Abstract
In placental mammals, estradiol levels are chronically elevated during pregnancy, but quickly drop to prepartum levels following birth. This may produce an “estrogen withdrawal” state that has been linked to changes in affective states in humans and rodents during the postpartum period. The neural mechanisms underlying these affective changes, however, are understudied. We used a hormone-simulated pseudopregnancy (HSP), a model of postpartum estrogen withdrawal, in adult female C57BL/6 mice to test the impact of postpartum estradiol withdrawal on several behavioral measures of anxiety and motivation. We found that estradiol withdrawal following HSP increased anxiety-like behavior in the elevated plus maze, but not in the open field or marble burying tests. Although hormone treatment during HSP consistently increased sucrose consumption, sucrose preference was generally not impacted by hormone treatment or subsequent estradiol withdrawal. In the social motivation test, estradiol withdrawal decreased the amount of time spent in proximity to a social stimulus animal. These behavioral changes were accompanied by changes in the expression of ΔFosB, a transcription factor correlated with stable long-term plasticity, in the nucleus accumbens (NAc). Specifically, estrogen-withdrawn females had higher ΔFosB expression in the nucleus accumbens core, but ΔFosB expression did not vary across hormone conditions in the nucleus accumbens shell. Using transgenic reporter mice, we found that this increase in ΔFosB occurred in both D1- and D2-expressing cells in the NAc core. Together, these results suggest that postpartum estrogen withdrawal impacts anxiety and motivation and increases ΔFosB in the NAc core.
Keywords: Estradiol, Neuroplasticity, Anxiety, Motivation, Postpartum, Dopamine
Introduction
Epidemiological studies consistently show that mood and anxiety disorders are more prevalent during life stages when estrogens are in flux. Indeed, puberty, pregnancy, and menopause are all associated with an increased incidence of mood and anxiety disorders, suggesting that fluctuations in estrogens may confer additional vulnerability to psychiatric disorders (Soares and Zitek 2008). Among these life stages, the peripartum period stands out as extraordinarily volatile, both in terms of the associated fluctuations in estrogens, and the increased incidence of mental health disorders. By the end of the third trimester, estradiol levels increase up to 1000-fold (Hendrick, Altshuler, and Suri 1998). However, after birth and the expulsion of the placenta, estradiol levels quickly drop to below pre-partum levels and remain suppressed until ovulation resumes (McNeilly 2001). It has been hypothesized that this steep decline in estradiol levels results in an “estrogen withdrawal state” that contributes to the etiology of postpartum psychiatric disturbances (Hendrick, Altshuler, and Suri 1998; Bloch, Daly, and Rubinow 2003; Douma et al. 2005). This idea is supported by clinical studies demonstrating that the symptoms of postpartum depression can be attenuated by estradiol treatment (Sichel et al. 1995), and that experimentally-induced estrogen withdrawal can cause depressive symptoms in people with a history of postpartum depression (Bloch 2000).
To better understand the mechanisms by which postpartum estrogen withdrawal contributes to the etiology of psychological disorders, researchers have turned to rodent models. In particular, several groups have employed a hormone-simulated pseudopregnancy (HSP) model to directly test the impact of estradiol withdrawal on the brain and behavior. Initially developed in female Long-Evans rats (Galea, Wide, and Barr 2001), this model has successfully been replicated (Green, Barr, and Galea 2009) and extended to Sprague-Dawley rats (Schiller et al. 2013; Stoffel and Craft 2004; Suda et al. 2008; Navarre, Laggart, and Craft 2010; Baka et al. 2017), ICR mice (Zhang et al. 2016; Yang et al. 2017), and Syrian hamsters (Hedges et al. 2021), suggesting that it is a highly reproducible way to assess the impact of postpartum estradiol withdrawal across multiple species. In this model, females are ovariectomized and given daily injections that approximate estradiol levels in early- to mid-pregnancy, late pregnancy, and the postpartum period. In particular, females in an “estrogen-withdrawn” condition, which models the abrupt postpartum drop in estradiol, can be compared to females in an “estrogen-sustained” condition, in which estradiol continues to be administered during the simulated postpartum period, or vehicle-treated females. Consistent with the clinical literature, estradiol withdrawal following HSP results in a behavioral phenotype that may reflect depressed mood (Galea, Wide, and Barr 2001; Green, Barr, and Galea 2009; Schiller et al. 2013; Stoffel and Craft 2004; Suda et al. 2008; Navarre, Laggart, and Craft 2010) and/or heightened anxiety (Suda et al. 2008; Zhang et al. 2016; Yang et al. 2017) in rodents. In many cases, these behavioral changes can be prevented by continued administration of estradiol during the postpartum period, consistent with the data from postpartum human subjects.
The nucleus accumbens (NAc) is a behaviorally-relevant site of neuroplasticity following postpartum estradiol withdrawal. Located in the ventral striatum, the NAc receives projections from dopaminergic neurons in the midbrain ventral tegmental area (VTA), and this mesolimbic pathway plays a well-established role in motivated behaviors (Salamone et al. 2015), including maternal behaviors (Seip and Morrell 2009; Numan et al. 2005; Afonso et al. 2009; Numan 2007). This pathway also plays an important role in modulating mood/affect, as altered activity of NAc-projecting VTA neurons is associated with susceptibility to depression and anxiety behaviors in humans and animals (Russo and Nestler 2013; Tye et al. 2013, Lowes et al., 2021). Structural plasticity in the NAc has been linked to behavioral changes that reflect increased stress susceptibility, modeling a core symptom of mood and affective disorders (Christoffel et al. 2011; Bessa et al. 2013). Finally, the NAc is sensitive to estrogens (Creutz and Kritzer 2002; Mitra et al. 2003; Almey, Milner, and Brake 2015), making it a logical candidate for peripartum hormonal fluctuations to negatively impact the brain and behavior.
Within the NAc, the transcription factor ΔFosB is central to the molecular pathway that mediates structural and behavioral changes following chronic activation of the mesolimbic pathway (Nestler 2008), such as might be predicted following sustained hormonal changes during pregnancy and the postpartum period. Indeed, previous microarray data from postpartum hsd:ICR outbred mice shows increased FosB gene expression in the NAc (Zhao et al. 2014), and mutant mice lacking the FosB gene show deficits in parental and affective behaviors (Brown et al. 1996; Kuroda et al. 2008). However, the relationship between peripartum estradiol fluctuations and ΔFosB expression in the NAc is unknown. The present study therefore investigated the effect of estradiol withdrawal following HSP on affective and motivated behaviors, as well as the expression of ΔFosB, in C57BL/6 mice. We found that estradiol withdrawal following HSP increased anxiety-like behavior and decreased social motivation, while concurrently increasing ΔFosB in the NAc core. ΔFosB can have opposing effects on stress susceptibility in D1 vs. D2 neurons (Hamilton et al. 2018). Therefore, we also used transgenic fluorescent reporter mice to determine whether this increase in ΔFosB occurred selectively in NAc MSNs containing dopamine receptor 1 (D1) or dopamine receptor 2 (D2), and found that it occurred in both cell subtypes. Together, these findings suggest that peripartum estradiol fluctuations increase ΔFosB in D1 and D2 neurons in the NAc core, and that these changes are concurrent with increased anxiety and decreased social motivation during estradiol withdrawal.
