Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2024 Mar 1.
Published in final edited form as: Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2023 Jan 9;101:102103. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2023.102103

Emotion Regulation and Executive Function: Associations with Depression and Anxiety in Autism

Caitlin M Conner 1, Rebecca Elias 2,3, Isaac C Smith 4, Susan W White 5
PMCID: PMC9897310  NIHMSID: NIHMS1863886  PMID: 36741741

Abstract

Background:

Adolescents and young adults with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) are prone to experience co-occurring mental health conditions such as mood or anxiety disorders, as well as impairments in emotion regulation and executive functioning. However, little research has examined inter-relationships among these constructs, despite evidence of additional stressors and increased risk of internalizing disorders at this age, relative to non-autistic individuals. If either emotion regulation or executive functioning are shown to have patterns of association with mental health, this can inform mechanism-based intervention.

Method:

Fifty-seven autistic adolescents and adults (16–25 years) with ASD in a transition intervention completed questionnaires and clinician-administered measures at baseline. Analyses assessed whether executive functioning impairment, above and beyond emotion regulation impairment, were associated with depression and anxiety symptoms.

Results:

ASD characteristics, emotion regulation, anxiety, and depression were significantly correlated. ASD characteristics was a significant contributor to depression and emotion regulation impairments were significant contributors to anxiety and depression. Findings indicated that inhibition difficulties did not uniquely contribute to depression or anxiety above emotion regulation impairment. Difficulties in cognitive flexibility were associated with depression above and beyond ASD characteristics, IQ, and emotion regulation, but not associated with anxiety.

Conclusions:

Although preliminary, findings suggest that inflexibility and regulatory impairment should be considered in depression remediation approaches. Improving ER, on the other hand, may have broader transdiagnostic impact across both mood and anxiety symptoms in ASD.

Keywords: emotion regulation, executive functioning, depression, anxiety, cognitive flexibility, inhibition


Diagnostically, Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) is comprised of difficulties in social communication in conjunction with the presence of restricted and repetitive behaviors and sensory differences (APA, 2013). In addition to these core diagnostic features, individuals with ASD commonly present with executive dysfunction, emotion regulation impairment, and co-occurring psychopathology (Conner, White, et al., 2020; Mazefsky et al., 2013). Executive function (EF) is defined as a complex set of skills which allows someone to engage in goal-directed behavior (Diamond, 2013; Miyake & Friedman, 2012; Roberts & Pennington, 1996). These skills follow a developmental trajectory, with full maturity, behaviorally and neurobiologically, by adulthood (Ferguson et al., 2021). EF is also considered a component under the larger construct of self-regulation, defined as one’s ability to regulate your own cognitions, emotions, and behaviors (Bridgett et al., 2013; Karoly, 1993). Self-regulation encompasses concepts such as EF, emotion regulation, and self-control (Baumeister & Vohs, 2003). Further, EF has been increasingly examined in context with and without other cognitive processes (termed ‘hot’ and ‘cool’ EF, respectively) (Zelazo & Carlson, 2012), which considers how EF is influenced by motivations and emotional factors.

In ASD, there is evidence of impaired EF; however, findings have differed by sample composition (i.e., age, whether community or clinically referred participants) and methodology (i.e., self/parent-report questionnaires vs. performance-based tasks) (Demetriou et al., 2018; Kenworthy et al., 2008; Lai et al., 2017). Impaired levels of EF measured via both questionnaires and performance-based tasks have been associated with core ASD characteristics, such as restricted and repetitive behaviors and interests (Faja & Nelson Darling, 2018; Wallace et al., 2016). Caregiver report of the level of EF impairment increases as their children age into adolescence, suggesting that EF skills may either stall, mature more slowly, worsen, or become more noticeable in youth with ASD as they age (Rosenthal et al., 2013). Further, in ASD, EF impairments have been associated with worse adaptive functioning (Bertollo et al., 2020; Kenny et al., 2019; Pugliese et al., 2016), mental health (Hollocks et al., 2021; Uljarević et al., 2017; Wallace et al., 2016; Zimmerman et al., 2017), quality of life (de Vries & Geurts, 2015), social functioning (Bertollo et al., 2020; Vogan et al., 2018), and academic outcomes (Dijkhuis et al., 2020; Freeman et al., 2017). Additionally, a cross-sectional study comparing age- and IQ- matched ASD, ADHD, and neurotypical youth suggested that individuals with ASD have less severe and persistent EF symptoms on average compared to those with ADHD, although their profiles of EF differed by diagnosis (Happé et al., 2006). Across the literature, impairments in the facets of cognitive flexibility and inhibition have been observed most frequently in ASD (Ros & Graziano, 2019).

Cognitive flexibility and inhibition are two specific domains of EF which are particularly susceptible to impairment across the lifespan among individuals with ASD (Corbett et al., 2009; Pennington & Ozonoff, 1996; Verté et al., 2006). Cognitive flexibility is the ability to shift between different thoughts or tasks (Monsell, 2003). When faced with a variety of situational demands, ideally an individual flexibly and frequently modifies their behavioral or cognitive approaches. There is some evidence to suggest that individuals with ASD struggle with this aspect of EF more so than neurotypical individuals and at a level comparable to individuals with other developmental disorders such as ADHD (see Uddin, 2021, for review). Further, cognitive inflexibility is associated with mental health symptoms across adolescence (Hollocks et al., 2021). Cognitive inflexibility can be tied to core ASD characteristics such as rigid adherence to behavioral routines and a desire for sameness, and even limited perspective-taking in social situations (Geurts, de Vries, et al., 2014). Relatedly, inhibition encapsulates one’s ability to reduce ruminative thinking, suppress automatic responses or ignore distracting stimuli in order to engage in a more productive behavior (Geurts, van den Bergh, et al., 2014). Meta-analyses of both of these facets of inhibition indicated that they are impaired in ASD (Geurts, van den Bergh, et al., 2014). In fact, difficulties in response inhibition via performance-based tasks remained in a sample of youth with ASD after controlling for ADHD symptoms when compared to both youth with ADHD and neurotypical youth. This suggests that inhibition problems may be uniquely tied to ASD, rather than a result of common ADHD comorbidity (Karalunas et al., 2018).

Self-regulation also concerns the ability to change one’s own emotional states (Baumeister & Vohs, 2003; Hofmann et al., 2012). Emotion regulation (ER) is often defined as one’s ability to manage or modify their emotional state and arousal to accomplish their goals (Thompson, 1994). Previous research has found that youth with ASD frequently have clinically elevated levels of ER impairment by caregiver report (Conner et al., 2021) and that ASD symptomatology is positively associated with the level of caregiver-reported ER impairment (Berkovits et al., 2017; Samson et al., 2014). Furthermore, individuals with ASD have been found to engage in fewer and less adaptive ER strategies and to deploy ER strategies less flexibly as compared to individuals without ASD (Cai et al., 2018; Khor et al., 2014; Mazefsky et al., 2014; Samson et al., 2015). In a study of preschool-aged children with ADHD, ASD and ADHD, and neurotypical children who were administered both lab and caregiver-/teacher-report questionnaires, those with ASD/ADHD were more likely to present with the poorest EF and those with ADHD alone were more likely to present with poorest ER (Ros & Graziano, 2019). While few studies have ever compared older samples or individuals with only ASD and only ADHD, ER impairment has also been observed in multiple ADHD samples (Bunford et al., 2020; England-Mason, 2020; Ros & Graziano, 2019).

Among people with ASD, both caregiver-reported ER and EF are associated with co-occurring psychopathology (Conner et al., 2021; Lawson et al., 2015). In particular, ER impairment in ASD has been associated with co-occurring psychiatric concerns such as anxiety, depression, and suicidality (Charlton et al., 2020; Conner, Golt, et al., 2020) and has also been correlated with social and academic outcomes and global quality of life (Jahromi et al., 2013). Inflexibility in those with ASD is similarly associated with anxiety and depression (Lawson et al., 2015).

