Skip to main content
Cerebral Cortex (New York, NY) logoLink to Cerebral Cortex (New York, NY)
. 2022 Mar 4;33(3):597–611. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhac087

Altered prefrontal signaling during inhibitory control in a salient drug context in cocaine use disorder

Ahmet O Ceceli 1,2, Muhammad A Parvaz 2,3,2, Sarah King 4, Matthew Schafer 5, Pias Malaker 6, Akarsh Sharma 7, Nelly Alia-Klein 8,9, Rita Z Goldstein 10,11,
PMCID: PMC9890460  PMID: 35244138

Abstract

Introduction

Drug addiction is characterized by impaired response inhibition and salience attribution (iRISA), where the salience of drug cues is postulated to overpower that of other reinforcers with a concomitant decrease in self-control. However, the neural underpinnings of the interaction between the salience of drug cues and inhibitory control in drug addiction remain unclear.

Methods

We developed a novel stop-signal functional magnetic resonance imaging task where the stop-signal reaction time (SSRT–a classical inhibitory control measure) was tested under different salience conditions (modulated by drug, food, threat, or neutral words) in individuals with cocaine use disorder (CUD; n = 26) versus demographically matched healthy control participants (n = 26).

Results

Despite similarities in drug cue-related SSRT and valence and arousal word ratings between groups, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) activity was diminished during the successful inhibition of drug versus food cues in CUD and was correlated with lower frequency of recent use, lower craving, and longer abstinence (Z > 3.1, P < 0.05 corrected).

Discussion

Results suggest altered involvement of cognitive control regions (e.g. dlPFC) during inhibitory control under a drug context, relative to an alternative reinforcer, in CUD. Supporting the iRISA model, these results elucidate the direct impact of drug-related cue reactivity on the neural signature of inhibitory control in drug addiction.

Keywords: craving, cue reactivity, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, response inhibition, stop-signal task

Introduction

The impaired response inhibition and salience attribution (iRISA) model of drug addiction posits that addicted individuals attribute excessive salience to drug and drug-related cues at the expense of nondrug-related reinforcers, with concomitant decreases in inhibitory control (Goldstein and Volkow 2002, 2011). Consistent with the animal literature (Phillips et al. 2003), in human neuroimaging studies increased responses in the mesencephalon (Goldstein, Tomasi, et al. 2009), anterior cingulate cortex, and ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC; Kober et al. 2016; Konova et al. 2019), and connectivity between salience, reward, and executive networks (Ray et al. 2015), have been interpreted to reflect the excessive salience attributed to drug cues in drug-addicted individuals. Inhibitory control (in nondrug contexts) has instead been associated with hypoactivations in the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) in this population as previously reviewed (Zilverstand et al. 2018; Ceceli et al. 2021). However, the interaction between inhibitory control and drug cue reactivity, crucial for more accurately examining the neurobehavioral bases of drug addiction’s core phenomenology, has yet to be well characterized.

Several neuroimaging studies in addicted individuals have used drug-related versus nondrug-related cues in the context of tasks approximating inhibitory control such as the Go/No-Go (Ames et al. 2014; Czapla et al. 2017; Gilman et al. 2018) and Stroop tasks (Carpenter et al. 2006; Hester et al. 2006; Goldstein et al. 2007; Goldstein, Alia-Klein, et al. 2009; Goldstein, Tomasi, et al. 2009b; Smith and Ersche 2014; DeVito et al. 2018). Using the Go/No-Go task, increased activations in the dlPFC, vmPFC, and insula during alcohol-related No-Go versus non-alcohol Go trials have been reported in alcohol-dependent subjects (Ames et al. 2014; Czapla et al. 2017), with negative results in smokers (Gilman et al. 2018). Instead of measuring the classical Stroop conflict induced by reading (faster) versus naming (slower) of incongruent color words (Jensen and Rohwer 1966), the drug-Stroop task measures the attentional interference induced by color-naming of drug versus non-drug related words. This drug-related attentional interference manifests as prolonged response times (Ersche et al. 2010) and/or altered neural activation in the brain’s reward/salience circuitry (OFC, anterior cingulate, striatum, and midbrain; Goldstein, Alia-Klein, et al. 2009; Goldstein, Tomasi, et al. 2009; Smith and Ersche 2014). However, while the Go/No-Go task captures response selection (Raud et al. 2020), it does not measure the ability to stop after response initiation, and the drug-Stroop task does not create an inherent inhibitory control demand, rendering both task types insufficient in informing potential lapses in self-control under a motivationally challenging state, such as craving.

The stop-signal task (SST) permits the investigation of inhibitory control by estimating the competition between the “Go” and “Stop” processes (Verbruggen and Logan 2008) and has previously been used to demonstrate the neural correlates of inhibitory control in drug addiction (Li et al. 2008, 2009; Matuskey et al. 2013; Elton et al. 2014; Sjoerds et al. 2014; Hu et al. 2015; Harlé et al. 2016, 2019; Wang et al. 2018; Zhang et al. 2018; Sakoglu et al. 2019; Zhukovsky et al. 2021). However, a gap remains in testing the potential modulation of inhibitory control by drug cue salience for the inspection of their interaction at the brain level. In individuals with cocaine use disorder (CUD) and matched healthy control (HC) subjects, we present results of a novel SST that allowed for the parametric/trial-by-trial modulation of inhibitory control by salient drug, other salient non-drug (e.g. food), and neutral cues (words) during functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Further, we conservatively preselected our data to abide by recent recommendations of best practices in estimating stopping ability (Verbruggen et al. 2019). We probed 3 core hypotheses: (i) regardless of cue type and group, prefrontal regions would drive inhibitory control processes on this hybrid task; (ii) inhibitory control-related prefrontal signaling would be altered in the CUD group; and (iii) the dlPFC (and other inhibitory PFC regions, e.g. the IFG) would be crucial for inhibitory control under drug cue reactivity in CUD, and this effect would be supported by a link with cocaine use severity.

Materials and methods

Participants

Participants were recruited using flyers, newspaper ads, and by word of mouth. From a dataset of 79 participants with valid behavioral data [i.e. behavioral task performance that permitted an estimation of inhibitory control via stop-signal reaction time (SSRT)], we preselected 52 age-, education-, and verbal IQ-matched (using the R package MatchIt; Ho et al. 2011), right-handed (given the verbal nature of this task and concerns about lateralization differences, as estimated with the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory score > 75%) individuals with complete behavioral and fMRI data (n = 26 per group, 43 men, mean age: 43.4 ± 7.6; see Behavioral data analysis and blood oxygen level–dependent (BOLD)-fMRI data analysis sections for details and Table 1 for sample profile).

Table 1.

Sample profile.

CUD (n = 26) HC (n = 26) Significance test
Age 44.07 ± 8.18
Min: 28, Max: 59
42.66 ± 7.05
Min: 27, Max: 55
t(50) = 0.67, P = 0.509
Sex (M/F) 22/4 21/5 χ2(1) = 0.13, P = 0.718
Race (Black/White/Other/Unreported) 17/3/4/2 16/4/6/0 χ2(2) = 1.69, P = 0.430
Education (years; 12 = high school grad) 12.27 ± 1.59
Min: 9, Max: 16
12.92 ± 1.72
Min: 10, Max: 16
t(50) = 1.42, P = 0.160
Verbal IQ (std. WRAT-III score) 95.04 ± 11.65
Min: 76, Max: 116
99.00 ± 11.30
Min: 75, Max: 113
t(50) = 1.24, P = 0.219
Non-verbal IQ (matrix reasoning score) 8.96 ± 2.99
Min: 3, Max: 14
11.12 ± 2.16
Min: 4, Max: 15
t(50) = 2.98, P = 0.004*
Cigarette use per day (cigarettes) 5.11 ± 6.20
Min: 0, Max: 25
2.92 ± 6.21
Min: 0, Max: 20
t(50) = 1.27, P = 0.209
Depression (BDI score) 7.81 ± 7.65
Min: 0, Max: 24
4.46 ± 5.69
Min: 0, Max: 20
t(50) = 1.79, P = 0.079
Duration of cocaine use (years) 16.00 ± 8.15
Min: 3, Max: 30
Past month cocaine use (days/month) 6.08 ± 8.34
Min: 0, Max: 25
Days since last cocaine use 293.23 ± 530.65
Min: 0, Max: 1825
Cocaine Severity of Dependence Scale 5.04 ± 4.87
Min: 0, Max: 15
Cocaine Selective Severity Assessment 16.35 ± 11.33
Min: 0, Max: 37
Cocaine Craving Questionnaire 13.73 ± 13.78
Min: 0, Max: 45

Note: Significant group differences are flagged with an asterisk. ± denotes SD.

Participants underwent a series of clinical and neuropsychological assessments delivered by trained staff under a clinical psychologist’s supervision. These interviews included the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM IV for Axis I Disorders (American Psychiatric Association 2013); the Addiction Severity Index (McLellan et al. 1992)—a semi-structured interview that assesses the severity as well as recent and lifetime history of alcohol- and drug-related problems; the Cocaine Selective Severity Assessment (Kampman et al. 1998) for evaluating cocaine abstinence/withdrawal signs and symptoms (i.e. sleep impairment, anxiety, energy levels, craving, and depressive symptoms) 24 h within time of interview; the 5-item Cocaine Craving Questionnaire (Tiffany et al. 1993); and the 5-item Severity of Dependence Scale (Gossop et al. 1992). A brief physical examination encompassing height, weight, urine drug toxicology, breath alcohol and carbon monoxide (for recent cigarette use) levels, and medical history were also obtained by trained research staff.

