Abstract
A holistic, evolutionary framework about human cooperation must incorporate information about women's cooperative behaviour. Yet, most empirical research on human cooperation has centered on men's behaviour or been derived from experimental studies conducted in western, industrialized populations. These bodies of data are unlikely to accurately represent human behavioural diversity. To address this gap and provide a more balanced view of human cooperation, this issue presents substantial new data and multi-disciplinary perspectives to document the complexity of women's cooperative behaviour. Research in this issue 1) challenges narratives about universal gender differences in cooperation, 2) reconsiders patrilocality and access to kin as constraints on women's cooperation, 3) reviews evidence for a connection between social support and women's health and 4) examines the phylogenetic roots of female cooperation. Here, we discuss the steps taken in this issue toward a more complete and evidence-based understanding of the role that cooperation plays in women's and girls' lives and in building human sociality.
This article is part of the theme issue ‘Cooperation among women: evolutionary and cross-cultural perspectives’.
Keywords: women, cooperation, evolution, sex differences, prosociality
1. Introduction
Over the past several million years, humans have become a globally dominant species by greatly expanding the scale at which we participate in cooperative activities and leveraging our capacity for cumulative culture [1,2]. Cooperation is a central and universal element in human societies and foundational to derived human traits, such as food sharing, pooled energy budgets and the division of labour [3–5]. There is also a growing consensus that cooperation is critical to sustaining elements of human life history and cognition [6–9]. Much attention has been paid to the cooperative childrearing networks required to buffer the costs human mothers face in caring for multiple dependent offspring at the same time [10–14]. The breadth of a mother's cooperative childcare network, composed of her children, female relatives, other kin and nonkin, is well documented across a number of societies [15–21], as is women's cooperative food production [22–27]. However, much less attention has been paid to the many ways that women and girls engage in cooperative political, ceremonial, economic and social institutions, collectively form coalitions and develop exchange and support networks. Women's cooperative activities and roles across these domains are likely important cohesive forces in building human societies and maintaining intergroup relations [28–32]. Yet, empirical syntheses, quantitative investigations and theoretic development on this topic remain limited. Consequently, women's cooperation has been neglected in reconstructions of the evolution of human sociality [4,33–36]. This special issue urges a more balanced understanding of gender differences in the propensity to cooperate and the evolutionary processes that shaped human society. In this introduction, we review the theoretical backdrop for this theme issue and synthesize ways in which contributions in the issue illuminate the diversity and complexity of women's cooperation.
Theoretical models and empirical analyses of cooperation's role in human societies have often focused on male-dominated domains, particularly hunting, warfare and leadership. Researchers have emphasized the importance of men's cooperation in hunting because returns from hunting are often highly variable and sharing among hunters reduces variance in food acquisition [8,37,38]. Others have emphasized men's inclination to cooperate in large groups, even when individuals have relatively shallow relationships, to protect territory and engage in intergroup warfare [39–42]. Hunting, inter-group defense and coalitionary political support have all been argued to favour male alliances and cooperation and are considered less integral to how women organize [39,42–51]. Emphasis on men's cooperation also stems from common but unsupported expectations about the prevalence of male philopatry and patrilocality during human evolution ([47,52,53]; but see [54]). The assumed centrality of male bonding has overemphasized women's isolation from their kin; competition with other women for mating opportunities, allocare and resources; and men's primacy to band together, and has led some scientists to theorize about biological sex differences in male and female predispositions to cooperate [42,46,55,56]. Further supporting this perspective, some experimental psychology studies have emphasized gender differences in the propensity to cooperate, suggesting that men's and boys’ same-sex relationships are more cooperative and that they tend to cooperate in larger groups [43,45,53,55–66]. However, the majority of these experiments were conducted in western, industrialized populations. Collectively, these biases in current research are unlikely to accurately represent the full range of human behavioural diversity. In this issue, we approach the male-dominated narrative through a critical lens and ask whether gender differences in cooperation are supported when women's behaviour is examined from a broader, developmental and cross-cultural perspective.
This issue brings together research from behavioural ecology, anthropology, health psychology, developmental psychology and behavioural economics to construct a more complete view of the roles that cooperation and competition play in the lives of women and girls. Papers explore the phylogenetic legacy of female cooperation by considering sex differences in cooperation among mammals and the nature of female relationships in our close living relatives, chimpanzees and gorillas. Several ethnographic case studies highlight the breadth of women's cooperative activities, how women structure their social networks and the range of contextual factors that influence the extent of female cooperation, including relatedness, inheritance systems and social norms. Contributions also assess experimental evidence for gender differences in prosocial behaviour during development and adulthood and review the relationship between female social ties and physical, psychological and relationship health.
Taken together, the research in this issue does not support universal gender differences in cooperative tendencies. Rather, the evidence points to the important function that cooperation plays in the lives of women, who build multifaceted cooperative relationships across many domains and ages, and in a range of household, community and intergroup settings.
2. A note about terms
This issue focuses on topics linked to sex and gender across nonhuman and human species, from biological and cultural perspectives. Though we are hypothesizing about the evolution of behaviours potentially linked to sex, we are discussing gendered experiences in humans; thus, we generally use the terms women, girls, men and boys when referring to humans, and use female and male when making cross-species comparisons or discussing reproductive biology. However, the use of these terms is nuanced. Authors use sex or gender terms according to what is most appropriate for their human participants. Additionally, we expect some variation across studies in the extent to which participants identify with sex or gender terms given that they have different meanings across cultures. Addressing the full spectrum of gendered experiences in cooperation is important complementary research but goes beyond the scope of the present issue.
