Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2023 Feb 24;18(2):e0282243. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0282243

Creating a foundation for origin of life outreach: How scientists relate to their field, the public, and religion

Karl Wienand 1,2,*, Lorenz Kampschulte 2, Wolfgang M Heckl 1,2
Editor: Margaret Williams3
PMCID: PMC9956591  PMID: 36827404

Abstract

Origins of life research is particularly challenging to communicate because of the tension between its many disciplines and its nearness to traditionally philosophical or religious questions. To authentically represent scientists’ perspective in a museum exhibition, we interviewed 46 researchers from diverse backgrounds. We investigated how they perceive their field, science communication, and the relation with religion. Results show that researchers actively participate in resolving the scientific debate, but delegate the resolution of controversies involving non-scientific institutions. Advocating for science is the foremost communication goal in all contexts. Career stage, research subject, religiosity, etc. influence the approach to controversies and communication.

Introduction

The research on the origins of life (OoL) naturally touches on questions like “How did life come to be?” or “Where do we come from?”. These issues have historically been the purview of philosophy and religious traditions. OoL research bring them to the natural sciences, spanning disciplines as diverse as physics, chemistry, biology, astronomy, and materials science. This diversity is both a blessing and a curse for the field. On the one hand, it fosters holistic and creative solutions; on the other, it deepens decade-old divides between approaches, focus points, and hypotheses originating from different fields [1].

We can picture the interaction between OoL research and society as a triangle (see Fig 1), connecting research, science communication, and religion. Despite a keen interest from the public, OoL can prove challenging for outreach. For instance, OoL research seldom focuses on applications, which would ease the communication with laypeople [25]. Moreover, the controversies in the field often revolve around relatively technical details. At the same time, OoL addresses existential, age-old questions of wide interest. Adding to the field’s appeal, these questions may remain forever unsolved. At this junction, religion [6, 7] comes into play. Most religions and mythologies in the world include creation stories, which recount how the world, life, and human beings came to be [8].

Fig 1. The triangle of mutual social dependencies between OoL research, science communication, and religion.

Fig 1

These three aspects contribute to connect OoL to society and relate to each other. In this work we address how scientists approach these topics and their mutual dependencies.

The coexistence of scientific and religious narratives is a controversial topic in science communication. For some, these worlds can exist side-by-side, but for others, research and religion inevitably clash [911]. The very framing of the question drives the nature of the ensuing discussion [12], which can have wide societal ramifications, as the debate on creationism shows [1216]. The scientific community has differentiated relationships with religion, much like the general public. Scientific communities are typically more secularized than the general population. For example 30–39% of Western-European researchers identify with “some religious affiliation” [17, 18]. 30–37% of scientists identify as non-believers or atheists, and an additional 10–28% as agnostic (with wide geographical differences) [17, 19]. In comparison, between 16% and 48% of Western-Europeans identify as non-affiliated (with wide differences between countries, median 24% [20]). In the EU overall, 27% of citizens identify as atheist, agnostic or non-believers [21]. However, like the general population, the scientific community presents a range of attitudes towards religious belief, spanning from peaceful separation [9] to acceptance, to full-on opposition [17, 18, 22].

With this work, we want to shed light on how OoL scientists navigate the relations between their research, its public communication, and religious beliefs. Concretely, we address three issues:

How do researchers see the controversy within the field of OoL research;

What approach do they prefer when communicating to the public;

How do they see–and wish to communicate–the controversy between science and religion in the context of OoL.

The sample population for this study are the researchers of the Munich-based Collaborative Research Center (CRC) 235 “Emergence of life”. The CRC is a highly interdisciplinary research network, bringing together scientists with diverse backgrounds to cooperatively work on different hypotheses and approaches to OoL research.

The results are presented in three parts: In the first we analyze the views that researchers have on the scientific controversy. In the second we analyze how scientists prefer to communicate the scientific controversy. In the last part, we focus on how scientists relate to religion and how that influences their communication patterns.

Methods and sample

The data for this study originates from qualitative interviews conducted with members of the Collaborative Research Center (CRC) 235 “Emergence of life” between September 2019 and May 2020 as preparation for a planned museum exhibition about the emergence of life. Of 52 members of the CRC initially contacted (i.e., the entire network), six did not respond or declined the interview. Each workgroup/lab is represented in the survey by at least one member. Although the sample originates from only one research cluster, this is one of the largest on OoL research worldwide and is highly diverse in terms of professions, research backgrounds and hypothesis being followed. Therefore, while the study is not representative of the entire OoL field, it encompasses a significant slice of the population and offers comprehensive insights into the diverse mindset of OoL researchers. The final sample comprises 46 scientists at different career stages: 29 graduate students, 2 postdoctoral researchers, 2 non-professor faculty, and 13 professors. In terms of gender, 15 respondents identify as female, 31 as male. Most interviewees are German citizens, 13 have non-German citizenships (including one German-French binational) from a variety of countries in Europe, the Middle East, East Asia, and North America. 12 participants identified as atheist, 9 as agnostic, 14 as believing in some form of universal justice or spiritual force, 11 as affiliated to some denomination of Christianity: either as nominally affiliated, sporadically practicing, or regularly attending functions. The interviewees raised in religions different from Christianity no longer identify with any affiliation.

All 46 interviews were carried out in English, in which all participants are fluent (two native speakers). Most interviews (37) took place in person at the researchers’ institutions, while nine were videocalls. All participants agreed to the recording of the interview and the anonymous collection of data. They were informed about the uses of the interview contents. Verbal consent was recorded in the interviews. The interview audios were transcribed and subsequently analyzed with the inductive method of qualitative analysis [23] to identify emerging topics. The categorization was carried out by one of the authors (KW) for all interviews, and by a second author (LK, independently) on a random sample (25%). Disagreements were negotiated until consensus was reached and applied uniformly to the remaining interviews.

Approval from an ethics committee was not sought for this study, as neither of the competent institutions (the Deutsches Museum and the TUM School of Social Sciences and Technology) had an appropriate council at the beginning of data collection. Furthermore, our study presented no risk of harm or injury for the participants, who were not subject to physical treatments, procedures or tests. The mental labor by the interviewees was limited to a conversation, in which they answered questions about their own perspective and experience on the field. No question was directed to the recall of negative memories. The extent to which to disclose such experiences, if at all, was entirely at the interviewee’s discretion. Interviewees were also offered to opt out of any questions they would rather answer or not answer in full. Interviewees were all older than 18 and participated on an exclusively volunteer and non-compensated basis.