Participants were excluded for the following: (i) history of major psychiatric disorders [barring other substance use disorders and highly comorbid illnesses such as post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) for CUD, and nicotine dependence for HC]; (ii) history of seizures or other central nervous system disorders; (iii) cardiovascular, endocrinological, metabolic, oncological, or autoimmune disease; (iv) MRI contraindications; (v) presence of any psychoactive drugs or their metabolites (except for cocaine in the CUD group) in urine assays; (vi) evidence of alcohol and/or any other drug-related intoxication at the time of participation; (vii) positive pregnancy test (determined via a urine assay); and (viii) color blindness (assessed with the Ishihara color blindness test; Clark 1924).

All participants in the CUD group met the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV criteria for substance use disorder (with cocaine and/or crack cocaine as the primary substance of choice, 23 meeting criteria for dependence, and 3 for abuse). Urine screen results confirmed the presence of cocaine in 11 participants in the CUD group. Urine screens for all other drugs were negative (the HC participants tested negative for all drugs, including cocaine). Comorbid diagnoses within CUD included current intermittent explosive disorder (n = 5), PTSD in full remission (n = 1), specific phobias (n = 2), marijuana dependence (n = 13), alcohol abuse and/or dependence (n = 26), opioid dependence (n = 8), amphetamine dependence (n = 1), hallucinogen abuse and/or dependence (n = 2), and polysubstance dependence (n = 3). Diagnoses among HC included intermittent explosive disorder (current: n = 10, past: n = 1), PTSD in full remission (n = 2), specific phobias (n = 1), marijuana abuse and/or dependence (n = 3), and alcohol dependence (n = 6). All substance use comorbidities were in full remission (except for one CUD participant who met criteria for marijuana dependence, partial remission). Participants received full information about the research and provided written consent in accordance with the Institutional Review Board of the Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai and Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association (Declaration of Helsinki; Rickham 1964).

Experimental design and statistical analysis

Emotional stop-signal task

In the traditional SST, participants are instructed to respond to a frequent neutral Go signal as quickly as possible and inhibit responses to this cue when it is followed by a rare stop-signal [e.g. a visual cue or an auditory tone (Verbruggen and Logan 2008) superimposed on the Go signal; see Fig. 1, left].

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

The emotional stop-signal task. Participants are instructed to respond via button presses as quickly and accurately as possible to the change in word color from white to blue/green, and to suppress their responses when the word color turns red after a variable delay (i.e. the SSD). Uniquely, word categories comprised neutral words, as well as drug, food, and threat-related words, permitting the examination of emotional reactivity during inhibitory control. Typical SST figure adapted from Aron and Poldrack 2006.

Here, during fMRI, we presented drug, food, and threat-related words in addition to neutral words as the Go signals in a modified emotional SST (see Fig. 1, right). These words were matched in length and frequency in the English language (mean frequency: drug words = 23.69, food words = 15.70, threat words = 19.87, neutral words = 21.97; Francis et al. 1982). Each trial started with a fixation cross that jittered in duration between 2000 and 2600 ms, which was then followed by a drug, food, threat, or neutral word in white text over a black background (1000 ms). Next, the word in white text changed in color to blue or green, to which the participants were instructed to respond with their right (for blue font) or left (for green font) index finger using magnetic resonance compatible response gloves. These Go trials comprised 50% of trials; in 25% of trials, a stop-signal followed the change in color, such that the blue or green font turned red after a variable delay (i.e. the stop-signal delay, or SSD). This change instructed the participants to suppress their response (when the word color turned red; see Fig. 1, right bottom). The SSD was set to an initial duration of 250 ms and adjusted in parallel to the participant’s stopping ability for each cue type (yielding 4 independent SSD adjustments to precisely estimate stopping speed to each cue). When the participant successfully stopped, the SSD increased by 50 ms (making the next stop trial more difficult), and when the participant failed to stop, the SSD decreased by 50 ms (making the next stop trial easier). The remaining 25% of trials were empty trials in the form of a fixation cross (2 s; same as the Go trial duration) included to minimize anticipatory effects and improve signal detection (Hagberg et al. 2001; Wager and Nichols 2003). Participants underwent 4 runs of this task, each containing 48 Go and 24 Stop trials (18 trials in each word category per run) for a total of 192 Go trials, 96 Stop trials (and 72 trials in each word category). After the magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) session, participants were instructed to rate each word using a 5-point scale on valence [“Please rate how you currently feel about the above words from PLEASANT (leftmost) to UNPLEASANT (rightmost) using the scale.”] and arousal [“Please rate how emotional you currently feel about the above words from VERY EMOTIONAL (leftmost) to CALM (rightmost) using the scale.”].

MRI data acquisition

Scanning was conducted on a Siemens 3 T Skyra (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany), using a 32-channel head coil. The BOLD-fMRI responses were measured as a function of time using a T2*-weighted single shot multi-band accelerated gradient-echo EPI sequence [echo time (TE)/repetition time (TR) = 35/1000 ms, 2.1 mm isotropic resolution, no gap, 70 axial slices for whole brain (14.7 cm) coverage, field of view (FOV) 206 × 181 mm, matrix size 96 × 84, 60°-flip angle (approximately Ernst angle), multi-band factor of 7, blipped CAIPIRINHA phase-encoding shift = FOV/3, ~ 2 kHz/Pixel bandwidth with ramp sampling, echo spacing 0.68 ms, and echo train length 57.1 ms]. Each of the 4 fMRI task runs were ~6 min 30 s in length, totaling about 26 min. T1-weighted anatomical images were acquired using a 3D magnetization-prepared rapid gradient-echo (MPRAGE) sequence [FOV 256 × 256 × 179 mm3, 0.8-mm isotropic resolution, TR/TE/inversion time (TI) = 2400/2.07/1000 ms, flip angle 8° with binomial (1, −1) fat saturation, bandwidth 240 Hz/pixel, echo spacing 7.6 ms, and in-plane acceleration gene ralized autocalibrating partial parallel acquisition (GRAPPA) factor of 2, with a total acquisition time of ~ 7 min]. The 90-min scan session included additional procedures (presented in a randomized order to circumvent possible order effects) reported elsewhere (Moeller et al. 2018).

MRI data preprocessing

Raw fMRI data in DICOM format were converted to NIFTI using dcm2niix (Li et al. 2016) and adapted to Brain Imaging Data Structure (BIDS) standards to enhance neuroimaging data portability and reproducibility (Gorgolewski et al. 2016). BIDS-validated data were then preprocessed via the Nipype-based fMRIPrep pipeline (version 1.5.0) (Gorgolewski et al. 2011; Esteban et al. 2019). FMRIPrep is a robust fMRI preprocessing pipeline that recruits tools from well-established neuroimaging software (e.g. FSL, Freesurfer, AFNI) to standardize and optimize fMRI preprocessing (Esteban et al. 2019). FMRIPrep’s workflow is summarized as follows: structural images were corrected for intensity nonuniformity and skull-stripped using ANTS (Tustison et al. 2010). These structural images were spatially normalized to the ICBM 152 Nonlinear Asymmetrical template via nonlinear registration using ANTS (Avants et al. 2008; Fonov et al. 2009). Brain tissue was segmented into white matter, gray matter, and cerebrospinal fluid using FSL’s FAST (Zhang et al. 2001). Functional data were corrected for motion artifacts using FSL’s MCFLIRT and for distortion using spin-echo field maps acquired in opposing phase encoding directions via AFNI’s 3dQwarp (Cox 1996; Jenkinson et al. 2002). Motion and distortion corrected images were then coregistered to the participant’s structural images using boundary-based registration with 9 degrees of freedom via FSL’s FLIRT (Jenkinson and Smith 2001; Fonov et al. 2009). These correction, transformation, and registration steps were integrated into a single-step transformation workflow using ANTS. In addition to the fMRIPrep workflow, we identified volumes with spikes in translation and rotation parameters in relation to a reference volume. Using a typical boxplot threshold (75th percentile + 1.5 * interquartile range) via FSL’s fsl_motion_outliers package, we regressed out an average of 5.92% of outlier volumes in each run (range: 0.51–14.32%). Average number of outlier volumes regressed out from participants in the CUD group did not significantly differ from those in the HC group, t(50) = 0.71, P = 0.483. As a result, there remained no substantial volume-to-volume movement (framewise displacement was below voxel dimensions at mean motion: 0.05 mm; maximum motion: 0.17 mm), with no significant differences across groups, t(50) = 0.61, P = 0.542. The preprocessed data from the fMRIPrep pipeline were filtered with a high-pass filter (100 s cutoff) to ignore scanner drift and spatially smoothed using a Gaussian kernel (5-mm full-width at half maximum) to improve signal to noise ratio. We set an a priori threshold for mean voxel intensity values in all preprocessed images for quality control: BOLD runs with mean voxel intensity at 3 standard deviations (SDs) above or below the mean of the sample were removed from analyses to mitigate the potential effects of scanner artifacts (excluded CUD n = 2).