Methods
Experimental Design and Hormone-Simulated Pseudopregnancy
A hormone-simulated pseudopregnancy (HSP) was used to test the impact of postpartum estradiol withdrawal on the behavior and the brain (Figure 1). Following recovery from ovariectomy (see below), subjects began the HSP injection regimen. On days 1–16, mice received daily subcutaneous injections containing a low dose (0.5 μg) of estradiol benzoate (Sigma) and a high dose (0.8 mg) of progesterone (Sigma) dissolved in 0.1mL cottonseed oil (Sigma). On days 17–23, mice received daily subcutaneous injections containing a high dose of estradiol benzoate (10 μg) dissolved in 0.1mL cottonseed oil (Sigma). On days 24–28, mice were divided into two experimental conditions: estradiol-withdrawn (“withdrawn”) females received daily vehicle injections, modeling the dramatic drop in estradiol that occurs during the postpartum period, while females in the estradiol-sustained (“sustained”) group continued to receive daily injections of a high dose (10 μg) of estradiol. In some experiments, these experimental groups were also compared to a vehicle control group that received oil injections throughout the duration of the HSP (“no hormone”). The doses and time course of the HSP hormone injections were initially chosen for their ability to elicit maternal responses in nulliparous, ovariectomized female rats (Galea, Wide, and Barr 2001), then modified for use in mice (Zhang et al. 2016). In rats, these doses produce serum concentrations that are supraphysiological for the high dose of estradiol, but are consistent with the level of progesterone reported during pregnancy (Bridges 1984; Garland et al. 1987; Galea, Wide, and Barr 2001).
The role of estradiol withdrawal following HSP on anxiety-like behaviors was assessed using elevated plus, open field, and marble burying tasks. Elevated plus and open field were assessed in the same cohort of subjects (Experiment 1), whereas marble burying was assessed in a separate cohort of subjects (Experiment 2), which only included sustained vs. withdrawn mice. The role of estradiol withdrawal following HSP on motivated behaviors was assessed using sucrose preference and social motivation tasks. Motivated behaviors were assessed in a separate cohort (Experiment 3) from either anxiety behavior cohort.
Following HSP (Day 28, 5 days post-withdrawal), subjects were euthanized and their brains were processed for FosB immunoreactivity (FosB-ir) in the NAc (core and shell). In a subset of mice, ΔFosB protein levels were measured in the NAc using Western blots. The results of these experiments indicated that estradiol withdrawal following HSP increased ΔFosB in the NAcC. However, ΔFosB may be expressed differentially, and have different functional properties, in D1 vs. D2 neurons in the NAc (Gagnon et al., 2017; Grueter et al., 2013). Therefore, a separate cohort of mice (Experiment 4) was used to test the role of estradiol withdrawal on ΔFosB expression following HSP in two different lines of female transgenic reporter mice on a C57BL/6 background. The use of these two mouse lines allowed for cell-type-specific localization of ΔFosB within functionally-distinct populations of D1 and D2 neurons in the NAc. Anxiety-like behavior in the elevated plus maze and open field tests was also assessed in these transgenic mice. These experiments only compared sustained vs. withdrawn mice.
Subjects
A total of 90 mice were used in this study. Experiments 1 (n = 24), 2 (n = 16), and 3 (n = 24) used adult female C57BL/6 mice ordered at 8–12 weeks of age from Charles Rivers Laboratories (Wilmington, MA, USA). Experiment 4 used two different lines of adult female bacterial artificial chromosome (BAC) transgenic reporter mice on a C57BL/6 background. The first mouse line, D1-tdtomato (n = 8), expresses a red fluorescent protein under control of the D1 receptor promoter. The second line, D2-GFP (n = 8), expresses a green fluorescent protein under control of the D2 receptor promoter. Transgenic mice were shipped from Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai to Haverford College at 7–13 weeks of age.
Stimulus mice (n = 10) for the social motivation test were juvenile female C57BL/6 mice, ordered at three weeks of age from Charles Rivers Laboratories. Prior to social motivation tests, juvenile mice were acclimated to being placed into a metal mesh cup within one of the holding chambers for five minutes for three consecutive days.
In all experiments, mice were housed in pairs in plexiglass cages (28cm × 17cm × 12cm) with aspen bedding, cotton nesting material, and unlimited access to food and water. Following arrival at the facility, all mice, including stimulus mice, were quarantined for one week and then acclimated to being handled for one week prior to ovariectomy. Mouse holding rooms were temperature- and humidity-controlled and maintained on a 12h/12h reversed light-dark cycle. All animal procedures were carried out in accordance with the National Institutes of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals and approved by the Haverford College Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee.
Ovariectomy
All surgery was conducted using aseptic surgical technique and under isoflurane anesthesia (2–3% vaporized in oxygen, Piramal, Bethlehem, PA, USA). Analgesic (Meloxicam, 5 mg/kg, Portland, ME, USA) was administered subcutaneously prior to the start of surgery and for three days postoperatively. For ovariectomies, subjects’ bilateral flanks were shaved and cleaned with three alternating scrubs of 70% ethanol and Betadine before being transferred to a temperature-regulated, sterile surgical field. Isoflurane anesthesia was maintained via a nose cone. Subjects’ ovaries were extracted via bilateral incisions of the smooth muscle under the flanks and then removed via cauterization of the uterine horn and blood vessels. Polydioxanone absorbable suture (Ethicon, Sommerville, NJ, USA) and wound clips (Fine Science Tools, Foster City, CA, USA) were used to close the smooth muscle and skin incisions, respectively.
Behavior Testing
Elevated Plus.
On day 25 or 26 (“postpartum” days 2 and 3), subjects in experiment 1 or 4 were tested for thigmotaxis behavior as an assay of anxiety using the elevated plus. The apparatus consists of a plus-shaped maze elevated 73 cm above the floor, with two open arms (51 × 11.5 cm) and two enclosed arms (51 × 11.5 × 39.5 cm). Mice were placed in the center of the maze facing a closed arm and allowed to freely explore the maze for five min under white light. The testing chamber was cleaned thoroughly with 70% EtOH between subjects. Tests were video recorded and the amount of time spent in each arm, as well as the total distance traveled and average velocity were quantified using Noldus Ethovision XT (version 14, Noldus Information Technology, Wageningen, The Netherlands).
Open Field.
On day 25 or 26 (“postpartum” days 2 and 3), subjects in experiment 1 or 4 were tested for thigmotaxis behavior as an assay of anxiety using the open field. The apparatus consists of an open field (40.5 × 40.5 × 30cm) composed entirely of solid, light gray plastic (Maze Engineers, Glenview, IL, USA). Mice were placed in the center of the apparatus and allowed to freely explore for five min under white light. The testing chamber was cleaned thoroughly with 70% EtOH between subjects. Tests were video recorded and the amount of time spent in the center of the field, periphery of the field, as well as the total distance traveled and average velocity were quantified using Noldus Ethovision XT.
Marble Burying.
On day 27 (“postpartum” day 4), subjects in Experiment 2 were tested for compulsive-like behavior using a marble burying test. Clean mouse cages were prepared with aspen bedding (approximately 5 cm deep) and 15 multicolor glass marbles (16-mm diameter) that had been cleaned with 70% EtOH. Marbles were evenly-distributed on the surface of the bedding, such that they were 2.5 cm from the sides of the cage and with ~4 cm between each other. Prior to the initiation of HSP, subjects were tested for baseline marble burying behavior so that marble burying could be balanced across hormone conditions. On day 27 (“postpartum” day 4), subjects were again tested for their marble burying behavior. During both baseline tests and experimental tests, mice were placed into the prepared cage for 30 minutes and allowed to freely interact with the marbles. After 30 minutes, mice were carefully removed from the cage and the number of buried marbles was counted by two experimenters blind to the condition of the subjects. A marble was considered buried if more than two thirds of the marble was covered in bedding.
Sucrose Preference.
Subjects in Experiment 3 were tested for motivation for palatable solutions, or possible anhedonia, using a sucrose preference test. On Days 21–23 (“late pregnancy”), all mice were acclimated to the two-bottle choice task. On these days, two bottles were available in each home cage: one bottle was filled with tap water and the other with 2% sucrose solution. Prior to testing, subjects were water deprived for 5 hr. Subsequently, each subject was individually housed in a clean testing cage with one bottle of tap water and one bottle of 2% sucrose solution for 3 hr before being returned to their home cage. Bottle side was carefully switched at the 1.5-hour mark to account for potential side bias. Each bottle was weighed before and after the 3-hr period on each day in order to determine fluid intake. On Days 24–26 (“postpartum” days 1–3), the 5-hr water deprivation followed by 3-hr two-bottle test procedure was repeated. Sucrose preference was calculated as (sucrose solution consumption) / (sucrose solution consumption + water consumption) × 100%.
