousness, recurrent activity in the circuit is required, responsible for ignition in a wide range of high-order functional areas including visual, parieto-temporal, thalamic, and frontal areas (Aru et al., 2020; Baars, 2002; Dehaene and Changeux, 2011; Mashour et al., 2020). The subcortical fronto-temporoparietal network and frontoparietal network obtained in a data-driven fashion conjugates with the latter recurrent circuit proposed to be essential for consciousness by the GNW recurrent circuit. The breakdown of spatio-temporal coordination in the subcortical fronto-temporoparietal network and frontoparietal network was associated to reduced states of consciousness. So far, the importance of metastability has mainly been addressed in the context of recovery of consciousness from anaesthesia (Cavanna et al., 2018). Here, we underscore this finding and demonstrate that a reduction of subcortical–cortical spatiotemporal functional organization into metastable states can even differentiate between UWS and MCS patients aligning with key factors for consciousness as proposed by GNW and mesocircuit hypothesis.

Despite the overlapping cortical circuits important for the mesocircuit model and GNW, differences between the two hypotheses exist. While the occipital cortex is considered part of the same mesocircuit, its role is mostly for feedforward propagation in the GNW. We found a separate visual network that conceptually overlaps more with the feedforward network described by the GNW. In line with GNW predictions, the temporal dynamics in this feedforward network did not distinguish different states of consciousness (Dehaene and Changeux, 2011; Mashour et al., 2020). On the other hand, the involvement of subcortical areas in the subcortical fronto-temporoparietal network is specifically predicted by the mesocircuit model (Fridman et al., 2014; Giacino et al., 2014). Last, the two theories highlight different aspects of brain activity required for consciousness. The GNW proposes specific effective connectivity patterns required for stimuli to reach consciousness. The mesocircuit model, on the other hand, proposes that balanced excitatory and inhibitory activity within the network is a prerequisite for conscious states to be supported. Our resting-state study conceptually thus resonates more with the concepts of the mesocircuit model, yet, we might hypothesize that relative preservation of the recurrent network crucial for the GNW meets the minimal requirement for conscious stimulus processing.

Our observation of altered spatio-temporal dynamics of functional connectivity that are also more restricted to the SC has been observed in pharmacological and pathological loss of consciousness (Demertzi et al., 2019; López-González et al., 2021; Mashour et al., 2021; Uhrig et al., 2018). During unconscious states it has been demonstrated that cerebral regional connectivity leads to an important reconfiguration of the repertoire of functional brain states, that forms simpler connectivity patterns in the brain, which is predominantly shaped by anatomical connectivity and is characterized by the reduction of complex long range connectivity patterns (Barttfeld et al., 2014; Demertzi et al., 2019; Mashour et al., 2021; Uhrig et al., 2018). Previous studies have already demonstrated a strong signature of the underlying structural network in functional networks in DoC. However, these previously described functional networks were mostly obtained using time-averaged estimation methods (Annen et al., 2016; Enciso-Olivera et al., 2021; Kuceyeski et al., 2016; Lant et al., 2016), and have not provided insight into a stronger structural functional coupling in DoC. Using time-resolved functional connectivity and its relationship to eigenmodes, we shed light on this observation. The underlying anatomical connectivity contains so-called inherent ‘hidden patterns’ or eigenmodes with different spatial structures. In a dynamical system such as the brain, these eigenmodes, or a combination of eigenmodes, can be sequentially activated or deactivated (Atasoy et al., 2016; Tewarie et al., 2019), and thereby shape the repertoire of possible functional network states. Since we assume that structural and functional networks share the same eigenmodes (Abdelnour et al., 2018; Naze et al., 2021; Tewarie et al., 2020), the weighting coefficients of the eigenmodes that result in the structural network can be considered as ‘steady state’. Reconfigurations of spatiotemporal networks for the healthy brain are induced by strong deviations from this steady state, demonstrated by strong modulations in eigenmode expression in our work. In DoC however, there is strong tendency towards the ‘steady state’ with minimal deviations of eigenmode expressions over time, and hence leading to a strong signature of structural connections in functional networks in DoC. This inability was even more pronounced for UWS than for MCS.

A few methodological aspects of our retrospective study deserve further discussion. First of all, we did not analyse the contributions of individual eigenmodes for two reasons: (1) although earlier studies have demonstrated that a limited set of eigenmodes could already explain observed functional connectivity pattern (Atasoy et al., 2016), in our case, assessing group differences between MCS and UWS on the basis of individual eigenmodes would impose a multiple comparisons problem; (2) our analytical approach is based on the assumption that all eigenmodes are necessary in the mapping to functional connectivity instead of a statistical selection of eigenmodes. Second, concatenated data from all groups were fed into the NNTF analysis, instead of per group. This assumes that the spatial network structure is similar across groups. Even though this is not necessarily the case, the amount of data to allow for stable NNTF results for individual groups was limited, especially for the UWS group. Future (multicentric) studies with more patients should verify whether the decomposition of the dynamic functional connectivity patterns into the observed subnetworks holds for the separate groups. However, our approach to concatenate data from all groups made group comparison much easier since there was now no need to ‘match’ potentially slightly different networks from the different groups. This also allowed us to focus on networks that were important in DoC. Lastly, we have used a high temporal resolution method to estimate time-resolved connectivity at every time point instead of a sliding window-based method. Previous studies using sliding window approaches have provided novel insights into brain dynamics of loss of consciousness, such as the occurrence and reconfiguration of brain states in DoC patients (Demertzi et al., 2019) and anaesthesia induced loss of consciousness (Barttfeld et al., 2014; Uhrig et al., 2018). However, sliding window approaches have limited sensitivity to nonstationarity in the fMRI BOLD signals (Hindriks et al., 2016). Recent work on time-resolved connectivity shows that brief periods in co-modulation of BOLD signals are an important driving factor for functional connectivity (Zamani Esfahlani et al., 2020; Hindriks et al., 2016), and sliding window approaches may be less sensitive to detect such brief periods in co-modulation. Furthermore, sliding window approaches in DoC have so far demonstrated limited success in retrieving canonical resting-state networks in DoC, while exploration of spatiotemporal aspects of well-known resting-state networks is important to better understand the relation between brain dynamics and loss of consciousness.

Taken together, we have demonstrated that a (semi) data-driven approach has extracted clinically meaningful time-resolved functional brain networks. This unique network-based spatiotemporal characterization accounts for structure–function coupling (i.e. eigenmodes), and shows a relationship with stability of brain dynamics. The measures that differed between UWS and MCS patients most, were the dominant eigenmodes (reflecting structure–function coupling) and time-resolved functional connectivity in the DMN, frontoparietal network, and the subcortical fronto-temporoparietal network. Interestingly, the latter network was generated by the (semi)data-driven approach to better fit the data, and was the sole network to show shorter dwell times in UWS compared to MCS patients. This suggests that the subcortical fronto-temporoparietal network might play a pivotal role in supporting conscious network interactions, as several theoretical and hypothesis-driven studies indicated. Future work will be required to assess to what extent these advanced aspects of connectivity can serve as biomarkers to aid diagnosis and prognosis in DoC.

Methods

Participants

Forty-four adult DoC patients, of whom 30 in MCS (11 females, age range 24–83 years; mean age ± SD, 45 ± 16 years) and 14 with the UWS (6 females, age range 20–74 years; mean age ± SD, 47 ± 16 years) and 34 age and gender matched HC subjects (14 females, age range 19–72 years; mean age ± SD, 40 ± 14 years) without premorbid neurological problems were included.

In this retrospective study, we used the same dataset as in López-González et al., 2021; Panda et al., 2021. The study was approved by the ethic committee of the the University Hospital of Liège (Belgium) (No. 2009-241), where all data were collected. Written informed consents to collect and published the data were obtained from all healthy subjects and the legal representative for DoC patients in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

The diagnosis of the DoC patients was confirmed through two gold standard approaches that is (1) behavioural and (2) fluorodeoxyglucose-positron emission tomography ([18F]FDG-PET), excluding patients for whom these two diagnostic approaches disagreed. (1) Patients were behaviourally diagnosed through the best of at least five Coma Recovery Scale Revised (CRS-R) assessments, evaluating auditory, visual, motor, oromotor function, communication, and arousal (Giacino et al., 2004). (2) Behavioural diagnosis was complemented with the visual assessment of preserved brain metabolism in the frontoparietal network using [18F]FDG-PET as a neurological proxy for consciousness (Stender et al., 2014). Patient-specific clinical information is presented in Supplementary file 1A. We only included patients for whom (1) MRI data were recorded without anaesthesia, (2) diagnosis was based on at least five repetitions of the CRS-R assessment, (3) diagnosed as UWS or MCS, and (4) the [18F]FDG-PET diagnosis was in agreement with the clinical diagnosis. We excluded the patients (1) for whom the patients the structural MRI segmentation was incorrect or (2) if there were excessive head movement artefacts during MR recordings. There were 46 MCS patients in which 16 were discarded due to mismatch of PET and CRS-R diagnosis, 8 for failed segmentation and 4 for head movement artefacts. Amongst the 28 UWS patients, 8 were discarded due to mismatch of the PET and CRS-R diagnosis, 4 for failure of segmentation, and 2 for head movement artefacts.