Results

In the following sections we analyze the answers from the 46 researchers in our sample. First we focus on how they view the scientific controversies in the origins of life (OoL) field. Then on their approach to communicate these controversies. Finally we turn our attention to their attitude towards the relations between science and religion.

Views about the scientific controversy

Most surveyed researchers perceive the scientific discussion about the origins of life (OoL) as conflictual. Table 1 presents the themes that emerged from the interviews regarding the controversy. Each theme was divided in classes of answers, shown in order of descending frequency. S1 Table presents more comprehensive and precise class definitions, including the specific rules that outline the boundaries of each class.

Table 1. Scientists’ views about the scientific controversy.

Theme Class description Exemplary quote
Conflict actors Groups or institutions collectively There’s definitely teams. (23)
Single individuals It’s always a history of person. And if two persons […] respect themselves, [the discussion] will be constructive, even though they have different opinions. (27)
Own role Determine which side is right I can, hopefully very objectively, decide if a process is possible or not. (35)
Provide information [W]hoever finally finds out which theory is the right one […], I hope that I contributed enough information for the final answer. (28)
Defend or advance a specific side I hope that I would […], excite people on the main road to try out the side road and widen their theoretical concept. (19)
Actively try to defuse conflict My role would be a mediator […] I do think that I have input on how that discussion should be held. (9)
Cause of conflict Active (willing) violation of rules or boundaries You shouldn’t be dogmatic about your research. And I think that’s a lesson we have to learn. (8)
Natural part of the process or of the actors involved That’s how we go from one point to more knowledge, right? To challenge the views, to challenge the beliefs, to challenge the established science. (21)
There is no conflict I went to several meetings where these questions were discussed […] and there the discussion was very open. (15)
Lack of information [T]he chemists or the physicists, they think other about things. (37)

Only 9 of the 46 interviewees (20%) describe the scientific controversy as a competition between equally valid ideas, integral to the scientific sense-making process. For most the controversy is far from this Mertonian ideal [24]. According to many, the conflict stems from “dogmatic”, “close-minded” attitudes, not rooted in “science”. “Most of the discussion is really not an open and scientific discussion in the way it should be: people are in their little camps, and then they shoot to the other camp and tell them that they’re doing stupid research.” (19, Professor) Similar violations appear in as many as 33 interviews (69%), for 28 of which they are the most cited cause of conflict.

For some, the conflictual nature of the controversy traces back to single individuals. In Table 1, for example, the interviewee finds the people involved to be key for how the discussion unfolds (“it is always a history of person”). Conflict ensues only when people who do not “respect [each other]” are involved. More frequently (48% of cases), interviewees found the conflict to be driven by groups—sorted by hypothesis preference, discipline, or personal allegiance. How the views about conflict actors correlate with those on the causes or the nature of the conflict is an interesting avenue of research. Our data, however, were insufficient to allow us to draw solid conclusions in that regard.

Almost half of the interviewed scientists (22) think that the problem is decreasing. Without any explicit prompt, they said that the most conflictual aspects are waning, thanks to the generational change and the increasing interdisciplinarity of the field: “So in some conferences … people are really shouting at each other… But sometimes I have the feeling … especially the younger people are much more open.” (24, Professor).

To better summarize how the scientists themselves approach the controversy, we used their utterances in the “Own Role” category to build two conflict profiles. The answering profile actively participates in answering the scientific question—either by promoting a specific hypothesis, or as impartial judges: “This is for me something where I can, hopefully very objectively, decide if a process is possible or not.” (35, Professor). The delegating profile, instead, delegates the decision to others, or brings the different sides to a discussion table: “I’m contributing valuable information to the topic. And whoever finally… gets the right answers, I hope that I contributed enough information.” (28, Student) S2 Table reports the exact combinations of features that define each profile.

As shown in Fig 2, the predominant approach to the controversy changes with career stage, discipline, and hypothesis preference. Junior scientists (PhD students) frequently characterize themselves as “too low-rank” to contribute and so delegate the resolution. Meanwhile, senior scientists (postdoctoral researcher and higher) lean towards the answering profile. Tradition and gatekeeping could, in a similar vein, underlie the high fraction of answering approaches observed among chemists. This discipline, in fact, has the longest-standing controversies in the field (which contributes to more entrenched positions). As a result, chemistry is perceived as the dominant discipline in the field, sometimes excluding others: “I feel people are very focused on, ‘If you’re not a chemist, then you don’t really know what you’re doing.’ Or, ‘If you’re not a chemist, then I don’t believe in a lot of things you say anyway’.” (23, Student).

Fig 2. Controversy profiles frequency.

Fig 2

Prevalence of the answering (orange) and delegating (blue) controversy profiles, when segmenting the sample by discipline, hypothesis preference, and career stage of the researchers (n = 45).

Communicating the scientific controversy

Table 2 shows how interviewees rated the importance of different aspects for a “good communication about the emergence of life” to a broad public, on a scale from 1 (negligible) to 6 (essential).

Table 2. Importance scientists gave to different aspects of communication about origin of life.

Aspect Mean Rating Standard Deviation
Scientific basics 5.17 1.19
Historical (“old”) theories about life and its origins 4.20 1.35
Methods and processes of research 3.84 1.40
Relationship to religion and philosophy 3.61 1.54
Everyday work of a researcher 3.46 1.41
Relationship to potential applications (e.g. industrial chemistry, synthetic biology) 3.37 1.57

Mean and standard deviation of ratings given by scientists during the interviews. Ratings were expressed on a Likert scale from 1 (negligible) to 6 (essential). N = 46

“Scientific basics” clearly have the highest rating. Because the survey was part of the preparations for a museum exhibition, the scientists might have been slightly biased in answering this question. Nevertheless, this singular focus on scientific basics also fits an emerging pattern. Indeed, other aspects are included, but subordinated to the goal of filling a gap in scientific knowledge. For example, historical theories serve as a good or captivating introduction to the modern view of facts. Some researchers also think about using their everyday work to engage the public and increase the acceptance of the results: “Everyday work of researchers: also super-fun. […] It can just, you know, make [the topic] stick in people’s minds” (25, Student). According to others, however, the daily grind of research may be distracting, boring, or even depressing: “Life as a researcher can be tough. And maybe people would feel a little bit down” (2, Student). As a result, this authenticity element scores quite low. A similar ambivalence underlies the low scores for potential applications: “In a sense, it’s not important, but there will always be people who say, ‘What is it good for?’ [But when] you say, ‘Well, if we create an artificial cell then you can also use it to make detergents’ Maybe not” (41, Professor).

Table 3 presents the themes that emerged in interviews regarding the communication of Origin of Life (OoL) topics to a broad public. Each theme is divided in classes, shown in descending order of frequency. In this case, too, S1 Table collects more detailed definitions of each class.

Table 3. Views about communication to a broad public.

Theme Class description Exemplary quote
Public attitude Interested in topic or favors research They’re super interested. They really want to know. (20)
Hostile towards research or topic I know that a lot of people are opposed to [our work]. (10)
Controversy or topic unknown to public Over 95% of the population probably has no idea [the field] exists. (9)
Neutral towards research I think they would, in the first place, do not understand why we do [this research]. (28)
Target audience Undifferentiated target audience Everyone, everyone, really. So from kids to grandmas. (4)
Single, uniform target segment [P]eople who go to a museum and are interested in a little bit of science. (28)
Differentiated targets Everybody that goes to a presentation that is called "Origin of Life". And, yeah, also particularly children. (5)
Prior knowledge Insufficient information or education I think that many […] are lacking basics […], when they hear ‘proteins’, they think about going to the gym. (15)
Public informed enough If they’re not curious to know, I think it probably is enough for them. (23)
Public has wrong information [A] certain part of community likes to write books. And then people read those books, and they believe this is the consensus in the field. (8)
Communication Model Communication aims at filling knowledge gap [M]ost of the people […] do not know much about [OoL]. And it should be changed at some point, definitely. (18)
Two-way information flow Of course, they will care about [the research process]. Like they will ask you, "How did you sample it? How did you take your samples?" (6)
Cooperative sense-making process I would totally argue with […] showing that there is […] disagreement, and then let them take sides. And let’s see what […] comes from stakeholders on the street, […] what could it be important aspects. (19)
Own role Provide only interesting or understandable parts It’s a nice story you can tell. […] It’s easy to introduce the basic and to build […] up. (1)
Promote institution It’s kind of embarrassing to show to the public how researchers—which are thought to be the authority on knowledge—are fighting over if this molecule is prebiotic or not. (30)
Present all facts as transparently as possible We should always make clear, what are the scientific grounds? And where are we leaving the facts? […] What are the conclusions? […] What conclusions are […] just speculative? (19)
Patrol boundaries To present this field to the public, I think it’s important to stay within sciences that do use the scientific method and do not take just assumptions. (22)

We combined the statements of interviewees in the “Own role” and “Communication Model” theme to sort their communication approaches in one of three profiles (S3 Table for details). The teaching profile centers around the traditional deficit model. Teaching communicators unidirectionally transmit knowledge about scientific facts and the nature of science. “This notion of scientists only making statements that are based given some assumptions: it’s very important that people realize that” (16, Postdoc). The advocating profile aims at defending the institution of science, increasing its acceptance, or promoting the image of science and scientists. “It’s not dangerous, we’re not trying to clone or something” (15, Student). The discussing profile is less unidirectional. Discussing communicators report facts about the controversy for the sake of transparency or duty. “I think it will be nice to see the several approaches and where they’re also heading to each other… And if you can make it some summary for every step of life that could be [based on] several approaches” (14, Student).

As Fig 3(A) shows, advocating is the most prevalent communication profile (21 interviewees), followed by teaching (10), while discussing is rather rare (3). 13 scientists did not clearly fit any of the profiles, thus were not taken into account in this analysis. Looking at the demographic breakdown, teaching is particularly prevalent among junior scientists, chemists, and those expressing a preference for any of the prevailing hypotheses for the origins of life (OoL). More senior scientists and physicists, instead, overwhelmingly tend towards advocating.

Fig 3. Communication profiles prevalence and view of the public (n = 33).

Fig 3

(a) Prevalence of the teaching (yellow), advocating (green), and discussing (purple) profiles when segmenting the sample by career stage, field, and hypothesis preference of the researchers; (b) View of public attitude towards OoL (brown series), target public (blue series), and public’s prior knowledge of OoL (green series) seen by all scientists (overall), as well as by the different communication profiles.

Fig 3(B) shows that different communication profiles are associated to different views about the public. Most interviewees—particularly in the advocating and discussing profiles—envision a public composed of one or more well-defined segments. The segments they mention reflect the typical audiences of informal scientific learning. One common example is visitors of science museums, families, and kids (clearly influenced by the exhibition activity connected to the interviews). Another example are people that the researchers meet at their institutions’ open days or at public lectures.

The interviewees fitting the teaching profile stand out from the average in their views of the public. For one, the majority imagines a generic, undifferentiated target public. Moreover, they more often expect the public to be wrongly informed about OoL research and to have a negative attitude towards it.

S1 Fig in the supplementary material also shows that different profiles prioritize different content in their communication. Teaching communicators, for example, focus more than average on history, scientific basics, and everyday life, while leaving methods and applications in the background. On the opposite end of the spectrum, advocating communicators prioritize relation to religion, methods, and applications more than their colleagues.

Relating to religion and communicating the controversy

Origins of life (OoL), more than other research topics, shares a discussion space with non-scientific approaches, like philosophy and religion. This brings to the fore how scientists relate to belief.

We analyzed the interview contents using the same rules and classes deployed in the above sections (see Tables 1, 2 and S1S3 Tables). Table 4 shows the prevalent answer classes in the themes that emerged discussing the scientists’ views on dealing with the science-religion controversy and how to communicate about it. Most interviewees view science and religion as strictly independent and separated, although not intrinsically in conflict. Those that see conflict take the side of science: “There’s a lot of conflict, but there doesn’t have to be. If there were to be no conflict, it would be religion who would have to accommodate” (9, Student). Almost one third of the interviewees (28%) said—unprompted—that they experienced or imagine the conflict being more pronounced in the United States or countries with a strong Islamic influence than it is in the Germany and Western Europe.

Table 4. Scientists’ views about the science-religion relationship and its communication.

Theme Class description Exemplary quote
Conflict actors Groups or institutions collectively Religion’s really not the promoter of science. […], there will be conflicts, definitely. (34)
Single individuals I think in some people beliefs are very contradicting, but I think […] they can complement each other. (17)
Own role in conflict Advance specific side If we do our jobs well we will be able to prove one day that the origin of life didn’t have to be mystical, it didn’t have to be created by God. (9)
Provide information I think we should engage these people […] and say, “[…] Earth is four and a half billion years old. And after 3.8 billion years there was life […]” And maybe they’ll just walk away and not believe you but at least they heard it. (33)
Actively try to defuse conflict Why not to make connection with what was traditionally explained […] and what is the scientific explanation or interpretation? [A]lso science is an interpretation of reality. (40)
Determine which side is right [W]e can just generate facts and if questions come, then we have to think about it. (27)
Cause of conflict Lack of information If you think that God created the Earth in seven days […] that’s not gonna work. (11)
There is no conflict [S]cience and the beauty of mathematics, […], of how the world works, and how beautiful and how logical everything is, this is for me the best proof that there’s some God. (5)
Active (willing) violation of rules or boundaries There’s a lot of conflict, but there doesn’t have to be. […] If there were to be no conflict, it would be religion who would have to accommodate. (9)
Own role in communication Patrols boundaries between institutions I think already showing that the origin of life research is not just research to disprove that God exists. (12)
Presents all facts transparently But in principle, historically spoken, it’s. . . it’s. . . of course it’s important and one should consider and keep in mind. (32)
Promotes an institution [N]ot so much as an equivalent theory, because […] one is popular belief, and the others are being tested through the scientific method. (22)

Fig 4(A) shows the distribution of religious self-identification among scientists in our survey. The sample splits roughly equally between atheist, agnostic, spiritual (who believe in some form of universal justice or spiritual force), and religiously affiliated (ranging from nominally affiliated to sporadically practicing to regularly attending functions).

Fig 4. Religion and science in origins of life.

Fig 4

(a) Religious composition of the sample, roughly equally divided between atheists, agnostics, spiritually inclined and religiously affiliated to some degree (N = 46). (b) Prevalence of answering (orange) and delegating (blue) profiles in the science-religion controversy (n = 28). (c) How different religious segments see: the public attitude towards OoL (brown series), the target public (blue series), and the public’s prior knowledge of OoL (green series) (N = 46). (d) Prevalence of the teaching (yellow), advocating (green), and discussing (purple) profile by religiosity (n = 18).

To analyze the scientists’ attitude towards the science-religion controversy, we followed the same method as for the scientific controversy, namely creating profiles. Some interviewees fit the answering profile and actively participate in resolving the controversy. Others fit the delegating profile: they foster dialogue and delegate the conflict resolution. The delegating profile is much more prevalent in the controversy with religion (64%) than in the scientific controversy (32%). Furthermore, this profile becomes even more frequent among senior scientists. This trend is diametrically opposed to what we observed in the scientific controversy (see Fig 2).

Segmenting the sample by religiosity, we see that scientists identifying with a religious affiliation are less likely to fit the answering profile. However, the delegating profile is also prevalent among self-identified atheists. In the context of the scientific controversy, increased religiosity corresponds to a more frequent answering profile (see S2 Fig).

We also considered how interviewees communicate the controversy arising from the different value systems of science and religion. 33 of them (72%) said their communication should primarily trace boundaries between the domains, either by clarifying differences between the approaches, or pointing out limitations—sometimes with the explicit goal of preventing or defusing the conflict: “[We should show] that the origin of life research is not just research to disprove that God exists” (12, Professor) “We should always make clear: what are the scientific grounds? And where are we leaving the facts? Well, what are the conclusions? What are the possible conclusions? And what conclusions are […] just speculative?” (19, Professor) Thus the most widespread approach aligns with S.J. Gould’s “Non-Overlapping Magisteria” argument [9]. There are, however, notable exceptions. Some interviewees, for example, wanted to explicitly connect science and religion as different expressions of the same sense-making process. “They’re connected like everything that is part of our culture. Why not to make connection with what was the interpretation that was given in that field? Also science is an interpretation of reality.” (40, Postdoc).

As Fig 4(C) shows, different religiosities were associated to different communication approaches in our interviews. Religiously affiliated and spiritually inclined scientists, for example, are least likely to envision a generic, undifferentiated audience. They also see the public as informed “enough” and with a positive attitude towards OoL research. Self-identified agnostics, instead, have the most pessimistic view of the public: they are the least likely to perceive a positive public attitude or to see the public as informed “enough” about OoL. Agnostics were also the most likely to target an undifferentiated public with their communication.

Finally, we summarized the scientists’ communication approach in three profiles (Fig 4(D)): a deficit-model-based teaching profile, an advocating profile focused on promoting and defending science (none of the interviewees presented themselves as defending religion over science), and a dialogue-oriented discussing profile. Though the small sample limits the scope of conclusions, we can see atheists taking a predominantly advocating profile, whereas spiritually inclined researchers tend to the teaching profile.

Discussion

We interviewed 46 scientists from the Collaborative Research Center 235 “Emergence of Life” (CRC) to explore: (i) how they view the scientific controversies in the origins of life (OoL) field; (ii) their approach to communicate them; and (iii) their attitude towards the relations between science and religion. Although the sample encompasses members of only one research cluster, it is diverse enough to offer a snapshot of the attitudes in the field (see Methods and Sample). The most frequent approach to the OoL controversy is to actively participate in its resolution, particularly among senior scientists. The controversy with religion, meanwhile, presents the exact opposite scenario: a more cooperative and delegating approach, especially in senior scientists. The communication of both controversies aims most often to advocate for science. The specific communication profile each individual fits is also linked to their view about the target public.

Laherto et al. already observed that, when approaching the public, scientists sometimes prioritize to communicate other aspects of the field, rather than what they find important in research work [25]. Here we also see that communication and controversy profile do not appear linked. For example, the approach to controversy among chemists resembles that of senior scientists, while their communication profile is closer to that of junior scientists.

The more passive, delegating approach of junior scientists is not surprising, as they are professionally subordinate. We can therefore expect current graduate students to become more answering as they gain experience, confidence, and status. However, there is also a widespread sense (across different career stages) that the generational shift is leading the discourse towards a more constructive dialogue and away from conflict and dogmatic positions.

Our results confirm that state-of-the art science communication cannot be demanded from scientists “out of the box”. Their direct involvement is crucial for an engaging communication. However, untrained researchers have a natural tendency towards the deficit model. Experience and training in public communication counteract this trend. Our results also suggest that communication experience alone moves the needle towards advocating for science rather than to dialogic models. Therefore, specific communication training is also necessary before expecting scientists to adhere to those modern models.

The scientists surveyed here have very different communication priorities than, for example, the researchers in nanoscience and -technology (NST) surveyed in [25]. Prominently, scientific basics are the top priority for OoL researchers, but next to the bottom for nanoscientists. The scientific process was also frequently mentioned in our interviews, but had much lower priority in NST. Meanwhile, methods were often classified as too complex in our survey, and thus were given much lower priority than in surveys of nanoscientists. Finally, applications offer a curious point of comparison. Both NST and OoL researchers viewed applications as marginal to their daily work. Nevertheless, nanoscientists put applications, products, and usefulness at the top of their communication priority. OoL researchers, instead, gave applications the lowest priority rating, despite the close links of their research to potentially hot topics, such as synthetic biology.

Our sample includes a wide spectrum of religious self-identification, almost evenly split between atheists (26%), agnostics (20%), spiritual (30%), and religiously affiliated (24%). This distribution roughly aligns with comparable surveys in Europe and North America [1719], in which 30–37% of the scientists identify as atheist, 10–28% as agnostic, and 30–39% identifies with “some religious affiliation” [17, 18]. Also compared religiosity in scientists and in the general population of the same country. Consistently with those findings, we also see non-affiliation being much more prevalent in our sample than in the general (Western-) European population. In polls from 2018 and 2019, less than 30% of Europeans identified as atheist, agnostic, or generally non-believer [20, 21] whereas atheists and agnostics alone make up 46% of our sample.

Most surveyed scientists subscribe to the idea that science and religion must be kept separate as much as possible. To that end, they engage in boundary work, similarly to what was observed in previous studies [17, 22], for example so-called conciliatory boundary work [22], dividing “good” religion (secular, accommodating, and compatible with science) from “bad” religion (dogmatic, incompatible with science). “If you think that God created the Earth in seven days that’s not gonna work. But if you think that maybe God is more a force of nature or something, then yes.” (11, Student).

The interviewees’ religiosity seems to be associated with their communication approach. Religiously affiliated and spiritually inclined scientists, for example, expressed the most positive view of the public. Self-identified agnostics, instead, were the most pessimistic, more than atheists. The data gives no conclusive evidence on what underlies this difference. One could suspect that atheists are more actively engaged in the controversy, hence their profile resembles that of experienced communicators. Conversely, some of the people who view the debate negatively might identify as agnostic to avoid engaging with it.

OoL is an exciting, lively field with a strong potential to involve a diverse public within academia and outside of it. Our study portrays a considerable diversity of views, goals, and approaches. As a tool to create outreach, the study offers a snapshot to convey an authentic picture of OoL research, as well as the professional and personal views of the scientists behind it.

The following are available online at https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.14703060:

Supporting information

S1 Fig. Communication aspects priority for communication profiles.

How different communication profiles prioritize aspects of communication.

(PDF)

S2 Fig. Additional demographics for controversy and communication profiles.

Controversy and communication profiles related to the scientific controversy and the controversy with religion, segmented by field of work, hypothesis preference, seniority, religiosity, number of countries researchers lived in.

(PDF)

S1 Table. Classification rules.

Classification table with rules that defined each of the classes and profiles.

(PDF)

S2 Table. Definitions of controversy profiles.

Composition of each controversy profile in terms of most frequent answer classes.

(PDF)

S3 Table. Definitions of communication profiles.

Composition of each communication profile in terms of most frequent answer classes.

(PDF)

S1 File. Minimal dataset.

Anonymized minimal dataset to replicate all figures and conclusions.

(XLSX)

Acknowledgments

We wish to thank the members of the CRC 235 Emergence of Life for their kind and generous cooperation during the interview process and Zara Gough for language help.

Data Availability

All relevant data are within the paper.

Funding Statement

Karl Wienand was funded by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG, German Research Foundation) with Project-ID 364653263—TRR 235 The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Preiner M, Asche S, Becker S, Betts HC, Boniface A, Camprubi E, et al. The Future of Origin of Life Research: Bridging Decades-Old Divisions. Life. 2020;10: 20. doi: 10.3390/life10030020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Boholm Å, Larsson S. What is the problem? A literature review on challenges facing the communication of nanotechnology to the public. J Nanopart Res. 2019;21: 86. doi: 10.1007/s11051-019-4524-3 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Biotechnology Brossard D., communication and the public: Keys to delve into the social perception of science. Metode Science Studies Journal. 2019;0: 39–45. doi: 10.7203/metode.9.11347 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Cacciatore MA, Scheufele DA, Corley EA. From enabling technology to applications: The evolution of risk perceptions about nanotechnology. Public Underst Sci. 2011;20: 385–404. doi: 10.1177/0963662509347815 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Kurath M, Gisler P. Informing, involving or engaging? Science communication, in the ages of atom-, bio- and nanotechnology. Public Underst Sci. 2009;18: 559–573. doi: 10.1177/0963662509104723 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Abelson RP. Differences Between Belief and Knowledge Systems*. Cognitive Science. 1979;3: 355–366. doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog0304_4 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Usó-Doménech JL, Nescolarde-Selva J. What are Belief Systems? Found Sci. 2016;21: 147–152. doi: 10.1007/s10699-015-9409-z [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Leeming D. The Oxford Companion to World Mythology. Oxford University Press; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Gould SJ. Nonoverlapping Magisteria. Natural History. 1997: 16–22. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Vaidyanathan B, Johnson DR, Prickett PJ, Howard Ecklund E. Rejecting the conflict narrative: American Jewish and Muslim views on science and religion. Social Compass. 2016;63: 478–496. doi: 10.1177/0037768616664473 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Johnson DR, Ecklund EH, Di D, Matthews KRW. Responding to Richard: Celebrity and (mis)representation of science. Public Underst Sci. 2018;27: 535–549. doi: 10.1177/0963662516673501 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Elsdon-Baker F. Creating creationists: The influence of ‘issues framing’ on our understanding of public perceptions of clash narratives between evolutionary science and belief. Public Underst Sci. 2015;24: 422–439. doi: 10.1177/0963662514563015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Noy S O ’Brien TL. A Nation Divided: Science, Religion, and Public Opinion in the United States. Socius. 2016;2: 237802311665187. doi: 10.1177/2378023116651876 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Hameed S. Making sense of Islamic creationism in Europe. Public Underst Sci. 2015;24: 388–399. doi: 10.1177/0963662514555055 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Hanley P. Controversy in school?: Origin of life and the science/religion overlap. Heriot-Watt University, Edinburgh; 2008. p. 18. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Allgaier J, Holliman R. The emergence of the controversy around the theory of evolution and creationismin UK newspaper reports. The Curriculum Journal. 2006;17: 263–279. doi: 10.1080/09585170600909738 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Ecklund EH, Johnson DR, Scheitle CP, Matthews KRW, Lewis SW. Religion among Scientists in International Context: A New Study of Scientists in Eight Regions. Socius. 2016;2: 237802311666435. doi: 10.1177/2378023116664353 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Ecklund EH, Scheitle CP. Religion among Academic Scientists: Distinctions, Disciplines, and Demographics. Soc Probl. 2007;54: 289–307. doi: 10.1525/sp.2007.54.2.289 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Falcão EBM. Religious Beliefs: Their dynamics in two groups of life scientists. International Journal of Science Education. 2008;30: 1249–1264. doi: 10.1080/09500690701765863 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Pew Research Center. Being Christian in Western Europe. 2018. Available: https://www.pewresearch.org/religion/wp-content/uploads/sites/7/2018/05/Being-Christian-in-Western-Europe-FOR-WEB1.pdf [Google Scholar]
  • 21.European Commission. Discrimination in the European Union. 2019. Available: https://europa.eu/eurobarometer/api/deliverable/download/file?deliverableId=71116 [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Sorrell K, Ecklund EH. How UK Scientists Legitimize Religion and Science Through Boundary Work. Sociol Relig. 2018; 1–22. doi: 10.1093/socrel/sry047 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Mayring P, Fenzl T. Qualitative Inhaltsanalyse. In: Baur N, Blasius J, editors. Handbuch Methoden der empirischen 1Sozialforschung. Wiesbaden: Springer Fachmedien Wiesbaden; 2014. pp. 543–556. doi: 10.1007/978-3-531-18939-0_38 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Merton RK. The sociology of science: theoretical and empirical investigations. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1973. Available: http://archive.org/details/sociologyofscien0000mert [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Laherto A, Tirre F, Parchmann I, Kampschulte L, Schwarzer S. Scientists’ perceptions on the nature of nanoscience and its public communication. Problems of Education in the 21st Century. 2018;76: 43–57. [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Steve Zimmerman

12 Sep 2022

PONE-D-22-02824Creating a foundation for Origin of Life outreach: How scientists relate to their field, the public, and religionPLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Wienand,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

==============================

Please note that we have only been able to secure a single reviewer to assess your manuscript. We are issuing a decision on your manuscript at this point to prevent further delays in the evaluation of your manuscript. Please be aware that the editor who handles your revised manuscript might find it necessary to invite additional reviewers to assess this work once the revised manuscript is submitted. However, we will aim to proceed on the basis of this single review if possible.  The reviewer has made a number of suggestion for improvement (see comments below). 

Could you please carefully revise the manuscript to address all comments raised?==============================

Please submit your revised manuscript by Oct 24 2022 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Steve Zimmerman, PhD

Associate Editor, PLOS ONE

Journal requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1.Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf.

2. Please provide additional details regarding participant consent. In the ethics statement in the Methods and online submission information, please ensure that you have specified (1) whether consent was informed and (2) what type you obtained (for instance, written or verbal, and if verbal, how it was documented and witnessed). If your study included minors, state whether you obtained consent from parents or guardians. If the need for consent was waived by the ethics committee, please include this information.

If you are reporting a retrospective study of medical records or archived samples, please ensure that you have discussed whether all data were fully anonymized before you accessed them and/or whether the IRB or ethics committee waived the requirement for informed consent. If patients provided informed written consent to have data from their medical records used in research, please include this information.

3. Thank you for stating the following financial disclosure:

“Karl Wienand was funded by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG, German Research Foundation) with Project-ID 364653263—TRR 235”

Please state what role the funders took in the study.  If the funders had no role, please state: ""The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.""

If this statement is not correct you must amend it as needed.

Please include this amended Role of Funder statement in your cover letter; we will change the online submission form on your behalf.

4. Thank you for stating the following in the Acknowledgments Section of your manuscript:

“We wish to thank the members of the CRC 235 Emergence of Life for their kind and generous cooperation during the interview process and Zara Gough for language help. KW is grateful to the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG, German Research Foundation)—Project-ID 364653263—TRR 235 for funding.”

We note that you have provided funding information that is not currently declared in your Funding Statement. However, funding information should not appear in the Acknowledgments section or other areas of your manuscript. We will only publish funding information present in the Funding Statement section of the online submission form.

Please remove any funding-related text from the manuscript and let us know how you would like to update your Funding Statement. Currently, your Funding Statement reads as follows:

“Karl Wienand was funded by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG, German Research Foundation) with Project-ID 364653263—TRR 235”

Please include your amended statements within your cover letter; we will change the online submission form on your behalf.

5. In your Data Availability statement, you have not specified where the minimal data set underlying the results described in your manuscript can be found. PLOS defines a study's minimal data set as the underlying data used to reach the conclusions drawn in the manuscript and any additional data required to replicate the reported study findings in their entirety. All PLOS journals require that the minimal data set be made fully available. For more information about our data policy, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability.

Upon re-submitting your revised manuscript, please upload your study’s minimal underlying data set as either Supporting Information files or to a stable, public repository and include the relevant URLs, DOIs, or accession numbers within your revised cover letter. For a list of acceptable repositories, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-recommended-repositories. Any potentially identifying patient information must be fully anonymized.

Important: If there are ethical or legal restrictions to sharing your data publicly, please explain these restrictions in detail. Please see our guidelines for more information on what we consider unacceptable restrictions to publicly sharing data: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-unacceptable-data-access-restrictions. Note that it is not acceptable for the authors to be the sole named individuals responsible for ensuring data access.

We will update your Data Availability statement to reflect the information you provide in your cover letter.

6. We note that you have indicated that data from this study are available upon request. PLOS only allows data to be available upon request if there are legal or ethical restrictions on sharing data publicly. For more information on unacceptable data access restrictions, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-unacceptable-data-access-restrictions.

In your revised cover letter, please address the following prompts:

a) If there are ethical or legal restrictions on sharing a de-identified data set, please explain them in detail (e.g., data contain potentially sensitive information, data are owned by a third-party organization, etc.) and who has imposed them (e.g., an ethics committee). Please also provide contact information for a data access committee, ethics committee, or other institutional body to which data requests may be sent.

b) If there are no restrictions, please upload the minimal anonymized data set necessary to replicate your study findings as either Supporting Information files or to a stable, public repository and provide us with the relevant URLs, DOIs, or accession numbers. For a list of acceptable repositories, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-recommended-repositories.

We will update your Data Availability statement on your behalf to reflect the information you provide.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: I Don't Know

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: No

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: This article tackles an interesting subject. A number of revisions could improve the final version. Some are small, eg in line 47 the authors state that the majority of research on creationism has taken place in the US but they could also easily reference recent contributions on Europe. Similarly in lines 49-56 statistics are compared regarding scientists in Western Europe and the general German population- it would be better if the comparison groups were the same' ie German scientists w the German general population or Western European scientists with the general Western European population. Additionally, in lines 91-94 the religious identity of the scientists was described and it was not clear regarding those scientists of non-Christian backgrounds- there were no Muslim, Hindu or Jewish scientists? This needs some further elaboration. On the other hand it seemed that the material in lines 104-116 could be switched to a note and the sentences in lines 310-313 could be moved to an earlier section. Some of the quotations brought from the interviews were not always clear, eg Table 3, neutral towards research: "I think they would, in the first place, do not understand why we do it." There were also some occasional lines in the text that should be adjusted (e.g. line 369). The main substantive question I have is regarding the formulation of the tables. More discussion of the categories would be helpful to explain the choices made by the researchers and to describe further the contours of the typology they found (eg the examples brought for single actors vs groups were more focused on the constructive nature of differing views where as those who saw camps seemed to describe this in competitive terms-- is this significant, coincidental etc?) I look forward to seeing this further explication and expanded discussion as I am very interested in this research.

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

**********

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2023 Feb 24;18(2):e0282243. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0282243.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


24 Oct 2022

Dear Editor,

Thank you very much once again for considering my paper for publication in PLOS ONE.

Based on the Editor and Reviewer’s suggestions and comments, we were able to extensively edit the Manuscript. We clarified, improved, and extended the discussions as much as possible. Moreover we substantially expanded the supplementary materials to address the concerns of more specialized and/or interested readers.

Below our answers to each of the Reviewer’s comments.

Sincerely,

Karl Wienand

• In line 47 the authors state that the majority of research on creationism has taken place in the US but they could also easily reference recent contributions on Europe.

We thank the Reviewer for pointing this out. Indeed, limiting the discussion of creationism debates to the US environment could be reductive. We expanded the references (particularly Refs 14—16) to include the European context, as well as debates around non-Christian creationism.

• In lines 49-56 statistics are compared regarding scientists in Western Europe and the general German population- it would be better if the comparison groups were the same' ie German scientists w the German general population or Western European scientists with the general Western European population.

We thank the reviewer for the comment. Comparing more uniform data provides a better picture of secularization among scientists. We researched different sources (i.e., Refs 20, 21) to compare our sample with Western European and EU general population.

• In lines 91-94 the religious identity of the scientists was described and it was not clear regarding those scientists of non-Christian backgrounds- there were no Muslim, Hindu or Jewish scientists? This needs some further elaboration.

No, the sample happened to not include any scientists with Hindu or Jewish background. One was raised Muslim but no longer identifies as affiliated. We amended the statement for clarity, but left the part still intentionally vague to protect the participant’s anonymity.

• The material in lines 104-116 could be switched to a note

These considerations, though admittedly not necessary to replicate the study, had to be included in the Methods section following a request from PLOS ONE.

• The sentences in lines 310-313 could be moved to an earlier section.

We thank the reviewer for the suggestion. We added a similar text to the beginning of the Results section. However, we also left the original lines in place, as a quick recap for readers who may have skipped directly to the Discussion section.

• Some quotations brought from the interviews were not always clear

We thank the reviewer for the suggestion. In preparing the manuscript we had tried to leave the quotes as much as possible in their original form. As the reviewer points out, however, this caused problems, as it was sometimes unclear what the interviewees were referring to with “it”, “they”, etc. We edited quotes throughout the manuscript for clarity. All alterations to the original transcripts are denoted in square brackets. For example, the cited quote in Table 3 now reads: “I think they would, in the first place, do not understand why we do [this research].”

• Some occasional lines in the text that should be adjusted (e.g. line 369)

We thank the reviewer for the comment. We polished the entire text, of course starting from that line.

• More discussion of the categories would be helpful to explain the choices made by the researchers and to describe further the contours of the typology they found (eg the examples brought for single actors vs groups were more focused on the constructive nature of differing views where as those who saw camps seemed to describe this in competitive terms-- is this significant, coincidental etc?)

We thank the reviewer for the keen interest in the topic and the thoughtful remark. The example quote for “Single actors” was chosen because it stresses how the conflictual nature depends, according to this interviewee, only on the people involved (“it's always a history of person”). We expanded the manuscript text to clarify this example as well as the overall category.

We also agree that interested readers should have access to a more detailed treatment of our classification process and rules. During the analysis we worked with a more comprehensive table than the one shown in the manuscript. This complete classification table included specific a priori rules that determined to which class a sentence would be assigned. While we still believe the summarized table in the main manuscript is more accessible for the broad readership of PLOS ONE, we agree that it may leave the boundaries too blurry for the interested reader. Therefore we added to the supplementary material the full rule table (S1 Table) that was used to analyze the interview content, as well as the rules delineating each the controversy and communication profiles (Tables S2 and S3, respectively).

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response_to_reviewers.docx

Decision Letter 1

Margaret Williams

13 Feb 2023

Creating a foundation for Origin of Life outreach: How scientists relate to their field, the public, and religion

PONE-D-22-02824R1

Dear Karl Wienand

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Margaret Williams, Ph.D

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: Comments-

Specifics-

1- I sent emails regarding my comments on the abstract provided and a different version I found in the online supplementary file, but did not get a response (edits I had originally suggested on the abstract are below as are copies of the emails I sent).

2- The phrase “blessing and curse” is a bit strong and line 25 can be adjusted.

3- In line 30 the word “scientific” should be added before research to clarify.

4- Lines 37-38 could be expanded. These issues touch on people’s identity and self-understandings, but perhaps that will be in future publications.

5- The statistics offered in lines 51-57 are still not entirely parallel and clear, e.g. the percentage in the scientific community who identify with some religious affiliation is given, but not the parallel percentage in the general population.

6- Line 63— perhaps “controversies” instead of “the controversy”?

7- Lines 73-74 refers to “the scientific controversy” again—it seems this controversy should be explained/detailed. The phrase comes up later again as well e.g. line 156 etc- the controversy or even main controversies in the field call out for a line or two of explanation for the reader.

8- Line 116-17 need slight adjustment—people could opt out of questions they would rather not answer or not answer in full.

9- Line 139-40 Perhaps: Similar accusations (instead of violations)? Second half of sentence not entirely clear, perhaps: dogmatic attitudes are the most cited source of conflict?

10- 171-72- Which “discipline”?

11- 183 no “a” before “good”

12- Line 359- out of the box may not be the clearest phrase to use here

13- 383 consistent

I did not do a fully copy editing of the document, but rather pointed out some issues that jumped out at me. Additionally while obviously quotations from informants need to be reported accurately, some of the quotes could use further adjustment for clarity, e.g.: I think already showing that the origin of life research is not just research to disprove that God exists. (12) It's kind of embarrassing to show to the public how researchers—which are thought to be the authority on knowledge—are fighting over if this molecule is prebiotic or not. (30) I think they would, in the first place, do not understand why we do [this research]. (28)

General comment and question—

1- As I wrote in the initial review, I believe this subject of the interface of scientific research and science communication in the development of museum exhibits to be extremely interesting and important. I think that the authors have brought forth valuable new information. I also think that the article would benefit from having a second reviewer. I could offer a potential recommendation if needed.

2- The authors say that all of the data is available, but this does not include recordings or transcriptions of interviews—what is required to be available for a qualitative study?

Editing I had previous done of abstract:

11 Origins of life (Ool) research is particularly challenging to communicate because of the

12 tension between its many disciplines and its nearness (find better word, connection?) to traditionally philosophical or

13 religious questions. To authentically represent scientists’ perspectives in the development of a museum

14 exhibition on Ool, we interviewed 46 researchers from diverse backgrounds (their backgrounds were diverse in some ways but not others correct? Perhaps this needs further thought?). We investigated

15 how they perceive their field (what do you mean? In terms of…?), (the nature of?) science communication, and the relation (of what?) with religion.

16 Results show that researchers actively participate in resolving the scientific debates, but

17 delegate the resolution of controversies involving non-scientific institutions (to whom?). Advocating

18 for science is the foremost communication goal in all contexts. Career stage, research

19 subject, religiosity, etc. (etc. is not great to use- suggest to adjust accordingly) influence the approach (adopted) to (relate to?) controversies and communication.

Emails I sent:

Question about article I am reviewing PONE-D-22-02824R1

Inbox

Mon, Nov 7, 4:12 PM (10 days ago)

to PLOS

Dear Editor,

I am currently reviewing an article, and I have noticed something that I would like to discuss with you. When I went to the website where supplementary information for the manuscript is stored, I found a better version of the abstract of the paper than that which I received. In fact I had begun to suggest a number of edits to the abstract as it was sent to me, but in the version I then found online the issues I had pointed to are already resolved. This makes me wonder if I have the correct, most updated, version of the article for review. I will paste the abstracts below, and will await advice from you. The rest of the paper could also use additional language editing, and of course would be a shame if I am wasting time on an old version. There are substantive comments to make, but the linguistic issues have been distracting.

Thank you,

The abstract in the manuscript sent to me:

Abstract

11 Origins of life research is particularly challenging to communicate because of the

12 tension between its many disciplines and its nearness to traditionally philosophical or

13 religious questions. To authentically represent scientists’ perspective in a museum

14 exhibition, we interviewed 46 researchers from diverse backgrounds. We investigated

15 how they perceive their field, science communication, and the relation with religion.

16 Results show that researchers actively participate in resolving the scientific debate, but

17 delegate the resolution of controversies involving non-scientific institutions. Advocating

18 for science is the foremost communication goal in all contexts. Career stage, research

19 subject, religiosity, etc. influence the approach to controversies and communication.

The improved abstract I found online (https://figshare.com/articles/figure/Supplementary_information_to_Creating_a_foundation_for_Origin_of_Life_outreach_How_scientists_relate_to_their_field_the_public_and_religion_/14703060/3)

Origins of life (OoL) is a multi-disciplinary field at the cutting edge of research. At the same time, the field touches on traditionally philosophical and religious questions, like “What is life?” and “Where do we come from?” This tension makes it particularly challenging for science communi-cation. To better understand OoL researchers and to authentically incorporate their views in a planned exhibition project, this study analyses the perspectives scientist have on their filed, on science communication, and the relation of science and religion. A total of 46 researchers from diverse disciplines and backgrounds were interviewed. While they actively participate and re-solve the scientific debate within OoL, they keep religion separate and delegate the resolution of conflicts with it. The communication of both controversies aims most often to advocate for sci-ence. Conflict and communication profiles are linked to a variety of factors such as career stage, research subject, and religiosity.

Fri, Nov 11, 2:34 PM (6 days ago)

to PLOS

Hello-

I wrote earlier in the week with questions regarding discrepancies I found between versions of the abstract of the article in the on line supplemental folder and the one in the paper I downloaded. If in any case I should just review the article as is, I will work on it next week. If there is a response to the question I sent I would be glad to hear.

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

**********

Acceptance letter

Margaret Williams

15 Feb 2023

PONE-D-22-02824R1

Creating a foundation for Origin of Life outreach: How scientists relate to their field, the public, and religion

Dear Dr. Wienand:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Professor Margaret Williams

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Fig. Communication aspects priority for communication profiles.

    How different communication profiles prioritize aspects of communication.

    (PDF)

    S2 Fig. Additional demographics for controversy and communication profiles.

    Controversy and communication profiles related to the scientific controversy and the controversy with religion, segmented by field of work, hypothesis preference, seniority, religiosity, number of countries researchers lived in.

    (PDF)

    S1 Table. Classification rules.

    Classification table with rules that defined each of the classes and profiles.

    (PDF)

    S2 Table. Definitions of controversy profiles.

    Composition of each controversy profile in terms of most frequent answer classes.

    (PDF)

    S3 Table. Definitions of communication profiles.

    Composition of each communication profile in terms of most frequent answer classes.

    (PDF)

    S1 File. Minimal dataset.

    Anonymized minimal dataset to replicate all figures and conclusions.

    (XLSX)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response_to_reviewers.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    All relevant data are within the paper.


    Articles from PLOS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES