Skip to main content
Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience logoLink to Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience
. 2023 Jan 10;16:1090248. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2022.1090248

Designing animal-friendly behavioral tests for neuroscience research: The importance of an ethological approach

Raffaele d'Isa 1,*, Robert Gerlai 2,*
PMCID: PMC9871504  PMID: 36703720

Behavioral tests have three key elements: (1) a motivating factor (what motivates the animal in the test); (2) an observable behavior (which behaviors we may expect the animal exhibits in response to the test); (3) a measurable outcome (a quantifiable variable associated with the behavioral response).

For example, in the rodent step-through passive avoidance test (see d'Isa et al., 2014 for a brief history of the test), the animal is released into a strongly illuminated chamber connected to a dark zone. Being naturally photophobic and preferring dark areas, mice and rats will rapidly move from the well-illuminated zone into the dark zone, a behavior that in the wild is useful to avoid being seen by predators. When the animal enters the dark zone, it receives an electric shock. After the initial exposure to this apparatus (the training), the animal is released a second time in the same apparatus for a memory test, but this time without the shock deliverer being active. If the animal remembers receiving the shock, the dark zone should now be perceived as dangerous and hence avoided. In this test, the motivation is fear, the observable behavior is avoidance of a dangerous (dark) zone, and the quantifiable outcome is the latency to enter the dangerous zone, which thus serves as an index of memory. The longer is the latency, the stronger is the memory of the past electric shock exposure.

Behavioral testing of rodents in a laboratory setting started in the 1890's with the studies of Thomas Wesley Mills (1847–1915) from McGill University (Montreal, Canada) and of Linus Ward Kline (1866–1961) and Willard Stanton Small (1870–1943), both from Clark University (Worcester, Massachusetts, USA) (Mills, 1895, 1898; Kline, 1899a,b; Small, 1899, 1900, 1901). These studies were preceded by ethological, purely observational, studies on rodent behavior in the wild, e.g., Mills's studies on squirrel behavior (Mills, 1888, 1890, 1893), but it was only in the period 1895–1900 that behavioral tests for studying rodent behavior and psychology in a laboratory setting started to be designed. Mills also observed the behavior of two squirrels he captured and kept for few months, reporting, for example, how ethologically relevant behaviors could be observed, like nest-building and food storing, and how one of them learned to eat from his hand and enjoyed running on a running wheel that was installed in its home-cage (Mills, 1888). Although this was a first step for the study of rodent behavior in a controlled environment, the report with the findings was only anecdotal and appeared inserted within a paper on the behavior of squirrels in the wild. Two important elements of scientific testing were absent: systematic observations (i.e., observations at pre-set time-points according to a specific rationale) and choice of one or more quantitatively measurable behavioral outcomes as variables of interest. A systematic and quantitative study of rodent behavior in laboratory had yet to come.

Mills was the first, in the mid 1890's, to introduce for rodents the ontogenetic diary method, which consisted of observing and describing step by step the different developmental stages of a species, starting from the day of birth (Mills, 1895). Applying this method, he studied systematically the physical and psychological development of guinea pigs through a daily monitoring in a laboratory setting. In addition to purely observational studies of development, he also used some basic behavioral tests, e.g., reflex tests or taste reactivity tests, in which only qualitative responses were recorded (Mills, 1895, 1898). On the other hand, Kline and Small introduced behavioral tests aimed at specifically evaluating cognition. In 1898, a Clark University colleague of Kline and Small, Colin Campbell Stewart (1873–1944), published a study on the effects of alcohol, barometric pressure and type of diet on rat daily voluntary wheel running activity, as assessed by a revolving drum connected to automatic counters recording the total number of revolutions (Stewart, 1898). Stewart was the first to perform a quantitative rodent test of motor activity. The experiments of Stewart, from the Biology Faculty, inspired Kline, from the Psychology Faculty, the idea to choose rats as animal models for his research on learning processes (Kline, 1928; Miles, 1930). Kline designed a rat problem box (Kline, 1899a), while Small, who worked in the same laboratory as Kline, was the first to use a maze in the history of behavioral neuroscience (Small, 1901). In Kline's test, the task was finding the entrance of a box and retrieving the food contained inside it. Time to retrieve the food was recorded over multiple trials to assess learning. In Small's test, the task was finding food placed in the central zone of a complex maze inspired by the design of Hampton Court Maze, the well-known hedge labyrinth in England. Time to find the food and number of errors (entering a blind alley) were recorded over multiple trials. The ones performed by Kline and Small were the first quantitative rodent cognitive tests in the history of behavioral neuroscience. Regarding the motivating factor, both Kline and Small employed hungry rats in their cognitive tests, and food deprivation became a widely employed protocol in the subsequent studies using appetitive (reward-based) learning tasks.

However, since the dawn of behavioral neuroscience, researchers have been aware of the fact that hunger is only one possible motivator. Small himself wrote: “I trust the reader will not “jump” to the conclusion that no other motive would be workable. Hunger is merely the most fundamental” (Small, 1900). Noticing that well-fed rats still retrieved food during the task, he added: “Their performance of the task without the incitement of hunger can hardly be accounted for except upon the basis of a hoarding instinct almost as imperative as hunger” (Small, 1900). Nevertheless, these specifications seem to have been scarcely considered by the following researchers. Indeed, most behavioral tests designed up to the 1950's were based on rewards or punishments and commonly employed food deprivation or electric shocks, respectively.

Another classical avoidance task is, for example, the shuttle-box active avoidance, first conceived by Lucien Warner in the early 1930's (Warner, 1932), where instead of having to stay in the illuminated zone to avoid the shock, i.e., instead of not moving and being passive, the appropriate response is to actively move over to an opposite zone to avoid the shock when a stimulus (a tone or a light) announces its imminent release. In this task, the experimenter may have to employ a large number of shocks (even hundreds) over several days of training before animals reach high rates of shock avoidance and cognitively impaired animals could still show low rates of shock avoidance even at the end of the training (Montag-Sallaz and Montag, 2003; Cain, 2019). Painful stimulation, along with being ethically undesirable when dealing with any sentient organism, also leads to methodological complications for the experimenters. Indeed, pain generates stress, which is a major confounding factor in animal research. Still today, classical avoidance tests employ electric shocks, while many other traditional tests, although not utilizing painful stimuli, feature highly stressful conditions including starvation, water deprivation or pharmacologically induced sickness to motivate the animals to perform a task. However, an increasingly accepted view is that absence of pain and reduction of stress during behavioral testing are fundamental for both animal welfare and reproducibility of experimental results, and unless pain or stress is the main focus of the study, these conditions should be avoided as much as possible. In order to respond to such ethical and methodological concerns, several animal-friendly tests have been designed. However, since not all behavioral domains of investigation currently have such animal-friendly options, the development of new animal-friendly tests is an important goal for modern behavioral neuroscience.

How to design an animal-friendly behavioral test

Compared to shuttle-box active avoidance, step-through passive avoidance, introduced in the 1960's (Kopp et al., 1966; Jarvik and Kopp, 1967), is considerably less stressful and in the test session no shock delivery is actually present. Nevertheless, the training session still features a brief painful stimulus. In an ideal animal-friendly behavioral test, the motivating factor should not be painful or stressful. Furthermore, the observable behavior should be natural (i.e., an ethologically relevant species-specific motor or postural pattern). Finally, the outcome variable associated with this behavioral response should be practical to measure in a laboratory setting through a method that is safe for the animals (e.g., direct observation, videorecording, videotracking, audiorecording of ultrasonic vocalizations, photocell actimetry, weight sensors, infrared thermometry and other non-invasive methods). These are the three main characteristics that a behavioral test should have to be qualified as animal-friendly. Let us first focus on the motivational aspect of behavioral tests, the first of the key components mentioned above.

Ethologists have been stressing the argument that motivating factors are species-specific (Gerlai and Clayton, 1999a; Gerlai, 2021). A stimulus that is appetitive (rewarding) or aversive (punishing) for one species, may be neutral, or may have the opposite reinforcing value to another. Even among closely related species, the rewarding value of a stimulus may be remarkably different. Among felids, for instance, tigers like to bath in water, while lions do not. Hence a swimming pool may be a reward for the former, but not for the latter species, as found, for example, by Allison Hedgecoth who provided a water pool to a lioness and a tigress living together in the same environment in the Noah's Ark Animal Sanctuary of Locust Grove, Georgia, United States (Harries et al., 2020). The main issue, however, in the behavioral neuroscience literature is that systematic analysis of what motivates animals used in laboratory settings is often lacking, or that ethology research often does not intersect with biomedical studies.