3. Key themes and findings from this issue
Several themes and findings emerge from the special issue and highlight the ways in which female cooperation needs to be reconsidered.
(a) . No compelling evidence for universal gender differences in cooperative networks or prosocial behaviour
Sexual selection theory has been extended to make predictions about how men and women may use cooperative networks differently [41,42,46,47,53,55,56,67,68]. In brief, among mammals, male reproduction is generally limited by access to mates. This, combined with the pace of human life history, favours male engagement in high-risk strategies, mate guarding and pursuit of high social status. By extension, men and boys are expected to form political alliances and engage in collective mate defense through intergroup aggression, and benefit from forming broad, diffuse social networks. On the other hand, the reproductive success of mammalian females is generally limited by access to resources. As a consequence, women are expected to engage in low-risk strategies that enhance their own health and survival and that of their children. Women and girls are expected to build ties that are linked to childcare, be highly selective in choosing cooperative partners, form narrow social networks, exclude competitors and cooperate mainly with close kin.
Evidence for this theoretical framework has been mixed. In support, several studies characterize women and girls as forming fewer same-sex friendships, investing more in each relationship, being more likely to terminate relationships over social transgressions and being less cooperative than men and boys in same-sex interactions [43,45,53,55–61,63–66]. On the other hand, research from social and health psychology indicates that women are more likely to respond to stressful scenarios by seeking and providing support and engaging in other-oriented strategies despite interacting with strangers, whereas men are more likely to respond selfishly and competitively ([69–72], reviewed in [73]). Further, a review on children's prosocial behaviour found that girls were often found to have smaller playgroups, but they were more prosocial than boys in most other respects [64]. However, these studies have mainly been conducted with children and undergraduate students in western, educated, industrial, religious, democratic (WEIRD) societies [74–76].
The ethnographic record is also mixed. One study comparing ethnographic records from small-scale societies argued that men often cooperate in larger groups while women are more likely to engage in non-cooperative, parallel activities [42]. By contrast, in a study with Hadza hunter–gatherers in Tanzania, men and women did not differ in their number of preferred campmates or in the number of individuals with whom they would share gifts [77]. An absence of gender differences in social networks has also been found in other studies [78–80]. These findings are consistent with two studies in this issue that found no substantial gender differences in cooperative networks [81,82]. In detailed studies of two Tamil villages (South Indian mixed economy), Simpson and Power [82] showed that men and women use somewhat different strategies to obtain help, but these differences were statistically modest and did not generate substantive gender differences in the structure of support networks. Similarly, in a study of matrilineal and patrilineal Mosuo communities (Tibetan-descended agriculturalists), Mattison and colleagues [81] found that differences in men's and women's cooperative networks did not follow expected gender patterns.
The ethnographic evidence of relatively subtle and inconsistent differences in network characteristics in small-scale societies is reflected in experimental games that assess prosociality and cooperation across cultures. In this issue, House and colleagues [83] found no evidence for gender differences in prosociality and fairness among children in 21 diverse societies. Complementing this developmental study, Spadaro and colleagues ([84], in this issue) found no consistent gender differences in experiments designed to evaluate cooperative tendencies among adults in 20 industrialized societies, in contrast to a previous but less diverse meta-analysis [57]. Echoing Hruschka [85], the papers in this special issue indicate that claims about universal gender differences in cooperation are overstated. Given the range of findings across studies, we suggest that further investigation into the situational and societal contexts that intensify or diminish cooperative gender differences will be more fruitful than searching for universal differences.
(b) . Women's cooperation extends beyond kinship lines
As with other female mammals, cooperation among women and girls often falls along kinship lines [20,86–89]. Importantly, though, the notion that access to kin limits female cooperation contradicts the empirical record and needs to be reconsidered.
Discussions of cooperation among women often highlight the consequences of postmarital residence, with patrilocal residence expected to limit women's access to cooperative partners [42,47,52,53]. However, longitudinal ethnographic research has shown that residence patterns are more flexible than previously realized [90–93]. Even where patrilocality is prevalent, women develop workarounds that enable them to find new partners or maintain connections with natal kin [54,94–97]. In an analysis that compared supportive relationships among women in rural Bangladesh who emigrated to their husband's village and those who stayed in their natal village when they married, Hruschka and colleagues ([97], in this issue) found that patrilocality did not isolate women. Women who migrated from their natal community initially lived in proximity to few kin, but they quickly built social support relations with close affines and friends. In a similar finding, the social group size of Tsimane women (South American horticulturalists) did not differ between women in different post-marital residence settings when they engaged in activities such as gardening and wage labour, manufacturing and resource acquisition. However, the distance a woman lived from her parents, regardless of whether she lived patrilocally, matrilocally or neolocally, influenced her social group size and her probability of receiving allocare ([98], in this issue). Likewise, among the patrilineal Mosuo, women's food preparation networks were better predicted by geographical distance than by genetic relatedness ([81], in this issue). Finally, Simpson & Power ([82], in this issue) assessed help-seeking among patrilocal Tamil men and women and found no evidence that women's social network ties are limited to kin. Taken as a whole, findings from this issue challenge the conventional assumption that patrilocality constrains women's cooperation and urge us to consider the importance of affinal kin and friendships in women's cooperation.
Women's capacity to flexibly form cooperative ties both with kin and non-kin may stem from a long evolutionary history of navigating relationships in contexts with differential access to kin. Supporting this idea, research on female great apes demonstrates that between-species variation in access to kin does not easily predict levels of female cooperation. Though great ape social systems vary widely, females in all species typically disperse from their natal groups and it is uncommon for adult females to live with female kin [99]. Regardless, in some species females form well-differentiated relationships and cooperate with one another. At one end of the spectrum, bonobo females are highly social and cooperative with each other, despite living in a male philopatric social system, and even engage in friendly interactions with females from neighbouring groups [100–103]. Female chimpanzees are less social than bonobos, but engage in selective, socially tolerant relationships that support cooperative, agonistic coalitions ([104], in this issue). Occasionally strong, kin-based relationships occur when female chimpanzees remain in their natal community as adults, yet neither strong bonds nor kinship bolster cooperation ([104], in this issue). In both western and mountain gorillas, females form differentiated, stable partner preferences, despite natal and secondary dispersal occurring regularly and limiting both access to kin and long-term investment in relationships ([105], in this issue). Female orangutans express yet another unique ape pattern, as they most often range only with offspring, but have some access to kin because their ranges frequently overlap with other female relatives [106–109]. Despite occasional access to kin and low rates of affiliation, kinship still biases social tolerance among adult female orangutans [110].
Further substantiating the argument that access to kin does not sufficiently explain variation in cooperation, Smith and colleagues ([111], in this issue) found no phylogenetic signal for sex differences in intragroup coalition formation across mammals. They also found no support for the prediction that in female philopatric species, females are more likely to exhibit intragroup coalitions than females in other species. Collectively, studies in humans, non-human apes and across mammals support the proposition that female cooperation extends beyond kin. Females may develop cooperative bonds regardless of where they reside after maturity, such that proximity to kin alone is an insufficient predictor of female social relationships.
(c) . Variation in women's cooperation reflects need, risk and cultural norms
Post-marital residence is only one facet of the socioecological context that affects women's cooperation. Kramer ([112], in this issue) argues that women's cooperation is responsive to a variety of factors, including cultural norms, life-history stage, subsistence strategy and household demography. In a similar vein, research in experimental settings indicates that women's cooperative behaviour may be more sensitive to their social partners' needs and behaviour than men's, and women may be more likely to shift social strategies depending on the costs and benefits of each scenario compared to men ([73,82,113], all in this issue).
In this issue, authors tested several novel predictions about how women's cooperation is influenced by socioecological context. For example, Page and colleagues ([114], in this issue) hypothesized that women's childcare networks respond to major livelihood transitions. They predicted that in the shift from mobile to sedentary residence, childcare networks should decrease in size since wealth accumulates with sedentarism, reducing the need to rely on large networks of cooperators. However, they did not find this to be the case for Agta women (Philippine foragers); rather, mothers had large and diverse childcare networks across both mobile and sedentary communities. In a cross-cultural study, Kraft and colleagues ([115], in this issue) found support for the prediction that the size and composition of cooperative food networks vary with the risks associated with food acquisition strategies. Their comparison of Batek (Malaysian hunter–gatherers) and Tsimane (Bolivian horticulturalists) showed that Batak women, who experienced greater variability in daily foraging success, had more diffuse and diverse networks in contrast to Tsimane women, who relied on fewer, dependable cooperative partners, who were most often close kin, for horticultural labour. As another perspective, Bedrov & Gable ([73], in this issue) proposed that cross-cultural differences in cooperation should vary with society-level individualism versus collectivism, as women seek and benefit from cooperation differently in these contexts. Together, these studies highlight that women's cooperation is responsive to socioecological context and local cultural norms. The continued development of new hypotheses will be required to produce a holistic perspective on the evolution of cooperation in humans that accounts for nuance and variation in women's cooperative strategies, as has been done with men's cooperation.
(d) . Cooperation and competition are intimately interconnected
Competition and cooperation are closely linked social strategies. The behavioural ecology literature provides many examples of males and females in group-living species using cooperative tactics to offset individual differences in rank and power and alter the outcome of intragroup conflicts [89,111,116,117]. The connection between competition and cooperation also emerges in research on women. For example, several researchers have proposed that women cooperatively engage in subtle, non-confrontational tactics, such as gossip and exclusion of others, to compete for status or mates [52,118–120]. In this issue, Cassar and Rigdon [113] hypothesize that women may suppress competitive behaviours in an effort to maintain the potential for cooperative relationships. In their overview, they highlight studies showing that girls frequently prefer cooperative over competitive games and are less tolerant of social status differences among peers than boys. The authors suggest that this is why women are less competitive in ‘winner takes all’ experimental games, but more competitive when experiments have the option for winners to share with losers.
In real-world scenarios, women's inclination to cooperate or compete can directly influence their reproductive fitness. In human societies, women depend on others for help in cooperatively raising offspring, and if multiple women rely on the same pool of helpers, it raises the potential for competition over access to help (reviewed in [121], in this issue). Most empirical studies of reproductive conflict have been conducted in patrilocal–patrilineal societies and among agro-pastoralists, and the findings have been mixed. Hackman & Kramer ([121], in this issue) assessed potential fitness effects of reproductive conflict in a group of Savanna Pumé women (South American hunter–gatherers) who typically live in their natal camps after marriage and rely on a common pool of helpers. Longitudinal data showed that the number of women living in the same camp, whether they are kin or non-kin, did not negatively affect women's fertility or their children's survivorship. This study points to the many ways that residential mobility and bilateral kin access reassort women to labour needs and resource constraints, readjust tensions and reduce the potential for reproductive conflict.
Research on women's cooperation will benefit from further considering how cooperation intersects with competition. Given that women's competition is expected to change in different contexts [54,113,120], and that women can use cooperation to compete, extending hypotheses about variation in competition will augment explanations about variation in cooperation.
(e) . Women's cooperation extends beyond childcare
Discussions of female cooperation in real-world settings have largely been confined to reproductive concerns—childcare and provisioning of children. However, women and girls cooperate across domains as broad-reaching as those of men and boys, including coalition formation, political, ceremonial and social institutions, and exchange and support networks ([112], in this issue). Although their participation may be less formalized than men's, the ethnographic evidence summarized by Kramer [112] suggests that we need to reconsider the notion that the scope for cooperation among women and girls is limited by reproductive constraints.
The strategies women employ to form support networks should reflect the demands of the activity they are engaging in. For example, Mattison and colleagues ([81], in this issue) proposed that cooperative networks centred around food preparation, which is primarily women's work, and farm equipment lending, primarily men's work, in matrilineal and patrilineal Mosuo communities were shaped more by the type of activity than by expected gender norms around cooperation. Furthermore, women and girls are adept at exchanging cooperation across domains to balance multiple needs, such as childcare and productive labour. Starkweather and colleagues ([122], in this issue) showed that the cooperative childcare and work partner networks of Shodagor women in rural Bangladesh differed according to women's economic activities. Women who cooperated together as traders were also likely to provide childcare for each other. Since trading is incompatible with childcare, this exchange allowed them to solve the tradeoff between allocating time to childcare and economic activities. By contrast, fishing is compatible with childcare, and women who worked as fishers had narrower allocare networks.
Considering that women's cooperative networks change with the task at hand and that women cooperate across diverse domains, more evidence-based ethnographic support is needed to capture the breadth of women's cooperative behaviour. Without this data, understanding the factors that shape women's cooperation across societies or making comparisons with men's cooperation will remain cursory.
4. Conclusion
The multi-disciplinary research in this issue situates women's cooperative behaviour in a larger, cross-cultural, evolutionary context. In doing so, contributions challenge the narrative of clear-cut, biologically rooted gender differences in cooperation and confront the current perspective that women's cooperation is rare. Authors repeatedly demonstrate that women's cooperative bonds extend beyond networks of close kin, push us to think about women's cooperation outside of childcare, and suggest that women's ability to flexibly leverage relationships with available partners is rooted in our evolutionary past. Additionally, research in this issue urges us to consider variables beyond marital residence patterns, access to kin and competition as determinants of the scope of women's and girl's cooperation. Authors break further ground by proposing novel hypotheses about how women's cooperation responds to risk, wealth accumulation, cultural norms and economic needs. Yet, we remain in need of broad, theoretical discussions of the conditions that promote and constrain women's behavioural strategies. Studies examining how women's cooperation is shaped by changes over their lifespans, including life-history transitions and demographic changes in access to cooperative partners, and how women's cooperation differs in between- and within-group contexts are sparse. With the accumulation of large-scale, long-term datasets, researchers should also endeavour to answer questions about the impact that cooperation has on women's health, survival and reproductive outcomes. These advances will be necessary to establish an improved, holistic model of human sociality that recognizes the importance of cooperation for both men and women, adults and children.
Data accessibility
This article has no additional data.
Authors' contributions
S.A.F.: conceptualization, investigation, writing—original draft; B.S.: conceptualization, investigation, writing—review and editing; J.S.: conceptualization, investigation, writing—review and editing; K.L.K.: conceptualization, investigation, writing—original draft.
All authors gave final approval for publication and agreed to be held accountable for the work performed therein.
Conflict of interest declaration
This theme issue was put together by the Guest Editor team under supervision from the journal's Editorial staff, following the Royal Society's ethical codes and best-practice guidelines. The Guest Editor team invited contributions and handled the review process. Individual Guest Editors were not involved in assessing papers where they had a personal, professional or financial conflict of interest with the authors or the research described. Independent reviewers assessed all papers. Invitation to contribute did not guarantee inclusion.
Funding
We received no funding for this study.
References
- 1.Henrich J. 2016. The secret of our success: how culture is driving human evolution, domesticating our species, and making us smarter. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Tomasello M, Melis AP, Tennie C, Wyman E, Herrmann E. 2012. Two key steps in the evolution of human cooperation: the interdependence hypothesis. Curr. Anthropol. 53, 673-692. ( 10.1086/668207) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Brown DE. 1991. Human universals. New York, NY: McGraw Hill. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Kaplan HS, Hooper PL, Gurven M. 2009. The evolutionary and ecological roots of human social organization. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 364, 3289-3299. ( 10.1098/rstb.2009.0115) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Kramer KL, Ellison PT. 2010. Pooled energy budgets: resituating human energy–allocation trade-offs. Evol. Anthropol.: Issues News and Rev. 19, 136-147. ( 10.1002/evan.20265) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Gurven M, Stieglitz J, Hooper PL, Gomes C, Kaplan H. 2012. From the womb to the tomb: the role of transfers in shaping the evolved human life history. Exp. Gerontol. 47, 807-813. ( 10.1016/j.exger.2012.05.006) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Hrdy SB. 2009. Mothers and others: the evolutionary origins of mutual understanding. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 8.Kaplan H, Hill K, Lancaster J, Hurtado AM. 2000. A theory of human life history evolution: Diet, intelligence, and longevity. Evol. Anthropol.: Issues News Rev. 9, 156-185. () [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Kramer KL. 2018. The cooperative economy of food: Implications for human life history and physiology. Physiol. Behav. 193, 196-204. ( 10.1016/j.physbeh.2018.03.029) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Hawkes K, O'Connell JF, Jones NGB, Alvarez H, Charnov EL. 1998. Grandmothering, menopause, and the evolution of human life histories. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 95, 1336-1339. ( 10.1073/pnas.95.3.1336) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Hrdy SB. 2007. Evolutionary context of human development: the cooperative breeding model. In Family relationships: an evolutionary perspective (eds Salmon CA, Shackelford TK), pp. 39-68. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 12.Kramer KL, Otárola-Castillo E. 2015. When mothers need others: The impact of hominin life history evolution on cooperative breeding. J. Hum. Evol. 84, 16-24. ( 10.1016/j.jhevol.2015.01.009) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Lee RD, Kramer KL. 2002. Children's economic roles in the Maya family life cycle: Cain, Caldwell, and Chayanov revisited. Popul. Dev. Rev. 28, 475-499. ( 10.1111/j.1728-4457.2002.00475.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Meehan CL, Helfrecht C, Malcom CD. 2016. Implications of lengthy development and maternal life history. In Childhood: origins, evolution, and implications (eds Meehan CL, Crittenden AN), pp. 199-220. Santa Fe, NM: School for Advanced Research Press. [Google Scholar]
- 15.Crittenden AN, Marlowe FW. 2008. Allomaternal care among the Hadza of Tanzania. Hum. Nat. 19, 249. ( 10.1007/s12110-008-9043-3) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Gibson MA, Mace R. 2005. Helpful grandmothers in rural Ethiopia: a study of the effect of kin on child survival and growth. Evol. Hum. Behav. 26, 469-482. ( 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2005.03.004) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Helfrecht C, Roulette JW, Lane A, Sintayehu B, Meehan CL. 2020. Life history and socioecology of infancy. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 173, 619-629. ( 10.1002/ajpa.24145) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Ivey PK. 2000. Cooperative reproduction in Ituri forest hunter-gatherers: who cares for Efe infants? Curr. Anthropol. 41, 856-866. ( 10.1086/317414) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Kramer KL, Veile A. 2018. Infant allocare in traditional societies. Physiol. Behav. 193, 117-126. ( 10.1016/j.physbeh.2018.02.054) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Meehan CL, Helfrecht C, Quinlan RJ. 2014. Cooperative breeding and Aka children's nutritional status: is flexibility key? Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 153, 513-525. ( 10.1002/ajpa.22415) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Page AE, Emmott EH, Dyble M, Smith D, Chaudhary N, Viguier S, Migliano AB. 2021. Children are important too: juvenile playgroups and maternal childcare in a foraging population, the Agta. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 376, 20200026. ( 10.1098/rstb.2020.0026) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Bird RB, Scelza B, Bird DW, Smith EA. 2012. The hierarchy of virtue: mutualism, altruism and signaling in Martu women's cooperative hunting. Evol. Hum. Behav. 33, 64-78. ( 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2011.05.007) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Bliege Bird R, Bird DW. 2008. Why women hunt: risk and contemporary foraging in a Western Desert aboriginal community. Curr. Anthropol. 49, 655-693. ( 10.1086/587700) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Hurtado AM, Hawkes K, Hill K, Kaplan H. 1985. Female subsistence strategies among Ache hunter-gatherers of Eastern Paraguay. Hum. Ecol. 13, 1-28. ( 10.1007/BF01531086) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Lee RB. 1979. The !kung San: men, women and work in a foraging society. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 26.Marlowe F. 2010. The Hadza: hunter-gatherers of Tanzania. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press. [Google Scholar]
- 27.Noss AJ, Hewlett BS. 2001. The Contexts of Female Hunting in Central Africa. Am. Anthropol. 103, 1024-1040. ( 10.1525/aa.2001.103.4.1024) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Kerns V, Brown JK (eds). 1991. In her prime: new views of middle-aged women. Urbana, IL: University of Illinois Press. [Google Scholar]
- 29.Leonetti DL, Chabot-Hanowell B. 2011. The Foundation of Kinship. Hum. Nat. 22, 16-40. ( 10.1007/s12110-011-9111-y) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Purkayastha B, Subramaniam M. 2004. The power of women's informal networks: lessons in social change from south Asia and West Africa. Lanham, MD: Lexington Books. [Google Scholar]
- 31.Reiter RR (ed). 1975. Toward an anthropology of women. New York, NY: Monthly Review Press. [Google Scholar]
- 32.Zimbalist Rosaldo M, Lamphere L (eds). 1974. Woman, culture, and society. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 33.Alvard MS, Nolin DA. 2002. Rousseau's whale hunt? Coordination among big-game hunters. Curr. Anthropol. 43, 533-559. ( 10.1086/341653) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Glowacki L, von Rueden C. 2015. Leadership solves collective action problems in small-scale societies. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 370, 20150010. ( 10.1098/rstb.2015.0010) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Hooper PL, Kaplan HS, Boone JL. 2010. A theory of leadership in human cooperative groups. J. Theor. Biol. 265, 633-646. ( 10.1016/j.jtbi.2010.05.034) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Pisor AC, Surbeck M. 2019. The evolution of intergroup tolerance in nonhuman primates and humans. Evol. Anthropol.: Issues News Rev. 28, 210-223. ( 10.1002/evan.21793) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Gurven M. 2004. Reciprocal altruism and food sharing decisions among Hiwi and Ache hunter–gatherers. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 56, 366-380. ( 10.1007/s00265-004-0793-6) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Kaplan H, Gurven M, Hill K, Hurtado AM. 2005. The natural history of human food sharing and cooperation: a review and a new multi-individual approach to the negotiation of norms. Moral Sentim. Mater. Interests: Found. Coop. Econ. Life 6, 75-113. [Google Scholar]
- 39.Glowacki L, Isakov A, Wrangham RW, McDermott R, Fowler JH, Christakis NA. 2016. Formation of raiding parties for intergroup violence is mediated by social network structure. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 113, 12 114-12 119. ( 10.1073/pnas.1610961113) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Glowacki L, Wilson ML, Wrangham RW. 2020. The evolutionary anthropology of war. J. Econ. Behav. Org. 178, 963-982. ( 10.1016/j.jebo.2017.09.014) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 41.McDonald MM, Navarrete CD, Van Vugt M. 2012. Evolution and the psychology of intergroup conflict: the male warrior hypothesis. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 367, 670-679. ( 10.1098/rstb.2011.0301) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Wrangham RW, Benenson J. 2017. Cooperative and Competitive Relationships within Sexes. In Chimpanzees and human evolution (eds Muller MN, Wrangham RW, Pilbeam DR), pp. 509-547. Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 43.Bailey DH, Winegard B, Oxford J, Geary DC. 2012. Sex differences in in-group cooperation vary dynamically with competitive conditions and outcomes. Evol. Psychol. 10, 147470491201000130. ( 10.1177/147470491201000112) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Benenson JF. 2019. Sex differences in human peer relationships: a primate's-eye view. Curr. Direct. Psychol. Sci. 28, 124-130. ( 10.1177/0963721418812768) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 45.David-Barrett T, Rotkirch A, Carney J, Izquierdo IB, Krems JA, Townley D, McDaniell E, Byrne-Smith A, Dunbar RIM. 2015. Women favour dyadic relationships, but men prefer clubs: cross-cultural evidence from social networking. PLoS ONE 10, e0118329. ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0118329) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Foley RA, Lee PC. 1989. Finite social space, evolutionary pathways, and reconstructing hominid behavior. Science 243, 901-906. ( 10.1126/science.2493158) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Geary DC, Byrd-Craven J, Hoard MK, Vigil J, Numtee C. 2003. Evolution and development of boys' social behavior. Dev. Rev. 23, 444-470. ( 10.1016/j.dr.2003.08.001) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Redhead D, Rueden CRv. 2021. Coalitions and conflict: a longitudinal analysis of men's politics. Evol. Hum. Sci. 3, e31. ( 10.1017/ehs.2021.26) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.von Rueden C, Gurven M, Kaplan H. 2008. The multiple dimensions of male social status in an Amazonian society. Evol. Hum. Behav. 29, 402-415. ( 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2008.05.001) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.von Rueden C, Alami S, Kaplan H, Gurven M. 2018. Sex differences in political leadership in an egalitarian society. Evol. Hum. Behav. 39, 402-411. ( 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2018.03.005) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.von Rueden CR, Redhead D, O'Gorman R, Kaplan H, Gurven M. 2019. The dynamics of men's cooperation and social status in a small-scale society. Proc. R. Soc. B 286, 20191367. ( 10.1098/rspb.2019.1367) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Reynolds TA. 2021. Our grandmothers' legacy: challenges faced by female ancestors leave traces in modern women's same-sex relationships. Arch. Sex. Behav. 51, 3225-3256. ( 10.1007/s10508-020-01768-x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Vigil JM. 2007. Asymmetries in the friendship preferences and social styles of men and women. Hum. Nat. 18, 143-161. ( 10.1007/s12110-007-9003-3) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Rucas SL, Alami S. 2021. Female–female competition occurs irrespective of patrilocality. Arch. Sex. Behav. ( 10.1007/s10508-021-02221-3) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Baumeister RF, Sommer KL. 1997. What do men want? Gender differences and two spheres of belongingness: comment on Cross and Madson (1997). Psychol. Bull. 122, 38-44. ( 10.1037/0033-2909.122.1.38) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Benenson JF. 2014. Warriors and worriers: the survival of the sexes. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 57.Balliet D, Li NP, Macfarlan SJ, Van Vugt M. 2011. Sex differences in cooperation: a meta-analytic review of social dilemmas. Psychol. Bull. 137, 881-909. ( 10.1037/a0025354) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Benenson JF. 1990. Gender differences in social networks. J. Early Adolesc. 10, 472-495. ( 10.1177/0272431690104004) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Benenson JF, Christakos A. 2003. The greater fragility of females' versus males’ closest same-sex friendships. Child Dev. 74, 1123-1129. ( 10.1111/1467-8624.00596) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Benenson JF, Heath A. 2006. Boys withdraw more in one-on-one interactions, whereas girls withdraw more in groups. Dev. Psychol. 42, 272-282. ( 10.1037/0012-1649.42.2.272) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Bhattacharya K, Ghosh A, Monsivais D, Dunbar RIM, Kaski K. 2016. Sex differences in social focus across the life cycle in humans. R. Soc. Open Sci. 3, 160097. ( 10.1098/rsos.160097) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Hall JA. 2011. Sex differences in friendship expectations: a meta-analysis. J. Soc. Pers. Relatsh. 28, 723-747. ( 10.1177/0265407510386192) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 63.MacEvoy JP, Asher SR. 2012. When friends disappoint: boys' and girls’ responses to transgressions of friendship expectations. Child Dev. 83, 104-119. ( 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01685.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Rose AJ, Rudolph KD. 2006. A review of sex differences in peer relationship processes: potential trade-offs for the emotional and behavioral development of girls and boys. Psychol. Bull. 132, 98-131. ( 10.1037/0033-2909.132.1.98) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Roy C, Bhattacharya K, Dunbar RIM, Kaski K. 2022. Turnover in close friendships. Sci. Rep. 12, 11018. ( 10.1038/s41598-022-15070-4) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Williams KEG, Krems JA, Ayers JD, Rankin AM. 2022. Sex differences in friendship preferences. Evol. Hum. Behav. 43, 44-52. ( 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2021.09.003) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Low BS. 2005. Women's lives there, here, then, now: a review of women's ecological and demographic constraints cross-culturally. Evol. Hum. Behav. 26, 64-87. ( 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2004.08.011) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Manson JH, Wrangham RW. 1991. Intergroup aggression in chimpanzees and humans. Curr. Anthropol. 32, 369-390. ( 10.1086/203974) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Nickels N, Kubicki K, Maestripieri D. 2017. Sex differences in the effects of psychosocial stress on cooperative and prosocial behavior: evidence for ‘flight or fight’ in males and ‘tend and befriend’ in females. Adapt. Hum. Behav. Physiol. 3, 171-183. ( 10.1007/s40750-017-0062-3) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Taylor SE. 2006. Tend and Befriend: biobehavioral bases of affiliation under stress. Curr. Direct. Psychol. Sci. 15, 273-277. ( 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2006.00451.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Taylor SE, Klein LC, Lewis BP, Gruenewald TL, Gurung RAR, Updegraff JA. 2000. Biobehavioral responses to stress in females: tend-and-befriend, not fight-or-flight. Psychol. Rev. 107, 411-429. ( 10.1037/0033-295X.107.3.411) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Turton S, Campbell C. 2005. Tend and befriend versus fight or flight: gender differences in behavioral response to stress among university students. J. Appl. Biobehav. Res. 10, 209-232. ( 10.1111/j.1751-9861.2005.tb00013.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Bedrov A, Gable SL. 2022. Thriving together: the benefits of women's social ties for physical, psychological and relationship health. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 378, 20210441. ( 10.1098/rstb.2021.0441) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Apicella C, Norenzayan A, Henrich J. 2020. Beyond WEIRD: a review of the last decade and a look ahead to the global laboratory of the future. Evol. Hum. Behav. 41, 319-329. ( 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2020.07.015) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Henrich J, Heine SJ, Norenzayan A. 2010. The weirdest people in the world? Behav. Brain Sci. 33, 61-83. ( 10.1017/S0140525X0999152X) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Nielsen M, Haun D, Kärtner J, Legare CH. 2017. The persistent sampling bias in developmental psychology: a call to action. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 162, 31-38. ( 10.1016/j.jecp.2017.04.017) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Apicella CL, Marlowe FW, Fowler JH, Christakis NA. 2012. Social networks and cooperation in hunter-gatherers. Nature 481, 497-501. ( 10.1038/nature10736) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Dyble M, Salali GD, Chaudhary N, Page A, Smith D, Thompson J, Vinicius L, Mace R, Migliano AB. 2015. Sex equality can explain the unique social structure of hunter-gatherer bands. Science 348, 796-798. ( 10.1126/science.aaa5139) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Dyble M, Migliano AB, Page AE, Smith D. 2021. Relatedness within and between Agta residential groups. Evol. Hum. Sci. 3, e49. ( 10.1017/ehs.2021.46) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Mattison SM, et al. 2021. Gender differences in social networks based on prevailing kinship norms in the Mosuo of China. Soc. Sci. 10, 253. ( 10.3390/socsci10070253) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Mattison SM, MacLaren NG, Sum C-Y, Shenk MK, Blumenfield T, Wander K. 2022. Does gender structure social networks across domains of cooperation? An exploration of gendered networks among matrilineal and patrilineal Mosuo. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 378, 20210436. ( 10.1098/rstb.2021.0436) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Simpson CR, Power EA.. 2022. Dynamics of cooperative networks associated with gender among South Indian Tamils. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 378, 20210437. ( 10.1098/rstb.2021.0437) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.House B, Silk JB, McAuliffe K. 2022. No strong evidence for universal gender differences in the development of cooperative behaviour across societies. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 378, 20210439. ( 10.1098/rstb.2021.0439) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Spadaro G, Jin S, Balliet D. 2022. Gender differences in cooperation across 20 societies: a meta-analysis. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 378, 20210438. ( 10.1098/rstb.2021.0438) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Hruschka DJ. 2010. Friendship: development, ecology, and evolution of a relationship. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press. [Google Scholar]
- 86.Hawkes K, O'Connell JF, Blurton Jones NG. 1997. Hadza women's time allocation, offspring provisioning, and the evolution of long postmenopausal life spans. Curr. Anthropol. 38, 551-577. ( 10.1086/204646) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Sear R, Mace R. 2008. Who keeps children alive? A review of the effects of kin on child survival. Evol. Hum. Behav. 29, 1-18. ( 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2007.10.001) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Smith JE. 2014. Hamilton's legacy: kinship, cooperation and social tolerance in mammalian groups. Anim. Behav. 92, 291-304. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.02.029) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Smith JE, Van Horn RC, Powning KS, Cole AR, Graham KE, Memenis SK, Holekamp KE. 2010. Evolutionary forces favoring intragroup coalitions among spotted hyenas and other animals. Behav. Ecol. 21, 284-303. ( 10.1093/beheco/arp181) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Koster J, et al. 2019. Kinship ties across the lifespan in human communities. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 374, 20180069. ( 10.1098/rstb.2018.0069) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Kramer KL, Greaves RD. 2011. Postmarital residence and bilateral kin associations among hunter-gatherers. Hum. Nat. 22, 41. ( 10.1007/s12110-011-9115-7) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Marlowe FW. 2004. Marital residence among foragers. Curr. Anthropol. 45, 277-284. ( 10.1086/382256) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Scelza B, Bliege Bird R. 2008. Group structure and female cooperative networks in Australia's Western Desert. Hum. Nat. 19, 231-248. ( 10.1007/s12110-008-9041-5) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Perry G. 2017. Going home. Hum. Nat. 28, 219-230. ( 10.1007/s12110-016-9282-7) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Power EA, Ready E. 2019. Cooperation beyond consanguinity: post-marital residence, delineations of kin and social support among South Indian Tamils. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 374, 20180070. ( 10.1098/rstb.2018.0070) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Scelza BA. 2011. Female mobility and postmarital kin access in a patrilocal society. Hum. Nat. 22, 377-393. ( 10.1007/s12110-011-9125-5) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Hruschka DJ, Munira S, Jesmin K. 2022. Starting from scratch in a patrilocal society: how women build networks after marriage in rural Bangladesh. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 378, 20210432. ( 10.1098/rstb.2021.0432) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Seabright E, et al. 2022. Repercussions of patrilocal residence on mothers' social support networks among Tsimane forager–farmers. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 378, 20210442. ( 10.1098/rstb.2021.0442) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Watts DP. 2012. The apes: taxonomy, biogeography, life histories, and behavioral ecology. In Evolution of primate societies (eds Mitani John, Call Josep, Kappeler Peter, Silk Joan), pp. 113-142. Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
- 100.Furuichi T. 2011. Female contributions to the peaceful nature of bonobo society. Evol. Anthropol. 20, 131-142. ( 10.1002/evan.20308) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Hohmann G, Fruth B. 2002. Dynamics in social organization of bonobos (Pan paniscus). In Behavioural diversity in chimpanzees and bonobos (eds Boesch C, Hohmann G, Marchant LF), pp. 138-150. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 102.Parish AR. 1996. Female relationships in bonobos (Pan paniscus). Hum. Nat. 7, 61-96. ( 10.1007/BF02733490) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Tokuyama N, Furuichi T. 2016. Do friends help each other? Patterns of female coalition formation in wild bonobos at Wamba. Anim. Behav. 119, 27-35. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2016.06.021) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Fox SA, Muller MN, González NT, Enigk DK, Machanda ZP, Otali E, Wrangham R, Thompson ME. 2022. Weak, but not strong, ties support coalition formation among wild female chimpanzees. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 378, 20210427. ( 10.1098/rstb.2021.0427) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Young C, Robbins MM.. 2022. Association patterns in female gorillas. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 378, 20210429. ( 10.1098/rstb.2021.0429) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Morrogh-Bernard HC, Morf NV, Chivers DJ, Krützen M. 2011. Dispersal patterns of orang-utans (Pongo spp.) in a Bornean peat-swamp forest. Int. J. Primatol. 32, 362-376. ( 10.1007/s10764-010-9474-7) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Nater A, et al. 2011. Sex-biased dispersal and volcanic activities shaped phylogeographic patterns of extant orangutans (genus: Pongo). Mol. Biol. Evol. 28, 2275-2288. ( 10.1093/molbev/msr042) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Nietlisbach P, Arora N, Nater A, Goossens B, Van Schaik CP, Krützen M. 2012. Heavily male-biased long-distance dispersal of orang-utans (genus: Pongo), as revealed by Y-chromosomal and mitochondrial genetic markers. Mol. Ecol. 21, 3173-3186. ( 10.1111/j.1365-294X.2012.05539.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Singleton I, van Schaik CP. 2002. The social organisation of a population of Sumatran orang-utans. Folia Primatol. 73, 1-20. ( 10.1159/000060415) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110. –. [Google Scholar]
- 111.Smith JE, Jaeggi AV, Holmes RK, Silk JB. 2022. Sex differences in cooperative coalitions: a mammalian perspective. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 378, 20210426. ( 10.1098/rstb.2021.0426) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112.Kramer KL. 2022. Female cooperation: evolutionary, cross-cultural and ethnographic evidence. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 378, 20210425. ( 10.1098/rstb.2021.0425) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113.Cassar A, Rigdon M. 2022. Sustaining the potential for cooperation as female competitive strategy. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 378, 20210440. ( 10.1098/rstb.2021.0440) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114.Page AE, Migliano AB, Dyble M, Major-Smith D, Viguier S, Hassan A. 2022. Sedentarization and maternal childcare networks: role of risk, gender and demography. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 378, 20210435. ( 10.1098/rstb.2021.0435) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115.Kraft TS, et al. 2022. Female cooperative labour networks in hunter–gatherers and horticulturalists. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 378, 20210431. ( 10.1098/rstb.2021.0431) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116.Bissonnette A, Perry S, Barrett L, Mitani JC, Flinn M, Gavrilets S, Waal FBMd. 2015. Coalitions in theory and reality: a review of pertinent variables and processes. Behaviour 152, 1-56. ( 10.1163/1568539X-00003241) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 117.Harcourt AH, de Waal FBM (eds) 1992. Coalitions and alliances in humans and other animals. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 118.Campbell A. 2013. A mind of her own: the evolutionary psychology of women. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 119.Hess NH, Hagen EH. 2021. Competitive gossip: the impact of domain, resource value, resource scarcity and coalitions. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 376, 20200305. ( 10.1098/rstb.2020.0305) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120.Rucas SL, Gurven M, Winking J, Kaplan H. 2012. Social aggression and resource conflict across the female life-course in the Bolivian Amazon. Aggress. Behav. 38, 194-207. ( 10.1002/ab.21420) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 121.Hackman J, Kramer KL. 2022. Kin networks and opportunities for reproductive cooperation and conflict among hunter–gatherers. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 378, 20210434. ( 10.1098/rstb.2021.0434) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122.Starkweather KE, Reynolds AZ, Zohora F, Alam N. 2022. Shodagor women cooperate across domains of work and childcare to solve an adaptive problem. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 378, 20210433. ( 10.1098/rstb.2021.0433) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
This article has no additional data.