Skip to main content
Elsevier - PMC COVID-19 Collection logoLink to Elsevier - PMC COVID-19 Collection
. 2021 May 13;42:101016. doi: 10.1016/j.ehb.2021.101016

Fathers matter: Intrahousehold responsibilities and children's wellbeing during the COVID-19 lockdown in Italy

Lucia Mangiavacchi a,b, Luca Piccoli c,b,, Luca Pieroni a
PMCID: PMC9760207  PMID: 34044352

Abstract

The lockdown imposed during the spring of 2020 as a result of the COVID-19 pandemic upset families lives, in addition to the health consequences of the virus, forcing parents to completely reorganize their labor, domestic work and childcare time. At the same time, school closures forced children to rearrange their lives and learning processes: in Italy, schools and nurseries were closed for four months, and the incidence and quality of distance learning activities was heterogeneous across education levels and among schools. Using real-time survey data on families with under-16 children collected in April 2020, which include information on parents’ market and household work, and their perception of their children's wellbeing, we estimate how the lockdown has affected children's use of time, their emotional status and their home learning, and whether the reallocation of intrahousehold responsibilities during the lockdown played a role in this process. Changes in the parental division of household tasks and childcare, mostly induced by the labor market restrictions imposed during the lockdown, point to a greater involvement of fathers in childcare and homeschooling activities. This positive variation in fathers’ involvement is accompanied by an increase in children's emotional wellbeing and by a reduction in TV and passive screen time. On the other hand, the quality of children's home learning does not appear to depend on which parent is overseeing their work, but rather on the type of distance learning activities proposed by their teachers.

Keywords: Parenting, Childcare, Children's education, Emotional skills, COVID-19

1. Introduction

From a family perspective, the spring 2020 COVID-19 lockdown had apparent consequences on two fundamental assets of families’ wellbeing: parents’ work and children's education. Children were out of school, with very limited childcare possibilities and without access to group activities, team sports, or playgrounds. Parents were attempting to work remotely while caring for children at the same time. In Italy, the situation was particularly severe since the country was in an almost complete lockdown from March 8 to May 3, while some regions and municipalities started as early as February 21. Italy was the first country in Europe to close schools and nurseries (March 4, 2020). Step-wise reductions of limitations started on May 4, yet a comprehensive reopening of economic activities and traveling options across regions only began on June 15, delaying the return to school to September 14 for most regions, i.e. after the three-month long summer vacation.

The impact of the lockdown on children's educational progress (Engzell et al., 2021, Grewenig et al., 2020) is likely to have been particularly strong in the Italian context, which is characterized by high educational inequality among socioeconomic groups (Brunello and Checchi, 2005) and regions (Angrist et al., 2017), with schools, teachers and parents substantially unprepared to manage a distance learning educational model. Schools suffered from underdeveloped IT infrastructure, lacking PCs both in classes and for teachers, most of whom were not trained in the use of computer-based or web-based educational instruments. In this context, parents struggled to cope with work, either because they were no longer able to work or because they were limited to teleworking solutions, while having to take care of children and the home without external help. Social distancing limitations imply that most working parents could not take advantage of any form of childcare, such as babysitting or grandparents, the latter being one of the pillars of the Italian childcare system.

The current paper studies how parents reacted to the lockdown in terms of gender distribution of housework and childcare1 and analyzes, for the first time, how these changes affected children's emotional wellbeing, educational outcomes, and use of time according to their parents. To address these research questions, we use data collected on a sample of families with children from April 7 to May 3 (in the middle of the most restrictive Italian lockdown period, “Phase I”) and estimate household and child fixed-effect models.

This paper offers several contributions to the existing literature. First, we contribute to the growing literature on the changes in market and household work during the COVID-19 crisis by examining the effects of those changes on children under age 16. Alon et al. (2020) and Hupkau and Petrongolo (2020) predicted that a number of fathers would temporarily become the main caregivers and that this temporary change could lead to shifts in gender social norms. A number of studies have already tested this hypothesis in different countries affected by the COVID-19 pandemic between March and April, confirming the prediction. Amuedo-Dorantes et al. (2020) show that in the United States school closures reduce labor supply of mothers more than fathers. Del Boca et al. (2020) for Italy and Farré et al. (2020) for Spain analyzed the gender division of household tasks and childcare and found that most of the additional household workload due to the COVID-19 crisis fell on women's shoulders, with very limited contributions from their partners. On the other hand, childcare activities were more equally shared during the lockdown, especially when mothers continued to work away from home. Sevilla and Smith (2020) focused on childcare in UK families with young children and also found that the gender gap in childcare was smaller during the lockdown. We also observe a substantial increase in fathers’ involvement in childcare and estimate whether this variation is associated with a differential impact of the lockdown on children's wellbeing. This temporary shift in gender roles is particularly interesting in Italy, a country characterized by persistent gender gaps and very low involvement of fathers in raising children (Bloemen et al., 2010, Barigozzi et al., 2020). We bring forward this stream of literature by further studying how household tasks temporary reallocation due to the spring 2020 lockdown may impact on children's outcomes. This is perhaps the main contribution of the present study.

Second, we offer some evidence supporting the importance of fathers’ involvement in child development as already analyzed in the literature on maternal employment (Harris et al., 1998, Hsin and Felfe, 2014) and in the literature on the intergenerational transmission of attitudes and behaviors (Giménez-Nadal et al., 2019, Pieroni and Lanari, 2018). Exploiting the exogenous shock generated by the first generalized lockdown in spring 2020, we analyze the impact of the increased participation of fathers in childcare activities on how parents perceive their children's wellbeing. More specifically, we analyze how parents perceived changes in their children's emotional status, use of time and educational progress with the lockdown at large and, more specifically, how these changes were related to fathers becoming the main caregivers.

Third, we contribute to the stream of literature that considers the role of parental inputs and the allocation of time on children's cognitive and non-cognitive development (Fiorini and Keane, 2014, Del Boca et al., 2017, Mangiavacchi and Piccoli, 2018, Moroni et al., 2019). The lockdown left children with limited and fragmented educational inputs from schools, with parents also taking up the role of teachers. Overall, most children spent much less time in educational activities, thus we evaluate how children reallocated this time and test the mediating effects of parental inputs.

Finally, our analysis offers evidence on how school closures affected children's educational progress. This is especially relevant for Italy, where schools remained closed for four months and the reopening process was delayed until September 2020. The lockdown situation had the potential to widen educational inequalities in a manner similar to what has happened in other contexts in which schools have remained closed for a long period due to other kinds of shocks or policy changes (see for instance Jaume and Willén, 2019, Pischke, 2007). We identify the potential detrimental effect of the lockdown on their education progress, focusing on how well parents could support children in their homeschooling experience.

In our sample, we observe that more mothers than fathers completely stopped working during the lockdown (28% vs 25%), but as men worked more hours on average before (37 vs 25 h per week), they had more additional hours to spend on childcare, while at the same time, grandparents and babysitter hours fell to approximately one-third of their pre-lockdown value. This causes an overall small average reduction in women's share of childcare tasks, but with notable heterogeneity. In fact, the share of households in which the father is the main caregiver increases by 6.6 percentage points to almost 35% of families: a significant variation that could have an impact on children's emotional wellbeing, the quality of their use of time and their home learning process. By estimating household and child fixed-effect models, we find that the increase in fathers’ involvement in childcare and homeschooling during the lockdown is accompanied by an improvement in children's emotional status, relationship with parents and a reduction in TV hours. On the other hand, parents had difficulties in accomplishing their teaching role, and the quality of children's home learning process is mostly determined by the distance learning activities offered by the schools.

The rest of the paper is organized as follows. Section 2 sets the analysis within the institutional setting and the existing empirical research. Section 3 describes data and the empirical strategy. Section 4 presents and discusses the results, and Section 5 concludes.

2. Background

2.1. Institutional setting: the Italian COVID-19 lockdown

In Italy, the first two cases of COVID-19 were identified on January 30, 2020, in Rome, and the Italian government declared a state of emergency, suspending flights from China the following day. By February 21, 79 cases had already been identified, most of which were concentrated in specific municipalities of the Lombardy (54 cases) and Veneto (17 cases) regions. On the following day, the number of recognized cases almost doubled to 152. This was the real start of the pandemic in Italy.2

The government's initial attempt to contain the pandemic started with the complete lockdown of schools, economic activities and transportation in 10 villages in Lombardy and 1 in Veneto, including prohibition to travel to/from these municipalities, applied on February 23. Immediately afterwards, several northern regions suspended school activities and public events for one week, with the possibility of extending the date according to the evolution of the pandemic. In a very rapid escalation on March 4, the government suspended all schools and sport activities at national level, and a few days later, on March 9, Italy was set under a full lockdown, forbidding individual movements unless driven by necessity (health, work, purchasing food or medicines), and on March 11, the closure of most retail shops except grocery shops and pharmacies was imposed. The complete lockdown was set on March 22/23, when all non-essential production activities were closed and any transfer outside the municipality of domicile was forbidden (except for health, work or absolute urgency reasons). Briscese et al. (2020) show that while the government communication strategy may have been suboptimal to maximize compliance with the restrictions imposed, at least half of the population complied with all restrictions, while a very small proportion did not follow any of the government's indications (less than 3%). All these restrictions were in place until May 3, when “Phase I” ended and a gradual weakening of these restrictions was planned. This complete lockdown period is the object of this study, as a relevant share of the adult population was forced to temporarily stop working and children remained at home, without the possibility of moving from the house of domicile and without any use of external childcare service other than co-resident adults.

2.2. Related empirical research

Several studies have already analyzed the evolution of time allocation within the household during the lockdown following the first COVID-19 outbreak in spring 2020.

In the early period of the pandemic in Europe, Alon et al. (2020) and Hupkau and Petrongolo (2020), using pre-existing survey data for the US and UK, predict that women would take on more of the household and childcare responsibilities due to the impossibility to use formal (schools and nursery) and informal (grandparents or babysitter) childcare. Nevertheless, these studies point out that a relevant proportion of fathers would be forced to become primary caregivers if their wives were still working and they were not. Hopefully, this temporary change due to the lockdown measures would turn out to be persistent, leading to a shift in social norms. Successive studies confirmed these initial predictions by collecting and analyzing real-time data during the COVID-19 pandemic in the US, UK, Germany, Spain and Italy (Adams-Prassl et al., 2020, Andrew et al., 2020a, Biroli et al., 2020, Del Boca et al., 2020, Farré et al., 2020, Sevilla and Smith, 2020).

Farré et al. (2020) show that, in a representative sample of Spanish households, while men only increased their participation in household chores slightly, the increase in childcare needs (from 48 to 60 h of childcare done by parents in a week) was absorbed by both mothers and fathers. Sevilla and Smith (2020), making use of real-time data collected in the UK, find that the allocation of additional hours of childcare has become more equal than the previous allocation of childcare. The gender childcare gap has narrowed from 30.5 percentage points to 27.2 percentage points. They also document that this increase has been driven by families in which men were working from home or, to a greater extent, where men have stopped working.

In Italy, the baseline situation is particularly unequal; Barigozzi et al. (2020) document that mothers with young children perform a total amount of 60 h of work per week (25 h of paid work and 35 h of housework and childcare), while their male partners provide 47 h per week (a gender gap of 13 h per week). Del Boca et al. (2020), using a representative sample of working women interviewed at the end of April 2020, find that the additional housework associated with the lockdown has fallen on women, while childcare and home learning activities have been more equally shared. They find that 68% of women and 40% of men have spent more time on housework and childcare during the lockdown in the general sample. However, restricting the sample to families where only women continue to go to their usual place of work and their partner does not work at all, the share of husbands who performed more household tasks increased to 58% for housework (vs 61% of wives) and 54% for childcare (vs 31% of wives).

In the current paper, we focus on couples with children in Italy and are able to identify families in which fathers became the primary caregivers. Taking the analysis one step forward, we then estimate how this evolution in childcare responsibilities affected children's emotional status, time use and home learning progress.

3. Data and empirical strategy

3.1. Survey and data description

In such a worrying context, we developed a web survey specifically aiming at analyzing how Italian families reacted to the COVID-19 lockdown, collecting data right in the middle of the lockdown crisis (during the second month of the Italian “Phase I” of the lockdown). Access to and completion of the survey has been proposed on a voluntary base. The questionnaire was completely anonymous and there were no rewards for participation to the study. A total of 3352 families completed the surveys from April 7 to May 3, with most of the advertising campaign conducted on Facebook.3 The survey was conducted without a sampling strategy; thus the data are not representative of the Italian population. Nevertheless, due to the relevant sample size and the ability to reach all Italian regions and different socioeconomic groups, as reported in Table A1, several key variables used in the analysis are in line with national statistics reported by ISTAT (the Italian National Institute of Statistics). Regarding the number of children for couples, our sample slightly over-represents couples with 2 children, at the expenses of couples with 1 child or with 3 or more children. In terms of geographical distribution, an overrepresentation of northern and central Italy is observed, although southern regions were much less affected by COVID-19, and thus it was expected that we would have lower response rates. Perhaps the most relevant difference concerns the proportion of individuals with a university degree, which was double the national average for a similarly aged population group. A significantly larger share of individuals are working and individuals of non-Italian nationality are under-represented.4 Nevertheless, the key variables of interest, i.e., couple's division of housework and childcare and children's hours of TV and reading, are very much in line with national statistics, except hours of reading, which are larger in our sample.

The survey asked for detailed information about respondents’ previous and current work arrangements and on the division of household tasks, asking respondents to report their own and their partner's previous and current employment, sector of employment, labor supply evolution and hours of teleworking. The survey contained a section on couples’ division of housework and childcare tasks. It collected detailed information on respondents’ background characteristics including gender, age, highest level of education, marital status, and number of children below 16, and a specific section on children and on homeschooling during the lockdown. For individuals with children up to 16 years old living in the household, we asked about the number of hours spent on active childcare and on homeschooling and their evolution with the lockdown. The last section collected detailed information on each child based on his/her age and school grade. In particular, we asked questions on time use and its evolution (hours spent studying, performing extracurricular activities, reading and watching TV/passive screen/social networks), parents’ subjective opinions on the child's educational progress, emotional status, parent-child relationship, whether and which type of e-learning technology their teachers have adopted, availability of computers/tablets, and so on.5

To analyze how the COVID-19 lockdown modified the family division of childcare responsibilities and, in turns, children's emotional and educational wellbeing, we use a sample of 2101 households, which include 3619 children under 16.6 The descriptive statistics of household level variable are presented in Table A2, while those that are child specific are presented in Table A3.

The descriptive statistics reported in Table A2 highlight how the family organization changed during the lockdown: mothers’ share of housework was more than 75% before the lockdown and decreased to 71% during the lockdown, a reduction of 4.4 percentage points. A similar pattern is observed for the mothers’ share of childcare, which decreased by 2.2 percentage points from 68.9% to 66.7%. Although the reduction was smaller, the proportion of fathers that became the main caregiver, i.e., that carried out at least 50% of childcare tasks, rose substantially from 28.1% to 34.8%. Approximately 28% of mothers and 25% of fathers completely stopped working (smart workers were not included in these figures) during the lockdown. As their average daily hours of work shrunk approximately by 1.4 h for mothers and 1.6 h for fathers, this reduction was almost completely determined by the exogenous suspension of their work activity that was determined by the government, not by their choice. This boosted the daily hours available for housework and/or childcare, as highlighted by the couple's amount of hours spent doing homework with their children, which rose from 3.1 to 6.5 h per week. In contrast, as expected, both grandparents’ and babysitters’ hours of childcare fell to approximately one third of their pre-lockdown values. These are also exogenous variations, as induced by the lockdown and not by family choices. During the first pandemic outbreak, a relatively modest share of the sample was under strict lockdown, either because they were COVID positive or in contact with a positive case (3% of mothers and 1.8% of fathers).

Regarding time-invariant characteristics, we observe that prior to the lockdown, mothers contributed to 34% of family income, were on average 41 years old, had a university degree in approximately 37% of cases, and were those who responded most often to the survey (93% of respondents are female).7 Fathers were only slightly older (nearly 44 years old) and were more educated (almost 59% had a university degree). More than half of the sampled households had 2 or more children (55%), and approximately 12% of them had other adults living in the same dwelling. Only 1.5% of the sample is composed of parents of foreign nationality.

Table A3 describes the observed variations implied by the lockdown for child level variables. Parents have observed an overall reduction in their children's emotional wellbeing. Recalling that categories take values 0 for unchanged in a −2 to 2 scale, the average value of −0.59 seems to suggest a significant reduction. At the same time, we observe a slight improvement in their personal relationship with their children (about 0.2). As approximately 50% of mothers and 70% of fathers were still working during the lockdown (either physically because working in a sector of national interest or because shifting to smart working), we observe an increase of 1.5 daily hours of TV, passive screen time or social network use. An increase is also observed for reading time, but by a much smaller amount, just approximately 15 min per day. In terms of educational progress, parents evaluate it as largely insufficient, with 4.8 on a scale from 0 to 10.

A particularly relevant variation in children's activities due to the COVID-19 lockdown is the reduction in extracurricular activities, which decreased by almost half an hour per day. The average age of the children of interviewed couples is 7, where 50.7% of them are males who spent an average of 1.2 h per day on live online classes. 37.5% of children attend kindergarten, 40.3% primary school, and the remaining were in secondary-level education, either middle school or the first two years of high school.

3.2. Empirical strategy

The empirical analysis is conducted in two steps. In the first step the focus is on how the lockdown modified household division of work, using 3 indicators as dependent variables in the regression analysis: (i) the share of housework tasks carried out by women; (ii) the share of childcare carried out by women; and (iii) whether the father is the main child caregiver, a dummy variable taking the value of 1 when the father carries out at least 50% of the childcare tasks. Each of these variables is available before and during the lockdown, allowing us to perform fixed-effect regressions of the form:

Sht=βXht+LDt+γLDt·Wh+uh+eht, (1)

where S ht is the selected outcome for household h at time t (two time periods: before and during the lockdown), X ht is a set of time-varying regressors and W h is a set of time-invariant regressors. LD is to be interpreted as the residual effect of the lockdown on the dependent variable after controlling for covariates, while the γ coefficients are to be interpreted as the differential effects of the lockdown depending on the values taken by time-invariant characteristics W h. The parameters of Eq. (1) are estimated using a first difference estimator, specified by taking first differences of Eq. (1):

ΔSh=LD+γWh+βΔXh+eh, (2)

where LD corresponds to the constant and is to be interpreted as the residual impact of the lockdown on the change in the outcome. Parameters γ are the differential impact of the lockdown for households with specific characteristics, W h, and β represent the impact of the variation of explanatory variables X h.

The main aim of this first step is to verify whether the main driver of changes in the household division of work is the work status of both parents, which accounts for the fact that during the lockdown, everyone except those working in sectors of special national interest had to temporarily stop working by law. This would translate into an exogenous increase in the time available to perform household tasks by those who stopped working. For this reason, at variance with Del Boca et al. (2020), parents that switched to smart-working arrangements are considered to be still working during the lockdown. While it is true that in some cases, smart working may allow flexible arrangements, anecdotal evidence suggests that such experience has been frustrating and stressful since most smart workers had to accommodate a full-time job schedule. In addition, considering childcare tasks performed as a secondary activity while working would imply very little value added for the children since parents’ time would be mostly passive. Another potentially relevant factor for explaining the variation in the division of household tasks is the reduction of childcare time by grandparents and babysitters: during the lockdown, there was a sharp reduction in such hours since babysitters were not allowed to work (unless already living in the same household) and grandparents were not allowed to travel to visit the family (again unless already living in the same household).

The main explanatory variables included in X it are whether the mother, the father, or both stopped working, whether the mother switched to smart working, i.e. whether she increased the hours of smart working with respect to the pre-lockdown situation,8 and the variation in the hours of work of both parents (including hours of smart-working). In addition, we also include the variation in childcare hours both by grandparents and babysitter.

The time-invariant characteristics W i include: the share of family income earned by the woman before the lockdown (as a proxy for her bargaining power within the household), the age of both partners and its difference, the gender of the respondent, the number of children (as a dummy indicating whether 2 or more children are present), the presence of other adults in addition to parents in the household, whether parents have a university degree and whether both parents have non-Italian nationality. As the COVID-19 pandemic had a very heterogeneous geographical spread, we included a province-specific lockdown effect (province FE). As the duration of the lockdown may have worsened some outcomes, we included the date of interview as a time trend. Finally, as the prime minister typically made a speech to the population on Friday nights, updating the status of the pandemic and indicating whether changes in the restrictive policies were to be expected in the following days, we include specific lockdown effects for the day of the week in which the survey was completed also. Standard errors of the regressions are clustered at the province level.9

The second step, the core of our analysis, aims at analyzing how such a change in the division of household tasks affected children. For the analysis of children's outcomes Y it, we rely on 5 indicators: (i) the evolution in the emotional status of the child10 ; (ii) the evolution in the personal relationship that the child had with parents (with the same categories of emotional status); (iii) the daily hours spent by the child watching TV, passive screen time (YouTube and similar) and social media; (iv) the daily hours spent by the child reading (or listening to an adult reading for the younger kids); and (v) an evaluation of the educational progress of the child.11 Similarly to Eq. (1), each of these variables is available as a time variation, allowing us to perform fixed-effect regressions:

Yit=βXit+LDt+γLDt·Wi+ui+eit, (3)

Here, regressions are conducted at the individual rather than household level, thus since many families have more than one child, standard errors are clustered at the household level (although the alternative of clustering at the province level has barely any effect on standard errors). Apart from this, the analysis is conducted in similar way to Eq. (2), i.e.

ΔYi=LD+γWi+βΔXi+ei. (4)

The main explanatory variables included in x it are those describing the parental division of household work, that is whether the father is the main child caregiver and the share of housework carried out by the mother, but also the increased amount of productive time –in a human capital perspective– that parents spend with their children, and the hours of distance learning live classes offered by the teachers. Other time-varying variables included whether both parents stopped working during the lockdown, whether the mother switched to smart working, and the variation in the hours of extracurricular activities.

The other time-invariant explanatory variables included in W i are: the ours of live online classes for each school level attended (kindergarten, primary or secondary), gender and age of the child, the number of children in the household, whether at least one parent is not Italian, whether each parents have a university degree, whether both parents were under strict lockdown and whether the respondent is male. Province and day of the week fixed-effects are also included.

4. Results

4.1. Parents’ division of housework and childcare

A closer look at the descriptive analysis of the effects the COVID-19 lockdown had on the distribution of household tasks reveals substantial heterogeneous effects. Fig. 1 plots the share of childcare tasks performed by the mother before and during the lockdown in relation to the share of family income she earned before the lockdown. It shows that fathers took up significantly more childcare only for those mothers who earned up to 50% of the family income. The effect could plausibly be driven by bargaining power. In those households where most of the income is earned by the father, specialization may be at work (Becker, 1981): the husband is more productive in the market work and the wife is more productive in childcare tasks. However, the lockdown forced many workers to stop working, therefore the specialization argument no longer holds. Having more time available, it becomes optimal for fathers to put that time to good use, leading to a reduction in the mothers’ share of childcare. In addition it is worth considering that a large share of essential (low wage) workers working in grocery stores, supermarkets, pharmacies, care centers, and so on, are women. They still had to go to work during the lockdown, while their husbands stayed home. This may explain why the statistically significant difference is observed only when the mother earns up to 50% of family income.12

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Variation in the mothers’ share of childcare by share of income earned.

This evidence suggests that the Beckerian household framework can explain the variations observed during the lockdown, where most of the changes in the gender distribution of household tasks would be driven by the changes in the available time of both partners, as implied by their contingent work situation (Becker, 1981).

This hypothesis is tested by a regression analysis, whose results are reported in Table 1 .13 The first row reports the overall unconditional variation of the selected outcome during the COVID-19 lockdown and tests whether this is significantly different from zero. The results confirm that the lockdown had a balancing effect on the parents’ division of household tasks, reducing the mothers’ share of housework by 4.3 percentage points and the mothers’ share of childcare tasks by 2.1 percentage points. Although the reduction in childcare tasks is relatively modest, the lockdown increased the probability that the father became the main caregiver by almost 6.6 percentage points.

Table 1.

The division of housework and childcare during the lockdown.

(1) (2) (3)
Mothers’ share of housework Mothers’ share of childcare Father main caregiver
Unconditional variation statistics
Baseline values of the dependent variables before the lockdown (s.d. in parenthesis) 0.7534 0.6889 0.2819
(0.1875) (0.1828) (0.4500)
Variation of the dependent variables during the lockdown −0.0433*** −0.0212*** 0.0660***
(0.0033) (0.0061) (0.0131)



Coefficients of interest (FE regressions)
Lockdown 0.230 −0.0888 0.725
(0.270) (0.440) (1.093)
Father stopped working −0.0523*** −0.0408*** 0.169***
(0.0125) (0.0142) (0.0369)
Mother stopped working 0.0237** 0.0312** −0.108***
(0.0103) (0.0120) (0.0355)
Both parents stopped working −0.0207 −0.0597** 0.0228
(0.0170) (0.0228) (0.0580)
Mother switched to smartworking 0.00782 0.0179* −0.0566*
(0.00981) (0.0106) (0.0324)
Mother's hours of work (inc. Smartworking) −0.00328 −0.00937*** 0.00745
(0.00198) (0.00275) (0.00597)
Father's hours of work (inc. Smartworking) 0.00403*** 0.00737*** −0.0112**
(0.00141) (0.00194) (0.00555)
Observations 1930 1910 1910
R-squared 0.139 0.155 0.111

All regressions include province and day of the week fixed effects. Full estimation results, including coefficients for all control variables, are presented in Table A4. *** p < 0.01, ** p < 0.05, * p < 0.1; Clustered standard errors in parentheses (province level), unless otherwise specified.

The regression coefficients reported in Table 1 confirm that which parent stopped working and the reduction of the workload during the lockdown were important factors for determining the parents’ division of household tasks. For all three outcomes, when only the father stops working, he takes on more household tasks, while when only the mother stops working, the opposite happens. The mother's coefficient is always smaller than that of the father, which clearly highlights a heterogeneity that explains the small overall variation in the intrahousehold division of housework and childcare during the lockdown. For instance, when the father stops working, the share of housework performed by the mother decreases by 5.3 percentage points. Regarding childcare tasks, the heterogeneous effects of the lockdown is even larger: when only the father stops working, the mother's share of childcare drops by 4.2 percentage points but in addition there is an important effect of the reduction in fathers’ working hours. It follows that the probability that the father becomes the main child caregiver during the lockdown increases by 14.5 percentage points when he stops working. A less prominent effect is observed when mothers switch to smart working. The coefficient is only significant at 10% and only for childcare tasks, indicating that when the mother switches to smart working she takes on more childcare tasks and the probability that the father becomes the main caregiver is lower.14

When accounting for the chosen set of additional control variables and considering the differential impacts by province and day of the week, the lockdown coefficient loses its significance. Full estimation results are reported in Table A4, together with the results of the robustness analysis described in Section 4.3. The table provides some further insights into the analysis: (i) the stop working variable coefficients are very similar when including a large set of covariates, pointing toward the robustness of the results; (ii) the stop working variables alone are not sufficient to make the lockdown coefficient lose significance; (iii) very few of the selected covariates are significant, and the only one significant in all regressions is the share of family income earned by the mother, confirming that families in which there was more gender inequality from the start experienced a stronger equalizing effect during the lockdown; (iv) for childcare, parents education and nationality are the only other relevant variables in explaining the variation in the division of childcare tasks; the effect is substantially larger when both parents have a university degree, while the opposite happens when both parents have a foreign nationality; (v) the duration of the lockdown, measured by the date of interview, tend to have a negative effect on parents’ evaluation of both the emotional status and the personal relationship with the child. The hours of TV tend to increase with the date of interview, while the hours of reading tend to decrease. No effect is observed for the evaluation of children's learning progress.

In summary, the regression results confirm that the changes in the gender division of household tasks induced by the COVID-19 lockdown passed through changes in the work situation and the consequent increase in the time available for housework and childcare (as found also by Sevilla and Smith, 2020, for the UK). In addition, specifically regarding childcare tasks, parents’ education also played a key role (as shown in Del Boca et al., 2020), possibly because families in which both parents have a university degree may be more conscious of the potentially beneficial effects of the additional time spent with children by fathers.

4.2. Children's emotional and educational wellbeing

If the spring 2020 COVID-19 lockdown had a tremendous impact on adults’ lives, it may have had an even stronger one on the lives of children. In Italy, schools were closed at the very beginning of the pandemic and did not reopen until September 14,15 leaving children with limited interaction with teachers and schoolmates for as much as 6 months.

Fig. 2, for instance, shows how the time breakdown of the typical day of children changed during the lockdown. There is a clear reduction in “productive” activities from a human capital perspective, such as school, homework, and extracurricular activities. Reading time is an exception, although the overall increase is rather limited. External childcare activities see a sharp decrease, for obvious reasons, while non-productive time, including TV, passive screen time, social networks and others, see a dramatic increase Andrew et al. (2020b, see also][for similar results in the UK). Overall, although productive activities occupied slightly more than 60% of the typical day before the pandemic upsurge, during Phase I of the lockdown, they sum up to less than 40% of the considered activities. This is particularly worrying, as the four-month reduced school activity period was directly followed by the usual three-month summer vacation period.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Typical daily use of time of children, before and during the lockdown.

The negative effect of school closures is likely to be heterogeneous not only by family socioeconomic status and level of education (Mancini et al., 2017, Mencarini et al., 2019), but also by school level. In fact, according to our data, while approximately 95% of children attending secondary school had access to distance education classes and material, only approximately 60% of primary school children had some live online classes, and about 30% of them only received some learning material accompanied by homework to be delivered through an electronic registry. The situation for pre-school children was even worse, who were practically abandoned by the school system: only 18% of them had sporadic live online classes, and 37% received material from their teachers. An astonishing 45% of these children were never involved in any school activity at the date of interview. Knowing the importance of early childhood education (see Cunha et al., 2010, and the following stream of research on the topic), these children deserve particular attention from policymakers.

The left panel of Fig. 3 provides evidence of such a heterogeneous effect by looking at the evolution of children's TV/screen/social network and reading time by age class. The starting point of TV time, i.e., before the lockdown, shows how kids of age 3–10 already spent one and a half hours per day watching TV, a figure that rises to 2 h for older kids. During the lockdown, all children watched much more TV, approximately twice as much. Reading time, as the only educational activity that could to some extent compensate for the decrease in school time and extracurricular activities, also increased but to a much smaller extent (less than half an hour for younger kids and about 10 min for older ones).

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3

The evolution of children's TV and reading time by age class, and the parents’ evaluation of their children's educational progress.

Parents were clearly worried about the educational progress of their children during the lockdown, as on a scale from 0 to 10, the average evaluation was approximately 4.8, but there was substantial heterogeneity by school level and, most importantly, by whether the school attended by the child implemented live online classes. The right panel of Fig. 3 highlights how parents of preschool children were particularly worried about their educational progress, with a 2-point difference in the evaluation when the child had some online classes. For primary school children, the evaluation was clearly better, although it approached a score of 6 only for children who attended online classes. A similar result was observed for secondary school children, but in this case, for the few children who did not have online classes, the evaluation decreased by 3 full points.

In addition to the educational outcomes, the COVID-19 lockdown is likely to have produced effects on children's emotional wellbeing (Moroni et al., 2020). Our survey asked two questions in this respect: the first concerns the emotional status of the child, and the second is about the quality of the parent-child relationship. Fig. 4 shows that children of all ages and gender experienced a negative variation of emotional wellbeing during the lockdown, with a slightly stronger impact for boys and a slightly lighter one for secondary school boys and girls. On the other hand, the lockdown moderately improved children's personal relationship with their parents, especially for girls. This effect, although small in magnitude, is notable as it was measured right in the middle of the most difficult phase of the COVID-19 lockdown, with parents struggling to balance smart-working options, housework tasks, children's homework and distance learning education, and possibly economic difficulties.

Fig. 4.

Fig. 4

Variation in the emotional wellbeing and personal relationship by gender and school level.

The variations of the children outcomes under investigation is presented in Table 2 . These include whether the father became the main child caregiver during the lockdown, the couple-total amount of hours devoted to helping children with their homework, and the mother's share of housework.

Table 2.

Variation of children outcomes during the lockdown.

Variables of interest (1) (2) (3) (4) (5)
Emotional status Personal relationship Hours of TV Reading hours Educational progress
All children −0.588*** 0.194*** 1.516*** 0.240*** −5.225***
(0.0189) (0.0204) (0.0300) (0.0179) (0.0590)



School level
Kindergarten −0.598*** 0.264*** 1.421*** 0.365*** −7.166***
(0.0278) (0.0308) (0.0402) (0.0281) (0.102)
Primary −0.696*** 0.110*** 1.727*** 0.205*** −4.637***
(0.0286) (0.0311) (0.0450) (0.0247) (0.0754)
Secondary −0.522*** 0.230*** 1.550*** 0.0678* −3.999***
(0.0387) (0.0399) (0.0723) (0.0394) (0.0895)

*** p < 0.01, ** p < 0.05, * p < 0.1 (the null hypothesis is that the variation is equal to zero.)

Looking at the overall variations, the results confirm that the lockdown had a significant effect on children's lives: the emotional status reduces by almost 0.6 points on a scale of −2 to 2, while personal relationships improved by 0.2 points on the same scale. Hours of TV/screen/social network increase on average by one and a half hours, while reading hours increased only by less than 15 min. The reduction in the educational progress was valued at 5.2 points in a 0–10 scale. When disentangling what happened to children enrolled in different school levels, a few notable results emerge. First, younger children suffered a little more in terms of emotional wellbeing, but considerably more in terms of educational outcomes. On the other hand, young children were the group that increased their reading time the most during the lockdown.

The econometric analysis of the same outcomes is presented in Table 3 , reporting coefficients for the main variables of interest (full estimation results are reported in Table A5, Table A6, Table A7, Table A8, Table A9, column (2)).

Table 3.

Shift in childcare responsibilities and children's outcomes (FE regressions).

Variables of interest (1) (2) (3) (4) (5)
Emotional status Personal relationship Hours of TV Reading hours Educational progress
Lockdown −0.850*** 0.149 1.907*** 0.244 −8.786***
(0.157) (0.182) (0.258) (0.152) (0.453)
Father main caregiver 0.154*** 0.124** −0.147* −0.0166 0.195
(0.0469) (0.0534) (0.0753) (0.0464) (0.127)
Total parental homework hours −0.0177** 0.00950 0.0216* 0.00970 −0.0566***
(0.00780) (0.00809) (0.0126) (0.00795) (0.0186)
Mother’ share of housework −0.00898 −0.141 0.633** −0.00953 0.301
(0.161) (0.183) (0.297) (0.203) (0.440)
Observations 2640 2648 2470 2441 2578
R-squared 0.134 0.089 0.094 0.083 0.316

Full estimates are presented in Table A5, Table A6, Table A7, Table A8, Table A9. *** p < 0.01, ** p < 0.05, * p < 0.1; Clustered standard errors in parentheses (household level).

The most notable result is that when the father was the main caregiver, the negative effect of the lockdown on children's emotional status was significantly smaller, while the personal relationship improved more. These results seem to stem directly from the additional time fathers can spend with their children, rather than from the increase in childcare time. Also hours of TV increased slightly less when the father was the main caregiver. The positive moderating effect of the father's care on children's emotional status is consistent with the findings in Harris et al. (1998), supporting the idea that fathers’ involvement had a positive effect on children's emotional skills both when taking care of young children and when helping older kids with homeschooling activities. On the other hand, the father's involvement had no effects on reading hours and on the educational progress.

The amount of time parents spend helping their children with homework or educational activities had, instead, a negative effect of children's emotional status and educational progress, as suggested also in the previous literature (Hsin and Felfe, 2014). This, rather than as a causal effect, should probably be interpreted as a measurement effect: if parents spend more time with their children they could possibly be more aware of the negative effect that the lockdown had on them. A higher involvement of parents in their children educational process is also associated with an increase in TV time. Finally, in line with expectations, parental division of housework was not important in explaining children's emotional wellbeing and educational progress, but when mothers had to do a larger share of housework tasks, children spent more time watching TV.

It is worth noting that even controlling for a large set of covariates, the lockdown still had a direct impact on children's outcomes: even when the lockdown coefficient in Table 3 is not significant, its magnitude – which represent the residual impact of the lockdown after controlling for the set of covariates – is always similar or larger than the overall variation of the dependent variables with the lockdown. This means that several factors were able to attenuate some of the negative effects of the lockdown, thus the descriptive analysis could possibly underestimate such negative effects.

Table A5, Table A6, Table A7, Table A8, Table A9 show the full set of coefficients for each outcome.16 Among the notable results, Table A5 shows how children's emotional status worsened less when doing more live on-line classes and extra-curricular activities, when the child had siblings (the more there are, the better), when at least one parent is not Italian, when parents are more educated, and, perhaps surprisingly, when both parents were under strict lockdown. Table A6 highlights similar patterns for the contribution of the covariates to the parent-child relationship, with the exception of observing significant negative effect of being boys an no effects of the number of siblings.

As to the change in hours of TV (Table A7), the increase was attenuated only by the hours of extracurricular activities and by having both parents with a university degree. In this case, the effect of the lockdown seems to stem directly from the increased availability of time and stronger preferences for TV versus reading or other activities that could be done at home. In terms of reading hours (Table A8), the only significant factor in the FE regression is having highly educated parents and having both parents under strict lockdown, both of which have a positive coefficient. Having al least one parent that is not Italian also moderately increases reading time, but the coefficient is significant only at 10% and not in all specifications.

Finally, as shown in Table A9, results for the evaluation of educational progress indicate that most of the negative effect is explained by the number of distance learning hours and by the hours of extracurricular activities.17 The educational progress evaluation has been worse for boys and younger children, and, again, having both parents under strict lockdown improved their evaluation. The latter result seems unrelated with the increased availability of time to be spent with children, as the coefficient of parental time spent in helping children with homework is negative and significant.

4.3. Robustness analysis

The previous pages presented the results of the linear fixed-effect regressions introduced in Section 3.2 to study how a greater involvement of fathers in childcare tasks during the spring 2020 lockdown may have influenced children's emotional wellbeing and educational progress. Although Section 4.1 highlighted how this shift in parental responsibilities was largely due to the exogenous labor supply shock induced by the lockdown, in this section we propose a battery of robustness test on the possible concern that the results may be driven by particular characteristics of the data and/or the empirical strategy.

First, we compare the coefficient of interest in the main regressions with those from the same regression but without the large set of covariates. Stability of the coefficients is an indication that significant relationships do not emerge from spurious correlation and that it is unlikely to be severely affected by unobserved heterogeneity issues. Tables A5, A6 and A7 show that when the coefficient of the variables of interest are significant in the main regressions (column (2) in the Tables), they are very stable with respect to the exclusion of all covariates and fixed effects.

A second robustness test provides evidence for the quality of the data. As explained in Section 3.1, the survey was developed urgently right after the onset of the first lockdown, and we were able to start the dissemination in less than one month. For this reason, the survey was conducted without a sampling strategy. This implies that the data cannot claim to be representative of the Italian population in any dimension. While some characteristics of the sample were very close to national statistics, as exposed in Section 3.1 and Table A1, we have detected significant deviations that may affect our results. In particular, we refer to a much higher proportion of individuals with a university degree, a lower proportion of foreign born individuals, a higher participation in the labor market and an under-representation of southern regions. To be sure that these differences do not drive our results, we have computed post-stratification weights calibrated on most of the characteristics reported in Table A1 using a iterative proportional fitting, or raking, procedure.18 Then the main regressions have been replicated using the calibrated weights, and the results are presented in columns (3) of Table A5, Table A6, Table A7, Table A8, Table A9. The use of calibrated weights did not affect significantly the coefficient of interest except for the coefficients for TV hours, which become all non-significant, and for the coefficient of father main caregiver on the educational progress, which becomes positive and significant at 5%.

Another possible concern regarding the data is that most respondents to the survey are female. This implies that the results mostly reflect mothers opinion on children's outcomes, but as fathers opinion may be radically different, besides controlling for respondent's gender using a dummy, we also perform the same regressions on the subsample of female respondents only (see Table A5, Table A6, Table A7, Table A8, Table A9, columns (3)), finding no significant differences in the parameters of interest.

The fourth robustness check is about the choice of constructing a dummy variable for the father becoming the main caregiver rather using directly the continuous variable father share of childcare. While our choice was motivated by ease of interpretation, column (4) of Table A5, Table A6, Table A7, Table A8, Table A9 report the results of the main regressions using the continuous variable rather than the dummy. The sign and significance of the coefficients are confirmed for all outcomes except for parent-child relationship, which becomes non-significant. The last robustness analysis is related to the econometric model to be applied. As educational status and personal relationship are categorical variables whose numerical values have little relevance, it could be appropriate to estimate them using ordered logistic regressions. The same is also applied to TV hours, reading hours and educational evaluation because, although theoretically continuous variables, they are quite concentrated around some integer values as if they were categorical. The results are presented in column (5) of Table A5, Table A6, Table A7, Table A8, Table A9 and are all strongly consistent with the main estimates, sometimes with gains in significance of a few control variables.

5. Conclusions

Italian families, as well as many other families around the world, were challenged during the COVID-19 pandemic with an almost complete lockdown. Children were heavily affected: schools were immediately closed as well as any form of childcare services or extracurricular activities. In both cases, attempts to provide distance learning solutions were highly heterogeneous, both in quantity and in quality, with likely negative long-run effects on human capital accumulation and educational inequality. In addition, the imposition of staying at home except in cases of absolute necessity strongly limited children's social lives and their possibility to develop soft skills through peer interactions, possibly affecting their emotional wellbeing.

In this study, we analyzed how the intrahousehold division of housework and childcare have changed during the lockdown, and how these, in turn, have influenced children's emotional wellbeing and learning process. To accomplish this, we performed descriptive analyses and fixed-effect regressions using real-time survey data developed for this purpose and collected right in the middle of the stronger Italian COVID-19 lockdown phase, in April 2020. While recent studies have investigated the consequences of the COVID-19 outbreak on the reallocation of market and unpaid work, this is, to the best of our knowledge, the first study to investigate how these changes affected children's wellbeing jointly with the lockdown itself.

The results suggest that changes in the intrahousehold distribution of housework and childcare tasks can be largely explained by the exogenous increase in the available time for those parents that had to stop working during the lockdown. Taking advantage of this exogenous variation, we evaluate the effects of the lockdown on children's emotional wellbeing and learning process and to which degree the shift in childcare roles toward fathers had an influence on children. We find that fathers matter: when they become the main child caregiver, children's emotional wellbeing increases significantly, both in terms of emotional status and relationship with their parents. In addition, they watch a little less TV, an unproductive activity that increased substantially with the lockdown. These results survive a battery of robustness checks on the empirical strategy.

Despite data limitations, our results provide support to implement policies aimed at increasing fathers’ involvement in childcare activities, such as increased mandatory paternity leave periods and more flexible work arrangements. All of these potential implementations would have positive effects on children's wellbeing with the additional advantage of contributing to re-balancing women's workload (at home and in the market), and would possibly shift Italian gender norms from the traditional family structure to more egalitarian role models in the longer term.

Authors’ contribution

Lucia Mangiavacchi: Conceptualization, Methodology, Software, Formal analysis, Investigation, Data Curation, Writing – Original Draft, Writing – Review & Editing, Visualization. Luca Piccoli: Conceptualization, Methodology, Software, Formal analysis, Investigation, Data Curation, Writing – Original Draft, Writing – Review & Editing, Visualization. Luca Pieroni: Conceptualization, Methodology, Software, Formal analysis, Investigation, Data Curation, Writing - Original Draft, Writing – Review & Editing, Visualization.

Footnotes

1

In Italy, intrahousehold responsibilities follow traditional gender norms, with women bearing the main responsibility of household production and childcare tasks (Barigozzi et al., 2020).

2

Most of the data are drawn from the Italian Ministry of Health, through its daily news, with additional data provided by the Civil Protection Department at http://www.protezionecivile.gov.it.

3

The advertising campaign was targeted towards Italian residents in working age living with a partner, both with or without children. Facebook seemed an adequate platform to conduct the campaign as about 93% of the Italian population aged 25–54 is active on Facebook, with a good gender balanced, as 50.6% of users are men (source: NapoleonCat stats, based of Facebook advertising API). In the advertising campaign, we acknowledged that the survey was part of a scientific research project to collect data on how families – especially children – managed to run their life under the lockdown. People may had the feeling that their contribution to a scientific research could be important in such a critical situation and for this reason we obtained a good response to the call, but also probably determined a selection of respondents towards educated mothers.

4

To check that such differences do not drive our main results, as a robustness check we replicate the analysis applying post-stratification weights calibrated on the population values of all these variables. See Section 4.3 for more details.

5

The English translation of the questionnaire for Italy is available as supplementary material.

6

The large sample reduction is mainly due to selecting only families with children, which reduced the family units by about 1000. The sample is further reduced by about 200 units because mono-parental families were also excluded. The remaining drops in sample size are due to missing values in the variables used in the regressions, which are generally modest in magnitude.

7

The fact that such a large proportion of respondents is composed by women, is mostly due to the contents of the Facebook advertising campaign, which stressed the aspects of family life and child wellbeing. In fact a similar survey conducted in France reports 89.3% of respondents being women (Champeaux et al., 2021). Similarly data collected in Spain, Germany and Austria, which however did not focus on children, report 75–78% of female respondents (Farré et al., 2020).

8

A similar variables for fathers is not included because very few of them switched to smart working in the early lockdown period, less than 1.6% vs 14% of mothers.

9

In Italy there exist 107 provinces, and they represent the smallest territorial administrative units above municipalities. The number of clusters exceeding the 50 minimum threshold commonly considered to be safe for computing clustered standard errors.

10

The categorical variable takes the following values: −2 for “substantially worsened”; −1 for “slightly worsened”; 0 for “unchanged”, 1 for “slightly improved”; and 2 for “substantially improved”.

11

The variable takes values from 0 for “not progressing at all” to 10 “progressing at the same pace as when she/he was attending classes at school.”

12

Similar arguments would hold for housework tasks.

13

Full estimation tables are reported in the Appendix, Table A4.

14

There may be a concern that work status is nearly collinear with the lockdown. However, the correlation index is only 0.28 for men and 0.25 for women, so correlation is low, and excluding all work status variables from the regressions presented in Table 1 do not change in a meaningful way the lockdown coefficients.

15

As the educational system comes under regional control, despite the government's guidelines to open on September 14, some regions decided to re-open schools on different dates. For instance, Puglia and Campania re-opened on September 24 because of local elections, other southern regions opened around September 24, while the only anticipation was for the autonomous province of Bolzano, which started on September 7. Individual schools could decide to postpone reopening depending on the number of positive cases within the academic and administrative staff.

16

These tables also present the results of the robustness analyses presented in Section 4.3.

17

See Champeaux et al. (2021) for a more detailed analysis of the impact of distance learning activities on children's educational outcomes

18

In particular the raking procedure was targeted to the actual regional distribution of the population, the number of children, the proportion of women and men of comparable age with university degree, their working condition, and their nationality.

Appendix B

Supplementary data associated with this article can be found, in the online version, at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ehb.2021.101016.

Appendix A. Additional tables

Table A1, Table A2, Table A3, Table A4, Table A5, Table A6, Table A7, Table A8, Table A9

Table A1.

Comparison of key variables with ISTAT surveys.

Variables Our sample ISTAT
Family type1
Couples with 1 child 44.0% 47.9%
Couples with 2 children 46.8% 41.7%
Couples with 3 or more children 9.1% 10.4%



Geographical distribution2
North 53.6% 46.0%
Center 26.7% 19.9%
South 19.7% 34.1%



University degree3
Females 59.9% 29.2%
Males 38.8% 19.5%



Working3
Females 78.6% 62.8%
Males 96.0% 84.9%



Foreign born4
Females 4.7% 8.9%
Males 4.5% 8.7%



Parents5
Mother’ share of housework 73.0% 73.3%
Mother’ share of childcare 66.8% 62.7%



Children 3–55
Hours of TV 1.37 1.25
Hours of reading 1.02 0.42



Children 6–105
Hours of TV 1.45 1.57
Hours of reading 0.94 0.46



Children 11–145
Hours of TV 1.96 1.72
Hours of reading 0.90 0.55

Notes: (1) Reference population shares are drawn from the Multipurpose Survey on Households: Aspects of Daily Life 2019. (2) Reference values are drawn from Resident Municipal Population on January 1 2019. (3) Reference values are the share of males and females aged 30–49 with a university degree, drawn from the 2019 Labour Force Survey. (4) Source Resident Population at January 1st 2020. (5). Reference values are drawn to the 2013 Italian Time Use Survey, for a sample of bi-active couples with children, with the woman in the age range 25–64. For comparative purposes, the same conditions have been applied to our samples, whose reference values reflect what was stated as the pre-lockdown situation.

Table A2.

Descriptive statistics for the sample of couples with children.

Variable Before Lockdown
During lockdown
Mean S.d. Mean S.d.
Mother's share of housework 0.754 0.188 0.710 0.195
Mother's share of childcare 0.689 0.183 0.667 0.199
Father main caregiver 0.281 0.450 0.348 0.476
Total parental homework hours 3.108 2.745 6.453 4.458
Mother's work status (1 = working, 0=other) 0.786 0.410 0.507 0.500
Father's work status (1 = working, 0=other) 0.960 0.197 0.704 0.457
Mother's daily hours of work (inc. smart working) 4.181 2.662 2.810 2.942
Father's daily hours of work (inc. smart working) 6.163 2.514 4.561 3.322
Grandparents’ hours of childcare 2.210 2.532 0.790 2.218
Babysitter's hours of childcare 0.343 1.126 0.109 0.750
Mother under strict lockdown 0.030 0.171
Father under strict lockdown 0.018 0.132
Mother switched to smart working during lockdown 0.140 0.347
Share of family income earned by the mother 0.341 0.236
Age of mother 41.234 5.736
Age of father 43.854 6.360
Age difference ratio 0.515 0.026
Respondent is male 0.069 0.253
2 or more children in the household 0.554 0.497
Presence of other adults in the household 0.118 0.322
Father has a university degree 0.587 0.493
Mother has a university degree 0.374 0.484
Both parents have non-Italian nationality 0.015 0.121



Observations 2101

Table A3.

Descriptive statistics for the sample of children.

Variable Before Lockdown
During lockdown
Variation
Mean S.d. Mean S.d. Mean S.d.
Emotional status1 −0.585 0.957
Personal relationship1 0.193 1.019
Evaluation of educational progress2 4.778 2.861
TV hours per day 1.444 0.961 2.913 1.615 1.510 1.468
Reading hours per day 0.971 0.733 1.206 0.964 0.239 0.880
Extracurricular activities hours per week 0.597 0.510 0.137 0.324 −0.461 0.518
Child is male 0.507 0.500
Age of child 7.082 4.121
Distance learning hours 1.199 1.418
School level
Kindergarten 0.375 0.484
Primary 0.403 0.491
Secondary 0.222 0.415



Observations 3619

Notes: (1) In a −2 (seriously worsened) to 2 (notably improved) scale, 0 meaning no change was perceived. (2) In a 0 to 10 scale, with 0 meaning no progress at all is being made, 10 the child is progressing as if s/he was attending school.

Table A4.

Full regression estimates for parents’ outcomes.

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6)
Mother's share of housework Mother's share of childcare Father main caregiver
Constant −0.0418*** 0.230 −0.0213*** −0.0888 0.0661*** 0.725
(0.00525) (0.270) (0.00558) (0.440) (0.0172) (1.093)
Father stopped working during the lockdown −0.0496*** −0.0523*** −0.0309** −0.0408*** 0.140*** 0.169***
(0.0119) (0.0125) (0.0139) (0.0142) (0.0339) (0.0369)
Mother stopped working during the lockdown 0.0221** 0.0237** 0.0362*** 0.0312** −0.104*** −0.108***
(0.00943) (0.0103) (0.0105) (0.0120) (0.0349) (0.0355)
Both parents stopped working during the lockdown −0.0153 −0.0207 −0.0523** −0.0597** 0.0147 0.0228
(0.0159) (0.0170) (0.0202) (0.0228) (0.0526) (0.0580)
Mother switched to smart working during the lockdown 0.0169* 0.00782 0.0142 0.0179* −0.0595* −0.0566*
(0.00935) (0.00981) (0.0104) (0.0106) (0.0318) (0.0324)
Mother's hours of work (inc. Smart working) −0.00391** −0.00328 −0.00880*** −0.00937*** 0.00803 0.00745
(0.00184) (0.00198) (0.00258) (0.00275) (0.00560) (0.00597)
Father's hours of work (inc. Smart working) 0.00403*** 0.00391*** 0.00772*** 0.00737*** −0.0125** −0.0112**
(0.00141) (0.00128) (0.00209) (0.00194) (0.00536) (0.00555)
Variation in grandparents’ hours of childcare 0.00164 0.000955 −0.00311
(0.00110) (0.00124) (0.00330)
Variation in babysitter's hours of childcare 0.000402 −0.00485 0.00971
(0.00336) (0.00358) (0.00903)
Share of family income earned by the mother 0.0605*** 0.0433** −0.121**
(0.0120) (0.0209) (0.0474)
Age of mother −0.00345 −0.000383 −0.00653
(0.00310) (0.00459) (0.0120)
Age of father 0.00362 0.000857 0.00275
(0.00290) (0.00458) (0.0117)
Age difference ratio −0.531 0.0522 −1.103
(0.508) (0.881) (2.169)
Respondent is male −0.0240* 0.00531 −0.0175
(0.0135) (0.0236) (0.0484)
2 or more children in the household −0.0185** 0.00426 0.0131
(0.00878) (0.00898) (0.0267)
Presence of other adults in the household −0.0138 −0.0180 0.0825**
(0.00854) (0.0144) (0.0380)
Only father has a university degree −0.00917 −0.0229 0.0438
(0.0173) (0.0150) (0.0469)
Only mother has a university degree 0.00297 −0.0218** 0.0375
(0.00799) (0.0108) (0.0257)
Both parents have a university degree −0.00514 −0.0375*** 0.0815***
(0.00948) (0.0107) (0.0283)
Both parents have non-Italian nationality −0.0219 0.0652*** −0.156**
(0.0202) (0.0247) (0.0764)



Province FE Yes Yes Yes
Day of week FE Yes Yes Yes



Observations 2098 1930 2067 1910 2067 1910
R-squared 0.059 0.139 0.084 0.155 0.047 0.111

*** p < 0.01, ** p < 0.05, * p < 0.1; Robust standard errors in brackets (clustered at province level).

Table A5.

Full regression estimates for children's emotional status.

Emotional status (1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6)
OLS OLS Female resp. Weight Cont. Var. Ologit1
Constant −0.553*** −0.850*** −0.879*** −0.849*** −0.826***
(0.0325) (0.157) (0.170) (0.237) (0.159)
Father main caregiver 0.158*** 0.154*** 0.160*** 0.124** 0.345***
(0.0415) (0.0469) (0.0492) (0.0557) (0.0966)
Variation in father's share of childcare 0.321**
(0.146)
Total parental homework hours −0.0241*** −0.0177** −0.0168** −0.0266*** −0.0163** −0.0425***
(0.00659) (0.00780) (0.00814) (0.00925) (0.00775) (0.0163)
Mother's share of housework −0.00848 −0.00898 −0.0186 −0.0116 0.0561 −0.0537
(0.149) (0.161) (0.173) (0.211) (0.170) (0.335)
Child is male −0.0483 −0.0493 0.0190 −0.0470 −0.116
(0.0356) (0.0374) (0.0520) (0.0357) (0.0744)
Age of child −0.0130 −0.0121 −0.00546 −0.0123 −0.0356
(0.0109) (0.0117) (0.0144) (0.0109) (0.0230)
Distance learning hours # kindergarten 0.0718 0.0630 0.101 0.0746 0.125
(0.0640) (0.0667) (0.0822) (0.0640) (0.132)
Distance learning hours # Primary school 0.176*** 0.167*** 0.136** 0.166*** 0.375***
(0.0517) (0.0541) (0.0683) (0.0519) (0.112)
Distance learning hours # Secondary school 0.365*** 0.378*** 0.269** 0.355*** 0.813***
(0.0886) (0.0950) (0.122) (0.0885) (0.191)
Hours of extra curricular activities 0.304*** 0.317*** 0.269*** 0.303*** 0.584***
(0.0463) (0.0495) (0.0655) (0.0466) (0.0914)
2 children in the household 0.0837* 0.101** 0.0545 0.0823* 0.216**
(0.0476) (0.0501) (0.0596) (0.0477) (0.0990)
3 children in the household 0.217*** 0.236*** 0.165* 0.200*** 0.582***
(0.0707) (0.0741) (0.0930) (0.0704) (0.151)
4 children in the household 0.555** 0.574** 0.723*** 0.572** 1.354***
(0.227) (0.233) (0.188) (0.238) (0.491)
More than 4 children in the household 1.022*** 1.012** 0.963*** 0.957** 2.184***
(0.393) (0.394) (0.293) (0.397) (0.548)
At least one parent is not Italian 0.174*** 0.175*** 0.230*** 0.175*** 0.411***
(0.0643) (0.0678) (0.0752) (0.0644) (0.120)
Mother university x Father university
No, Yes 0.107 0.0557 0.149 0.101 0.254
(0.0899) (0.0945) (0.126) (0.0899) (0.192)
Yes, No 0.171*** 0.153*** 0.118** 0.176*** 0.402***
(0.0529) (0.0548) (0.0589) (0.0533) (0.113)
Yes, Yes 0.195*** 0.190*** 0.222*** 0.202*** 0.410***
(0.0557) (0.0589) (0.0645) (0.0558) (0.118)
Mother stop working x Father stop working
No, Yes −0.0642 −0.0865 −0.129* −0.0632 −0.207
(0.0658) (0.0664) (0.0708) (0.0667) (0.144)
Yes, No 0.000288 0.00219 −0.0466 −0.00360 −0.0114
(0.0608) (0.0644) (0.0731) (0.0602) (0.131)
Yes, Yes −0.0785 −0.0798 −0.0882 −0.0851 −0.135
(0.0723) (0.0780) (0.0954) (0.0736) (0.146)
Mother switched to smart working −0.135** −0.122** −0.207*** −0.136** −0.332***
(0.0587) (0.0620) (0.0775) (0.0589) (0.128)
Both parents under strict lockdown 0.594*** 0.656*** 0.795** 0.631*** 1.097**
(0.216) (0.222) (0.384) (0.219) (0.460)
Survey respondent is male −0.0166 0.00831 0.0245 0.0937
(0.0749) (0.106) (0.0735) (0.151)
Date of interview −0.102*** −0.0931*** −0.0855** −0.109*** −0.210***
(0.0320) (0.0354) (0.0399) (0.0319) (0.0651)



Province FE Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes
Day of week FE Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes



Observations 3359 2640 2465 2625 2638 2688
R-squared2 0.011 0.134 0.134 0.179 0.132 0.0583

*** p < 0.01, ** p < 0.05, * p < 0.1; Robust standard errors in brackets (clustered at household level). Notes: (1) The constant is not reported for ordered logit estimation as it is separate for each level of the dependent variable. For the same reason, for each explanatory variable the table reports the coefficient, not the marginal effects. (2) For ordered logit estimation, the pseudo R-squared is reported.

Table A6.

Full regression estimates for parent-child relationship.

Personal relationship (1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6)
OLS OLS Female resp. Weight Cont. Var. Ologit1
Constant 0.130*** 0.149 0.166 0.178 0.176
(0.0334) (0.182) (0.195) (0.247) (0.182)
Father main caregiver 0.178*** 0.124** 0.114** 0.175*** 0.260***
(0.0452) (0.0534) (0.0561) (0.0634) (0.101)
Variation in father's share of childcare −0.0727
(0.166)
Total parental homework hours 0.00179 0.00950 0.00762 −0.000420 0.0118 0.0165
(0.00687) (0.00809) (0.00847) (0.0103) (0.00806) (0.0151)
Mother's share of housework 0.0544 −0.141 −0.0230 0.0550 −0.174 −0.340
(0.162) (0.183) (0.190) (0.229) (0.192) (0.357)
Child is male −0.131*** −0.137*** −0.0993* −0.131*** −0.253***
(0.0390) (0.0409) (0.0566) (0.0391) (0.0739)
Age of child −0.0181 −0.0183 −0.0306* −0.0177 −0.0386*
(0.0122) (0.0132) (0.0165) (0.0122) (0.0227)
Distance learning hours # kindergarden 0.0339 0.0229 −0.00886 0.0304 0.116
(0.0680) (0.0705) (0.0898) (0.0681) (0.130)
Distance learning hours # Primary school 0.106* 0.0998 0.112 0.103* 0.239**
(0.0596) (0.0630) (0.0753) (0.0595) (0.114)
Distance learning hours # Secondary school 0.323*** 0.329*** 0.311** 0.321*** 0.675***
(0.104) (0.112) (0.138) (0.104) (0.196)
Hours of extra curricular activities 0.151*** 0.142** 0.103 0.158*** 0.218***
(0.0528) (0.0558) (0.0712) (0.0527) (0.0842)
2 children in the household −0.0978* −0.107* −0.135** −0.104* −0.149
(0.0547) (0.0569) (0.0682) (0.0548) (0.103)
3 children in the household −0.0610 −0.0599 −0.132 −0.0843 −0.0595
(0.0777) (0.0818) (0.0920) (0.0769) (0.144)
4 children in the household 0.198 0.173 0.361 0.196 0.336
(0.214) (0.214) (0.309) (0.213) (0.421)
More than 4 children in the household 0.346 0.331 0.366 0.318 0.732
(0.406) (0.408) (0.298) (0.404) (0.617)
At least one parent is not Italian 0.147** 0.149** 0.150* 0.139** 0.258*
(0.0703) (0.0739) (0.0769) (0.0706) (0.135)
Mother university x Father university
No, Yes 0.0811 0.107 0.103 0.0789 0.143
(0.105) (0.112) (0.129) (0.105) (0.201)
Yes, No 0.0896 0.104 0.0615 0.0986 0.174
(0.0635) (0.0656) (0.0660) (0.0639) (0.119)
Yes, Yes 0.128** 0.148** 0.133* 0.146** 0.253**
(0.0615) (0.0642) (0.0748) (0.0614) (0.115)
Mother stop working x Father stop working
No, Yes −0.118* −0.130* −0.0250 −0.0983 −0.248*
(0.0709) (0.0725) (0.0855) (0.0711) (0.135)
Yes, No 0.0776 0.0869 0.195** 0.0560 0.122
(0.0692) (0.0725) (0.0853) (0.0702) (0.129)
Yes, Yes −0.0108 −0.00458 0.0841 −0.00568 −0.103
(0.0931) (0.100) (0.115) (0.0935) (0.173)
Mother switched to smart working −0.0627 −0.0735 −0.0777 −0.0609 −0.121
(0.0693) (0.0732) (0.0839) (0.0692) (0.134)
Both parents under strict lockdown 1.031*** 1.127*** 1.071*** 1.043*** 2.052***
(0.186) (0.165) (0.356) (0.184) (0.401)
Survey respondent is male −0.0172 0.135 0.0145 −0.00603
(0.0862) (0.138) (0.0865) (0.160)
Date of interview −0.110*** −0.115*** −0.102** −0.114*** −0.222***
(0.0334) (0.0362) (0.0464) (0.0334) (0.0618)



Province FE Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes
Day of week FE Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes



Observations 3375 2648 2473 2633 2646 2696
R-squared2 0.007 0.089 0.092 0.129 0.086 0.0339

*** p < 0.01, ** p < 0.05, * p < 0.1; Robust standard errors in brackets (clustered at household level). Notes: (1) The constant is not reported for ordered logit estimation as it is separate for each level of the dependent variable. For the same reason, for each explanatory variable the table reports the coefficient, not the marginal effects. (2) For ordered logit estimation, the pseudo R-squared is reported.

Table A7.

Full regression estimates for children's hours of TV.

Hours of TV (1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6)
OLS OLS Female resp. Weight Cont. Var. Ologit1
Constant 1.492*** 1.907*** 1.931*** 2.256*** 1.887***
(0.0518) (0.258) (0.269) (0.464) (0.258)
Father main caregiver −0.156** −0.147* −0.132* 0.00156 −0.220**
(0.0652) (0.0753) (0.0782) (0.105) (0.0968)
Variation in father's share of childcare −0.428*
(0.220)
Total parental homework hours 0.0256** 0.0216* 0.0182 0.00379 0.0209* 0.0388**
(0.0101) (0.0126) (0.0131) (0.0195) (0.0125) (0.0161)
Mother's share of housework 0.455* 0.633** 0.636** 0.784 0.533* 0.764**
(0.252) (0.297) (0.311) (0.502) (0.306) (0.359)
Child is male 0.0242 0.0274 −0.0302 0.0223 0.140*
(0.0611) (0.0633) (0.0916) (0.0611) (0.0783)
Age of child −0.0134 −0.0181 −0.0654** −0.0137 0.0112
(0.0186) (0.0195) (0.0305) (0.0186) (0.0225)
Distance learning hours # kindergarden −0.128 −0.142 −0.365** −0.133 −0.173
(0.101) (0.104) (0.144) (0.100) (0.128)
Distance learning hours # Primary school 0.146* 0.152* 0.181 0.155* 0.163
(0.0883) (0.0907) (0.136) (0.0883) (0.110)
Distance learning hours # Secondary school 0.0621 0.0398 0.225 0.0726 0.0262
(0.160) (0.167) (0.275) (0.160) (0.200)
Hours of extra curricular activities −0.400*** −0.432*** −0.434*** −0.395*** −0.518***
(0.0830) (0.0867) (0.129) (0.0832) (0.0968)
2 children in the household −0.0477 −0.0547 0.00963 −0.0480 −0.111
(0.0847) (0.0875) (0.125) (0.0848) (0.104)
3 children in the household −0.124 −0.117 −0.0645 −0.115 −0.256*
(0.118) (0.124) (0.162) (0.118) (0.149)
4 children in the household 0.249 0.242 0.370 0.241 0.320
(0.277) (0.276) (0.338) (0.274) (0.425)
More than 4 children in the household 0.520 0.608 0.536 0.590 0.912
(0.671) (0.642) (0.760) (0.682) (0.770)
At least one parent is not Italian 0.0370 0.0327 0.148 0.0337 0.0803
(0.121) (0.127) (0.134) (0.121) (0.158)
Mother university x Father university
No, Yes −0.0358 0.0350 0.153 −0.0352 −0.0487
(0.168) (0.170) (0.157) (0.167) (0.208)
Yes, No −0.0323 −0.0274 0.0159 −0.0398 −0.0930
(0.0949) (0.0973) (0.114) (0.0954) (0.125)
Yes, Yes −0.208** −0.198** −0.228** −0.213** −0.337***
(0.0909) (0.0938) (0.112) (0.0921) (0.117)
Mother stop working x Father stop working
No, Yes 0.102 0.117 0.152 0.108 0.220
(0.117) (0.119) (0.153) (0.119) (0.149)
Yes, No −0.107 −0.0882 0.119 −0.111 −0.0976
(0.0979) (0.102) (0.162) (0.0981) (0.121)
Yes, Yes 0.206 0.247* 0.296* 0.216* 0.249
(0.127) (0.131) (0.163) (0.127) (0.164)
Mother switched to smart working −0.0556 −0.0852 0.194 −0.0532 −0.0619
(0.111) (0.115) (0.174) (0.111) (0.144)
Both parents under strict lockdown −0.0461 0.0164 0.688 −0.0870 −0.396
(0.296) (0.310) (0.536) (0.298) (0.349)
Survey respondent is male −0.171 −0.280 −0.212 −0.211
(0.162) (0.221) (0.164) (0.177)
Date of interview 0.121** 0.0953 0.126 0.128** 0.129*
(0.0610) (0.0641) (0.0981) (0.0599) (0.0734)



Province FE Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes
Day of week FE Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes



Observations 3140 2470 2333 2455 2468 2511
R-squared2 0.007 0.094 0.096 0.127 0.094 0.0326

*** p < 0.01, ** p < 0.05, * p < 0.1; Robust standard errors in brackets (clustered at household level). Notes: (1) The constant is not reported for ordered logit estimation as it is separate for each level of the dependent variable. For the same reason, for each explanatory variable the table reports the coefficient, not the marginal effects. (2) For ordered logit estimation, the pseudo R-squared is reported.

Table A8.

Full regression estimates for children's reading hours.

Reading hours (1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6)
OLS OLS Female resp. Weight Cont. Var. Ologit1
Constant 0.152*** 0.244 0.258 0.317 0.242
(0.0311) (0.152) (0.160) (0.215) (0.152)
Father main caregiver 0.0273 −0.0166 −0.0350 0.0239 −0.0910
(0.0373) (0.0464) (0.0474) (0.0549) (0.112)
Variation in father's share of childcare −0.0538
(0.140)
Total parental homework hours 0.0240*** 0.00970 0.0108 0.0173 0.00964 0.0257
(0.00649) (0.00795) (0.00838) (0.0107) (0.00790) (0.0189)
Mother's share of housework 0.119 −0.00953 −0.0169 0.122 −0.0201 0.103
(0.171) (0.203) (0.210) (0.218) (0.204) (0.381)
Child is male −0.0423 −0.0402 −0.0699 −0.0425 −0.174**
(0.0367) (0.0383) (0.0487) (0.0368) (0.0853)
Age of child −0.00978 −0.0129 −0.0203 −0.00985 −0.0491*
(0.0111) (0.0117) (0.0147) (0.0112) (0.0271)
Distance learning hours # kindergarden 0.00286 −0.0229 0.136 0.00208 −0.0705
(0.0633) (0.0651) (0.0968) (0.0634) (0.153)
Distance learning hours # Primary school −0.0286 −0.0265 0.0656 −0.0273 −0.00522
(0.0505) (0.0528) (0.0770) (0.0505) (0.128)
Distance learning hours # Secondary school −0.0798 −0.0569 0.00707 −0.0782 −0.0360
(0.0987) (0.106) (0.147) (0.0992) (0.233)
Hours of extra curricular activities 0.0649 0.101** 0.0765 0.0657 0.0715
(0.0443) (0.0468) (0.0684) (0.0444) (0.0986)
2 children in the household −0.0467 −0.0540 −0.0677 −0.0471 −0.165
(0.0471) (0.0482) (0.0598) (0.0471) (0.112)
3 children in the household −0.0697 −0.0881 −0.00170 −0.0690 −0.244
(0.0625) (0.0649) (0.0797) (0.0620) (0.162)
4 children in the household −0.176 −0.195 −0.189 −0.178 −0.716**
(0.144) (0.141) (0.164) (0.143) (0.297)
More than 4 children in the household 0.273 0.264 0.450** 0.281 0.478
(0.249) (0.252) (0.224) (0.250) (0.551)
At least one parent is not Italian 0.118 0.121 0.0169 0.117 0.151
(0.0724) (0.0746) (0.0686) (0.0729) (0.154)
Mother university x Father university
No, Yes −0.118 −0.114 −0.200* −0.118 −0.0348
(0.147) (0.158) (0.119) (0.147) (0.270)
Yes, No 0.0840 0.0878 0.111* 0.0833 0.190
(0.0525) (0.0538) (0.0566) (0.0526) (0.130)
Yes, Yes 0.186*** 0.189*** 0.200*** 0.186*** 0.470***
(0.0540) (0.0556) (0.0699) (0.0543) (0.130)
Mother stop working x Father stop working
No, Yes −0.0188 −0.0150 0.0328 −0.0176 −0.0557
(0.0582) (0.0592) (0.0750) (0.0589) (0.146)
Yes, No −0.0399 −0.0478 0.0867 −0.0405 −0.108
(0.0586) (0.0628) (0.0757) (0.0585) (0.134)
Yes, Yes −0.0544 −0.0428 −0.00524 −0.0531 −0.241
(0.0744) (0.0783) (0.0859) (0.0747) (0.177)
Mother switched to smart working −0.0182 −0.0415 −0.0597 −0.0178 0.00436
(0.0627) (0.0650) (0.0928) (0.0630) (0.156)
Both parents under strict lockdown 0.646** 0.691** 0.116 0.641** 1.149**
(0.275) (0.287) (0.366) (0.275) (0.510)
Survey respondent is male 0.0467 0.0700 0.0427 0.0856
(0.0811) (0.0828) (0.0795) (0.181)
Date of interview −0.0629* −0.0889** −0.0166 −0.0620* −0.144*
(0.0342) (0.0369) (0.0459) (0.0342) (0.0798)



Province FE Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes
Day of week FE Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes



Observations 3107 2441 2306 2426 2439 2482
R-squared2 0.008 0.083 0.088 0.124 0.083 0.0388

*** p < 0.01, ** p < 0.05, * p < 0.1; Robust standard errors in brackets (clustered at household level). Notes: (1) The constant is not reported for ordered logit estimation as it is separate for each level of the dependent variable. For the same reason, for each explanatory variable the table reports the coefficient, not the marginal effects. (2) For ordered logit estimation, the pseudo R-squared is reported.

Table A9.

Full regression estimates for children's educational progress.

Educational progress (1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6)
OLS OLS Female resp. Weight Cont. Var. Ologit1
Constant −4.787*** −8.786*** −8.919*** −8.416*** −8.774***
(0.0952) (0.453) (0.464) (0.698) (0.454)
Father main caregiver 0.000515 0.195 0.203 0.360** 0.137
(0.134) (0.127) (0.132) (0.180) (0.0976)
Variation in father's share of childcare 0.251
(0.378)
Total parental homework hours −0.126*** −0.0566*** −0.0629*** −0.0709** −0.0542*** −0.0412***
(0.0198) (0.0186) (0.0191) (0.0295) (0.0185) (0.0142)
Mother's share of housework 0.176 0.301 0.206 0.0923 0.256 0.197
(0.472) (0.440) (0.460) (0.656) (0.452) (0.344)
Child is male −0.248*** −0.243** −0.338** −0.244** −0.204***
(0.0963) (0.0997) (0.144) (0.0965) (0.0724)
Age of child 0.230*** 0.232*** 0.213*** 0.234*** 0.173***
(0.0295) (0.0318) (0.0469) (0.0294) (0.0221)
Distance learning hours # kindergarden 1.735*** 1.669*** 1.672*** 1.741*** 1.348***
(0.190) (0.195) (0.260) (0.190) (0.142)
Distance learning hours # Primary school 2.465*** 2.452*** 2.195*** 2.440*** 1.842***
(0.160) (0.169) (0.249) (0.160) (0.126)
Distance learning hours # Secondary school 1.806*** 1.817*** 1.721*** 1.771*** 1.309***
(0.259) (0.281) (0.441) (0.259) (0.190)
Hours of extra curricular activities 0.258** 0.330*** 0.0945 0.256** 0.0901
(0.118) (0.123) (0.188) (0.119) (0.0790)
2 children in the household 0.113 0.114 0.227 0.118 0.0801
(0.132) (0.137) (0.179) (0.132) (0.0991)
3 children in the household 0.529*** 0.522*** 0.492* 0.512*** 0.362***
(0.187) (0.193) (0.252) (0.186) (0.137)
4 children in the household −0.419 −0.407 −0.479 −0.393 −0.324
(0.400) (0.393) (0.403) (0.417) (0.334)
More than 4 children in the household −0.236 −0.172 0.475 −0.300 −0.213
(0.712) (0.701) (0.959) (0.723) (0.636)
At least one parent is not Italian 0.243 0.288 0.0959 0.243 0.169
(0.191) (0.197) (0.197) (0.191) (0.145)
Mother university x Father university
No, Yes 0.582** 0.613** 0.622** 0.574** 0.380*
(0.253) (0.266) (0.292) (0.251) (0.199)
Yes, No 0.166 0.171 0.0512 0.174 0.0551
(0.152) (0.156) (0.171) (0.153) (0.115)
Yes, Yes 0.130 0.160 0.0546 0.133 0.0771
(0.151) (0.158) (0.185) (0.151) (0.113)
Mother stop working x Father stop working
No, Yes 0.0475 −0.0168 −0.0327 0.0516 0.0607
(0.178) (0.182) (0.246) (0.179) (0.137)
Yes, No 0.0423 0.00801 0.191 0.0308 −0.0190
(0.163) (0.171) (0.201) (0.164) (0.121)
Yes, Yes −0.0243 0.0170 0.102 −0.0257 −0.0500
(0.223) (0.232) (0.268) (0.225) (0.171)
Mother switched to smart working −0.0857 0.0184 0.146 −0.0987 −0.0228
(0.171) (0.178) (0.294) (0.171) (0.127)
Both parents under strict lockdown 1.274** 1.241** 1.427** 1.316** 1.239**
(0.577) (0.615) (0.669) (0.572) (0.486)
Survey respondent is male −0.408* −0.140 −0.359 −0.351**
(0.230) (0.342) (0.230) (0.171)
Date of interview 0.0163 0.0543 0.0141 0.00521 0.0158
(0.0793) (0.0870) (0.123) (0.0794) (0.0589)



Province FE Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes
Day of week FE Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes



Observations 2796 2578 2407 2563 2576 2626
R-squared2 0.019 0.316 0.317 0.299 0.315 0.0810

*** p < 0.01, ** p < 0.05, * p < 0.1; Robust standard errors in brackets (clustered at household level). Notes: (1) The constant is not reported for ordered logit estimation as it is separate for each level of the dependent variable. For the same reason, for each explanatory variable the table reports the coefficient, not the marginal effects. (2) For ordered logit estimation, the pseudo R-squared is reported.

Appendix C. Supplementary Data

The following are the supplementary data to this article:

mmc1.pdf (96.7KB, pdf)

References

  1. Adams-Prassl A., Boneva T., Golin M., Rauh C. Institute of Labor Economics (IZA); 2020. Inequality in the Impact of the Coronavirus Shock: Evidence from Real Time Surveys. IZA Discussion Papers 13183. [Google Scholar]
  2. Alon T.M., Doepke M., Olmstead-Rumsey J., Tertilt M. 2020. The Impact of COVID-19 on Gender Equality. NBER Working Paper No. 26947. [Google Scholar]
  3. Amuedo-Dorantes C., Marcén M., Morales M., Sevilla A. Institute of Labor Economics (IZA); 2020. October COVID-19 School Closures and Parental Labor Supply in the United States. IZA Discussion Papers 13827. [Google Scholar]
  4. Andrew A., Cattan S., Costa-Dias M., Farquharson C., Kraftman L., Krutikova S., Phimister A., Sevilla A. 2020. Learning During the Lockdown: Real-Time Data on Children’s Experiences During Home Learning. IFS Briefing Note BN288. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Andrew A., Cattan S., Costa Dias M., Farquharson C., Kraftman L., Krutikova S., Phimister A., Sevilla A. Institute of Labor Economics (IZA); 2020. The Gendered Division of Paid and Domestic Work under Lockdown. IZA Discussion Papers 13500. [Google Scholar]
  6. Angrist J.D., Battistin E., Vuri D. In a small moment: class size and moral hazard in the Italian mezzogiorno. Am. Econ. J. Appl. Econ. 2017;9(4):216–249. [Google Scholar]
  7. Barigozzi F., Di Timoteo C., Monfardini C., et al. 2020. Italian Families in the 21st Century: Gender Gaps in Time Use and Their Evolution. IZA Discussion Paper No. 13348. [Google Scholar]
  8. Becker G.S. Harvard University Press; 1981. A Treatise on the Family. [Google Scholar]
  9. Biroli P., Bosworth S., Della Giusta M., Di Girolamo A., Jaworska S., Vollen J. 2020. Family Life in Lockdown. IZA Discussion Papers 13398. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bloemen H.G., Pasqua S., Stancanelli E.G. An empirical analysis of the time allocation of Italian couples: are they responsive? Rev. Econ. Household. 2010;8(3):345–369. [Google Scholar]
  11. Briscese G., Lacetera N., Macis M., Tonin M. 2020. Compliance with COVID-19 Social-Distancing Measures in Italy: The Role of Expectations and Duration. NBER Working Paper No. 26916. [Google Scholar]
  12. Brunello G., Checchi D. School quality and family background in Italy. Econ. Educ. Rev. 2005;24(5):563–577. [Google Scholar]
  13. Champeaux H., Mangiavacchi L., Piccoli L., Marchetta F. Learning at home: distance learning solutions and child development during the COVID-19 lockdown. CEPR Covid Econ. 2021;68:69–99. [Google Scholar]
  14. Cunha F., Heckman J.J., Schennach S.M. Estimating the technology of cognitive and noncognitive skill formation. Econometrica. 2010;78(3):883–931. doi: 10.3982/ECTA6551. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Del Boca D., Monfardini C., Nicoletti C. Parental and child time investments and the cognitive development of adolescents. J. Labor Econ. 2017;35(2):565–608. [Google Scholar]
  16. Del Boca D., Oggero N., Profeta P., Rossi M. 2020. Women’s Work, Housework and Childcare, Before and During COVID-19. IZA Discussion Paper No. 13409. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Engzell, P., Frey, A., Verhagen M.M., Learning inequality during the covid-19 pandemic. SocArXiv ve4z7.
  18. Farré L., Fawaz Y., González L., Graves J. 2020. How the COVID-19 Lockdown Affected Gender Inequality in Paid and Unpaid Work in Spain. IZA Discussion Paper No. 13434. [Google Scholar]
  19. Fiorini M., Keane M.P. How the allocation of children’s time affects cognitive and noncognitive development. J. Labor Econ. 2014;32(4):787–836. [Google Scholar]
  20. Giménez-Nadal J.I., Mangiavacchi L., Piccoli L. Keeping inequality at home: the genesis of gender roles in housework. Labour Econ. 2019;58:52–68. [Google Scholar]
  21. Grewenig E., Lergetporer P., Werner K., Woessmann L., Zierow L., et al. CESifo; 2020. COVID-19 and Educational Inequality: How School Closures Affect Low-and High-Achieving Students. Technical Report. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Harris K.M., Furstenberg F.F., Marmer J.K. Paternal involvement with adolescents in intact families: the influence of fathers over the life course. Demography. 1998;35(2):201–216. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Hsin A., Felfe C. When does time matter? Maternal employment, children's time with parents, and child development. Demography. 2014;51(5):1867–1894. doi: 10.1007/s13524-014-0334-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Hupkau C., Petrongolo B. Centre for Economic Performance, LSE; 2020. May Work, Care and Gender During the COVID-19 Crisis. CEP COVID-19 Briefings cepcovid-19-002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Jaume D., Willén A. The long-run effects of teacher strikes: evidence from Argentina. J. Labor Econ. 2019;37(4):1097–1139. [Google Scholar]
  26. Mancini A.L., Monfardini C., Pasqua S. Is a good example the best sermon? children’s imitation of parental reading. Rev. Econ. Household. 2017;15(3):965–993. [Google Scholar]
  27. Mangiavacchi L., Piccoli L. Parental alcohol consumption and adult children’s educational attainment. Econ. Hum. Biol. 2018;28:132–145. doi: 10.1016/j.ehb.2017.12.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Mencarini L., Pasqua S., Romiti A. Single-mother families and the gender gap in children’s time investment and non-cognitive skills. Rev. Econ. Household. 2019;17(1):149–176. [Google Scholar]
  29. Moroni G., Nicoletti C., Tominey E. 2019. Child Socio-Emotional Skills: The Role of Parental Inputs, IZA Discussion Paper No. 12432. [Google Scholar]
  30. Moroni G., Nicoletti C., Tominey E. CEPR; 2020. Children’s Socio-Emotional Skills and the Home Environment During the COVID-19 Crisis. VoxEU.org. [Google Scholar]
  31. Pieroni L., Lanari D. The role of fathers in the criminal careers of juveniles in Italy. J. Demogr. Econ. 2018;84(4):419–445. [Google Scholar]
  32. Pischke J.-S. The impact of length of the school year on student performance and earnings: evidence from the German short school years. Econ. J. 2007;117(523):1216–1242. [Google Scholar]
  33. Sevilla A., Smith S. 2020. Baby Steps: The Gender Division of Childcare During the COVID-19 Pandemic. IZA Discussion Paper No. 13302. [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

mmc1.pdf (96.7KB, pdf)

Articles from Economics and Human Biology are provided here courtesy of Elsevier

RESOURCES