Considering, for example, laboratory rodents, the house mouse (Mus musculus) and the common rat (Rattus norvegicus) are the two most widely used species in biomedical research. Their employment is almost universal in translational research studying mechanisms of central nervous system disorders. Most studies that require aversive stimuli with rodents use electric shocks. But electric shocks are rather unnatural stimuli. However, almost no one considers what consequences may result from the unnatural aspect of this stimulus. It is just assumed that pain is pain, and that electric shock-induced pain is relevant and strongly motivating. Most scientists do not even consider what complication this electricity passively running through the body of the animal, including its brain, may cause with respect to neuronal activity: such electric currents may alter synaptic function and numerous underlying molecular mechanisms. Similarly, studies that employ appetitive stimuli, almost always use food that the experimenter picks out based on tradition, habit or just personal preference. Comparative analyses of what food types, food quantities, food textures and food sizes are most preferred by rats or mice are often not considered, or have not even been conducted. Briefly, as animals are, through evolutionary processes, adapted to their natural environment, they possess species-specific characteristics that represent genetic predispositions, instincts in colloquial terms, that determine, or at least heavily influence what they like, what they dislike, and how they respond to these stimuli. Taking these species-specific features into account is thus a must in animal-friendly experimental designing (Gerlai and Clayton, 1999a,b). In rodents, a typical animal-friendly motivating factor is neophilia (attraction for novelty), which drives, for instance, object exploration behavior in the object recognition test (d'Isa et al., 2014), head-dipping in the hole-board test (d'Isa et al., 2021a) and arm alternation in the spontaneous alternation T-maze (d'Isa et al., 2021b). A similar example is the continuous spontaneous alternation test using a T-maze, which utilizes novel place preference to study short-term spatial memory in rodents (Gerlai, 1998).

Regarding the second key element mentioned above, the observable behavior in an animal-friendly test should be naturally displayed by the animal (e.g., should be part of the ethogram). Preferably, it should be a spontaneous behavior, an instinctive response that requires no pre-training, during which typically punishments and rewards are used by the experimenter to lead to a target behavior. Punishments are commonly painful stimuli (as electric shocks), while rewards, as food or liquids, are often associated with food-deprivation or water-deprivation, in order to use hunger or thirst as motivating factors. Lack of the need for pre-training makes the test more animal-friendly because it avoids punishments and deprivations, and is also time-saving for the experimenter. It is, however, also possible to use conditioned behaviors in an animal-friendly way, if certain conditions are respected. In particular, rewards should not be associated with a previous aversive state. Chow and colleagues, for instance, designed a reward-based cognitive test for gray squirrels in which no food-deprivation or water-deprivation was employed (Chow et al., 2017). The motivation of the rodents was ensured simply by using food rewards (hazelnuts) that were different from their daily diet (seeds, fresh fruit and vegetables), i.e., novelty alone was sufficient to motivate the animals. Novelty-seeking and exploratory drive (i.e., the motivation to learn about new places and/or new inanimate or animate components of the environment) are almost universal among animal species, and certainly have been shown for laboratory rodents (Gerlai et al., 1990; Crusio, 2001). In fact, stabilizing natural selection has been inferred for exploratory behaviors from fish to mammals, as it leads to an optimal level of activity ensuring the ability of the animal to find resources, including food, water and mates, as well as escape routes leading away from predators (Gerlai et al., 1990; Crusio, 2001). The use of novelty as a motivator may not be appropriate in some research contexts and, for certain studies, aversive stimulation may be required. However, even in such cases, painful punishments could be and should be substituted with non-painful aversive alternatives, for example, air-puffs. Indeed, air-puffs have been efficiently employed to elicit robust conditioned place avoidance negating the need for using any painful stimuli (d'Isa et al., 2011). Even for studies specifically focused on fear reactions, alternatives to painful stimulation are available. Odor of predators (e.g., fox's urine, or an extract from it) has been efficiently used to induce avoidance reactions and fear without previous painful stimulation (Blanchard et al., 2003).

The main steps for designing an animal-friendly test can be summarized as follows: (a) prepare a list of behaviors typical of the species (the ethogram), along with what stimuli may induce these behaviors, i.e., the motivating forces; (b) exclude behaviors induced by pain, physical suffering or psychological stress; (c) from the remaining, choose a behavior that can be studied through an apparatus that can be used in a laboratory setting; (d) choose which outcomes could be measured, safely for the animals, in the most efficient and precise way in order to provide quantitative experimental data. Let us examine an experimental example of how these steps may be accomplished.

A typical behavior of rodents is food hoarding, that is, collecting and hiding food as supply storage for times of food scarcity. This behavior can be observed in more than 180 rodents (Zhang et al., 2022). This is an adaptive behavior that is observable both in nature and in the laboratory setting. It is an instinctive behavior that does not require pre-training. Two main strategies are adopted by food hoarding rodents. Scatter hoarders, as gray squirrels, hide food in many dispersed small hoards. On the other hand, larder hoarders, as hamsters, store food in one large hoard, named the larder. A classification of the hoarding strategies of 183 rodents is provided by Zhang and colleagues (Zhang et al., 2022). These hoarding behaviors may be utilized by the experimenter to devise behavioral tests of motivation (during the food accumulation phase) or of spatial memory (during the subsequent phase of food retrieval from the spatially separated hoards). For motivation tests, easier to study in larder hoarding rodents, the measurable outcome could be the total weight of the seeds or pellets collected and stored in a fixed amount of time. For spatial memory tests, which would be best studied with scatter hoarding rodents, the recorded outcome could be the number of errors in finding the hoarding sites containing the previously stored food. Alternatively, spatial memory could be studied also in larder hoarders if, during the accumulation phase, the sources of food are multiple. Number of errors (returning to an already depleted food site) would serve as memory index. An apparatus for the testing of food hoarding behavior in a laboratory setting has been realized, for example, by Robert Deacon at Oxford University (Deacon, 2006).

An ethological approach may be useful to devise animal-friendly behavioral tests for two reasons. On the one hand, it may help researchers to choose among the elements of the ethogram a behavior that does not require painful or stressful motivating factors. On the other hand, among a taxonomical family of species (for example rodents), it may help researchers to select the most suitable species for a certain test. Let us return to the example we mentioned above. Laboratory mice are larder hoarders, just like hamsters, but their propensity to hoard is relatively low under baseline conditions. In order to avoid food-deprivation, long testing sessions may be required to obtain replicable results, including, e.g., overnight testing sessions (Deacon, 2006). Hamsters, on the other hand, have a high propensity to hoard (Vander Wall, 1990; Harris, 2017). Up to 90 kg of food have been found in hamster burrows (Nowak and Paradiso, 1983). Among food hoarders, they display a specific behavior known as cheek pouching, that is accumulating food in cheek pouches, specialized pockets that allow food transportation. Instead of eating the food items, hamsters keep the food items in their mouth to carry them to a safe place for storage (the larder). Importantly, hamsters easily show this behavior even when they are not hungry, with a latency to hoard within 2 min (Montoya and Gutiérrez, 2016). This peculiarity of hamsters makes them particularly suitable as animal models for scientists who want to design an animal-friendly reward-based memory test that does not require any previous starvation.

Another rodent, the chinchilla (Chinchilla lanigera), displays a peculiar behavior known as sand-bathing: when presented with a box full of sand, it will readily start rolling in the box, rotating along its longitudinal axis, to rub its fur in the sand (Stern and Merari, 1969). This natural and spontaneous behavior can be easily elicited in a laboratory setting and sand could be used as an animal-friendly reward in instrumental learning tests without the need of any previous deprivation condition (Redman, 1974).

Eastern woodrats (Neotoma floridana), also known as pack rats, have a special attraction for shiny objects, which they readily approach, pick up and bring to their nest, where they collect them (Bradley et al., 2022). This natural tendency of woodrats could be used in behavioral tests, employing small metal objects, as stripes or balls of aluminum foil, as motivators (Kaufman and Kaufman, 1984).

Of course, the issue is that quite often neurobiological, genetic, or other methods may not be as readily available, or as sophisticated, for such species as hamsters and chinchillas as for the favorites of biomedical research, mice and rats. How can we solve this conundrum? Firstly, we could improve biotechnological methods for the so called “alternative” species. Secondly, we could improve our understanding of the ethology, the natural species-specific behavioral characteristics, of the preferred model organisms, e.g., of mice and rats. Certainly, advances in both of these areas have been made during the past few decades. Regarding the first area, numerous novel techniques may now be equally useable with mice and hamsters (and many other species). The CRISPR/Cas technology is a clear example (Kampmann, 2020). Concerning the second area, there have been research efforts adopting ethological approaches in mouse neurobehavioral genetics, as for instance testing mouse mutants in the wild (Dell'Omo et al., 2000; Vyssotski et al., 2002) or in laboratory environments more closely resembling a natural habitat, like Eco-HAB (Puścian et al., 2016; Winiarski et al., 2022). Anders Ågmo's research group at University of Tromsø recreated a seminatural environment in the laboratory for the evaluation of rat behavior (Chu and Ågmo, 2014), a method that has been employed in several subsequent studies (Chu et al., 2015; Houwing et al., 2019; Le Moëne et al., 2020; Heinla et al., 2021). In the testing sessions, which may last days, rat behavior is continuously video-recorded and subsequently scored off-line by the researchers. Notably, another important ethological approach of the new century is testing the animals not in a setting designated uniquely for testing sessions, but rather in the permanent housing environment in which they commonly live (Mingrone et al., 2020; Voikar and Gaburro, 2020). In nature, most rodents build burrows (or occupy pre-existing holes or burrows) to use them as homes (long-term inhabiting spaces), in which they return to sleep, store food, seek shelter from the elements, keep warm, hide from predators, give birth, raise the pups and share a social life with conspecifics. Rodents develop a strong bond with their home and show a territorial behavior toward it, actively defending it from possible invaders. In the laboratory, if an unfamiliar conspecific is placed in the home-cage of mice or rats, the intruder will rapidly be attacked by the resident animal (Koolhaas et al., 2013; Ruzza et al., 2015). The home-cage is the place where laboratory rodents feel safest and where they are more likely to display spontaneous natural behaviors. Thus, the idea of testing in the home-cage has been gaining considerable attention in the past few years, and several home-cage automated multi-variable recording systems have been developed, e.g., the IntelliCage (Galsworthy et al., 2005; Kiryk et al., 2020; Iman et al., 2021), PhenoMaster (Urbach et al., 2008; König et al., 2020), Actual-HCA (Bains et al., 2016; Mitchell et al., 2020) and SmartKage (Ho et al., 2022). Automated home-cage testing systems have several advantages: (a) they allow behavioral phenotyping without human interference and without the consequent handling-related stress; (b) the animals are not tested in an external apparatus but in their familiar and well-known housing environment, which eliminates confounds arising from anxiety; (c) data collection is not restricted to a specific moment of the day, but can be performed continuously, 24 h a day, 7 days a week, allowing a more precise and realistic assessment of behavior; d) long longitudinal studies (lasting weeks, months or years), or even life-long studies, can be performed on the same animals with a continuous behavioral assessment, which is particularly relevant for developmental neuroscience and aging neuroscience; (e) interactive elements (e.g., levers, nose-poking ports, motorized doors and running wheels) may be installed in these home-cages, allowing not only detailed motor assessment, but also complex cognitive testing; (f) animals are tested in a natural social context while living together with other conspecifics, thus providing motor and cognitive measurements with a higher ethological validity and allowing additionally to monitor and analyze complex social interactions. Some of these automated home-cage testing systems are modular, allowing the connection of multiple cages to create a more complex envinronment. For instance, IntelliCage can be connected to two social boxes containing different social stimuli (Mitjans et al., 2017), while in ColonyRack mice can freely roam across 70 cages, arranged in a two-sided rack with fivs columns and seven rows, in which the cages are connected both horizontally and vertically (Zocher et al., 2020; Kempermann et al., 2022). The most recent innovation within this automated behavioral testing approach is connecting home-cages to mazes (Mei et al., 2020; Kohler et al., 2022), granting the experimental subjects free access to the novel test environment. This allows the animals to decide voluntarily when and for how long they explore the maze, similarly to what would happen in nature when rodents decide to leave their burrow for external exploratory excursions.

We believe that bringing closer the fields of ethology and neurobehavioral genetics or behavioral neuroscience will be the solution and will lead to cross-fertilization of these fields. Similarly to how the application of neuroscience-related knowledge to ethology led to the birth of neuroethology (i.e., the study of the neural basis of natural behaviors), the reverse could lead to an ethologically based neuroscience, or ethological neuroscience, which can be defined as the employment of knowledge of the natural behavior of animals in the wild to develop animal models of behavior and behavioral tests for neuroscience research. This ethologically based neuroscience can lead to animal-friendly testing approaches that will not only be more oriented toward the welfare of the animals involved, but also will provide more reliable and more replicable results for the experimenters.

Concluding remarks: Reproducibility, replicability and refinement

Reproducibility is when we obtain the same results repeatedly by using identical methods (Kafkafi et al., 2018; Gerlai, 2019), whereas replicability is when we reach similar conclusions by adopting different methodologies (Kafkafi et al., 2018; Gerlai, 2019). Minimizing stress of the tested animals is a value in itself from an ethical point of view. However, since stress is a confounding factor that increases variability of experimental outcomes, minimizing stress is also fundamental to achieve methodologically sound scientific research. Why does research that ignores species-specific features lead to increased variability? Why is stress a confounding factor that reduces reproducibility? These are intriguing questions that would deserve specific research. The answer may lay in the fact that stress causes activation of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis, which in turn alters physiological processes regulating cognition and behavior (Moreira et al., 2016). HPA reactivity depends on genetic, epigenetic and environmental factors (Holmes et al., 2005), which makes it more difficult to predict than instinctive responses. Let us make some overarching theoretical points. Most animal research includes human handling. Human handling is extremely difficult to standardize (Crabbe et al., 1999). Even if handling was perfectly standardized, stress reactivity of the animals would not. Animals experiencing more stress due to the experimental procedures will be more responsive to human handling, which then will lead to elevated error variation in the behavioral test. In order to maximize experiment reproducibility, the best option is to minimize handling-related stress (Gouveia and Hurst, 2017). Considering, for instance, mice, although tail picking is the most commonly employed method of handling (Ueno et al., 2020), this method features tail lifting, tail suspension and swinging the animal over a void, which are highly stressful for the mice. Indeed, tail lifting, compared with alternative handling methods that do not require tail lifting, increases anxiety in the open-field test (Gouveia and Hurst, 2019) and elevated plus maze (Hurst and West, 2010), and it has been shown to reduce exploratory activity (Gouveia and Hurst, 2017), to increase aversion for the human handler in voluntary interaction test (Hurst and West, 2010) and to impair responsiveness to sucrose reward, indicating a reduction of reward's hedonic value (Clarkson et al., 2018). Several animal-friendly approaches are now available to avoid the negative impact of human handling on mice: (a) adopting non-aversive manual handling techniques, as open-hand retrieval through the cupping method (Hurst and West, 2010; Gouveia and Hurst, 2017, 2019; d'Isa et al., 2021b; Davies et al., 2022); (b) employing a tool to handle the mice, as a plastic handling tunnel (Hurst and West, 2010; Gouveia and Hurst, 2013, 2017, 2019; Sensini et al., 2020; Davies et al., 2022); (c) using automated home-cage testing systems in which behavioral outcomes are recorded without physical interaction with the human experimenter (Kiryk et al., 2020; König et al., 2020; Mitchell et al., 2020; Ho et al., 2022; Kohler et al., 2022; Winiarski et al., 2022).

Furthermore, not knowing the species-specific characteristics of the studied organism, for example, applying inappropriate motivators, forcing the animal to exhibit behavioral responses it would not normally perform, and measuring the behavior under artificial conditions that do not have much to do with the natural environment in which the animal evolved, all can elevate random error, simply because the individuals tested this way may have to find unique solutions to the problems, considerably increasing individual differences in the study (Gerlai and Clayton, 1999a,b). To put it in the words of the aforementioned pioneer of experimental behavioral research Willlard Stanton Small, “the experiments must conform to the psycho-biological character of an animal if sane results are to be obtained” (Small, 1901).

Animal-friendly tests utilizing species-specific features of the studied organism may not be always available or applicable, but, when they are, they should be employed as a first option, in order to maximize both animal welfare and repeatability of experimental results. When fully animal-friendly tests are not available, then the least stressful available test should be employed. In Table 1 we present a rating scale for behavioral tests based on their impact on animal welfare. This rating is not meant to be final, but rather a starting point to stimulate reflection and discussion on the differential stress impact of behavioral tests. We hope that in future an increasing number of studies will employ tests of class A (animal-friendly) and B (minimally stressful) and that, in accordance with a progressive refinement principle, new animal-friendly tests will be designed to substitute the more stressful alternatives.

Table 1.

Rating of the impact of behavioral tests on animal welfare.

Welfare rating Features Behavioral tests References
A • Spontaneous behaviors
• Conditioning only through rewards not associated with previous aversive situations or through non-painful disincentives
• No food deprivation
• No water deprivation
• No forced water immersion
• No painful stimuli
• No distressful conditions
Novel object recognition d'Isa et al., 2014
Object location test Murai et al., 2007
Object exploration test Steinbach et al., 2016
Spontaneous alternation T-maze d'Isa et al., 2021b
Continuous alternation Y-maze Detrait et al., 2010
Continuous alternation T-maze Gerlai, 1998
Spontaneous 8-arm radial maze (no food deprivation, unbaited) Haga, 1995
Spontaneous 6-arm radial maze (no food deprivation, unbaited) Alessandri et al., 1994; Opitz et al., 1997
Spontaneous Dashiell hexagonal maze (no food deprivation, unbaited) Giménez-Llort et al., 2007
Free access rewarded 8-arm radial maze (no food deprivation, baited) Mei et al., 2020; Kohler et al., 2022
Hole-board test d'Isa et al., 2021a
Locomotor activity test Visigalli et al., 2010
Open-field test (in dim light) McReynolds et al., 1967; Võikar and Stanford, 2023
Emergence test Paré et al., 2001
Sociability test in the three-chambered apparatus Gu et al., 2022
Social vs. object preference test in the three-chambered apparatus Lammert et al., 2018
Social novelty preference test in the three-chambered apparatus Kaidanovich-Beilin et al., 2011
Social recognition test Jacobs et al., 2016
Opposite-sex partner preference test in the satellite cages apparatus Linnenbrink and von Merten, 2017
Mate choice test in the three-chambered apparatus Nomoto et al., 2018; Guarraci and Frohardt, 2019
Paced mating sexual behavior test Zipse et al., 2000; Nedergaard et al., 2004
Paced mating-induced conditioned place preference Paredes and Alonso, 1997; Camacho et al., 2009
Two-bottle taste preference test Gaillard and Stratford, 2016; Strekalova, 2023
Saccharin consumption test Inostroza et al., 2012
Voluntary wheel running Goh and Ladiges, 2015
Successive alleys test Deacon, 2013a
Nest-building test Neely et al., 2019; Dorninger et al., 2020
Burrowing test Deacon, 2009
Food hoarding test (without food deprivation) Deacon, 2006
Marble burying test Angoa-Pérez et al., 2013; Witkin and Smith, 2023
CatWalk gait analysis Crowley et al., 2018; Pitzer et al., 2021
IntelliCage automated home-cage testing Vannoni et al., 2014; Kiryk et al., 2020
SmartKage automated home-cage testing Ho et al., 2022
Eco-HAB automated home-cage testing Puścian et al., 2016; Winiarski et al., 2022
PhenoMaster automated home-cage testing Robinson et al., 2013; König et al., 2020
Actual-HCA automated home-cage testing Bains et al., 2018; Mitchell et al., 2020
B • Low psychological stress
• No food deprivation
• No water deprivation
• No forced water immersion
• No painful stimuli
Barnes maze Rosenfeld and Ferguson, 2014
Open-field test (in bright light) Seibenhener and Wooten, 2015
Light-dark transition test Takao and Miyakawa, 2006
Elevated plus maze Walf and Frye, 2007
Rotarod Papale et al., 2017
Pole test Zhu et al., 2017
Beam walking test Luong et al., 2011
Pup retrieval test Lee et al., 2021; Winters et al., 2022
Pup-rewarded auditory learning test Besosa et al., 2020
C • Moderate psychological stress
• No food deprivation
• No water deprivation
• No painful stimuli
Morris water maze d'Isa et al., 2011
Grip strength test Mandillo et al., 2008
Inverted screen test Deacon, 2013b
Prepulse inhibition test Valsamis and Schmid, 2011; Ioannidou et al., 2018
Visual threat flight test Huang et al., 2020; Yang et al., 2020
D • Brief food deprivation (up to 18 h)
• Brief water deprivation (up to 9 h)
• No painful stimuli
Socially transmitted food preference test Wrenn et al., 2003; Plucinska et al., 2012
Social vs. food preference test Reppucci and Veenema, 2020
Hyponeophagia test Deacon, 2011
Water-rewarded social cooperation test Feng et al., 2021; Shin and Ko, 2021
E • High psychological stress
• No food deprivation
• No water deprivation
• No painful stimuli
Forced swimming test Castagné et al., 2011
Tail suspension test Can et al., 2012
Predator odor test Otsuka, 2017
Counter-current swimming test Matsumoto et al., 1996; Mizunoya et al., 2002
F • Brief painful stimulation ( ≤ 2 s) Passive avoidance test Papale et al., 2017
Single-shock fear conditioning Poulos et al., 2016
Shock-probe defensive burying test Fucich and Morilak, 2018
G • Repeated (2–5 events) or extended (>2 and ≤ 10 total s) painful stimulation Multiple-shock fear conditioning Shoji et al., 2014; Müller and Fendt, 2023
Multiple-shock passive avoidance Takahashi et al., 2018
Tail-flick test Chidiac et al., 2021
Hot plate test Lee et al., 2018
H • Prolonged food restriction (from 18 h to weeks)
• Prolonged water restriction (from 9 h to weeks)
8-arm radial maze Crusio and Schwegler, 2005
Cross maze Pittenger et al., 2006
Food-rewarded 5-choice serial reaction time task Asinof and Paine, 2014
Novelty-suppressed feeding test Fukumoto and Chaki, 2015
Water-rewarded 5-choice serial reaction time task Birtalan et al., 2020
Water-rewarded auditory decision making test Jaramillo and Zador, 2014
Water-rewarded labyrinth Rosenberg et al., 2021
I • Sickness induction Lithium chloride-induced conditioned taste aversion Lavi et al., 2018
Lithium cloride-induced conditioned place aversion Frisch et al., 1995
J • Repeated (>5 events) or extended (>10 total s) painful stimulation Shuttle-box active avoidance Montag-Sallaz and Montag, 2003
Learned helplessness test Vollmayr and Henn, 2001; Silveira and Joca, 2023
Formalin test Teng and Abbott, 1998
K • Potential physical injury (mild to severe) Resident-intruder aggression test Koolhaas et al., 2013
Resident-intruder violence test Haller et al., 2006; Koolhaas et al., 2013
L • Lethal tests Terminal sleep deprivation Everson et al., 1989
Drowning test Richter, 1957

A suggested rating scale for behavioral tests in order of severity (from A to L) of the impact on the wellbeing and welfare of laboratory rodents is presented. For each rating class, features are described and a list of example behavioral tests is provided. The scale comprises twelve welfare classes, from A (no negative impact on animal welfare) to L (most severe negative impact). Important notes:

1. Test impact on animal welfare is species-specific. The suggested rating is specific for crepuscular/nocturnal rodents (as mice, rats and hamsters). For different species of rodents, the ratings may differ. For instance, for the degu (Octodon degus), a diurnal rodent species in which adults show strong preference for the lit compartment in the light-dark transition test (Popović et al., 2009), the Barnes maze would have a rating of A instead of B.

2. This classification of behavioral tests is based on the normal responses expected from wild-type or untreated/unmanipulated control rodents. However, as test-associated stress always derives from the interaction between the test and the experimental manipulations (e.g., mutations induced or drug treatment employed), even behavioral tests that are minimally stressful for untreated wild-types may lead to high distress in manipulated animals. Thus, the experimenter must always consider not only what wild-type control animals will do in the test, but also closely monitor how the mutant/treated animals respond in pilot studies, and revise the experimental protocol or choose alternative tests accordingly.

3. This scale is ordinal, but not linear. We are not assuming equal distances between classes.

4. Due to the degree of suffering inflicted on the animals, the tests reported in category J are considered unethical according to current ethical standards, and nowadays would not be approved by the institutional animal care and use committees (IACUCs), nor would receive legal authorization, in most countries performing scientific research.

5. The rating scale is only a suggested scale, a non-comprehensive working document that is meant to be debated, updated and expanded by the rodent research community.

Author contributions

Rd'I and RG: conceptualization, writing, revision, and final approval of the manuscript. Both authors provided funding. Both authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding Statement

The present work was funded by the Natural Science and Engineering Research Council (NSERC) of Canada (Discovery Grant #311637 to RG), by University of Toronto Mississauga Distinguished Professorship Award to RG and by Rd'I.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

  1. Alessandri B., FitzGerald R. E., Schaeppi U., Krinke G. J., Classen W. (1994). The use of an unbaited tunnel maze in neurotoxicology: I. Trimethyltin-induced brain lesions. Neurotoxicology 15, 349–357 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Angoa-Pérez M., Kane M. J., Briggs D. I., Francescutti D. M., Kuhn D. M. (2013). Marble burying and nestlet shredding as tests of repetitive, compulsive-like behaviors in mice. J. Vis. Exp. 82, 50978. 10.3791/50978 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Asinof S. K., Paine T. A. (2014). The 5-choice serial reaction time task: a task of attention and impulse control for rodents. J. Vis. Exp. 90, e51574. 10.3791/51574 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bains R. S., Cater H. L., Sillito R. R., Chartsias A., Sneddon D., Concas D., et al. (2016). Analysis of individual mouse activity in group housed animals of different inbred strains using a novel automated home cage analysis system. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 10, 106. 10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bains R. S., Wells S., Sillito R. R., Armstrong J. D., Cater H. L., Banks G., et al. (2018). Assessing mouse behaviour throughout the light/dark cycle using automated in-cage analysis tools. J. Neurosci. Methods 300, 37–47. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2017.04.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Besosa C. I., Dunlap A. G., Lu K., Liu R. C. (2020). Using the pup retrieval instinct as reinforcement for efficient auditory learning in mice. MethodsX 7, 101051. 10.1016/j.mex.2020.101051 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Birtalan E., Bánhidi A., Sanders J. I., Balázsfi D., Hangya B. (2020). Efficient training of mice on the 5-choice serial reaction time task in an automated rodent training system. Sci. Rep. 10, 22362. 10.1038/s41598-020-79290-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Blanchard D. C., Griebel G., Blanchard R. J. (2003). Conditioning and residual emotionality effects of predator stimuli: some reflections on stress and emotion. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 27, 1177–1185. 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2003.09.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bradley R. D., Edwards C. W., Lindsey L. L., Bateman J. R., Cajimat M. N. B., Milazzo M. L., et al. (2022). Reevaluation of the phylogenetic relationships among Neotomini rodents (Hodomys, Neotoma, and Xenomys) and comments on the woodrat classification. J. Mammal. 103, 1221–1236. 10.1093/jmammal/gyac031 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Cain C. K. (2019). Avoidance problems reconsidered. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 26, 9–17. 10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.09.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Camacho F. J., García-Horsman P., Paredes R. G. (2009). Hormonal and testing conditions for the induction of conditioned place preference by paced mating. Horm. Behav. 56, 410–415. 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2009.07.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Can A., Dao D. T., Terrillion C. E., Piantadosi S. C., Bhat S., Gould T. D. (2012). The tail suspension test. J. Vis. Exp. 59, e3769. 10.3791/3769 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Castagné V., Moser P., Roux S., Porsolt R. D. (2011). Rodent models of depression: forced swim and tail suspension behavioral despair tests in rats and mice. Curr. Protoc. Neurosci. 55, 8.10A1–8.10A14. 10.1002/0471142301.ns0810as55 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Chidiac C., Xue Y., Muniz Moreno M. D. M., Bakr Rasheed A. A., Lorentz R., Birling M. C., et al. (2021). The human SCN10AG1662S point mutation established in mice impacts on mechanical, heat, and cool sensitivity. Front. Pharmacol. 12, 780132. 10.3389/fphar.2021.780132 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Chow P. K. Y., Lea S. E. G., Hempel de Ibarra N., Robert T. (2017). How to stay perfect: the role of memory and behavioural traits in an experienced problem and a similar problem. Anim. Cogn. 20, 941–952. 10.1007/s10071-017-1113-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Chu X., Ågmo A. (2014). Sociosexual behaviours in cycling, intact female rats (Rattus norvegicus) housed in a seminatural environment. Behaviour 151, 1143–1184. 10.1163/1568539X-00003177 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  17. Chu X., Guarraci F. A., Ågmo A. (2015). Sociosexual behaviors and reproductive success of rats (Rattus norvegicus) in a seminatural environment. Physiol. Behav. 151, 46–54. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.07.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Clarkson J. M., Dwyer D. M., Flecknell P. A., Leach M. C., Rowe C. (2018). Handling method alters the hedonic value of reward in laboratory mice. Sci. Rep. 8, 2448. 10.1038/s41598-018-20716-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Crabbe J. C., Wahlsten D., Dudek B. C. (1999). Genetics of mouse behavior: interactions with laboratory environment. Science 284, 1670–1672. 10.1126/science.284.5420.1670 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Crowley S. T., Kataoka K., Itaka K. (2018). Combined catwalk index: an improved method to measure mouse motor function using the automated gait analysis system. BMC Res. Notes 11, 263. 10.1186/s13104-018-3374-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Crusio W. E. (2001). Genetic dissection of mouse exploratory behaviour. Behav. Brain Res. 125, 127–132. 10.1016/S0166-4328(01)00280-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Crusio W. E., Schwegler H. (2005). Learning spatial orientation tasks in the radial-maze and structural variation in the hippocampus in inbred mice. Behav. Brai. Funct. 1, 3. 10.1186/1744-9081-1-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Davies J. R., Purawijaya D. A., Bartlett J. M., Robinson E. S. J. (2022). Impact of refinements to handling and restraint methods in mice. Animals 12, 2173. 10.3390/ani12172173 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Deacon R. M. (2006). Assessing hoarding in mice. Nat. Protoc. 1, 2828–2830. 10.1038/nprot.2006.171 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Deacon R. M. (2009). Burrowing: a sensitive behavioural assay, tested in five species of laboratory rodents. Behav. Brain Res. 200, 128–133. 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.01.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Deacon R. M. (2011). Hyponeophagia: a measure of anxiety in the mouse. J. Vis. Exp. 51, 2613. 10.3791/2613 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Deacon R. M. (2013a). The successive alleys test of anxiety in mice and rats. J. Vis. Exp. 76, 2705. 10.3791/2705 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Deacon R. M. (2013b). Measuring the strength of mice. J. Vis. Exp. 76, 2610. 10.3791/2610 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Dell'Omo G., Ricceri L., Wolfer D. P., Poletaeva I. I., Lipp H. (2000). Temporal and spatial adaptation to food restriction in mice under naturalistic conditions. Behav. Brain Res. 115, 1–8. 10.1016/S0166-4328(00)00234-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Detrait E., Brohez C., Hanon E., De Ryck M. (2010). Automation of continuous spontaneous alternation to increase the throughput for in vivo screening of cognitive enhancers. Optimization of the Ethovision system for the Y-maze test in mice. Proceed. Meas. Behav. 2010, 141–144 [Google Scholar]
  31. d'Isa R., Brambilla R., Fasano S. (2014). Behavioral methods for the study of the Ras-ERK pathway in memory formation and consolidation: passive avoidance and novel object recognition tests. Methods Mol. Biol. 1120, 131–156. 10.1007/978-1-62703-791-4_9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. d'Isa R., Clapcote S. J., Voikar V., Wolfer D. P., Giese K. P., Brambilla R., et al. (2011). Mice lacking ras-GRF1 show contextual fear conditioning but not spatial memory impairments: convergent evidence from two independently generated mouse mutant lines. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 5, 78. 10.3389/fnbeh.2011.0007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. d'Isa R., Comi G., Leocani L. (2021a). The 4-hole-board test for assessment of long-term spatial memory in mice. Curr. Protoc. 1, e228. 10.1002/cpz1.228 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. d'Isa R., Comi G., Leocani L. (2021b). Apparatus design and behavioural testing protocol for the evaluation of spatial working memory in mice through the spontaneous alternation T-maze. Sci. Rep. 11, 21177. 10.1038/s41598-021-00402-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Dorninger F., Zeitler G., Berger J. (2020). Nestlet shredding and nest building tests to assess features of psychiatric disorders in mice. Bio-Protoc. 10, e3863. 10.21769/BioProtoc.3863 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Everson C. A., Bergmann B. M., Rechtschaffen A. (1989). Sleep deprivation in the rat: III. Total sleep deprivation. Sleep 12, 13–21. 10.1093/sleep/12.1.13 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Feng W., Zhang Y., Wang Z., Wang T., Pang Y., Zou Y., et al. (2021). A water-reward task assay for evaluating mouse mutualistic cooperative behaviour. bioRxiv [preprint]. 10.1101/2021.02.06.430037 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Frisch C., Hasenöhrl R. U., Mattern C. M., Häcker R., Huston J. P. (1995). Blockade of lithium chloride-induced conditioned place aversion as a test for antiemetic agents: comparison of metoclopramide with combined extracts of Zingiber officinale and Ginkgo biloba. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 52, 321–327. 10.1016/0091-3057(95)00073-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Fucich E. A., Morilak D. A. (2018). Shock-probe defensive burying test to measure active versus passive coping style in response to an aversive stimulus in rats. Bio-Protoc. 8, e2998. 10.21769/BioProtoc.2998 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Fukumoto K., Chaki S. (2015). Involvement of serotonergic system in the effect of a metabotropic glutamate 5 receptor antagonist in the novelty-suppressed feeding test. J. Pharmacol. Sci. 127, 57–61. 10.1016/j.jphs.2014.09.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Gaillard D., Stratford J. M. (2016). Measurement of behavioral taste responses in mice: two-bottle preference, lickometer, and conditioned taste-aversion tests. Curr. Protoc. Mouse Biol. 6, 380–407. 