Co-occurring anxiety and depression are highly prevalent and are associated with diminished global functioning and quality of life among individuals with ASD. Although estimates vary, lifetime prevalence figures of approximately 40% have been reported for both anxiety and depression (Hollocks et al., 2019). Other studies have observed higher rates of co-occurring psychiatric conditions and symptoms (Simonoff et al., 2008), including findings that 63% of young adults with ASD were clinically elevated on measures of psychiatric symptoms (McCauley et al., 2020). Prior research has examined whether core ASD characteristics are associated with the presence of co-occurring anxiety and depression. Co-occurring conditions like anxiety could represent a separate co-occurring condition, reflect overlap from core ASD characteristics, or be missed entirely due to diagnostic overshadowing (see Rosen et al., 2018, for review). Hollocks and colleagues (2014) assessed adolescents with ASD to examine the relationships between anxiety, depression, and tasks measuring EF and social cognition (e.g., theory of mind). Findings did not indicate a relationship between the social cognition tasks and anxiety or depression. In a longitudinal study of youth with ASD, social anxiety predicted worse social communication impairment, but the inverse was not found (Duvekot et al., 2017). However, in an adult ASD sample, ASD severity was inversely related to anxiety in terms of quality of life, such that those with less severe ASD characteristics reported lower quality of life if they also had significant anxiety, but anxiety was not associated with quality of life in adults with more severe ASD characteristics (Smith et al., 2019). These internalizing diagnoses are likely to be of particular concern for transition-age youth with ASD, given that this developmental period is associated with increased risk for psychopathology among emerging adults who are neurotypical (Copeland et al., 2014; Lei et al., 2020; McCauley et al., 2020), as well as neural and developmental differences that may render additional social and adaptive skills demands particularly difficult for youth with ASD (Picci & Scherf, 2015). Much less research has examined how EF and ER are temporally related to psychopathology; for instance, whether the transdiagnostic factors increase risk for psychiatric diagnoses, are a byproduct of the presence of psychopathology, or if bi-directional or a mixture of individual temporal relationships best capture these relationships. Prior research in ASD has found that impairments in specific EF components contribute differently to later psychopathology. In a longitudinal study of youth with ASD, Vogan and colleagues (2018) found that poorer parent-rated ability to inhibit, set-shift, and maintain emotional control was predictive of internalizing symptoms (depression and anxiety) and externalizing symptoms two years later, but that other EF skills (e.g., planning, organization, initiation, monitoring) were not predictive of later psychiatric symptoms.

Since ER and EF appear to be related to a host of outcomes, directly targeting them in interventions may be particularly efficacious. Similar to the push for transdiagnostic treatments for mood and anxiety disorders in neurotypical populations using ER (e.g., Barlow et al., 2017; Ehrenreich-May et al., 2017; Kennedy et al., 2019), targeting EF has been previously demonstrated in ASD. The most well-studied EF intervention is a group-based, transdiagnostic program called Unstuck and On Target (Cannon et al., 2011), which has been studied in school- and clinic-based settings with degrees of parental involvement and training and found improvement in flexibility and problem solving, as well as improvements in ER-related measures (Dickson et al., 2020; Elias et al., 2019; Kenworthy et al., 2014, 2022). These findings suggest that examining ER and EF together may be particularly useful to assess their relationships to anxiety and depression.

The Present Study

Taken together, the relationship between EF, ER, and co-occurring psychopathology remains poorly understood, especially in transition-aged youth with ASD. Much of the prior literature examines the relationship between either ER or EF and psychopathology (or other outcomes) but does not account for the overlap of these transdiagnostic factors. Self-regulatory difficulties, in particular EF (inflexibility, disinhibition) and ER, may differentially relate to co-occurring depression and anxiety symptoms. The aim of the current study was to examine whether and how ER and EF were uniquely associated with anxiety and depression. Specifically, we aimed to assess whether an individual’s inhibition difficulties or level of cognitive flexibility, as measured by a performance-based EF test, were associated uniquely, beyond ER impairment, with symptoms of anxiety and depression. We hypothesized that inhibition difficulties and worse cognitive flexibility and would be associated with greater self-reported symptoms of anxiety and depression, above and beyond ER impairment, in a sample of adolescents and emerging adults. Such findings would inform potential treatment targets, including for ER and EF, in individuals with ASD.

Methods

Participants

Participants were 57 adolescents and young adults with ASD (Mage= 18.56, Range= 16–25 years) enrolled in a randomized controlled trial (RCT). The experimental intervention was a transition support program for secondary or postsecondary students interested in pursuing higher education ((White et al., 2021). For the trial, participants either had to have autism listed as an educational classification in their individualized education plans, or an ASD diagnosis was confirmed by a research-reliable administration of the ADOS-2 (Lord et al., 2012). This approach to diagnostic confirmation is consistent with studies that have demonstrated that a prior community- or school-based diagnosis is largely consistent with a research ADOS administration (Daniels et al., 2012; Maddox et al., 2020). Participants also had a full-scale IQ of ≥ 85 on the WASI-II (Wechsler, 2011). Lastly, RCT eligibility required no changes in psychotropic medications during the course of treatment, and no participation in concurrent psychotherapy. Approximately one-third of the participants were actively taking a prescribed psychotropic medication, and over 70% of participants met criteria for an anxiety disorder at baseline via either caregiver- or self-report on the Anxiety Disorders Interview Schedule for DSM-5 (Albano & Silverman,in press; Brown & Barlow, 2014). The majority of the sample identified as White (86%) and Male (80%). See (White et al., 2021 for further details on the RCT and exclusion criteria. Recruitment occurred in a southeastern state in both rural and small city/suburban settings, including efforts to recruit through local school districts and two- and four-year postsecondary programs. All measures in the current study were collected at baseline, prior to enrollment in the transition support program (See Table 1).

Table 1.

Demographics and Descriptive Data

n (%)

Gender  Male 46 (80.70)
 Female 11 (19.30)
Race/Ethnicity  Asian 5 (8.77)
 Black 2 (3.51)
 White 49 (85.96)
 Prefer not to indicate 1 (1.75)
 Hispanic/Latino 3 (5.26)
Current Psychotropic Medication 21 (36.84)
Co-occurring psychiatric diagnosis Any anxiety disorder 42 (71.19)
ADHD 10 (16.95)
Any mood disorder 11 (18.64)
Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder 4 (6.78)
PTSD 3 (5.08)

M(SD) n (%) Range

Age 18.56 (2.04) 16–24
WASI-II 2 Scale IQ 105.82 (12.40) 84–131
BDI-II total score 13.67 (11.63) 0–42
 Minimal range 13 (24.01%) 0–13
 Mild range 8 14–19
 Moderate range 8 20–28
 Severe range 7 29–42
BAI total score 11.37 (9.41) 0– 43
 Minimal range 23 0–7
 Mild range 17 8–15
 Moderate range 10 16–25
 Severe range 4 26–43
SRS-2 parent-report total T-score 68.67 (10.33) 48–90
SRS-2 self-report total T-score 63.71 (10.12) 44– 83
DERS total score 94.13 (24.79) 46–153
Nonacceptance 14.23 (7.24) 6– 30
Goals 16.57 (3.97) 6– 25
Impulse 13.48 (5.44) 6– 28
Strategies 19.96 (7.93) 8– 38
D-KEFS Standard Score
Sorting Test Confirmed Correct Sorts 9.43 (2.95) 3–15
Combined Total Description 10.35 (5.21) 2–27
Color-Word Interference Test Inhibition 9.37 (2.99) 1–14
Inhibition/Switching 9.04 (2.90) 1–15
Trail Making Test Number-Letter Switching 7.87 (3.63) 1–13

Note: WASI-II= Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence, Second Edition; BDI-II= Beck Depression Inventory, Second Edition; BAI= Beck Anxiety Inventory; SRS-2= Social Responsiveness Scale, Second Edition; DERS=Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale; D-KEFS= Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System

Measures

Autism Characteristics

Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule- Second Edition (ADOS-2; Lord et al., 2012). The ADOS-2 is a semi-structured clinical task used to aid clinicians in distinguishing behaviors characteristic of autism from those seen in the typical population. The ADOS-2 was administered by trained, research-reliable clinicians. Module 4, appropriate for older adolescents and adults with fluent verbal speech, was used for this study to confirm ASD diagnoses for participants who did not have autism listed as a diagnosis on a pre-existing individualized education plan, which represented half of the sample.