Behavioral data analysis

A core assumption of the stop-signal paradigm is that Go and Stop processes are in competition to exert control over motor output—a phenomenon described by the horse-race model of response inhibition (Logan and Cowan 1984). To ensure that we accurately estimated stopping ability using our novel task, we followed a recently documented consensus guide for upholding the validity of the horse-race model while analyzing SST data (Verbruggen et al. 2019). We used the integration method to calculate SSRT, in that we identified the nth RT in the Go RT distribution, with n being the number of Go RTs multiplied by the proportion of trials where a response was made to a stop-signal. The nth represents the finishing time of the stopping process (and improves on the common “mean RT” method) (Verbruggen et al. 2019). Accordingly, we only included data from task runs in which the participant displayed mean Go accuracy of 60% or higher, mean Stop accuracy greater than or equal to 25% but less than or equal to 75% (ensuring that the SSD indeed resulted in a competition between Go and Stop processes), and positive mean SSRT. After removing task runs in which the above criteria were violated, there were no significant differences between groups in number of valid task runs [t(77) = 0.45, P = 0.652].

We performed a mixed-design repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA), with SSRT as the dependent variable, Group (CUD, HC) as the between- and Cue (Drug, Food, Threat, Neutral) as the within-subject factors. We followed up on significant interactions with post hoc t-tests where appropriate. Because the data were preselected based on Go and Stop accuracy performance (needed by definition to ascertain validity of the use of this task as an inhibitory control measure), we did not focus on statistical differences based on these variables. To inspect for potential covariates and the impact of clinical measures, select demographics and drug use severity measures were used in Spearmen’s correlational analyses with average and Drug SSRT, Holm-corrected for multiple comparisons. Post-task ratings to the word cues were entered into 2-way ANOVAs (Group as between-, Cue as within-subject factors, valence and arousal as separate dependent variables) to test whether participants differentially perceived the valence and arousal of the words across cue types and groups.

BOLD-fMRI data analysis

Parameter estimates were generated for each participant using the general linear model (GLM) approach via FSL’s FEAT (version 5.98; Woolrich et al. 2001). Four events (Go_Success for successful Go responses, Go_Fail for missed Go trials or response selection errors, Stop_Success for successfully inhibited responses following a stop-signal, and Stop_Fail for failed inhibitions following a stop-signal) per cue type (drug, food, threat, and neutral) were modeled, yielding 16 cue-specific regressors (e.g. Drug_Stop_Success, Food_Stop_Fail, etc.). Regressors were sampled from the onset of the corresponding trials’ Go signals using 1 sec events and convolved with a canonical hemodynamic response function along with their temporal derivatives to be entered into the GLM. Fixation events were not modeled, in that they contributed to the task baseline for linear contrasts—a practice that is in line with prior analyses of the SST (Aron and Poldrack 2006). We used each cue’s Go_Fail events and the fsl_motion_outlier outputs denoting volumes with excessive spikes in intensity as regressors of no interest. We removed individuals who did not yield complete fMRI regressors to represent behavioral performance in line with the horse-race model assumptions. For example, if a participant’s non-missing task runs were insufficient in yielding all the necessary task events (e.g. a missing Drug_Stop_Success because the participant had no successful stops during Drug cues in their only remaining run), we excluded the participant from analyses. We based our behavioral and neural analyses on a final pool of 52 participants for consistency and examinations of behavioral performance vis-à-vis BOLD activity (see Table 1 for sample profile).

In each of the 4 runs, the first level analyses comprised all regressors’ contrasts with baseline (e.g. Drug_Go_Success > Baseline to examine Go-related processes in drug cue trials). Importantly, we used Stop_Success > Stop_Fail as the hallmark contrast of inhibitory control. Thus, the first-level GLM included contrasts for detecting cue-general (Stop_Success > Stop_Fail regardless of cue type) and cue-specific (e.g. Drug_Stop_Success > Drug_Stop_Fail) inhibitory control processes. We contrasted drug with food—a competing, non-drug positively valenced reinforcer [i.e. (Drug_Stop_Success > Drug_Stop_Fail) > (Food_Stop_Success > Food_Stop_Fail)] to focus specifically on regulation of salient drug versus non-drug-related inhibitory control—a practice with precedence and specificity to predicting drug use in CUD (Martinez et al. 2009; Moeller et al. 2009, 2010, 2018). We also contrasted drug-related inhibition with the non-salient neutral category of cues [i.e. (Drug_Stop_Success > Drug_Stop_Fail) > (Neutral_Stop_Success > Neutral_Stop_Fail)], permitting the examination of drug-related arousal. These contrasts in each run were entered into a fixed-effects model to generate aggregate parameter estimates representing general and cue-specific inhibitory control processes at the participant level. Other iterations of cue contrasts (e.g. Food > Threat, Drug > Threat) were also modeled for consistency, although we did not have an a priori interest in these comparisons for the current purposes. In other words, although we used in the GLM threat cues as the non-drug category that accounts for the potentially negative valence of drug cues, direct comparisons involving threat are outside the scope of our primary interests regarding the iRISA interaction for the current purposes.

To investigate the neural mechanisms of inhibitory control across the CUD and HC groups, the resulting participant-level activation maps were analyzed in a higher level analysis using FSL’s FLAME 1 & 2 (FMRIB’s Local Analysis of Mixed Effects), which improves group-level variance estimates via Markov Chain Monte Carlo simulations to allow for population inferences (Beckmann et al. 2003). We chose a cluster defining threshold of P < 0.001, corrected to a cluster-extent threshold of P < 0.05, in line with common practices to minimize Type I error (Eklund et al. 2016).

To detect cue-general inhibitory control related neural activation associated with overall stopping ability in the task, we performed a similar higher-level analysis with identical thresholding and variance estimation methods, with the addition of mean SSRT as a covariate. We also included cue-specific SSRT covariates that mirrored the BOLD contrasts for cue comparisons. For instance, a covariate containing “Drug SSRT minus Food SSRT” values was created to detect activation patterns during drug versus food inhibition that varied based on behavioral performance to drug versus food cues. Finally, we used select drug use severity measures (i.e. years of cocaine use, frequency of use in past 30 days, days since last use, severity of dependence, withdrawal, and craving) to reveal whether these estimates correlated with any regional activation in the brain during drug cue-related inhibitory control.

Results

Behavioral results

The mixed-design repeated measures ANOVA of SSRT revealed no main effect of Group, F(1,50) = 3.13, P = 0.083, no main effect of Cue, F(3,150) = 0.65, P = 0.584, but a significant Group x Cue interaction, F(3,150) = 3.15, P = 0.027, where HC exhibited prolonged SSRT to Food and Neutral cues (see Fig. 2 and Table 2). Descriptive statistics for Go and Stop accuracy and Go RT-related group differences are included in Table 2 (see also Supplemental Table 1 for the same measures across 79 participants).

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

CUD and HC groups’ SSRT in response to each cue type. We found no significant main effects of group or cue but found a significant group × cue interaction (p = 0.027), where HC were significantly slower to stop than the CUD group in response to food and neutral cues. No drug-cue-related group differences were evident in SSRT.

Table 2.

Behavioral inhibitory control performance.

CUD (n = 26) HC (n = 26) Significance testb
Averagea
Go RT 802.48 ± 73.39
Min: 641.51, Max: 909.12
777.66 ± 59.03
Min: 640.56, Max: 860.80
t(50) = 1.34, P = 0.185
SSRT 396.07 ± 52.02
Min: 312.25, Max: 556.25
423.66 ± 60.14
Min: 280.25, Max: 616.64
t(50) = 1.77, P = 0.083
Go % 77.79 ± 9.48
Min: 62.5, Max: 98.96
84.17 ± 11.35
Min: 62.5, Max: 100
t(50) = 2.20, P = 0.032
Stop % 53.43 ± 9.04
Min: 25.00, Max: 70.83
56.65 ± 9.10
Min: 37.14, Max: 72.09
t(50) = 1.28, P = 0.207
Drug
Go RT 805.75 ± 80.93
Min: 640.54, Max: 931.86
773.47 ± 61.81
Min: 619.23, Max: 860.12
t(50) = 1.62, P = 0.112
SSRT 409.60 ± 68.37
Min: 241.00, Max: 636.00
404.83 ± 91.19
Min: 254.00, Max: 606.89
t(50) = 0.21, P = 0.832
Go % 77.58 ± 12.07
Min: 54.17, Max: 100.00
84.41 ± 11.85
Min: 62.5, Max: 100
t(50) = 2.06, P = 0.045
Stop % 52.35 ± 10.91
Min: 16.67, Max: 66.67
58.45 ± 12.86
Min: 27.87, Max: 83.33
t(50) = 1.85, P = 0.071
Food
Go RT 799.42 ± 70.55
Min: 655.71, Max: 908.67
779.67 ± 66.90
Min: 638.64, Max: 874.24
t(50) = 1.03, P = 0.305
SSRT 388.83 ± 67.55
Min: 219.00, Max: 495.67
439.81 ± 66.62
Min: 323.78, Max: 622.67
t(50) = 2.74, P = 0.008*
Go % 78.84 ± 11.35
Min: 55.56, Max: 100.00
83.98 ± 13.96
Min: 50.00, Max: 100.00
t(50) = 1.46, P = 0.151
Stop % 54.38 ± 11.23
Min: 33.33, Max: 83.33
55.50 ± 9.52
Min: 33.33, Max: 80.00
t(50) = 0.39, P = 0.699
Threat
Go RT 804.35 ± 83.30
Min: 626.92, Max: 925.00
775.37 ± 57.58
Min: 656.21, Max: 880.30
t(50) = 1.46, P = 0.151
SSRT 395.82 ± 81.20
Min: 209.44, Max: 580.00
408.28 ± 80.87
Min: 137.78, Max: 612.44
t(50) = 0.55, P = 0.582
Go % 76.34 ± 11.66
Min: 50.00, Max: 100.00
83.79 ± 12.63
Min: 58.33, Max: 100.00
t(50) = 2.21, P = 0.031
Stop % 52.62 ± 12.32
Min: 16.67, Max: 83.33
57.67 ± 9.23
Min: 44.44, Max: 83.33
t(50) = 1.67, P = 0.101
Neutral
Go RT 802.18 ± 69.55
Min: 642.83, Max: 905.67
783.41 ± 60.69
Min: 647.83, Max: 870.04
t(50) = 1.04, P = 0.305
SSRT 390.03 ± 79.38
Min: 247.83, Max: 540.22
441.70 ± 60.76
Min: 371.00, Max: 624.56
t(50) = 2.64, P = 0.011*
Go % 78.42 ± 13.08
Min: 55.56, Max: 100.00
84.48 ± 13.60
Min: 50.00, Max: 100.00
t(50) = 1.64, P = 0.101
Stop % 54.38 ± 9.13
Min: 33.33, Max: 83.33
56.08 ± 9.08
Min: 33.33, Max: 72.73
t(50) = 0.67, P = 0.503