10.1002/cpmo.18 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Galsworthy M. J., Amrein I., Kuptsov P. A., Poletaeva I. I., Zinn P., Rau A., et al. (2005). A comparison of wild-caught wood mice and bank voles in the intellicage: assessing exploration, daily activity patterns and place learning paradigms. Behav. Brain Res. 157, 211–217. 10.1016/j.bbr.2004.06.021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Gerlai R. (1998). A new continuous alternation task in T-maze detects hippocampal dysfunction in mice. A strain comparison and lesion study. Behav. Brain Res. 95, 91–101. 10.1016/S0166-4328(97)00214-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Gerlai R. (2019). Reproducibility and replicability in zebrafish behavioral neuroscience research. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 178, 30–38. 10.1016/j.pbb.2018.02.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Gerlai R. (2021). From genes to behavior: the question of evolutionary conservation and the role of ethology in the analysis of the zebrafish. Front. Neuroanat. 15, 809967. 10.3389/fnana.2021.809967 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Gerlai R., Clayton N. S. (1999a). Analysing hippocampal function in transgenic mice: an ethological perspective. Trends Neurosci. 22, 47–51. 10.1016/S0166-2236(98)01346-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Gerlai R., Clayton N. S. (1999b). Tapping artificially into natural talents – reply. Trends Neurosci. 22, 301–302. 10.1016/S0166-2236(99)01421-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Gerlai R., Crusio W. E., Csányi V. (1990). Inheritance of species specific behaviors in the paradise fish (Macropodus opercularis): a diallel study. Behav. Genet. 20, 487–498. 10.1007/BF01067715 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Giménez-Llort L., Schiffmann S. N., Shmidt T., Canela L., Camón L., Wassholm M., et al. (2007). Working memory deficits in transgenic rats overexpressing human adenosine A2A receptors in the brain. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 87, 42–56. 10.1016/j.nlm.2006.05.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Goh J., Ladiges W. (2015). Voluntary wheel running in mice. Curr. Protoc. Mouse Biol. 5, 283–290. 10.1002/9780470942390.mo140295 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Gouveia K., Hurst J. L. (2013). Reducing mouse anxiety during handling: effect of experience with handling tunnels. PLoS ONE 8, e66401. 10.1371/journal.pone.0066401 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Gouveia K., Hurst J. L. (2017). Optimising reliability of mouse performance in behavioural testing: The major role of non-aversive handling. Sci. Rep. 7, 44999. 10.1038/srep44999 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Gouveia K., Hurst J. L. (2019). Improving the practicality of using non-aversive handling methods to reduce background stress and anxiety in laboratory mice. Sci. Rep. 9, 20305. 10.1038/s41598-019-56860-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Gu Y., Han Y., Ren S., Zhang B., Zhao Y., Wang X., et al. (2022). Correlation among gut microbiota, fecal metabolites and autism-like behavior in an adolescent valproic acid-induced rat autism model. Behav. Brain Res. 417, 113580. 10.1016/j.bbr.2021.113580 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Guarraci F. A., Frohardt R. J. (2019). “What a girl wants”: what can we learn from animal models of female sexual motivation? Front. Behav. Neurosci. 13, 216. 10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00216 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Haga Y. (1995). Effects of food deprivation and food reward on the behavior of rats in the radial- arm maze. Jpn. Psychol. Res. 37, 252–257. 10.4992/psycholres1954.37.2527991224 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  57. Haller J., Tóth M., Halasz J., De Boer S. F. (2006). Patterns of violent aggression-induced brain c-fos expression in male mice selected for aggressiveness. Physiol. Behav. 88, 173–182. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.03.030 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Harries C., Mourgue G., Purefoy J. (2020). Amazing Animal Friends. Documentary Series, Episode 1-03. London: Oxford Scientific Films. [Google Scholar]
  59. Harris R. B. S. (2017). “The hamster as a model for human ingestive behavior”, in Appetite and Food Intake: Central Control, 2nd edn, eds R. B. S. Harris (Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press/Taylor and Francis), Chapter 3. [Google Scholar]
  60. Heinla I., Chu X., Ågmo A., Snoeren E. (2021). Rat ultrasonic vocalizations and novelty-induced social and non-social investigation behavior in a seminatural environment. Physiol. Behav. 237, 113450. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2021.113450 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Ho H., Kejzar N., Sasaguri H., Saito T., Saido T. C., De Strooper B., et al. (2022). A novel fully-automated system for lifelong continuous phenotyping of mouse cognition and behaviour. bioRxiv [preprint]. 10.1101/2022.06.18.496688 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Holmes A., le Guisquet A. M., Vogel E., Millstein R. A., Leman S., Belzung C. (2005). Early life genetic, epigenetic and environmental factors shaping emotionality in rodents. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 29, 1335–1346. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.04.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Houwing D. J., Heijkoop R., Olivier J. D. A., Snoeren E. M. S. (2019). Perinatal fluoxetine exposure changes social and stress-coping behavior in adult rats housed in a seminatural environment. Neuropharmacology 151, 84–97. 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2019.03.037 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Huang Y., Li L., Dong K., Tang H., Yang Q., Jia X., et al. (2020). topological shape changes weaken the innate defensive response to visual threat in mice. Neurosci. Bull. 36, 427–431. 10.1007/s12264-019-00454-w [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Hurst J. L., West R. S. (2010). Taming anxiety in laboratory mice. Nat. Methods 7, 825–826. 10.1038/nmeth.1500 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Iman I. N., Yusof N. A. M., Talib U. N., Ahmad N. A. Z., Norazit A., Kumar J., et al. (2021). The intellicage system: a review of its utility as a novel behavioral platform for a rodent model of substance use disorder. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 15, 683780. 10.3389/fnbeh.2021.683780 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Inostroza M., Cid E., Menendez de la Prida L., Sandi C. (2012). Different emotional disturbances in two experimental models of temporal lobe epilepsy in rats. PLoS ONE 7, e38959. 10.1371/journal.pone.0038959 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Ioannidou C., Marsicano G., Busquets-Garcia A. (2018). Assessing prepulse inhibition of startle in mice. Bio-Protoc. 8, e2789. 10.21769/BioProtoc.2789 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Jacobs S. A., Huang F., Tsien J. Z., Wei W. (2016). Social recognition memory test in rodents. Bio-Protoc. 6, e1804. 10.21769/BioProtoc.1804 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  70. Jaramillo S., Zador A. M. (2014). Mice and rats achieve similar levels of performance in an adaptive decision-making task. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 8, 173. 10.3389/fnsys.2014.00173 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Jarvik M. E., Kopp R. (1967). An improved one-trial passive avoidance learning situation. Psychol. Rep. 21, 221–224. 10.2466/pr0.1967.21.1.221 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Kafkafi N., Agassi J., Benjamini Y., Chesler E., Crabbe J., Crusio W., et al. (2018). Reproducibility and Replicability of mouse phenotyping in pre-clinical studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 87, 218–232. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.01.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Kaidanovich-Beilin O., Lipina T., Vukobradovic I., Roder J., Woodgett J. R. (2011). Assessment of social interaction behaviors. J. Vis. Exp. 48, 2473. 10.3791/2473 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Kampmann M. (2020). CRISPR-based functional genomics for neurological disease. Nat. Rev. Neurol. 16, 465–480. 10.1038/s41582-020-0373-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Kaufman D. M., Kaufman D. W. (1984). Size preference for novel objects by the eastern woodrat (Neotoma floridana) under field conditions. Trans. Kans. Acad. Sci. 87, 129–131. 10.2307/3627848 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  76. Kempermann G., Lopes J. B., Zocher S., Schilling S., Ehret F., Garthe A., et al. (2022). The individuality paradigm: automated longitudinal activity tracking of large cohorts of genetically identical mice in an enriched environment. Neurobiol. Dis. 175, 105916. 10.1016/j.nbd.2022.105916 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Kiryk A., Janusz A., Zglinicki B., Turkes E., Knapska E., Konopka W., et al. (2020). IntelliCage as a tool for measuring mouse behavior - 20 years perspective. Behav. Brain Res. 388, 112620. 10.1016/j.bbr.2020.112620 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Kline L. W. (1899a). Methods in animal psychology. Am. J. Psychol. 10, 256–279. 10.2307/1412481 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  79. Kline L. W. (1899b). Suggestions toward a laboratory course in comparative psychology. Am. J. Psychol. 10, 399–430. 10.2307/1412142 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  80. Kline L. W. (1928). Letter to Walter Richard Miles, March 27, 1928. [Google Scholar]
  81. Kohler J., Mei J., Banneke S., Winter Y., Endres M., Emmrich J. V. (2022). Assessing spatial learning and memory in mice: classic radial maze versus a new animal-friendly automated radial maze allowing free access and not requiring food deprivation. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 16, 1013624. 10.3389/fnbeh.2022.1013624 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. König C., Plank A. C., Kapp A., Timotius I. K., von Hörsten S., Zimmermann K. (2020). Thirty mouse strain survey of voluntary physical activity and energy expenditure: influence of strain, sex and day-night variation. Front. Neurosci. 14, 531. 10.3389/fnins.2020.00531 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Koolhaas J. M., Coppens C. M., de Boer S. F., Buwalda B., Meerlo P., Timmermans P. J. (2013). The resident-intruder paradigm: a standardized test for aggression, violence and social stress. J. Vis. Exp. 77, e4367. 10.3791/4367 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Kopp R., Bohdanecky Z., Jarvik M. E. (1966). Long temporal gradient of retrograde amnesia for a well-discriminated stimulus. Science. 153, 1547–1549. 10.1126/science.153.3743.1547 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Lammert C. R., Frost E. L., Bolte A. C., Paysour M. J., Shaw M. E., Bellinger C. E., et al. (2018). Cutting edge: critical roles for microbiota-mediated regulation of the immune system in a prenatal immune activation model of autism. J. Immunol. 201, 845–850. 10.4049/jimmunol.1701755 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Lavi K., Jacobson G. A., Rosenblum K., Lüthi A. (2018). Encoding of conditioned taste aversion in cortico-amygdala circuits. Cell Rep. 24, 278–283. 10.1016/j.celrep.2018.06.053 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Le Moëne O., Hernández-Arteaga E., Chu X., Ågmo A. (2020). Rapid changes in sociosexual behaviors around transition to and from behavioral estrus, in female rats housed in a seminatural environment. Behav. Processes 174, 104101. 10.1016/j.beproc.2020.104101 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Lee Y. C., Lu S. C., Hsieh Y. L. (2018). Establishing a mouse model of a pure small fiber neuropathy with the ultrapotent agonist of transient receptor potential vanilloid type 1. J. Vis. Exp. 132, 56651. 10.3791/56651 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Lee Y. J., Lin H. T., Chaudhary M. A., Lee Y. C., Wang D. C. (2021). Effects of prenatal phthalate exposure and childhood exercise on maternal behaviors in female rats at postpartum: a role of oxtr methylation in the hypothalamus. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 22, 9847. 10.3390/ijms22189847 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Linnenbrink M., von Merten S. (2017). No speed dating please! Patterns of social preference in male and female house mice. Front. Zool. 14, 38. 10.1186/s12983-017-0224-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Luong T. N., Carlisle H. J., Southwell A., Patterson P. H. (2011). Assessment of motor balance and coordination in mice using the balance beam. J. Vis. Exp. 49, 2376. 10.3791/2376 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Mandillo S., Tucci V., Hölter S. M., Meziane H., Banchaabouchi M. A., Kallnik M., et al. (2008). Reliability, robustness, and reproducibility in mouse behavioral phenotyping: a cross-laboratory study. Physiol. Genomics 34, 243–255. 10.1152/physiolgenomics.90207.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Matsumoto K., Ishihara K., Tanaka K., Inoue K., Fushiki T. (1996). An adjustable-current swimming pool for the evaluation of endurance capacity of mice. J. Appl. Physiol. (1985) 81, 1843–1849. 10.1152/jappl.1996.81.4.1843 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. McReynolds W. E., Weir M. W., DeFries J. C. (1967). Open-field behavior in mice: effect of test illumination. Psychon. Sci. 9, 277–278. 10.3758/BF03332220 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  95. Mei J., Kohler J., Winter Y., Spies C., Endres M., Banneke S., et al. (2020). Automated radial 8-arm maze: a voluntary and stress-free behavior test to assess spatial learning and memory in mice. Behav. Brain Res. 381, 112352. 10.1016/j.bbr.2019.112352 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Miles W. R. (1930). On the history of research with rats and mazes: a collection of notes. J. Gen. Psychol. 3, 324–337. 10.1080/00221309.1930.9918210 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  97. Mills T. W. (1888). Squirrels: their habits and intelligence, with special reference to feigning. Proceed. Trans. Royal Soc. Canada (1st Series) 5, 175–188. [Google Scholar]
  98. Mills T. W. (1890). Intelligence of squirrels. Popular Sci. Monthly 36, 829–835. [Google Scholar]
  99. Mills T. W. (1893). Hibernation and allied states in animals. Proceed. Trans. Royal Soc. Canada (1st Series) 10, 49–66.12379486 [Google Scholar]
  100. Mills T. W. (1895). The psychic development of young animals and its physical correlation: V – the rabbit and the cavy or guinea pig. Proceed. Trans. Royal Soc. Canada (2nd Series) 1, 231–239. [Google Scholar]
  101. Mills T. W. (1898). The nature and development of animal intelligence. New York, NY: Macmillan. [Google Scholar]
  102. Mingrone A., Kaffman A., Kaffman A. (2020). The promise of automated home-cage monitoring in improving translational utility of psychiatric research in rodents. Front. Neurosci. 14, 618593. 10.3389/fnins.2020.618593 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Mitchell E. J., Brett R. R., Armstrong J. D., Sillito R. R., Pratt J. A. (2020). Temporal dissociation of phencyclidine: induced locomotor and social alterations in rats using an automated homecage monitoring system - implications for the 3Rs and preclinical drug discovery. J. Psychopharmacol. 34, 709–715. 10.1177/0269881120920455 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Mitjans M., Begemann M., Ju A., Dere E., Wüstefeld L., Hofer S., et al. (2017). Sexual dimorphism of AMBRA1-related autistic features in human and mouse. Transl. Psychiatry 7, e1247. 10.1038/tp.2017.213 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Mizunoya W., Oyaizu S., Ishihara K., Fushiki T. (2002). Protocol for measuring the endurance capacity of mice in an adjustable-current swimming pool. Biosci. Biotechnol. Biochem. 66, 1133–1136. 10.1271/bbb.66.1133 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Montag-Sallaz M., Montag D. (2003). Severe cognitive and motor coordination deficits in tenascin-R-deficient mice. Genes Brain Behav. 2, 20–31. 10.1034/j.1601-183X.2003.00003.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Montoya B., Gutiérrez G. (2016). Social context modulates food hoarding in Syrian hamsters. Int. J. Psychol. Res. 9, 61–69. 10.21500/20112084.2646 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  108. Moreira P. S., Almeida P. R., Leite-Almeida H., Sousa N., Costa P. (2016). Impact of chronic stress protocols in learning and memory in rodents: systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS ONE 11, e0163245. 10.1371/journal.pone.0163245 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Müller I., Fendt M. (2023). Fear conditioning in laboratory rodents. Neuromethods 190, 119–160. 10.1007/978-1-0716-2748-8_8 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  110. Murai T., Okuda S., Tanaka T., Ohta H. (2007). Characteristics of object location memory in mice: behavioral and pharmacological studies. Physiol. Behav. 90, 116–124. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.09.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Nedergaard P., Sanchez C., Mellerup E. (2004). Different roles of 5-HT2A and 5-HT2C receptors in regulation of female rat paced mating behaviour. Behav. Brain Res. 149, 151–157. 10.1016/S0166-4328(03)00215-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Neely C. L. C., Pedemonte K. A., Boggs K. N., Flinn J. M. (2019). Nest building behavior as an early indicator of behavioral deficits in mice. J. Vis. Exp. 152, e60139. 10.3791/60139 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Nomoto K., Ikumi M., Otsuka M., Asaba A., Kato M., Koshida N., et al. (2018). Female mice exhibit both sexual and social partner preferences for vocalizing males. Integr. Zool. 13, 735–744. 10.1111/1749-4877.12357 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Nowak R. M., Paradiso J. L. (1983). Walker's Mammals of the World (4th Edition). Baltimore: The Johns Hopkins University Press. [Google Scholar]
  115. Opitz B., Mothes H. K., Clausing P. (1997). Effects of prenatal ethanol exposure and early experience on radial maze performance and conditioned taste aversion in mice. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 19, 185–190. 10.1016/S0892-0362(96)00225-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Otsuka S. (2017). Predator odor-induced freezing test for mice. Bio-Protoc. 7, e2534. 10.21769/BioProtoc.2534 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Papale A., d'Isa R., Menna E., Cerovic M., Solari N., Hardingham N., et al. (2017). Severe intellectual disability and enhanced gamma-aminobutyric acidergic synaptogenesis in a novel model of rare rasopathies. Biol. Psychiatry 81, 179–192. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2016.06.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Paré W. P., Tejani-Butt S., Kluczynski J. (2001). The emergence test: effects of psychotropic drugs on neophobic disposition in Wistar Kyoto (WKY) and Sprague Dawley rats. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 25, 1615–1628. 10.1016/S0278-5846(01)00204-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Paredes R. G., Alonso A. (1997). Sexual behavior regulated (paced) by the female induces conditioned place preference. Behav. Neurosci. 111, 123–128. 10.1037/0735-7044.111.1.123 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Pittenger C., Fasano S., Mazzocchi-Jones D., Dunnett S. B., Kandel E. R., Brambilla R. (2006). Impaired bidirectional synaptic plasticity and procedural memory formation in striatum-specific cAMP response element-binding protein-deficient mice. J. Neurosci. 26, 2808–2813. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5406-05.2006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Pitzer C., Kurpiers B., Eltokhi A. (2021). Gait performance of adolescent mice assessed by the CatWalk XT depends on age, strain and sex and correlates with speed and body weight. Sci. Rep. 11, 21372. 10.1038/s41598-021-00625-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Plucinska K., Strachan L., Peeters D., Platt B., Riedel G. (2012). Social transmission of food preference in C57BL/6 mice. Proceed. Meas. Behav. 2012, 488–490. [Google Scholar]
  123. Popović N., Baño-Otálora B., Rol M. A., Caballero-Bleda M., Madrid J. A., Popović M. (2009). Aging and time-of-day effects on anxiety in female octodon degus. Behav. Brain 200, 117–121. 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.01.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Poulos A. M., Mehta N., Lu B., Amir D., Livingston B., Santarelli A., et al. (2016). Conditioning- and time-dependent increases in context fear and generalization. Learn. Mem. 23, 379–385. 10.1101/lm.041400.115 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Puścian A., Łeski S., Kasprowicz G., Winiarski M., Borowska J., Nikolaev T., et al. (2016). Eco-HAB as a fully automated and ecologically relevant assessment of social impairments in mouse models of autism. Elife 5, e19532. 10.7554/eLife.19532.040 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Redman K. R. (1974). The Effectiveness of Dusting as a Reinforcer for the Chinchilla (Chinchilla Lanigera). University of North Dakota Theses and Dissertations 3448, 1–88 (Master's Degree Thesis: ). Grand Forks, ND: University of North Dakota. [Google Scholar]
  127. Reppucci C. J., Veenema A. H. (2020). The social versus food preference test: a behavioral paradigm for studying competing motivated behaviors in rodents. MethodsX 7, 101119. 10.1016/j.mex.2020.101119 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. Richter C. P. (1957). On the phenomenon of sudden death in animals and man. Psychosom. Med. 19, 191–198. 10.1097/00006842-195705000-00004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. Robinson L., Plano A., Cobb S., Riedel G. (2013). Long-term home cage activity scans reveal lowered exploratory behaviour in symptomatic female Rett mice. Behav. Brain Res. 250, 148–156. 10.1016/j.bbr.2013.04.041 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  130. Rosenberg M., Zhang T., Perona P., Meister M. (2021). Mice in a labyrinth show rapid learning, sudden insight, and efficient exploration. Elife 10, e66175. 10.7554/eLife.66175.sa2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Rosenfeld C. S., Ferguson S. A. (2014). Barnes maze testing strategies with small and large rodent models. J. Vis. Exp. 84, e51194. 10.3791/51194 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  132. Ruzza C., Asth L., Guerrini R., Trapella C., Gavioli E. C. (2015). Neuropeptide S reduces mouse aggressiveness in the resident/intruder test through selective activation of the neuropeptide S receptor. Neuropharmacology 97, 1–6. 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2015.05.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  133. Seibenhener M. L., Wooten M. C. (2015). Use of the open field maze to measure locomotor and anxiety-like behavior in mice. J. Vis. Exp. 96, e52434. 10.3791/52434 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  134. Sensini F., Inta D., Palme R., Brandwein C., Pfeiffer N., Riva M. A., et al. (2020). The impact of handling technique and handling frequency on laboratory mouse welfare is sex-specific. Sci. Rep. 10, 17281. 10.1038/s41598-020-74279-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  135. Shin J. and Ko, J.. (2021). Protocol for quantitative assessment of social cooperation in mice. STAR Protoc. 2, 100305. 10.1016/j.xpro.2021.100305 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  136. Shoji H., Takao K., Hattori S., Miyakawa T. (2014). Contextual and cued fear conditioning test using a video analyzing system in mice. J. Vis. Exp. 85, 50871. 10.3791/50871 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  137. Silveira K. M., Joca S. (2023). Learned helplessness in rodents. Neuromethods 190, 161–184. 10.1007/978-1-0716-2748-8_9 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  138. Small W. S. (1899). Notes on the psychic development of the young white rat. Am. J. Psych. 11, 80–100. 10.2307/1412730 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  139. Small W. S. (1900). An experimental study of the mental processes of the rat. Am. J. Psych. 11, 133–165. 10.2307/1412267 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  140. Small W. S. (1901). Experimental study of the mental process of the rat. II. Am. J. Psych. 12, 206–239. 10.2307/1412534 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  141. Steinbach J. M., Garza E. T., Ryan B. C. (2016). Novel object exploration as a potential assay for higher order repetitive behaviors in mice. J. Vis. Exp. 114, 54324. 10.3791/54324 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  142. Stern J. J., Merari A. (1969). The bathing behavior of the chinchilla: effects of deprivation. Psychon. Sci. 14, 115. 10.3758/BF03332734 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  143. Stewart C. C. (1898). Variations in daily activity produced by alcohol and by changes in barometric pressure and diet, with a description of recording methods. Am. J. Physiol. 1, 40–56. 10.1152/ajplegacy.1898.1.1.40 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  144. Strekalova T. (2023). How the sucrose preference succeeds or fails as a measurement of anhedonia. Neuromethods 190, 89–102. 10.1007/978-1-0716-2748-8_615627051 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  145. Takahashi S., Ohmiya M., Honda S., Ni K. (2018). The KCNH3 inhibitor ASP2905 shows potential in the treatment of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. PLoS ONE 13, e0207750. 10.1371/journal.pone.0207750 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  146. Takao K., Miyakawa T. (2006). Light/dark transition test for mice. J. Vis. Exp. 1, 104. 10.3791/104 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  147. Teng C. J., Abbott F. V. (1998). The formalin test: a dose-response analysis at three developmental stages. Pain 76, 337–347. 10.1016/S0304-3959(98)00065-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  148. Ueno H., Takahashi Y., Suemitsu S., Murakami S., Kitamura N., Wani K., et al. (2020). Effects of repetitive gentle handling of male C57BL/6NCrl mice on comparative behavioural test results. Sci. Rep. 10, 3509. 10.1038/s41598-020-60530-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  149. Urbach Y. K., Raber K. A., Appl T., Häberle L., von Horsten S. (2008). Automated phenotyping using intra-home-cage technology. Proceed. Measur. Behav. 2008, 330. [Google Scholar]
  150. Valsamis B., Schmid S. (2011). Habituation and prepulse inhibition of acoustic startle in rodents. J. Vis. Exp. 55, e3446. 10.3791/3446 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  151. Vander Wall S. B. (1990). Food Hoarding in Animals. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
  152. Vannoni E., Voikar V., Colacicco G., Sánchez M. A., Lipp H. P., Wolfer D. P. (2014). Spontaneous behavior in the social homecage discriminates strains, lesions and mutations in mice. J. Neurosci. Methods 234, 26–37. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2014.04.026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  153. Visigalli I., Delai S., Politi L. S., Di Domenico C., Cerri F., Mrak E., et al. (2010). Gene therapy augments the efficacy of hematopoietic cell transplantation and fully corrects mucopolysaccharidosis type I phenotype in the mouse model. Blood 116, 5130–5139. 10.1182/blood-2010-04-278234 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  154. Voikar V., Gaburro S. (2020). Three pillars of automated home-cage phenotyping of mice: novel findings, refinement, and reproducibility based on literature and experience. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 14, 575434. 10.3389/fnbeh.2020.575434 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  155. Võikar V., Stanford S. C. (2023). The open field test. Neuromethods 190, 9–29. 10.1007/978-1-0716-2748-8_2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  156. Vollmayr B., Henn F. A. (2001). Learned helplessness in the rat: improvements in validity and reliability. Brain Res. Brain Res. Protoc. 8, 1–7. 10.1016/S1385-299X(01)00067-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  157. Vyssotski A. L., Dell'Omo G., Poletaeva I. I., Vyssotsk D. L., Minichiello L., Klein R., et al. (2002). Long-term monitoring of hippocampus-dependent behavior in naturalistic settings: mutant mice lacking neurotrophin receptor TrkB in the forebrain show spatial learning but impaired behavioral flexibility. Hippocampus 12, 27–38. 10.1002/hipo.10002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  158. Walf A. A., Frye C. A. (2007). The use of the elevated plus maze as an assay of anxiety-related behavior in rodents. Nat. Protoc. 2, 322–328. 10.1038/nprot.2007.44 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  159. Warner L. (1932). The association span of the white rat. J. Genet. Psychol. 41, 57–90. 10.1080/08856559.1932.9944143 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  160. Winiarski M., Kondrakiewicz L., Kondrakiewicz K., Jedrzejewska-Szmek J., Turzyński K., Knapska E., et al. (2022). Social deficits in BTBR T+ Itpr3tf/J mice vary with ecological validity of the test. Genes Brain Behav. 21, e12814. 10.1111/gbb.12814 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  161. Winters C., Gorssen W., Ossorio-Salazar V. A., Nilsson S., Golden S., D'Hooge R. (2022). Automated procedure to assess pup retrieval in laboratory mice. Sci. Rep. 12, 1663. 10.1038/s41598-022-05641-w [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  162. Witkin I. M., Smith J. L. (2023). Marble burying in mice. Neuromethods 190, 71–88. 10.1007/978-1-0716-2748-8_5 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  163. Wrenn C. C., Harris A. P., Saavedra M. C., Crawley J. N. (2003). Social transmission of food preference in mice: methodology and application to galanin-overexpressing transgenic mice. Behav. Neurosci. 117, 21–31. 10.1037/0735-7044.117.1.21 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  164. Yang X., Liu Q., Zhong J., Song R., Zhang L., Wang L. (2020). A simple threat-detection strategy in mice. BMC Biol. 18, 93. 10.1186/s12915-020-00825-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  165. Zhang Y., Yu F., Yi X., Zhou W., Liu R., Holyoak M., et al. (2022). Evolutionary and ecological patterns of scatter- and larder-hoarding behaviours in rodents. Ecol. Lett. 25, 1202–1214. 10.1111/ele.13992 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  166. Zhu K., Sun J., Kang Z., Zou Z., Wu G., Wang J. (2017). Electroacupuncture promotes remyelination after cuprizone treatment by enhancing myelin debris clearance. Front. Neurosci. 10, 613. 10.3389/fnins.2016.00613 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  167. Zipse L. R., Brandling-Bennett E. M., Clark A. S. (2000). Paced mating behavior in the naturally cycling and the hormone-treated female rat. Physiol. Behav. 70, 205–259. 10.1016/S0031-9384(00)00242-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  168. Zocher S., Schilling S., Grzyb A. N., Adusumilli V. S., Bogado Lopes J., Günther S., et al. (2020). Early-life environmental enrichment generates persistent individualized behavior in mice. Sci. Adv. 6, eabb1478. 10.1126/sciadv.abb1478 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Frontiers Media SA

RESOURCES