Social Responsiveness Scale, Second Edition Self-Report (SRS-2; Constantino & Gruber, 2012). The SRS-2 is a 65-item measure of social difficulties and restricted and repetitive behaviors in ASD. The self-report version of this measure was used for the purposes of this study. Items are scored on a four-point Likert scale (1= “not true,” 2= “sometimes true,” 3= “often true,” and 4= “almost always true”). Five treatment subscales (Social Awareness, Social Cognition, Social Communication, Social Motivation, and Restricted and Repetitive Behaviors) are summed into a Total Score and converted into T-scores. Internal consistency for the self-report Total Score in this study was high (α= .95).

Emotion and Behavior Assessments

Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI; Beck et al., 1988; Beck & Steer, 1990). The BAI is a 21-item self-report questionnaire for anxiety symptoms. Items are rated on a 4-point Likert scale (from 0= “not at all,” to 3= “severe”), and higher summed scores indicate more severe anxiety symptoms. The BAI has been used extensively in research and clinical settings, including in several previous studies of adults with ASD (McVey et al., 2021). Internal consistency in the current study was high (α= .91).

Beck Depression Inventory, Second Edition (BDI-II; Beck et al., 1996). The BDI-II is a 21-item self-report questionnaire that assesses depression symptoms. Items are rated on a 4-point Likert scale (from 0= “not at all,” to 3= “severe”), and higher summed scores indicate more severe depressive symptoms. The BDI-II also has extensive previous use in research and clinical settings, including among adults with ASD (McVey et al., 2021; Williams, Everaert, et al., 2021). Internal consistency in this study was high (α= .92). Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS; Gratz & Roemer, 2004). The DERS is a 36-item, self-report questionnaire that assesses ER impairment. Items are scored on a 5-point Likert scale from 1 (“almost never”) to 5 (“almost always”), with 11 reverse-scored items. The DERS yields six subscales: Nonacceptance, Goals, Impulse, Awareness, Strategies, and Clarity. Previous research on the DERS has not consistently replicated the six-factor structure and has suggested that the Awareness subscale of the DERS be removed or modified (combined with Clarity) due to low internal consistency (Bardeen et al., 2016; Lee et al., 2016). A recent study examining the DERS in ASD samples observed similar difficulties with the Awareness subscale and recommended against using a DERS Total score (McVey et al., 2021). Thus, the current study used the Nonacceptance, Goals, Impulse, and Strategies subscales only. Internal consistency for these subscales was acceptable in this sample: Nonacceptance α= .93, Goals α= .79, Impulse α= .87, Strategies α= .97.

Executive Function Assessments

Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS; Delis et al., 2001). The D-KEFS is a series of clinician-administered tests of EF domains (organization, planning, initiation, concept formation, and cognitive flexibility). Each test can be administered separately. For this sample, the Trail-Making, Color-Word Interference, and Card Sorting tasks were administered due to their relationships with cognitive flexibility and inhibition. The Sorting test measures cognitive flexibility and conceptual reasoning. Participants are provided six distinct cards and are asked to sort the cards repeatedly according to perceptual and semantic characteristics. The test yields a Confirmed Correct Sorts score which measures concept formation. Further, this study uses the Total Description Score that assesses the participant’s ability to describe sort categories. The Number-Letter Switching task of the Trail Making Test measures cognitive flexibility. Participants switch between connecting number and letter categories in order (e.g., A-1, B-2, etc.). For the Color-Word Interference Test (CWI), participants are asked to name color words printed in the wrong color (Inhibition) and again where participants are asked to switch to verbalizing the ink color or color written throughout the task (Inhibition/Switching). The D-KEFS has been used in research with both children and adults with ASD (Semrud-Clikeman et al., 2010; Zimmerman et al., 2017). All test scores are scaled scores (M=10, SD=3).

Cognitive Assessments

Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence, Second Edition (WASI-II; Wechsler, 2011). The WASI-II provides a general estimate of cognitive ability for individuals 6–90 years old. For this study, a clinician conducted the two-subscale version (one verbal subtest and one perceptual reasoning subtest) that yields an overall IQ score. The 2-subscale IQ score has strong validity and reliability with a full-scale IQ (Wechsler, 2011).

Analysis

Statistics were computed using IBM SPSS statistics version 27.0. Any participant who had missing DERS, D-KEFS, BDI-II, or BAI scores was removed listwise for analyses (n= 8). Descriptive statistics and Pearson correlations were first calculated to characterize the sample. No outliers (defined as a score > 3 SDs from the mean) were observed. We then conducted two hierarchical linear regressions, one with the BAI total score as the dependent variable and one with the BDI-II total score as the dependent variable. Step one consisted of the WASI-II two-subscale IQ score and the SRS-2 self-report Total T-score, step two included four DERS subscales (Nonacceptance, Goals, Strategies, and Impulse Control), and step three included the two D-KEFS Sorting subtest scores (Sorting Confirmed Correct Sorts score and Sorting Total Combined Description score). Two additional hierarchical linear regressions were conducted with step three as Inhibition-based tasks (CWI Inhibition, CWI Inhibition/Switching, Trail Making Number-Letter Switching).

Results

First, Pearson correlations were examined. WASI-II two-subscale IQ scores were significantly and positively correlated with the DERS Goals subscale (r = .36, p = .005), D-KEFS confirmed correct sorts (r = .41, p = .002), and D-KEFS total combined description scores (r = .30, p = .02; See Table 2). Correlations among D-KEFS subscales and BAI/BDI-II scores were nonsignificant. SRS-2 self-report total T-scores were significantly and positively correlated with all DERS subscales (rs = .39-.58, ps < .05). Correlations among depression scores, SRS-2 scores, and DERS subscales were moderate to strong and statistically significant (rs = .49-.57, ps < .001). Anxiety symptoms as measured by the BAI demonstrated similar associations with SRS-2 and DERS scores (rs = .33-.59, ps < .01). Next, we conducted the regression analyses and examined model assumptions. No multicollinearity (no variance inflation factor > 3.2), interdependence (Durbin-Watson= 1.91), non-linearity, or heteroscedasticity were detected.

Table 2.

Correlations

1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. 12. 13. 14.

1. WASI-II --
2. BDI-II Total −.19 --
3. BAI Total −.16 .78** --
4. DERS Nonacceptance −.12 .63** .57** --
5. DERS Goals .36** .49** .34* .60** --
6. DERS Impulse −.09 .54** .53** .44** .46** --
7. DERS Strategies −.07 .72** .54** .67** .63** .47** --
8. DERS Total −.02 .77** .64** .81** .70** .69** .87** --
9. SRS-2 Total −.05 .62** .51** .53** .36** .38** .46** .60** --
10. DKEFS Confirmed Correct .41** −.03 −.11 −.05 .20 −.01 .05 .10 .07 --
11. DKEFS Combined Desc .28* −.18 −.10 −.03 −.02 −.09 .02 .09 −.07 .63** --
12. DKEFS Inhibition .40** .10 .16 .07 .17 .14 .01 .13 .28 .29** .1 --
13. DKEFS Inhibition/switching .45** .17 .13 .18 .38** .24 .14 .24 .29 .35** .13 .72** --
14. DKEFS Number/Letter .41** −.05 −.09 −.03 .08 −.01 −.12 −.01 −.03 .28* .13 .49** .51** --

Note: WASI-II= Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence Second Edition; BDI-II= Beck Depression Inventory Second Edition; BAI= Beck Anxiety Inventory; SRS-2= Social Responsiveness Scale Second Edition; DERS= Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale; D-KEFS= Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System

Inhibition

The overall model using inhibition to predict BAI scores accounted for 46% of the model variance. Step 1 contributed about one quarter of the variance, ER impairment added 27% of the model variance, and inhibition added 2.7% additional variance. Significant predictors of higher BAI scores were higher DERS Nonacceptance scores (β= .37, p = .036) and higher DERS Impulse Control scores (β= .38, p = .006).

When using the inhibition model to BDI-II scores, it accounted for 68% of the variance. IQ and ASD characteristics accounted for 43% of the model, ER impairment (step 2) contributed 30%, and inhibition (step 3) only added 1.2% to the model. Higher SRS-2 total scores (β= .22, p = .047), worse DERS Impulse Control (β= .27, p = .01), and worse DERS Strategy use (β= .40, p = .004) were significant predictors of higher BDI-II scores.

Cognitive Flexibility

The overall model using IQ, ASD characteristics, ER impairment, and cognitive flexibility to predict BAI scores accounted for 44% of the score variance (See Table 4). IQ and ASD characteristics (step 1) contributed to 23% of the model, ER impairment (step 2) contributed 27%, and cognitive flexibility (step 3) added only 0.3% to the model. Only higher DERS Nonacceptance scores (β= .39, p = .03) and higher DERS Impulse Control scores (β= .40, p = .004) were significant predictors of anxiety symptoms.

Table 4.

Regression models predicting Anxiety

Dependent Variable: Anxiety (BAI) β F Adj. R2 ΔR2

Step 1 8.121* .229 --
 WASI-II −.133
 SRS-2 .479**

Step 2 7.903** .463 .269**
 WASI-II .023
 SRS-2 .130
 DERS Nonacceptance .376*
 DERS Goals −.248
 DERS Impulse control .396**
 DERS Regulatory Strategies .193

Step 3 (Sorting) 5.708** .440 .003
 WASI-II .048
 SRS-2 .124
DERS Nonacceptance .387*
 DERS Goals −.272
 DERS Impulse control .395**
 DERS Regulatory strategies .206
 D-KEFS Confirmed correct sorts .010
 D-KEFS Sorting combined description score −.062

Step 3 (Inhibition) 5.466** .456 .027
 WASI-II −.044
 SRS-2 .075
 DERS Nonacceptance .365*
 DERS Goals −.206
 DERS Impulse Control .377**
 DERS Regulatory Strategies .225
 D-KEFS CWI Inhibition .246
D-KEFS CWI Inhibition/Switching −.100
D-KEFS Trail-making Number-letter switching −.034
*

p < .05

**

p < .01

Note: WASI-II= Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence Second Edition; BDI-II= Beck Depression Inventory Second Edition; BAI= Beck Anxiety Inventory; SRS-2= Social Responsiveness Scale Second Edition Total Score; DERS= Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale; D-KEFS= Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System; CWI= Color-Word Interference

Overall, the model predicting BDI-II scores accounted for approximately 71% of the variance in BDI-II total score (See Table 3). IQ and ASD characteristics (step 1) contributed 40% to the model variance, ER impairment (step 2) added 30%, and cognitive flexibility (step 3) added only an additional 2.7% to the model. Self-reported ASD severity (SRS-2) (β= .21, p = .045), Impulse Control impairment (DERS) (β= .29, p = .004), Strategies impairment (DERS) (β= .41, p = .002), and inflexibility (lower D-KEFS Sorting Description scores) (β= −.23, p = .046) were significant predictors of depression.

Table 3.

Regression models predicting Depression

Dependent Variable: Depression (BDI-II) β F Adj. R2 ΔR2

Step 1 17.20** .403 --
 WASI-II −.140
 SRS-2 .624**

Step 2 18.714** .689 .300**
 WASI-II −.081
 SRS-2 .248*
 DERS Nonacceptance .183
 DERS Goals −.042
 DERS Impulse control .283**
 DERS Regulatory Strategies .374**

Step 3 (Sorting) 15.374** .710 .027
 WASI-II −.045
 SRS-2 .209*
 DERS Nonacceptance .244
 DERS Goals −.142
 DERS Impulse control .288**
 DERS Regulatory strategies .407**
 D-KEFS Confirmed correct sorts .177
 D-KEFS Sorting combined description score −.226*

Step 3 (Inhibition) 12.323** .680 .012
 WASI-II −.162
 SRS-2 .222*
 DERS Nonacceptance .161
 DERS Goals −.012
 DERS Impulse Control .267*
 DERS Regulatory Strategies .397**
 D-KEFS CWI Inhibition .085
 D-KEFS CWI Inhibition/Switching −.010
 D-KEFS Trail-making Number-letter switching .079
*

p < .05

**

p < .01

Note: WASI-II= Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence Second Edition; BDI-II= Beck Depression Inventory Second Edition; BAI= Beck Anxiety Inventory; SRS-2= Social Responsiveness Scale Second Edition Total Score; DERS= Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale; D-KEFS= Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System; CWI= Color-Word Interference

Discussion

The current study investigated how an individual’s impairment in inhibition or cognitive flexibility, two facets of EF, were related to anxiety and depression above and beyond self-reported ER impairment in transition-aged individuals with ASD. Little of the prior research in this area has observed both ER and EF in tandem with anxiety or depression, and likewise, much of the previous research has not examined transition-aged youth, a time characterized by a heightened risk for anxiety and depression in ASD and neurotypical samples (Copeland et al., 2014; McCauley et al., 2020; Picci & Scherf, 2015). As expected, poorer ER was correlated with both depression and anxiety symptoms and with ASD characteristics, and poor ER was significantly associated with both depression and anxiety after controlling for IQ and ASD characteristics. Inhibition was not uniquely associated with anxiety or depression symptoms above and beyond ER impairment. Similarly, impairments in cognitive flexibility were not significantly related to anxiety symptoms above and beyond ER impairment. However, poorer cognitive flexibility was uniquely and significantly associated with increased depression symptoms. This finding, while preliminary due to the cross-sectional nature of the study, may suggest that an individual’s ability to behaviorally shift between tasks is linked to risk for depressive symptoms.

Across both anxiety and depression, ER (particularly DERS Impulse Control subscale) was more strongly associated than the performance-based EF tasks in the regression analyses. All of the selected DERS subscales were significantly correlated with anxiety and depression symptoms, whereas no EF task was significantly correlated with anxiety or depression. In part, these findings may be due to the fact that ER is more directly related to psychopathology, while EF also encompasses cognitive processes that are not emotional in nature. Prior research has consistently demonstrated that ER is associated with co-occurring psychiatric symptoms in ASD samples (Charlton et al., 2020; Conner et al., 2021). Similarly, ER, anxiety, and depression were also positively correlated with ASD characteristics in the current study. These findings mirror prior research in youth samples that have shown that core ASD characteristics are associated with poor ER (Berkovits et al., 2017; Samson et al., 2014), and that anxiety and depression symptoms are also correlated with ASD characteristics (Hollocks et al., 2014; Keenan et al., 2017; McCauley et al., 2020).

In all models, more variance was explained in self-report depressive symptoms than in self-report anxiety symptoms, which may suggest that the relationship between different facets of self-regulatory impairments and depression symptoms is stronger, perhaps due to ruminative cognition seen in depression (Williams, McKenney, et al., 2021). Research in non-ASD samples has long found an association between poor EF and depression (Holler et al., 2014). Interestingly, a majority of the sample reported a prior diagnosis of an anxiety disorder (>70%), while about one-fifth of the sample reported a prior diagnosis of a mood disorder. The treatment-seeking nature of the current sample may have led to a higher prevalence of anxiety in this sample; additionally, prior research has been inconclusive about whether higher IQ (such as seen in this post-secondary education-seeking sample) is associated specifically with higher prevalence of anxiety (Hollocks et al., 2014; Witwer & Lecavalier, 2010). Nonetheless, this high prevalence of anxiety diagnoses in the current sample limits the ability to generalize findings to individuals with autism who do not present with anxiety.

EF and ER are commonly considered to be two interrelated components of self-regulation. In previous research and in contrast to the current study, EF impairment (via parent- and self-report questionnaires) has been associated with internalizing and externalizing symptoms in longitudinal studies of children with ASD (Vogan et al., 2018), and both anxiety and depression symptoms in adults on the autism spectrum (Wallace et al., 2016). In accordance with our findings, previous research has found that poor ER has been associated with depression and anxiety symptoms in adolescents and young adults with ASD (Charlton et al., 2020). However, our study is among the first to examine how individual differences in EF and ER, measured via a performance-based task, may be uniquely associated with psychopathology in this population. When examining EF impairment, above and beyond ER, the findings seem less clear. Our finding that poor cognitive flexibility, but not inhibition, is associated with depression diverges from Wallace et al.’s (2016) findings, wherein self-report of poor flexibility was associated with anxiety and metacognitive difficulties (planning, initiation, monitoring) were associated with depression. Perhaps findings differ due to the use of performance-based EF tests versus self-report questionnaires. Cognitive inflexibility, behaviorally manifested as rumination and other cognitive ‘stickiness’, may be especially salient symptoms for those with ASD and depression (Gotham et al., 2015; Gotham et al., 2018). Lastly, because the current study was underpowered to detect smaller effects, these findings may not reflect the potential relationships between the constructs of EF, ER, and depression or anxiety.

Despite research indicating that poor inhibition is common in ASD (Geurts, van den Bergh, et al., 2014), inhibition was not significantly and uniquely associated with depression or anxiety above and beyond ER impairment in the current study. It is possible that the link to psychopathology in inhibition overlaps with ER impairments. For example, perhaps poor inhibition itself is only associated with co-occurring mental health conditions when paired with heightened emotional reactivity (Mazefsky et al., 2013). In this study, the DERS Impulse Control subscale may have accounted specifically for this specific difficulty with inhibitory functioning in emotion-laden situations. Future research should investigate these potential relationships by measuring and controlling for ER and further assessing how EF may differ when emotionally activated and when not. Again, the current study’s findings may also reflect inability to detect small effects due to sample size limitations.

Relatedly, other factors may also have influenced these findings. One example is slow processing speed. While not explicitly measured in this sample, it is possible that slow processing speed, seen commonly in ASD (Duncan et al., 2018; Reinvall et al., 2017), could complicate findings. A study utilizing the same sample as this project found that almost one-third of this sample exhibited clinically significant levels of sluggish cognitive tempo by caregiver-report, which consists of slow processing speed, daydreaming, and mental fogginess ((Brewe, Simmons, Capriola-Hall, & White, 2020). Further, sluggish cognitive tempo was associated with heightened EF impairment and depressive symptoms. These findings suggest that factors such as poor processing speed may uniquely relate to depression rather than anxiety in ASD. Again, more research is needed to disentangle these factors in adolescents and adults with ASD.

Additionally, overall higher IQ (not assessing for processing speed) was correlated with worse ER but better EF in this sample, but IQ was not a significant predictor of either depression or anxiety. While these findings are discordant with prior research suggesting that higher IQ may be associated with higher levels of anxiety in ASD (Lecavalier et al., 2019), they are consistent with prior studies of individuals with ASD and no cognitive impairment. Further, aside from EF tasks, the current study relied on self-report data of ER, anxiety, depression, and autism characteristics, and common method variance may have thus influenced the results.

This study is not without limitations, foremost of which is reliance on cross-sectional data, which does not allow us to determine causality (i.e., we cannot state that EF or ER causes anxiety and depression symptoms or vice versa), and future longitudinal work will be necessary to assess causal relationships. We also acknowledge that the small sample size and lack of a control or comparison group may have limited our ability to detect smaller effects and draw comparisons to other populations, respectively. Additionally, this study relied on either IEP classification of autism or an ADOS-2, rather than confirming ASD diagnosis via ADOS-2 for all participants, which limits ability to generalize findings. Difficulties with self-regulation and mental health symptoms may also have been overrepresented among our intervention-seeking sample. In addition, this study’s sample was predominantly White, male, and required to have a full-scale IQ >85 for participation in the RCTs. These factors may limit generalizability of our findings to non-treatment-seeking samples, females, and individuals with IQ < 85.

The current study has a methodological strength in using self-report measures of ER, depression, and anxiety, and a performance-based measure of EF. On the other hand, reliance on questionnaire data, that are primarily self-report, raises concern about shared method and rater variance, respectively, which could at least partially explaining the relationship between ER and symptoms, with less of a relationship between EF and anxiety and depression symptoms. Future studies can also focus on using self-report measures that are specifically designed for individuals with ASD, including measures of depression, anxiety, and ER (Conner, White, et al., 2020; Rodgers et al., 2020; Williams, Everaert, et al., 2021). Optimally, both multiple reporter and performance-based EF measures would be used to be able to both assess the ecological validity of the performance-based tasks and investigate the real-world impact of EF impairment. Multi-method assessment of ER is also needed in future research, including observational coding and psychophysiological measures alongside questionnaires and clinical interviews (Northrup et al., 2020). In the current study, the majority of participants were diagnosed with a co-occurring anxiety disorder and approximately one-sixth of participants were diagnosed with co-occurring ADHD, which may have influenced results. Thus, future research should consider including other measures of core ASD characteristics as well as controlling for co-occurring psychiatric conditions.

Research into EF and ER in those with ASD continues to accumulate, particularly in the past 20 years. Mapping inter-relationships among these constructs, within a larger self-regulatory framework, may help elucidate reasons for the frequently reported ability-performance discrepancies seen in ASD research (Kenworthy et al., 2008; Tonizzi et al., 2021). As Geurts and colleagues (2009) argued in their review of cognitive inflexibility research in ASD, and others have often posited, there is a large gulf between performance-based tasks and everyday situations that call for EF skills, requiring the field to consider how best to create and use more ecologically based measures. And these real-world challenges are often exacerbated with ER impairment, but much of our research investigates these constructs independently as opposed to intertwined, as opposed to research into ‘hot’ EF that incorporates how emotionally-laden situations can differ (Zelazo & Carlson, 2012). Instead, research should incorporate measurement of self-regulation-based impairments (EF, ER, and attentional challenges) more broadly, especially into interventions, to better understand the relationships between these constructs and their impacts on overall functioning.

Implications

Professionals and caregivers should attend to mental health symptoms such as anxiety and depression, and their association with self-regulatory difficulties, in transition-aged youth with ASD. Therapies addressing anxiety and depression, while vital, may be optimized by training to improve ER and EF. For example, treating depression using CBT may be augmented with more instruction on coping skills, including identifying when and how to use cognitive flexibility and how to identify and shift perseverative thinking that is more negative (ruminative) in nature. However, while prior research has demonstrated efficacy in treating anxiety in individuals with ASD (Wood et al., 2020), response rates of CBT for youth with ASD are lower in research compared to neurotypical youth (Kreslins et al., 2015; Storch et al., 2021). Mindfulness-based approaches have also demonstrated utility for improving ER in ASD (Conner et al., 2019; Hartmann et al., 2019; Ritschel et al., 2021). It is also important to assess self-regulatory difficulties in clients with ASD using multiple methods, including questionnaires and interviewing clients and caregivers (when relevant) to understand how EF and ER difficulties appear in their daily lives and contribute to presenting concerns. Addressing self-regulation more broadly may be especially pertinent for those with ASD to improve general outcomes.

Highlights.

  • Assessed the roles of executive functioning and emotion regulation together

  • IQ, autism characteristics, and emotion regulation contribute to depression/anxiety

  • Inhibitory difficulties did not uniquely contribute to depression or anxiety

  • Cognitive flexibility difficulties contributed minorly to depression, not anxiety

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank all participants and their families. This research was funded by the National Institute of Mental Health (R34MH114906; S. White). Funders did not have a role in the decision to submit this article for publication, including data collection, analyses, or interpretation.

Footnotes

Conflict of Interest

The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare that are relevant to the content of this article.

Declaration of interests

The authors declare that they have no known competing financial interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence the work reported in this paper.

Ethics Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. The study was approved by the Virginia Tech Institutional Review Board.

Consent to participate

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study. For all minors who participated, informed consent was obtained from legal guardians and minors provided assent.

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  1. Albano AM, & Silverman WK (n.d.). Anxiety Disorders Interview Schedule for DSM-5, Child and Parent Versions. Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  2. American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th Edition. American Psychiatric Association. [Google Scholar]
  3. Bardeen JR, Fergus TA, Hannan SM, & Orcutt HK (2016). Addressing psychometric limitations of the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale through item modification. Journal of Personality Assessment, 98(3), 298–309. 10.1080/00223891.2015.1091774 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Barlow DH, Farchione TJ, Bullis JR, Gallagher MW, Murray-Latin H, Sauer-Zavala S, Bentley KH, Thompson-Hollands J, Conklin LR, Boswell JF, Ametaj A, Carl JR, Boettcher HT, & Cassiello-Robbins C (2017). The unified protocol for transdiagnostic treatment of Emotional Disorders compared with diagnosis-specific protocols for anxiety disorders: A randomized clinical trial. JAMA Psychiatry, 74(9), 875–884. 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2017.2164 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Baumeister RF, & Vohs KD (2003). Self- regulation and the executive function of the self. Handbook of Self and Identity, 197–217. [Google Scholar]
  6. Beck AT, Epstein N, Brown G, & Steer RA (1988). An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety: Psychometric properties. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56(6), 893. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Beck AT, Steer RA, & Brown GK (1996). Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory-II. Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
  8. Beck Aaron T., & Steer RA (1990). Beck Anxiety Inventory Manual. Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
  9. Berkovits L, Eisenhower A, & Blacher J (2017). Emotion Regulation in Young Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 47(1), 68–79. 10.1007/s10803-016-2922-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bertollo JR, Strang JF, Anthony LG, Kenworthy L, Wallace GL, & Yerys BE (2020). Adaptive Behavior in Youth with Autism Spectrum Disorder: The Role of Flexibility. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 50(1), 42–50. 10.1007/s10803-019-04220-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bridgett DJ, Oddi KB, Laake LM, Murdock KW, & Bachmann MN (2013). Integrating and differentiating aspects of self-regulation: Effortful control, executive functioning, and links to negative affectivity. Emotion, 13(1), 47–63. 10.1037/a0029536 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Brown TA, & Barlow DH (2014). Anxiety and Related Disorders Interview Schedule for DSM-5, Adult Version. Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  13. Bunford N, Dawson AE, Evans SW, Ray AR, Langberg JM, Owens JS, DuPaul GJ, & Allan DM (2020). The Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale–Parent Report: A Psychometric Investigation Examining Adolescents With and Without ADHD. Assessment, 27(5), 921–940. 10.1177/1073191118792307 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Cai RY, Richdale AL, Foley K-R, Trollor J, & Uljarević M (2018). Brief report: Cross-sectional interactions between expressive suppression and cognitive reappraisal and its relationship with depressive symptoms in autism spectrum disorder. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 45, 1–8. 10.1016/j.rasd.2017.10.002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  15. Cannon L, Kenworthy L, Alexander K, Adler Werner M, & Anthony L (2011). Unstuck and On Target!: An Executive Function Curriculum to Improve Flexibility for Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Paul H Brookes Publishing. [Google Scholar]
  16. Charlton AS, Smith IC, Mazefsky CA, & White SW (2020). The Role of Emotion Regulation on Co-occurring Psychopathology in Emerging Adults with ASD. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 50(7), 2585–2592. 10.1007/s10803-019-03983-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Conner CM, Golt J, Righi G, Shaffer R, Siegel M, & Mazefsky CA (2020). A Comparative Study of Suicidality and Its Association with Emotion Regulation Impairment in Large ASD and US Census-Matched Samples. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 50(10), 3545–3560. 10.1007/s10803-020-04370-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Conner CM, Golt J, Shaffer RC, Righi G, Siegel M, & Mazefsky CA (2021). Emotion dysregulation is substantially elevated in autism compared to the general population: Impact on psychiatric services. Autism Research, 14(1), 169–181. 10.1002/aur.2450 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Conner CM, White SW, Beck KB, Golt J, Smith IC, & Mazefsky CA (2019). Improving emotion regulation ability in autism: The Emotional Awareness and Skills Enhancement (EASE) program. Autism, 23(5), 1273–1287. 10.1177/1362361318810709 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Conner CM, White SW, Scahill L, & Mazefsky CA (2020). The role of emotion regulation and core autism symptoms in the experience of anxiety in autism. Autism, 24(4), 931–940. 10.1177/1362361320904217 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Constantino JN, & Gruber CP (2012). Social responsiveness scale-2nd Edition (SRS-2). Western Psychological Services. [Google Scholar]
  22. Copeland WE, Angold A, Shanahan L, & Costello EJ (2014). Longitudinal patterns of anxiety from childhood to adulthood: The great smoky mountains study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 53(1), 21–33. 10.1016/j.jaac.2013.09.017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Corbett BA, Constantine LJ, Hendren R, Rocke D, & Ozonoff S (2009). Examining executive functioning in children with autism spectrum disorder, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and typical development. Psychiatry Research, 166(2–3), 210–222. 10.1016/j.psychres.2008.02.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Daniels AM, Rosenberg RE, Anderson C, Law JK, Marvin AR, & Law PA (2012). Verification of parent-report of child autism spectrum disorder diagnosis to a web-based autism registry. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(2), 257–265. 10.1007/s10803-011-1236-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. de Vries M, & Geurts H (2015). Influence of Autism Traits and Executive Functioning on Quality of Life in Children with an Autism Spectrum Disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45(9), 2734–2743. 10.1007/s10803-015-2438-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Delis DC, Kaplan E, & Kramer JH (2001). Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS). The Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
  27. Demetriou EA, Lampit A, Quintana DS, Naismith SL, Song YJC, Pye JE, Hickie I, & Guastella AJ (2018). Autism spectrum disorders: A meta-analysis of executive function. Molecular Psychiatry, 23(5), 1198–1204. 10.1038/mp.2017.75 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Diamond A (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 135–168. 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Dickson KS, Aarons GA, Anthony LG, Kenworthy L, Crandal BR, Williams K, & Brookman-Frazee L (2020). Adaption and pilot implementation of an autism executive functioning intervention in children’s mental health services: A mixed-methods study protocol. Pilot and Feasibility Studies, 6(1), 1–13. 10.1186/s40814-020-00593-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Dijkhuis R, de Sonneville L, Ziermans T, Staal W, & Swaab H (2020). Autism Symptoms, Executive Functioning and Academic Progress in Higher Education Students. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 50(4), 1353–1363. 10.1007/s10803-019-04267-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Duncan A, Tamm L, Birnschein AM, & Becker SP (2018). Clinical correlates of sluggish cognitive tempo in adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Autism, 23(6), 1354–1362. 10.1177/1362361318811329 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Duvekot J, van der Ende J, Verhulst FC, & Greaves-Lord K (2017). Examining bidirectional effects between the autism spectrum disorder (ASD) core symptom domains and anxiety in children with ASD. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 10.1111/jcpp.12829 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Ehrenreich-May J, Rosenfield D, Queen AH, Kennedy SM, Remmes CS, & Barlow DH (2017). An initial waitlist-controlled trial of the unified protocol for the treatment of emotional disorders in adolescents. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 46, 46–55. 10.1016/j.janxdis.2016.10.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Elias R, Murphy HG, Turner KA, & White SW (2019). Feasibility and Preliminary Efficacy of an Adapted Transdiagnostic Treatment for Children With Executive Function Deficits. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 33(4), 343–356. 10.1891/0889-8391.33.4.343 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. England-Mason G (2020). Emotion Regulation as a Transdiagnostic Feature in Children with Neurodevelopmental Disorder. Current Developmental Disorders Reports, 7(4), 130–138. 10.1007/s40474-020-00200-2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  36. Faja S, & Nelson Darling L (2018). Variation in restricted and repetitive behaviors and interests relates to inhibitory control and shifting in children with autism spectrum disorder. Autism, 23(5), 1262–1272. 10.1177/1362361318804192 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Ferguson HJ, Brunsdon VEA, & Bradford EEF (2021). The developmental trajectories of executive function from adolescence to old age. Scientific Reports, 11(1), 1–17. 10.1038/s41598-020-80866-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Freeman LMM, Locke J, Rotheram-Fuller E, & Mandell D (2017). Brief report: Examining executive and social functioning in elementary-aged children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 47(6), 1890–1895. 10.1007/s10803-017-3079-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Geurts HM, de Vries M, & van den Bergh SFWM (2014). Executive Functioning Theory and Autism. In Goldstein S & Naglieri JA (Eds.), Handbook of Executive Functioning (pp. 121–141). Springer; New York. 10.1007/978-1-4614-8106-5_8 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  40. Geurts HM, van den Bergh SFWM, & Ruzzano L (2014). Prepotent response inhibition and interference control in autism spectrum disorders: Two Meta-Analyses. Autism Research, 7(4), 407–420. 10.1002/aur.1369 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Gotham K, Bishop SL, Brunwasser S, & Lord C (2014). Rumination and Perceived Impairment Associated With Depressive Symptoms in a Verbal Adolescent–Adult ASD Sample. Autism Research, 7, 381–391. 10.1002/aur.1377 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Gotham KO, Siegle GJ, Han GT, Tomarken AJ, Crist RN, Simon DM, & Bodfish JW (2018). Pupil response to social-emotional material is associated with rumination and depressive symptoms in adults with autism spectrum disorder. Plos One, 13(8), e0200340. 10.1371/journal.pone.0200340 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Gratz KL & Roemer L (2004). Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS). Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 26, 41–54. 10.1023/B:JOBA.0000007455.08539.94 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Happé F, Booth R, Charlton R, & Hughes C (2006). Executive function deficits in autism spectrum disorders and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Examining profiles across domains and ages. Brain and Cognition, 61(1), 25–39. 10.1016/j.bandc.2006.03.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Hartmann K, Urbano MR, Raffaele CT, Qualls LR, Williams TV, Warren C, Kreiser NL, Elkins DE, & Deutsch SI (2019). Sexuality in the Autism Spectrum Study ( SASS ): Reports from Young Adults and Parents. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 49(9), 3638–3655. 10.1007/s10803-019-04077-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Hofmann W, Schmeichel BJ, & Baddeley AD (2012). Executive functions and self-regulation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(3), 174–180. 10.1016/j.tics.2012.01.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Holler K, Kavanaugh B, & Cook NE (2014). Executive Functioning in Adolescent Depressive Disorders. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 23(8), 1315–1324. 10.1007/s10826-013-9789-z [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  48. Hollocks MJ, Charman T, Baird G, Lord C, Pickles A, & Simonoff E (2021). Exploring the impact of adolescent cognitive inflexibility on emotional and behavioural problems experienced by autistic adults. Autism, 136236132110461. 10.1177/13623613211046160 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Hollocks MJ, Jones CRG, Pickles A, Baird G, Happé F, Charman T, & Simonoff E (2014). The Association Between Social Cognition and Executive Functioning and Symptoms of Anxiety and Depression in Adolescents With Autism Spectrum Disorders. Autism Research, 7(2), 216–228. 10.1002/aur.1361 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Hollocks MJ, Lerh JW, Magiati I, Meiser-Stedman R, & Brugha TS (2019). Anxiety and depression in adults with autism spectrum disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine, 49(4), 559–572. 10.1017/S0033291718002283 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Jahromi LB, Bryce CI, & Swanson J (2013). The importance of self-regulation for the school and peer engagement of children with high-functioning autism. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 7(2), 235–246. 10.1016/j.rasd.2012.08.012 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  52. Karalunas SL, Hawkey E, Gustofsson H, Miller M, Langhorst M, Cordova M, Fair D, & Nigg JT (2018). Overlapping and distinct cognitive impairments in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity and Autism Spectrum Disorder without Intellectual Disability. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 46(8), 1705–1716. 10.1007/s10802-017-0394-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Karoly P (1993). Mechanisms of self-regulation: A systems view. Annual Review of Psychology, 44, 23–52. [Google Scholar]
  54. Keenan EG, Gotham K, & Lerner MD (2017). Hooked on a feeling: Repetitive cognition and internalizing symptomatology in relation to autism spectrum symptomatology. Autism, 136236131770960. 10.1177/1362361317709603 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Kennedy SM, Bilek EL, & Ehrenreich-May J (2019). A Randomized Controlled Pilot Trial of the Unified Protocol for Transdiagnostic Treatment of Emotional Disorders in Children. Behavior Modification, 43(3), 330–360. 10.1177/0145445517753940 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Kenny L, Cribb SJ, & Pellicano E (2019). Childhood Executive Function Predicts Later Autistic Features and Adaptive Behavior in Young Autistic People: a 12-Year Prospective Study. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 47(6), 1089–1099. 10.1007/s10802-018-0493-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Kenworthy L, Anthony LG, Naiman DQ, Wills MC, Luong-tran C, Werner MA, Katie C, Strang J, Bal E, Sokoloff JL, & Wallace GL (2014). Randomized Controlled Effectiveness Trial of Executive Function Invention for Children on the Autism Spectrum. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 55(4), 374–383. 10.1111/jcpp.12161 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Kenworthy L, Childress D, Armour AC, Verbalis A, Zhang A, Troxel M, Handsman R, Kocher K, Myrick Y, Werner M, Alexander KC, Cannon L, & Anthony LG (2022). Leveraging technology to make parent training more accessible: Randomized trial of in-person versus online executive function training for parents of autistic children. Autism. 10.1177/13623613221111212 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Kenworthy L, Yerys BE, Anthony LG, & Wallace GL (2008). Understanding executive control in autism spectrum disorders in the lab and in the real world. Neuropsychological Review, 18(4), 320–338. 10.1007/s11065-008-9077-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Khor AS, Melvin GA, Reid SC, & Gray KM (2014). Coping, daily hassles and behavior and emotional problems in adolescents with high-functioning autism / Asperger’s disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44, 593–608. 10.1007/s10803-013-1912-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Kreslins A, Robertson AE, & Melville C (2015). The effectiveness of psychosocial interventions for anxiety in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health, 9(1), 22. 10.1186/s13034-015-0054-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Lai CLE, Lau Z, Lui SSY, Lok E, Tam V, Chan Q, Cheng KM, Lam SM, & Cheung EFC (2017). Meta-analysis of neuropsychological measures of executive functioning in children and adolescents with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research, 10(5), 911–939. 10.1002/aur.1723 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Lawson RA, Papadakis AA, Higginson CI, Barnett JE, Wills MC, Strang JF, Wallace GL, & Kenworthy L (2015). Everyday Executive Function Impairments Predict Comorbid Psychopathology in Autism Spectrum and Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorders. Neuropsychology, 29(3), 445–453. 10.1037/neu0000145 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Lecavalier L, McCracken CE, Aman MG, McDougle CJ, McCracken JT, Tierney E, Smith T, Johnson C, King B, Handen B, Swiezy NB, Eugene Arnold L, Bearss K, Vitiello B, & Scahill L (2019). An exploration of concomitant psychiatric disorders in children with autism spectrum disorder. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 88, 57–64. 10.1016/j.comppsych.2018.10.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Lee DJ, Witte TK, Bardeen JR, Davis MT, & Weathers FW (2016). A Factor Analytic Evaluation of the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 72(9), 933–946. 10.1002/jclp.22297 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Lei J, Ashwin C, Brosnan M, & Russell A (2020). Differences in anxieties and social networks in a group-matched sample of autistic and typically developing students transitioning to university. Autism, 24(5), 1138–1151. 10.1177/1362361319894830 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Lord C, Rutter M, DiLavore PC, Risi S, Gotham K, &, & Bishop SL (2012). Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-2. Western Psychological Services. [Google Scholar]