MRI data acquisition

For the DoC dataset, structural (T1 and DWI) and functional MRI data were acquired using a Siemens 3T Trio scanner. 3D T1-weighted MP-RAGE images (120 transversal slices, repetition time = 2300 ms, voxel size = 1.0 × 1.0 × 1.2 mm3, flip angle = 9°, field of view = 256 × 256 mm2) were acquired prior to the 10 min of BOLD fMRI resting-state (i.e. task free) (EPI, gradient echo, volumes = 300, repetition time = 2000 ms, echo time = 30 ms, flip angle = 78°, voxel size = 3 × 3 × 3 mm3, field of view = 192 × 192 mm2, 32 transversal slices). HC subjects were instructed to keep eyes open and to be in relaxed state during the fMRI data acquisition. Last, diffusion-weighted MRI (DWI) was acquired in 64 directions (b-value = 1000 s/mm2, voxel size = 1.8 × 1.8 × 3.3 mm3, field of view = 230 × 230 mm2, repetition time = 5700 ms, echo time = 87 ms, 45 transverse slices, 128 × 128 voxel matrix) preceded by a single unweighted image (b0).

Resting-state fMRI preprocessing

Preprocessing was performed as in López-González et al., 2021 using MELODIC (Multivariate Exploratory Linear Optimized Decomposition into Independent Components) version 3.14, which is part of FMRIB’s Software Library (FSL, http://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl). The preprocessing consisted of the following steps: the first five functional images were discarded to reduce scanner inhomogeneity, motion correction was performed using MCFLIRT, non-brain tissue was removed using Bet Extraction Tool (BET), temporal band-pass filtering with sigma 100 s, spatial smoothing was applied using a 5-mm FWHM Gaussian kernel, rigid-body registration was performed, and finally single-session ICA with automatic dimensionality estimation was employed (Griffanti et al., 2014). Then, FIX (FMRIB’s ICA-based X-noiseifier) was applied to remove the noise components and the lesion-driven for each subject. Specifically, FSLeyes in Melodic mode was used to manually identify the single-subject independent components (ICs) into ‘good’ for cerebral signal, ‘bad’ for noise or injury-driven artefacts, and ‘unknown’ for ambiguous components. Each component was evaluated based on the spatial map, the time series, and the temporal power spectrum (Griffanti et al., 2014). Next, for each subject, FIX was applied with default parameters to remove bad and unknown components. Subsequently, the Shen et al., functional atlas (without cerebellum) was applied for brain parcellation to obtain the BOLD time series of the 214 cortical and subcortical brain areas in each individual’s native EPI space (Finn et al., 2015; Shen et al., 2013). The cleaned functional data were co-registered to the T1-weighted structural image by using FLIRT (Jenkinson and Smith, 2001). Then, the T1-weighted image was co-registered to the standard MNI space by using FLIRT (12 DOF) and FNIRT (Andersson et al., 2007). The transformations matrices were inverted and applied to warp the resting-state atlas from MNI space to the single-subject functional data using a nearest-neighbour interpolation method to ensure the preservation of the labels. Finally, the time series for each of the 214 brain areas were extracted using fslmaths and fslmeants.

Probabilistic tractography analysis

A whole-brain SC matrix was computed for each subject as reported in our previous study (López-González et al., 2021). Briefly, the b0 image was co-registered to the T1 structural image using FLIRT and the T1 structural image was co-registered to the MNI space using FLIRT and FNIRT (Andersson et al., 2007; Jenkinson and Smith, 2001). The transformation matrices were inverted and applied to warp the resting-state atlas from MNI space to the native diffusion space using a nearest-neighbour interpolation method. Analysis of diffusion images was applied using the FMRIB’s Diffusion Toolbox (FDT) (https://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki). BET was computed, eddy current distortions and head motion were corrected using eddy correct tool (Andersson and Sotiropoulos, 2016). Crossing fibres were modelled by using BEDPOSTX, and the probability of multi-fibre orientations was calculated to enhance the sensitivity of non-dominant fibre populations (Behrens et al., 2007; Behrens et al., 2003). Then, probabilistic tractography analysis was calculated in native diffusion space using PROBTRACKX to compute the connectivity probability of each brain region to each of the other 213 brain regions. Subsequently, the value of each brain area was divided by its corresponding number of generated tracts to obtain the structural probability matrix. Finally, the SCpn matrix was then symmetrized by computing their transpose SCnp and averaging both matrices.

Metastability and time-resolved functional connectivity

BOLD time series for every ROI k were filtered within the narrowband of 0.01–0.9 Hz. We estimated both time-resolved functional connectivity and static functional connectivity. Static functional connectivity (FCstatic) was estimated by the Pearson correlation coefficient between pairwise narrowband time courses. Time-resolved connectivity was estimated from the instantaneous phases. The instantaneous phase of the BOLD signal, φkt , was extracted from the analytic signal as obtained from the Hilbert transform. The synchronization between pairs of brain regions was characterized as the difference between their instantaneous phases. At each time point, the phase difference between two regions j and k was used as estimate for instantaneous phase connectivity φjkt (Glerean et al., 2012). Evaluation of pairwise connectivity at each time point resulted in a functional connectivity tensor (number of ROIs × number of ROIs × time points).

Furthermore, we investigated how the synchronization between different nodes fluctuates across time using the concept of metastability. In the current sense, metastability quantifies how variable the states of phase configurations are as a function of time. We quantified metastability (i.e. our proxy measure for metastability) in terms of the standard deviation of the Kuramoto order parameter, Rt=1NkNeiφkt , where N is the number of ROIs and i denotes the imaginary unit (Deco et al., 2017).

Similar to Ponce-Alvarez et al., 2015; Tewarie et al., 2019, we extracted time-evolving networks using NNTF (Bro, 1997). We used the N-way toolbox (version 1.8) for MATLAB for this analysis (Andersson and Bro, 2000). NNTF can be considered as a higher-order principal component analysis. The goal of the approach is to decompose the functional connectivity tensor FC into components, such that the approximation of the functional connectivity tensor FC~ can be written as FC~=lLal×bl×cl . Here, al and bl correspond to vectors decoding spatial information for component l, and cl represents the vector that contains information on temporal fluctuations of component l. Note that the outer product al×bl stands for the spatial pattern of functional connectivity of component l. The number of components L can be estimated from the data using established algorithms (Timmerman and Kiers, 2000). However, here, we fixed the number of components based on the number of a priori expected networks in which we were interested. We fed the NNTF algorithm with initial conditions for al and bl based on the spatial components of six expected resting-state networks: salience network, FPN, DMN, subcortical network, sensorimotor network, and visual network. We added a residual network to account for the unexplained variance in the functional connectivity tensor. Note that the spatial initial conditions did not indicate that these spatial components were kept fixed during NNTF calculation; rather, these spatial components were free to be adjusted according to the maximization of the explained variance. Data from all subjects and groups were concatenated to allow convergence to stable results.

Relationship between structural eigenmodes and time-resolved functional connectivity

For every subject, we extracted structural eigenmodes from the graph Laplacian QA of the SC matrix defined by QA=KSC-SC, where KSC refers to the diagonal degree matrix of SC. We further applied symmetric normalization to obtain a normalized Laplacian QSC . Subsequently, eigenvectors ui and eigenvalues (together called eigenmodes) were extracted using diagonalization of QSC , resulting in N eigenmodes (N = number of ROIs). Using a recently introduced approach we mapped functional brain networks at each time point from the structural eigenmodes (Tewarie et al., 2022; Tewarie et al., 2020). In other words, we estimated to what extent functional connectivity at each time point FCt could be explained by a linear combination of the eigenmodes as follows:

, FCtKFCt-KFC12tUPtUTKFC12t (1)

where KFCt is the diagonal node strength matrix of the functional connectivity matrix FCt at time t. The matrix P(t) corresponds to the weighting coefficient matrix for the eigenmodes and is obtained after optimization and equal to

Pt=diagu1TQFCtu1,,uNTQFCtuN (2)

Hence, modulations of eigenmode expressions over time are expressed in Pt . Here, QFCt is the normalized graph Laplacian of FCt at time t, and ui is the ith eigenvector of QSC and the ith column of U.

Analysis steps

Time-resolved functional connectivity

Individual temporal time courses clind for expression of component l for every subject were extracted from cl . A high value of cl at a certain time point indicates strong expression of this spatial pattern of functional connectivity (al×bl) at that time point. At each time point, we determined the component with the strongest expression (maxclind), and assumed that connectivity at that point in time was dominated by this state or component. The duration that this component retained the strongest expression was considered as state duration or dwell time (see Figure 1). In addition, we also characterized the amount of nonstationarity in cl (Hindriks et al., 2016; Zalesky et al., 2014), that is excursions from the median. The rationale for using this metric is its sensitivity to detect modulations if the underlying system is indeed dynamic (Hindriks et al., 2016). Mann–Whitney U tests were used to test, for each network separately, differences in state durations and excursion from the median between groups.

Structural vs. functional brain networks

We first estimated the relationship between static functional connectivity and the SC itself (without decomposing SC into eigenmodes). This relationship was estimated in terms of the Pearson correlation coefficient between the SC and FCstatic , denoted as corrFCstatic,SC . We secondly analysed the extent to which time-varying functional networks could be explained by expressions of the structural eigenmodes. We therefore computed the Pearson correlation between the empirical FCt and the eigenmode predicted FCt , denoted as corrFC,eigenmodes . In order to be able to test whether there was a difference in fluctuations of eigenmode expression over time between groups, we quantified the eigenmode modulation strength defined as eigenmode=tk=1,tl=1T,TPtk-Ptj , where tj and tk correspond to different points in time and T to the total duration of the recording. Since the first eigenmodes can be considered as dominant eigenmodes and more important to shape functional brain networks (Atasoy et al., 2016), we evaluated the eigenmode modulation strength for the dominant eigenmodes i=1,,N/2 and non-dominant eigenmodes i=N/2,,N separately. We further computed the correlation between eigenmode modulation strength and metastability in all groups separately in order to test whether modulations in eigenmode expression related to our proxy measure for metastability. Group differences for all metrics was tested using Mann–Whitney U tests.

In order to test whether eigenmode predictions of functional connectivity could be obtained by chance, we created surrogate data and redid analysis for the surrogate data. Surrogate data for fMRI BOLD time series were obtained using the circular time shifted method (Quian Quiroga et al., 2002). Time-resolved phase connectivity was estimated in the same way as for genuine empirical data. Time-resolved connectivity obtained from surrogate data was subsequently predicted using the eigenmodes.

False discovery rate was used to correct for multiple comparisons for analysis steps 1 and 2: number of metrics (excursions * seven networks, dwell time * seven networks, metastability, corrFCstatic,SC, corrFC,eigenmodes, non-dominant and dominant eigenmodes) * 3 comparisons + surrogate comparison with genuine data 3 * 3, (corrFC,eigenmodes, non-dominant and dominant eigenmodes) comparisons = 66 tests (Benjamini and Hochberg, 1995).

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection, and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Jitka Annen, Email: jitka.annen@uliege.be.

Prejaas Tewarie, Email: prejaas.tewarie@nottingham.ac.uk.

Muireann Irish, University of Sydney, Australia.

Jonathan Roiser, University College London, United Kingdom.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • Horizon 2020 Framework Programme 945539 to Gustavo Deco, Steven Laureys.

  • Horizon 2020 Framework Programme 785907 to Steven Laureys.

  • Horizon 2020 Framework Programme 778234 to Steven Laureys.

  • The Belgian National Funds for Scientific Research (F.R.S-FNRS) to Rajanikant Panda, Aurore Thibaut, Olivia Gosseries, Steven Laureys.

  • European Space Agency to Steven Laureys.

  • Swiss National Science Foundation Sinergia (170873) to Ane Lopez-Gonzalez.

  • Belgian Federal Science Policy Office PRODEX Programme to Steven Laureys.

  • Fondazione Europea di Ricerca Biomedica to Steven Laureys.

  • Bial Foundation to Olivia Gosseries, Steven Laureys.

  • The fund Generet to Steven Laureys.

  • King Baudouin Foundation to Steven Laureys.

  • Leon Fredericq Foundation to Steven Laureys, Aurore Thibaut, Olivia Gosseries.

  • The DoCMA Project EU-H2020-MSCA–RISE–778234 to Aurore Thibaut.

  • FLAG-ERA JTC PCI2018-092891 to Gustavo Deco.

  • The Spanish Ministry Project PSI2016-75688-P (AEI/FEDER) to Gustavo Deco.

  • The Catalan Research Group Support 2017 SGR 1 to Gustavo Deco.

  • Ministerio de Ciencia, Innovación y Universidades to Gustavo Deco.

  • Agencia Estatal de Investigación to Gustavo Deco.

  • European Regional Development Fund to Gustavo Deco.

  • Fonds De La Recherche Scientifique - FNRS PDR project T.0134.21 to Aurore Thibaut, Olivia Gosseries.

  • Mind Science Foundation to Steven Laureys.

  • The Télévie Foundation to Olivia Gosseries.

  • The Public Utility Foundation ‘Université Européenne du Travail’ to Steven Laureys.

  • The Mind-Care foundation to Steven Laureys.

  • National Natural Science Foundation of China Joint Research Project 81471100 to Steven Laureys.

  • The European Foundation of Biomedical Research FERB Onlus to Steven Laureys.

  • The ERA-Net FLAG-ERA JTC2021 project ModelDXConsciousness (Human Brain Project Partnering Project) to Olivia Gosseries.

  • University and University Hospital of Liege to Aurore Thibaut, Olivia Gosseries, Steven Laureys.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review and editing.

Conceptualization, Data curation, Writing – review and editing, Methodology.

Software, Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – review and editing.

Software, Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – review and editing.

Validation, Methodology, Writing – review and editing.

Formal analysis, Supervision, Investigation, Visualization, Writing – review and editing.

Conceptualization, Supervision, Validation, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review and editing.

Conceptualization, Resources, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review and editing.

Conceptualization, Resources, Data curation, Funding acquisition, Project administration, Writing – review and editing.

Conceptualization, Resources, Formal analysis, Supervision, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review and editing.

Conceptualization, Software, Formal analysis, Investigation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review and editing.

Ethics

Written informed consent was obtained from all healthy subjects and the legal representative for DoC patients.

Additional files

Supplementary file 1.

(A) Details about the patient population. (B) Brain regions involved in the extracted networks by non-negative tensor factorisation.

elife-77462-supp1.docx (41KB, docx)
MDAR checklist

Data availability

Connectivity matrices have been made available open access through EBRAINS of the Human Brain Project.

The following previously published dataset was used:

López-González A, Panda R, Barra A, Bonin EAC, Ponce-Alvarez A, Zamora-López G, Sanz LRD, Escrichs A, Cecconi B, Martial C, Thibaut A, Gosseries O, Annen J, Deco G, Laureys S. 2021. Phase-interaction matrices of the phase relationships of different pathological states of consciousness. Human Brain Project.

References

  1. Abdelnour F, Dayan M, Devinsky O, Thesen T, Raj A. Functional brain connectivity is predictable from anatomic network’s Laplacian eigen-structure. NeuroImage. 2018;172:728–739. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.02.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Amico E, Marinazzo D, Di Perri C, Heine L, Annen J, Martial C, Dzemidzic M, Kirsch M, Bonhomme V, Laureys S, Goñi J. Mapping the functional connectome traits of levels of consciousness. NeuroImage. 2017;148:201–211. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.01.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Andersson CA, Bro R. The n-way toolbox for matlab. Chemometrics and Intelligent Laboratory Systems. 2000;52:1–4. doi: 10.1016/S0169-7439(00)00071-X. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  4. Andersson JLR, Jenkinson M, Smith S. Non-linear registrationakaSpatial normalisation. FMRIB Analysis Group; 2007. https://www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/datasets/techrep/tr07ja2/tr07ja2.pdf [Google Scholar]
  5. Andersson JLR, Sotiropoulos SN. An integrated approach to correction for off-resonance effects and subject movement in diffusion MR imaging. NeuroImage. 2016;125:1063–1078. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.10.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Annen J, Heine L, Ziegler E, Frasso G, Bahri M, Di Perri C, Stender J, Martial C, Wannez S, D’ostilio K, Amico E, Antonopoulos G, Bernard C, Tshibanda F, Hustinx R, Laureys S. Function-structure connectivity in patients with severe brain injury as measured by MRI-DWI and FDG-PET. Human Brain Mapping. 2016;37:3707–3720. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23269. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Aqil M, Atasoy S, Kringelbach ML, Hindriks R. Graph neural fields: A framework for spatiotemporal dynamical models on the human connectome. PLOS Computational Biology. 2021;17:e1008310. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1008310. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Aru J, Suzuki M, Larkum ME. Cellular mechanisms of conscious processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2020;24:814–825. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2020.07.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Atasoy S, Donnelly I, Pearson J. Human brain networks function in connectome-specific harmonic waves. Nature Communications. 2016;7:10340. doi: 10.1038/ncomms10340. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Atasoy S, Deco G, Kringelbach ML, Pearson J. Harmonic brain modes: a unifying framework for linking space and time in brain dynamics. The Neuroscientist. 2018;24:277–293. doi: 10.1177/1073858417728032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Avena-Koenigsberger A, Misic B, Sporns O. Communication dynamics in complex brain networks. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience. 2018;19:17–33. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2017.149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Baars BJ. The conscious access hypothesis: origins and recent evidence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2002;6:47–52. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01819-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Barttfeld P, Uhrig L, Sitt JD, Sigman M, Jarraya B, Dehaene S. Signature of consciousness in the dynamics of resting-state brain activity. PNAS. 2014;112:887–892. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1418031112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Behrens TEJ, Woolrich MW, Jenkinson M, Johansen-Berg H, Nunes RG, Clare S, Matthews PM, Brady JM, Smith SM. Characterization and propagation of uncertainty in diffusion-weighted MR imaging. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine. 2003;50:1077–1088. doi: 10.1002/mrm.10609. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Behrens TEJ, Berg HJ, Jbabdi S, Rushworth MFS, Woolrich MW. Probabilistic diffusion tractography with multiple fibre orientations: What can we gain? NeuroImage. 2007;34:144–155. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.09.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society. 1995;57:289–300. doi: 10.1111/j.2517-6161.1995.tb02031.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  17. Blumenfeld H. Brain mechanisms of conscious awareness: detect, pulse, switch, and wave. The Neuroscientist. 2021;1:378. doi: 10.1177/10738584211049378. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Bro R. PARAFAC: Tutorial and applications. Chemometrics and Intelligent Laboratory Systems. 1997;38:149–171. doi: 10.1016/S0169-7439(97)00032-4. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  19. Cao B, Chen Y, Yu R, Chen L, Chen P, Weng Y, Chen Q, Song J, Xie Q, Huang R. Abnormal dynamic properties of functional connectivity in disorders of consciousness. NeuroImage. Clinical. 2019;24:102071. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2019.102071. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Cavanna F, Vilas MG, Palmucci M, Tagliazucchi E. Dynamic functional connectivity and brain metastability during altered states of consciousness. NeuroImage. 2018;180:383–395. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.09.065. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Crone JS, Bio BJ, Vespa PM, Lutkenhoff ES, Monti MM. Restoration of thalamo-cortical connectivity after brain injury: recovery of consciousness, complex behavior, or passage of time? Journal of Neuroscience Research. 2018;96:671–687. doi: 10.1002/jnr.24115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Deco G, Ponce-Alvarez A, Mantini D, Romani GL, Hagmann P, Corbetta M. Resting-state functional connectivity emerges from structurally and dynamically shaped slow linear fluctuations. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33:11239–11252. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1091-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Deco G, Kringelbach ML. Metastability and coherence: extending the communication through coherence hypothesis using a whole-brain computational perspective. Trends in Neurosciences. 2016;39:125–135. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2016.01.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Deco G, Kringelbach ML, Jirsa VK, Ritter P. The dynamics of resting fluctuations in the brain: metastability and its dynamical cortical core. Scientific Reports. 2017;7:3095. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-03073-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Dehaene S, Changeux JP. Experimental and theoretical approaches to conscious processing. Neuron. 2011;70:200–227. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.03.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Dehaene S, Changeux JP, Naccache L. The global neuronal workspace model of conscious access: from neuronal architectures to clinical applications. Charact Conscious From Cogn to Clin. 2011;1:55–84. doi: 10.1007/978-3-642-18015-6_4. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  27. Del Pozo SM, Laufs H, Bonhomme V, Laureys S, Balenzuela P, Tagliazucchi E. Unconsciousness reconfigures modular brain network dynamics. Chaos. 2021;31:093117. doi: 10.1063/5.0046047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Demertzi A, Soddu A, Laureys S. Consciousness supporting networks. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2013;23:239–244. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2012.12.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Demertzi A, Antonopoulos G, Heine L, Voss HU, Crone JS, de Los Angeles C, Bahri MA, Di Perri C, Vanhaudenhuyse A, Charland-Verville V, Kronbichler M, Trinka E, Phillips C, Gomez F, Tshibanda L, Soddu A, Schiff ND, Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Laureys S. Intrinsic functional connectivity differentiates minimally conscious from unresponsive patients. Brain: A Journal of Neurology. 2015;138:2619–2631. doi: 10.1093/brain/awv169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Demertzi A, Tagliazucchi E, Dehaene S, Deco G, Barttfeld P, Raimondo F, Martial C, Fernández-Espejo D, Rohaut B, Voss HU, Schiff ND, Owen AM, Laureys S, Naccache L, Sitt JD. Human consciousness is supported by dynamic complex patterns of brain signal coordination. Science Advances. 2019;5:eaat7603. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.aat7603. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Edlow BL, Claassen J, Schiff ND, Greer DM. Recovery from disorders of consciousness: mechanisms, prognosis and emerging therapies. Nature Reviews. Neurology. 2021;17:135–156. doi: 10.1038/s41582-020-00428-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Enciso-Olivera CO, Ordóñez-Rubiano EG, Casanova-Libreros R, Rivera D, Zarate-Ardila CJ, Rudas J, Pulido C, Gómez F, Martínez D, Guerrero N, Hurtado MA, Aguilera-Bustos N, Hernández-Torres CP, Hernandez J, Marín-Muñoz JH. Structural and functional connectivity of the ascending arousal network for prediction of outcome in patients with acute disorders of consciousness. Scientific Reports. 2021;11:22952. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-98506-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Escrichs A, Perl YS, Uribe C, Camara E, Türker B, Pyatigorskaya N, López-González A, Pallavicini C, Panda R, Annen J, Grosseries O, Laureys S, Naccache L, Sitt JD, Laufs H, Tagliazucchi E, Kringelbach ML, Deco G. Unifying turbulent dynamics framework distinguishes different brain states. bioRxiv. 2021 doi: 10.1101/2021.10.14.464380. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  34. Finn ES, Shen X, Scheinost D, Rosenberg MD, Huang J, Chun MM, Papademetris X, Constable RT. Functional connectome fingerprinting: identifying individuals using patterns of brain connectivity. Nature Neuroscience. 2015;18:1664–1671. doi: 10.1038/nn.4135. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Fridman EA, Beattie BJ, Broft A, Laureys S, Schiff ND. Regional cerebral metabolic patterns demonstrate the role of anterior forebrain mesocircuit dysfunction in the severely injured brain. PNAS. 2014;111:6473–6478. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1320969111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Giacino JT, Ashwal S, Childs N, Cranford R, Jennett B, Katz DI, Kelly JP, Rosenberg JH, Whyte J, Zafonte RD, Zasler ND. The minimally conscious state: definition and diagnostic criteria. Neurology. 2002;58:349–353. doi: 10.1212/wnl.58.3.349. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Giacino JT, Kalmar K, Whyte J. The jfk coma recovery scale-revised: measurement characteristics and diagnostic utility. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation. 2004;85:2020–2029. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2004.02.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Giacino JT, Fins JJ, Laureys S, Schiff ND. Disorders of consciousness after acquired brain injury: the state of the science. Nature Reviews. Neurology. 2014;10:99–114. doi: 10.1038/nrneurol.2013.279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Glerean E, Salmi J, Lahnakoski JM, Jääskeläinen IP, Sams M. Functional magnetic resonance imaging phase synchronization as a measure of dynamic functional connectivity. Brain Connectivity. 2012;2:91–101. doi: 10.1089/brain.2011.0068. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Golkowski D, Willnecker R, Rösler J, Ranft A, Schneider G, Jordan D, Ilg R. Dynamic patterns of global brain communication differentiate conscious from unconscious patients after severe brain injury. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience. 2021;15:e919. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2021.625919. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Griffanti L, Salimi-Khorshidi G, Beckmann CF, Auerbach EJ, Douaud G, Sexton CE, Zsoldos E, Ebmeier KP, Filippini N, Mackay CE, Moeller S, Xu J, Yacoub E, Baselli G, Ugurbil K, Miller KL, Smith SM. ICA-based artefact removal and accelerated fMRI acquisition for improved resting state network imaging. NeuroImage. 2014;95:232–247. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.03.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Heine L, Soddu A, Gómez F, Vanhaudenhuyse A, Tshibanda L, Thonnard M, Charland-Verville V, Kirsch M, Laureys S, Demertzi A. Resting state networks and consciousness. Frontiers in Psychology. 2012;3:295. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00295. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Hindriks R, Adhikari MH, Murayama Y, Ganzetti M, Mantini D, Logothetis NK, Deco G. Can sliding-window correlations reveal dynamic functional connectivity in resting-state fMRI? NeuroImage. 2016;127:242–256. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.11.055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Jenkinson M, Smith S. A global optimisation method for robust affine registration of brain images. Medical Image Analysis. 2001;5:143–156. doi: 10.1016/s1361-8415(01)00036-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Kuceyeski A, Shah S, Dyke JP, Bickel S, Abdelnour F, Schiff ND, Voss HU, Raj A. The application of a mathematical model linking structural and functional connectomes in severe brain injury. NeuroImage. Clinical. 2016;11:635–647. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2016.04.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Lant ND, Gonzalez-Lara LE, Owen AM, Fernández-Espejo D. Relationship between the anterior forebrain mesocircuit and the default mode network in the structural bases of disorders of consciousness. NeuroImage. Clinical. 2016;10:27–35. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2015.11.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Laureys S, Faymonville ME, Luxen A, Lamy M, Franck G, Maquet P. Restoration of thalamocortical connectivity after recovery from persistent vegetative state. The Lancet. 2000;355:1790–1791. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(00)02271-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Laureys S, Celesia GG, Cohadon F, Lavrijsen J, León-Carrión J, Sannita WG, Sazbon L, Schmutzhard E, von Wild KR, Zeman A, Dolce G, European Task Force on Disorders of Consciousness Unresponsive wakefulness syndrome: A new name for the vegetative state or apallic syndrome. BMC Medicine. 2010;8:68. doi: 10.1186/1741-7015-8-68. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. López-González A, Panda R, Ponce-Alvarez A, Zamora-López G, Escrichs A, Martial C, Thibaut A, Gosseries O, Kringelbach ML, Annen J, Laureys S, Deco G. Loss of consciousness reduces the stability of brain hubs and the heterogeneity of brain dynamics. Communications Biology. 2021;4:1037. doi: 10.1038/s42003-021-02537-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Luppi AI, Craig MM, Pappas I, Finoia P, Williams GB, Allanson J, Pickard JD, Owen AM, Naci L, Menon DK, Stamatakis EA. Consciousness-specific dynamic interactions of brain integration and functional diversity. Nature Communications. 2019;10:4616. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-12658-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Mashour GA, Roelfsema P, Changeux JP, Dehaene S. Conscious processing and the global neuronal workspace hypothesis. Neuron. 2020;105:776–798. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2020.01.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Mashour GA, Palanca BJA, Basner M, Li D, Wang W, Blain-Moraes S, Lin N, Maier K, Muench M, Tarnal V, Vanini G, Ochroch EA, Hogg R, Schwartz M, Maybrier H, Hardie R, Janke E, Golmirzaie G, Picton P, McKinstry-Wu AR, Avidan MS, Kelz MB. Recovery of consciousness and cognition after general anesthesia in humans. eLife. 2021;10:e59525. doi: 10.7554/eLife.59525. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Monti MM, Rosenberg M, Finoia P, Kamau E, Pickard JD, Owen AM. Thalamo-frontal connectivity mediates top-down cognitive functions in disorders of consciousness. Neurology. 2015;84:167–173. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000001123. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Naccache L. Minimally conscious state or cortically mediated state? Brain. 2018;141:949–960. doi: 10.1093/brain/awx324. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Naze S, Proix T, Atasoy S, Kozloski JR. Robustness of connectome harmonics to local gray matter and long-range white matter connectivity changes. NeuroImage. 2021;224:117364. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117364. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Panda R, López-González A, Gilson M, Gosseries O, Thibaut A, Frasso G, Cecconi B, Escrichs A, Deco G, Laureys S, Zamora-López G, Annen J. Posterior integration and thalamo-frontotemporal broadcasting are impaired in disorders of consciousness. bioRxiv. 2021 doi: 10.1101/2021.11.08.467694. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  57. Ponce-Alvarez A, Deco G, Hagmann P, Romani GL, Mantini D, Corbetta M. Resting-state temporal synchronization networks emerge from connectivity topology and heterogeneity. PLOS Computational Biology. 2015;11:e1004100. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Preti MG, Van De Ville D. Decoupling of brain function from structure reveals regional behavioral specialization in humans. Nature Communications. 2019;10:1–7. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-12765-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Quian Quiroga R, Kraskov A, Kreuz T, Grassberger P. Performance of different synchronization measures in real data: A case study on electroencephalographic signals. Physical Review. E, Statistical, Nonlinear, and Soft Matter Physics. 2002;65:041903. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevE.65.041903. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Rizkallah J, Annen J, Modolo J, Gosseries O, Benquet P, Mortaheb S, Amoud H, Cassol H, Mheich A, Thibaut A, Chatelle C, Hassan M, Panda R, Wendling F, Laureys S. Decreased integration of EEG source-space networks in disorders of consciousness. NeuroImage. Clinical. 2019;23:101841. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2019.101841. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Robinson PA, Zhao X, Aquino KM, Griffiths JD, Sarkar S, Mehta-Pandejee G. Eigenmodes of brain activity: Neural field theory predictions and comparison with experiment. NeuroImage. 2016;142:79–98. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.04.050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Sanz Perl Y, Pallavicini C, Pérez Ipiña I, Demertzi A, Bonhomme V, Martial C, Panda R, Annen J, Ibañez A, Kringelbach M, Deco G, Laufs H, Sitt J, Laureys S, Tagliazucchi E. Perturbations in dynamical models of whole-brain activity dissociate between the level and stability of consciousness. PLOS Computational Biology. 2021;17:e1009139. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1009139. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Schiff ND. Recovery of consciousness after brain injury: a mesocircuit hypothesis. Trends in Neurosciences. 2010;33:1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2009.11.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Schnakers C, Chatelle C, Vanhaudenhuyse A, Majerus S, Ledoux D, Boly M, Bruno M-A, Boveroux P, Demertzi A, Moonen G, Laureys S. The Nociception Coma Scale: a new tool to assess nociception in disorders of consciousness. Pain. 2010;148:215–219. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2009.09.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Shen X, Tokoglu F, Papademetris X, Constable RT. Groupwise whole-brain parcellation from resting-state fMRI data for network node identification. NeuroImage. 2013;82:403–415. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.081. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Stender J, Gosseries O, Bruno MA, Charland-Verville V, Vanhaudenhuyse A, Demertzi A, Chatelle C, Thonnard M, Thibaut A, Heine L, Soddu A, Boly M, Schnakers C, Gjedde A, Laureys S. Diagnostic precision of PET imaging and functional MRI in disorders of consciousness: a clinical validation study. Lancet. 2014;384:514–522. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(14)60042-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Suárez LE, Markello RD, Betzel RF, Misic B. Linking structure and function in macroscale brain networks. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2020;24:302–315. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2020.01.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Tewarie P, Abeysuriya R, Byrne Á, O’Neill GC, Sotiropoulos SN, Brookes MJ, Coombes S. How do spatially distinct frequency specific MEG networks emerge from one underlying structural connectome? The role of the structural eigenmodes. NeuroImage. 2019;186:211–220. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.10.079. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Tewarie P, Prasse B, Meier JM, Santos FAN, Douw L, Schoonheim MM, Stam CJ, Van Mieghem P, Hillebrand A. Mapping functional brain networks from the structural connectome: Relating the series expansion and eigenmode approaches. NeuroImage. 2020;216:116805. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116805. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Tewarie P, Prasse B, Meier J, Mandke K, Warrington S, Stam CJ, Brookes MJ, Van Mieghem P, Sotiropoulos SN, Hillebrand A. Predicting time-resolved electrophysiological brain networks from structural eigenmodes. Human Brain Mapping. 2022;1:e5967. doi: 10.1002/hbm.25967. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Timmerman ME, Kiers HAL. Three-mode principal components analysis: choosing the numbers of components and sensitivity to local optima. The British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology. 2000;53 (Pt 1):1–16. doi: 10.1348/000711000159132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Uhrig L, Sitt JD, Jacob A, Tasserie J, Barttfeld P, Dupont M, Dehaene S, Jarraya B. Resting-state dynamics as a cortical signature of anesthesia in monkeys. Anesthesiology. 2018;129:942–958. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0000000000002336. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Weng L, Xie Q, Zhao L, Zhang R, Ma Q, Wang J, Jiang W, He Y, Chen Y, Li C, Ni X, Xu Q, Yu R, Huang R. Abnormal structural connectivity between the basal ganglia, thalamus, and frontal cortex in patients with disorders of consciousness. Cortex; a Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior. 2017;90:71–87. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.02.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Zalesky A, Fornito A, Cocchi L, Gollo LL, Breakspear M. Time-resolved resting-state brain networks. PNAS. 2014;111:10341–10346. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1400181111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Zamani Esfahlani F, Jo Y, Faskowitz J, Byrge L, Kennedy DP, Sporns O, Betzel RF. High-amplitude cofluctuations in cortical activity drive functional connectivity. PNAS. 2020;117:28393–28401. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2005531117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Editor's evaluation

Muireann Irish 1

This is an important paper providing convincing evidence for altered brain dynamics in patients in a minimally conscious state and those with unresponsive wakefulness syndrome relative to healthy control participants. The results indicate reduced metastability and a contracted network repertoire in disorders of consciousness. Overall, the study provides important new information on mechanisms of disorders of consciousness and the functional brain networks involved.

Decision letter

Editor: Muireann Irish1
Reviewed by: Patricio Orio2

Our editorial process produces two outputs: i) public reviews designed to be posted alongside the preprint for the benefit of readers; ii) feedback on the manuscript for the authors, including requests for revisions, shown below. We also include an acceptance summary that explains what the editors found interesting or important about the work.

Decision letter after peer review:

Thank you for submitting your article "Disruption in structural-functional network repertoire and time-resolved subcortical-frontoparietal connectivity in disorders of consciousness" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by 2 peer reviewers, and the evaluation has been overseen by a Reviewing Editor and Jonathan Roiser as the Senior Editor. The following individual involved in the review of your submission has agreed to reveal their identity: Patricio Orio (Reviewer #1).

The reviewers have discussed their reviews with one another, and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this to help you prepare a revised submission.

Essential revisions:

1. There is frequent reference to "subcortical" and related networks, but I see no description in the text of which subcortical structures are involved. Panel N of Figure 2 is helpful but I think that more explicit detail is important, especially given the specific predictions of mesocircuit theory.

2. The authors have highlighted parallels with pharmacological states of unconsciousness and recovery therefrom. Two additional references to consider: Uhrig et al., (Anesthesiology, 2018, PMID: 30028727, which found similar convergence of structural/functional connectivity in the nonhuman primate neuronal workspace), and Mashour et al., (eLife, 2021, PMID: 33951427, which found support for global neuronal workspace in a human study of recovery from deep anaesthesia).

3. Similarly, although the global neuronal workspace does posit a critical role for recurrent frontal-parietal networks, can the authors be more specific about the nodes of the proposed workspace and what they found empirically?

4. The Results section gives the impression of a disconnected set of analyses that do not hang well together. In particular, the section about the structural eigenmodes and their relationship with the time-resolved FC seems to have little connection with the rest of the work, except for confirming (yet again) that DOC patients have a less dynamic FC. More elaboration about the relevance of these results, and what they say about DOC (that other dynamical FC analyses don't), is needed both in the Introduction and Discussion.

5. The classification sensitivity/specificity did not, in my opinion, add much to the manuscript, especially since the number of patients is not remotely close to what would be required for a population-based diagnostic approach. If the authors chose to include this with any reference to diagnosis (highlighted in the introduction and elsewhere), I would encourage a comparison with similar data from other clinical or neuroimaging-based diagnostic approaches. However, I think the value of the study resides more in terms of its mechanistic understanding than diagnosis.

6. Some comments about the connection(s) of these analyses with the commonly used FCD analysis (based on sliding windows of pair-wise correlations) will be useful, to put better this work into the big picture of time evolution of the functional connectivity.

7. The authors discuss how their data support the mesocircuit hypothesis and the global neuronal workspace hypothesis of conscious processing. However, these theories are neither interchangeable nor typically considered together. Could the authors be more specific in terms of how these theories interface vis-à-vis their empirical findings? Could they also be more explicit about whether their findings are inconsistent with other theories (e.g., IIT, predictive coding, higher-order thought)?

8. As a general point, the authors may have attempted too hard to give the paper a clinical/diagnostic (i.e., practical) emphasis; and in doing so, overlooked the clarification of some important issues. For example, clarifying the specific nodes under analysis and the relationship between the mesocircuit and global neuronal workspace theories is important. In addition, the authors need to consider whether this is primarily a neurobiological mechanisms study or a diagnostic/classification study. I would favour the former because the sensitivity/specificity analyses are not high yield and will require much more context in terms of other approaches.

Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

1) The authors discuss how their data support the mesocircuit hypothesis and the global neuronal workspace hypothesis of conscious processing. However, these theories are neither interchangeable nor typically considered together. Could the authors be more specific in terms of how these theories interface vis-à-vis their empirical findings? Could they also be more explicit about whether their findings are inconsistent with other theories (e.g., IIT, predictive coding, higher-order thought)?

2) The authors have highlighted parallels with pharmacological states of unconsciousness and recovery therefrom. Two additional references to consider: Uhrig et al., (Anesthesiology, 2018, PMID: 30028727, which found similar convergence of structural/functional connectivity in the nonhuman primate neuronal workspace), and Mashour et al., (eLife, 2021, PMID: 33951427, which found support for global neuronal workspace in a human study of recovery from deep anesthesia).

The authors are to be commended for this important contribution to the field.

eLife. 2022 Aug 2;11:e77462. doi: 10.7554/eLife.77462.sa2

Author response


Essential revisions:

1. There is frequent reference to "subcortical" and related networks, but I see no description in the text of which subcortical structures are involved. Panel N of Figure 2 is helpful but I think that more explicit detail is important, especially given the specific predictions of mesocircuit theory.

We have provided details for the subcortical networks presented in the Panel N of Figure 2. In the manuscript we provide a textual description of the brain areas that are part of the network. To improve the clarity of the description of the network, we also now refer to it as “subcortical fronto-temporoparietal (Sub-FTPN)”.

In the result section, it read as:

“This modulated subcortical fronto-temporoparietal network consist of the following brain regions: bilateral thalamus, caudate, right putamen, bilateral anterior and middle cingulate, inferior and middle frontal areas, supplementary motor cortex, middle and inferior temporal gyrus, right superior temporal, bilateral inferior parietal and supramarginal gyrus.”

2. The authors have highlighted parallels with pharmacological states of unconsciousness and recovery therefrom. Two additional references to consider: Uhrig et al., (Anesthesiology, 2018, PMID: 30028727, which found similar convergence of structural/functional connectivity in the nonhuman primate neuronal workspace), and Mashour et al., (eLife, 2021, PMID: 33951427, which found support for global neuronal workspace in a human study of recovery from deep anaesthesia).

We thank the reviewer for pointing us towards more relevant literature. We have now discussed and cited these two papers in the manuscript. The discussion on this topic now reads:

“Our observation of altered spatio-temporal dynamics of functional connectivity that are also more restricted to the structural connectivity has been observed in pharmacological and pathological loss of consciousness (Demertzi et al., 2019; López-González et al., 2021; Mashour et al., 2021; Uhrig et al., 2018). During unconscious states it has been demonstrated that cerebral regional connectivity leads to an important reconfiguration of the repertoire of functional brain states, that forms simpler connectivity patterns in the brain, which is predominantly shaped by anatomical connectivity and is characterized by the reduction of complex long range connectivity patterns (Barttfeld et al., 2014b; Demertzi et al., 2019; Mashour et al., 2021; Uhrig et al., 2018).”

3. Similarly, although the global neuronal workspace does posit a critical role for recurrent frontal-parietal networks, can the authors be more specific about the nodes of the proposed workspace and what they found empirically?

As above mentioned, we have provided more details about the regions part of the “subcortical fronto-temporoparietal”. As the reviewers rightfully noted, this network also shows some overlap with the Global Neuronal Workspace. We refer to that in more detail in the discussion, highlighting how our functional networks overlap and differ with the two networks (i.e., one feedforward only, one with recurrent activity), and with the predictions of the mesocircuit model. For more detail, please refer to our reply to point 7.

4. The Results section gives the impression of a disconnected set of analyses that do not hang well together. In particular, the section about the structural eigenmodes and their relationship with the time-resolved FC seems to have little connection with the rest of the work, except for confirming (yet again) that DOC patients have a less dynamic FC.

We understand the reviewer’s position. Part one of our work covers time-resolved FC and spatiotemporal networks in DoC. Part two covers the relationship between time-resolved FC and eigenmodes of the structural network. The rationale for including part two is the following: there is a lot of literature that shows that eigenmodes of the structural network can be considered as ‘building blocks’ or basis functions/vectors for spatiotemporal networks at the functional level (Aqil et al., 2021; Atasoy et al., 2016, 2018; Deslauriers-Gauthier et al., 2020; Gabay et al., 2018; Gabay and Robinson, 2017; Robinson et al., 2016; Robinson, 2021; Tewarie et al., 2019, 2020; Wang et al., 2017). Ideally to link part one and two, you would take this notion further by analysing if the magnitude eigenmode coefficients differed between UWS, MCS and healthy controls and how this would relate to dwell times or expression of spatiotemporal networks. However, this would lead to an immense multiple testing issue, which would be impossible to overcome with our sample size. An important link between part one and two of our work is the relationship between change in eigenmode expression and metastability. Our measure for metastability is only a proxy for metastability. Lack of change in eigenmode expressions seems to confirm this result of metastability.

To allow for better integration of part one and two of our work, we have added to the introduction:

“These eigenmodes can be considered as patterns of ‘hidden connectivity’ that come to expression at the level of functional networks. It has been postulated that eigenmodes form elementary building blocks for spatiotemporal dynamics (Aqil et al., 2021). There is evidence that the well-known resting state networks can be explained by activation of a small set of eigenmodes (Atasoy et al., 2018).”

We have also clarified in the result section:

“As resting-state network activity can be explained by activation of structural eigenmodes, we next analyse the role of fluctuations in eigenmode expression over time.”

More elaboration about the relevance of these results, and what they say about DOC (that other dynamical FC analyses don't), is needed both in the Introduction and Discussion.

We have now elaborated on the relevance of the eigenmode results in the discussion, which now reads:

“Previous studies have already demonstrated a strong signature of the underlying structural network in functional networks in DoC. However, these previously described functional networks were mostly obtained using time-averaged estimation methods (Annen et al., 2016; Enciso-Olivera et al., 2021; Kuceyeski et al., 2016; Lant et al., 2016), and have not provided insight into a stronger structural functional coupling in DoC. Using time-resolved functional connectivity and its relationship to eigenmodes, we shed light on this observation. The underlying anatomical connectivity contains so-called inherent ‘hidden patterns’ or eigenmodes with different spatial structures. In a dynamical system such as the brain, these eigenmodes, or a combination of eigenmodes, can be sequentially activated or deactivated (Atasoy et al., 2016; Tewarie et al., 2019), and thereby shape the repertoire of possible functional network states. Since we assume that structural and functional networks share the same eigenmodes (Abdelnour et al., 2018; Naze et al., 2021; Tewarie et al., 2020), the weighting coefficients of the eigenmodes that result in the structural network can be considered as ‘steady state’. Reconfigurations of spatiotemporal networks for the healthy brain are induced by strong deviations from this steady state, demonstrated by strong modulations in eigenmode expression in our work. In DoC however, there is strong tendency towards the ‘steady state’ with minimal deviations of eigenmode expressions over time, and hence leading to a strong signature of structural connections in functional networks in DoC. This was even more pronounced for UWS than for MCS.”

For clinical evaluation the structural support of the functional networks is less relevant as most variance is indeed explained by function, yet to understand the cerebral dysfunction a more coherent view is preferred. Moreover, as explained more in the response to point 5, we have removed the classification results from the manuscript to not deviate from our main findings.

5. The classification sensitivity/specificity did not, in my opinion, add much to the manuscript, especially since the number of patients is not remotely close to what would be required for a population-based diagnostic approach. If the authors chose to include this with any reference to diagnosis (highlighted in the introduction and elsewhere), I would encourage a comparison with similar data from other clinical or neuroimaging-based diagnostic approaches. However, I think the value of the study resides more in terms of its mechanistic understanding than diagnosis.

We agree with your suggestions that the primary aim of our work is to provide a mechanistic understanding of loss of consciousness. Therefore, we have removed the classification part from the paper and explain our findings focusing on mechanism of pathological unconsciousness rather than its potential as a clinical diagnostic tool. This change has required several textual edits throughout the manuscript.

6. Some comments about the connection(s) of these analyses with the commonly used FCD analysis (based on sliding windows of pair-wise correlations) will be useful, to put better this work into the big picture of time evolution of the functional connectivity.

We have now discussed sliding window-based analysis in the context of our work in the methodology section.

“Lastly, we have used a high temporal resolution method to estimate time-resolved connectivity at every time point instead of a sliding window-based method. Previous studies using sliding window approaches have provided novel insights into brain dynamics of loss of consciousness, such as the occurrence and reconfiguration of brain states in DoC patients (Demertzi et al., 2019) and anaesthesia induced loss of consciousness (Barttfeld et al., 2014a; Uhrig et al., 2018). However, sliding window approaches have limited sensitivity to non-stationarity in the fMRI BOLD signals (Hindriks et al., 2016). Recent work on time-resolved connectivity shows that brief periods in co-modulation of BOLD signals are an important driving factor for functional connectivity (Esfahlani et al., 2020; Hindriks et al., 2016), and sliding window approaches may be less sensitive to detect such brief periods in co-modulation. Furthermore, sliding window approaches in DoC have so far demonstrated limited success in retrieving canonical resting-state networks in DoC, while exploration of spatiotemporal aspects of well-known resting state networks is important to better understand the relation between brain dynamics and loss of consciousness.”

7. The authors discuss how their data support the mesocircuit hypothesis and the global neuronal workspace hypothesis of conscious processing. However, these theories are neither interchangeable nor typically considered together. Could the authors be more specific in terms of how these theories interface vis-à-vis their empirical findings? Could they also be more explicit about whether their findings are inconsistent with other theories (e.g., IIT, predictive coding, higher-order thought)?

Indeed, many other theories for the support of consciousness exist, and we do not discuss all. This is mostly because our study, from the methodological point of view, is limited to network function. Information integration, crucial for IIT, is not part of the study and linking our results to IIT would become too speculative. Likewise, we believe there are more appropriate experimental designs to study the predictive, that specifically allow to sensory input and error levels. The empirical predictions for the higher-order thought theory are not well developed, impairing the interpretation of our results in light to this theory.

The reviewer is correct that there is important overlap between the regions associated to the mesocircuit model and the GNW. We have therefore taken this opportunity to better clarify that in the discussion, which now reads:

“The mesocircuit hypothesis postulates that increased subcortical (specifically thalamic nuclei) inhibition and reduced excitatory output from the thalamus to the cortex (frontal, parietal, temporal, occipital) leads to weak neural activity and hampers consciousness in DoC patients (Fridman et al., 2014; Giacino et al., 2014; Schiff, 2010). Although our method does not allow to assess excitatory and inhibitory activity, it may be tempting to interpret that the subcortical fronto-temporoparietal network may play a crucial role in orchestrating global network interactions and dwell times.

Later in GNW paragraph, we added:

“Specifically, the GNW postulates that conscious perception should rely on two neural circuits. First, the feedforward circuit encompasses the visual and sensory areas and propagates sensory input to higher order integration areas. Second, in order for sensory input to be accessible to consciousness, recurrent activity in the circuit is required, responsible for ignition in a wide range of high-order functional areas including visual, parieto-temporal, thalamic and frontal areas (Aru et al., 2020; Baars, 2002; Dehaene and Changeux, 2011; Mashour et al., 2020). The subcortical fronto-temporoparietal network and frontoparietal network obtained in a data-driven fashion conjugates with the latter recurrent circuit proposed to be essential for consciousness by the GNW recurrent circuit. The breakdown of spatio-temporal coordination in the subcortical fronto-temporoparietal network and frontoparietal network was associated to reduced states of consciousness. So far, the importance of metastability has mainly been addressed in the context of recovery of consciousness from anaesthesia (Cavanna et al., 2018). Here, we underscore this finding and demonstrate that a reduction of subcortical-cortical spatiotemporal functional organization into metastable states can even differentiate between UWS and MCS patients aligning with key factors for consciousness as proposed by GNW and mesocircuit hypothesis.

Despite the overlapping cortical circuits important for the mesocircuit model and GNW, differences between the two hypotheses exist. While the occipital cortex is considered part of the same mesocircuit, its role is mostly for feedforward propagation in the GNW. We found a separate functional network that conceptually overlaps more with the feedforward network described by the GNW. In line with GNW predictions, the temporal dynamics in this feedforward network did not distinguish different states of consciousness (Dehaene and Changeux, 2011; Mashour et al., 2020). On the other hand, the involvement of subcortical areas in the subcortical fronto-temporoparietal network is specifically predicted by the mesocircuit model (Fridman et al., 2014; Giacino et al., 2014; Schiff, 2010). Last, the two theories highlight different aspects of brain activity required for consciousness. The GNW proposes specific effective connectivity patterns required for stimuli to reach consciousness. The mesocircuit model on the other hand proposes that balanced excitatory and inhibitory activity within the network is a prerequisite for conscious states to be supported. Our resting-state study conceptually thus resonates more with the concepts of the mesocircuit model, yet, we might hypothesize that relative preservation of the recurrent network crucial for the GNW meets the minimal requirement for conscious stimulus processing.”

8. As a general point, the authors may have attempted too hard to give the paper a clinical/diagnostic (i.e., practical) emphasis; and in doing so, overlooked the clarification of some important issues. For example, clarifying the specific nodes under analysis and the relationship between the mesocircuit and global neuronal workspace theories is important. In addition, the authors need to consider whether this is primarily a neurobiological mechanisms study or a diagnostic/classification study. I would favour the former because the sensitivity/specificity analyses are not high yield and will require much more context in terms of other approaches.

We agree with the reviewer. We have now focused the paper on mechanistic understanding of loss of consciousness rather than clinical/diagnostic impact.

We have modified the introduction and discussion in this direction in the current version of the paper. In the last paragraph of the Introduction, we explicitly state the aim of study:

“Finally, we conceptually link the findings of altered spatiotemporal dynamics underlying activity observed in the brain of DoC patients with several consciousness theories to increase our understanding of the mechanism behind pathological unconsciousness”.

Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

1) The authors discuss how their data support the mesocircuit hypothesis and the global neuronal workspace hypothesis of conscious processing. However, these theories are neither interchangeable nor typically considered together. Could the authors be more specific in terms of how these theories interface vis-à-vis their empirical findings? Could they also be more explicit about whether their findings are inconsistent with other theories (e.g., IIT, predictive coding, higher-order thought)?

Indeed, many other theories for the support of consciousness exist, and we do not discuss all. This is mostly because our study, from the methodological point of view, is limited to network function. Information integration, crucial for IIT, is not part of the study and linking our results to IIT would become too speculative. Likewise, we believe there are more appropriate experimental designs to study the predictive, that specifically allow to sensory input and error levels. The empirical predictions for the higher-order thought theory are not well developed, impairing the interpretation of our results in light to this theory.

The reviewer is correct that there is important overlap between the regions associated to the mesocircuit model and the GNW. We have therefore taken this opportunity to better clarify that in the discussion, which now reads:

“The mesocircuit hypothesis postulates that increased subcortical (specifically thalamic nuclei) inhibition and reduced excitatory output from the thalamus to the cortex (frontal, parietal, temporal, occipital) leads to weak neural activity and hampers consciousness in DoC patients (Fridman et al., 2014; Giacino et al., 2014; Schiff, 2010). Although our method does not allow to assess excitatory and inhibitory activity, it may be tempting to interpret that the subcortical fronto-temporoparietal network may play a crucial role in orchestrating global network interactions and dwell times.

Later in GNW paragraph, we added:

Specifically, the GNW postulates that conscious perception should rely on two neural circuits. First, the feedforward circuit encompasses the visual and sensory areas and propagates sensory input to higher order integration areas. Second, in order for sensory input to be accessible to consciousness, recurrent activity in the circuit is required, responsible for ignition in a wide range of high-order functional areas including visual, parieto-temporal, thalamic and frontal areas (Aru et al., 2020; Baars, 2002; Dehaene and Changeux, 2011; Mashour et al., 2020). The subcortical fronto-temporoparietal network and frontoparietal network obtained in a data-driven fashion conjugates with the latter recurrent circuit proposed to be essential for consciousness by the GNW recurrent circuit. The breakdown of spatio-temporal coordination in the subcortical fronto-temporoparietal network and frontoparietal network was associated to reduced states of consciousness. So far, the importance of metastability has mainly been addressed in the context of recovery of consciousness from anaesthesia (Cavanna et al., 2018). Here, we underscore this finding and demonstrate that a reduction of subcortical-cortical spatiotemporal functional organization into metastable states can even differentiate between UWS and MCS patients aligning with key factors for consciousness as proposed by GNW and mesocircuit hypothesis.

Despite the overlapping cortical circuits important for the mesocircuit model and GNW, differences between the two hypotheses exist. While the occipital cortex is considered part of the same mesocircuit, its role is mostly for feedforward propagation in the GNW. We found a separate functional network that conceptually overlaps more with the feedforward network described by the GNW. In line with GNW predictions, the temporal dynamics in this feedforward network did not distinguish different states of consciousness (Dehaene and Changeux, 2011; Mashour et al., 2020). On the other hand, the involvement of subcortical areas in the subcortical fronto-temporoparietal network is specifically predicted by the mesocircuit model (Fridman et al., 2014; Giacino et al., 2014; Schiff, 2010). Last, the two theories highlight different aspects of brain activity required for consciousness. The GNW proposes specific effective connectivity patterns required for stimuli to reach consciousness. The mesocircuit model on the other hand proposes that balanced excitatory and inhibitory activity within the network is a prerequisite for conscious states to be supported. Our resting-state study conceptually thus resonates more with the concepts of the mesocircuit model, yet, we might hypothesize that relative preservation of the recurrent network crucial for the GNW meets the minimal requirement for conscious stimulus processing.”

2) The authors have highlighted parallels with pharmacological states of unconsciousness and recovery therefrom. Two additional references to consider: Uhrig et al., (Anesthesiology, 2018, PMID: 30028727, which found similar convergence of structural/functional connectivity in the nonhuman primate neuronal workspace), and Mashour et al., (eLife, 2021, PMID: 33951427, which found support for global neuronal workspace in a human study of recovery from deep anesthesia).

We thank the reviewer for pointing us towards more relevant literature. We have now discussed and cited these two papers in the manuscript. The discussion on this topic now reads:

“Our observation of altered spatio-temporal dynamics of functional connectivity that are also more restricted to the structural connectivity has been observed in pharmacological and pathological loss of consciousness (Demertzi et al., 2019; López-González et al., 2021; Mashour et al., 2021; Uhrig et al., 2018). During unconscious states it has been demonstrated that cerebral regional connectivity leads to an important reconfiguration of the repertoire of functional brain states, that forms simpler connectivity patterns in the brain, which is predominantly shaped by anatomical connectivity and is characterized by the reduction of complex long range connectivity patterns (Barttfeld et al., 2014b; Demertzi et al., 2019; Mashour et al., 2021; Uhrig et al., 2018).”

References

Abdelnour F, Dayan M, Devinsky O, Thesen T, Raj A. 2018. Functional brain connectivity is predictable from anatomic network’s Laplacian eigen-structure. Neuroimage 172. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.02.016

Annen J, Heine L, Ziegler E, Frasso G, Bahri M, Di Perri C, Stender J, Martial C, Wannez S, D’ostilio K. 2016. Function–structure connectivity in patients with severe brain injury as measured by MRI‐DWI and FDG‐PET. Hum Brain Mapp 37:3707–3720.

Aqil M, Atasoy S, Kringelbach ML, Hindriks R. 2021. Graph neural fields: A framework for spatiotemporal dynamical models on the human connectome. PLoS Comput Biol 17. doi:10.1371/JOURNAL.PCBI.1008310

Aru J, Suzuki M, Larkum ME. 2020. Cellular Mechanisms of Conscious Processing. Trends Cogn Sci. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2020.07.006

Atasoy S, Deco G, Kringelbach ML, Pearson J. 2018. Harmonic Brain Modes: A Unifying Framework for Linking Space and Time in Brain Dynamics. Neuroscientist. doi:10.1177/1073858417728032

Atasoy S, Donnelly I, Pearson J. 2016. Human brain networks function in connectome-specific harmonic waves. Nat Commun 7. doi:10.1038/ncomms10340

Baars BJ. 2002. The conscious access hypothesis: Origins and recent evidence. Trends Cogn Sci. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01819-2

Barttfeld P, Uhrig L, Sitt JD, Sigman M, Jarraya B, Dehaene S. 2014a. Signature of consciousness in the dynamics of resting-state brain activity. Proc Natl Acad Sci 112:201418031.

Barttfeld P, Uhrig L, Sitt JD, Sigman M, Jarraya B, Dehaene S. 2014b. Signature of consciousness in the dynamics of resting-state brain activity. Proc Natl Acad Sci 112:201418031. doi:10.1073/pnas.1418031112

Cavanna F, Vilas MG, Palmucci M, Tagliazucchi E. 2018. Dynamic functional connectivity and brain metastability during altered states of consciousness. Neuroimage 180:383–395.

Dehaene S, Changeux JP. 2011. Experimental and Theoretical Approaches to Conscious Processing. Neuron. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2011.03.018

Demertzi A, Tagliazucchi E, Dehaene S, Deco G, Barttfeld P, Raimondo F, Martial C, Fernández-Espejo D, Rohaut B, Voss HU, Schiff ND, Owen AM, Laureys S, Naccache L, Sitt JD. 2019. Human consciousness is supported by dynamic complex patterns of brain signal coordination. Sci Adv 5. doi:10.1126/sciadv.aat7603

Deslauriers-Gauthier S, Zucchelli M, Frigo M, Deriche R. 2020. A unified framework for multimodal structure–function mapping based on eigenmodes. Med Image Anal 66. doi:10.1016/j.media.2020.101799

Enciso-Olivera CO, Ordóñez-Rubiano EG, Casanova-Libreros R, Rivera D, Zarate-Ardila CJ, Rudas J, Pulido C, Gómez F, Martínez D, Guerrero N, Hurtado MA, Aguilera-Bustos N, Hernández-Torres CP, Hernandez J, Marín-Muñoz JH. 2021. Structural and functional connectivity of the ascending arousal network for prediction of outcome in patients with acute disorders of consciousness. Sci Rep 11. doi:10.1038/s41598-021-98506-7

Esfahlani FZ, Jo Y, Faskowitz J, Byrge L, Kennedy DP, Sporns O, Betzel RF. 2020. High-amplitude cofluctuations in cortical activity drive functional connectivity. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 117. doi:10.1073/pnas.2005531117

Fridman EA, Beattie BJ, Broft A, Laureys S, Schiff ND. 2014. Regional cerebral metabolic patterns demonstrate the role of anterior forebrain mesocircuit dysfunction in the severely injured brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci 111. doi:10.1073/pnas.1320969111

Gabay NC, Babaie-Janvier T, Robinson PA. 2018. Dynamics of cortical activity eigenmodes including standing, traveling, and rotating waves. Phys Rev E 98. doi:10.1103/PhysRevE.98.042413

Gabay NC, Robinson PA. 2017. Cortical geometry as a determinant of brain activity eigenmodes: Neural field analysis. Phys Rev E 96. doi:10.1103/PhysRevE.96.032413

Giacino JT, Fins JJ, Laureys S, Schiff ND. 2014. Disorders of consciousness after acquired brain injury: the state of the science. Nat Rev Neurol 10:99.

Hindriks R, Adhikari MH, Murayama Y, Ganzetti M, Mantini D, Logothetis NK, Deco G. 2016. Can sliding-window correlations reveal dynamic functional connectivity in resting-state fMRI? Neuroimage 127:242–256.

Kuceyeski A, Shah S, Dyke JP, Bickel S, Abdelnour F, Schiff ND, Voss HU, Raj A. 2016. The application of a mathematical model linking structural and functional connectomes in severe brain injury. NeuroImage Clin 11. doi:10.1016/j.nicl.2016.04.006

Lant ND, Gonzalez-Lara LE, Owen AM, Fernández-Espejo D. 2016. Relationship between the anterior forebrain mesocircuit and the default mode network in the structural bases of disorders of consciousness. NeuroImage Clin 10. doi:10.1016/j.nicl.2015.11.004

López-González A, Panda R, Ponce-Alvarez A, Zamora-López G, Escrichs A, Martial C, Thibaut A, Gosseries O, Kringelbach ML, Annen J, Laureys S, Deco G. 2021. Loss of consciousness reduces the stability of brain hubs and the heterogeneity of brain dynamics. Commun Biol 4:2020.11.20.391482. doi:10.1038/s42003-021-02537-9

Mashour GA, Palanca BJA, Basner M, Li D, Wang W, Blain-Moraes S, Lin N, Maier K, Muench M, Tarnal V, Vanini G, Ochroch EA, Hogg R, Schwartz M, Maybrier H, Hardie R, Janke E, Golmirzaie G, Picton P, McKinstry-Wu AR, Avidan MS, Kelz MB. 2021. Recovery of consciousness and cognition after general anesthesia in humans. eLife 10. doi:10.7554/eLife.59525

Mashour GA, Roelfsema P, Changeux JP, Dehaene S. 2020. Conscious Processing and the Global Neuronal Workspace Hypothesis. Neuron. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2020.01.026

Naze S, Proix T, Atasoy S, Kozloski JR. 2021. Robustness of connectome harmonics to local gray matter and long-range white matter connectivity changes: Sensitivity analysis of Connectome Harmonics. Neuroimage 224. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117364

Robinson PA. 2021. Discrete spectral eigenmode-resonance network of brain dynamics and connectivity. Phys Rev E 104. doi:10.1103/PhysRevE.104.034411

Robinson PA, Zhao X, Aquino KM, Griffiths JD, Sarkar S, Mehta-Pandejee G. 2016. Eigenmodes of brain activity: Neural field theory predictions and comparison with experiment. Neuroimage 142:79–98.

Schiff ND. 2010. Recovery of consciousness after brain injury: a mesocircuit hypothesis. Trends Neurosci. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2009.11.002

Tewarie P, Abeysuriya R, Byrne Á, O’Neill GC, Sotiropoulos SN, Brookes MJ, Coombes S. 2019. How do spatially distinct frequency specific MEG networks emerge from one underlying structural connectome? The role of the structural eigenmodes. Neuroimage 186. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.10.079

Tewarie P, Prasse B, Meier JM, Santos FAN, Douw L, Schoonheim MM, Stam CJ, Van Mieghem P, Hillebrand A. 2020. Mapping functional brain networks from the structural connectome: Relating the series expansion and eigenmode approaches. Neuroimage 216. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116805

Uhrig L, Sitt JD, Jacob A, Tasserie J, Barttfeld P, Dupont M, Dehaene S, Jarraya B. 2018. Resting-state Dynamics as a Cortical Signature of Anesthesia in Monkeys. Anesthesiology 129. doi:10.1097/ALN.0000000000002336

Wang MB, Owen JP, Mukherjee P, Raj A. 2017. Brain network eigenmodes provide a robust and compact representation of the structural connectome in health and disease. PLoS Comput Biol 13. doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005550

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Data Citations

    1. López-González A, Panda R, Barra A, Bonin EAC, Ponce-Alvarez A, Zamora-López G, Sanz LRD, Escrichs A, Cecconi B, Martial C, Thibaut A, Gosseries O, Annen J, Deco G, Laureys S. 2021. Phase-interaction matrices of the phase relationships of different pathological states of consciousness. Human Brain Project. [DOI]

    Supplementary Materials

    Supplementary file 1.

    (A) Details about the patient population. (B) Brain regions involved in the extracted networks by non-negative tensor factorisation.

    elife-77462-supp1.docx (41KB, docx)
    MDAR checklist

    Data Availability Statement

    Connectivity matrices have been made available open access through EBRAINS of the Human Brain Project.

    The following previously published dataset was used:

    López-González A, Panda R, Barra A, Bonin EAC, Ponce-Alvarez A, Zamora-López G, Sanz LRD, Escrichs A, Cecconi B, Martial C, Thibaut A, Gosseries O, Annen J, Deco G, Laureys S. 2021. Phase-interaction matrices of the phase relationships of different pathological states of consciousness. Human Brain Project.


    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES