Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2022 Oct 5;17(10):e0274833. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0274833

Effects of contact with a dog on prefrontal brain activity: A controlled trial

Rahel Marti 1,2,*, Milena Petignat 1, Valentine L Marcar 2,3,4, Jan Hattendorf 5, Martin Wolf 4, Margret Hund-Georgiadis 2, Karin Hediger 1,2,5,6
Editor: Anna Manelis7
PMCID: PMC9534402  PMID: 36197880

Abstract

Background

There is a broad range of known effects of animal contact on human mental and physical health. Neurological correlates of human interaction with animals have been sparsely investigated. We investigated changes in frontal brain activity in the presence of and during contact with a dog.

Methods

Twenty-one healthy individuals each participated in six sessions. In three sessions, participants had contact with a dog, and in three control sessions they interacted with a plush animal. Each session had five two-minute phases with increasing intensity of contact to the dog or plush animal from the first to the fourth phase. We measured oxygenated, deoxygenated, and total hemoglobin and oxygen saturation of the blood in the frontal lobe/frontopolar area with functional near-infrared spectroscopy (SenSmart Model X-100) to assess brain activity.

Findings

In both conditions, the concentration of oxygenated hemoglobin increased significantly from the first to the fourth phase by 2.78 μmol/l (CI = 2.03–3.53, p < .001). Oxygenated hemoglobin concentration was 0.80 μmol/l higher in the dog condition compared to in the control condition (CI = 0.27–1.33, p = .004). Deoxygenated-hemoglobin concentration, total hemoglobin concentration, and oxygen saturation showed similar patterns.

Conclusion

Prefrontal brain activation in healthy subjects increased with the rise in interaction closeness with a dog or a plush animal. Moreover, interaction with a dog stimulated more brain activity compared to the control condition, suggesting that interactions with a dog can activate stronger attentional processes and elicit more emotional arousal than interacting with a nonliving stimulus.

1 Introduction

Although the effects of contact with animals on human mental and physical health have received increasing attention [15], the neurophysiological correlates of these effects are not yet fully understood [6, 7]. These correlates are, however, highly relevant to understanding the mechanisms underlying human–animal relationship [811] and to designing effective animal-assisted interventions. Authors of several studies have reported that positive interaction with a dog reduces stress parameters, such as blood pressure, heart rate, or cortisol level [1214], and leads to an increase of neurochemicals associated with bonding or affiliation, such as β-endorphin, oxytocin, and prolactin [1517]. However, the results for these parameters remain inconclusive [13, 18, 19].

Investigations into neurological correlates in the context of human–animal interaction are scarce. Initial studies have investigated neurological reactions to interactions with animals using neuroimaging techniques [2027]. Most of these studies presented images of animals, whereas only a small number of investigations have addressed the effects of real animals. A positron-emission-tomography (PET) study observed that brain areas associated with stress and sympathetic arousal were less activated in the presence of a familiar dog than in a relaxing condition [23]. Other investigators have observed lateralization with greater activity in the right frontopolar area while petting a horse compared to petting a plush animal, seeing a horse, or seeing a plush animal [24]. Another study measuring hemodynamic response found that participants reacted with activation in the left inferior frontal gyrus while petting a cat [25]. Moreover, children showed higher activity in the prefrontal cortex in an attention task after interacting with a dog than after interacting with a robot dog [26]. Similarly, in a small pilot study, participants had a stronger brain reaction to a live animal than to a mechanical toy animal [27]. While these studies provide first insights into neurological correlates of the human–animal interaction, additional research is needed to understand what happens in different forms of human–animal interactions. The knowledge gained will be crucial for conducting effective animal-assisted interventions [28]. Dogs are the most common animals used in animal-assisted interventions [4, 29, 30]. The aim of this study was to investigate neurological correlates of different forms of human–dog contact in an animal-assisted intervention setting using a strong study design. To ensure that the results would be as valuable as possible for practical application, we investigated the reactions of the participants in an animal-assisted intervention setting in a clinic and involving direct contact and interaction with a dog. This also enabled us to control for different amounts of contact with the dog.

Interacting with an animal is a social situation that is emotionally relevant to most people [7, 3134]. Several reviews have identified the prefrontal cortex as the key region for different aspects of social cognitive processing, such as theory of mind/mentalizing [35] and understanding self and others [36]. Activity in the prefrontal cortex is thus important for investigating the underlying mechanisms of human–animal interactions.

Our study aimed to investigate brain activation in the prefrontal cortex of healthy subjects with functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) in a controlled trial. We compared different forms of interaction with a dog and different forms of interaction with a plush animal. We expected, first, that the increase of closeness in contact with a dog or plush animal would correlate with an increased amount of stimulation and therefore also with increased brain activity. Second, we hypothesized that participants would exhibit higher brain activity in the dog condition compared to the control condition with the plush animal.

2 Materials and methods

2.1 Study design

The study had a controlled, within-subject crossover design with repeated measurements. Participants were measured during six standardized sessions (1–6) consisting of three sessions with a live dog and three control sessions with a plush animal. The six sessions took place within 2 weeks. The sequence of the conditions within these six sessions was influenced by the presence of the dog and therefore only partly randomized. The study design was approved by the local ethics committee, Ethics Commission Northwest and Central Switzerland (Project ID 2017–00540), and by the Veterinary Office of the Canton of Basel-Stadt, Switzerland (No. 2713) and was registered at clinicaltrials.gov (NCT03341325). The study design followed the Animals (Scientific Procedure) Act 1986, European Directive EU 2010/63, and the guidelines for handling animals in research as outlined by the Association for Studies on Animal Behavior and the Society for Animal Behavior. All sessions were conducted according to the guidelines of the International Association for Human–Animal Interaction Organizations and the Helsinki guidelines [37, 38]. We planned to compare the results of this study with a study population of patients with severe disorders of consciousness in a future trial, so the study design complied with the requirements for measuring a group of patients with severe disorders of consciousness.

2.2 Participants

Twenty-one healthy subjects (10 women, 11 men) participated in this study. Participants were over 18 years old and without allergies or phobias toward dogs. They were recruited with flyers at the Faculty of Psychology at the University of Basel and via an advertisement on the university’s website. We obtained written informed consent from every participant before the study started. The sample size was determined a priori based on data from a previous study [39] and with regard to the pilot character of this study.

2.3 Procedure

The sessions were held in a room at the neurorehabilitation center REHAB Basel in Switzerland from February 2018 until July 2018. During the experiments, the participants sat upright on a Bobath therapy couch. They faced a white wall located at a distance of 1.5 m. The study staff attached two fNIRS sensors to measure oxygen saturation on the participants’ foreheads. Three of the six sessions per participant were conducted in the presence of a dog and three with a plush animal (see Fig 1). The participants therefore had a first, second, and third contact with both the dog and the plush animal. All sessions were videotaped, and heart rate and electrodermal activity were recorded. Each session consisted of five 2-minute phases, which were always conducted in a similar way and in the same order in both the dog and plush-animal conditions. Before each phase, the study staff verbally instructed the participant according to a standardized protocol. The first phase served as a baseline where the participant looked straight at the white wall and relaxed (neutral 1). In the next phase, the participant watched a dog or a plush animal from a distance of 1 m (watching). The dog or plush animal was placed or asked to lie on a mat and a blanket on a height-adjustable table. Then the dog lay down next to the participant on the couch or the plush animal was placed on the participant’s thigh. The participant could passively feel the animal but was not yet allowed to pet it (feeling). Next, the participant petted the dog or the plush animal (petting). Finally, there was a second neutral phase where the participant again looked at the white wall while the dog or the plush animal was out of sight (neutral 2). Each phase concluded after 2 minutes, and then there was a short break in which the study staff prepared the room for the next phase. Interactions between each participant and the dog or the plush animal were standardized and comparable regarding the amount of contact.

Fig 1. Study procedure.

Fig 1

For every participant, we scheduled three of the sessions in the morning and three in the afternoon to control for time of day. The order of the phases was not counterbalanced because the same design was also used for patients with severe disorders of consciousness. These patients need time and a lot of context to understand a situation. A random order with a sudden increase of contact to the animal would not be ethically justifiable. For the same reason, it was not possible to measure a pretask and posttest baseline for each phase.

2.4 Dogs

The dogs participating in the study were used to human contact and trained to work with patients in a hospital setting. The dogs were a female Jack Russel (6 years of age), a female Goldendoodle (4 years of age), and a female Golden Retriever (4 years of age). Each dog participated in a maximum of two sessions in a row. The dogs and their owner were in the room before the sessions started, which enabled them to become acquainted with the room and to feel safe. The dog owner was present throughout the session and was responsible for handling the dog but was instructed not to interact with the participant during the measurements. The dogs were trained to lie silently on the table and beside the participant in contact with the participant’s thigh, but they could choose their position themselves. Owners monitored their dogs for signs of stress and predetermined stop criteria. Due to the highly standardized situations and interaction, the behavior of the dog was comparable between the sessions within and between participants.

2.5 Plush animal

For the control sessions, we used a lion plush animal. The plush animal (58 × 40 × 20 cm) contained in its body a hot water bottle that was filled with warm water before the sessions started to control not only for the sensation of soft fur but also for the body temperature and weight of a dog. We introduced the plush animal to participants as “Leo.”

2.6 Functional near-infrared spectroscopy

We chose fNIRS to measure the response in the prefrontal cortex as it is particularly suited for investigating the neuronal correlates of such a complex social situation of human–animal interaction. fNIRS has been used as a noninvasive technique to measure brain activity within the context of human–animal interactions [24, 25, 27, 40, 41]. Compared to functional magnetic-resonance imaging (fMRI) or PET, participants are not confined to a scanner but can sit or stand during measurements. This makes the test situation more comparable to clinical situations. fNIRS also has other advantages: there are no disturbing sounds, and the device is easy to handle. fNIRS is a vascular-based neuroimaging technology that measures the oxygen saturation of hemoglobin and changes in total hemoglobin concentration (tHb) based on the characteristic hemoglobin-absorption spectra in the near-infrared range. This technology relies on the well-known tight neurovascular coupling, which induces changes in oxygen saturation and tHb in response to neuronal activity. An increase in oxygenated hemoglobin (O2Hb) in the region of an activated cortical area mirrors increased brain activity [42].

We recorded percent oxygen saturation (%) and tHb (g/dl) in the prefrontal cortex using a Nonin fNIRS device (SenSmart Model X-100). Two sensors of the device (Model 8004CA Sensors–Adhesive) were placed right and left of the midline on the forehead as close to the hairline as possible and then attached with an adjustable band. This corresponded to locations F1, F3, F2, and F4 on the frontopolar area according to the international 10–20 system and to the Brodmann areas 9, 10, and 46. The wavelength of the infrared light was 730, 760, 810, and 880 nm, and measurements were recorded at a frequency of 0.25 Hz. After recording, data were transferred to a laptop using SenSmart software (version 1.0.1.0).

Within this study, we also measured other physiological endpoints such as heart rate, heart-rate variability, and skin conductance. These data will be published separately.

2.7 Data processing and analysis

We converted the data from g/dl to μmol/l based on the molar mass of hemoglobin of 64458 g/mol. We calculated the concentration of O2Hb and HHb from raw data. To exclude unreliable data due to measurement errors, two raters independently rated plots of the data for reliability. The raters were blinded for the condition. Conflicts were resolved by a third rater (R. M.).

For all included data, we calculated the mean concentration of O2Hb, HHb, and tHb and mean oxygen saturation in each phase. To do so, we cut the data from one session into segments of five 2-minute phases at the markings. The data between the phases was not used. We were interested in changes from phase to phase, so we subtracted the mean of the first phase from each following phase within the same session for each participant.

O2Hb reflects the neuronal-discharge frequency, while HHb reflects the quantity of recruited neurons [43]. We chose O2Hb as the primary outcome because O2Hb more directly reflects task-related cortical activation than does HHb [44]. HHb, tHb, and oxygen saturation served as secondary outcomes. For the primary and secondary outcomes, we conducted prespecified linear mixed-effect models and used the mean difference as the effect size. Within the models, condition and phase were used as fixed effects, and an intercept for the participant was used as the random effect. We conducted the same models again with visibility of the dog owner as a fixed effect.

We conducted explorative analyses because repetition of contact with the dog or the plush animal seemed to influence the outcome. Within these nonprespecified linear mixed-effect models, condition and contact (first, second, or third contact between participant and dog or plush animal) were used as fixed effects. Moreover, we included an interaction term and an intercept for the participant as the random effect.

We visually checked the normality (q-q plot, histogram of residuals), linearity, and homoscedasticity (residuals vs. fitted plot), and influential outliers (leverage and Cook’s distance). Leverage was checked with the R package influence.ME [45]. The significance level was set at .05. All analyses were conducted with R 4.1.0 [46] and R package lme4 [47].

3 Results

Of the 21 participants measured between January and July 2018, one participant dropped out after one session. We conducted 119 of the 126 planned sessions (Fig 2). Of these 119 sessions, we excluded data from one channel for 55 sessions and from both channels for 10 sessions due to low data quality (Fig 2). Six of these 10 completely removed datasets originated from one participant who dropped out of the analysis, while the other removed datasets were distributed among different participants. We thus analyzed 108 sessions (53 dog conditions, 55 plush-animal conditions) of 19 participants with at least one of the two channels available.

Fig 2. Flow diagram of the study.

Fig 2

These 19 participants compromised nine women and 10 men. The mean age was 32.4 years (SD = 12.8) and did not differ between the sexes (estimate = 2.2, CI = −15.4–11.1, p = .732). On average, we analyzed 2.89 control sessions and 2.84 dog sessions per participant. The number of analyzed sessions per participant did not differ between the conditions (M = 2.87, SD = 0.34; estimate = 0.05, CI = 0.18–0.28, p = .642). The first session was significantly more often the dog condition (14/19, p = .025), and the second session was significantly more often the control condition (14/18, p = .025). In sessions three to six, the number of sessions per condition did not differ significantly. In two-thirds of the sessions in the dog condition, the participant could see the dog owner during the measurement. No adverse or unintended effects in participants or in the involved dogs occurred during data collection.

3.1 Primary analysis

With increased stimulation, oxygenated hemoglobin (O2Hb) in the prefrontal lobe increased significantly from phase neutral 1 to phase petting by 2.78 μmol/l (CI = 2.03–3.53, p < .001). After removal of the stimulation in phase neutral 2, O2Hb stayed constant and was still significantly higher compared to phase neutral 1 (estimate = 2.91 μmol/l, CI = 2.16–3.65, p < .001).

O2Hb was 0.80 μmol/l higher in the presence of the dog compared to in the presence of the plush animal (CI = 0.27–1.33, p = .004). The difference between the conditions was highest in the phase petting (Fig 3A). This result was not influenced by the visibility of the dog owner.

Fig 3. Effects of condition and phase on O2Hb, HHb, tHb, and oxygen saturation.

Fig 3

(A) O2Hb, (B) HHb, (C) tHb, and (D) oxygen saturation. Error bars denote confidence interval. Data is shown as relative change from phase neutral 1.

3.2 Secondary analysis

3.2.1 Deoxygenated hemoglobin

When stimulation increased, deoxygenated hemoglobin (HHb) in the prefrontal lobe decreased significantly from phase neutral 1 to the petting phase by 1.23 μmol/l (CI = −1.75 to −0.72, p = .003). After removal of the stimulation in phase neutral 2, HHb stayed constant and was still significantly lower compared to phase neutral 1 (estimate = −1.20 μmol/l, CI = −1.72 to −0.69, p = .005).

HHb tended to be lower in the presence of the dog compared to in the presence of the plush animal (estimate = -0.35 μmol/l, CI = −0.71–0.02, p = 0.064). The difference was highest in phase neutral 2 (Fig 3B). This result was not influenced by the visibility of the dog owner.

3.2.2 Total hemoglobin

When stimulation increased, total hemoglobin (tHb) in the prefrontal lobe increased significantly from phase neutral 1 to the petting phase by 1.54 μmol/l (CI = 1.08–2.01, p < .001). After removal of the stimulation in phase neutral 2, tHb stayed constant and was still significantly higher compared to phase neutral 1 (estimate = 1.70 μmol/l, CI = 1.24–2.17, p < .001).

The concentration of tHb was significantly higher by 0.45 μmol/l in the presence of the dog compared to in the presence of the plush animal (CI = 0.12–0.78, p = .008). The difference was highest in the petting phase (Fig 3C). In the dog condition, tHb was lower when the participant could see the dog owner than when the dog owner was out of sight (estimate = −0.84, CI = −1.33 to −0.33, p < .001). The results of the other factors in the model, including visibility of the dog owner, remained unchanged.

3.2.3 Oxygen saturation

When stimulation increased, oxygen saturation in the prefrontal lobe increased significantly from phase neutral 1 to the petting phase by 0.93% (CI = 0.64–1.22, p < .001). After removal of the stimulation in phase neutral 2, saturation stayed constant and was still significantly higher compared to phase neutral 1 (estimate = 0.97%, CI = 0.68–1.27, p < .001).

Oxygen saturation was significantly higher by 0.21% in the presence of the dog compared to in the presence of the plush animal (CI = 0.00–0.42, p = .047). The difference was highest in phase neutral 2 (Fig 3D). The visibility of the dog owner had no effect.

3.3 Explorative analysis

During the first contact (first session), there was no relevant difference in O2Hb between the dog condition and the plush-animal condition (estimate dog = 2.15 μmol/l, estimate plush animal = 2.60 μmol/l). We observed a significant interaction, which indicates that with repeated contact over time, there was an increasing difference between the dog condition and the plush-animal condition (second contact: p = .001, third contact: p = .023, Table 1, Fig 4A).

Table 1. Marginal effects of condition by number of contacts.

95% CI  
Estimate Lower limit Upper limit
O 2 Hb
Dog condition
First contact 2.15 1.35 2.95
Second contact 3.43 2.62 4.25
Third contact 3.36 2.52 4.19
Plush-animal condition
First contact 2.60 1.78 3.41
Second contact 1.62 0.81 2.44
Third contact 2.25 1.45 3.05
HHb
Dog condition
First contact −0.98 −1.53 −0.43
Second contact −1.70 −2.26 −1.14
Third contact −1.31 −1.89 −0.74
Plush-animal condition
First contact −1.16 −1.72 −0.60
Second contact −0.44 −1.00 0.12
Third contact −1.32 −1.87 −0.76
tHb
Dog condition
First contact 1.17 0.66 1.68
Second contact 1.74 1.22 2.26
Third contact 2.04 1.50 2.57
Plush-animal condition
First contact 1.44 0.92 1.96
Second contact 1.18 0.66 1.70
Third contact 0.94 0.42 1.45
Oxygen saturation
Dog condition
First contact 0.77 0.47 1.08
Second contact 1.10 0.78 1.41
Third contact 0.98 0.66 1.30
Plush-animal condition
First contact 0.80 0.49 1.11
Second contact 0.45 0.13 0.76
Third contact 0.95 0.64 1.26

Marginal effects were estimated by condition and contact number, and an intercept for participant as random effect.

Fig 4. Effects of condition and number of contacts on O2Hb, HHb, tHb, and oxygen saturation.

Fig 4

(A) O2Hb, (B) HHb, (C) tHb, and (D) oxygen saturation. Error bars denote confidence intervals. Data are shown as relative change from phase neutral 1. The data for phase neutral 1 are not included in the presented means.

There was no relevant difference in HHb between the dog condition and the plush-animal condition during the first contact (dog = −0.98 μmol/l, plush animal = −1.16 μmol/l). We observed a significant interaction between the condition and number of contacts with an effect on HHb in the second contact but not in the third (second contact: p = .002, third contact: p = .695, Table 1, Fig 4B).

During the first contact, there was no relevant difference in tHb between the dog condition and the plush-animal condition (dog = 1.17 μmol/l, plush animal = 1.44 μmol/l). We observed a significant interaction effect on tHb, which indicates that the difference between the dog condition and the plush-animal condition increased with repeated contact over time (second contact: p = .053, third contact: p = .001, Table 1, Fig 4C).

There was no relevant difference in oxygen saturation between the dog condition and the plush-animal condition during the first contact (dog = 0.77%, plush animal = 0.80%). We observed a significant interaction between the condition and number of contacts with an effect on oxygen saturation in the second contact but not in the third (second contact: p = .010, third contact: p = .823, Table 1, Fig 4D).

4 Discussion

This study compared the prefrontal brain activity of healthy adults during contact with a dog and contact with a plush animal. Prefrontal activity increased with increased intensity of contact with a dog or a plush animal. This confirms our first hypothesis that more stimulation correlates with higher brain activity. It also corroborates previous studies linking closer contact with animals or control stimuli with increased frontal brain activation [24, 25, 27].

The participants had higher prefrontal brain activity when they interacted with a dog than when they interacted with a plush animal. This confirms our second hypothesis. In the presence of the dog, O2Hb, tHb, and oxygen saturation were significantly higher while HHb tended to be lower compared to the control condition. This pattern indicates increased oxygen consummation in prefrontal areas and thus higher brain activation in the presence of a dog [48, 49]. This result is in line with previous studies. An fNIRS pilot study with patients in a minimally conscious state and healthy controls found that three of four participants showed a higher hemodynamic response when stroking a live animal (dog, rabbit, or guinea pig) compared to stroking a mechanical toy [27]. Children who underwent a 20-min session with a therapy dog after surgery showed faster electroencephalogram diffuse beta activity, while children in the control group who received standard postoperative care showed no beta activity [41]. The passive infrared hemoencepahlography signal of children who performed an attention test was significantly higher after the interaction with a real dog compared to after the interaction with a robotic dog [26].

4.1 Comparison with other studies

We found that prefrontal brain activity increased with a rise in the intensity of contact with a dog or a plush animal. From watching the animal to feeling it passively to actively petting the animal, the interactional closeness increased and, with it the intensity of stimulation as well as the number of senses involved. This led to an increase in brain activation. We detected the same pattern in a pilot study with a similar study design and comparable forms of contact to an animal [27]. In line with this, another study revealed higher frontopolar activity when participants stroked a plush animal or a miniature horse compared to just seeing them [24]. Moreover, stroking a cat stimulated higher activation of the inferior frontal gyrus compared to just touching a cat [25].

We observed clear differences in brain activity in the presence of the dog compared to the plush animal. This contrasts with a study reporting that healthy participants had similar activation patterns of the inferior frontal gyrus when petting a cat or a plush animal [25]. That study also noted that female and male participants showed different activation patterns. A PET study observed deactivation in the left middle frontal gyrus, the right fusiform gyrus, the left putamen, and the thalamus in healthy participants during the presence of a familiar dog compared to a resting condition [23]. The authors suggested that this deactivation signaled a reduction in emotional stress induced by the presence of the familiar dog. These results cannot be directly compared with our results, because fNIRS cannot reach areas like the putamen or the thalamus. Nevertheless, the tasks in our design might have been more activating and our imaging technology less stressful.

Other studies identified lateralized activation patterns in frontal areas during petting a horse or a cat compared to a plush animal [24, 25]. For example, participants exhibited lateralization in the right frontopolar cortex while petting a real horse compared to no lateralization while petting a plush horse[24]. The authors attributed the lateralized activity to differences in function of the left and right frontal regions. We did not test for lateralization in the present study, but visual inspection of our data does not suggest lateralization. However, future studies should address the possibility of lateralization.

Summing up, the current literature indicates that frontal brain activation patterns in humans correlate with the level of interaction with animals. Our results show that this is also the case with a live dog compared to a plush animal and that the intensity of interaction is relevant. Looking at a dog correlates with the lowest frontal activity, while passive contact with more and active stroking correlates with the highest frontal activity.

4.2 Brain activity across sessions

In the second neutral phase, brain activation did not return to the level of the first neutral phase. We assume that activation persisted in both conditions and did not decline as quickly as expected. We therefore assume that the subjects were basically more activated in the second neutral phase than in the first.

We also found a pattern in O2Hb and tHb levels indicating that prefrontal brain activity increased with repeated contact to the dog while it did not increase with repeated contact in the plush-animal condition. There seems to be a difference, especially between the first and the second contact with the dog suggesting that familiarity might play a different role in interactions with live and plush animals. However, the other two outcomes (HHb and oxygen saturation) did not show an increase with repeated contact and do not support this hypothesis. This result of this explorative analysis therefore needs to be further investigated in future studies.

4.3 Hypothesis about underlying mechanisms

We have different hypotheses explaining our result of higher activation in the dog condition compared to the plush-animal condition. The prefrontal cortex is known to be involved not only in executive functions such as attention control, working memory, and problem-solving but also in social and emotional processes [50, 51]. It has reciprocal connections with brain regions that are involved in emotional processing such as the amygdala and higher-order sensory regions within the temporal cortex [51].

Social interactions with animals are highly emotionally relevant for a majority of people [7, 3134]. We thus hypothesize that interacting with the dog led to higher emotional involvement in the participants compared to interacting with the plush animal. This higher emotional involvement correlates with higher frontal activity. Previous studies using neuroimaging or behavioral outcomes support this hypothesis of higher emotional arousal by live animals [21, 39, 5255].

Potential higher emotional involvement might in parallel also lead to more attention for and a stronger focus on the dog compared to the plush animal. Several authors have shown that interactions with animals can promote attention and activate attention networks [20, 21, 26, 56, 57]. Attentional processes such as attentional set-shifting or attention monitoring are located in the frontal cortex [50, 58, 59].

Another consequence of higher emotional arousal or of touching a live dog can be increased physiological arousal [60]. This arousal can be related to a positive state, but interacting with a dog could also cause higher stress than interacting with a plush animal. Further parameters such as heart rate or skin conductance are needed to distinguish physiological arousal from other processes such as emotional involvement and attention. Further, the increase in activation might also have been caused by a greater cognitive load as a dog is a more complex stimulus than a plush animal [61, 62]. A last hypothesis might be that motor control played a role [63, 64] as stroking a live dog might demand different motor adaption in the participants.

In sum, there are several possible explanations for our results that would benefit from being investigated in the future. Based on the recent literature, we hypothesize that emotional involvement might be a central underlying mechanism of the neurological frontal brain correlates of human–animal interaction. We therefore suppose that the increase in brain activity in the dog condition over the three contacts might be explained based on a developing relationship between the participant and the dog. Familiarity and a relationship with the dog could have raised the salience of the dog, kept the participant’s attention on the dog’s behavior, and increased emotional arousal during the experiment. An fMRI study on pet attachment found a correlation between pet attachment and brain activity in areas involved in increasing attention and attentional load [21].

4.4 Implications for clinical practice

It is important that future research tries to replicate our findings because they could have important implications for clinical practice such as animal-assisted therapy. Our results indicate that interactions with a dog might activate more attentional processes and elicit stronger emotional arousal than comparable nonliving stimuli. Moreover, it seems that especially close and active physical contact to a familiar dog might promote social attention in humans. This is especially relevant for patients with deficits in motivation, attention, and socioemotional functioning. High involvement is a crucial factor for learning, as has been shown in several studies [65, 66]. For example, it has been shown that emotional relevance is central [67].

If patients with deficits in motivation, attention, and socioemotional functioning show higher emotional involvement in activities connected to a dog, then such activities could increase the chance of learning and of achieving therapeutic aims. These hypotheses should be investigated in future studies, as they suggest that integrating animals into therapeutic interventions might be a promising approach for improving emotional involvement and attention.

4.5 Limitations and strengths

Blinding was not possible due to the nature of the study. Moreover, randomizing the sequence of the conditions was not completely possible because of the irregular presence of the dogs. It should also be noted that there was an additional person present during the presence of the dog. The dog owners did not interact with the participants during the measurements, but participants could see the dog owners in two-thirds of the sessions. For most of the outcomes, visibility of the dog owner had no effect, but this factor should be controlled in future studies. Moreover, we did not assess attitudes toward animals. The sample size reflects the pilot character of the study. The results thus must be interpreted carefully.

While fNIRS technology has several advantages, measurements of regional cerebral oxygen saturation can be affected by skull thickness, gyration, hemoglobin concentration, or extracranial blood flow [68, 69]. We decided to use fNIRS because it allowed the study to take place in a natural environment and did not produce any sounds that could irritate the participants or the dogs. Since we repeatedly measured the outcomes for each condition and had a within-subject design where each participant served as their own control, these issues are limited. In addition, the probe design with a multidistance approach naturally reduces sensitivity to extracranial effects [70]. Drifts are also not likely because the fNIRS device corrects for that. Further, O2Hb concentration and oxygen saturation show the same pattern, which would not be the case if there was a drift. It could be argued that we should have detrended for the difference from the first to the second neutral phase. But the carry-over effect in the second neutral phase is not the same in the dog condition and the plush-animal condition. Detrending could thus have covered up effects that we assume reflect real changes.

The strengths of the study are that we investigated the effects of live dogs on neuronal activation instead of dogs presented via photos or videos and that we controlled for different levels of closeness and physical contact between the participant and the dog or the plush animal. We also carefully controlled the environmental factors in the room, the wording of the instructions, and the time of day of the sessions. Interactions between participants and the dog or the plush animal were standardized and kept as similar as possible. With regard to the plush animal used in the control condition, we controlled for tactile inputs such as its fur, warmth, and weight, and it was named and called by a name just as the dogs were called by a name in the study.

4.6 Future research

Future studies should take into account participants’ characteristics like gender, pet ownership, and attitude toward animals. It has been shown that participants who loved horses exhibited lateralization while petting a horse. In contrast, participants who only “kind of liked” horses did not exhibit lateralization [24]. A study on brain activity during cat petting indicated a gender difference [25], and in an fMRI study, pet owners showed greater activation than non-pet owners while looking at images of unfamiliar pets [21]. Future research should replicate our findings with larger sample sizes and different participants. Moreover, the effects of direct interaction with a live dog could be investigated with other neuroimaging techniques that can measure brain activity in different brain areas simultaneously. It is important to further understand the effect of familiarity and relationship as well as of the type of interaction with the dog. To do so, future studies could use different interactions such as speaking to the dog or include reciprocal interactions such as playing with the dog. Familiarity and relationship should be systematically controlled by involving unfamiliar dogs, unfamiliar dogs with repeated contact, and participants’ own pet dogs. It would be interesting to compare the effects of different animal species or of different features of dogs’ appearances and to use different control conditions. Obtaining subjective ratings of the different interactions such as perceived pleasantness, stress, and relationship with the dog or plush animal should be introduced in the future. Moreover, imposing a concurrent cognitive task might be useful to see if the presence of a real dog has facilitating effects on behavioral performance. Moreover, it is important to test our hypotheses regarding clinical relevance. Future studies should involve patients with deficits in motivation, attention, and socioemotional functioning and investigate if the same results can be found regarding brain activity and also look at therapeutic outcomes such as achieving rehabilitation goals.

With regard to standardization, we recommend implementing a manipulation test to check for motor functions, to randomize the phases, and to control for the number of people in the room, the position of the dog owner, and the handedness of the participants. If it is possible, we would recommend implementing a pretask and posttask baseline. The length of the neutral phase should be longer to avoid carry-over effects.

5 Conclusion

The present study demonstrates that prefrontal brain activity in healthy subjects increased with a rise in interactional closeness with a dog or a plush animal. Moreover, participants had higher brain activation in the presence of a dog compared to in the presence of a plush animal. This indicates that interactions with a dog might activate more attentional processes and elicit stronger emotional arousal than comparable nonliving stimuli. Our results also suggest that a relationship with the dog might be a crucial factor. The results are clinically relevant for patients with deficits in motivation, attention, and socioemotional functioning. Integrating animals into therapeutic interventions might therefore be a promising approach for improving emotional involvement and attention.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Daniel Ostojic for his advice on collecting and interpreting the fNIRS data. A great thanks goes to all the students who helped with data collection and preparation. We especially thank Felicitas Theis, Christina Zimmer, and Sabine Probst for their participation with their dogs Emma, Winnie, and Perla.

Data Availability

Data is are available from the Harvard Dataverse database at https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/U57AK0.

Funding Statement

This work was supported by the Swiss National Science Foundation under an Ambizione [grant number PZ00P1_174082/1] to K. H. and by the Stiftung pro REHAB Basel. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Waite TC, Hamilton L, O’Brien W. A meta-analysis of animal assisted interventions targeting pain, anxiety and distress in medical settings. Complement Ther Clin Pract 2018;33:49–55. doi: 10.1016/j.ctcp.2018.07.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Wells DL. The effects of animals on human health and well-being. J Soc Issues 2009;65:523–43. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Spattini L, Mattei G, Raisi F, Ferrari S, Pingani L, Galeazzi GM. Efficacy of animal assisted therapy on people with mental disorders: an update on the evidence. Minerva Psichiatr 2018;59:54–66. doi: [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Charry-Sánchez JD, Pradilla I, Talero-Gutiérrez C. Animal-assisted therapy in adults: A systematic review. Complement Ther Clin Pract 2018;32:169–80. doi: 10.1016/j.ctcp.2018.06.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Cook M, Busch S. health benefits of pet ownership for older adults. Grace Peterson Nurs Res Colloquium 2019;9. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Beetz AM. Theories and possible processes of action in animal assisted interventions. Appl Dev Sci 2017;21:139–49. doi: 10.1080/10888691.2016.1262263 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Borgi M, Cirulli F. Pet face: Mechanisms underlying human-animal relationships. Front Psychol 2016;7:1–11. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00298 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Kazdin AE. Strategies to improve the evidence base of animal-assisted interventions. Appl Dev Sci 2017;21:150–64. doi: 10.1080/10888691.2016.1191952 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.López-Cepero J. current status of animal-assisted interventions in scientific literature: A Critical comment on their internal validity. Animals 2020;10:985. doi: 10.3390/ani10060985 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Rodriguez KE, Herzog H, Gee NR. variability in human-animal interaction research. Front Vet Sci 2021;7:1207. doi: 10.3389/fvets.2020.619600 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Serpell J, McCune S, Gee N, Griffin JA. Current challenges to research on animal-assisted interventions. Appl Dev Sci 2017;21:223–33. doi: 10.1080/10888691.2016.1262775 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Beetz A, Uvnäs-Moberg K, Julius H, Kotrschal K. Psychosocial and psychophysiological effects of human-animal interactions: the possible role of oxytocin. Front Psychol 2012;3:234. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00234 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Ein N, Li L, Vickers K. The effect of pet therapy on the physiological and subjective stress response: A meta-analysis. Stress Heal 2018;34:477–89. doi: 10.1002/smi.2812 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Rathish D, Rajapakse RPVJ, Weerakoon KGAD. The role of cortisol in the association of canine-companionship with blood pressure, glucose, and lipids: a systematic review. High Blood Press Cardiovasc Prev 2021;28:447–55. doi: 10.1007/s40292-021-00469-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Odendaal JS., Meintjes R. Neurophysiological correlates of affiliative behaviour between humans and dogs. Vet J 2003;165:296–301. doi: 10.1016/s1090-0233(02)00237-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Nagasawa M, Mitsui S, En S, Ohtani N, Ohta M, Sakuma Y, et al. Oxytocin-gaze positive loop and the coevolution of human-dog bonds. Science (80-) 2015;348:333–6. doi: 10.1126/science.1261022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Petersson M, Uvnäs-Moberg K, Nilsson A, Gustafson L-L, Hydbring-Sandberg E, Handlin L. oxytocin and cortisol levels in dog owners and their dogs are associated with behavioral patterns: An exploratory study. Front Psychol 2017;8:1796. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01796 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Powell L, Edwards KM, Michael S, McGreevy P, Bauman A, Guastella AJ, et al. effects of human–dog interactions on salivary oxytocin concentrations and heart rate variability: A four-condition cross-over trial. Anthrozoos 2020;33:37–52. doi: 10.1080/08927936.2020.1694310 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Powell L, Guastella AJ, McGreevy P, Bauman A, Edwards KM, Stamatakis E. The physiological function of oxytocin in humans and its acute response to human-dog interactions: A review of the literature. J Vet Behav 2019;30:25–32. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2018.10.008 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Böttger S, Haberl R, Prosiegel M, Audebert H, Rumberg B, Forsting M, et al. Differences in cerebral activation during perception of optokinetic computer stimuli and video clips of living animals: An fMRI study. Brain Res 2010;1354:132–9. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.07.056 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Hayama S, Chang L, Gumus K, King GR, Ernst T. Neural correlates for perception of companion animal photographs. Neuropsychologia 2016;85:278–86. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.03.018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Balconi M, Vanutelli ME. Emotions and BIS/BAS components affect brain activity (ERPs and fNIRS) in observing intra-species and inter-species interactions. Brain Imaging Behav 2016;10:750–60. doi: 10.1007/s11682-015-9443-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Sugawara A, Masud MM, Yokoyama A, Mizutani W, Watanuki S, Yanai K, et al. effects of presence of a familiar pet dog on regional cerebral activity in healthy volunteers: a positron emission tomography study. Anthrozoos 2012;25:25–34. doi: 10.2752/175303712X13240472427311 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Matsuura A, Aiba N, Yamamoto H, Takahashi M, Kido H, Suzuki T, et al. stroking a real horse versus stroking a toy horse: effects on the frontopolar area of the human brain. Anthrozoos 2020;33:673–83. doi: 10.1080/08927936.2020.1799564 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Kobayashi A, Yamaguchi Y, Ohtani N, Ohta M. The effects of touching and stroking a cat on the inferior frontal gyrus in people. Anthrozoos 2017;30:473–86. doi: 10.1080/08927936.2017.1335115 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Hediger K, Turner DC. Can Dogs increase children’ s attention and concentration performance? A randomised performance trial. Human-Animal Interact Bull 2014;2:21–39. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Arnskötter W, Marcar VL, Wolf M, Hund-Georgiadis M, Hediger K. Animal presence modulates frontal brain activity of patients in a minimally conscious state: A pilot study. Neuropsychol Rehabil 2021:1–13. doi: 10.1080/09602011.2021.1886119 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Fine AH. Handbook on animal-assisted therapy. Elsevier; 2019. doi: 10.1016/C2017-0-03315-3 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Bert F, Gualano MR, Camussi E, Pieve G, Voglino G, Siliquini R. Animal assisted intervention: A systematic review of benefits and risks. Eur J Integr Med 2016;8:695–706. doi: 10.1016/j.eujim.2016.05.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Chang SJ, Lee J, An H, Hong W, Lee JY. animal‐assisted therapy as an intervention for older adults: A systematic review and meta‐analysis to guide evidence‐based practice. Worldviews Evidence-Based Nurs 2021;18:60–7. doi: 10.1111/wvn.12484 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Kurdek LA. Young Adults’ Attachment to pet dogs: Findings from open-ended methods. Anthrozoos 2009;22:359–69. doi: 10.2752/089279309X12538695316149 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Kurdek LA. Pet dogs as attachment figures for adult owners. J Fam Psychol 2009;23:439–46. doi: 10.1037/a0014979 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Aragunde-Kohl U, Gómez-Galán J, Lázaro-Pérez C, Martínez-López JÁ. Interaction and emotional connection with pets: A descriptive analysis from Puerto Rico. Animals 2020;10:2136. doi: 10.3390/ani10112136 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Hawkins R, Williams J. Childhood attachment to pets: Associations between pet attachment, attitudes to animals, compassion, and humane behaviour. Int J Environ Res Public Health 2017;14:490. doi: 10.3390/ijerph14050490 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Amodio DM, Frith CD. Meeting of minds: the medial frontal cortex and social cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci 2006;7:268–77. doi: 10.1038/nrn1884 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Frith CD, Frith U. The neural basis of mentalizing. Neuron 2006;50:531–4. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.05.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Jegatheesan B. Beetz A. Ormerod E. Johnson R. Fine A. Yamazaki, et al. IAHAIO White paper 2014 (updated for 2018). The IAHAIO definitions for animal-assisted intervention and guidelines for wellness of animals involved in AAI. 2018. [cited August 5, 2022] Available from: https://iahaio.org/wp/wp-content/uploads/2018/04/iahaio_wp_updated-2018-final.pdf [Google Scholar]
  • 38.World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki: ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. World Medical Association. JAMA 2013;310:2191. doi: 10.1001/jama.2013.281053 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Hediger K, Thommen S, Wagner C, Gaab J, Hund-Georgiadis M. Effects of animal-assisted therapy on social behaviour in patients with acquired brain injury: a randomised controlled trial. Sci Rep 2019;9:5831. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-42280-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Aoki J, Iwahashi K, Ishigooka J, Fukamauchi F, Numajiri M, Ohtani N, et al. Evaluation of cerebral activity in the prefrontal cortex in mood [affective. disorders during animal-assisted therapy (AAT) by near-infrared spectroscopy (NIRS): A pilot study. Int J Psychiatry Clin Pract 2012;16:205–13. doi: 10.3109/13651501.2011.644565 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Calcaterra V, Veggiotti P, Palestrini C, De Giorgis V, Raschetti R, Tumminelli M, et al. Post-operative benefits of animal-assisted therapy in pediatric surgery: A randomised study. PLoS One 2015;10:e0125813. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0125813 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Quaresima V, Ferrari M. Medical near infrared spectroscopy: A prestigious history and a bright future. NIR News 2016;27:10–3. doi: 10.1255/nirn.1575 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Marcar VL, Loenneker T. The BOLD response: a new look at an old riddle. Neuroreport 2004;15:1997–2000. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200409150-00001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Fu L, Xiang D, Xiao J, Yao L, Wang Y, Xiao L, et al. Reduced prefrontal activation during the tower of london and verbal fluency task in patients with bipolar depression: A multi-channel NIRS study. Front Psychiatry 2018;9:214. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00214 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Nieuwenhuis R, Te Grotenhuis M, Pelzer B. influence.ME: tools for detecting influential data in mixed effects models 2012;4:38–47. [Google Scholar]
  • 46.R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing (Version R 4.1.0) R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria. 2021. Available from: https://www.R-project.org/.
  • 47.Bates D, Mächler M, Bolker B, Walker S. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J Stat Softw 2015;67:R package version 0.999999–0. doi: 10.18637/jss.v067.i01 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Scholkmann F, Kleiser S, Metz AJ, Zimmermann R, Mata Pavia J, Wolf U, et al. A review on continuous wave functional near-infrared spectroscopy and imaging instrumentation and methodology. Neuroimage 2014;85:6–27. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Ferrari M, Quaresima V. A brief review on the history of human functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) development and fields of application. Neuroimage 2012;63:921–35. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.03.049 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Kuo M-F, Nitsche MA. Exploring prefrontal cortex functions in healthy humans by transcranial electrical stimulation. Neurosci Bull 2015;31:198–206. doi: 10.1007/s12264-014-1501-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Grossmann T. The role of medial prefrontal cortex in early social cognition. Front Hum Neurosci 2013;7:340. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00340 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Shinozaki J, Hanakawa T, Fukuyama H. Heterospecific and conspecific social cognition in the anterior cingulate cortex. Neuroreport 2007;18:993–7. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3281ac2161 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Mitchell JP, Banaji MR, Macrae CN. General and specific contributions of the medial prefrontal cortex to knowledge about mental states. Neuroimage 2005;28:757–62. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.03.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Ebener J, Oh H. A Review of Animal-Assisted Interventions in Long-Term Care Facilities. Act Adapt Aging 2017;41:107–28. doi: 10.1080/01924788.2017.1306380 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Wesenberg S, Mueller C, Nestmann F, Holthoff‐Detto V. Effects of an animal‐assisted intervention on social behaviour, emotions, and behavioural and psychological symptoms in nursing home residents with dementia. Psychogeriatrics 2019;19:219–27. doi: 10.1111/psyg.12385 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Gocheva V, Hund-Georgiadis M, Hediger K. Effects of animal-assisted therapy on concentration and attention span in patients with acquired brain injury: A randomized controlled trial. Neuropsychology 2018;32:54–64. doi: 10.1037/neu0000398 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Gee NR, Belcher JM, Grabski JL, DeJesus M, Riley W. The presence of a therapy dog results in improved object recognition performance in preschool children. Anthrozoos 2012;25:289–300. doi: 10.2752/175303712X13403555186172 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Carlén M. What constitutes the prefrontal cortex? Science (80-) 2017;358:478–82. doi: 10.1126/science.aan8868 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Basso Moro S, Cutini S, Ursini ML, Ferrari M, Quaresima V. Prefrontal cortex activation during story encoding/retrieval: a multi-channel functional near-infrared spectroscopy study. Front Hum Neurosci 2013;7:925. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00925 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Hediger K, Petignat M, Marti R, Hund-Georgiadis M. Animal-assisted therapy for patients in a minimally conscious state: A randomized two treatment multi-period crossover trial. PLoS One 2019;14:e0222846. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0222846 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Barkaszi I, Czigler I, Balázs L. Stimulus complexity effects on the event-related potentials to task-irrelevant stimuli. Biol Psychol 2013;94:82–9. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.05.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Müller V, Lutzenberger W, Preißl H, Pulvermüller F, Birbaumer N. Complexity of visual stimuli and non-linear EEG dynamics in humans. Cogn Brain Res 2003;16:104–10. doi: 10.1016/s0926-6410(02)00225-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Fuster JM. Prefrontal cortex in motor control. In: Handbook of physiology: The nervous system II. 1981; 1149–78. doi: 10.1002/cphy.cp010225 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Goto K, Hoshi Y, Sata M, Kawahara M, Takahashi M, Murohashi H. Role of the prefrontal cortex in the cognitive control of reaching movements: near-infrared spectroscopy study. J Biomed Opt 2011;16:127003. doi: 10.1117/1.3658757 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Tyng CM, Amin HU, Saad MNM, Malik AS. The influences of emotion on learning and memory. Front Psychol 2017;8:1454. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01454 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Schupp HT, Stockburger J, Codispoti M, Junghofer M, Weike AI, Hamm AO. Selective visual attention to emotion. J Neurosci 2007;27:1082–9. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3223-06.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Perrin F, Castro M, Tillmann B, Luauté J. Promoting the use of personally relevant stimuli for investigating patients with disorders of consciousness. Front Psychol 2015;6:1–9. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01102 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Tanaka Y, Suzuki M, Yoshitani K, Sakamoto A, Bito H. Anatomical and physiological variables influencing measurement of regional cerebral oxygen saturation by near infrared spectroscopy using the Sensmart Model X-100TM. J Clin Monit Comput 2021;35:1063–8. doi: 10.1007/s10877-020-00567-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Ostojic D, Jiang J, Isler H, Kleiser S, Karen T, Wolf M, et al. Impact of skull thickness on cerebral NIRS Oximetry in Neonates: An in silico study. Adv. Exp. Med. Biol., vol. 1232, Springer; 2020, p. 33–8. doi: 10.1007/978-3-030-34461-0_5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Franceschini MA, Fantini S, Paunescu LA, Maier JS, Gratton E. Influence of a superficial layer in the quantitative spectroscopic study of strongly scattering media. Appl Opt 1998;37:7447. doi: 10.1364/ao.37.007447 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Anna Manelis

15 Jul 2022

PONE-D-22-10049Effects of contact with a dog on prefrontal brain activity: A controlled trialPLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Marti,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process. The reviewers' feedback is below. Please submit your revised manuscript by Aug 29 2022 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Anna Manelis, Ph.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and 

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. We note that you have stated that you will provide repository information for your data at acceptance. Should your manuscript be accepted for publication, we will hold it until you provide the relevant accession numbers or DOIs necessary to access your data. If you wish to make changes to your Data Availability statement, please describe these changes in your cover letter and we will update your Data Availability statement to reflect the information you provide

Please review your reference list to ensure that it is complete and correct. If you have cited papers that have been retracted, please include the rationale for doing so in the manuscript text, or remove these references and replace them with relevant current references. Any changes to the reference list should be mentioned in the rebuttal letter that accompanies your revised manuscript. If you need to cite a retracted article, indicate the article’s retracted status in the References list and also include a citation and full reference for the retraction notice.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Partly

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: No

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: The abstract should be more structured it is written rather casually. There are several areas in the paper where they are also casual in their writing such as this in the methods "We opted fNIRS to measure..." It should not come across as a random selection of technology It should be a scientifically rational and intentional selection of measurement.

Line 52 pg. 1 uses the term neurochemicals it should be neurotransmitters

Methods In the methods section line 99-101 the design is described as following protocol for subjects are described as patients with severe disorder of consciousness this needs to be clarified it is very unclear what this means. They then go on to describe healthy participants. Please clarify why it is relevant to follow a method for people with sever disorder of consciousness.

The authors need t provide more specific information about the brain regions measured with fNIRS it simply states The study staff attached two fNIRS sensors to measure oxygen saturation on the participants’ foreheads. They also refer to This corresponded to locations F1, F3, F2 and F4 on the frontopolar area what Broca brain regions are these?

Please spell check the paper carefully in the first paragraph of results it says "planed sessions" it should be planned sessions.

An explanation of lateralization is needed so readers can understand why it is important to consider it is introduced in section 4.1 without background.

The hypotheses in the discussion session are clearer than the hypotheses stated in the introduction. Please revise them to be clearer currently it is wrotten as 'First, we expected the degree of closeness in contact with a

88 dog or plush animal to be correlated with the amount of stimulation and therefore also with

89 brain activity. Second, we hypothesized that.."

Perhaps this paper would provide an example of the level of detail needed Bergen-Cico, D., Grant, T. et al. (2021). Using fNIRS to Examine Neural Mechanisms of Change Associated with Mindfulness-Based Interventions for Stress and Trauma: Results of a Pilot Study for Women. Mindfulness, 12(9), 2295-2310.

Reviewer #2: The study aimed to know effects of animal contact on human mental and physical health by investigating changes in frontal brain activity. The manuscript is well written and most of the details were well described. The statistical methods were well strong and well explained. However there are some suggestions that should be considered in a revised manuscript.

(1) The authors should give more details of how to determine the sample size. In my understanding, if the sample size (n=21) is not enough, then the authors should discuss this limitation deeply in the discussion section.

(2) There are some factors that may influence the interaction effects, such as the social-emotional competence of participants, and the features of dogs’ face (eg. cute VS vicious), among others. The authors should discuss a little bit about this points.

(3) The authors should discuss deeply the difference of prefrontal brain activity between different contracts with dogs.

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: Yes: Dessa Bergen-Cico

Reviewer #2: No

**********

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2022 Oct 5;17(10):e0274833. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0274833.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


29 Aug 2022

Dear Prof. Dr. Manelis

We would like to resubmit our manuscript entitled “Effects of Contact with a Dog on Prefrontal Brain Activity: A Controlled Trial" after responding to all the comments of the reviewers and changing our manuscript accordingly. We uploaded two versions of our manuscript, a clean version without track changes and a version where all changes are visible.

As requested, this cover letter includes a point-by point reply to each criticism of the referees and a detailed indication of the changes made in our manuscript. All our responses are in navy blue.

We checked and corrected the format of the reference section as requested. Further, we saw that the figure references in the text were inconsistent. We therefore modified them.

We changed our Data Availability statement and provided a link to get access to our data file (see track changes on page 23, lines 535–536:

“Data are available from the Harvard Dataverse database at https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/U57AK0.”

We hope you find our manuscript suitable for publication and we look forward to hearing from you in due course.

Sincerely,

M.Sc. Rahel Marti

Assistant

Reviewer #1

The abstract should be more structured it is written rather casually.

Thank you very much for your comment. We checked the language and changed it accordingly. Further, we structured the abstract, added more detailed information about the study design and added a paragraph on the study’s findings. See track changes on page 2, lines 30-51:

“Background: There is a broad range of known effects of animal contact on human mental and physical health. Neurological correlates of human interaction with animals have been sparsely investigated. We investigated changes in frontal brain activity in the presence of and during contact with a dog.

Methods: Twenty-one healthy individuals each participated in six sessions. In three sessions, participants had contact with a dog, and in three control sessions they interacted with a plush animal. Each session had five two-minute phases with increasing intensity of contact to the dog or plush animal from the first to the fourth phase. We measured oxygenated, deoxygenated, and total hemoglobin and oxygen saturation of the blood in the frontal lobe/frontopolar area with functional near-infrared spectroscopy (SenSmart Model X-100) to assess brain activity.

Findings: In both conditions, the concentration of oxygenated hemoglobin increased significantly from the first to the fourth phase by 2.78 �mol/l (CI = 2.03–3.53, p < .001). Oxygenated hemoglobin concentration was 0.80 �mol/l higher in the dog condition compared to in the control condition (CI = 0.27–1.33, p = .004). Deoxygenated-hemoglobin concentration, total hemoglobin concentration, and oxygen saturation showed similar patterns.

Conclusion: Prefrontal brain activation in healthy subjects increased with the rise in interaction closeness with a dog or a plush animal. Moreover, interaction with a dog stimulated more brain activity compared to the control condition, suggesting that interactions with a dog can activate stronger attentional processes and elicit more emotional arousal than interacting with a nonliving stimulus.”

There are several areas in the paper where they are also casual in their writing such as this in the methods "We opted fNIRS to measure..." It should not come across as a random selection of technology It should be a scientifically rational and intentional selection of measurement.

Thank you for this helpful comment. We changed the term and let the manuscript be checked by a professional language proofreader who checked the text with a distinct focus on avoiding casual language:

“We chose fNIRS to measure the response in the prefrontal cortex as it is particularly suited for investigating the neuronal correlates of such a complex social situation of human–animal interaction.” (See track changes on page 7, lines 176–178)

To see further adjustments made by the professional language proofreader, you can look at the track changes in the text.

Line 52 pg. 1 uses the term neurochemicals it should be neurotransmitters

Thank you very much for your comment and your suggestion. �-endorphin and oxytocin are, as you wrote, neuropeptides and neurotransmitters whereas prolactin is a hormone not functioning as a neurotransmitter. Therefore, we chose the word neurochemicals as an umbrella term to summarize all three.

Methods In the methods section line 99-101 the design is described as following protocol for subjects are described as patients with severe disorder of consciousness this needs to be clarified it is very unclear what this means. They then go on to describe healthy participants. Please clarify why it is relevant to follow a method for people with sever disorder of consciousness.

We thank you for pointing out this important issue. The way we wrote it was indeed unclear to the reader. We changed the locating of this sentence to give it more context and added more information regarding a planned second study with patients with severe disorders of consciousness:

“We planned to compare the results of this study with a study population of patients with severe disorders of consciousness in a future trial, so the study design complied with the requirements for measuring a group of patients with severe disorders of consciousness.” (See track changes on pages 5, lines 114–117)

The authors need t provide more specific information about the brain regions measured with fNIRS it simply states The study staff attached two fNIRS sensors to measure oxygen saturation on the participants’ foreheads. They also refer to This corresponded to locations F1, F3, F2 and F4 on the frontopolar area what Broca brain regions are these?

Thank you for your comment. The Broca area is a distinct area corresponding to Brodmann areas 44 and 45 and we did not measure on Broca’s area. To answer this concern, we added the corresponding Brodmann areas for the prefrontal cortex which are number 9, 10, and 46, see page 8, lines 193–195:

“This corresponded to locations F1, F3, F2, and F4 on the frontopolar area according to the international 10–20 system and to the Brodmann areas 9, 10, and 46.”

Please spell check the paper carefully in the first paragraph of results it says "planed sessions" it should be planned sessions.

Thank you for finding this spelling error and pointing this out. We changed this and, as stated above, checked the manuscript by a professional proofreader:

“We conducted 119 of the 126 planned sessions (Fig 2).” (See track changes on page 10, line 231)

An explanation of lateralization is needed so readers can understand why it is important to consider it is introduced in section 4.1 without background.

Thank you very much for your comment and your suggestion. Indeed, it was not introduced nicely so it was hard to understand for the readers. Based on your suggestion we changed the order of the text so that it is now embedded in the context of findings of other studies to make it clear why lateralization is important. See track changes on page 17, lines 372–378:

“Other studies identified lateralized activation patterns in frontal areas during petting a horse or a cat compared to a plush animal [24, 25]. For example, participants exhibited lateralization in the right frontopolar cortex while petting a real horse compared to no lateralization while petting a plush horse [24]. The authors attributed the lateralized activity to differences in function of the left and right frontal regions. We did not test for lateralization in the present study, but visual inspection of our data does not suggest lateralization. However, future studies should address the possibility of lateralization.”

The hypotheses in the discussion session are clearer than the hypotheses stated in the introduction. Please revise them to be clearer currently it is wrotten as 'First, we expected the degree of closeness in contact with a

88 dog or plush animal to be correlated with the amount of stimulation and therefore also with

89 brain activity. Second, we hypothesized that.."

We thank you for pointing this out. We adapted the wording of the first hypothesis:

“We expected, first, that the increase of closeness in contact with a dog or plush animal would correlate with an increased amount of stimulation and therefore also with increased brain activity.” (See track changes on page 4, lines 95–97)

Perhaps this paper would provide an example of the level of detail needed Bergen-Cico, D., Grant, T. et al. (2021). Using fNIRS to Examine Neural Mechanisms of Change Associated with Mindfulness-Based Interventions for Stress and Trauma: Results of a Pilot Study for Women. Mindfulness, 12(9), 2295-2310.

Thank you for your comment and direct our attention to this study. We added a new figure to make extend the description of the procedures and make it more accessible for the readers.

See Figure 1, on page 6, line 155.

Reviewer #2

(1) The authors should give more details of how to determine the sample size. In my understanding, if the sample size (n=21) is not enough, then the authors should discuss this limitation deeply in the discussion section.

Thank you very much for your comment and your suggestion. Since the follow-up project of this pilot study aimed at conducting the same procedure with patients with severe disorders of consciousness, the sample size was determined based on feasibility regarding the number of patients with severe disorders of consciousness in this hospital as well as the results of a previous study. In this study, we analyzed behavioral outcomes from 19 patients with acquired brain injury and found a significant effect of animal-assisted therapy on positive emotions and social behavior and a piloting trial with the same design where we analyzed four participants [1].

The power of the current study was adequate as it was possible to find differences between conditions. However, since this was a pilot study, we mentioned this fact and the sample size in the limitations section:

“The sample size reflects the pilot character of the study. The results thus must be interpreted carefully.“ (See track changes on page20, line 457-459)

(2) There are some factors that may influence the interaction effects, such as the social-emotional competence of participants, and the features of dogs’ face (eg. cute VS vicious), among others. The authors should discuss a little bit about this points.

Thank you very much for your comment and your suggestion. Indeed, socio-emotional competencies of participants and features of the dogs play a role in the interaction between humans and dogs. However, in our study design, these factors cannot influence the results because we compared the participants with themselves (within-subject design). But we agree with you that it would be important for future research to investigate more closely what characteristics of the human participants and of the dogs predict different reactions within such a human-animal interaction.

In lines 503–505 on page 22 we pointed out, that future trials should investigate participants with varying amount of socio-emotional competences. Based on your suggestion, we also added on page 22 in lines 496–498:

“It would be interesting to compare the effects of different animal species or of different features of dogs’ appearances and to use different control conditions.”

(3) The authors should discuss deeply the difference of prefrontal brain activity between different contracts with dogs.

Thank you for your comment. We extended the discussion about the effects of the increasing intensity of contact and added the following text: (page 16, line 352–360):

“We found that prefrontal brain activity increased with a rise in the intensity of contact with a dog or a plush animal. From watching the animal to feeling it passively to actively petting the animal, the interactional closeness increased and, with it the intensity of stimulation as well as the number of senses involved. This led to an increase in brain activation. We detected the same pattern in a pilot study with a similar study design and comparable forms of contact to an animal [27]. In line with this, another study revealed higher frontopolar activity when participants stroked a plush animal or a miniature horse compared to just seeing them [24]. Moreover, stroking a cat stimulated higher activation of the inferior frontal gyrus compared to just touching a cat [25].”

1. Arnskötter W, Marcar VL, Wolf M, Hund-Georgiadis M, Hediger K. Animal presence modulates frontal brain activity of patients in a minimally conscious state: A pilot study. Neuropsychol Rehabil 2021:1–13. doi:10.1080/09602011.2021.1886119.

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers_Marti et al.pdf

Decision Letter 1

Anna Manelis

5 Sep 2022

Effects of contact with a dog on prefrontal brain activity: A controlled trial

PONE-D-22-10049R1

Dear Dr. Marti,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Anna Manelis, Ph.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Acceptance letter

Anna Manelis

12 Sep 2022

PONE-D-22-10049R1

Effects of contact with a dog on prefrontal brain activity: A controlled trial

Dear Dr. Marti:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Anna Manelis

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers_Marti et al.pdf

    Data Availability Statement

    Data is are available from the Harvard Dataverse database at https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/U57AK0.


    Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES