Abstract
HIV-related stigma exacerbates Latino immigrants’ risk of HIV infection and delayed care. Following the implementation of the social marketing campaign Sólo Se Vive Una Vez (You Only Live Once) to increase HIV testing that addressed stigmatizing beliefs, we conducted a survey among Latinos in Baltimore, Maryland (N = 357). The aims of this paper are to 1) characterize the sociodemographic characteristics, HIV-related stigma beliefs, and testing behaviors of the survey respondents by campaign exposure, and 2) model the effects of Vive exposure on stigma beliefs and testing behaviors. Comparing post-campaign survey respondents exposed and unexposed to the campaign to survey findings previously obtained and reported before the campaign implementation, respondents to the post-Vive survey continued to hold high levels of stigma beliefs, and compared to the pre-Vive survey sample, were more likely to hold four or more stigmatizing beliefs (from the six survey items). Among the post-Vive survey respondents, those for whom religion was important or very important had an increased odds of 1.6 of holding four or more stigmatizing beliefs. Survey respondents who were exposed to the campaign, however, had an increased odds of 2.25 of reporting ever having been tested for HIV. Our findings demonstrate the importance of the changing social context in addressing stigma within emerging immigrant communities and highlight the critical role of religious leaders in efforts to address HIV-related stigma.
Introduction
HIV/AIDS disproportionately affects Latinos in the United States (U.S.) [1, 2], and despite substantial progress made in the U.S. to reduce HIV incidence, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) estimates suggest new infections continue to rise among Latinos. Between 2010 and 2016, HIV incidence in the U.S. increased by 14% among Latinos, while it decreased 6% overall [3]. Moreover, Latinos are more likely to report never having been offered an HIV test [4], are less likely to be aware of their HIV status [3], and present to healthcare services with more advanced disease than their non-Hispanic counterparts [5–7].
Latino immigrants account for a third of all HIV diagnoses among Latinos [8] and are at greater risk than their U.S.-born peers for delayed diagnosis and presentation to care [9, 10]. Latino immigrants face numerous challenges in the U.S. that exacerbate their vulnerability to HIV infection and limit their access to healthcare services [11–14]. Importantly, HIV-related stigma has been associated with reluctance to be HIV tested among Latinos and produces a barrier to HIV education and linkage to care [6, 12, 15, 16]. Stigma is a powerful discrediting social label that dramatically impacts how individuals view themselves and are viewed by others. Cultural meanings given to stigmatized attributes, such as HIV infection, are reproduced through social interactions and are therefore linked to the actions of groups of people within specific contexts [17, 18]. Among Latinos in Baltimore surveyed in 2014–2015, for example, half of respondents agreed that people living with or thought to be living with HIV lose respect of standing, and three-quarters agreed that people get HIV because they engage in irresponsible behavior [13]. This stigmatizing association of HIV with “irresponsible behavior” is often tied to religious taboos on homosexuality, multiple sex partners, and drug use [19, 20].
HIV-related stigma reduction is a priority for reducing HIV disparities within the national HIV/AIDS prevention strategy as well as by the national Hispanic/Latinx Delegation on HIV/AIDS [21, 22]. Specifically, Guilamo-Ramos and colleagues [22] argue that stigma-reduction campaigns are needed to decrease HIV-related stigma in Latino communities at large, and not only through targeting at-risk or HIV-positive individuals.
Social marketing has been shown to be an effective strategy for promoting HIV screening and reducing HIV stigma [23, 24]. To improve HIV testing and linkage to care among Latinos in Baltimore, a new immigrant receiving community with a rapidly growing immigrant population, we developed the multilevel social marketing campaign Sólo Se Vive Una Vez (You Only Live Once, henceforth referred to as Vive) through partnerships with the Baltimore City Health Department (BCHD) Latino outreach team, community members and leaders, Altavista Studios, Maryland Institute College of Art (MICA), and academic investigators from Johns Hopkins University [25–28]. The individual-level component of the campaign was a website (www.solovive.org) with culturally sensitive video modules which individuals could select based on their perceived barrier(s) to HIV testing and/or HIV-related stigmatizing beliefs. The community-level components of the campaign included advertisements on multiple mediums (e.g., social media, dating applications used by men who have sex with men, local Spanish-language radio stations, billboards, buses, newspapers, and posters) as well as community outreach events.
Within the individual- and community-level components, Vive featured messaging that addressed the most common stigmatizing beliefs and barriers to testing, as guided by prior research. These barriers were (translated from Spanish): I am in a stable relationship, I don’t have legal documentation to be in the U.S., I feel fine/healthy, I am scared of the result, I am not at risk for HIV, I don’t have health insurance, I am afraid of needles, I don’t have time, I am ashamed/embarrassed, and I don’t have sex with just anybody [12, 13, 26]. Videos and corresponding advertisements (e.g., social media ads, posters) also included the motivational messages “Saber es poder” (Knowledge is power) and “El VIH solo es peligroso cuando lo ignoras” (HIV only is dangerous when you ignore it) as well as information about Spanish-language services at the health department and contact information for Latino outreach workers.
The Sólo Se Vive Una Vez campaign was implemented in Baltimore between June 22, 2018 and January 12, 2019. The aims of this paper are to 1) characterize the sociodemographic characteristics, HIV-related stigma beliefs, and testing behaviors of the survey respondents by campaign exposure, and 2) model the effects of Vive exposure on stigma beliefs and testing behaviors. Findings on the campaign’s reach and broader impact on HIV screening among Latinos in Baltimore are published elsewhere [27, 28].
Methods
Study design
We conducted a cross sectional study between September and December 2019 to characterize HIV-related stigma among a convenience sample of Spanish-speaking Latino/Latina adults in Baltimore, Maryland and to assess exposure to the Vive campaign. The same survey, utilizing the same methodology, was also conducted within this community prior to the Vive campaign launch (September 2014- August 2015) [13].
The study design is described in detail elsewhere [13], but briefly, trained research assistants recruited respondents at street- and community-based venues frequented by foreign-born Latinos, as informed by our prior work mapping and assessing locations for sampling the Latino immigrant population in Baltimore [29]. Participants were eligible for inclusion if they were over 18 years of age, self-identified as Latino/Latina, and were able to complete the survey in Spanish. Since the goal was to explore HIV-related stigma in the local community of Latino immigrants, U.S. born Latinos interacting with Latino immigrants in these spaces were also able to participate if they met the eligibility requirements. Survey data was collected in Spanish on a tablet using Qualtrics formatted with audio. The survey included a statement of consent and took approximately 5–10 minutes to complete. Participants received $5 compensation upon completion of the survey. The Johns Hopkins School of Medicine Institutional Review Board (IRB) approved the study.
Measures
The survey included questions regarding socio-demographic characteristics and HIV test history, as well as six items to assess HIV-related stigma. Sociodemographic characteristics collected included age, gender, country of origin, education, religious affiliation, importance of religion, time in the U.S., as well as differences in HIV testing history (ever being tested for HIV, and time since last test if ever tested), and knowing someone infected with or thought to be infected with HIV. To assess HIV-related stigma, we utilized validated items recommended by the UNAIDS-funded HIV Stigma Network, selected to incorporate multiple stigma domains: fear of transmission and disease, anticipated stigma, association with shame, blame and judgment, and support for discriminatory actions or policies against people living with HIV/AIDS (PLHA) [30]. The latter three domains address personal attitudes as well as perceived community responses towards PLHA. The items and domains were selected to allow differentiation between specific drivers and causes of HIV stigma, such as fear of infection due to lack of knowledge about transmission and moralistic attitudes and beliefs surrounding PLHA. Response choices for each stigma item included “Agree” and “Disagree,” or “Yes,” “No,” and “Depends,” depending on the phrasing of the item (as a statement or question). All items also had the response choices “I don’t know/No opinion” and “I prefer not to respond.” The Vive campaign was informed by our formative research findings, including the initial pre-Vive survey findings. As a result, the Vive campaign did not address each stigma item in this survey directly; however, these stigma measures were repeated in the post-Vive survey for comparison to the pre-Vive survey findings. These repeated measures may provide insight into the effectiveness of new ideas and information communicated through the Vive campaign, as education is understood to be an important component of stigma reduction [31].
To assess exposure to the Vive campaign, participants were asked: “Have you previously seen or heard of the ¡Solo Se Vive Una Vez! campaign?” Participants who answered “Yes” were then asked if they considered testing, or did get tested, for HIV because of seeing the campaign. They were also asked to report all the ways they saw or heard about the campaign from a list of possible sources (i.e., website, Facebook, billboard, poster, radio).
Analysis
Guided by the literature [16, 19, 20, 32] and our previous research [13], sociodemographic variables were dichotomized as follows. Education was dichotomized as completing secondary school or higher versus not having completed secondary school. Importance of religion was dichotomized between important or very important versus somewhat important or not important. Time in the U.S. was dichotomized as 5 or more years versus less than 5 years. Time since last test was dichotomized as within the past year versus over one year ago (never, over one year ago, or could not remember when).
Self-reported exposure to the campaign was used to categorize three groups of survey respondents: the pre-Vive group consisting of those surveyed prior to development and implementation of the campaign [13]; the post-Vive-unexposed group surveyed after the campaign was implemented, but who did not self-report seeing or hearing of the campaign; and the post-Vive-exposed group surveyed after the campaign was implemented and who self-reported exposure.
Aim 1: Characterize the sociodemographic characteristics, HIV-related stigma beliefs, and testing behaviors of the survey respondents by campaign exposure
We assessed possible differences in sociodemographic characteristics thought to influence HIV-related stigma beliefs and testing behaviors between the three cohort/exposure groups–pre-Vive (n = 312), unexposed post-Vive (n = 218) and exposed post-Vive (n = 139)–using a one-way ANOVA to test for significant differences in continuous age distributions, and Pearson’s chi-squared tests for all categorical variables.
We also assessed possible differences in the percent of respondents in agreement with HIV-related stigma beliefs and self-reported testing behaviors between the three cohort/exposure groups. Pearson’s chi-squared tests were used to test for statistical significance of these differences in agreement with each stigma item between the samples. Chronbach’s alpha was calculated on the six stigma items to test internal consistency. Chronbach’s alpha was 0.50, indicating low internal consistency; thus, each stigma item was analyzed individually.
Aim 2: Model effects of Vive exposure on HIV-related stigma beliefs and HIV testing behaviors
To test the hypothesis that Vive campaign exposure would reduce agreement with HIV-related stigma and increase HIV testing behaviors, we constructed logistic regression models with the pre-Vive category as the reference group and compared the effects non-exposure and exposure on stigma and testing outcomes. Adjusted logistic regression models controlled for factors observed to differ significantly between the groups (education level, importance of religion, and time spent in the U.S.) as well as for factors typically associated with stigma from the literature (gender, age, and knowing PLHA). None of the adjusted models violated the Hosmer-Lemeshow goodness-of-fit test. All models and analyses were conducted using STATA Software Version 16.0.
Results
Aim 1: Characterize the sociodemographic characteristics, HIV-related stigma beliefs, and testing behaviors of the survey respondents by campaign exposure
The post-Vive survey was completed by 357 members of the Latino community; 162 (45.4%) identified as male and 195 (54.6%) as female, with a mean age of 35.7 (SD = 11.4) and 35.2 (SD = 10.5) years, respectively. The majority reported their country of origin as Mexico (30.5%), Honduras (23.0%), or El Salvador (19.6%); few participants were born in the U.S. mainland (6.4%). The majority surveyed had not completed a secondary school education (52.1%), reported that their religion was important or very important to them (67.2%), and had lived in the U.S. for over 5 years (61.9%). No significant socio-demographic differences were found between the male and female participants.
Over one-third (n = 139, 38.9%) of post-Vive survey respondents reported exposure to the Vive campaign; 34.6% of males and 42.6% of females reported seeing or hearing about the campaign. Community events were identified as the most common way respondents learned about Vive (39.6%), followed by Facebook (35.3%). Considering the samples of pre-Vive, post-Vive-unexposed, and post-Vive-exposed, statistically significant differences were observed for education level, importance of religion, and time lived in the U.S. (Table 1).
Table 1. Socio-demographic characteristics of the pre-Vivea (n = 312), post-Vive-unexposed (n = 218), and post-Vive-exposed (n = 139) respondents survey respondents.
Sociodemographic characteristic | Pre-Vive n (%) | Post-Vive- unexposed n (%) | Post-Vive- exposed n (%) | p-valueb |
---|---|---|---|---|
Age (mean, SD) | 35.4 (10.6) | 34.5 (10.9) | 36.9 (10.8) | 0.107 |
Gender | 0.297 | |||
Female | 173 (55.5) | 112 (51.4) | 83 (59.7) | |
Male | 139 (44.5) | 106 (48.6) | 56 (40.3) | |
Country of Origin | 0.131 | |||
Mexico | 101 (32.4) | 55 (25.2) | 54 (38.9) | |
El Salvador | 49 (15.7) | 44 (20.2) | 26 (18.7) | |
Guatemala | 26 (8.3) | 17 (7.8) | 8 (5.8) | |
Honduras | 70 (22.4) | 56 (25.7) | 26 (18.7) | |
United States (mainland) | 13 (4.2) | 12 (5.5) | 11 (7.9) | |
Other | 53 (17.0) | 34 (15.6) | 14 (10.1) | |
Education | 0.021 | |||
Did not complete secondary school | 191 (61.2) | 107 (49.1) | 79 (56.8) | |
Completed secondary school | 121 (38.8) | 111 (50.9) | 60 (43.2) | |
Religion | 0.057 | |||
Catholic | 193 (61.9) | 106 (48.6) | 81 (58.3) | |
Evangelic/Protestant | 60 (19.2) | 59 (27.1) | 29 (20.9) | |
Other | 32 (10.3) | 21 (9.6) | 15 (10.8) | |
No religion | 27 (8.7) | 32 (14.7) | 14 (10.1) | |
Importance of religion | 0.010 | |||
Important or very important | 74 (23.7) | 78 (35.8) | 39 (28.1) | |
Somewhat or not important | 238 (76.3) | 140 (64.2) | 100 (71.9) | |
Time in the U.S. | 0.027 | |||
Less than 5 years | 96 (30.8) | 91 (41.7) | 45 (32.4) | |
5 years of more | 216 (69.2) | 127 (58.3) | 94 (67.6) | |
Know someone living with HIV | 0.698 | |||
Yes | 24 (7.7) | 13 (6.0) | 11 (7.9) | |
No / unsure | 288 (92.3) | 205 (94.0) | 128 (92.1) |
a Solo Se Vive Una Vez (Vive) was implemented between June 22, 2018 and January 12, 2019. The pre-Vive survey was conducted September 2014-August 2015 to inform the development of the Vive campaign. The post-Vive survey was conducted between September and December 2019.
b One-way ANOVA (continuous age) and Pearson’s chi-squared (categorical) tests for observed sociodemographic differences between Vive cohort/exposure groups.
Agreement with each HIV-related stigma item and self-reported HIV testing behaviors are presented for the pre-Vive, post-Vive-unexposed, and post-Vive-exposed cohorts in Table 2. Among respondents in all groups, over half agreed with the statements “I fear that I could contract HIV if I came into contact with the saliva of a person living with HIV,” “People living or thought to be living with HIV lose respect or standing,” and “People get HIV because they engage in irresponsible behavior.” A statistically significant difference among the three groups was observed only for the item “I would be ashamed if someone in my family had HIV.” Among post-Vive-unexposed respondents, 42.7% agreed to this statement compared to 41.0% of post-Vive-exposed and 25.9% of pre-Vive respondents (p < 0.001). Post-Vive respondents, however, were significantly more likely to endorse four or more of the six stigma items than the pre-Vive respondents (p = 0.036).
Table 2. Agreementa with HIV-related stigma beliefs and self-reported testing behaviors among pre-Vive (n = 312), post-Vive-unexposed (n = 218), and post-Vive-exposed (n = 139) survey respondents.
Pre-Vive n (%) | Post-Vive- unexposed n (%) | Post-Vive- exposed n (%) | p-valueb | |
---|---|---|---|---|
HIV-related Stigma Item | ||||
I feel hesitant to take an HIV test due to fear of people’s reaction if the test result is positive for HIV | 63 (21.4) | 53 (23.9) | 35 (25.2) | 0.647 |
I fear that I could contract HIV if I came into contact with the saliva of a person living with HIV | 177 (59.0) | 121 (55.5) | 87 (62.6) | 0.408 |
I think children living with HIV should not be able to attend school with children who are HIV negative | 115 (38.1) | 85 (39.0) | 49 (35.3) | 0.769 |
I would be ashamed if someone in my family had HIV | 75 (25.9) | 93 (42.7) | 57 (41.0) | <0.001 |
People living or thought to be living with HIV lose respect or standing | 151 (50.7) | 123 (56.4) | 72 (51.8) | 0.418 |
People get HIV because they engage in irresponsible behavior | 220 (74.1) | 172 (78.9) | 116 (83.5) | 0.079 |
Agreement with four or more stigma beliefs | 69 (27.0) | 78 (35.8) | 53 (38.1) | 0.036 |
Testing Behavior | ||||
Ever tested for HIV | 213 (68.3) | 138 (63.3) | 114 (82.0) | <0.001 |
Tested for HIV in the past 1 yearc | 109 (51.2) | 52 (37.7) | 65 (57.0) | 0.006 |
a Agreement with the stigma belief items were based on “Yes/Agree,” “Depends,” and “I don’t know” responses, except for the item related to children with HIV attending school with children who are HIV negative. For this item, agreement was based on the response of “No” and “I don’t know.”
b Pearson’s chi-squared tests for observed differences in stigma and testing between Vive cohort/exposure groups
c Includes only those who have ever tested for HIV
Survey respondents differed significant with respect to ever testing for HIV and testing for HIV in the past year based on exposure to the Vive campaign. Among post-Vive-exposed respondents, 82.0% self-reported ever testing for HIV, compared to 68.3% of pre-Vive and 63.3% of post-Vive-unexposed respondents (p < 0.001). Among those ever testing for HIV, 57.0% of post-Vive-exposed respondents reported testing in the past year, compared to 51.2% of pre-Vive and 37.7% of post-Vive-unexposed respondents (p = 0.006).
Aim 2: Model effects of Vive exposure on HIV-related stigma beliefs and HIV testing behaviors
The adjusted odds ratio (AOR) modelling effects of exposure to the Vive campaign on agreement with stigma belief items and HIV testing behaviors are provided in Tables 3 and 4, respectively. After controlling for observed differences in education, religiosity, and time in the U.S., as well as the effects of gender, age, and knowing someone living with HIV, both post-Vive groups had statistically significantly greater likelihood of agreement with the statement “I would be ashamed if someone in my family had HIV” as compared to the pre-Vive cohort (p < .001). The increased likelihood was less pronounced, however, among the post-Vive-exposed group compared to the post-Vive-unexposed (AORs 2.09 and 2.35, respectively). Exposure to the Vive campaign was associated with increased likelihood of agreement that people who get HIV through engaging in irresponsible behaviors, compared to the pre-Vive cohort (AOR = 1.85, 95% CI (1.08, 3.15), p = 0.025). The pro-blame trend was mirrored, but not statistically significant, in the unexposed respondents (AOR = 1.32, 95% CI (0.85, 2.04), p = 0.211). Finally, both post-Vive groups had statistically significantly greater likelihood of agreement with four or more stigma items.
Table 3. Effects of cohort and Vive-campaign exposure on agreement with stigma beliefs.
AORa | (95% CI) | p-value | |
---|---|---|---|
I feel hesitant to take an HIV test due to fear of people’s reaction if the test result is positive for HIV | |||
Cohort / Campaign Exposure | |||
Post-unexposed | 1.16 | (0.75, 1.78) | 0.510 |
Post-exposed | 1.29 | (0.80, 2.09) | 0.297 |
Completed secondary school | 0.73 | (0.49, 1.07) | 0.102 |
Religion is important or very important | 1.44 | (0.93, 2.21) | 0.100 |
Spent 5 years or longer in the U.S. | 0.73 | (0.48, 1.11) | 0.141 |
Male gender | 1.50 | (1.04, 2.18) | 0.032 |
Age (continuous) | 0.99 | (0.97, 1.01) | 0.371 |
Knows someone living or thought to be living with HIV | 0.48 | (0.20, 1.17) | 0.106 |
I fear that I could contract HIV if I came into contact with the saliva of a person living with HIV | |||
Cohort / Campaign Exposure | |||
Post-unexposed | 0.95 | (0.65, 1.37) | 0.772 |
Post-exposed | 1.27 | (0.83, 1.95) | 0.273 |
Completed secondary school | 0.55 | (0.40, 0.77) | <0.001 |
Religion is important or very important | 1.65 | (1.16, 2.36) | 0.006 |
Spent 5 years or longer in the U.S. | 1.08 | (0.75, 1.56) | 0.669 |
Male gender | 2.02 | (1.46, 2.81) | <0.001 |
Age (continuous) | 1.00 | (0.98, 1.01) | 0.639 |
Knows someone living or thought to be living with HIV | 0.70 | (0.37, 1.31) | 0.261 |
I think children living with HIV should not be able to attend school with children who are HIV negative | |||
Cohort / Campaign Exposure | |||
Post-unexposed | 1.20 | (0.83, 1.74) | 0.335 |
Post-exposed | 0.93 | (0.61, 1.43) | 0.737 |
Completed secondary school | 0.56 | (0.40, 0.78) | <0.001 |
Religion is important or very important | 1.95 | (1.34, 2.85) | <0.001 |
Spent 5 years or longer in the U.S. | 1.03 | (0.72, 1.49) | 0.858 |
Male gender | 1.06 | (0.77, 1.46) | 0.728 |
Age (continuous) | 1.00 | (0.98, 1.02) | 0.899 |
Knows someone living or thought to be living with HIV | 0.78 | (0.40, 1.49) | 0.443 |
I would be ashamed if someone in my family had HIV | |||
Cohort / Campaign Exposure | |||
Post-unexposed | 2.33 | (1.58, 3.45) | <0.001 |
Post-exposed | 2.14 | (1.38, 3.32) | <0.001 |
Completed secondary school | 0.57 | (0.40, 0.81) | 0.002 |
Religion is important or very important | 1.39 | (0.95, 2.03) | 0.091 |
Spent 5 years or longer in the U.S. | 0.85 | (0.58, 1.24) | 0.389 |
Male gender | 1.13 | (0.81, 1.58) | 0.465 |
Age (continuous) | 0.99 | (0.98, 1.01) | 0.387 |
Knows someone living or thought to be living with HIV | 0.51 | (0.24, 1.06) | 0.072 |
People living or thought to be living with HIV lose respect or standing | |||
Cohort / Campaign Exposure | |||
Post-unexposed | 1.28 | (0.90, 1.84) | 0.172 |
Post-exposed | 1.07 | (0.71, 1.61) | 0.747 |
Completed secondary school | 0.74 | (0.54, 1.01) | 0.058 |
Religion is important or very important | 1.00 | (0.71, 1.42) | 0.985 |
Spent 5 years or longer in the U.S. | 0.97 | (0.68, 1.38) | 0.860 |
Male gender | 1.33 | (0.97, 1.82) | 0.072 |
Age (continuous) | 1.00 | (0.98, 1.02) | 0.988 |
Knows someone living or thought to be living with HIV | 0.93 | (0.51, 1.69) | 0.805 |
People get HIV because they engage in irresponsible behavior | |||
Cohort / Campaign Exposure | |||
Post-unexposed | 1.32 | (0.85, 2.04) | 0.211 |
Post-exposed | 1.85 | (1.08, 3.15) | 0.025 |
Completed secondary school | 0.71 | (0.48, 1.05) | 0.084 |
Religion is important or very important | 1.61 | (1.07, 2.42) | 0.022 |
Spent 5 years or longer in the U.S. | 0.51 | (0.33, 0.80) | 0.004 |
Male gender | 1.62 | (1.09, 2.39) | 0.016 |
Age (continuous) | 1.02 | (1.00, 1.04) | 0.053 |
Knows someone living or thought to be living with HIV | 0.43 | (0.22, 0.84) | 0.013 |
Four or more stigma beliefs | |||
Cohort / Campaign Exposure | |||
Post-unexposed | 1.62 | (1.08, 2.43) | 0.020 |
Post-exposed | 1.76 | (1.12, 2.76) | 0.015 |
Completed secondary school | 0.53 | (0.37, 0.77) | <0.001 |
Religion is important or very important | 1.60 | (1.08, 2.39) | 0.020 |
Spent 5 years or longer in the U.S. | 0.80 | (0.54, 1.19) | 0.282 |
Male gender | 1.66 | (1.17, 2.36) | 0.005 |
Age (continuous) | 1.00 | (0.99, 1.02) | 0.635 |
Knows someone living or thought to be living with HIV | 0.48 | (0.22, 1.05) | 0.068 |
a AOR modelling effects of exposure to the Solo Se Vive Una Vez social marketing campaign on agreement with stigma belief items among exposed (n = 139) and unexposed (n = 218) post-Vive survey respondents, with pre-Vive survey respondents (n = 312) as the comparison group for the cohort/exposure effects. AOR models control for statistically significant sociodemographic differences between pre- and post-Vive survey populations (education, importance of religion, and time spent in the U.S.) as well as for factors typically associated with stigma from the literature (gender, age, knowing PLHA).
Table 4. Effects of cohort and Vive-campaign exposure on HIV testing behaviors.
AORa | (95% CI) | p-value | |
---|---|---|---|
Ever tested for HIV | |||
Cohort / Campaign Exposure | |||
Post-unexposed | 0.97 | (0.66, 1.43) | 0.867 |
Post-exposed | 2.25 | (1.35, 3.77) | 0.002 |
Completed secondary school | 0.76 | (0.53, 1.08) | 0.129 |
Religion is important or very important | 1.17 | (0.80, 1.72) | 0.419 |
Spent 5 years or longer in the U.S. | 2.61 | (1.77, 3.85) | <0.001 |
Male gender | 0.44 | (0.31, 0.62) | <0.001 |
Age (continuous) | 1.00 | (0.98, 1.02) | 0.867 |
Knows someone living or thought to be living with HIV | 2.02 | (0.95, 4.29) | 0.066 |
Tested for HIV in the past year | |||
Cohort / Campaign Exposure | |||
Post-unexposed | 0.55 | (0.35, 0.87) | 0.010 |
Post-exposed | 1.25 | (0.79, 1.99) | 0.343 |
Completed secondary school | 0.96 | (0.65, 1.40) | 0.818 |
Religion is important or very important | 0.79 | (0.52, 1.21) | 0.281 |
Spent 5 years or longer in the U.S. | 0.85 | (0.55, 1.31) | 0.457 |
Male gender | 1.02 | (0.70, 1.50) | 0.909 |
Age (continuous) | 1.00 | (0.98, 1.02) | 0.837 |
Knows someone living or thought to be living with HIV | 1.03 | (0.52, 2.05) | 0.929 |
a AOR modelling effects of exposure to the Solo Se Vive Una Vez social marketing campaign on HIV testing behaviors among exposed (n = 139) and unexposed (n = 218) post-Vive survey respondents, with pre-Vive survey respondents (n = 312) as the comparison group for the cohort/exposure effects. AOR models control for statistically significant sociodemographic differences between pre- and post-Vive survey populations (education, importance of religion, and time spent in the U.S.) as well as for factors typically associated with stigma from the literature (gender, age, knowing PLHA).
In the adjusted models, a statistically significant increase in agreement with three of the stigma items is observed among those who report that religion is important or very important to them. Respondents for whom religion is important or very important had an increased odds of 1.65 of fearing contracting HIV from the saliva of PLHA (p = 0.006), increased odds of 1.95 of believing that children living with HIV should not be able to attend school with children who are HIV negative (p < 0.001), and an increased odds of 1.61 of agreeing that people get HIV because they engage in irresponsible behavior (p = 0.022). Overall, respondents for whom religion is important or very important had an increased odds of 1.60 of endorsing four or more stigma items (p = 0.02). Males were also significantly more likely to endorse four or more stigma items (AOR = 1.66, 95% CI (1.17, 2.36), p = 0.005).
Exposure to the Vive campaign was strongly associated with ever testing for HIV (AOR = 2.25, 95% CI (1.35, 3.77), p = 0.002), but testing in the past year was not significant. Conversely, there was steep and statistically significant reduction in likelihood of recent testing pre-to-post-Vive among those unexposed to the campaign (AOR = 0.55, 95% CI (0.35, 0.87), p = 0.01).
Discussion
Sólo Se Vive Una Vez, a multilevel social marketing campaign that included messages addressing the most common HIV-related stigmatizing beliefs and barriers to testing as guided by prior research, was implemented in Baltimore, Maryland over a 6-month period. This study aimed to examine current HIV-related stigma beliefs and HIV testing behaviors in Baltimore, differences in HIV-related stigma beliefs and HIV testing behaviors pre- and post-Vive implementation, and differences in HIV-related stigma beliefs and HIV testing behaviors among those exposed to the Vive campaign and those not exposed. Respondents to the post-Vive survey continued to hold high levels of stigma beliefs, and compared to the pre-Vive survey sample, were more likely to hold four or more stigmatizing beliefs (from the six survey items). This pro-stigma trend was less pronounced in the post-Vive-exposed respondents, suggesting the campaign may have blunted pro-stigma temporal trends, but was insufficient to negate them. Survey respondents who were exposed to the campaign, however, had an increased odds of 2.25 for reporting ever having been tested for HIV, versus more consistent testing pre-to-post among the unexposed. The unexposed saw a steep and statistically significant decline in recent testing (within the past year) while the opposite trend—a slight, but not statistically significant, increase in recent testing–is observed for the exposed.
Findings from the current survey and the broader Vive evaluation demonstrate that the campaign reached a considerable fraction of the Latino community and promoted HIV testing among this group [27, 28]. However, respondents in the post-Vive campaign period showed more stigmatizing beliefs than those surveyed pre-Vive. The increase in stigmatizing beliefs does not appear to be attributable to the campaign, as pro-stigma trends were observed for both those who were exposed to the campaign versus not. One exception is the blame item, which the campaign may have unintentionally amplified. High levels of HIV-related stigma have been found in Latin American countries [33–35], and immigrants often bring these beliefs with them as they settle in the U.S. Additional research is warranted to determine the role of the messaging of the campaign in influencing beliefs, the alignment of these messages with the beliefs of newly arriving immigrants, and the role of the changing social context in the effectiveness of HIV-related stigma messaging.
Notably, the pre-Vive survey occurred prior to the 2016 U.S Presidential election. The Trump presidential campaign and the subsequent actions of the Trump Administration have had numerous detrimental effects on Latino immigrant communities [36–40]. This social context, which increased anti-immigrant sentiment across the U.S. [37, 41], may be an important consideration for understanding stigma within Latino immigrant communities. Stigmatization is a social process linked to the actions of groups of people within specific contexts of culture, power, and inequality [17]. Research on stigma [18, 42] and social identity theory [43] suggest that membership within a stigmatized group, in this case Latino immigrant communities during a time of heightened anti-immigrant sentiment and structural policies increasingly fostering exclusion, can negatively impact a person’s sense of wellbeing and self-esteem. This relationship is complex and situational [44], but when stigmatization negatively impacts self-esteem, people often engage in strategies to overcome this. Stigmatizing others may be one such strategy as it can produce an enhanced sense of wellbeing and improve self-esteem through downward comparison [45] and may provide the perception of control [46]. Members of stigmatized groups have been found to engage in downward comparisons or stigmatization of others within their group as a strategy for improving their own sense of wellbeing, self-esteem, and control [47–49]. Thus, it is possible that the increased stigma observed post-Vive may be influenced by the social context of increased anti-immigrant sentiment. The current anti-immigrant landscape certainly does not fully explain HIV-related stigma within Baltimore’s Latino immigrant community; however, in attempting to combat it, this social context may necessitate additional strategies.
Our findings highlight the need for continued efforts within religious and community institutions as a critical component of HIV-related stigma reduction strategies. Among the post-Vive survey respondents, those for whom religion was important or very important had an increased odds of 1.6 of holding four or more stigmatizing beliefs (out of six possible). Additionally, these respondents were more likely to blame people living with HIV, believing that irresponsible behaviors lead to infection. Working with non-governmental partners, including faith leaders and institutions, to increase HIV testing and reduce HIV-related stigma is identified in the National HIV/AIDS Strategy as a necessary component to reducing HIV [21]. Religious leaders often have the credibility to mobilize resources to disseminate information, and church-based interventions have shown promise in reducing HIV stigma and promoting HIV testing in Latinos [49–51]. Additionally, immigrant community institutions may be important in shifting stigmatizing beliefs in general and in relation to religion. Soon after arrival in a host location, immigrants create community institutions, such as immigrant community centers and churches. These provide the immigrants with practical, social, and emotional support as they navigate the challenges of life in their new location, while also reproducing various cultural and lifestyle elements from the immigrants’ countries of origin [52]. As such, their involvement in efforts aiming to reduce HIV-related stigma may alter perceptions of the appropriateness of these efforts, influence the beliefs of immigrants who do not attend religious institutions, and increase receptivity to efforts aiming to reduce HIV-related stigma within religious institutions [53].
Despite the significance of our findings, there are several limitations that should be noted. The cross-sectional design limits the ability to make causal associations. Because the study was primarily conducted in street venues, we selected previously validated HIV-related stigma items to develop a short questionnaire that could feasibly be administered in a brief period of time. We conducted the survey using Qualtrics software with audio formatted for each question and response, similar to ACASI, which has been shown to improve response rates and increased reporting of certain behaviors, but there is always potential for social desirability bias. For this reason, the survey did not include measures of enacted stigma, such as asking respondents if they themselves engage in stigmatizing behavior towards others. The pre- and post-campaign surveys were implemented four years apart. Although we attempted to minimize this gap, this time was necessary to develop and implement the campaign. Since this population is rapidly growing, the population currently may be very different than that of four years ago. The pre-Vive survey was included in research that informed the campaign; however, specific stigma items were not necessarily directly addressed in the final Vive messaging. Although education is a critical component of reducing stigma, the use of measures directly related to the campaign limits our ability to fully understand its effectiveness in reducing stigma. Finally, this survey was conducted primarily among Spanish-speaking, foreign-born Latinos living in Baltimore, English proficiency is generally low among Latino immigrants in Baltimore; however, this was not assessed among the survey participants. Our findings may not be generalizable to Latino populations in other areas; however, our results may be relevant to other urban areas with rapidly growing immigrant Latino communities.
Our findings demonstrate the importance of the changing social context in addressing stigma and the need to regularly assess the messaging and reach of campaigns within emerging immigrant communities to ensure their relevance and impact as the population changes. Additionally, given the association of HIV-related stigmatizing beliefs among those for whom religion was important, religious leaders and institutions must remain a critical partner in efforts to address HIV-related stigma and improve HIV-related outcomes. Continued research is warranted to identify effective social marketing strategies for rapidly changing communities as well as to better understand the influence of anti-immigrant environments on HIV stigmatizing beliefs.
Supporting information
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank our additional study team members for their efforts: Ana Ortega Meza, Sabrina McCray, Ana Cervantes, Altavista Studios, and Healthy Teen Network Inc.
Data Availability
All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.
Funding Statement
This work was supported by a Center for AIDS Research (CFAR) Adelante grant to SMDG. The CFAR Adelante program funding was provided by the US National Institutes of Health as a supplement to the Emory University CFAR (P30AI050409). This work was also supported by a 2016 developmental grant awarded to KRP from the Johns Hopkins University CFAR (P30AI094189). CFAF is a National Institutes of Health (NIH) funded program supported by the following NIH co-funding and participating institutes and centers: NIAID, NCI, NICHD, NHLBI, NIDA, NIA, NIGMS, NIDDK, NIMHD. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1.Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. HIV and Hispanics/Latinos. 2020. [Cited 2020 February 11]. Available from: https://www.cdc.gov/hiv/group/racialethnic/hispaniclatinos/index.html [Google Scholar]
- 2.Office of Minority Health. Profile: Hispanic/Latino Americans. 2019. [Cited 2020 May 15]. Available from: https://minorityhealth.hhs.gov/omh/browse.aspx?lvl=3&lvlid=64 [Google Scholar]
- 3.Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Estimated HIV incidence and prevalence in the United States, 2010–2016. HIV Surveillance Supplemental Report 2019; 24(1). [Cited 2020 May 15] Available from: http://www.cdc.gov/hiv/library/reports/hiv-surveillance.html [Google Scholar]
- 4.Febo-Vazquez I, Copen CE, Daugherty J. Main reasons for never testing for HIV among women and men aged 15–44 in the United States, 2011–2015. National Health Statistics Reports; no 107. Hyattsville, MD: National Center for Health Statistics. 2018. [Cited 2020 May 15]. Available from: https://www.cdc.gov/nchs/data/nhsr/nhsr107.pdf [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Dennis AM, Napravnik S, Seña AC, Eron J. Late entry to HIV care among Latinos compared with non-Latinos in a southeastern US cohort. Clin Infect Dis. 2011; 53(5): 480–7. doi: 10.1093/cid/cir434 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Chen NE, Gallant JE, Page KR. A systematic review of HIV/AIDS survival and delayed diagnosis among Hispanics in the United States. J Immigr Minor Health. 2012; 14(1): 65–81. doi: 10.1007/s10903-011-9497-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Lopes BL, Eron J, Mugavero MJ, Miller W, Napravnik S. HIV care initiation delay among rural residents in the Southeastern United States, 1996 to 2012. J Acquir Immune Defic Syndr. 2017; 76(2): 171–76. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Diagnoses of HIV infection in the United States and dependent areas, 2017. HIV Surveillance Report 2018; 29. [Cited 2020 May 18]. Available from: http://www.cdc.gov/hiv/library/reports/hiv-surveillance.html [Google Scholar]
- 9.Espinoza L, Hall HI, Selik RM, Hu X. Characteristics of HIV infection among Hispanics, United States 2003–2006. J Acquir Immune Defic Syndr. 2008; 49(1): 94–101. doi: 10.1097/QAI.0b013e3181820129 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Sheehan DM, Trepka MJ, Dillon FR. Latinos in the United States on the HIV/AIDS care continuum by birth country/region: A systematic review of the literature. Int J STD AIDS. 2015; 26(1): 1–12. doi: 10.1177/0956462414532242 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Earnshaw VA, Bogart LM, Dovidio JF, Williams DR. Stigma and racial/ethnic HIV disparities: Moving toward resilience. Stigma Health. 2015; 1(S): 60–74. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Grieb SMD, Desir F, Flores-Miller A, Page K. Qualitative assessment of HIV prevention challenges and opportunities among Latino immigrant men in a new receiving city. J Immigr Minor Health. 2015; 17(1): 118–24. doi: 10.1007/s10903-013-9932-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Grieb SMD, Shah H, Flores-Miller A, Zelaya C, Page KR. HIV-related stigma among Spanish-speaking Latinos in an emerging immigrant receiving city. J Immigr Minor Health. 2017; 19(4): 868–75. doi: 10.1007/s10903-016-0497-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Levison JH, Bogart LM, Khan IF, Mejia D, Alegria M, Safren S. “Where It Falls Apart”: Barriers to retention in HIV care in Latino immigrants and migrants. AIDS Patient Care STDs. 2017; 31(9): 394–405. doi: 10.1089/apc.2017.0084 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Dang BN, Giordano TP, Kim JH. Sociocultural and structural barriers to care among undocumented Latino immigrants with HIV infection. J Immigr Minor Health. 2012;14(1):124–31 doi: 10.1007/s10903-011-9542-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Levy V, Prentiss D, Balmas G, Chen S, Israelski D, Katzenstein D, et al. Factors in the delayed HIV presentation of immigrants in Northern California: implications for voluntary counseling and testing programs. J Immigr Minor Health. 2007;9(1):49–54. doi: 10.1007/s10903-006-9015-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Parker R, Aggleton P. HIV and AIDS-related stigma and discrimination: A conceptual framework and implications for action. Sco Sci Med. 2003; 57(1): 13–24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Goffman E. Stigma: Notes on the Management of Spoiled Identity. Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall; 1963. [Google Scholar]
- 19.Varas-Díaz N, Neilands TB, Malavé Rivera S, Betancourt E. Religion and HIV/AIDS stigma: Implications for health professionals in Puerto Rico. Glob Public Health. 2010;5(3):295–312. doi: 10.1080/17441690903436581 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Derose KP, Kanouse DE, Bogart LM, Griffin BA, Haas A, Stucky BD, et al. Predictors of HIV-related stigmas among African American and Latino religious congregants. Cultur Divers Ethnic Minor Psychol. 2016;22(2):185–95. doi: 10.1037/cdp0000062 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Office of National AIDS Policy. National HIV/AIDS strategy for the United States: Updated to 2020. [Cited 2020 May 18]. Available from: https://files.hiv.gov/s3fs-public/nhas-update.pdf [Google Scholar]
- 22.Guilamo-Ramos V, Thimm-Kaiser M, Benzekri A, Chacón G, López OR, Scaccabarrozzi L, et al. The invisible US Hispanic/Latino HIV crisis: Addressing gaps in the national response. Am J Public Health. 2020; 110(1): 27–31. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2019.305309 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Beer L et al. Recent US Centers for Disease Control and Prevention activities to reduce HIV stigma. J Int Assoc of Provid AIDS Care. 2019; 18: 2325958218823541. doi: 10.1177/2325958218823541 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Friedman AL, Kachur RE, Noar SM, McFarlane M. Health communication and social marketing campaigns for sexually transmitted disease prevention and control: What is the evidence of their effectiveness? Sex Transm Dis. 2016; 43(2S): S83–101. doi: 10.1097/OLQ.0000000000000286 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Grieb SMD, Flores-Miller A, Gulledge N, Clifford R, Page K. ¡Vive!: Designing an intervention to improve timely HIV diagnosis among Latino immigrant men. Prog Community Health Partnersh. 2016; 10(3): 365–72. doi: 10.1353/cpr.2016.0043 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Grieb SMD, Flores-Miller A, Page KR. ¡Sólo Se Vive Una Vez! (You only live once): A pilot evaluation of individually-tailored video modules aiming to increase HIV testing among foreign-born Latino men. J Acquir Immune Defic Syndr. 2017; 74(Suppl 2): S104–12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Shah HS, Grieb SMD, Flores-Miller A, Greenbaum A, Castellanos-Aguirre J, Page KR. Sólo Se Vive Una Vez: The implementation and reach of an HIV screening campaign for Latinx immigrants. AIDS Educ Prev. 2020; 32(3): 229–42 doi: 10.1521/aeap.2020.32.3.229 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Shah HS, Grieb SM, Flores-Miller A, Yenokyan K, Castellanos-Aguirre J, Greenbaum A, et al. Sólo Se Vive Una Vez: Evaluation of a Social Marketing Campaign Promoting HIV Screening and Prevention for Immigrant Latinxs. AIDS Behav. Forthcoming 2021. doi: 10.1007/s10461-021-03165-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Leite L, Buresh M, Rios N, Conley A, Flys T, Page KR. Cell phone utilization among foreign-born Latinos: A promising tool for dissemination of health and HIV information. J Immigr Minor Health. 2014; 16(4): 661–69. doi: 10.1007/s10903-013-9792-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Stigma Action Network. Working for a world free of HIV-related stigma. [Cited 2016 February 22]. Available from: http://san.kwantu.net/
- 31.Heijnders M, Van Der Meij S. The fight against stigma: an overview of stigma-reduction strategies and interventions. Psychol Health Med. 2006; 11(3): 353–63. doi: 10.1080/13548500600595327 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Darrow WW, Montanea JE, Gladwin H. AIDS-related stigma among Black and Hispanic young adults. AIDS and Behavior. 2009. Dec;13(6):1178–88. doi: 10.1007/s10461-009-9601-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Johnson M, Samarina A, Xi H, Valdez Ramalho Madruga J, Hocqueloux L, Loutfy M, et al. Barriers to access to care reported by women living with HIV across 27 countries. AIDS Care. 2015; 27(10): 1220–30. doi: 10.1080/09540121.2015.1046416 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Ugarte WJ, Högberg U, Valladares EC, Essén B. Measuring HIV- and AIDS-related stigma and discrimination in Nicaragua: Results from a community-based study. AIDS Edu Prev. 2013; 25(2): 164–78. doi: 10.1521/aeap.2013.25.2.164 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Pineirua A, Sierra-Madero J, Cahn P, Palmero RN, Buitrago EM, Young B, et al. The HIV care continuum in Latin America: Challenges and opportunities. Lancet Infec Dis. 2015; 15(7): 833–39. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Talamantes E, Aguilar-Gaxiola S. Perspective: POTUS Trump’s Executive orders–implications for immigrants and health care. Ethn Dis. 2017; 27(2): 121–24. doi: 10.18865/ed.27.2.121 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Brooks B. Victims of anti-Latino hate crimes soar in the U.S.: FBI report. Reuters. 2019. [Cited 2020 September 16]. Available from: https://www.reuters.com/article/us-hatecrimes-report/victims-of-anti-latino-hate-crimes-soar-in-u-s-fbi-report-idUSKBN1XM2OQ?feedType=RSS&feedName=domesticNews [Google Scholar]
- 38.Nienhusser HK, Oshio T. Awakened hatred and heightened fears: “The Trump Effect” on the lives of mixed-status families. Cult Stud Crit Methodol. 2019; 19(3): 173–83. [Google Scholar]
- 39.Callaghan T, Washburn DJ, Nimmons K, Duchicela D, Gurram A, Burdine J. Immigrant health access in Texas: policy, rhetoric, and fear in the Trump era. BMC Health Serv Res. 2019; 19(1): 1–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Roche KM, Vaquera E, Delbasso CA, Kuperminc GP, Cordon M, Rivera MI. Worry, behavior change, and daily adversity: How U.S. Latino/a parents experience contemporary immigration actions and news. J Fam Issues. 2020; 41(9): 1546–68. [Google Scholar]
- 41.Anti-Defamation League. Mainstreaming hate: The anti-immigrant movement in the U.S. [Cited 2020 September 18]. Available from: https://www.adl.org/the-anti-immigrant-movement-in-the-us
- 42.Jones EE, Farina A, Hastorf A, Marks H, Miller D, Scott R, et al. Social stigma: The psychology of marked relationships. New York, NY: W.H. Freeman & Co.; 1984 [Google Scholar]
- 43.Tajfel H, Turner J. An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. In: Austin WG, Worchel S, eds. The Social Psychology of Intergroup Relations. Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole; 1979. pp. 33–48. [Google Scholar]
- 44.Crocker J. Social stigma and self-esteem: Situational construction of self-worth. J Exp Soc Psychol. 1999; 35(1): 89–107. [Google Scholar]
- 45.Wills TA. Downward comparison principles in social psychology. Psychol Bull. 1981; 90(2): 245–71. [Google Scholar]
- 46.Mackie DM, Hamilton DL, Susskind J, Rosselli F. Social psychological foundations of stereotype formation. In: Macrae CN, Stangor C, Hewstone M, eds. Stereotypes and Stereotyping. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 1996. pp 41–78. [Google Scholar]
- 47.Finlay WM, Lyons E. Social categorizations, social comparisons and stigma: Presentations of self in people with learning difficulties. Br J Soc Psychol. 2000; 39(1): 129–46. doi: 10.1348/014466600164372 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Van Vuuren M, Teurlings J, Bohlmeijer ET. Shared fate and social comparison: Identity work in the context of a stigmatized occupation. Journal Manag Organ. 2012; 18(2): 263–80. [Google Scholar]
- 49.Furst RT, Evans DN. An exploration of stigma in the lives of sex offenders and heroin abusers. Deviant Behav. 2015; 36(2): 130–45. [Google Scholar]
- 50.Sutton MY, Parks CP. HIV/AIDS prevention, faith, and spirituality among Black/African American and Latino communities in the United States: Strengthening scientific faith-based efforts to shift the course of the epidemic and reduce HIV-related health disparities. J Relig Health. 2013; 52(2): 514–30. doi: 10.1007/s10943-011-9499-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Derose KP, Griffin BA, Kanouse DE, Bogart LM, Williams MV, Haas AC, et al. Effects of a pilot church-based intervention to reduce HIV stigma and promote HIV testing among African Americans and Latinos. AIDS Behav. 2016; 20(8): 1692–1705. doi: 10.1007/s10461-015-1280-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Basch LG, Schiller NG. Nations unbound: Transnational projects, postcolonial predicaments, and deterritorialized Nation-States. Routledge; 2005. [Google Scholar]
- 53.Chin JJ, Neilands TB, Weiss L, Mantell JE. Paradigm shifters, professionals, and community sentinels: Immigrant community institutions’ roles in shaping places and implications for stigmatized public health initiatives. Health Place. 2008; 14(4): 866–82. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2008.01.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.