Note: All RT values in milliseconds. ± denotes standard deviation. a Average (i.e. collapsed across all cue types) accuracy and RT values were used for inclusion criteria to abide by the horse-race model assumptions of the stop-signal task (i.e. positive SSRT, Go Accuracy ≥60%, Stop Accuracy between 25–75%). b Significance tests reflect independent samples t-tests for each variable between groups. Because only the SSRT variable was of interest (and yielded a significant Group x Cue interaction), no other RT or accuracy variables are flagged with asterisks when P < 0.05.

The total score on the Severity of Dependence Scale positively correlated with overall SSRT (Spearman’s r = 0.40, P = 0.043, Holm-corrected), such that higher severity of cocaine dependence was associated with slower SSRT (worse performance). No other clinical measure showed a significant correlation with behavior (all ps > 0.05).

Due to missing data, we were able to extract post-task word rating data from 45 out of the 52 participants included in the analyses (CUD: n = 21; HC: n = 24). A 2-way ANOVA with valence as the dependent variable, Group as between-, and Cue as within-subject factors revealed no main effect of Group, F(1,37) = 0.92, P = 0.343, a significant main effect of Cue, F(3,37) = 19.75, P < 0.001, and no Group × Cue interaction, F(3,37) = 0.25, P = 0.859. Pairwise comparisons of valence ratings between cue types across all participants indicated the following (from most to least positive): Food > Neutral > Drug > Threat, t(44) ≥ 7.27, P < 0.001. An identical 2-way ANOVA with arousal as the dependent variable revealed no main effect of Group, F(1,37) = 1.64, P = 0.209, a significant main effect of Cue, F(3,37) = 5.53, P = 0.003, and no Group × Cue interaction, F(3,37) = 2.28, P = 0.095. Pairwise comparisons of arousal ratings between cue types across all participants indicated the following (from most to least arousing): Threat > Drug > Neutral, t(44) ≥ 2.58, P ≤ 0.013; Food cues, greater than neutral cues, t(44) = 2.33, P = 0.024, did not significantly differ from Threat or Drug cues (both Ps > 0.05). Thus, across all subjects, drug words were rated as more negative than food words; their level of arousal did not differ.

BOLD-fMRI results

Inhibitory control-related neural signaling (across all participants)

The whole-brain analysis of overall inhibitory control across all participants (i.e. neural signaling during Stop_Success > Stop_Fail regardless of cue type and group) yielded significant clusters in classical inhibitory control-associated regions, such as right vmPFC (Brodmann’s Area, or BA10), left OFC (BA47), left IFG (BA45), right frontal pole/dmPFC (BA8; among several other regions listed in Table 3, top, see Supplemental Fig. 1 for activation maps). Next, we used mean SSRT (regardless of cue type) as a covariate to detect cue-general inhibitory control and found significant clusters namely in the right frontal pole/anterior PFC (BA10), right paracingulate gyrus (BA9), bilateral middle frontal gyrus/dlPFC (BA8 in both hemispheres), and among several others (see Table 3, bottom).

Table 3.

Inhibitory control related BOLD signal, all participants.

Side Voxels Peak Z x y z BA
Overall inhibitory control related BOLD signal (all cues)
Medial frontal cortex R 271 5.17 1 50 −13 BA10
Frontal pole L 209 5.12 −19 46 44 BA8
OFC L 75 4.61 −27 33 −13 BA47
Superior frontal gyrus R 121 4.59 26 29 54 BA8
IFG L 51 4.02 −51 29 10 BA45
Subcallosal cortex R 265 5.41 5 5 −13
Anterior superior temporal gyrus R 336 5.1 59 −5 −13 BA22
Middle temporal gyrus L 181 5.12 −53 −8 −17 BA21
Anterior parahippocampal gyrus R 95 4.49 22 −16 −19
Posterior superior temporal gyrus L 54 4.17 −66 −25 2 BA22
Posterior superior temporal gyrus R 148 4.49 54 −35 6 BA22
Precuneus L 384 4.88 −4 −57 17 BA23
Superior lateral occipital cortex R 209 4.69 52 −66 29 BA39
Superior lateral occipital cortex L 231 4.68 −45 −76 40 BA39
Superior lateral occipital cortex R 53 4.41 26 −78 46 BA7
Superior lateral occipital cortex R 59 4.28 22 −83 17 BA19
Occipital pole L 71 4.19 −15 −98 14 V2
Correlation of mean SSRT and overall inhibitory control related BOLD signal (all cues)
Frontal pole R 58 4.54 29 59 8 BA10
Paracingulate gyrus R 52 4.47 7 42 27 BA9
Middle frontal gyrus R 106 4.51 33 29 31 BA9
Middle frontal gyrus R 99 4.24 41 16 50 BA8
Middle frontal gyrus L 85 4.38 −36 16 40 BA8
Thalamus R 323 5.46 5 −14 8
Putamen L 51 4.17 −27 −27 0
Posterior cingulate gyrus R 135 5.08 3 −40 25 BA23
Posterior supramarginal gyrus R 108 4.24 52 −44 46 BA40
Superior lateral occipital cortex L 92 4.65 −45 −61 46 BA39
Precuneus R 58 4.83 7 −70 40 BA7
Occipital pole R 147 5.48 24 −100 0 V2

Note: Inhibitory control related BOLD signal derived by the hallmark Stop_Success > Stop_Fail contrast.

Group differences in inhibitory control-related neural signaling

Whole-brain analysis of overall inhibitory control (regardless of cue type) between groups revealed decreased activation in the CUD group relative to HC in the left frontal pole/dmPFC (BA10: MNI space −4, 57, 21; peak Z = 5.04, 97 voxels) and left lateral occipital cortex (BA39: MNI space −42, −63, 27; peak Z = 4.16, 70 voxels; see Fig. 3).

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3

Brain activity differences across CUD and HC groups during inhibitory control, regardless of cue type. The analysis of overall inhibitory control revealed significantly lower activity in the left dmPFC in the CUD group compared with HC (peak voxel MNI coordinates: x = −4, y = 57, z = 21). Cluster defining threshold: Z > 3.1 (P < 0.001), cluster corrected at P < 0.05. The bar plot of activation patterns shows relative activation to all cue types within the voxels that display peak activation as a result of the overall inhibitory control contrast. Inhibitory control was modeled via the hallmark Stop_Success > Stop_Fail contrast of task events corresponding to all cues.

We also used mean SSRT as a covariate to determine potential group differences in the brain regions that correlated with stopping ability and found significantly higher activations in CUD compared with HC in the posterior cingulate (BA31: MNI space −2, −35, 40; peak Z = 4.28, 54 voxels), left precuneus (BA7: MNI space −8, −66, 52; peak Z = 4.88, 105 voxels), and cuneal cortices (BA19: MNI space −23, −72, 19; peak Z = 4.22, 59 voxels), such that the higher the activity in these regions, the slower the SSRT (worse performance) specifically in CUD.

In sum, these results suggest that in CUD compared with HC the dmPFC is hypoactivated during inhibitory control, and greater activity in the posterior cingular/parietal regions is more closely associated with worse stopping ability.

Group differences in inhibitory control-related neural signaling under drug cue reactivity

Importantly, to capture the hypothesized iRISA interaction, we performed whole-brain analyses that focused on group differences in BOLD signal during drug versus food related inhibitory control, which allowed us to examine specific neural responses to drug cue reactivity compared with another salient pleasant (positively valenced) cue that is associated with consummatory behavior. The results revealed significant left dlPFC hypoactivations in the CUD group compared with HC during Drug > Food-related inhibition (BA8: MNI space −53, 12, 38; peak Z = 3.89, 62 voxels; see Fig. 4), suggesting that specifically in CUD, this region exhibited decreased signaling during the inhibitory control of drug words (which were rated as more negatively valenced compared with food words in both groups). Similar analyses with Drug > Neutral-related inhibition did not yield significant results.

Fig. 4.

Fig. 4

Brain activity differences across CUD and HC groups during inhibitory control under drug cue reactivity. The linear contrast of drug cue- versus a competing, nondrug positive reinforcer, food cue-related inhibitory control revealed significantly lower activity in the left dlPFC in the CUD group compared with HC (peak voxel MNI coordinates: x = −53, 12, 38). Cluster defining threshold: Z > 3.1 (P < 0.001), cluster corrected at P < 0.05. The bar plot of activation patterns shows relative activation to all cue types within the voxels that display peak activation as a result of the drug > food inhibitory control contrast. Inhibitory control was modeled via the hallmark Stop_Success > Stop_Fail contrast of task events corresponding to drug and food cues.

We also used Drug SSRT > Food SSRT as a covariate to determine group differences in which brain cue-sensitive inhibitory control regions correlated with Drug > Food-related stopping ability. A whole-brain correlation found that the right superior lateral occipital cortex activity (BA39: MNI space 46, −66, 21; peak Z = 4.09, 61 voxels) negatively correlated with Drug > Food SSRT in HC compared with CUD, such that increased activity was associated with quicker stopping to drug compared with food cues in HC, and this correlation was significantly stronger than in the CUD group. Similar analyses with Drug > Neutral SSRT did not yield significant group differences in correlations.

Cocaine use severity and inhibitory control-related signaling under drug cue reactivity in the CUD group

CUD-focused analyses of drug use severity (the 6 cocaine use variables in Table 1) were conducted via unbiased, whole-brain correlations, effectively localizing the regions in which each of these drug use measures tracked the BOLD signal during drug-related (Drug > Food and Drug > Neutral) inhibition (Table 4). Most notably, we found a significant positive correlation between recent frequency of use (days in the past month) and Drug > Food inhibition-related right OFC (BA47), right superior frontal gyrus/dmPFC (BA6), and left dlPFC (BA6) activity in the CUD group (Fig. 5). Using this same contrast (i.e. Drug > Food), a positive correlation was also observed with cocaine craving ratings in the right dlPFC (BA9), right IFG (BA44), and right middle temporal gyrus (BA21). Consistent with this direction of associations, negative correlations were observed with length of abstinence in the right anterior PFC (BA10), right dlPFC (BA44), and right precentral gyrus (BA6). Together, these results suggest that the more frequent the recent cocaine use, the higher the craving, and the shorter the abstinence, the higher the activity mainly in PFC regions during drug compared with food-related inhibitory control. Similarly, Drug > Neutral inhibition-related BOLD signal positively correlated with recent cocaine use frequency in the left dlPFC (BA8) and cocaine craving in the left anterior PFC (BA8), left dlPFC (BA10), right dmPFC (BA6), left IFG (BA44), and the right hippocampus. Thus, similarly to the Drug > Food contrast, the Drug > Neutral contrast showed that higher prefrontal cortical activations (during drug compared with neutral-related inhibitory control) are related to more frequent cocaine use and higher craving.

Table 4.

Whole-brain Drug > Food and Drug > Neutral inhibitory control related BOLD signal correlations with drug use measures, CUD group only.

Side Voxels Peak Z x y z BA
Drug > Food inhibitory control related BOLD signal
Past 30-day cocaine use
OFC R 99 4.39 33 35 −9 BA47
Superior frontal gyrus R 69 4.15 20 25 54 BA6
Middle frontal gyrus L 133 4.27 −40 5 50 BA6
Cocaine craving
Middle frontal gyrus R 61 4.32 46 44 25 BA9
IFG R 71 4.2 44 14 25 BA44
Middle temporal gyrus R 153 4.54 52 −5 −21 BA21
Length of abstinence
Frontal pole R 59 -4.25 29 53 12 BA10
Middle frontal gyrus R 109 -4.17 48 14 35 BA44
Precentral gyrus R 163 -4.42 35 −5 67 BA6
Drug > Neutral inhibitory control-related BOLD signal
Past 30-day cocaine use
Middle frontal gyrus L 66 4.16 −42 14 46 BA8
Cocaine craving
Frontal Pole L 53 4.17 −19 48 48 BA8
Middle frontal gyrus L 68 5.11 −42 38 27 BA10
Superior frontal gyrus R 90 4.35 3 23 63 BA6
IFG L 88 4.82 −53 23 25 BA44
Hippocampus R 55 4.21 22 −20 −13
Length of abstinence
No significant clusters

Note: Inhibitory control related BOLD signal derived by the hallmark Stop_Success > Stop_Fail contrast.

Fig. 5.

Fig. 5

Whole-brain correlational analysis of cocaine use severity as assayed by recent cocaine use frequency in the past month, and its relationship with inhibitory control under drug cue reactivity (drug > food inhibitory control contrast). When entered into the GLM as a covariate in a whole-brain analysis, recent cocaine use frequency showed significant positive correlations with activity in the right OFC, right dmPFC, and left dlPFC in the CUD group, such that the more severe the cocaine use, the higher the activation in these prefrontal regions during inhibitory control under drug cue reactivity (peak voxel MNI coordinates: x = 33, y = 35, z = −9). Cluster defining threshold: Z > 3.1 (P < 0.001), cluster corrected at P < 0.05. Inhibitory control was modeled via the hallmark Stop_Success > Stop_Fail contrast of task events corresponding to drug and food cues.

Discussion

Despite consistent evidence for inhibitory control and salience attribution impairments in individuals with substance use disorders (see reviews: Zilverstand et al. 2018; Ceceli et al. 2021), our understanding of how these hallmark addiction symptoms interact, and the neural underpinnings of this interaction, remains incomplete. Here, a novel fMRI SST permitted the neurobehavioral examination of the interaction between inhibitory control and salient drug and nondrug cue processing in CUD. Significant activations in the vmPFC, OFC, IFG, and dmPFC during successful compared with failed stops, independent of cue type or group, support our first aim to validate this novel task, showing its consistency with the PFC’s role in driving inhibitory control (Verbruggen and Logan 2008). Hypoactivations in the dmPFC during overall inhibitory control in the preselected group of individuals with CUD support our second hypothesis, contributing to the reliably documented body of work implicating decreased PFC signaling during a wide range of neuropsychological functions, including response inhibition, in this population (Zilverstand et al. 2018). Third, and most importantly, we revealed evidence for the iRISA interaction in the CUD brain. Despite group similarities in both drug cue behavioral performance and arousal/valence ratings, and despite meeting the stringent criteria for SST’s horse-race model assumptions, compared with the HC group, CUD participants showed decreased dlPFC function during drug versus food (a consummatory nondrug positive reinforcer) cue inhibition. The lower these dlPFC responses to drug versus food cues, the lower the drug use severity encompassing less frequency of recent use, lower craving and longer abstinence.

The OFC and IFG activations during successful stopping in all participants regardless of cue type suggest that our novel task effectively captures inhibitory control in the brain. These regions are known to comprise the brain’s cognitive control network, with specific involvement in the suppression of inappropriate behaviors, especially during the SST (Aron and Poldrack 2006; Aron et al. 2007; Brockett and Roesch 2021). The significant dmPFC hypoactivations in CUD during overall inhibitory control agree with previous evidence where a similar dmPFC region was also hypoactivated in CUD compared with HC during inhibitory control—interestingly during Go/No-Go performance with neutral letter stimuli (Hester and Garavan 2004). Notably, the lack of behavioral performance deficits in CUD and comparable valence and arousal ratings between groups suggest that these PFC hypoactivations during inhibitory control are not driven by compromises in task comprehension, performance, or perception of stimuli. In general, while CUD may be expected to perform worse than HC in this task, evidence for (Fillmore and Rush 2002; Li, Milivojevic, et al. 2006; Morein-Zamir et al. 2013; Wang et al. 2018) and against (Li et al. 2008; Vonmoos et al. 2013; Chao et al. 2019; Zhukovsky et al. 2021) prolonged SSRT in CUD versus HC exists.

Agnostic to group and cue type, mean SSRT was positively correlated with dlPFC and paracingulate activity (among other regions), suggesting increased activation in these regions with slower stopping. This result disagrees with the direction of the correlation reported in several studies (Aron and Poldrack 2006; Li, Huang, et al. 2006; Zhao et al. 2019; Chevrier and Schachar 2020; Lee and Hsieh 2017; but see de Wit et al. 2012). These studies largely reported a negative correlation between SSRT and cortical and subcortical activity (namely in the PFC and basal ganglia) including the default mode network—a set of brain regions that exhibit low-frequency oscillations at “rest,” typically associated with self-referential thought (Raichle et al. 2001). The source of the different direction of correlations is not clear but could be attributed to the variability introduced by the word stimuli, which may more readily elicit self-referential brain activity, especially in CUD. Indeed, compared with HC, in CUD the precuneus and posterior cingulate—2 of the functional hubs of the default mode network (Raichle et al. 2001; Utevsky et al. 2014)—were more positively correlated with SSRT, suggesting that those who were slower to stop also exhibited higher default mode network activity during inhibitory control.

Most importantly, for the first time we demonstrate the neural signature of the iRISA interaction in CUD. In behaviorally comparable demographically matched CUD and HC samples, we found evidence for decreased inhibitory control signal in the dlPFC only in CUD during drug cues relative to a nondrug reinforcer (food cues). One explanation for this dlPFC deactivation invokes the suppression of salient drug cue reactivity, in line with the dlPFC’s role in orchestrating craving, potentially by modulating the OFC, which combines the value of the craving-inducing cues with affective states and drug availability (George and Koob 2013), overall leading to decreased craving when the dlPFC is inactivated. Indeed, smokers who underwent dlPFC inactivation via transcranial magnetic stimulation (1 Hz) displayed reduced craving when cigarettes were immediately available, and dlPFC inactivation decreased craving-related OFC activity during a smoking cue reactivity task (Hayashi et al. 2013). Adopting this perspective, our results suggest that to resemble the HC behavioral performance, the CUD group needed to decrease dlPFC signaling during inhibitory control especially during the potentially craving enhancing drug (vs. other salient) cues. Accordingly, those CUD individuals with the most severe drug use patterns (i.e. more frequent recent use, higher craving, and shorter abstinence) were less likely to exhibit decreased dlPFC (and OFC) response in the face of drug (vs. food) cues. It remains to be tested whether this pattern is a biomarker of resilience, whereby the ability to exert control during drug cue reactivity in the lab predicts craving and drug use outside the lab. An alternative explanation invokes self-medication (and vulnerability)—whereby those with more frequent recent cocaine use may be normalizing (increasing dlPFC activity, similarly to the HC) underlying neurocognitive dysfunction, masking (with cocaine, a stimulant) the neuropsychological deficits associated with cocaine addiction (Woicik et al. 2009; Parvaz et al. 2012, 2015). This latter interpretation is more consistent with the expected direction of effect for the dlPFC, whereby reduced activation during inhibitory control is a common marker of deficit. Accordingly, lateral occipital activity was associated with quicker SSRT in HC (but not CUD) in the drug context, which, combined with the relationship between slower SSRT and higher default mode network activity in CUD, alludes to a mechanism by which these non-PFC regions may be compensating by aiding performance in HC but not CUD. Inhibitory control-related comparisons between recent cocaine users and abstainers (or as a function of abstinence longitudinally) would be needed to contrast these perspectives in future efforts.

Limitations of the present study include high rates of data exclusion due to the stringent SSRT estimation parameters. While data filtering to validate SSRT is integral in capturing inhibitory control in the lab, performance feedback between task runs to remind participants of the instructions (e.g. ensuring that the participant does not wait for the stop-signal) may minimize the loss of data and statistical power (Verbruggen et al. 2019). Furthermore, although behavioral investigations show inhibitory slowing to emotionally salient picture (Verbruggen and Houwer 2007; Kalanthroff et al. 2013; Ding et al. 2020) and word stimuli (Herbert and Sütterlin 2011) in the general population, we found prolonged SSRT in response to food and neutral cues in the HC group, with no evidence for slowing for the other arousing cue types or in the CUD group. The group differences in food and neutral SSRT should be interpreted with caution, however, as these patterns were not a priori hypothesized and were not supported in the larger sample of available data (Supplemental Table 1). Thus, our task, which used words did not elicit the expected behavioral effect (e.g. slowing driven by drug words in the CUD). These null effects may have been driven by variability in recency of drug use (with 42% of our CUD sample actively using cocaine), as remains to be tested with a larger sample that includes comparable subsamples of both current users and abstainers of a wider range of days since last use. Supplementing these results by hunger and food craving ratings, and other relevant measures (e.g. time since last meal), is warranted in similar future studies. Using visual cues (e.g. drug use images or video clips) may have more effectively induced sustained cue reactivity than word stimuli; however, the word stimuli were preferred for our trial-by-trial design and for rendering verbal features such as length and frequency uniform between conditions. While the study sample includes a large proportion of African-American participants who are typically understudied in clinical research in general (Shavers-Hornaday et al. 1997), or for studying cognitive control and salience processing more specifically, these results should also be replicated with larger, more sex-balanced samples that can inform about potential sex differences in the neural mechanisms of inhibitory control. Lastly, although we statistically accounted for neural responses to food and threat cues, in-depth analyses of these behavioral and neural patterns await samples that better represent relevant comorbidities (e.g. eating disorders, intermittent explosive disorder).

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first report of the brain systems that regulate inhibitory control under drug cue reactivity in human cocaine addiction. To better represent the addiction experience in humans, where reminders of cocaine use can exert control over behavior, we developed a novel SST to reveal the neural mechanisms of the iRISA interaction. Our results ascribe an important role to the dlPFC in the control of behavior under drug cue salience. Specifically, although the task promotes increased PFC function in cognitive control-related regions, drug versus food cue-related hypoactivations were evident in CUD compared with HC, as associated with less frequent recent use, craving and with longer abstinence. This dlPFC drug cue suppression may be a marker of resilience (dampening craving-related processes) or a signature of vulnerability (where cocaine is used to normalize impaired cognitive processes in the short term, yet with long-term negative consequences). Together, our results point to the mechanisms behind lapses in self-control when faced with a salient drug context in addicted individuals. A closer examination with only treatment-seeking individuals is warranted to test whether these neurobiological patterns are related to deviations from treatment goals. Results could also offer the possibility to refine neuromodulation to mitigate the effects of craving on self-control and other drug-related behaviors in drug addiction.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Institute on Drug Abuse at the National Institutes of Health (F32DA033088 and K01DA043615 to M.A.P., and 1R01DA041528, R01DA023579, and R21DA034954 to R.Z.G.).

Conflict of interest statement. None declared.

Supplementary Material

essrt_ms_cercor_supp_final_bhac087

Contributor Information

Ahmet O Ceceli, Department of Psychiatry, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, 1 Gustave L. Levy Place, Box 1230, New York, NY 10029, United States.

Muhammad A Parvaz, Department of Psychiatry, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, 1 Gustave L. Levy Place, Box 1230, New York, NY 10029, United States; Department of Neuroscience, Friedman Brain Institute, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, 1 Gustave L. Levy Place, Box 1065, New York, NY 10029, United States.

Sarah King, Department of Neuroscience, Friedman Brain Institute, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, 1 Gustave L. Levy Place, Box 1065, New York, NY 10029, United States.

Matthew Schafer, Department of Neuroscience, Friedman Brain Institute, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, 1 Gustave L. Levy Place, Box 1065, New York, NY 10029, United States.

Pias Malaker, Department of Psychiatry, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, 1 Gustave L. Levy Place, Box 1230, New York, NY 10029, United States.

Akarsh Sharma, Department of Psychiatry, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, 1 Gustave L. Levy Place, Box 1230, New York, NY 10029, United States.

Nelly Alia-Klein, Department of Psychiatry, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, 1 Gustave L. Levy Place, Box 1230, New York, NY 10029, United States; Department of Neuroscience, Friedman Brain Institute, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, 1 Gustave L. Levy Place, Box 1065, New York, NY 10029, United States.

Rita Z Goldstein, Department of Psychiatry, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, 1 Gustave L. Levy Place, Box 1230, New York, NY 10029, United States; Department of Neuroscience, Friedman Brain Institute, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, 1 Gustave L. Levy Place, Box 1065, New York, NY 10029, United States.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association , editors. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 5th ed.: American Psychiatric Association; 2013 [Google Scholar]
  2. Ames  SL, Wong  SW, Bechara  A, Cappelli  C, Dust  M, Grenard  JL, Stacy  AW. Neural correlates of a go/NoGo task with alcohol stimuli in light and heavy young drinkers. Behav Brain Res. 2014:0:382–389. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Aron  AR, Poldrack  RA. Cortical and subcortical contributions to stop signal response inhibition: role of the subthalamic nucleus. J Neurosci. 2006:26:2424–2433. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Aron  AR, Behrens  TE, Smith  S, Frank  MJ, Poldrack  RA. Triangulating a cognitive control network using diffusion-weighted magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) and functional MRI. J Neurosci. 2007:27:3743–3752. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Avants  BB, Epstein  CL, Grossman  M, Gee  JC. 2008. Symmetric diffeomorphic image registration with cross-correlation: evaluating automated labeling of elderly and neurodegenerative brain. Medical Image Analysis, Special Issue on the Third International Workshop on Biomedical Image Registration – WBIR 2006. 12:26–41. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Beckmann  CF, Jenkinson  M, Smith  SM. General multilevel linear modeling for group analysis in FMRI. NeuroImage. 2003:20:1052–1063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Brockett  AT, Roesch  MR. The ever-changing OFC landscape: what neural signals in OFC can tell us about inhibitory control. Behav Neurosci. 2021:135:129–137. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Carpenter  KM, Schreiber  E, Church  S, McDowell  D. Drug Stroop performance: relationships with primary substance of use and treatment outcome in a drug-dependent outpatient sample. Addict Behav. 2006:31:174–181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Ceceli  AO, Bradberry  CW, Goldstein  RZ. The neurobiology of drug addiction: cross-species insights into the dysfunction and recovery of the prefrontal cortex. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2021:276. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Chao  T, Haney  M, Cooper  ZD, Vadhan  NP, Van Dam  NT, Van Snellenberg  J, Bedi  G. Cognitive function in aging cocaine smokers. J Psychopharmacol. 2019:33:801–810. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Chevrier  A, Schachar  RJ. BOLD differences normally attributed to inhibitory control predict symptoms, not task-directed inhibitory control in ADHD. J Neurodevelop Disord. 2020:12:8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Clark  JH. The Ishihara test for color blindness. Am J Physiol Optics. 1924:5:269–276. [Google Scholar]
  13. Cox  RW. AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Comput Biomed Res. 1996:29:162–173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Czapla  M, Baeuchl  C, Simon  JJ, Richter  B, Kluge  M, Friederich  H-C, Mann  K, Herpertz  SC, Loeber  S. Do alcohol-dependent patients show different neural activation during response inhibition than healthy controls in an alcohol-related fMRI go/no-go-task?  Psychopharmacology. 2017:234:1001–1015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. DeVito  EE, Kiluk  BD, Nich  C, Mouratidis  M, Carroll  KM. Drug Stroop: mechanisms of response to computerized cognitive behavioral therapy for cocaine dependence in a randomized clinical trial. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2018:183:162–168. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Ding  J, Wang  Y, Wang  C, d’Oleire Uquillas  F, He  Q, Cheng  L, Zou  Z. Negative impact of sadness on response inhibition in females: an explicit emotional stop signal task fMRI study. Front Behav Neurosci. 2020:14:119. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Eklund  A, Nichols  TE, Knutsson  H. Cluster failure: why fMRI inferences for spatial extent have inflated false-positive rates. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2016:113:7900–7905. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Elton  A, Young  J, Smitherman  S, Gross  RE, Mletzko  T, Kilts  CD. Neural network activation during a stop-signal task discriminates cocaine-dependent from non-drug-abusing men. Addict Biol. 2014:19:427–438. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Ersche  KD, Bullmore  ET, Craig  KJ, Shabbir  SS, Abbott  S, Müller  U, Ooi  C, Suckling  J, Barnes  A, Sahakian  BJ, et al.  Influence of compulsivity of drug abuse on dopaminergic modulation of attentional bias in stimulant dependence. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010:67:632–644. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Esteban  O, Markiewicz  CJ, Blair  RW, Moodie  CA, Isik  AI, Erramuzpe  A, Kent  JD, Goncalves  M, DuPre  E, Snyder  M, et al.  fMRIPrep: a robust preprocessing pipeline for functional MRI. Nat Methods. 2019:16:111–116. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Fillmore  MT, Rush  CR. Impaired inhibitory control of behavior in chronic cocaine users. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2002:66:265–273. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Fonov  V, Evans  A, McKinstry  R, Almli  C, Collins  D. 2009. Unbiased nonlinear average age-appropriate brain templates from birth to adulthood. NeuroImage, Organization for Human Brain Mapping 2009 Annual Meeting. 47:S102. [Google Scholar]
  23. Francis  W, Kučera  H, Mackie  AW. Frequency analysis of English usage: lexicon and grammar. Boston: Houghton Mifflin; 1982 [Google Scholar]
  24. George  O, Koob  GF. Control of craving by the prefrontal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2013:110:4165–4166. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Gilman  JM, Radoman  M, Schuster  RM, Pachas  G, Azzouz  N, Fava  M, Evins  AE. Anterior insula activation during inhibition to smoking cues is associated with ability to maintain tobacco abstinence. Addict Behav Rep. 2018:7:40–46. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Goldstein  RZ, Volkow  ND. Drug addiction and its underlying neurobiological basis: neuroimaging evidence for the involvement of the frontal cortex. Am J Psychiatry. 2002:159:1642–1652. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Goldstein  RZ, Volkow  ND. Dysfunction of the prefrontal cortex in addiction: neuroimaging findings and clinical implications. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2011:12:652–669. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Goldstein  RZ, Tomasi  D, Rajaram  S, Cottone  LA, Zhang  L, Maloney  T, Telang  F, Alia-Klein  N, Volkow  ND. Role of the anterior cingulate and medial orbitofrontal cortex in processing drug cues in cocaine addiction. Neuroscience. 2007:144:1153–1159. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Goldstein  RZ, Alia-Klein  N, Tomasi  D, Carrillo  JH, Maloney  T, Woicik  PA, Wang  R, Telang  F, Volkow  ND. Anterior cingulate cortex hypoactivations to an emotionally salient task in cocaine addiction. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009a:106:9453–9458. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Goldstein  RZ, Tomasi  D, Alia-Klein  N, Carrillo  JH, Maloney  T, Woicik  PA, Wang  R, Telang  F, Volkow  ND. Dopaminergic response to drug words in cocaine addiction. J Neurosci. 2009b:29:6001–6006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Gorgolewski  K, Burns  CD, Madison  C, Clark  D, Halchenko  YO, Waskom  ML, Ghosh  SS. Nipype: a flexible, lightweight and extensible neuroimaging data processing framework in python. Front Neuroinform. 2011:5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Gorgolewski  KJ, Auer  T, Calhoun  VD, Craddock  RC, Das  S, Duff  EP, Flandin  G, Ghosh  SS, Glatard  T, Halchenko  YO, et al.  The brain imaging data structure, a format for organizing and describing outputs of neuroimaging experiments. Sci Data. 2016:3:1–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Gossop  M, Griffiths  P, Powis  B, Strang  J. Severity of dependence and route of administration of heroin, cocaine and amphetamines. Br J Addict. 1992:87:1527–1536. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Hagberg  GE, Zito  G, Patria  F, Sanes  JN. Improved detection of event-related functional MRI signals using probability functions. NeuroImage. 2001:14:1193–1205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Harlé  KM, Zhang  S, Ma  N, Yu  AJ, Paulus  MP. Reduced neural recruitment for Bayesian adjustment of inhibitory control in methamphetamine dependence. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. 2016:1:448–459. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Harlé  KM, Yu  AJ, Paulus  MP. Bayesian computational markers of relapse in methamphetamine dependence. NeuroImage: Clinical. 2019:22(101794):101794. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Hayashi  T, Ko  JH, Strafella  AP, Dagher  A. Dorsolateral prefrontal and orbitofrontal cortex interactions during self-control of cigarette craving. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2013:110:4422–4427. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Herbert  C, Sütterlin  S. Response inhibition and memory retrieval of emotional target words: evidence from an emotional stop-signal task. J Behav Brain Sci. 2011:1:153–159. [Google Scholar]
  39. Hester  R, Garavan  H. Executive dysfunction in cocaine addiction: evidence for discordant frontal, cingulate, and cerebellar activity. J Neurosci. 2004:24:11017–11022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Hester  R, Dixon  V, Garavan  H. A consistent attentional bias for drug-related material in active cocaine users across word and picture versions of the emotional Stroop task. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2006:81:251–257. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Ho  D, Imai  K, King  G, Stuart  EA. MatchIt: nonparametric preprocessing for parametric causal inference. J Stat Softw. 2011:42:1–28. [Google Scholar]
  42. Hu  S, Ide  JS, Zhang  S, Sinha  R, Li  CR. Conflict anticipation in alcohol dependence — a model-based fMRI study of stop signal task. NeuroImage: Clinical. 2015:8:39–50. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Jenkinson  M, Smith  S. A global optimisation method for robust affine registration of brain images. Med Image Anal. 2001:5:143–156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Jenkinson  M, Bannister  P, Brady  M, Smith  S. Improved optimization for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain images. NeuroImage. 2002:17:825–841. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Jensen  AR, Rohwer  WD. The Stroop color-word test: a review. Acta Psychol. 1966:25:36–93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Kalanthroff  E, Cohen  N, Henik  A. Stop feeling: inhibition of emotional interference following stop-signal trials. Front Hum Neurosci. 2013:7:78. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Kampman  KM, Volpicelli  JR, McGinnis  DE, Alterman  AI, Weinrieb  RM, D’Angelo  L, Epperson  LE. Reliability and validity of the cocaine selective severity assessment. Addict Behav. 1998:23:449–461. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Kober  H, Lacadie  CM, Wexler  BE, Malison  RT, Sinha  R, Potenza  MN. Brain activity during cocaine craving and gambling urges: an fMRI study. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2016:41:628–637. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Konova  AB, Parvaz  MA, Bernstein  V, Zilverstand  A, Moeller  SJ, Delgado  MR, Alia-Klein  N, Goldstein  RZ. Neural mechanisms of extinguishing drug and pleasant cue associations in human addiction: role of the VMPFC. Addict Biol. 2019:24:88–99. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Lee  H-H, Hsieh  S. Resting-state fMRI associated with stop-signal task performance in healthy middle-aged and elderly people. Front Psychol. 2017:8:766. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Li  CR, Huang  C, Constable  RT, Sinha  R. Imaging response inhibition in a stop-signal task: neural correlates independent of signal monitoring and post-response processing. J Neurosci. 2006a:26:186–192. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Li  CR, Milivojevic  V, Kemp  K, Hong  K, Sinha  R. Performance monitoring and stop signal inhibition in abstinent patients with cocaine dependence. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2006b:85:205–212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Li  CR, Huang  C, Yan  P, Bhagwagar  Z, Milivojevic  V, Sinha  R. Neural correlates of impulse control during stop signal inhibition in cocaine-dependent men. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008:33:1798–1806. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Li  C-SR, Luo  X, Yan  P, Bergquist  K, Sinha  R. Altered impulse control in alcohol dependence: neural measures of stop signal performance. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2009:33:740–750. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Li  X, Morgan  PS, Ashburner  J, Smith  J, Rorden  C. The first step for neuroimaging data analysis: DICOM to NIfTI conversion. J Neurosci Methods. 2016:264:47–56. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Logan  G, Cowan  W. On the ability to inhibit thought and action: a theory of an act of control. Psychol Rev. 1984:91:295–327. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Martinez  D, Slifstein  M, Narendran  R, Foltin  RW, Broft  A, Hwang  D-R, Perez  A, Abi-Dargham  A, Fischman  MW, Kleber  HD, et al.  Dopamine D1 receptors in cocaine dependence measured with PET and the choice to self-administer cocaine. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2009:34:1774–1782. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Matuskey  D, Luo  X, Zhang  S, Morgan  PT, Abdelghany  O, Malison  RT, Li  CR. Methylphenidate remediates error-preceding activation of the default mode brain regions in cocaine-addicted individuals. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging. 2013:214:116–121. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. McLellan  AT, Kushner  H, Metzger  D, Peters  R, Smith  I, Grissom  G, Pettinati  H, Argeriou  M. The fifth edition of the addiction severity index. J Subst Abus Treat. 1992:9:199–213. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Moeller  SJ, Maloney  T, Parvaz  MA, Dunning  JP, Alia-Klein  N, Woicik  PA, Hajcak  G, Telang  F, Wang  G-J, Volkow  ND, et al.  Enhanced choice for viewing cocaine pictures in cocaine addiction. Biol Psychiatry. 2009:66:169–176. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Moeller  SJ, Maloney  T, Parvaz  MA, Alia-Klein  N, Woicik  PA, Telang  F, Wang  G-J, Volkow  ND, Goldstein  RZ. Impaired insight in cocaine addiction: laboratory evidence and effects on cocaine-seeking behaviour. Brain. 2010:133:1484–1493. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Moeller  SJ, Zilverstand  A, Konova  AB, Kundu  P, Parvaz  MA, Preston-Campbell  R, Bachi  K, Alia-Klein  N, Goldstein  RZ. Neural correlates of drug-biased choice in currently-using and abstinent individuals with cocaine use disorder. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. 2018:3:485–494. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Morein-Zamir  S, Simon Jones  P, Bullmore  ET, Robbins  TW, Ersche  KD. Prefrontal hypoactivity associated with impaired inhibition in stimulant-dependent individuals but evidence for hyperactivation in their unaffected siblings. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2013:38:1945–1953. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Parvaz  MA, Maloney  T, Moeller  SJ, Woicik  PA, Alia-Klein  N, Telang  F, Wang  G-J, Squires  NK, Volkow  ND, Goldstein  RZ. Sensitivity to monetary reward is most severely compromised in recently abstaining cocaine addicted individuals: a cross-sectional ERP study. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging. 2012:203:75–82. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Parvaz  MA, Konova  AB, Proudfit  GH, Dunning  JP, Malaker  P, Moeller  SJ, Maloney  T, Alia-Klein  N, Goldstein  RZ. Impaired neural response to negative prediction errors in cocaine addiction. J Neurosci. 2015:35:1872–1879. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Phillips  PEM, Stuber  GD, Heien  MLAV, Wightman  RM, Carelli  RM. Subsecond dopamine release promotes cocaine seeking. Nature. 2003:422:614–618. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Raichle  ME, MacLeod  AM, Snyder  AZ, Powers  WJ, Gusnard  DA, Shulman  GL. A default mode of brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2001:98:676–682. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Raud  L, Westerhausen  R, Dooley  N, Huster  RJ. Differences in unity: the go/no-go and stop signal tasks rely on different mechanisms. NeuroImage. 2020:210:116582. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Ray  S, Haney  M, Hanson  C, Biswal  B, Hanson  SJ. Modeling causal relationship between brain regions within the drug-cue processing network in chronic cocaine smokers. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2015:40:2960–2968. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Rickham  PP. Human experimentation. Code of ETHICS of the WORLD medical ASSOCIATION. Declaration of Helsinki. Br Med J. 1964:2:177. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Sakoglu  U, Mete  M, Esquivel  J, Rubia  K, Briggs  R, Adinoff  B. Classification of cocaine-dependent participants with dynamic functional connectivity from functional magnetic resonance imaging data. J Neurosci Res. 2019:97:790–803. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Shavers-Hornaday  VL, Lynch  CF, Burmeister  LF, Torner  JC. Why are African Americans under-represented in medical research studies? Impediments to participation. Ethnicity Health. 1997:2:31–45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Sjoerds  Z, van den  Brink  W, Beekman  ATF, Penninx  BWJH, Veltman  DJ. Response inhibition in alcohol-dependent patients and patients with depression/anxiety: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Psychol Med. 2014:44:1713–1725. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Smith  DG, Ersche  KD. Using a drug-word Stroop task to differentiate recreational from dependent drug use. CNS Spectr. 2014:19:247–255. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Tiffany  ST, Singleton  E, Haertzen  CA, Henningfield  JE. The development of a cocaine craving questionnaire. Drug Alcohol Depend. 1993:34:19–28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Tustison  NJ, Avants  BB, Cook  PA, Zheng  Y, Egan  A, Yushkevich  PA, Gee  JC. N4ITK: improved N3 bias correction. IEEE Trans Med Imaging. 2010:29:1310–1320. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Utevsky  AV, Smith  DV, Huettel  SA. Precuneus is a functional Core of the default-mode network. J Neurosci. 2014:34:932–940. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Verbruggen  F, Houwer  JD. Do emotional stimuli interfere with response inhibition? Evidence from the stop signal paradigm. Cognit Emot. 2007:21:391–403. [Google Scholar]
  79. Verbruggen  F, Logan  GD. Response inhibition in the stop-signal paradigm. Trends Cogn Sci (Regul Ed). 2008:12:418–424. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Verbruggen  F, Aron  AR, Band  GP, Beste  C, Bissett  PG, Brockett  AT, Brown  JW, Chamberlain  SR, Chambers  CD, Colonius  H, et al.  A consensus guide to capturing the ability to inhibit actions and impulsive behaviors in the stop-signal task. elife. 2019:8:e46323. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Vonmoos  M, Hulka  LM, Preller  KH, Jenni  D, Schulz  C, Baumgartner  MR, Quednow  BB. Differences in self-reported and behavioral measures of impulsivity in recreational and dependent cocaine users. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2013:133:61–70. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Wager  TD, Nichols  TE. Optimization of experimental design in fMRI: a general framework using a genetic algorithm. NeuroImage. 2003:18:293–309. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Wang  W, Worhunsky  PD, Zhang  S, Le  TM, Potenza  MN, Li  C-SR. Response inhibition and fronto-striatal-thalamic circuit dysfunction in cocaine addiction. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2018:192:137–145. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. de  Wit  SJ, de  Vries  FE, van der  Werf  YD, Cath  DC, Heslenfeld  DJ, Veltman  EM, van  Balkom  AJLM, Veltman  DJ, van den  Heuvel  OA. Presupplementary motor area hyperactivity during response inhibition: a candidate endophenotype of obsessive-compulsive disorder. AJP. 2012:169:1100–1108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Woicik  PA, Moeller  SJ, Alia-Klein  N, Maloney  T, Lukasik  TM, Yeliosof  O, Wang  G-J, Volkow  ND, Goldstein  RZ. The neuropsychology of cocaine addiction: recent cocaine use masks impairment. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2009:34:1112–1122. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Woolrich  MW, Ripley  BD, Brady  M, Smith  SM. Temporal autocorrelation in univariate linear modeling of FMRI data. NeuroImage. 2001:14:1370–1386. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Zhang  Y, Brady  M, Smith  S. Segmentation of brain MR images through a hidden Markov random field model and the expectation-maximization algorithm. IEEE Trans Med Imaging. 2001:20:45–57. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Zhang  Y, Zhang  S, Ide  JS, Hu  S, Zhornitsky  S, Wang  W, Dong  G, Tang  X, Li  C-SR. Dynamic network dysfunction in cocaine dependence: graph theoretical metrics and stop signal reaction time. Neuroimage Clin. 2018:18:793–801. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Zhao  R, Zhang  X, Fei  N, Zhu  Y, Sun  J, Liu  P, Yang  X, Qin  W. Decreased cortical and subcortical response to inhibition control after sleep deprivation. Brain Imaging Behav. 2019:13:638–650. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Zhukovsky  P, Morein-Zamir  S, Ziauddeen  H, Fernandez-Egea  E, Meng  C, Regenthal  R, Sahakian  BJ, Bullmore  ET, Robbins  TW, Dalley  JW, et al.  Prefrontal cortex activation and stopping performance underlie the beneficial effects of atomoxetine on response inhibition in healthy and cocaine use disorder volunteers. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. 2021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Zilverstand  A, Huang  AS, Alia-Klein  N, Goldstein  RZ. Neuroimaging impaired response inhibition and salience attribution in human drug addiction: a systematic review. Neuron. 2018:98:886–903. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

essrt_ms_cercor_supp_final_bhac087

Articles from Cerebral Cortex (New York, NY) are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press

RESOURCES