Social Motivation.
On Day 27 (“postpartum” day 4), subjects in Experiment 3 were tested for their motivation to investigate a social stimulus. The apparatus consists of two lateral chambers (21 × 19 × 29.5 cm) composed of solid, light gray plastic, and a clear plastic holding chamber (21 × 6.5 × 29.5 cm) and removable doors (Maze Engineers). Each of the lateral chambers held an inverted mesh cup (9.5 × 9.5 × 12 cm): one cup contained a toy black mouse toy (Play Fur Mice Cat Toy, Penn-Plax) and the other contained a juvenile (30 day old) female stimulus mouse, both of which were previously unfamiliar to the subjects. Subjects were placed in the holding chamber for 5 minutes of acclimation, after which the doors were lifted and the subject was allowed to freely explore the two stimulus chambers for 10 minutes before being returned to their home cage. To control for side bias, the placement of the social and non-social stimuli was alternated between subjects. The testing chamber was cleaned thoroughly with 70% EtOH between subjects. Tests were video recorded and the amount of time spent in each chamber, and the time spent interacting with either stimulus, and the amount of time spent facing the social stimulus was quantified using Noldus Ethovision XT. Interactions were defined as a mouse having its nose within 2 cm of stimulus containers while having its nose pointed in the direction of the container. Relative duration in the social floor was calculated as the time in floor of social stimulus / total time in each floor. Relative time in proximity to social stimulus was calculated as the time spent within 2 cm of social stimulus / total time in proximity to both stimuli. Relative time facing the social stimulus was calculated as the time oriented towards the social stimulus / total time oriented to both stimuli.
Histology
Perfusion.
On Day 27 (“postpartum” day 5), subjects were given an overdose of sodium pentobarbital (0.2 mL Euthanasia III, Patterson Vet Supply, Devens, MA, USA) and transcardially perfused with approximately 25 mL of cold 25 mM PBS (pH 7.4), followed by 50 mL of cold 4% paraformaldehyde (Electron Microscopy Sciences, Hatfield, PA, USA). Brains were immediately removed and post-fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde overnight and then cryoprotected for at least 48 hr in 30% sucrose in PBS. Coronal sections (35 to 40-μm) of brain tissue were sectioned on a cryostat (−20°C), collected in a 1:4 series, and stored at −20°C in cryoprotectant until immunohistochemical processing. At the time of perfusion, mice were between 16 and 20 weeks old.
Immunohistochemistry and brightfield microscopy.
Tissue from experiments 1, 2, and a subset of animals from experiment 3 were processed for immunohistochemical detection of FosB proteins (ΔFosB and full-length FosB) in the NAc. Briefly, sections containing the NAc were removed from cryoprotectant and rinsed 5 × 5 min in 25 mM PBS. To reduce endogenous peroxidase activity, tissue sections were incubated in 0.3% hydrogen peroxide for 15 min. After 5 × 5 min rinses in PBS, sections were incubated in a monoclonal primary antibody against FosB (1: 2,000, (5G4) Rabbit mAb #2251, Cell Signaling) in 0.4% Triton-X 100 for 24 hr at room temperature. After incubation in primary antibody, sections were rinsed in five times in PBS and then incubated for 1 hr in biotinylated secondary antibody (anti rabbit, 1:600 Jackson Immunoresearch, West Grove, PA, USA) in PBS with 0.4% Triton-X 100. Sections were again rinsed five times in PBS and then incubated for 1 hr in avidin-biotin complex (4.5 μL each of A and B reagents/ml PBS with 0.4% Triton-X 100, ABC Elite Kit, Vector Laboratories, Burlingame, CA, USA). After rinsing in PBS, sections were incubated in 3,3’-diaminobenzidine HCl (0.2 mg/mL, Sigma) with hydrogen peroxide (0.83 μL/mL, Sigma) for 10 min, yielding an orange-brown product. The reaction was stopped by rinsing sections in PBS. Stained tissue sections were mounted onto subbed glass slides and allowed to air-dry overnight. Slides were then dehydrated in alcohols, cleared in xylenes, and cover slipped using Permount (Fisher Scientific, Waltham, MA, USA).
A light microscope (Nikon Eclipse E200) using a color camera (Diagnostic Instruments, Sterling Heights, MI, USA) and Spot Basic software was used to acquire images. 10X images of the bilateral NAc Shell and Core (2 unilateral images per mouse) were acquired. Selection of sections for analysis was based on neuroanatomical matching to stereotaxic atlas plates containing the NAc (Bregma +1.10 mm, Paxinos and Franklin, 2019). The caudal NAc was chosen because FosB expression in this region is impacted by estradiol fluctuations as well as by naturally-rewarding experiences (e.g. Peterson et al 2015; Staffend and Meisel 2012, Acaba et al 2019). Rectangular counting domains (200 × 500 μm) for the NAcC and NAcSh were consistently applied onto images using the anterior commissure as a landmark (see Figure 4). All cell counts were done manually by experimenters blind to the condition of the subject, and inter-rater reliability was confirmed. FosB-ir cell counts were also attempted in the medial preoptic area (MPOA), but FosB-ir was sparse in this brain region.
Double immunofluorescence and confocal microscopy.
Tissue sections from Experiment 4 were processed for FosB immunofluorescence in either D1- or D2-expressing neurons in the NAc. Briefly, sections containing the NAc were removed from cryoprotectant and rinsed 5 × 5 min in 25 mM PBS. Sections were incubated in the appropriate primary antibodies (see below) in 0.1 PBTX for 24 hours at room temperature. After incubation in primary antibody, sections were rinsed five times in PBS and then incubated in the appropriate fluorescent secondary antibodies (see below) in 0.1% PBTX for one hour at room temperature in the dark. In tissue from D1-tdtomato mice, primary antibodies were Rabbit anti-FosB (1:1000, Abcam #184938) and Goat anti-RFP (1:750, Rockland #200-01-397) and secondary antibodies were Alexa 488 anti-Rabbit and Alexa 594 anti-Goat (1:500, Jackson Immunoresearch, #111-545-144 and #705-585-147). In tissue from D2-GFP mice, primary antibodies were Rabbit anti-FosB (1:1000, Abcam #184938) and Goat anti-GFP (1:5000, Abcam #5450) for the primary antibodies and secondary antibodies were Alexa 594 anti-Rabbit and Alexa 488 anti-Goat (1:500, Jackson Immunoresearch, #711-585-152 and #705-546-147). After five final rinses in PBS, tissue was mounted onto subbed glass slides and coverslipped using ProLong Gold Antifade Mountant (Invitrogen, Waltham, MA, USA). Slides were stored in the dark until imaging.
Double-immunofluorescent tissue was imaged using a confocal microscope (Nikon C1 Confocal Microscope) using EZ-C1 software (version 3.90, Nikon). 20X z-stacked images of the bilateral NAc (2 unilateral images per mouse) simultaneously containing both red and green fluorophores were acquired. Selection of sections for analysis was based on neuroanatomical matching to stereotaxic atlas plates containing the NAc (Bregma +1.10 mm, Paxinos and Franklin 2019). Rectangular counting domains (233 μm × 333 μm) for the NAcC and NAcSh were consistently applied using the anterior commissure as a landmark (See Figure 4). Double-labeled cells were identified as having a FosB nuclear stain (green in D1-tdtomato mice and red in D2-GFP mice) co-localized with cytoplasmic staining for transgenic expression of either tdtomato (red) or GFP (green). This colocalization typically appeared as yellow (Figure 5). In tissue from D1-tdtomato mice, photomicrographs were post-processed using the “Find Edges” function in FIJI to more clearly differentiate D1 cells from background. All cell counts were done manually by at least two experimenters blind to the condition of the subject, and inter-rater reliability was confirmed.
Western Blots.
A subset of estrogen-withdrawn (n = 6) and estrogen sustained (n = 6) animals in experiment 1 were given an overdose (0.2 mL) of Euthanasia III (Patterson Vet Supply) and sacrificed by rapid decapitation. 1-mm thick coronal sections containing the NAc were taken and bilateral tissue punches (1-mm diameter) were immediately collected from both brain areas. NAc punches included both the shell and core, but were biased towards the core (i.e., included the anterior commissure). Bilateral punches were flash-frozen and stored at −80°C until Western blotting.
Bilateral tissue punches were homogenized in 1% sodium dodecyl sulfate (SDS) processing buffer and protein was quantified using the Bio-Rad protein DC assay (Bio-Rad Laboratories, Berkeley, CA). For each sample, 40 mg of total protein was loaded onto a 12–15% polyacrylamide gradient gel (Mini-PROTEAN TGX Precast Mini Gel; Bio-Rad Laboratories) and transferred to a nitrocellulose membrane following separation. Samples from each condition were counterbalanced across gels, and each sample was duplicated for technical replication. Membranes were blocked in 5% nonfat dried milk in TBS, incubated overnight in primary antibodies against FosB (1:1,000, Cell Signaling, Beverly MA) or Glyceraldehyde 3-phosphate dehydrogenase (GAPDH, 1:30,000, Millipore, Burlington, MA) diluted in Tris-buffered saline (TBS) + 0.1% Tween 20 (Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis, MO) for 24 hr at room temperature. The following day, membranes were incubated in the appropriate horseradish peroxidase (HRP)-conjugated secondary antibodies (FosB anti-rabbit HRP, GAPDH anti-mouse HRP, Cell signaling) at 1:500 for 1 hr at room temperature. These primary antibodies have been used previously and produced bands at the appropriate molecular weights (Acaba et al. 2019). Following primary antibody incubation, membranes were developed for chemiluminescent detection using SuperSignal West Femto Maximum Substrate Sensitivity (Thermo Fisher Scientific, Waltham, MA). Chemiluminescence was visualized and quantified by an experimenter blind to the condition of the sample using FluorChem Software (Protein Simple, San Jose, CA). ΔFosB levels were normalized to GAPDH levels to control for equal loading of samples across wells.
Statistical Analysis
Statistics were run using SPSS (IBM, Chicago, IL, USA) or Jamovi software (retrieved from https://www.jamovi.org) and graphed using Prism (GraphPad Software, San Diego, CA). Difference scores were calculated for the elevated plus maze (time spent in closed arms - time spent in open arms) and open field (time spent in periphery - time spent in center), and one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used to determine whether difference scores varied by experimental condition. Tukey’s honestly significant difference (HSD) post hoc tests were used to examine significant omnibus tests for group differences. For the marble burying task, a two-tailed unpaired t-test was used to determine if the total number of buried marbles varied by hormonal condition. Two-way ANOVAs were used to determine if sucrose consumption and preference varied by hormonal condition.
One-way ANOVAs or Unpaired t tests (two-tailed) were used to determine whether FosB cell counts varied by experimental conditions. Tukey’s HSD post hoc tests were used to examine significant omnibus tests for group differences. Unpaired t tests were used to determine whether FosB protein expression measured in Western blots differed between experimental conditions. Effect sizes were estimated for significant findings using eta squared (η2) for ANOVAs and t-tests and Cohen’s d for post-hoc pair-wise comparisons.
Results
Behavioral Assays of Anxiety
Elevated Plus.
In C57BL/6 mice (Experiment 1), one mouse from the no hormone condition was eliminated from the analysis because it jumped from the apparatus prior to the completion of the test. There was a significant effect of hormone condition on the duration of time spent in the closed arms minus the duration of time spent in the open arms of the elevated plus maze (F(2,20) = 15.02, p = 0.0001, η2 = 0.60). Post-hoc tests revealed that subjects in the no hormone group did not differ from withdrawn mice in that both spent more time in the closed than open arms of the apparatus (p = 0.59). However, sustained mice spent a similar amount of time in the open and closed arms, indicating a lower-anxiety phenotype than no hormone or withdrawn females. Post-hoc tests revealed that sustained mice differed significantly from both no hormone (p = 0.0018, d = 2.08) and withdrawn mice (p = 0.0001, d = 2.59). Similar to difference score analyses, there was a significant effect of hormone condition on the amount of time spent in the open arms of the maze (F(2,20) = 4.84, p = 0.019, η2 = 0.32). Sustained animals spent significantly more time in the open arms compared to withdrawn animals (p = 0.016, d = 1.53), whereas neither sustained or withdrawn animals differed from no hormone animals (p > 0.05). The total distance traveled (F(2,20) = 0.59, p = 0.55) and average velocity (F(2, 20) = 0.13, p = 0.88) did not vary by hormone condition (Figure 2).
In transgenic reporter mice (Experiment 4), the duration of time spent in the closed arms minus the duration of time spent in the open arms was significantly higher in withdrawn than sustained females (t(14) = 3.47, p = 0.0037, η2 = 0.46) (Figure 5). The total distance traveled (t(14) = 0.09, p = 0.92) and average velocity (t(14) = 0.09, p = 0.93) did not vary by hormone condition.
Open Field.
In C57BL/6 mice (Experiment 1), there was no effect of hormone condition on the duration of time spent in the periphery minus the duration of time spent in the center of the open field (F(2,20) = 0.37, p = 0.69). The total distance traveled (F(2,20) = 1.22, p = 0.31) and average velocity (F(2,20) = 1.13, p = 0.34) did not vary by hormone condition (Figure 2). In transgenic reporter mice (Experiment 4), there was no effect of hormone condition on the duration of time spent in the periphery minus the duration of time spent in the center of the open field (t(14) = 1.05, p = 0.30) (Figure 5). The total distance traveled (t(14) = 0.54, p = 0.59) and average velocity (t(14) = 0.49, p = 0.63) did not vary by hormone condition.
Marble Burying.
In C57BL/6 mice (Experiment 2),There was no effect of hormone condition on the number of marbles buried (t(14) = 0.79, p = 0.44). Likewise, there was no effect of hormone condition on the difference from baseline number of marbles buried (t(14) = 1.09, p = 0.29) (Figure 2).
Behavioral Assays of Motivation
Sucrose Preference.
In C57BL/6 mice (Experiment 3), there was a significant interaction between hormone condition and test day on sucrose consumption (F(10,100) = 3.61, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.23). During the acclimation period (Days 21–23), estradiol treatment increased sucrose consumption, reaching significance for the withdrawn group compared to the no hormone group on Day 21 (p = 0.003, d = 2.84), both the sustained (p = 0.002, d = 2.55) and withdrawn (p < 0.001, d = 3.91) groups compared to the no hormone group on Day 22, and the sustained group compared the no hormone group on Day 23 (p = 0.030, d = 1.45). During the testing period (Days 24–26), there was generally no effect of hormone treatment on sucrose consumption, although the withdrawn group did consume more sucrose than the no hormone group on Day 24 (p = 0.031, d = 1.75) (Figure 3).
There was a main effect of hormone condition on sucrose preference (F(2,20 = 5.82, p = 0.010, η2 = 0.59). During the acclimation period (Day 22), estradiol treatment increased sucrose preference in both sustained (p = 0.003, d = 1.60) and withdrawn (p = 0.016, d = 1.45) mice compared to no hormone mice. During the testing period (Days 24–26), there was generally no effect of hormone treatment on sucrose preference, although withdrawn group did have a significantly higher sucrose preference than the no hormone group on Day 26 (p = 0.027, d = 1.22), and the sustained group trended toward significance on Day 26 as well (p = 0.073, d = 1.07) (Figure 3).
Social Motivation.
In C57BL/6 mice (Experiment 3), there was a significant effect of hormone condition on the relative duration of time spent in proximity to the social stimulus (F(2,20) = 5.83, p = 0.010, η2 = 0.37). Post-hoc tests revealed that withdrawn mice spent significantly less time in proximity to the social stimulus than sustained (p = 0.009, d = 1.48) or no hormone mice, although the later comparison did not reach statistical significance (p = 0.069). There was no effect of hormone condition on the relative duration of time spent in the social stimulus chamber (F(2,20) = 0.06, p = 0.93) or the relative duration facing the social stimulus (F(2,20 = 0.05, p = 0.94) (Figure 3).
There was no effect of hormone condition on the cumulative duration in proximity to the social stimulus (F(2,20) = 0.08, p = 0.91) or the cumulative duration of time spent in the social stimulus chamber (F(2,20) = 0.41, p = 0.66). However, there was a significant effect of hormone condition on the cumulative duration facing the social stimulus (F(2,20) = 3.88, p = 0.049, η2 = 0.21). Post-hoc tests revealed that withdrawn mice spent significantly less time facing the social stimulus mice than no hormone mice (p = 0.044, d = 1.16) (Figure 3).
ΔFosB expression in the brain.
Immunoreactivity in the NAc.
Tissue from C57BL/6 mice showed that in the NAcC, there was a significant effect of hormone condition on the number of FosB-ir cells (F(2, 32) = 39.71, p < 0.0001, η2 = 0.72). Post-hoc analyses revealed that both sustained (p = 0.002, d = d = 1.52) and withdrawn (p < 0.0001, d = 3.68) mice had significantly more Fos-ir cells in the NAcC than no hormone mice. In addition, withdrawn mice had significantly more FosB-ir cells in the NAcC than sustained mice (p < 0.0001, d = 2.17). In the NAcSh, however, there was no effect of hormone condition on FosB-ir cells (F(2,35) = 3.07, p = 0.815 (Figure 4).
Western blots in the NAc.
Since FosB immunohistochemistry cannot distinguish between the two products of the FosB gene, ΔFosB and full-length FosB, Western blots were used to selectively measure ΔFosB protein levels in core-biased tissue punches from the NAc of sustained vs. withdrawn C57BL/6 females. Withdrawn females had significantly higher ΔFosB levels (normalized to GAPDH) than did sustained mice (t(10) = 2.30, p = 0.043, η2 = 0.35) (Figure 4). In contrast, there was no difference in levels of full-length FosB (t(10) = 0.22, p = 0.82).
Double-Immunofluorescence in D1 and D2 neurons in the NAc.
Tissue from transgenic reporter mice showed that estradiol withdrawal increased FosB-ir in both D1 and D2 neurons the NAcC, but not NAcSh. Tissue from D1-tdtomato mice showed that the number of double-immunofluorescent (FosB-ir + RFP-ir) cells was increased in withdrawn mice compared to sustained mice in the NAcC (t(14) = 3.72 p = 0.001, η2 = 0.49), but not in the NAcSh (t(14) = 1.09, p = 0.14) (Figure 5). Similarly, tissue from D2-GFP mice showed that the number of double-immunofluorescent cells (FosB-ir + GFP-ir) was increased in withdrawn mice compared to sustained mice in the NAcC (t(14) = 2.24, p = 0.020, η2 = 0.27), but not in the NAcSh (t(14) = 1.27, p = 0.11) (Figure 5).
Discussion
Here we demonstrate for the first time that in C57BL/6 mice, estradiol withdrawal following HSP alters affective behaviors in the elevated plus, sucrose preference, and social motivation tests, but not in the open field or marble burying tests. These behavioral changes are consistent with increases in anxiety-like behavior and decreases in motivation that have been demonstrated in previous studies using HSP to model postpartum estradiol withdrawal, and extend the HSP model to the common C57BL/6 strain for the first time, providing valuable information about species and strain similarities and differences. In addition, we find that estradiol withdrawal following HSP increases ΔFosB in the NAc core, but not in the NAc shell. This increase occurs in both D1 and D2 receptor-bearing medium spiny neurons (MSNs). This novel finding suggests that ΔFosB may mediate long-term neuroplasticity in the NAc following postpartum estradiol withdrawal, and that these neuroplastic changes may be related to behavioral changes during this period.
Our finding that withdrawn females showed a higher-anxiety behavioral phenotype than sustained females in the elevated plus is consistent with previous findings in ICR mice (Zhang et al. 2016; Yang et al. 2017), Sprague Dawley rats (Suda et al. 2008), and Syrian hamsters (Hedges et al. 2021). Findings in the open field following HSP are more variable both within (Yang et al. 2017; Zhang et al. 2016; Galea, Wide, and Barr 2001) and between (Hedges et al. 2021) species, suggesting that the elevated plus may be a more reliable thigmotaxis-based assay of anxiety-like behavior following HSP. A remaining question is whether estradiol treatment protects against anxiety or, in contrast, if estradiol withdrawal promotes anxiety? Our findings suggest that in C57BL/6 mice sustained high levels of estrogens may decrease anxiety, as sustained females differed significantly from both no hormone, ovariectomized control females and withdrawn females, whereas no hormone females did not differ from withdrawn mice in the elevated plus.
We did not observe a significant effect of hormone condition on marble burying behavior, although marble burying was generally higher in withdrawn mice than sustained mice. Low levels of burying across all experimental groups may have made it difficult to detect differences. Nonetheless, the impact of peripartum hormone fluctuations on the compulsive subtype of anxiety bears further investigation given the increased risk of OCD in the peripartum period (Zambaldi et al. 2009) and the diagnostic challenges associated with peripartum OCD symptoms (Meltzer-Brody et al. 2018). Future experiments would benefit from using a different behavioral assay, such as the signal attenuation model of compulsive lever pressing. In this model, withdrawal from chronic administration of estradiol increases compulsive lever pressing (Flaisher-Grinberg et al. 2009), whereas blockade of D1 receptors decreases compulsive lever pressing (Joel 2006). These results, when coupled with the known role of the NAc in compulsive anxiety (Sturm et al. 2003), suggest that the signal attenuation model of compulsive lever pressing is a compelling model to test the role of postpartum estradiol withdrawal and NAc plasticity on compulsive anxiety behaviors.
In the current study, hormone treatment increased sucrose consumption during the acclimation phase when estradiol levels were high, but this effect declined for both withdrawn and sustained mice by the second and third days of the simulated postpartum period. This is consistent with previous findings in rats (Green, Barr, and Galea 2009). In addition, mice in all hormone conditions reliably developed a sucrose preference, and hormone treatment significantly increased sucrose preference in both sustained and withdrawn females compared to no hormone females on the second day of the acclimation period. Although this preference was eliminated in both sustained and withdrawn mice on the first and second days of the simulated postpartum period, suggesting a possible anhedonic state, withdrawn females again had a significantly higher sucrose preference than no hormone females on the third day of the simulated postpartum period, and sustained females also trended towards a significantly higher preference than no hormone females on this day. These sucrose preference findings differ from previous findings in rats, in which withdrawn females exhibited a decrease in sucrose preference compared to females that continued to receive estradiol or an estrogen receptor beta agonist (Green, Barr, and Galea 2009). However, our findings are more consistent with recent data from Syrian hamsters, in which estradiol withdrawal following HSP did not impact sucrose preference (Hedges et al. 2021). It is difficult to interpret the observed increase in sucrose preference in withdrawn and sustained mice after a lack of preference on the previous two days. One possibility is that repeated presentation of sucrose across six days led to a tolerance for sucrose (Green, Barr, and Galea 2009), which would be consistent with the decrease in consumption during this time, and would limit the reliability of this test as an assay for anhedonia. Future experiments may benefit from further separating the acclimation period from the test period to minimize the potential impact of sucrose tolerance. Taken together, these data suggest that sucrose preference may be a species-dependent readout of motivation/anhedonia following HSP.
We also found that estradiol withdrawal following HSP decreased time spent in proximity to a social stimulus and facing a social stimulus, indicating a deficit in social motivation. This is the first time this has been established in an HSP model, but is consistent with a reduction in social motivation and other assays of behavioral despair in early postpartum female rats (Rincón-Cortés and Grace 2020). Our data suggest that deficits in social approach in naturally-parturient females may be mediated by estradiol withdrawal during the early postpartum period. Interestingly, in early postpartum female rats, deficits in social motivation and increased anxiety-like behavior in the elevated plus are correlated with decreases in the number of spontaneously active dopamine neurons in the ventral tegmental area (Rincón-Cortés and Grace 2020). This neural change is indicative of dopamine downregulation, or the deceased ability of the mesolimbic system to respond to behaviorally-relevant inputs. What’s more, it is consistent with microdialysis studies showing reduced baseline levels of extracellular dopamine in the NAc of early postpartum rats and ovariectomized females that are hormone-primed to induce maternal responsiveness (Afonso et al. 2009).
The induction of ΔFosB in NAc in response to chronic decreases in dopamine release is well-established in mice and humans. In mice, ΔFosB induction occurs selectively in D2 MSNs in response to chronic treatment with haloperidol, a D2 receptor antagonist (Lobo et al. 2013). In humans, ΔFosB induction occurs in NAc MSNs in response to dopamine depletion (Tekumalla et al. 2001), although the cell-type selectivity of this effect has not been established. Meanwhile, the induction of ΔFosB in D2 MSNs has been related to negative emotional states (Hamilton et al. 2018). These data map on to our current finding that withdrawn females show increased ΔFosB expression in the NAcC, as measured by both Western blot and immunohistochemistry. We established that this increase in ΔFosB following estradiol withdrawal occurs in both D1 and D2 MSNs in the NAcC. Unlike the NAcSh, which is thought to be involved in shorter-term aspects of motivation, the NAcC is involved in the development of patterned motor programs to obtain rewards (Alcaro, Huber, and Panksepp 2007), such as might be expected following long-term physiological and/or neuroplastic changes. Interestingly, the NAcC appears to be uniquely sensitive to fluctuating levels of estradiol. For example, in rats, the electrophysiological properties of MSNs differ by sex in the NAcC, but not the NAcSh (Willett et al., 2016). Likewise, excitatory synaptic inputs (as measured by mEPSC properties) are elevated in the NAcC in proestrus and estrus rats compared to diestrus females and males (Proaño et al., 2018). Acute estradiol treatment decreases dendritic spine density in the NAcC, but not NAcSh, in rats (Peterson, Mermelstein, and Meisel 2015) and Syrian hamsters (Staffend and Meisel 2012). The impact of chronic estradiol treatment and/or estradiol withdrawal on NAcC spine density is unknown, but the current results would predict an increase in dendritic spine density following postpartum estradiol withdrawal.
Evidence from functional neuroimaging studies in humans suggests that the striatum is dysregulated during postpartum psychiatric disorders. Specifically, people with postpartum depression have a blunted response in the striatum to positive, rewarding stimuli (Silverman et al. 2007), positive faces (Morgan et al. 2017), and their own infants’ joyful faces (Laurent and Ablow 2013). Despite this, very few studies have used animal models to investigate neuroplasticity in the mesolimbic pathway in relation to postpartum psychological states. In Flinders Sensitive Line (FSL) rats, a selectively-bred rodent model used to study depression, postpartum females show decreased extracellular dopamine levels in the NAc during pup interactions compared to Sprague-Dawley rats (Lavi-Avnon et al. 2008). Similarly, postpartum Sprague-Dawley rats that experienced chronic stress during gestation show reduced structural plasticity in the NAc, as well as reduced mobility in the forced swim test (Haim, Sherer, and Leuner 2014). However, this is the first study to examine the impact of estradiol withdrawal on molecular plasticity in the NAc mediating postpartum mood or affect. Future studies will benefit from this extension of the HSP model to C57BL/6 mice, in which genetic tools and behavioral assays can be leveraged to continue to investigate the role of peripartum fluctuations in estrogens on mesolimbic plasticity and affective changes.
Highlights.
Estrogen withdrawal after a simulated pregnancy increases anxiety behavior in mice
Estrogen withdrawal after a simulated pregnancy decreases social motivation in mice
These behavior changes are concurrent with increased ΔFosB in the nucleus accumbens core
Increased ΔFosB is found in both D1 and D2 medium spiny neurons
Acknowledgements
Research supported in this publication was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health and the National Institutes of Health under Award Number R15MH125282 to LEB. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health. The authors wish to thank Ezekiel Mouzon (Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai) for his assistance coordinating the shipping of transgenic reporter mice, Luke Troyon (Haverford College) for his assistance with confocal microscopy, and Victoria Lacorte and Victoria Bruce (Widener University) for their assistance in D1/FosB cell counts.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- Acaba Luis, Sidibe David, Thygesen John, Van der Kloot Harrison, and Been Laura E.. 2019. “Sex Experience Increases Delta FosB in Male and Female Hamsters, but Facilitates Sex Behavior Only in Females.” Behavioral Neuroscience 133 (4): 378–84. 10.1037/bne0000313. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Afonso Veronica M., King Samantha, Chatterjee Diptendu, and Fleming Alison S.. 2009. “Hormones That Increase Maternal Responsiveness Affect Accumbal Dopaminergic Responses to Pup- and Food-Stimuli in the Female Rat.” Hormones and Behavior 56 (1): 11–23. 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2009.02.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alcaro Antonio, Huber Robert, and Panksepp Jaak. 2007. “Behavioral Functions of the Mesolimbic Dopaminergic System: An Affective Neuroethological Perspective.” Brain Research Reviews 56 (2): 283–321. 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2007.07.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Almey Anne, Milner Teresa A., and Brake Wayne G.. 2015. “Estrogen Receptors in the Central Nervous System and Their Implication for Dopamine-Dependent Cognition in Females.” Hormones and Behavior 74 (August): 125–38. 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2015.06.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baka Judith, Csakvari Eszter, Huzian Orsolya, Dobos Nikoletta, Siklos Laszlo, Leranth Csaba, MacLusky Neil J., Duman Ronald S., and Hajszan Tibor. 2017. “Stress Induces Equivalent Remodeling of Hippocampal Spine Synapses in a Simulated Postpartum Environment and in a Female Rat Model of Major Depression.” Neuroscience 343 (February): 384–97. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.12.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bessa JM, Morais M, Marques F, Pinto L, Palha JA, Almeida OFX, and Sousa N. 2013. “Stress-Induced Anhedonia Is Associated with Hypertrophy of Medium Spiny Neurons of the Nucleus Accumbens.” Translational Psychiatry 3 (6): e266–e266. 10.1038/tp.2013.39. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bloch M 2000. “Effects of Gonadal Steroids in Women With a History of Postpartum Depression.” American Journal of Psychiatry 157 (6): 924–30. 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.6.924. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bloch Miki, Daly Robert C, and Rubinow David R. 2003. “Endocrine Factors in the Etiology of Postpartum Depression.” Comprehensive Psychiatry 44 (3): 234–46. 10.1016/S0010-440X(03)00034-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bridges Robert S. 1984. “A Quantitative Analysis of the Roles of Dosage, Sequence, and Duration of Estradiol and Progesterone Exposure in the Regulation of Maternal Behavior in the Rat *.” Endocrinology 114 (3): 930–40. 10.1210/endo-114-3-930. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown Jennifer R, Hong Ye, Bronson Roderick T, Dikkes Pieter, and Greenberg Michael E. 1996. “A Defect in Nurturing in Mice Lacking the Immediate Early Gene FosB.” Cell 86 (2): 297–309. 10.1016/S0092-8674(00)80101-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Christoffel DJ, Golden SA, Dumitriu D, Robison AJ, Janssen WG, Ahn HF, Krishnan V, et al. 2011. “I B Kinase Regulates Social Defeat Stress-Induced Synaptic and Behavioral Plasticity.” Journal of Neuroscience 31 (1): 314–21. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4763-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Creutz Lela M., and Kritzer Mary F.. 2002. “Estrogen Receptor-? Immunoreactivity in the Midbrain of Adult Rats: Regional, Subregional, and Cellular Localization in the A10, A9, and A8 Dopamine Cell Groups.” The Journal of Comparative Neurology 446 (3): 288–300. 10.1002/cne.10207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Douma SL, Husband C, OʼDonnell ME, Barwin BN, and Woodend AK. 2005. “Estrogen-Related Mood Disorders: Reproductive Life Cycle Factors.” Advances in Nursing Science 28 (4): 364–75. 10.1097/00012272-200510000-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Flaisher-Grinberg Shlomit, Albelda Noa, Gitter Liron, Weltman Keren, Arad Michal, and Joel Daphna. 2009. “Ovarian Hormones Modulate ‘Compulsive’ Lever-Pressing in Female Rats.” Hormones and Behavior 55 (2): 356–65. 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2008.10.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Galea Liisa A.M, Wide Jennifer K, and Barr Alasdair M. 2001. “Estradiol Alleviates Depressive-like Symptoms in a Novel Animal Model of Post-Partum Depression.” Behavioural Brain Research 122 (1): 1–9. 10.1016/S0166-4328(01)00170-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gagnon D, Petryszyn S, Sanchez MG, Bories C, Beaulieu JM, De Koninck Y, Parent A, Parent M, 2017. Striatal Neurons Expressing D1 and D2 Receptors are Morphologically Distinct and Differently Affected by Dopamine Denervation in Mice. Sci Rep 7, 41432. 10.1038/srep41432 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garland HO, Atherton JC, Baylis C, Morgan MRA, and Milne CM. 1987. “Hormone Profiles for Progesterone, Oestradiol, Prolactin, Plasma Renin Activity, Aldosterone and Corticosterone during Pregnancy and Pseudopregnancy in Two Strains of Rat: Correlation with Renal Studies.” Journal of Endocrinology 113 (3): 435–44. 10.1677/joe.0.1130435. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Green Amanda D., Barr Alasdair M., and Galea Liisa A. M.. 2009. “Role of Estradiol Withdrawal in ‘anhedonic’ Sucrose Consumption: A Model of Postpartum Depression.” Physiology & Behavior 97 (2): 259–65. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.02.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grueter BA, Robison AJ, Neve RL, Nestler EJ, Malenka RC, 2013. ΔFosB differentially modulates nucleus accumbens direct and indirect pathway function. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 110, 1923–1928. 10.1073/pnas.1221742110 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haim Achikam, Sherer Morgan, and Leuner Benedetta. 2014. “Gestational Stress Induces Persistent Depressive-like Behavior and Structural Modifications within the Postpartum Nucleus Accumbens.” European Journal of Neuroscience 40 (12): 3766–73. 10.1111/ejn.12752. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hamilton Peter J, Burek Dominika J, Lombroso Sonia I, Neve Rachael L, Robison Alfred J, Nestler Eric J, and Heller Elizabeth A. 2018. “Cell-Type-Specific Epigenetic Editing at the Fosb Gene Controls Susceptibility to Social Defeat Stress.” Neuropsychopharmacology 43 (2): 272–84. 10.1038/npp.2017.88. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hedges Valerie L., Heaton Elizabeth C., Amaral Claudia, Benedetto Lauren E., Bodie Clio L., D’Antonio Breanna I., Davila Portillo Dayana R., et al. 2021. “Estrogen Withdrawal Increases Postpartum Anxiety via Oxytocin Plasticity in the Paraventricular Hypothalamus and Dorsal Raphe Nucleus.” Biological Psychiatry 89 (9): 929–38. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2020.11.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hendrick Victoria, Altshuler Lori L., and Suri Rita. 1998. “Hormonal Changes in the Postpartum and Implications for Postpartum Depression.” Psychosomatics 39 (2): 93–101. 10.1016/S0033-3182(98)71355-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Joel Daphna. 2006. “The Signal Attenuation Rat Model of Obsessive–Compulsive Disorder: A Review.” Psychopharmacology 186 (4): 487–503. 10.1007/s00213-006-0387-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuroda Kumi O., Meaney Michael J., Uetani Noriko, and Kato Tadafumi. 2008. “Neurobehavioral Basis of the Impaired Nurturing in Mice Lacking the Immediate Early Gene FosB.” Brain Research 1211 (May): 57–71. 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.02.100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laurent Heidemarie K., and Ablow Jennifer C.. 2013. “A Face a Mother Could Love: Depression-Related Maternal Neural Responses to Infant Emotion Faces.” Social Neuroscience 8 (3): 228–39. 10.1080/17470919.2012.762039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lavi-Avnon Yael, Weller Aron, Finberg John P. M., Iris Gispan-Herman Noa Kinor, Stern Yaakov, Schroeder Mariana, et al. 2008. “The Reward System and Maternal Behavior in an Animal Model of Depression: A Microdialysis Study.” Psychopharmacology 196 (2): 281–91. 10.1007/s00213-007-0961-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lobo Mary Kay, Zaman Samir, Damez-Werno Diane M., Koo Ja Wook, Bagot Rosemary C., DiNieri Jennifer A., Nugent Alexandria, et al. 2013. “ΔFosB Induction in Striatal Medium Spiny Neuron Subtypes in Response to Chronic Pharmacological, Emotional, and Optogenetic Stimuli.” The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience 33 (47): 18381–95. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1875-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lowes DC, Chamberlin LA, Kretsge LN, Holt ES, Abbas AI, Park AJ, Yusufova L, Bretton ZH, Firdous A, Enikolopov AG, Gordon JA, Harris AZ, 2021. Ventral tegmental area GABA neurons mediate stress-induced blunted reward-seeking in mice. Nat Commun 12, 3539. 10.1038/s41467-021-23906-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McNeilly Alan S. 2001. “Lactational Control of Reproduction.” Reproduction, Fertility and Development 13 (8): 583. 10.1071/RD01056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meltzer-Brody Samantha, Howard Louise M., Bergink Veerle, Vigod Simone, Jones Ian, Munk-Olsen Trine, Honikman Simone, and Milgrom Jeannette. 2018. “Postpartum Psychiatric Disorders.” Nature Reviews Disease Primers 4 (1): 18022. 10.1038/nrdp.2018.22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitra Sudha Warrier, Hoskin Elena, Yudkovitz Joel, Pear Lisset, Wilkinson Hilary A., Hayashi Shinji, Pfaff Donald W., et al. 2003. “Immunolocalization of Estrogen Receptor β in the Mouse Brain: Comparison with Estrogen Receptor α.” Endocrinology 144 (5): 2055–67. 10.1210/en.2002-221069. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morgan Judith K, Guo Chaohui, Moses-Kolko Eydie L, Phillips Mary L, Stepp Stephanie D, and Hipwell Alison E. 2017. “Postpartum Depressive Symptoms Moderate the Link between Mothers’ Neural Response to Positive Faces in Reward and Social Regions and Observed Caregiving.” Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 12 (10): 1605–13. 10.1093/scan/nsx087. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Navarre Brittany M., Laggart Jillian D., and Craft Rebecca M.. 2010. “Anhedonia in Postpartum Rats.” Physiology & Behavior 99 (1): 59–66. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.10.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nestler Eric J. 2008. “Transcriptional Mechanisms of Addiction: Role of ΔFosB.” Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 363 (1507): 3245–55. 10.1098/rstb.2008.0067. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Numan Michael. 2007. “Motivational Systems and the Neural Circuitry of Maternal Behavior in the Rat.” Developmental Psychobiology 49 (1): 12–21. 10.1002/dev.20198. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Numan Michael, Numan Marilyn J., Pliakou Natalia, Stolzenberg Danielle S., Mullins Olivia J., Murphy Jennifer M., and Smith Carl D.. 2005. “The Effects of D1 or D2 Dopamine Receptor Antagonism in the Medial Preoptic Area, Ventral Pallidum, or Nucleus Accumbens on the Maternal Retrieval Response and Other Aspects of Maternal Behavior in Rats.” Behavioral Neuroscience 119 (6): 1588–1604. 10.1037/0735-7044.119.6.1588. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paxinos George, and Franklin Keith B. J.. 2019. Paxinos and Franklin’s The Mouse Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates. Fifth edition. London San Diego Cambridge; MA Kidlington, Oxford: Elsevier, Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
- Peterson Brittni M., Mermelstein Paul G., and Meisel Robert L.. 2015. “Estradiol Mediates Dendritic Spine Plasticity in the Nucleus Accumbens Core through Activation of MGluR5.” Brain Structure and Function 220 (4): 2415–22. 10.1007/s00429-014-0794-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Proaño SB, Morris HJ, Kunz LM, Dorris DM, Meitzen J, 2018. Estrous cycle-induced sex differences in medium spiny neuron excitatory synaptic transmission and intrinsic excitability in adult rat nucleus accumbens core. Journal of Neurophysiology 120, 1356–1373. 10.1152/jn.00263.2018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rincón-Cortés Millie, and Grace Anthony A.. 2020. “Postpartum Changes in Affect-Related Behavior and VTA Dopamine Neuron Activity in Rats.” Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry 97 (March): 109768. 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2019.109768. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Russo Scott J., and Nestler Eric J.. 2013. “The Brain Reward Circuitry in Mood Disorders.” Nature Reviews Neuroscience 14 (9): 609–25. 10.1038/nrn3381. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salamone John D., Pardo Marta, Yohn Samantha E., Laura López-Cruz Noemí SanMiguel, and Correa Mercè. 2015. “Mesolimbic Dopamine and the Regulation of Motivated Behavior.” In Behavioral Neuroscience of Motivation, edited by Simpson Eleanor H. and Balsam Peter D., 27:231–57. Cham: Springer International Publishing. 10.1007/7854_2015_383. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schiller Crystal Edler, O’Hara Michael W., Rubinow David R., and Johnson Alan Kim. 2013. “Estradiol Modulates Anhedonia and Behavioral Despair in Rats and Negative Affect in a Subgroup of Women at High Risk for Postpartum Depression.” Physiology & Behavior 119 (July): 137–44. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2013.06.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seip Katharine M., and Morrell Joan I.. 2009. “Transient Inactivation of the Ventral Tegmental Area Selectively Disrupts the Expression of Conditioned Place Preference for Pup- but Not Cocaine-Paired Contexts.” Behavioral Neuroscience 123 (6): 1325–38. 10.1037/a0017666. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sichel Deborah A., Cohen Lee S., Robertson Laura M., Ruttenberg Alexandra, and Rosenbaum Jerrold F.. 1995. “Prophylactic Estrogen in Recurrent Postpartum Affective Disorder.” Biological Psychiatry 38 (12): 814–18. 10.1016/0006-3223(95)00063-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silverman Michael E., Loudon Holly, Safier Michal, Protopopescu Xenia, Leiter Gila, Liu Xun, and Goldstein Martin. 2007. “Neural Dysfunction in Postpartum Depression: An FMRI Pilot Study.” CNS Spectrums 12 (11): 853–62. 10.1017/S1092852900015595. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Soares Claudio N., and Zitek Brook. 2008. “Reproductive Hormone Sensitivity and Risk for Depression across the Female Life Cycle: A Continuum of Vulnerability?” Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience: JPN 33 (4): 331–43. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Staffend NA, and Meisel RL. 2012. “Aggressive Experience Increases Dendritic Spine Density within the Nucleus Accumbens Core in Female Syrian Hamsters.” Neuroscience 227 (December): 163–69. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.09.064. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stoffel Erin C., and Craft Rebecca M.. 2004. “Ovarian Hormone Withdrawal-Induced ‘Depression’ in Female Rats.” Physiology & Behavior 83 (3): 505–13. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2004.08.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sturm Volker, Lenartz Doris, Koulousakis Athanasios, Treuer Harald, Herholz Karl, Klein Johannes Christian, and Klosterkötter Joachim. 2003. “The Nucleus Accumbens: A Target for Deep Brain Stimulation in Obsessive–Compulsive- and Anxiety-Disorders.” Journal of Chemical Neuroanatomy 26 (4): 293–99. 10.1016/j.jchemneu.2003.09.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Suda Shiro, Segi-Nishida Eri, Newton Samuel S., and Duman Ronald S.. 2008. “A Postpartum Model in Rat: Behavioral and Gene Expression Changes Induced by Ovarian Steroid Deprivation.” Biological Psychiatry 64 (4): 311–19. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.03.029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tekumalla PK, Calon F, Rahman Z, Birdi S, Rajput AH, Hornykiewicz O, Di Paolo T, Bédard PJ, and Nestler EJ. 2001. “Elevated Levels of DeltaFosB and RGS9 in Striatum in Parkinson’s Disease.” Biological Psychiatry 50 (10): 813–16. 10.1016/s0006-3223(01)01234-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tye Kay M., Mirzabekov Julie J., Warden Melissa R., Ferenczi Emily A., Tsai Hsing-Chen, Finkelstein Joel, Kim Sung-Yon, et al. 2013. “Dopamine Neurons Modulate Neural Encoding and Expression of Depression-Related Behaviour.” Nature 493 (7433): 537–41. 10.1038/nature11740. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Willett JA, Will T, Hauser CA, Dorris DM, Cao J, Meitzen J, 2016. No Evidence for Sex Differences in the Electrophysiological Properties and Excitatory Synaptic Input onto Nucleus Accumbens Shell Medium Spiny Neurons. eNeuro 3, ENEURO.0147-15.2016. 10.1523/ENEURO.0147-15.2016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang Rong, Zhang Baofeng, Chen Tingting, Zhang Suyun, and Chen Ling. 2017. “Postpartum Estrogen Withdrawal Impairs GABAergic Inhibition and LTD Induction in Basolateral Amygdala Complex via Down-Regulation of GPR30.” European Neuropsychopharmacology 27 (8): 759–72. 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2017.05.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zambaldi Carla Fonseca, Cantilino Amaury, Ana Carla Montenegro Juliana Alencar Paes, César de Albuquerque Thiago Lucena, and Sougey Everton Botelho. 2009. “Postpartum Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder: Prevalence and Clinical Characteristics.” Comprehensive Psychiatry 50 (6): 503–9. 10.1016/j.comppsych.2008.11.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang Zhuan, Hong Juan, Zhang Suyun, Zhang Tingting, Sha Sha, Yang Rong, Qian Yanning, and Chen Ling. 2016. “Postpartum Estrogen Withdrawal Impairs Hippocampal Neurogenesis and Causes Depression- and Anxiety-like Behaviors in Mice.” Psychoneuroendocrinology 66 (April): 138–49. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.01.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhao Changjiu, Brian Earl Eisinger Terri M. Driessen, and Gammie Stephen C.. 2014. “Addiction and Reward-Related Genes Show Altered Expression in the Postpartum Nucleus Accumbens.” Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 8 (November). 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00388. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]