Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2023 Oct 7.
Published in final edited form as: Soc Neurosci. 2022 Oct 7;17(5):414–427. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2022.2132285

Predictive Models for Social Functioning in Healthy Young Adults: A Machine Learning Study Integrating Neuroanatomical, Cognitive, and Behavioral Data

Kathleen Miley 1, Martin Michalowski 1, Fang Yu 2, Ethan Leng 3, Barbara J McMorris 1, Sophia Vinogradov 4
PMCID: PMC9707316  NIHMSID: NIHMS1840274  PMID: 36196662

Abstract

Poor social functioning is an emerging public health problem associated with physical and mental health consequences. Developing prognostic tools is critical to identify individuals at risk for poor social functioning and guide interventions. We aimed to inform prediction models of social functioning by evaluating models relying on bio-behavioral data using machine learning. With data from the Human Connectome Project Healthy Young Adult sample (age 22–35, N=1,101), we built Support Vector Regression models to estimate social functioning from variable sets of brain morphology to behavior with increasing complexity: 1) brain-only model, 2) brain-cognition model, 3) cognition-behavioral model, and 4) combined brain-cognition-behavioral model. Predictive accuracy of each model was assessed and the importance of individual variables for model performance was determined. The combined and cognition-behavioral models significantly predicted social functioning, whereas the brain-only and brain-cognition models did not. Negative affect, psychological wellbeing, extraversion, withdrawal, and cortical thickness of the rostral middle-frontal and superior-temporal regions were the most important predictors in the combined model. Results demonstrate that social functioning can be accurately predicted using machine learning methods. Behavioral markers may be more significant predictors of social functioning than brain measures for healthy young adults and may represent important leverage points for preventive intervention.

Keywords: Human Connectome Project, Functional outcome, Artificial intelligence, Multivariate pattern analysis, Support vector regression, Neuroimaging biomakers

Introduction

Poor social functioning, characterized by an individual’s lack of social support and the inability to sustain the interpersonal relationships required for meaningful engagement in society, is an emerging public health problem. It is associated with a range of negative physical and mental health consequences,1 such as early mortality2,3 and increased risk for cardiovascular,4 mood,57 anxiety,8 neurocognitive,9,10 and psychotic disorders.11,12 Conversely, individuals with adaptive social functioning have demonstrated better overall well-being13,14 and improved self-management of chronic conditions.15,16 Impairments in social functioning are also a hallmark of many psychiatric illnesses and could represent a transdiagnostic risk marker for mental health disorders.17,18

Due to the broad negative consequences of poor social functioning, developing prognostic tools is critical to identify individuals at risk for social decline. Research on the development of such tools is in an early stage, in part because social functioning is a highly complex phenomenon with multiple contributors ranging from neurobiology to behavior (e.g. see citations1921). Brain imaging studies have linked structural and functional brain abnormalities to social functioning outcomes in both clinical and non-clinical samples. For example, in individuals at high risk for psychosis, less gray matter density in frontal and limbic areas has been associated with greater functional decline regardless of their transition to psychosis, supporting a transdiagnostic biomarker for social functioning deficits.18 These trends persist in chronic schizophrenia22 and are partially replicated in bipolar disorder with associations between gray matter volume reductions in the superior and medial prefrontal cortex and social functioning.23 In non-clinical samples, loneliness and social isolation have been consistently associated with structural brain measures in the prefrontal cortex, insula, hippocampus, amygdala and posterior superior temporal cortex,24 and to altered functional connectivity in distributed brain networks, including reward, fronto-limbic, and default mode networks.2428

Social functioning is also dependent on the cognitive and psychological processes necessary for adaptive social behavior. A range of cognitive and social cognitive impairments are linked to poor social functioning in schizophrenia,2931 bipolar disorder32 and depression.33 In non-clinical samples, social cognition predicts overall social functioning34 and supports varied social abilities such as self-regulation in conflict and empathetic engagement with others.35,36 Additionally, a number of psychological traits such as depression, anxiety, neuroticism, negative affect, externalizing behaviors, and low psychological well-being have been linked to poor social functioning.3742

Machine learning methods may hold promise for achieving progress in improving social functioning in clinical and non-clinical populations through development of prognostic tools that could eventually be employed to identify individuals with high risk for social functioning deficits.43,44 Machine learning methods allow for integrating large numbers of heterogeneous predictors across data modalities (e.g., neuroimaging, cognitive, psychological, and clinical data), avoiding the need to select a small number of predictors and to specify their relationships a priori, allowing for potential identification of pertinent interactions between variables and domains.44,45 This can be beneficial for data domains that are inherently large, such as neuroimaging modalities, and can result in novel insights about the condition being studied, especially when associations between variables are not fully known (as is often in the case with brain-behavior relationships).45,46 Given the numerous predictors of social functioning reviewed above, machine learning approaches may be well situated to parse the large, multi-modal predictor space to identify a broad range of predictors that could be leveraged in a predictive model and could be candidate treatment targets.45 Additionally, in some cases, machine learning methods may facilitate predictions at the single-subject level due to the ability of some algorithms to uncover complex and potentially more flexible non-linear relationships amongst the data and the use of methods that promote generalization of the model to unseen data, such as robust cross-validation and model evaluation on independent datasets.43,47

The use of machine learning to predict social functioning has been increasingly explored in clinical populations, most notably psychotic disorders, where initial studies have investigated prediction of functional outcomes from clinical data,4853 with smaller proof of concept studies relying on structural brain imaging data.17,54 Early evidence in clinical high risk for psychosis and recent onset depression samples suggests that combining clinical and brain imaging measures may optimize model performance relative to models relying on only one predictor domain.17 However, most studies utilizing machine learning to predict social functioning outcomes have been limited by small sample sizes and models that consider the predictor modalities in isolation.43,53 Further work is needed to understand how the varied predictors of functional outcomes can best be utilized to optimize accuracy and clinical usefulness of models and prognostic tools.

In the current study, we present initial findings to inform the development of prospective prognostic tools predicting social functioning using machine learning. We leveraged the Human Connectome Project Healthy Young Adult (HCP-YA) dataset - which includes multiple domains of cognitive, psychological, and other behavioral measures as well as high-quality neuroimaging data for a sample of over 1000 participants. Machine learning methods were used to evaluate prognostic models for individual social functioning scores, in order to delineate the relative performance of models built from neurobiological, cognitive, psychological and other behavioral domains. Secondly, we investigated which are the most influential predictors to identify potential risk markers and candidate treatment targets for social functioning.

Methods

Data Acquisition

This study utilized data from the HCP-YA sample. Access to the de-identified HCP-YA restricted access dataset was granted from the HCP consortium on June 19, 2019. The de-identified dataset was obtained from the secure HCP server. The University of Minnesota IRB deemed this research exempt and not human subjects research.

Participants

The HCP-YA study enrolled 1,206 participants aged 22–35, including twin and non-twin siblings from 2012 to 2015. Recruitment and study procedures took place at Washington University in St. Louis, Missouri and at the University of Minnesota in Minneapolis, Minnesota. A detailed report of the recruitment procedures has been documented elsewhere.55 Inclusion criteria for HCP participants included age 22–35 and the ability to provide valid informed consent. Exclusion criteria included: history of psychiatric disorder, substance use disorder, neurological, endocrine or cardiovascular disease, genetic disorder, head injury, premature birth, history of chemotherapy or immunomodulatory agents, a score of ≤25 on the Folstein Mini-Mental State Exam,56 claustrophobia, pregnancy or metal in the body. Participants were excluded from the current study if they did not have structural MRI data available or if they were missing greater than 50% of the cognitive and behavioral data.

Variables

Outcome measure

Social functioning.

We derived the social functioning outcome variable from the Social Relationships scales included in the NIH Emotion Toolbox (NIH-ETB).57,58 An expert panel commissioned by the NIH Neuroscience Blueprint59 led the creation of the NIH-ETB, a set of self-report measures to assess positive and negative aspects of emotional functioning. Based on item response theory, the NIH-ETB uses computer adaptive testing with extensive item banks and has undergone norming and validation. The scales representing the Social Relationships domain have demonstrated strong convergent validity with objective measures of interpersonal support, loneliness, and negative social interactions.57

The social functioning composite used in this study was calculated using the formula provided by Babakhanyan et al.,60 which includes scores for dimensions of friendship, loneliness, emotional support, instrumental support and perceived rejection. As a computer adaptive measure, the specific items an individual receives for these domains are individualized based on responses to previous items. Each item is assessed on a five-point Likert scale with options ranging from “never” to “always.” The friendship domain assesses perceived engagement with friends and quality of friendships (i.e., “I feel like I am part of a group of friends,” “I feel like I have lots of friends,” and “I get invited to go out and do things with other people”). The loneliness domain assesses perceived social inclusion and feelings of being alone (i.e., “I feel alone and apart from others” and “I feel left out”). The emotional support domain assesses perceived access to support for personal problems and emotions (i.e., “I have someone I trust to talk with about my problems,” and “I feel there are people I can talk to if I am upset”). The instrumental support domain assesses the perceived support network for completing tasks when needed, such as shopping, transportation, and accessing medical care. The perceived rejection domain assesses the frequency of negative social interactions (i.e., how often others “don’t listen when I ask for help,” and “act like they don’t care about me”). A comprehensive set of potential items for each domain is detailed elsewhere.58 The summary score formula includes positive and negative factor loadings to reflect the positive (i.e., friendship) and negative (i.e., loneliness) aspects of social functioning.60 The individual scales that comprise the composite measure in the HCP-YA dataset are provided as T-scores (mean of 50, standard deviation of 10). In a healthy sample, approximately 16% of individuals are expected to score within the potentially problematic range, defined as one standard deviation below the mean, on the individual scales and composite meausure.60 Consitently, the distribution of social functioning scores in our sample follows a normal distribution with approximately 15% in the potentially problematic range.

Predictor variables by domain

Predictor variables are briefly described below; more detailed information for each variable collected by the HCP-YA study can be found online.61

Neurobiological predictors.

Cortical thickness and surface area for 33 cortical and 8 subcortical regions per hemisphere were obtained from structural MRI. All participants underwent MRI on 3T Siemens scanners using 32 channel head coils. A uniform MRI protocol and processing pipeline using FreeSurfer 5.1 was implemented between sites to control for variability in scanning procedures. Scanning protocols are detailed elsewhere,55 and data quality has been ensured by extensive quality assurance procedures.55 The automated segmentation of T1 and T2 weighted brain scans, which includes the standard parcellation maps using the Conte69 brain atlas was used. Surface areas were corrected for total intracranial volume.

Cognitive predictors.
Social Cognition.

Our primary measure of social cognition was the Penn Emotion Recognition Task,62 a measure of emotion processing which assesses the ability to discriminate emotions on human faces. We included both the correct responses and reaction time as separate variables. In addition, we included two in-scanner tasks: the HCP Theory of Mind task and the HCP Emotion Processing task.63

Cognition.

Cognitive variables included global cognition as measured by the Mini-Mental State Exam56 total score and discrete cognitive domains of attention, episodic memory, working memory, language, executive function and processing speed measured by the NIH-Toolbox Cognition Battery and scored as age-adjusted T-scores.

Behavioral predictors.

Psychological variables included the five domain scores from the Costa and McRae Neuroticism/Extroversion/Openness Five Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI)64 and the age-adjusted T scores from the Achenbach Adult Self Report (ASR) Syndrome Scales65 which includes subscales of anxious/depressed, withdrawn, somatic complaints, thought problems, attention problems, aggression, and rule breaking problems. Negative affect and psychological wellbeing were measured using the composite scores from these factors from the NIH-ETB scales.60 Reward processing and self-regulation were measured by the Delay Discounting task.66

Substance use variables included self-report measures of total number of alcoholic drinks in the last seven days, typical number of drinks per drinking day in the last 12 months, total times of use of any tobacco products in the last seven days, and total lifetime amount of cannabis use.

Physical functioning variables included the NIH Toolbox endurance, gait speed, dexterity and strength assessments,67 the Pittsburg Sleep Quality Questionnaire68 total score and Body Mass Index (BMI).

Sensory functioning was operationalized by four measures from the NIH Toolbox that assess sensory and neurological functioning: Words-in-Noise test (measure of audition), Odor Identification test, Regional Taste Intensity test, and the Pain Interference Survey. Vision measures included the Mars Contrast Sensitivity test.69

Statistical Analysis

Missing data

Individual participants were excluded if they had incomplete neuroimaging data or were missing 50% or greater of the cognitive or behavioral data. The remaining missing values were imputed using K-Nearest Neighbors (k = 5). As an inclusion criterion for this study was complete structural MRI data, imputation only applied to cognitive and behavioral variables. For the training dataset, imputation was performed separately in each cross-validation fold to prevent information leakage that could lead to overfitting.

Multicollinearity evaluation

We evaluated multicollinearity (Pearson’s r > 0.70) by examining a correlation matrix of all variables. Two variables were highly correlated with NEO-FFI neuroticism: Negative affect (r = 0.72) and the ASR anxiety/depression scale (r = 0.71). Multicollinearity was primarily addressed by using the Recursive Feature Elimination step during model training. Secondarily, multicollinearity was addressed by estimating models with and without NEO-FFI neuroticism. No significant differences in model performance were found, and thus this variable was included in the final model.

Descriptive statistics

Descriptive statistics were calculated for social functioning, cognition, behavioral variables, and demographics, with means (SD) for continuous variables and percentages for categorical variables. All descriptive analyses were calculated in SPSS version 26.70

Machine Learning Analysis

Model training and cross validation.

We used a linear support vector regression (SVR)71 to estimate individual participants’ social functioning score from brain morphological, cognitive, and/or behavioral data. We chose a regression, rather than a classification, approach because regression algorithms avoid the need to set arbitrary thresholds between groups (i.e., creating a social functioning cut-off score for good vs poor functioning), which can lead to loss of information and introduce bias into the model. Due to the NIH-ETB’s intended use in both healthy and clinical populations, it has been suggested that use of cut-points may be too restrictive and could result in overclassifying healthy individuals as having non-problematic functioning.57 Social functioning is a complex phenomenon that occurs on a continuum; in the absence of gold-standard thresholds or cut-points for the NIH-ETB, a regression approach allows for predictions to be made on a continuous scale (i.e., predicting where an individual falls on the continuum of social functioning), which can increase model accuracy. 47,72,73 The SVR is an established approach for uncovering non-linearities in the dataset that would not be uncovered by general linear models and is a robust approach for multi-modal data and large feature sets.

We trained four separate linear SVR models predicting social functioning composite scores: 1) a brain-only model, trained on structural MRI measures only; 2) a brain-cognition model, trained on brain and cognition measures only; 3) a cognition-behavioral model, trained on the cognition and behavioral measures only; and 4) a combined model, trained on the brain, cognition, and behavioral measures. The subsets of variables chosen for our models build in levels of biological to behavioral complexity, allowing for examination of which domains are necessary or sufficient for the prediction of social functioning. Additionally, as cognitive and behavioral measures are less invasive and expensive to obtain relative to brain measures, we tested these models separately because they are more practical for use in most clinical contexts. The machine learning analysis was implemented using the Scikit-learn Python toolbox74 which uses the LIBLINEAR library of functions.71

The full dataset was randomly split into a training and testing dataset, with 10% of participants assigned to the testing dataset (N = 111). In the model training set, a nested five-fold cross validation (CV) procedure was used. To address potential bias due to heritability, samples were split into CV folds such that all related individuals are in the same fold, preventing leakage of information across folds. Prior to training, the data were standardized to have a mean of zero and standard deviation of one. We used Recursive Feature Elimination with the ridge regression estimator as a feature selection step to reduce the feature space. This step was embedded within the CV to determine the optimal number of variables to select for each model. The frequency that a specific variable was selected in the five CV folds was calculated.

The optimal values for the hyperparameters for the linear SVR, including the C-value and epsilon, were determined during the CV procedure using grid search. The epsilon insensitive, or L1, loss function was used. The parameter “dual” was set to false, which is the default when the number of samples is greater than the number of features.

Model testing and evaluation.

Using the hyperparameters learned during the testing and cross validation phase for each model, the learned SVR algorithm was applied to the held-out test dataset to evaluate model performance on unseen data. Model performance was measured based on the coefficient of determination, R2, and the mean squared error (MSE) of the learned model. The R2 value represents the proportion of variance of the outcome variable that can be explained by the independent variables in the model and is a measure of goodness of fit for the model. A model which always predicts the correct score would have an R2 score of 1, and a constant model which always predicts the mean value of the outcome variable would have an R2 score of zero. Thus, the R2 score can be negative if it performs worse than a constant prediction of the mean. The MSE represents the mean of the squared differences between the actual outcome value and the model-predicted value (lower scores indicate better model performance).

We calculated 95% confidence intervals for the R2 and MSE metrics using a bootstrapping procedure with 1000 resamples. Significance was evaluated using the 95% confidence interval of each model’s R2 value. Performances of the four models were compared by calculating the 95% confidence interval for the differences in the bootstrapped R2 results of two models at a time. P-values (two tailed, alpha = 0.05) were calculated by inverting the confidence interval.

Exploratory analysis of mis-predicted cases

To identify demographic groups or individual profiles for whom the model may not generalize well, we examined trends in the performance of the combined model by examining individual cases in the test dataset. The methods and results of this analysis are detailed in the Supplemental Methods and Results file.

Results

Sample

One hundred and five participants were excluded for missing MRI data or having greater than 50% missing behavioral data. The remaining 1,101 participants met eligibility criteria and were included in the analysis and split into the training (N = 990) and testing (N = 111) datasets. Demographics and social functioning score descriptive statistics for the full analysis sample are provided in Table 1. The social functioning composite score ranged from −13.0 to 15.2 with a mean of 5.6 and standard deviation of 5.0. Descriptive statistics for the individual scales that comprise the social functioning composite score, including the proportion of individuals fall into the potentially problematic range,57 are provided in Supplementary Table 1. Descriptive statistics for all cognitive and behavioral variables are provided in Supplementary Table 2.

Table 1.

Demographics and Outcome Measure Descriptive Statistics (N = 1,101)

Mean (SD) or %
Demographics
Age (years) 28.8 (3.7)
Gender: female 54.4
Education (years) 14.9 (1.8)
Race
 American Indian or Alaskan Native 0.2
 Asian, Native Hawaiian or Other Pacific Islander 5.7
 Black or African American 14.9
 White 74.9
 More than one 2.5
 Unknown or not reported 1.7
Ethnicity: Hispanic or Latino 8.6
Outcome Measure
NIH Emotion Toolbox Social Functioning Composite Score 5.6 (5.0)

Note: SD = standard deviation

Machine Learning Analyses

Model performance

Model performance statistics, including R2 and MSE, for all models are provided in Table 2. The combined model and the cognition-behavioral model significantly predicted individual social functioning scores (R2 =0.53, 95% CI [0.38, 0.62] for each model). The brain-only model and the brain-cognition model performances were not significant in predicting social functioning scores (R2 = 0.06, 95% CI [−0.07, 0.16] and R2 = 0.11 95% CI [−0.05, 0.23], respectively). These models performed significantly worse than the combined and cognition-behavioral models. No significant differences in model performance were noted between the brain-only and brain-cognition models or between the combined and cognition-behavioral models (Table 2).

Table 2.

Model Performance Summary Statistics and Model Comparison

Model R2 (95% CI) Mean Square Error (MSE) (95% CI)
Brain 0.06 (−0.07, 0.16) 21.36 (16.85, 26.2)
Brain-cognition 0.11 (−0.05, 0.23) 20.16 (15.64, 24.65)
Cognition-behavioral* 0.53 (0.38, 0.62) 10.73 (8.86, 12.73)
Combined brain-cognition-behavioral* 0.53 (0.38, 0.62) 10.68 (8.60, 12.88)
Model Comparison
Model Point Estimate (95% CI)a p-value
Brain vs. Brain-cognition 0.05 (−0.06, 0.16) 0.35
Brain vs. Cognition-behavioral 0.47 (0.32, 0.60) <0.0001
Brain vs. Combined 0.47 (0.33, 0.60) <0.0001
Brain-Cognition vs. Cognition-behavioral 0.41 (0.27, 0.56) <0.0001
Brain-Cognition vs. Combined 0.42 (−0.26, 0.57) <0.0001
Cognition-behavioral vs. Combined −0.05 (−1.30, 1.20) 0.90

Note:

*

Statistically significant model based on 95% confidence interval

a

Point estimate and 95% confidence interval represents differences in the bootstrapped R2 values of the two models compared

Feature selection and importance

The selected features, or predictor variables, from the best performing models (combined and cognition-behavioral) are provided in Table 3, along with their feature coefficient and the frequency with which they were selected by the SVR algorithm during five-fold cross validation to be included in the model. The weight of the feature coefficient (absolute value) indicates the relative importance of the specific variable to the model. Features selected for the model at high frequencies during cross validation can be interpreted as being more reliable predictors relative to those selected infrequently. The highest weighted and most consistent features in the combined model included negative affect, psychological wellbeing, withdrawn symptoms, extraversion, and the cortical thickness in right and left rostral middle frontal gyri and the left superior temporal gyrus. The highest weighted and most consistent features in the cognition-behavioral model included negative affect, psychological wellbeing, withdrawn symptoms, extraversion, agreeableness, and aggression symptoms. A correlation table presenting the relationship between the variables in cognition-behavioral model and the social functioning composite are provided in Supplementary Table 3.

Table 3.

Feature Importance for Combined and Cognition-behavioral Models

Combined brain-cognitive-behavioral model
Feature Feature Coefficient CV Frequency (max = 5)
Negative affect –1.53 5
Psychological well-being 1.37 5
ASR withdrawn symptoms –0.82 5
NEO-FFI extraversion 0.76 5
R rostral middle frontal thickness –0.56 4
L superior temporal thickness 0.49 5
L supramarginal thickness –0.45 1
L rostral middle frontal thickness 0.42 3
L temporal pole area –0.37 1
NEO-FFI agreeableness 0.32 1
R superior parietal area 0.31 1
R inferior parietal thickness 0.25 1
Cognition-behavioral model
Feature Feature Coefficient CV Frequency (max = 5)
Negative Affect –1.71 5
Psychological Well-being 1.33 5
ASR Withdrawn symptoms –0.85 5
NEO-FFI Extraversion 0.76 5
NEO-FFI Agreeableness 0.37 4
ASR Aggression symptoms 0.35 4
Education level 0.19 1

Note: ASR = Achenbach Adult Self Report scale; NEO-FFI = Neuroticism, Extraversion, Openness Five Factor Inventory; L = left; R = right

Discussion

In this study, we leveraged a large dataset of integrated biological, cognitive, and behavioral data and machine learning to predict social functioning in a large sample of healthy young adults. We found that only the combined and cognition-behavioral models significantly predicted social functioning scores, and these models performed equally well. That is, adding neuroanatomical data to the cognition-behavioral models did not enhance model performance. Further, the performance of the cognition-behavioral model was driven only by psychological variables, without significant influence from cognition. Our findings thus suggest that behavioral variables in the psychological domain that capture aspects of negative affect, psychological well-being, and personality traits, may be more robust predictors of social functioning than cognitive or neuroanatomical measures in healthy young adults, and may inform future development of prospective individual prediction models. While combining neuroanatomical measures with cognitive and behavioral measures did not improve model accuracy, potential neuroanatomical regions that may be important to the prediction of social functioning when considered along with behavioral variables were identified in the combined model. Below we highlight key findings in relation to existing literature.

Predictive models of social functioning from integrated bio-behavioral data

The combined brain-cognition-behavioral model significantly predicted social functioning, however it performed at an equal accuracy as the cognition-behavioral model, suggesting no additional benefit of including neuroanatomical measures. These models performed at similar accuracies when compared to existing machine learning research seeking to predict mental health outcomes,7577 and thus the more cost-effective models relying on behavioral data may be useful tools in identifying those at risk of social functioning deficits if applied to longitudinal data.

We found that the brain-only and brain-cognition models performed very poorly in the prediction of social functioning. This was unexpected given 1) previous studies in psychiatric populations that have successfully used support vector machines to predict functional outcomes from neuroanatomical data in first episode psychosis,17 early depression,17 and bipolar disorder 23 and 2) research consistently linking cognitive and social cognitive impairments to social functioning in psychiatric disorders.29,7880 In healthy controls who are expected to have less variance in brain structure, however, these relationships may not be evident without much larger sample sizes to detect differences.81 The limited variance in cortical thickness and gray matter volumes in a healthy population likely contributed to the inability of the machine learning algorithm to discriminately use brain features in prediction. This is supported by Sartori et al,23 who found that neuroanatomical data led to successful prediction of social functioning in bipolar disorder, but not in the healthy control sample. Likewise, recent large scale replicability studies linking personality features and other psychological variables to brain morphometry in healthy adults have failed to detect associations, which has been attributed to limited variance and small effect sizes even in samples greater than 1000 participants.8183 Limited variance and overall higher performance in cognitive functioning (i.e. ceiling effects) in our healthy population likely also contributed to the poor performance of the brain-cognition model.

Salient variables and data modalities for the prediction of social functioning

In the combined and cognition-behavioral models, the most predictive variables included negative affect, psychological well-being, withdrawn symptoms, extraversion and agreeableness personality traits, and aggression symptoms. This is not surprising, as these measures – especially psychological well-being, withdrawn symptoms and extraversion - have some conceptual overlap with perceived social functioning. These results are in line with a large body of work linking emotion, personality features, and psychological well-being to social functioning across healthy and clinical populations.42,84 For example, negative affect is associated with the perceived quality and quantity of social relationships in multiple life stages, from childhood to older age.84,85 Negative affect has also been identified as a strong predictor of social functioning in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder, even after accounting for cognitive deficits and other psychiatric symptoms.86 Social psychology research shows that positive emotions may facilitate better social functioning through fostering and maintaining adaptive relationships and promoting positive evaluation of social interactions, while negative emotions relate to distancing from groups, negative bias in social attributions, and avoidant behavior.87,88 Longitudinal studies taking a developmental perspective have also identified personality traits, particularly extraversion and neuroticism, as primary drivers of social functioning and well-being.84,89 In turn, these personality traits are linked to positive and negative emotional experiences and overall psychological well-being.84

Taken together with previous research, our results suggest that the domains of negative and positive affect, psychological well-being, and personality characteristics may be useful for identifying those at risk for poor functional outcomes and may point to potential intervention targets, which are amenable to psychological therapies,9093 to improve social outcomes and longer-term well-being. Future longitudinal work could investigate whether there are distinct developmental trajectories of social functioning based on individual characteristics of negative affect, wellbeing, and personality traits. Individual characteristics may be associated with distinct trajectories of persistent social functioning deficits as opposed to time limited impairments that are more responsive to intervention, as has been demonstrated in disorders of social functioning such as antisocial behavior.94

Due to the large body of evidence linking cognitive and social cognitive impairments to social functioning in psychiatric disorders,29,7880 it was an unexpected finding that no cognitive variables were selected in our best performing models. While the importance of cognition could have been masked by the more heavily weighted variables in our combined and cognition-behavioral models, the brain-cognition model failed to significantly predict social functioning, which suggests that cognition was not highly predictive of social functioning in this healthy sample of young adults. As discussed above, this may be attributed to limited variance in cognitive functioning in a healthy young adult sample. Additionally, the construct of social functioning used in this study was very broad; the primary social cognition measure of emotion recognition may instead be a better predictor of specific aspects of social functioning.29,95,96

Interestingly, while the brain-only and brain-cognition models failed to significantly predict social functioning, in the combined model cortical thickness of the middle frontal and superior temporal regions were consistently selected as model variables. This could suggest that while neuroanatomical measures may not be necessary (i.e., do not provide additional value over behavioral variables) in the prediction of social functioning in healthy young adults, when considered alongside behavioral variables these regions may be investigated for playing a potential – but seemingly very modest - role in social functioning. These represent important hubs of the social brain and their structure (i.e., relative size) and function has been linked to higher order social cognitive processes such as theory of mind and emotion recognition.20,97100 The structure and function of these brain regions have been consistently linked to social functioning deficits in schizophrenia.22 Additionally, machine learning studies predicting functional outcomes from neuroanatomical data have found these regions to be important for their prediction models in bipolar disorder, clinical high risk, and recent onset depression samples.17,23 Thus, it is possible that structural measures of the middle frontal and temporal regions may serve as a transdiagnostic risk marker for social functioning across the spectrum of health to psychiatric illness. However, due to the poor performance of the brain-only model and the lower feature weights of these regions relative to the behavioral variables in the combined model, these results must be interpreted with caution. Future research is needed to characterize any relationship between these brain measures and social functioning in healthy samples.

Limitations and Future Research

The results of this study must be interpreted in the context of several limitations. Although efforts were made by the HCP research consortium to build a representative sample of participants based on U.S. Census data,55 the majority of young adult participants were non-Hispanic whites, and their average education attainment was relatively high. Therefore, the generalizability of results to diverse racial, cultural, and socioeconomic groups may be limited. Many measures used in this study – including the social functioning outcome score - were self-reports, which may be less reliable than objective measures. However, use of high quality and psychometrically sound self-report measures is becoming increasingly important in health research seeking to promote patient centered outcomes that reflect perceived quality of life.101103 Some of the measures included in our models may not be expected to meaningfully contribute to the prediction of social functioning in a healthy young adult sample, such as the Mini-Mental State Exam. We relied on a data-driven feature selection method to eliminate variables that did not contribute to model performance, in which the machine learning algorithm learns which variables are predictive. Previous research has shown that self-control plays a role in predicting other aspects of longitudinal social functioning (i.e., wealth and criminal activity).104 Our study was limited in not including a measure of self-control. Further, our models did not include sociodemographic measures that could influence social functioning. Although the top performing models had statistically significant performance, social functioning scores were mis-predicted in nearly 20% of the test sample.

This study analyzed cross sectional data in a proof-of-concept manner. Clinically useful models must predict longitudinal outcomes and future research should extend our models to the prediction of social functioning at later timepoints using baseline data. This study is limited by not developing a cutoff score that is clinically meaningful for screening individuals who have or are at risk for poor social functioning. Further work is needed to develop and validate such cut-off scores in healthy and clinical populations. Additionally, due to the relatively small proportion of individuals in this healthy sample with potentially problematic social functioning,57 further replication is needed in samples with higher incidence of poor social functioning, such as may be seen in clinical populations, to confirm whether these models can accurately identify those at risk for clinically meaningful poor social functioning.

Given the enormous burden of social functioning deficits on mental and physical health and the lack of treatment options for persistent social functioning deficits across psychiatric disorders, identifying individuals who are at risk in early adulthood – if not earlier -- and addressing potential treatment targets is an urgent research need. Models predicting longitudinal outcomes that demonstrate superiority over clinician prediction17 are needed to promote clinical utility.

Conclusions

Our findings indicate that machine learning models relying on psychological and behavioral measures – but not neuroanatomical measures – may be useful for predicting social functioning and provide directions for future prospective research. While a renewed focus on understanding and leveraging biological underpinnings of complex behaviors such as social functioning characterizes the state of the science, our results suggest that prognostic models utilizing less costly and more readily available psychological and behavioral data may be optimal. Further, the study of psychological constructs may be more impactful in understanding the potential intervention points for poor social functioning that can be targeted to improve an individual’s long-term well-being.

Supplementary Material

Supplementary Material

Acknowledgements:

Data were provided by the Human Connectome Project, WU-Minn Consortium (Principal Investigators: David Van Essen and Kamil Ugurbil; 1U54MH091657) funded by the 16 NIH Institutes and Centers that support the NIH Blueprint for Neuroscience Research; and by the McDonnell Center for Systems Neuroscience at Washington University.

Funding statement:

This work was supported by National Institute of Mental Health Grants 1F31MH124278 (KM), 1R01MH120589-01 and 5P50MH119569-02, and the National Institutes of Health’s National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences, Grants TL1R002493 and UL1TR002494 (KM, EL). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health.

Footnotes

Conflicts of Interest: Dr. Vinogradov serves on the Scientific Advisory Board for Psyberguide. She also has scientific collaborations with scientists at PositScience, Inc. Ms. Miley and Drs. Michalowski, Yu, Leng, and McMorris have no conflicts of interest to report.

References

  • 1.Holt-Lunstad J, Robles TF, Sbarra DA, Julianne Holt-Lunstad note. Advancing Social Connection as a Public Health Priority in the United States. 2010;72(6):517–530. doi: 10.1037/amp0000103.supp [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Holt-Lunstad J, Smith TB, Baker M, Harris T, Stephenson D. Loneliness and Social Isolation as Risk Factors for Mortality. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2015;10(2):227–237. doi: 10.1177/1745691614568352 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Pantell M, Rehkopf D, Jutte D, Syme SL, Balmes J, Adler N. Social isolation: a predictor of mortality comparable to traditional clinical risk factors. Am J Public Health. 2013;103(11):2056–2062. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2013.301261 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Valtorta NK, Kanaan M, Gilbody S, Ronzi S, Hanratty B. Loneliness and social isolation as risk factors for coronary heart disease and stroke: systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal observational studies. Heart. 2016;102(13):1009–1016. doi: 10.1136/heartjnl-2015-308790 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Santini ZI, Jose PE, York Cornwell E, et al. Social disconnectedness, perceived isolation, and symptoms of depression and anxiety among older Americans (NSHAP): a longitudinal mediation analysis. Lancet Public Heal. 2020;5(1):e62–e70. doi: 10.1016/S2468-2667(19)30230-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Hawkley LC, Capitanio JP. Perceived social isolation, evolutionary fitness and health outcomes: a lifespan approach. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci. 2015;370(1669):20140114. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2014.0114 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Cacioppo JT, Hughes ME, Waite LJ, Hawkley LC, Thisted RA. Loneliness as a specific risk factor for depressive symptoms: Cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses. Psychol Aging. 2006;21(1):140–151. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.21.1.140 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Santini ZI, Fiori KL, Feeney J, Tyrovolas S, Haro JM, Koyanagi A. Social relationships, loneliness, and mental health among older men and women in Ireland: A prospective community-based study. J Affect Disord. 2016;204:59–69. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2016.06.032 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Fratiglioni L, Wang H-X, Ericsson K, Maytan M, Winblad B. Influence of social network on occurrence of dementia: a community-based longitudinal study. Lancet. 2000;355(9212):1315–1319. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(00)02113-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Shankar A, Hamer M, McMunn A, Steptoe A. Social Isolation and Loneliness. Psychosom Med. 2013;75(2):161–170. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31827f09cd [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Green MF, Horan WP, Lee J, McCleery A, Reddy LF, Wynn JK. Social Disconnection in Schizophrenia and the General Community. Schizophr Bull. 2018;44(2):242–249. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbx082 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Fulford D, Niendam TA, Floyd EG, et al. Symptom dimensions and functional impairment in early psychosis: More to the story than just negative symptoms. Schizophr Res. 2013;147(1):125–131. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2013.03.024 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Chu P Sen Saucier DA, Hafner E. Meta-analysis of the relationships between social support and well-being in children and adolescents. J Soc Clin Psychol. 2010;29(6). doi: 10.1521/jscp.2010.29.6.624 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Siedlecki KL, Salthouse TA, Oishi S, Jeswani S. The Relationship Between Social Support and Subjective Well-Being Across Age. Soc Indic Res. 2014;117(2):561–576. doi: 10.1007/s11205-013-0361-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Vassilev I, Rogers A, Sanders C, et al. Social networks, social capital and chronic illness self-management: a realist review. Chronic Illn. 2011;7(1):60–86. doi: 10.1177/1742395310383338 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Gallant MP. The influence of social support on chronic illness self-management: a review and directions for research. Health Educ Behav. 2003;30(2):170–195. doi: 10.1177/1090198102251030 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Koutsouleris N, Kambeitz-Ilankovic L, Ruhrmann S, et al. Prediction Models of Functional Outcomes for Individuals in the Clinical High-Risk State for Psychosis or With Recent-Onset Depression. JAMA Psychiatry. 2018;75(11):1156. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2018.2165 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Reniers RL, Lin A, Yung AR, et al. Neuroanatomical Predictors of Functional Outcome in Individuals at Ultra-High Risk for Psychosis. Schizophr Bull. 2017;43(2):449–458. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbw086 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Cacioppo JT, Cacioppo S, Dulawa S, Palmer AA. Social neuroscience and its potential contribution to psychiatry. World Psychiatry. 2014;13(2):131–139. doi: 10.1002/wps.20118 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Dunbar RIM. The social brain hypothesis and its implications for social evolution. Ann Hum Biol. 2009;36(5):562–572. doi: 10.1080/03014460902960289 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Bierman KL, Welsh JA. Assessing Social Dysfunction: The Contributions of Laboratory and Performance-Based Measures. J Clin Child Psychol. 2000;29(4):526–539. doi: 10.1207/S15374424JCCP2904_6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Wojtalik JA, Smith MJ, Keshavan MS, Eack SM. A Systematic and Meta-analytic Review of Neural Correlates of Functional Outcome in Schizophrenia. 2018;43(6):1329–1347. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbx008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Sartori JM, Reckziegel R, Passos IC, et al. Volumetric brain magnetic resonance imaging predicts functioning in bipolar disorder: A machine learning approach. J Psychiatr Res. 2018;103(April):237–243. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2018.05.023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Lam JA, Murray ER, Yu KE, et al. Neurobiology of loneliness: a systematic review. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2021;46(11):1873–1887. doi: 10.1038/s41386-021-01058-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Feng C, Wang L, Li T, Xu P. Connectome-based individualized prediction of loneliness. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2019;14(4):353–365. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsz020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Cacioppo S, Capitanio JP, Cacioppo JT. Toward a neurology of loneliness. Psychol Bull. 2014;140(6):1464–1504. doi: 10.1037/a0037618 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Cacioppo JT, Norris CJ, Decety J, Monteleone G, Nusbaum H. In the Eye of the Beholder: Individual Differences in Perceived Social Isolation Predict Regional Brain Activation to Social Stimuli. J Cogn Neurosci. 2009;21(1):83–92. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Inagaki TK, Muscatell KA, Moieni M, et al. Yearning for connection? Loneliness is associated with increased ventral striatum activity to close others. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2016;11(7):1096–1101. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsv076 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Fett AKJ, Viechtbauer W, Dominguez M de G, Penn DL, van Os J, Krabbendam L. The relationship between neurocognition and social cognition with functional outcomes in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2011;35(3):573–588. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.07.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Green M, Kern R, Braff D, Mintz J. Neurocognitive Deficits and Functional Outcome in Schizophrenia: Are We Measuring the “Right Stuff”? Schizophr Bull. 2000;26(1):119–136. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a033430 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Halverson TF, Orleans-Pobee M, Merritt C, Sheeran P, Fett A-K, Penn DL. Pathways to functional outcomes in schizophrenia spectrum disorders: Meta-analysis of social cognitive and neurocognitive predictors. 2019. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.07.020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Gitlin MJ, Miklowitz DJ. The difficult lives of individuals with bipolar disorder: A review of functional outcomes and their implications for treatment. J Affect Disord. 2017;209(November 2016):147–154. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2016.11.021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Evans VC, Iverson GL, Yatham LN, Lam RW. The relationship between neurocognitive and psychosocial functioning in major depressive disorder: a systematic review. J Clin Psychiatry. 2014;75(12):1359–1370. doi: 10.4088/JCP.13r08939 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Oliver LD, Haltigan JD, Gold JM, et al. Lower- and Higher-Level Social Cognitive Factors Across Individuals With Schizophrenia Spectrum Disorders and Healthy Controls : Relationship With Neurocognition and Functional Outcome. 2018:1–10. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sby114 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Hooker CI, Gyurak A, Verosky SC, Miyakawa A, Ayduk Ö. Neural Activity to a Partner’s Facial Expression Predicts Self-Regulation After Conflict. Biol Psychiatry. 2010;67(5):406–413. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.10.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Hooker CI, Verosky SC, Germine LT, Knight RT, D’Esposito M. Neural activity during social signal perception correlates with self-reported empathy. Brain Res. 2010;1308:100–113. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.10.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Dawson EL, Shear PK, Strakowski SM. Behavior regulation and mood predict social functioning among healthy young adults. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2012;34(3):297–305. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2011.639297 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Murphy BC, Shepard SA, Eisenberg N, Fabes RA. Concurrent and Across Time Prediction of Young Adolescents’ Social Functioning: The Role of Emotionality and Regulation. Soc Dev. 2004;13(1):56–86. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2004.00257.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Santini ZI, Koyanagi A, Tyrovolas S, Mason C, Haro JM. The association between social relationships and depression: A systematic review. J Affect Disord. 2015;175:53–65. doi: 10.1016/J.JAD.2014.12.049 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Teo AR, Lerrigo R, Rogers MAM. The role of social isolation in social anxiety disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis. J Anxiety Disord. 2013;27(4):353–364. doi: 10.1016/J.JANXDIS.2013.03.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Chou K-L, Liang K, Sareen J. The Association Between Social Isolation and DSM-IV Mood, Anxiety, and Substance Use Disorders. J Clin Psychiatry. 2011;72(11):1468–1476. doi: 10.4088/JCP.10m06019gry [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Ryff CD. Psychological Well-Being Revisited: Advances in the Science and Practice of Eudaimonia. Psychother Psychosom. 2014;83(1):10–28. doi: 10.1159/000353263 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Dwyer DB, Falkai P, Koutsouleris N. Machine Learning Approaches for Clinical Psychology and Psychiatry. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2018;14(1):91–118. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032816-045037 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Obermeyer Z, Emanuel EJ. Predicting the Future - Big Data, Machine Learning, and Clinical Medicine. N Engl J Med. 2016;375(13):1216–1219. doi: 10.1056/NEJMp1606181 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Hahn T, Nierenberg A, Whitfield-Gabrieli S. Predictive analytics in mental health: applications, guidelines, challenges and perspectives. 2016. doi: 10.1038/mp.2016.201 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Orphanidou C, Wong D. Machine Learning Models for Multidimensional Clinical Data. In: Springer, Cham; 2017:177–216. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-58280-1_8 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.James G, Witten D, Hastie T, Tibshirani R. An Introduction to Statistical Learning. 1st ed. New York: Springer; 2013. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4614-7138-7 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.de Nijs J, Burger TJ, Janssen RJ, et al. Individualized prediction of three- and six-year outcomes of psychosis in a longitudinal multicenter study: a machine learning approach. npj Schizophr. 2021;7(1):34. doi: 10.1038/s41537-021-00162-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Haining K, Brunner G, Gajwani R, et al. The relationship between cognitive deficits and impaired short-term functional outcome in clinical high-risk for psychosis participants: A machine learning and modelling approach. Schizophr Res. 2021;231:24–31. doi: 10.1016/J.SCHRES.2021.02.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Koutsouleris N, Kahn RS, Chekroud AM, et al. Multisite prediction of 4-week and 52-week treatment outcomes in patients with first-episode psychosis: a machine learning approach. The Lancet Psychiatry. 2016;3(10):935–946. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(16)30171-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Leighton SP, Upthegrove R, Krishnadas R, et al. Development and validation of multivariable prediction models of remission, recovery, and quality of life outcomes in people with first episode psychosis: a machine learning approach. Lancet Digit Heal. 2019;1(6):e261–e270. doi: 10.1016/S2589-7500(19)30121-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Leighton SP, Krishnadas R, Chung K, et al. Predicting one-year outcome in first episode psychosis using machine learning. Acampora G, ed. PLoS One. 2019;14(3):e0212846. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0212846 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Sanfelici R, Dwyer DB, Antonucci LA, Koutsouleris N. Individualized Diagnostic and Prognostic Models for Patients With Psychosis Risk Syndromes: A Meta-analytic View on the State of the Art. Biol Psychiatry. 2020;88(4):349–360. doi: 10.1016/J.BIOPSYCH.2020.02.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Kambeitz-Ilankovic L, Meisenzahl EM, Cabral C, et al. Prediction of outcome in the psychosis prodrome using neuroanatomical pattern classification. Schizophr Res. 2016;173(3):159–165. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2015.03.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Van Essen DC, Smith SM, Barch DM, Behrens TE, Yacoub E, Ugurbil K. The WU-Minn Human Connectome Project: An Overview. 2013;71(2):233–236. doi: 10.1038/mp.2011.182.doi [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Folstein MF, Folstein SE, Mchugh PR. “Mini-Mental State.” A Practice Method for Grading Teh Cognitive State of Patients for the Clinician. Vol 12.; 1975. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Salsman JM, Butt Z, Pilkonis PA, et al. Emotion assessment using the NIH Toolbox. Neurology. 2013;80:S76–S86. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182872e11 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Cyranowski JM, Zill N, Bode R, et al. Assessing social support, companionship, and distress: National Institute of Health (NIH) Toolbox Adult Social Relationship Scales. Health Psychol. 2013;32(3):293–301. doi: 10.1037/a0028586 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Chung K, Wallace J, Kim SY, et al. Structural and molecular interrogation of intact biological systems. Nature. 2013;497(7449):332–337. doi: 10.1038/nature12107 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Babakhanyan I, McKenna BS, Casaletto KB, Nowinski CJ, Heaton RK. National Institutes of Health Toolbox Emotion Battery for English- and Spanish-speaking adults: normative data and factor-based summary scores. Patient Relat Outcome Meas. 2018;Volume 9:115–127. doi: 10.2147/prom.s151658 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Elam J. No Title. HCP-YA Data Dictionary- Updated for the 1200 Subject Release. https://wiki.humanconnectome.org/display/PublicData/HCP-YA+Data+Dictionary-+Updated+for+the+1200+Subject+Release. Published 2021. Accessed January 12, 2020.
  • 62.Gur RC, Sara R, Hagendoorn M, et al. A method for obtaining 3-dimensional facial expressions and its standardization for use in neurocognitive studies. J Neurosci Methods. 2002;115(2):137–143. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0270(02)00006-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Barch DM, Burgess GC, Harms MP, et al. Function in the human connectome: Task-fMRI and individual differences in behavior. Neuroimage. 2013;80:169–189. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2013.05.033 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.McCrae RR, Costa PT. A contemplated revision of the NEO Five-Factor Inventory. Pers Individ Dif. 2004;36(3):587–596. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(03)00118-1 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Achenbach TM, Rescorla LA. Manual for the ASEBA Adult Forms & Profiles An Integrated System of Multi-informant Assessment Reliability, Internal Consistency, Cross-Informant Agreement, and Stability. https://aseba.org/wp-content/uploads/2019/01/ASEBA-Reliability-and-Validity-Adult.pdf. Accessed November 14, 2019.
  • 66.Estle SJ, Green L, Myerson J, Holt DD. Differential effects of amount on temporal and probability discounting of gains and losses. Mem Cognit. 2006;34(4):914–928. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17063921. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Reuben DB, Magasi S, McCreath HE, et al. Motor assessment using the NIH Toolbox. Neurology. 2013;80(11 Suppl 3):S65–75. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182872e01 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Buysse DJ, Reynolds CF, Monk TH, Berman SR, Kupfer DJ. The Pittsburgh sleep quality index: A new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res. 1989;28(2):193–213. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(89)90047-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Arditi A. Improving Letter Contrast Sensitivity Testing. Invest Ophthalmol. 2004;45(13). [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Corp IBM. IBM SPSS Statistics for Macintosh, Version 26.0. 2019. [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Fan R-E, Chang K-W, Hsieh C-J, Wang X-R, Lin C-J. LIBLINEAR: A Library for Large Linear Classification. Vol 9.; 2008. http://www.csie.ntu.edu.tw/. Accessed November 16, 2020.
  • 72.Sarica A, Cerasa A, Quattrone A. Random Forest Algorithm for the Classification of Neuroimaging Data in Alzheimer’s Disease: A Systematic Review. Front Aging Neurosci. 2017;9:329. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00329 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Moradi E, Khundrakpam B, Lewis JD, Evans AC, Tohka J. Predicting symptom severity in autism spectrum disorder based on cortical thickness measures in agglomerative data. Neuroimage. 2017;144:128–141. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.09.049 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Pedregosa F, Varoquaux G, Gramfort A, et al. Scikit-Learn: Machine Learning in Python. Vol 12.; 2011. http://scikit-learn.sourceforge.net.
  • 75.Cearns M, Hahn T, Baune BT. Recommendations and future directions for supervised machine learning in psychiatry. Transl Psychiatry. 2019;9(1):271. doi: 10.1038/s41398-019-0607-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Schnack HG. Improving individual predictions: Machine learning approaches for detecting and attacking heterogeneity in schizophrenia (and other psychiatric diseases). Schizophr Res. 2019;214:34–42. doi: 10.1016/J.SCHRES.2017.10.023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Vieira S, Gong Q, Pinaya WHL, et al. Using Machine Learning and Structural Neuroimaging to Detect First Episode Psychosis: Reconsidering the Evidence. Schizophr Bull. 2020;46(1):17–26. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sby189 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Berk M, Berk L. Cognition in psychiatric disorders: from models to management. The Lancet Psychiatry. 2017;4(3):173–175. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(17)30040-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Iosifescu D V. The relation between mood, cognition and psychosocial functioning in psychiatric disorders. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2012;22:S499–S504. doi: 10.1016/J.EURONEURO.2012.08.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Millan MJ, Agid Y, Brüne M, et al. Cognitive dysfunction in psychiatric disorders: characteristics, causes and the quest for improved therapy. Nat Rev Drug Discov. 2012;11(2):141–168. doi: 10.1038/nrd3628 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Kharabian Masouleh S, Eickhoff SB, Hoffstaedter F, Genon S, Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative ADN. Empirical examination of the replicability of associations between brain structure and psychological variables. Elife. 2019;8. doi: 10.7554/eLife.43464 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Avinun R, Israel S, Knodt AR, Hariri AR. Little evidence for associations between the Big Five personality traits and variability in brain gray or white matter. Neuroimage. 2020;220:117092. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2020.117092 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Baranger DAA, Few LR, Sheinbein DH, et al. Borderline Personality Traits Are Not Correlated With Brain Structure in Two Large Samples. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. 2020;5(7):669–677. doi: 10.1016/J.BPSC.2020.02.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Huppert FA. Psychological Well-being: Evidence Regarding its Causes and Consequences. Appl Psychol Heal Well-Being. 2009;1(2):137–164. doi: 10.1111/j.1758-0854.2009.01008.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Sanmartin R, Ingles C, Vicent M, Gonzalvez C, Diaz-Herrero A, Garcia-Fernandez J. Positive and negative affect as predictors of social functioning in Spanish children. PLoS One. 2018;13(8):e0201698. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Grove TB, Tso IF, Chun J, et al. Negative affect predicts social functioning across schizophrenia and bipolar disorder: Findings from an integrated data analysis. Psychiatry Res. 2016;243:198–206. doi: 10.1016/J.PSYCHRES.2016.06.031 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Fischer AH, Manstead ASR. Social Functions of Emotion and Emotion Regulation. In: Lewis M, Haviland-Jones J, Barrett LF, eds. Handbook of Emotions. 4th ed. New York: Guilford; 2016:424–439. [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Keltner D, Kring A. Emotion, social function, and psychopathology. Rev Gen Psychol. 1998;2(3):320–342. [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Abbott RA, Croudace TJ, Ploubidis GB, Kuh D, Richards M, & Huppert FA. The relationship between early personality and midlife psychological well-being: Evidence from a UK birth cohort study. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2008;43(9):679–687. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Weiss LA, Westerhof GJ, Bohlmeijer ET. Can We Increase Psychological Well-Being? The Effects of Interventions on Psychological Well-Being: A Meta-Analysis of Randomized Controlled Trials. Coyne J, ed. PLoS One. 2016;11(6):e0158092. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0158092 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Fava GA, Ruini C. Development and characteristics of a well-being enhancing psychotherapeutic strategy: well-being therapy. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 2003;34(1):45–63. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7916(03)00019-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Dunn BD, German RE, Khazanov G, Xu C, Hollon SD, DeRubeis RJ. Changes in Positive and Negative Affect During Pharmacological Treatment and Cognitive Therapy for Major Depressive Disorder: A Secondary Analysis of Two Randomized Controlled Trials. Clin Psychol Sci. 2020;8(1):36–51. doi: 10.1177/2167702619863427 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Hofmann SG, Asnaani A, Vonk IJJ, Sawyer AT, Fang A. The Efficacy of Cognitive Behavioral Therapy: A Review of Meta-analyses. Cognit Ther Res. 2012;36(5):427–440. doi: 10.1007/s10608-012-9476-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Moffitt TE. Male antisocial behaviour in adolescence and beyond. Nat Hum Behav. 2018;2(3):177–186. doi: 10.1038/s41562-018-0309-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Janssens M, Lataster T, Simons CJP, et al. Emotion recognition in psychosis: No evidence for an association with real world social functioning. Schizophr Res. 2012;142(1–3):116–121. doi: 10.1016/J.SCHRES.2012.10.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Yager JA, Ehmann TS. Untangling Social Function Yager and Ehmann Untangling Social Function and Social Cognition: A Review of Concepts and Measurement. 10.1521/psyc.2006.69.1.47?casa_token=M-DsnBFTwDEAAAAA:1hF-Q3n88EtjqpjWRa0iZS8auOjzbM6Uu-65dgbfgnNkyLpQFzK_eaYxJom64zno9zEv6t52m4zB. Accessed October 7, 2020. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 97.Blakemore SJ. The social brain in adolescence. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9(4):267–277. doi: 10.1038/nrn2353 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Van Overwalle F. Social cognition and the brain: A meta-analysis. Hum Brain Mapp. 2009;30(3):829–858. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20547 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Adolphs R. The Social Brain: Neural Basis of Social Knowledge. Annu Rev Psychol. 2009;60(1):693–716. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163514 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Pinkham AE. Social cognition in schizophrenia. J Clin Psychiatry. 2014;75(SUPPL. 2):14–19. doi: 10.4088/JCP.13065su1.04 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Smith AW, Mitchell SA, Aguiar C K. De, et al. News from the NIH: Person-centered outcomes measurement: NIH-supported measurement systems to evaluate self-assessed health, functional performance, and symptomatic toxicity. Transl Behav Med. 2016;6(3):470–474. doi: 10.1007/s13142-015-0345-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Hahn EA, Cella D, Chassany O, et al. Precision of Health-Related Quality-of-Life Data Compared With Other Clinical Measures. Mayo Clin Proc. 2007;82(10):1244–1254. doi: 10.4065/82.10.1244 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Riley WT, Pilkonis P, Cella D. Application of the National Institutes of Health Patient-reported Outcome Measurement Information System (PROMIS) to mental health research. J Ment Health Policy Econ. 2011;14(4):201–208. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Moffitt TE, Arseneault L, Belsky D, et al. A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2011;108(7):2693–2698. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1010076108 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary Material

RESOURCES