  68. Maddox BB, Rump KM, Stahmer AC, Suhrheinrich J, Rieth SR, Nahmias AS, Nuske HJ, Reisinger EM, Crabbe SR, Bronstein B, & Mandell DS (2020). Concordance between a U.S. Educational Autism Classification and the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 49(4), 469–475. 10.1080/15374416.2019.1567345 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Mazefsky CA, Borue X, Day TN, & Minshew NJ (2014). Emotion regulation patterns in adolescents with high functioning autism spectrum disorder: Comparison to typically developing adolescents and association with psychiatric symptoms. Autism Research, 7(3), 344–354. 10.1002/aur.1366 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Mazefsky CA, Herrington J, Siegel M, Scarpa A, Maddox BB, Scahill L, & White SW (2013). The role of emotion regulation in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 52(7), 679–688. 10.1016/j.jaac.2013.05.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. McCauley JB, Elias R, & Lord C (2020). Trajectories of co-occurring psychopathology symptoms in autism from late childhood to adulthood. Developmental Psychopathology, 32(4), 1287–1302. 10.1017/S0954579420000826 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. McVey AJ, Schiltz HK, Coffman M, Antezana L, & Magnus B (2021). A Preliminary Psychometric Analysis of the Difficulties with Emotion Regulation Scale ( DERS ) Among Autistic Adolescents and Adults : Factor Structure, Reliability, and Validity. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, in press. 10.1007/s10803-021-05018-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Miyake A, & Friedman NP (2012). The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: Four general conclusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(1), 8–14. 10.1177/0963721411429458 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Monsell S (2003). Task switching. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(3), 134–140. 10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00028-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Northrup JB, Goodwin MS, Montrenes J, Vezzoli J, Golt J, Peura CB, Siegel M, & Mazefsky CA (2020). Observed emotional reactivity in response to frustration tasks in psychiatrically hospitalized youth with autism spectrum disorder. Autism, 24(4), 968–982. 10.1177/1362361320908108 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Pennington BF, & Ozonoff S (1996). Executive functions and developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 37(1), 51–87. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1996.tb01380.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Picci G, & Scherf KS (2015). A Two-Hit Model of Autism: Adolescence as the Second Hit. Clinical Psychological Science, 3(3), 349–371. 10.1177/2167702614540646 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Pugliese CE, Anthony LG, Strang JF, Dudley K, Wallace GL, Naiman DQ, & Kenworthy L (2016). Longitudinal Examination of Adaptive Behavior in Autism Spectrum Disorders: Influence of Executive Function. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(2), 467–477. 10.1007/s10803-015-2584-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Reinvall O, Kujala T, Voutilainen A, Moisio AL, Lahti-Nuuttila P, & Laasonen M (2017). Sluggish cognitive tempo in children and adolescents with higher functioning autism spectrum disorders: Social impairments and internalizing symptoms. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 58(5), 389–399. 10.1111/sjop.12379 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Ritschel LA, Guy L, & Maddox BB (2021). A pilot study of dialectical behaviour therapy skills training for autistic adults. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 1–16. 10.1017/s1352465821000370 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Roberts RJJ, & Pennington BF (1996). An interactive framework for examining prefrontal cognitive processes. Developmental Neuropsychology, 21(1), 105–126. 10.1080/87565649609540642 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  82. Rodgers J, Farquhar K, Mason D, Brice S, Wigham S, Ingham B, Freeston M, & Parr JR (2020). Development and Initial Evaluation of the Anxiety Scale for Autism-Adults. Autism in Adulthood, 2(1), 24–33. 10.1089/aut.2019.0044 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Ros R, & Graziano PA (2019). A Transdiagnostic Examination of Self-Regulation: Comparisons Across Preschoolers with ASD, ADHD, and Typically Developing Children. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 0(0), 1–16. 10.1080/15374416.2019.1591280 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Rosen TE, Mazefsky CA, Vasa RA, & Lerner MD (2018). Co-occurring psychiatric conditions in autism spectrum disorder. International Review of Psychiatry, 30(1), 40–61. 10.1080/09540261.2018.1450229 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Rosenthal M, Wallace GL, Lawson R, Wills MC, Dixon E, Yerys BE, & Kenworthy L (2013). Impairments in real-world executive function increase from childhood to adolescence in autism spectrum disorders. Neuropsychology, 27(1), 13–18. 10.1037/a0031299 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Samson AC, Hardan AY, Lee IA, Phillips JM, & Gross JJ (2015). Maladaptive Behavior in Autism Spectrum Disorder: The Role of Emotion Experience and Emotion Regulation. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45(11), 3424–3432. 10.1007/s10803-015-2388-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Samson AC, Phillips JM, Parker KJ, Shah S, Gross JJ, & Hardan AY (2014). Emotion Dysregulation and the Core Features of Autism Spectrum Disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44(7), 1766–1772. 10.1007/s10803-013-2022-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Semrud-Clikeman M, Walkowiak J, Wilkinson A, & Butcher B (2010). Executive functioning in children with Asperger syndrome, ADHD-combined type, ADHD-predominately inattentive type, and controls. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(8), 1017–1027. 10.1007/s10803-010-0951-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Simonoff E, Pickles A, Charman T, Chandler S, Loucas T, & Baird G (2008). Psychiatric disorders in children with autism spectrum disorders: prevalence, comorbidity, and associated factors in a population-derived sample. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 47(8), 921–929. 10.1097/CHI.0b013e318179964f [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Smith IC, Ollendick TH, & White SW (2019). Anxiety moderates the influence of ASD severity on quality of life in adults with ASD. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 62(3), 39–47. 10.1016/j.rasd.2019.03.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  91. Storch EA, Wood JJ, Guzick AG, Small BJ, Kerns CM, Ordaz DL, Schneider SC, & Kendall PC (2021). Moderators of Response to Personalized and Standard Care Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy for Youth with Autism Spectrum Disorder and Comorbid Anxiety. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 52(2), 950–958. 10.1007/s10803-021-05000-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Thompson RA (1994). Emotion Regulation: A Theme in Search of Definition. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59(2/3), 25–52. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Tonizzi I, Giofrè D, & Usai MC (2021). Inhibitory Control in Autism Spectrum Disorders: Meta-analyses on Indirect and Direct Measures. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 10.1007/s10803-021-05353-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Uddin LQ (2021). Brain Mechanisms Supporting Flexible Cognition and Behavior in Adolescents With Autism Spectrum Disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 89(2), 172–183. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2020.05.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Uljarević M, Richdale AL, Evans DW, Cai RY, & Leekam SR (2017). Interrelationship between insistence on sameness, effortful control and anxiety in adolescents and young adults with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Molecular Autism, 8(1), 11–14. 10.1186/s13229-017-0158-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Verté S, Geurts HM, Roeyers H, Oosterlaan J, & Sergeant J. a. (2006). Executive functioning in children with an Autism Spectrum Disorder: can we differentiate within the spectrum? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(3), 351–372. 10.1007/s10803-006-0074-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Vogan VM, Leung RC, Safar K, Martinussen R, Smith M Lou, & Taylor MJ (2018). Longitudinal examination of everyday executive functioning in children with ASD: Relations with social, emotional, and behavioral functioning over time. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1–11. 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01774 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Wallace GL, Kenworthy L, Pugliese CE, Popal HS, White EI, Brodsky E, & Martin A (2016). Real-world executive functions in adults with autism spectrum disorder: Profiles of impairment and associations with adaptive functioning and co-morbid anxiety and depression. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(3), 1071–1083. 10.1007/s10803-015-2655-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Wechsler D (2011). Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence, Second Edition (WASI-II). (2nd ed.). NCS Pearson. [Google Scholar]
  100. Williams ZJ, Everaert J, & Gotham KO (2021). Measuring Depression in Autistic Adults: Psychometric Validation of the Beck Depression Inventory–II. Assessment, 28(3), 858–876. 10.1177/1073191120952889 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Williams ZJ, McKenney EE, & Gotham KO (2021). Investigating the structure of trait rumination in autistic adults: A network analysis. Autism, 25(7), 2048–2063. 10.1177/13623613211012855 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Witwer AN, & Lecavalier L (2010). Validity of comorbid psychiatric disorders in youngsters with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 22(4), 367–380. 10.1007/s10882-010-9194-0 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  103. Wood JJ, Kendall PC, Wood KS, Kerns CM, Seltzer M, Small BJ, Lewin AB, & Storch EA (2020). Cognitive Behavioral Treatments for Anxiety in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Randomized Clinical Trial. JAMA Psychiatry, 77(5), 473–484. 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2019.4160 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Zelazo PD, & Carlson SM (2012). Hot and Cool Executive Function in Childhood and Adolescence: Development and Plasticity. Child Development Perspectives, 6(4), 354–360. 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2012.00246.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  105. Zimmerman D, Ownsworth T, O’Donovan A, Roberts J, & Gullo MJ (2017). Associations between executive functions and mental health outcomes for adults with autism spectrum disorder. Psychiatry Research, 253, 360–363. 10.1016/j.psychres.2017.04.023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES