Skip to main content
Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences logoLink to Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences
. 2022 Dec 13;31:e90. doi: 10.1017/S2045796022000725

Preclinical and clinical evidence on the approach-avoidance conflict evaluation as an integrative tool for psychopathology

F Rusconi 1,, M G Rossetti 2, C Forastieri 1, V Tritto 1, M Bellani 3,, E Battaglioli 1
PMCID: PMC9762142  PMID: 36510831

Abstract

The approach-avoidance conflict (AAC), i.e. the competing tendencies to undertake goal-directed actions or to withdraw from everyday life challenges, stands at the basis of humans' existence defining behavioural and personality domains. Gray's Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory posits that a stable bias toward approach or avoidance represents a psychopathological trait associated with excessive sensitivity to reward or punishment. Optogenetic studies in rodents and imaging studies in humans associated with cross-species AAC paradigms granted new emphasis to the hippocampus as a hub of behavioural inhibition. For instance, recent functional neuroimaging studies show that functional brain activity in the human hippocampus correlates with threat perception and seems to underlie passive avoidance. Therefore, our commentary aims to (i) discuss the inhibitory role of the hippocampus in approach-related behaviours and (ii) promote the integration of functional neuroimaging with cross-species AAC paradigms as a means of diagnostic, therapeutic, follow up and prognosis refinement in psychiatric populations.

Key words: Biological markers, brain imaging techniques, diagnosis and classification, psychological assessment


Decision-making results from a complex system of corticolimbic brain structures: the mesolimbic pathway is responsible for encoding motivation and promoting actions (Goto and Grace, 2005), while the hippocampus, the amygdala and the prefrontal cortex (PFC) assign an affective value to life experiences (Calhoon and Tye, 2015). These areas integrate the dopaminergic activity of the mesolimbic pathway, ultimately promoting or inhibiting the action (Adhikari et al., 2010; Abela et al., 2013; Abela and Chudasama, 2014). Thus, a healthy individual undertakes goal-directed actions as a result of an inner conflict among corticolimbic brain areas, which ultimately drives the behavioural choice to approach or avoid a life paradigm, following a continuous gradient of integration between needs, rewards and risks (Cornwell et al., 2014). This ‘conflict’ is usually referred to as the approach-avoidance conflict (AAC) and its disbalance, in both directions, represents a highly shared symptom of several psychiatric disorders (Aupperle and Paulus, 2010; Aupperle et al., 2015; Kirlic et al., 2017; Loijen et al., 2020). According to Gray's Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory (RST), approach-avoidance behaviour arises from a balanced interaction between the Behavioural Approach System (BAS) and the Behavioural Inhibition System (BIS) (Gray, 1970; Gray and McNaughton, 2003; Bijttebier et al., 2009). In mammals, BAS promotes behavioural responses to appetitive and rewarding stimuli, while BIS organises individuals' responses to punishing or threatening situations (Gray and McNaughton, 2003). BIS and BAS are informed by implicit and explicit information processing, involved in positive or negative valence attribution to specific cues (Loijen et al., 2020). Implicit information processing is fast and emotion-driven and depends on subcortical brain regions involved in affective processing (i.e. mesolimbic areas including the hippocampus and amygdala). Conversely, explicit information processing is relatively slow, requires intentional reasoning and attentional efforts, and seems to depend on frontal brain regions associated with cognitive control and emotional reappraisal (Wager et al., 2008; Aupperle and Paulus, 2010). Gray's RST assumes that individuals with an unbalanced BIS/BAS ratio are at risk of psychiatric drift (Loijen et al., 2020). Consistently, dysfunctional approach-avoidance tendencies have been implicated in the development and progression of several mental health disorders such as anxiety, depression, eating and addictive disorders and schizophrenia (Bijttebier et al., 2009; Struijs et al., 2017; Loijen et al., 2020). We aim to emphasise the translational role of AAC paradigms in clinical psychiatric research, by discussing preclinical and clinical evidence of the brain circuits associated with AAC.

Cross-species paradigms to assess AAC

Cross-species behavioural tests aim at resolving shared molecular and circuital determinants between humans and rodents that underlie homologous disease-relevant behaviours in the AAC domain (Bach, 2021). This approach should build a more solid basis to develop novel tools to help clinical diagnosis and maximise the predictive validity of AAC preclinical paradigms used to test pharmacological interventions for humans (Belzung and Lemoine, 2011). Cross-species behavioural tests to assess AAC mainly exploit paradigms of exploration (Gromer et al., 2021). Mammals' functioning is indeed based on an innate exploratory drive for territorial recognition, food supply and reproduction, which are in turn associated with a significant reward (Kidd and Hayden, 2015). However, exploration naturally entails a conflict between its reward component, and the objective risk (Arzate-Mejía et al., 2020; Italia et al., 2020). Thus, explorative engagement results from a fine balance between motivation (driven by reward) and refusal (supported by fear) (Blanco et al., 2013; La-Vu et al., 2020). Exploratory avoidance (which also includes to some extent social avoidance) is a pathological status that can either be caused by a loss of the natural desire to explore and socialise (anhedonia) or can be the result of a pathological fear (anxiety) (Kim and Kirkpatrick, 1996; Arzate-Mejía et al., 2020). The first case is more evocative of a depressive-like drift, the second is associated with an aberrant state of anxiety which prevents an individual from indulging in social and exploratory needs, even though social and exploratory desire can be intact (Bijttebier et al., 2009). In the following sections, we describe those AAC paradigms that are suitable for both humans and rodents.

In rodents, one of the most used exploratory paradigms to assess AAC is the Open Field (OF) test. It consists of a simple observation of animal deambulatory behaviour (Prut and Belzung, 2003). Modern software digitally traces the distance moved by the rodent from the perimetric walls to the centre of an arena. The longer the time spent and the distance walked in the centre, the more a rodent is prone to ‘approach’ exploration (Noldus et al., 2001). Conversely, animals walking near the perimetric walls of the cage (also known as thigmotaxis) adopt an instinctive behaviour aimed at protecting against a perceived novelty-related threat (Prut and Belzung, 2003). In literature, the OF test is almost often described as an anxiety-assessing paradigm. However, it is important to underline that decreased exploration is also due to decreased exploration-related reward sensitivity (Blanco et al., 2013; Cornwell et al., 2014; La-Vu et al., 2020).

Human versions of the OF test have been developed and administered to clinical/non-clinical groups. Kallai et al., measured human thigmotaxis during the exploration of virtual and physical spaces, showing that thigmotaxis positively correlated with fear and avoidance bias for fear-mobilising situations during early trials of both tasks, but not with self-reported trait anxiety (Kallai et al., 2007). Walz et al., instructed patients with agoraphobia and healthy controls to perform a 15 min solitary walk on a 146 × 79 m soccer field. Patients with agoraphobia and participants with high self-reported anxiety sensitivity exhibited enhanced thigmotaxis (Walz et al., 2016). In an additional virtual reality OF test performed on 141 individuals, the participants – like rodents in animal studies – preferred to stay in the outer region of the open field but there was no consistent association between thigmotaxis and self-report scales of anxiety and fear (Gromer et al., 2021). Overall, human OF test demonstrated cross-species validity, although, the modulatory effects of anxiety on human open-field behaviour should be further examined (Bach, 2021).

Another exploratory paradigm frequently used in rodents is the elevated plus maze (EPM) test (Walf and Frye, 2007). The maze, a cross-like structure mounted on elevated strut, is made up of two open and two closed arms where opaque plexiglass walls protect the rodent path (Rusconi et al., 2016). Closed arms are more comfortable for rodents and represent the preferred part of the maze. However, instinctive propensity for exploration prompts the animals to abandon the closed arms for the open ones for a short time. The percentage of time spent and the frequency of open arms entries represent a reliable readout of the rodents' propensity to approach. EPM test is largely used as an anxiety-assessment test that, however, does not take into consideration the reward-related drive to explore. Thus, these two tests specifically measure the AAC, as the result of the contribution of two components: anxiety and reward sensitivity (Cornwell et al., 2014; Bryant and Barker, 2020).

In humans, the EPM corresponding paradigm is the Mixed Reality version of EPM. Biedermann et al. (2017) translated the rodent EPM test to humans using a combination of real-world and virtual elements namely, a real-world wooden maze combined with a representation of this maze in virtual reality. Briefly, participants were instructed to step into the maze and walk slowly towards its centre, and wait for the scene to change before exploring the environment of the maze. After 90 s, the scenario switched and, instead of being in a virtual room, the maze was placed on a virtual rocky mountain surrounded by water. The subjects were allowed to explore the EPM for 300 s and, reporting higher anxiety about open arms, they preferentially avoided them. This tendency increased or decreased when they were given the anxiogenic yohimbine and the benzodiazepine lorazepam, respectively.

Other cross-species approaches featuring operant conflict tests were developed to emphasise both anxiety and reward sensitivity components thus further enhancing the conflict load of the choice (Bach, 2021). These tests are based on the association of a specific reward or punishment to a given action. In rodents, the Vogel conflict test (VCT) represents one of the best constructs of AAC (Millan and Brocco, 2003). In this paradigm, within a habituation phase, a thirsty animal learns to drink from a metallic gauge. During the trial phase, after a few licks, the animal receives a mild electric shock. Depending on the relative balance between the motivation to seek the drinking reward and facing the punishing shock, the rodent will stop or keep on drinking. The number of shocks the animal decides to stand, directly correlates with its approach behaviour. Another similar test, the Geller-Seifter Conflict Test (GSCT) (Geller et al., 1960) exploits a food-related reward instead of water. Although the human counterparts of the VCT and GSCT (described below) are less similar to the rodent variants than those of the OF and EPM, evidence suggests that these tests are equally valuable within cross-species approaches (Bach, 2021). Aupperle et al. (2011) developed a third-person view computer task, named ACC conflict task, in which human participants move an avatar on a runway to decide between their chances of receiving a conflict outcome (negative affective image/sound combined with point rewards) v. non-conflict outcome (affective image/sound coupled with no points). The trials were designed to elicit the simultaneous desire to approach the reward and avoid the negative affective punishment. A limitation of this paradigm is that the reward offered during the conflicting conditions can vary while the affective threat remains stable. As such, the task allows to study conflict-related neuronal activations that are associated with the higher salience of the reward, but not those that might be elicited by increased salience of the negative outcome. Within a similar rationale, two additional human-designed conflict tests, based on third-person view computer tasks, were developed by Bach et al. (2014). In these tasks, the player is instructed to press a key (Bach, 2015) or move on a rectangular grid (Bach et al., 2014) to collect virtual tokens under the threat of being caught by a virtual ‘predator’ and losing all previously collected tokens. Threat probability corresponds to the wake-up rate of the predator, and the magnitude of potential loss corresponds to the number of already collected tokens. The wake-up rate is signalled by different colours and tailored to result in 3 different wake-up probabilities if the player stays outside the safe place for 100 ms. The player cannot escape once the predator is active. When participants have to press the key, they tend to collect fewer tokens when the potential loss is higher (Bach, 2015). When participants have to move on the screen, they tend to explore and collect tokens early on, but as time progresses, the subjects retreat more to the safe place.

The inhibitory role of the hippocampus in approach-related behaviours

In the ‘80 Jeffrey Gray and Neil McNaughton suggested that the mammalian hippocampus may represent a central component of the BIS, hence sustaining avoidance within the AAC (McNaughton and Gray, 2000; Gray and McNaughton, 2003). This interesting theory initially accounted for robust data showing how partial or total surgical hippocampus ablation in rodents leads to increased approach behaviours. Similarly, it was shown that local hippocampal infusion of anxiolytic drugs that inhibit excitatory hippocampal neurotransmission enhances approach-related actions (Gray and McNaughton, 2003). Lately, many studies described hippocampal functional polarisation, showing how the hippocampus is grossly divided into two portions, dorsal and ventral hippocampus (dHIP; vHIP) in rodents, respectively involved in spatial memory consolidation and affective/emotional processing (Kheirbek et al., 2013; Jimenez et al., 2018). Interestingly, anatomical segregation of hippocampal circuits has also been described in humans, being the anterior hippocampus homologous to the rodent vHIP and the posterior to the dHIP (Clark and Squire, 2013). Thus, these studies better address the vHIP in rodents and the anterior in humans as a relevant seat of emotional information processing possibly related to behavioural avoidance. In the following sections, we examine the latest experimental evidence, in particular optogenetic-mediated surgical circuitry characterisation in rodents, and fMRI in humans, supporting the role of the hippocampus in behavioural avoidance, and further endowing Gray's RST with spatial, molecular and metabolic determinants.

Rodents optogenetic studies

Optogenetics refers to a biological technique to control the activity of genetically labelled neurons with light, an approach that significantly contributed in the last years to map brain functional connectivity (Adamantidis et al., 2015).

One of the first optogenetic evidence of hippocampal involvement in the approach-avoidance outcome showed that specific inhibition of glutamatergic neurons of the basolateral amygdala projecting to the vHIP promoted exploratory approach measured by the EPM test (Felix-Ortiz et al., 2013), while optogenetic activation of the same circuit limited exploration of the EPM open arms, increasing avoidance (Felix-Ortiz et al., 2013). Another interesting study outlined the role of an additional vHIP efferent pathway, directed to the medial PFC in the modulation of approach-avoidance (Padilla-Coreano et al., 2016). In particular, optogenetic inhibition of vHIP axon terminals projecting to the medial PFC biases the AAC towards approach behaviours measured as increased exploration in the EPM test, a profile that was further validated by the Novelty Suppressed Feeding test (Padilla-Coreano et al., 2016).

Recently, it was also described that optogenetic enhancement of the excitatory vHIP afferents that project to the lateral hypothalamus increases anxiety, shifting the AAC conflict toward exploratory avoidance as scored by the OF and EPM tests (Jimenez et al., 2018).

Unexpectedly, a similar positive optogenetic manipulation that was performed over those vHIP afferents that innervate basal amygdala (BA), limited fear memory encoding and retrieval in the contextual fear conditioning test but displayed no effect in OF test readouts (Jimenez et al., 2018). The authors concluded that positive manipulation of these two vHIP glutamatergic afferents affects different emotional domains in rodents. However, the inhibitory activity of the vHIP-BA circuit toward fear memory consolidation contrasts with the potential involvement of the hippocampus in behavioural avoidance. In general, the stronger the fear memory the less an animal will be engaged in approach behaviours. It is possible that, within the complexity of limbic circuitry, inner homoeostatic needs leave a minority of the hippocampal circuitry (including vHIP-BA) free to contribute to approach, while the majority, as reviewed here, contributes to behavioural avoidance; therefore, balanced functioning of these circuitries would serve to support adaptive behaviours. The aforementioned evidence suggests the hippocampus as a brain area involved in the discrimination of those advantages and potential threats that have to be weighted in decision making.

Chronic environmental stress including psychosocial trauma has been shown to modulate AAC towards avoidance in vulnerable mice (Toth and Neumann, 2013; Anacker et al., 2018). Thus, a question raises about whether the hippocampus contributes to translating stress into avoidance. In mice, chronic social defeat stress diminishes, in a subset of susceptible animals, the willingness to socially explore conspecific animals and exploratory behaviour. Such susceptibility is promptly reverted to resiliency by negatively regulating hippocampus excitability (Anacker et al., 2018). Interestingly, has also been shown that chronic treatment with imipramine, a tricyclic antidepressant drug increasing serotonin levels in the synaptic cleft, is able to restore a normal approach-avoidance balance in psychosocial stress susceptible mice previously evaluated as social avoidants (Tsankova et al., 2006). This effect is again mediated by an overall decrease of hippocampal excitability, in accordance with the inhibitory effect of the hippocampal serotonin receptors, 5HT1A, highly abundant in this area (Tsankova et al., 2006).

Humans' studies

Recent functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), and pharmacological and brain lesions studies confirm a relevant role for the hippocampus in the AAC in humans (Kheirbek et al., 2013; Bach et al., 2014; Weeden et al., 2015; Ito and Lee, 2016; Schumacher et al., 2018; Bach et al., 2019; Abivardi et al., 2020; Bryant and Barker, 2020; La-Vu et al., 2020; Yeates et al., 2020). For instance, Bach et al., conducted a fMRI study to investigate the role of the hippocampus in arbitrating ACC under various levels of potential threat, comparing neurologically healthy controls and patients with selective hippocampal lesions (Bach et al., 2014). Bold signal in the anterior hippocampus increased with the probability of predator attack, supporting the putative role of the hippocampus as a negative regulator of approach in AAC paradigms in humans. Importantly, the threat levels much less influenced the behaviour of patients with selective hippocampal lesions, which showed reduced passive avoidance behaviour and inhibition across all threat levels (Bach et al., 2014). The same AAC computer game was then used to investigate the impact of benzodiazepines and amygdala lesions on putative human anxiety-like behaviour (Korn et al., 2017). The task was administered to (i) a group of healthy controls after a single dose of lorazepam v. placebo and (ii) two patients with bilateral amygdala lesions v. age- and gender-matched healthy controls. Lorazepam and amygdala lesions reduced loss adaptation, decreasing patients' anxiety-related avoidance behaviour.

A more recent study from the same group (Bach et al., 2019) confirmed that, in humans, hippocampal lesions increase approach under conflict whereas amygdalar lesions impair the return to safety.

In summary, also human studies report a role of the hippocampus and amygdala in AAC under threat, linking the integrity of these regions to conditioned fear expression and inhibitory avoidance (Ito and Lee, 2016). Such new knowledge, however, warrants further inherent neuroimaging studies, based in particular on fMRI to better dissect inherent circuitry.

Conclusion and future directions

Deepening the knowledge of AAC circuitry and mechanisms in rodents and humans holds a huge translational potential as it may help to unravel psychopathological elements of several psychiatric disorders featuring unbalanced AAC. Further studies combining hippocampus-focused functional brain imaging using the described AAC cross-species paradigms with clinical (i.e. questionnaire-based) evaluation of AAC and anxiety, should be performed to validate preliminary observation of increased hippocampal activity as a biomarker of threat or punishment sensitivity and avoidance, ultimately helping to refine psychiatric patient stratification and diagnosis along with treatment options and prognosis.

Acknowledgements

We acknowledge Paolo Brambilla for stimulating discussions and critical reading of the manuscript. We also acknowledge Cariplo Foundation Grant 2016-0908 to EB, Competitive Research Grant KAUST Grant 2019 to EB, PSR_2019 to FR and SEED Seal of Excellence (University of Milan) to FR.

Data

All data used to write this paper is in the reference list.

Conflict of interest

None.

References

  1. Abela AR and Chudasama Y (2014) Noradrenergic α2A-receptor stimulation in the ventral hippocampus reduces impulsive decision-making. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 231, 521–531. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Abela AR, Dougherty SD, Fagen ED, Hill CJ and Chudasama Y (2013) Inhibitory control deficits in rats with ventral hippocampal lesions. Cerebral Cortex 23, 1396–1409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Abivardi A, Khemka S and Bach DR (2020) Hippocampal representation of threat features and behavior in a human approach-avoidance conflict anxiety task. Journal of Neuroscience 40, 6748–6758. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Adamantidis A, Arber S, Bains JS, Bamberg E, Bonci A, Buzsáki G, Cardin JA, Costa RM, Dan Y, Goda Y, Graybiel AM, Häusser M, Hegemann P, Huguenard JR, Insel TR, Janak PH, Johnston D, Josselyn SA, Koch C, Kreitzer AC, Lüscher C, Malenka RC, Miesenböck G, Nagel G, Roska B, Schnitzer MJ, Shenoy KV, Soltesz I, Sternson SM, Tsien RW, Tsien RY, Turrigiano GG, Tye KM and Wilson RI (2015) Optogenetics: 10 years after ChR2 in neurons – views from the community. Nature Neuroscience 18, 1202–1212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Adhikari A, Topiwala MA and Gordon JA (2010) Synchronized activity between the ventral hippocampus and the medial prefrontal cortex during anxiety. Neuron 65, 257–269. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Anacker C, Luna VM, Stevens GS, Millette A, Shores R, Jimenez JC, Chen B and Hen R (2018) Hippocampal neurogenesis confers stress resilience by inhibiting the ventral dentate gyrus. Nature 559(7712), 98–102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Arzate-Mejía RG, Lottenbach Z, Schindler V, Jawaid A and Mansuy IM (2020) Long-term impact of social isolation and molecular underpinnings. Frontiers in Genetics 11, 589621. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Aupperle RL and Paulus MP (2010) Neural systems underlying approach and avoidance in anxiety disorders. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience 12, 517–531. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Aupperle RL, Sullivan S, Melrose AJ, Paulus MP and Stein MB (2011) A reverse translational approach to quantify approach-avoidance conflict in humans. Behavioural Brain Research 225, 455–463. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Aupperle RL, Melrose AJ, Francisco A, Paulus MP and Stein MB (2015) Neural substrates of approach-avoidance conflict decision-making. Human Brain Mapping 36, 449–462. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bach DR (2015) Anxiety-like behavioural inhibition is normative under environmental threat-reward correlations. PLoS Computational Biology 11, e1004646. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Bach DR (2021) Cross-species anxiety tests in psychiatry: pitfalls and promises. Molecular Psychiatry. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Bach DR, Guitart-Masip M, Packard PA, Miró J, Falip M, Fuentemilla L and Dolan RJ (2014) Human hippocampus arbitrates approach-avoidance conflict. Current Biology 24, 1435. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Bach DR, Hoffmann M, Finke C, Hurlemann R and Ploner CJ (2019) Disentangling hippocampal and amygdala contribution to human anxiety-like behavior. Journal of Neuroscience 39, 8517–8526. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Belzung C and Lemoine M (2011) Criteria of validity for animal models of psychiatric disorders: focus on anxiety disorders and depression. Biology of Mood & Anxiety Disorders 1, 9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Biedermann SV, Biedermann DG, Wenzlaff F, Kurjak T, Nouri S, Auer MK, Wiedemann K, Briken P, Haaker J, Lonsdorf TB and Fuss J (2017) An elevated plus-maze in mixed reality for studying human anxiety-related behavior. BMC Biology 15, 125. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Bijttebier P, Beck I, Claes L and Vandereycken W (2009) Gray's reinforcement sensitivity theory as a framework for research on personality-psychopathology associations. Clinical Psychology Review 29, 421–430. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Blanco NJ, Otto AR, Maddox WT, Beevers CG and Love BC (2013) The influence of depression symptoms on exploratory decision-making. Cognition 129, 563–568. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Bryant KG and Barker JM (2020) Arbitration of approach-avoidance conflict by ventral hippocampus. Frontiers in Neuroscience 14, 615337. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Calhoon GG and Tye KM (2015) Resolving the neural circuits of anxiety. Nature Neuroscience 18, 1394–1404. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Clark RE and Squire LR (2013) Similarity in form and function of the hippocampus in rodents, monkeys, and humans. Proceeding National Academy of Science 110(Suppl. 2), 10365–10370. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Cornwell JF, Franks B and Higgins ET (2014) Truth, control, and value motivations: the ‘what’, ‘how’, and ‘why’ of approach and avoidance. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience 8, 194. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Felix-Ortiz AC, Beyeler A, Seo C, Leppla CA, Wildes CP and Tye KM (2013) BLA to vHPC inputs modulate anxiety-related behaviors. Neuron 79, 658–664. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Geller I, Demarco AO and Seifter J (1960) Delayed effects of nicotine on timing behavior in the rat. Science (New York, N.Y.) 131, 735–737. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Goto Y and Grace AA (2005) Dopaminergic modulation of limbic and cortical drive of nucleus accumbens in goal-directed behavior. Nature Neuroscience 8, 805–812. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Gray JA (1970) The psychophysiological basis of introversion-extraversion. Behavior Research and Therapy 8, 249–266. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Gray JA and McNaughton N (2003) The Neuropsychology of Anxiety: An Enquiry into the Function of the Septo-Hippocampal System, 2nd Edn. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  28. Gromer D, Kiser DP and Pauli P (2021) Thigmotaxis in a virtual human open field test. Scientific Reports 11, 6670. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Italia M, Forastieri C, Longaretti A, Battaglioli E and Rusconi F (2020) Rationale, relevance, and limits of stress-induced psychopathology in rodents as models for psychiatry research: an introductory overview. International Journal of Molecular Sciences 21, 7455. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Ito R and Lee ACH (2016) The role of the hippocampus in approach-avoidance conflict decision-making: evidence from rodent and human studies. Behavioural Brain Research 313, 345–357. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Jimenez JC, Su K, Goldberg AR, Luna VM, Biane JS, Ordek G, Zhou P, Ong SK, Wright MA, Zweifel L, Paninski L, Hen R and Kheirbek MA (2018) Anxiety cells in a hippocampal-hypothalamic circuit. Neuron 97, 670–683.e676. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Kallai J, Makany T, Csatho A, Karadi K, Horvath D, Kovacs-Labadi B, Jarai R, Nadel L and Jacobs JW (2007) Cognitive and affective aspects of thigmotaxis strategy in humans. Behavioral Neuroscience 121, 21–30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Kheirbek MA, Drew LJ, Burghardt NS, Costantini DO, Tannenholz L, Ahmari SE, Zeng H, Fenton AA and Hen R (2013) Differential control of learning and anxiety along the dorsoventral axis of the dentate gyrus. Neuron 77, 955–968. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Kidd C and Hayden BY (2015) The psychology and neuroscience of curiosity. Neuron 88, 449–460. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Kim JW and Kirkpatrick B (1996) Social isolation in animal models of relevance to neuropsychiatric disorders. Biological Psychiatry 40, 918–922. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Kirlic N, Young J and Aupperle RL (2017) Animal to human translational paradigms relevant for approach avoidance conflict decision making. Behaviour Research Therapy 96, 14–29. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Korn CW, Vunder J, Miró J, Fuentemilla L, Hurlemann R and Bach DR (2017) Amygdala lesions reduce anxiety-like behavior in a human benzodiazepine-sensitive approach-avoidance conflict test. Biological Psychiatry 82, 522–531. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. La-Vu M, Tobias BC, Schuette PJ and Adhikari A (2020) To approach or avoid: an introductory overview of the study of anxiety using rodent assays. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 14, 145. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Loijen A, Vrijsen JN, Egger JIM, Becker ES and Rinck M (2020) Biased approach-avoidance tendencies in psychopathology: a systematic review of their assessment and modification. Clinical Psychology Review 77, 101825. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. McNaughton N and Gray JA (2000) Anxiolytic action on the behavioural inhibition system implies multiple types of arousal contribute to anxiety. Journal of Affective Disorders 61, 161–176. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Millan MJ and Brocco M (2003) The Vogel conflict test: procedural aspects, gamma-aminobutyric acid, glutamate and monoamines. European Journal of Pharmacology 463, 67–96. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Noldus LP, Spink AJ and Tegelenbosch RA (2001) EthoVision: a versatile video tracking system for automation of behavioral experiments. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers 33, 398–414. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Padilla-Coreano N, Bolkan SS, Pierce GM, Blackman DR, Hardin WD, Garcia-Garcia AL, Spellman TJ and Gordon JA (2016) Direct ventral hippocampal-prefrontal input is required for anxiety-related neural activity and behavior. Neuron 89, 857–866. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Prut L and Belzung C (2003) The open field as a paradigm to measure the effects of drugs on anxiety-like behaviors: a review. European Journal of Pharmacology 463, 3–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Rusconi F, Grillo B, Ponzoni L, Bassani S, Toffolo E, Paganini L, Mallei A, Braida D, Passafaro M, Popoli M, Sala M and Battaglioli E (2016) LSD1 modulates stress-evoked transcription of immediate early genes and emotional behavior. Proceeding of the National Academy of Sciences 113, 3651–3656. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Schumacher A, Villaruel FR, Ussling A, Riaz S, Lee ACH and Ito R (2018) Ventral hippocampal CA1 and CA3 differentially mediate learned approach-avoidance conflict processing. Current Biology 28, 1318–1324.e1314. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Struijs SY, Lamers F, Vroling MS, Roelofs K, Spinhoven P and Penninx BWJH (2017) Approach and avoidance tendencies in depression and anxiety disorders. Psychiatry Research 256, 475–481. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Toth I and Neumann ID (2013) Animal models of social avoidance and social fear. Cell and Tissue Research 354, 107–118. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Tsankova NM, Berton O, Renthal W, Kumar A, Neve RL and Nestler EJ (2006) Sustained hippocampal chromatin regulation in a mouse model of depression and antidepressant action. Nature Neuroscience 9, 519–525. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Wager TD, Davidson ML, Hughes BL, Lindquist MA and Ochsner KN (2008) Prefrontal-subcortical pathways mediating successful emotion regulation. Neuron 59, 1037–1050. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Walf AA and Frye CA (2007) The use of the elevated plus maze as an assay of anxiety-related behavior in rodents. Nature Protocols 2, 322–328. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Walz N, Mühlberger A and Pauli P (2016) A human open field test reveals thigmotaxis related to agoraphobic fear. Biological Psychiatry 80, 390–397. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Weeden CS, Roberts JM, Kamm AM and Kesner RP (2015) The role of the ventral dentate gyrus in anxiety-based behaviors. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 118, 143–149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Yeates DCM, Ussling A, Lee ACH and Ito R (2020) Double dissociation of learned approach-avoidance conflict processing and spatial pattern separation along the dorsoventral axis of the dentate gyrus. Hippocampus 30, 596–609. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

All data used to write this paper is in the reference list.


Articles from Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences are provided here courtesy of Cambridge University Press

RESOURCES