Abstract
Objective:
Exposure therapy is a promising treatment for eating disorders (EDs). However, questions remain about the effectiveness of exposure to feared foods during the weight restoration phase of treatment, and the importance of between-session and within-session habituation.
Method:
We recruited 54 adolescents from a partial hospitalization program (PHP) for EDs which included daily food exposure. Throughout treatment, participants provided subjective units of distress (SUDS) ratings before and after eating a feared food, and completed measures of ED symptomatology.
Results:
Multilevel models found that pre-exposure SUDS decreased over time, providing some evidence that between-session habituation occurred. In contrast, the difference between pre-exposure and post-exposure SUDS did not decrease over time, indicating that within-session habituation did not occur. Weight gain predicted greater between-session habituation to feared foods, but did not predict within-session habituation. Between-session habituation, but not within-session habituation, predicted favorable treatment outcomes, including weight gain and improvements on the Children’s Eating Attitudes Test and Fear of Food Measure.
Discussion:
PHPs that include daily exposure to feared foods may be effective at decreasing anxiety about foods for adolescents with EDs who are experiencing weight restoration. Further research is warranted to replicate our findings challenging the importance of within-session habituation, and to better understand between-session habituation and inhibitory learning as mechanisms of change when conducting food exposure for EDs.
Keywords: eating disorders, anorexia nervosa, exposure therapy, food, habituation
Exposure therapy is the most effective psychological intervention for anxiety disorders (Abramowitz, 2013), and holds promise as a treatment for eating disorders (EDs). Anxiety disorders and EDs are highly comorbid (Pallister & Waller, 2008), and anxiety is a prominent feature of EDs (Schaumberg et al., 2020). Given the apparent role of anxiety in the development and maintenance of EDs, effective treatment might be achieved by targeting anxious cognitions and avoidance behaviors in patients with EDs via exposure therapy.
According to Foa and Kozak’s (1986) emotional processing theory, exposure therapy works by activating a “fear structure” using in vivo, imaginal, or interoceptive exposure to the feared stimulus (e.g., eating chocolate). Repeated exposure results in the development of safe, non-threatening associations with the feared stimulus, a process that is often referred to as extinction learning (Abramowitz, 2013). During exposure trials, extinction learning is theorized to be evidenced by habituation – or decreases in fear – both within and between exposure sessions.
Despite its initial widespread acceptance, a growing body of research suggests that habituation of fear may not be the operative process through which exposure therapy facilitates extinction learning (Craske et al., 2008). Prior studies have not consistently supported the hypothesis that greater within-session habituation during exposure therapy is associated with greater reductions of fear at follow-up (Peterman et al., 2019). Additionally, while between-session habituation is positively associated with exposure outcomes in some studies, other studies have failed to find this relationship (Knowles & Olatunji, 2019).
Little is known about the relationship between habituation and outcomes in individuals with EDs, and whether clinicians should emphasize or disregard fear reduction when providing exposure therapy for EDs. Studies that have measured changes in subjective units of distress (SUDS) ratings during food exposure for EDs found that individuals commonly report higher SUDS during and immediately after exposure sessions (Boutelle, 1998; Carter et al., 2002; Levinson et al., 2019). While SUDS tend to progressively reduce over the two hours following food exposure (Gray & Hoage, 1990), it is unclear if this decrease in distress is evidence of extinction learning. During this time, some patients may in fact be incorporating information that is incompatible with their feared beliefs about food (e.g., “eating chocolate was not as terrible as I thought it would be”). For other patients, however, reductions in SUDS hours after food exposure may serve to reinforce fears about food (e.g., “I feel much better when I’m not eating fatty foods like chocolate”). Research in the field of EDs is needed to understand the underlying mechanisms involved in exposure therapy’s effectiveness, and whether habituation facilitates extinction learning.
The lack of within-session habituation during food exposure may be related to the longer-term fears often reported by patients with EDs, particularly the belief that feared foods will lead to uncontrollable weight gain over time (Murray et al., 2016). Food exposure may be most effective when patients repeatedly eat feared foods and experience no or modest weight gain, which can be the case when treating patients with bulimia nervosa (BN) (Bulik et al., 2012) and binge-eating disorder (BED) (Grilo et al., 2011). On the other hand, patients who are underweight or weight suppressed – which include all patients with AN and many with other EDs – are expected to gain weight as they modify their eating behaviors. Accordingly, some have suggested that exposure to feared foods during the weight restoration phase of treatment may reinforce the belief that consumption of these foods results in weight gain (Murray et al., 2016).
While research dating back to the 1980s support the efficacy of food exposure for EDs, these studies focused on non-underweight patients with BN (Carter & Bulik, 1994). More recently, investigators have started to evaluate the efficacy of exposure therapy for individuals with AN (Reilly et al., 2017). Food exposure has resulted in increases in calorie intake and reductions in anxiety in weight-restored adults with AN (Steinglass et al., 2014), and significant decreases in ED psychopathology and weight gain in underweight adults with AN (Cardi et al., 2019). Others studies support the effectiveness of exposure therapy when integrated into family-based treatment (FBT) for AN (Hildebrandt et al., 2013), a partial hospitalization program (PHP) for adolescents with EDs (Iniesta Sepulveda et al., 2017), a PHP for adults with EDs (Farrell et al., 2019a), and an inpatient hospital for adults with EDs (Farrell et al., 2019b).
While three of the aforementioned studies investigated food exposure while participants were gaining weight, sample sizes were small (Hildebrandt et al., 2013; Iniesta Sepulveda et al., 2017) and/or the degree of weight gain was modest (Cardi et al., 2019). Additional research with a larger sample of adolescents with EDs undergoing weight restoration is needed to better understand the effectiveness of food exposure and the impact of weight gain on response to food exposure.
Present Study and Hypotheses
In this open trial, adolescents undergoing weight restoration while receiving treatment at a PHP for EDs were repeatedly exposed to a feared food every day. Participants provided SUDS ratings immediately before and after each food exposure, and completed a battery of measures at baseline and every three weeks.
Although debate continues about whether between-session habituation is an important mechanism of change, research supports that between-session habituation is, at the very least, an encouraging marker of change (Maples-Keller & Rauch, 2020). Thus, we hypothesized that participants would experience between-session habituation, or decreases in daily pre-exposure SUDS ratings for exposure foods, over the course of treatment. Despite concerns that weight gain could limit the effectiveness of food exposure (Murray et al., 2016), prior studies suggest that exposure therapy can be effective while patients with EDs are gaining weight (Hildebrandt et al., 2013; Iniesta Sepulveda et al., 2017). Thus, we predicted that weight gain would not impact between-session habituation.
In contrast to findings about between-session habituation, research suggests that individuals with EDs do not reliably report decreases in anxiety within food exposure trials. Thus, we hypothesized that participants would not experience within-session habituation throughout treatment. Additionally, we hypothesized that between-session habituation, but not within-session habitation, would predict improvements in body weight and ED symptomatology over the course of treatment.
Method
Participants
We recruited participants for an open trial study (i.e., no control condition) from a PHP for adolescents with EDs located at an academic medical center. Patients who were identified by a medical provider as requiring an inpatient or residential level of care (e.g., due to risk for refeeding syndrome) would not be accepted into the PHP, and were not included in our pool of potential participants. All patients at this PHP with a diagnosis of AN, BN, BED, and other specified feeding or eating disorder were eligible to participate. Patients with avoidant/restrictive food intake disorder were not eligible.
We approached 84 patients for this study, and obtained parental consent and adolescent assent from 73 patients (86.9% of recruitment pool). We excluded 19 participants (26.0% of initial sample) from analyses, as they did not complete at least two self-reported data collections. All 19 of these excluded participants spent less than one week in the PHP before being discharged, typically to an inpatient facility.
The final sample consisted of 54 participants. Participants were predominately female (n = 44, 81.5%) and White (n = 48, 88.9%). Mean age was 14.38 (SD = 1.64) years. Individuals included those diagnosed with AN (n = 39), atypical AN (n = 13), and BN (n = 2). Reflecting the characteristics of real-world clinical samples, comorbid diagnoses were common: 66.7% of participants were diagnosed with a comorbid anxiety disorder; 53.7% with a depressive disorder; 13.0% with obsessive-compulsive disorder; 11.1% with post-traumatic stress disorder; 3.7% with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder; and 1.9% with a psychotic disorder. The majority of participants (68.5%) were taking at least one psychotropic medication during treatment, including: anti-depressants (68.5% of participants), anti-anxiety medication (20.4%), anti-psychotics (13.0%), anticonvulsants (14.8%), stimulants (1.9%), and other psychiatric medications (1.9%). Length of stay in the program varied from two to 16 weeks, with a mean of 7.89 (SD = 3.03) weeks. Participants and their parent or legal guardian provided consent to participate upon admission to the PHP. This study was approved by the Penn State University intuitional review board.
Treatment
Upon admission to our PHP, participants were evaluated by a psychiatrist and/or clinical psychologist to determine ED and other psychiatric diagnoses. The PHP ran five days per week, and integrated components of CBT (Fairburn, 2008; Waller et al., 2007) and FBT (Lock & Le Grange, 2015). Similar to FBT (Lock & Le Grange, 2015), adolescents’ parents were trained to exert external control to assist their child with meeting “treatment non-negotiables” (Geller & Srikameswaran, 2006) such as adhering to a prescribed meal plan. At the same time, treatment integrated the CBT principles of collaborative empiricism, individual decision-making, and corrective learning as much as possible within the confines of these non-negotiables (Essayli & Vitousek, 2020).
Each day, participants ate an “exposure challenge” food for lunch. Participants were given a full-sized candy bar on Mondays, a baked good (e.g., cookies) on Tuesdays, pizza on Wednesdays, a dessert from the cafeteria on Thursdays, and a challenge breakfast item (e.g., pancakes) on Fridays. These exposure challenges were processed in weekly therapy sessions, which included discussion of participants’ feared predictions about eating these foods.
Measures
Challenge Food Questionnaire
On the Challenge Food Questionnaire, which was created for the present study, participants were asked to provide a SUDS rating immediately before and after each food exposure session on a scale from 0 (no distress) to 10 (extremely high distress). The SUDS scale is commonly used to assess subjective distress, including in individuals with EDs (e.g., Gray & Hoage, 1990; Hildebrandt et al., 2013; Steinglass et al., 2014). Participants’ daily pre-exposure SUDS ratings on this measure were used to determine whether they experienced between-session habituation. Differences between daily pre-exposure and post-exposure SUDS ratings on this measure were used to determine whether participants experienced within-session habituation.
Percent of Median Body Mass Index (%MBMI)
Patients were weighed by program staff at admission, discharge, and at least once per week throughout treatment. %MBMI was calculated according to the 50th percentile BMI-for-age using the 2000 Centers for Disease Control and Prevention growth charts (www.cdc.gov/growthcharts).
Children’s Eating Attitudes Test (ChEAT)
The ChEAT (Maloney et al., 1988) is a 26-item scale that assesses ED attitudes and behaviors in children. The ChEAT was administered at admission, every three weeks, and discharge.
Fear of Food Measure (FOFM)
The FOFM (Levinson & Byrne, 2014) is a 23-item self-report instrument with three subscales: anxiety about eating, food avoidance behaviors, and feared concerns related to eating. The FOFM was administered at admission, every three weeks, and discharge.
Data Analyses
Multilevel models were used to test all hypotheses. Observations (Level 1) were nested within participants (Level 2). Time was modeled as the number of days since admission. Time was centered at day = 0, such that the effect of time was relative to participants’ admission date.
All models were estimated in three steps. First, we ran empty models to obtain the intraclass correlation coefficients. Second, we ran random intercept only models. Third, we ran random intercept and random slope models. Given that the random intercept only models yielded the best fit, we present results from these models.
All analyses were performed in the R software environment (R Core Team, 2021). We used the dplyr package (Wickham, Françios, Henry, & Müller, 2021) for data restructuring, lmer (Bates, Maechler, Bolker, & Walker, 2015) and lmerTest (Kuznetsova, Brockhoff, & Christensen, 2017) for multilevel model estimation and interpretation, and ggplot2 (Wickham, 2016) for constructing figures.
Between-Session Habituation to Daily Food Exposure
To determine whether pre-exposure SUDS decreased over time, we estimated a multilevel model where observations were nested within participants, pre-exposure SUDS was the outcome, time was the predictor, and %MBMI was the covariate. %MBMI was participant-mean centered. Participant-level mean %MBMI was also included as a covariate at Level 2 to disaggregate within-person versus between-person effects of %MBMI.
Within-Session Habituation to Daily Food Exposure
To determine whether the difference between pre-exposure and post-exposure SUDS changed over time, we estimated another multilevel model where observations were nested within participants. The SUDS difference was the outcome, time was the predictor, and %MBMI was the covariate. We also performed a supplementary analysis where we computed each participant’s average pre-exposure SUDS and post-exposure SUDS, and used a paired samples t-test to determine whether post-exposure SUDS were, on average, lower than pre-exposure SUDS.
Participants completed between 4 and 52 daily food exposures throughout treatment; the average number of food exposures was 22.54 (SD = 10.82; median = 21). Given Level 2 and Level 1 sample sizes, at a target level of power ≥ 80%, we are powered to detect effect sizes ≥ approximately .15 (Arend & Schäfer, 2019).
Between-Session and Within-Session Habituation as Predictors of Improvements in Weight and ED Symptomatology
Five models were estimated to investigate between-session habituation and within-session habituation as predictors, with a separate model for each of the following outcomes: %MBMI, ChEAT total score, and the three FOFM subscale scores. The %MBMI model included the average of the pre-exposure SUDS and SUDS difference for all program days prior to and including the date of that day’s weight. The ChEAT and FOFM models included the average of the pre-exposure SUDS and SUDS difference for all program days over the three weeks prior to and including the date of the assessment administration.
Participants completed between two and six symptom assessments throughout the course of treatment; the average number of assessments was 3.68 (SD = 0.93, median = 4). Given Level 2 and Level 1 sample sizes, at a target level of power ≥ 80%, we are powered to detect effect sizes ≥ approximately .23 – .26 (Arend & Schäfer, 2019).
Results
Between-Session Habituation to Daily Food Exposure
As shown in Table 1, the fixed effect of time was negatively predictive of pre-exposure SUDS. This indicates that pre-exposure SUDS significantly decreased between sessions over time. On average, participants’ SUDS decreased from a 6.93 before their first exposure to a 4.56 before their last exposure.
Table 1.
B (95% CI) | SE | t | p | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Pre-exposure SUDS | ||||
Intercept | 1.63 (1.87, 2.80) | 2.88 | 0.57 | .57 |
Day | −0.01 (−0.03, −0.001) | 0.01 | −2.18 | .03 |
%MBMI group-mean centered | −0.09 (−0.15, −0.03) | 0.03 | −2.88 | .004 |
%MBMI group mean | 0.05 (−0.01 0.11) | 0.03 | 1.72 | .09 |
SUDS difference | ||||
Intercept | 0.82 (−0.84, 2.48) | 0.85 | 0.97 | .34 |
Day | −0.005 (−0.01, 0.002) | 0.004 | −1.44 | .15 |
%MBMI group-mean centered | 0.02 (−0.01, 0.06) | 0.02 | 1.33 | .19 |
%MBMI group mean | −0.01 (−0.02, 0.01) | 0.01 | −0.79 | .43 |
Day = centered at 0, indicating day of intake; %MBMI = percent median body mass index; %MBMI group-mean centered = group-mean (i.e., person-mean) centered %MBMI (level 1 variable); %MBMI group mean = group (i.e., person) %MBMI mean used to compute group-mean centered %MBMI (level 2 variable)
In contrast to expectations, the Level 1 effect of %MBMI on pre-exposure SUDS was significant and negative, indicating that, for a given participant, higher than average %MBMI (i.e., being further along in the weight restoration process) was associated with lower pre-exposure SUDS (i.e., greater between-session habituation).
Within-Session Habituation to Daily Food Exposure
The fixed effect of time did not significantly predict the difference between pre- versus post-exposure SUDS (see Table 1), suggesting that within-session habituation did not occur. The Level 1 effect of %MBMI on SUDS difference was not significant, indicating that weight restoration did not impact within-session habituation.
The supplemental paired samples t-test revealed that the average pre-exposure SUDS (mean = 6.19) and average post-exposure SUDS (mean = 6.02) did not significantly differ, [t(53) = 1.78, p = .08].
Between-Session and Within-Session Habituation as Predictors of Weight Gain and Improvements in ED Symptomatology
As shown in Table 2, time in treatment was independently associated with significant %MBMI increases and significant ChEAT and FOFM decreases. On average, participants’ %MBMI increased from 92.14% to 101.07% over the course of treatment. Participants with AN experienced a %MBMI increase from 86.56% to 96.89%.
Table 2.
B (95% CI) | SE | t | p | |
---|---|---|---|---|
%MBMI | ||||
Intercept | 85.99 (77.29, 94.69) | 4.44 | 19.17 | < .001 |
Day | 0.17 (0.16, 0.18) | 0.004 | 40.67 | < .001 |
SUDS pre (L1) | −0.15 (−0.24, −0.06) | 0.05 | −3.32 | < .001 |
SUDS pre (L2) | 1.30 (−0.02, 2.61) | 0.68 | 1.92 | .06 |
SUDS difference (L1) | 0.06 (−0.09, 0.21) | 0.08 | 0.82 | .41 |
SUDS difference (L2) | −0.35 (−4.41, 3.70) | 2.09 | −0.17 | .87 |
ChEAT Total | ||||
Intercept | 0.18 (−10.04, 10.44) | 5.28 | 0.03 | .97 |
Day | −0.22 (−0.29, −0.15) | 0.03 | −6.42 | < .001 |
SUDS pre (L1) | 2.14 (1.10, 3.17) | 0.53 | 4.03 | < .001 |
SUDS pre (L2) | 5.68 (4.15, 7.20) | 0.79 | 7.23 | < .001 |
SUDS difference (L1) | −0.92 (−2.66, 0.82) | 0.89 | −1.03 | .31 |
SUDS difference (L2) | −0.22 (−4.50, 4.08) | 2.21 | −0.10 | .92 |
FOFM Anxiety about Eating | ||||
Intercept | 0.82 (−0.22, 1.86) | 0.54 | 1.53 | .13 |
Day | −0.02 (−0.03, −0.01) | 0.004 | −4.06 | < .001 |
SUDS pre (L1) | 0.40 (0.28, 0.51) | 0.06 | 6.73 | < .001 |
SUDS pre (L2) | 0.63 (0.47, 0.78) | 0.08 | 7.84 | < .001 |
SUDS difference (L1) | −0.07 (−0.26, 0.12) | 0.10 | −0.71 | .48 |
SUDS difference (L2) | −0.11 (−0.54, 0.33) | 0.22 | −0.48 | .64 |
FOFM Food Avoidance Behaviors | ||||
Intercept | 0.74 (−0.21, 1.70) | 0.49 | 1.51 | .14 |
Day | −0.01 (−0.02, −0.007) | 0.003 | −4.20 | < .001 |
SUDS pre (L1) | 0.33 (0.23, 0.43) | 0.05 | 6.61 | < .001 |
SUDS pre (L2) | 0.49 (0.34, 0.63) | 0.07 | 6.59 | < .001 |
SUDS difference (L1) | 0.00 (−0.16, 0.17) | 0.08 | 0.03 | .97 |
SUDS difference (L2) | −0.05 (−0.45, 0.35) | 0.21 | −0.25 | .80 |
FOFM Feared Concerns | ||||
Intercept | 0.93 (0.01, 1.86) | 0.48 | 1.96 | .05 |
Day | −0.01 (−0.02, −0.008) | 0.003 | −4.36 | < .001 |
SUDS pre (L1) | 0.24 (0.13, 0.34) | 0.05 | 4.37 | < .001 |
SUDS pre (L2) | 0.60 (0.46, 0.73) | 0.07 | 8.40 | < .001 |
SUDS difference (L1) | −0.08 (−0.26, 0.11) | 0.09 | −0.81 | .42 |
SUDS difference (L2) | −0.20 (−0.60, 0.19) | 0.20 | −0.99 | .33 |
Day = centered at 0, indicating day of intake; %MBMI = percent median body mass index; SUDS = subjective units of distress; SUDS pre L1 = participant-mean centered pre-exposure SUDS (level 1 variable); SUDS pre L2 = participant mean pre-exposure SUDS (level 2 variable); SUDS difference L1 = participant-mean centered SUDS difference (level 1 variable); SUDS difference L2 = participant mean SUDS difference (level 2 variable); ChEAT = Children’s Eating Attitudes Test; FOFM = Fear of Food Measure
In line with hypotheses, the Level 1 effect of pre-exposure SUDS was significant and negatively related to %MBMI over time. Additionally, the Level 1 effect of pre-exposure SUDS was significant and positively predictive of ChEAT and FOFM scores over time.
Consistent with hypotheses, the Level 1 effect of SUDS difference was not significantly associated with any treatment outcome.
Discussion
Results from this open trial generally supported our hypotheses. Analyses revealed significant decreases in pre-exposure SUDS over time, providing preliminary evidence for between-session habituation across daily exposure sessions. Also in line with hypotheses, participants did not experience within-session habituation, defined as decreases from pre-exposure SUDS to post-exposure SUDS during food exposure trials. These findings add to a growing body of research indicating that individuals with EDs do not reliably experience within-session habituation during food exposure (e.g., Cardi et al., 2019; Carter et al., 2002; Levinson et al., 2019).
Results from analyses investigating treatment outcomes converge with prior research from our PHP (Bryson et al., 2018; Ornstein et al., 2017), and indicated that our exposure-oriented treatment program resulted in improvements for adolescents with AN and other EDs, including weight gain, and decreases in fear of food and other ED symptomatology. Adding to this prior evidence, we found that between-session habituation predicted improvements in these treatment outcomes. Within-session habituation, on the other hand, did not significantly predict any of these outcomes. These findings appear to challenge at least one tenet of the original emotional processing theory (Foa & Kozak, 1986) – the importance of within-session habituation – which may not be a critical mechanism of food exposure when treating adolescents with EDs, at least when within-session habituation is defined as change in SUDS from before eating a feared food to after eating a feared food. In contrast, our findings suggest that between-session habituation across food exposure trials may be a mechanism involved in extinction learning. However, given that this study was an open trial, we cannot conclude that food exposure is driving decreases in SUDS ratings across time. To answer this question, further research should use randomized controlled designs that compare exposure therapy to an alternative treatment.
Likewise, it is unclear whether between-session habituation is a mechanism of change, or merely a marker of extinction learning. Inhibitory learning, which is an alternative to the fear habituation model, suggests that violating expectancies about a feared stimulus and tolerating fear are more important than fear reduction (Craske et al., 2008). Preliminary evidence supports the effectiveness of treatments for EDs that adhere to principles of inhibitory learning, such as the ten-session version of CBT for EDs, which aims to maximize patients’ anxiety rather than achieve habituation using a gradual exposure hierarchy (Waller et al., 2019; Waller & Raykos, 2019). To better understand habituation and inhibitory learning as mechanisms of change in exposure therapy for EDs, future research should measure and compare these putative mechanisms. For example, participants could be asked to report the following across food exposure trials: (1) SUDS ratings to assess between-session habituation; (2) the likelihood that a feared outcome will occur on a 0 – 100 scale to assess expectancy violation; and (3) confidence in one’s ability to tolerate exposure on a 0 – 100 scale to assess fear tolerance (Deacon et al., 2013).
In contrast to expectations, %MBMI significantly predicted reductions in daily pre-exposure SUDS, indicating that weight gain in treatment was associated with greater between-session habituation. These findings suggest that weight gain does not interfere with extinction learning, and may in fact improve learning during food exposure. There are several possible explanations for this finding. Malnutrition and semi-starvation in individuals with AN and other EDs are associated with neurocognitive impairment (Pender et al., 2014; Zwipp et al., 2014) and low estrogen availability (Graham & Milad, 2013; Shufelt et al., 2017), both of which may limit the effectiveness of exposure therapy and other psychological treatments (Schaumberg et al., 2021). Consequently, exposure therapy may have been more effective for participants who gained more weight in treatment. In view of research supporting weight gain early in treatment as a predictor of positive outcomes (Linardon et al., 2016), there may be other variables driving both weight gain and decreases in self-reported SUDS, such as high parental self-efficacy (Byrne et al., 2015; Robinson et al., 2013) or strong internal motivation to recover (Vall & Wade, 2015). Additionally, food exposure may challenge beliefs about weight gain, even as individuals are gaining weight, by violating expectancies about the degree or rate of weight gain (Schaumberg et al., 2021). Moreover, weight gain itself may serve as a form of exposure that violates beliefs about the consequences of weight gain (Levinson et al., 2019), such as “my life will be worse off if I gain weight.”
This open trial has a number of limitations that should be considered when interpreting these findings. Similar to prior research (e.g., Farrell et al., 2019a; Hildebrandt et al., 2013), food exposure was one ingredient of a larger treatment program, and we did not have a control group. Thus, we cannot conclude whether our findings are specifically related to food exposure, other aspects of treatment, and/or other non-treatment variables (e.g., passage of time). Although larger than prior studies investigating the specific effects of food exposure in adolescents with EDs, our sample size of 54 participants was insufficient to detect very small effects. While exposure was a prominent feature of our PHP, the program included other treatments provided by an interdisciplinary team of therapists, physicians, psychiatrists, and dietitians. Without control or comparison groups, we cannot conclude that improvements are specifically related to the effects of food exposure. Our sample was predominantly White, female, and received treatment at a specialized clinic in an academic medical center. Results may not generalize to more diverse populations, older patients, or different treatment settings. With the exception of body weight, we relied on subjective self-report questionnaires to assess distress levels and ED symptomatology. Future research should consider using multimodal methods to evaluate the efficacy of exposure therapy at targeting fear and anxiety, including behavioral, physiological, and neurobiological measures. Participants provided self-report distress ratings immediately before and after eating feared foods, and we did not track SUDS ratings within the session or during longer time intervals following food exposure. Thus, our definition of within-session habituation is limited to this pre-eating to post-eating interval, and we did not capture potential habituation that occurred in the hours following food exposure. Future research should consider tracking SUDS ratings at multiple intervals throughout, and for longer periods following, exposure trials.
Despite these limitations, our findings provide support for integrating food exposure into PHPs for adolescents with EDs undergoing weight restoration. This study also adds to the limited existing knowledge about the relationship between habituation and outcomes of exposure-based treatment. We hope these findings inform larger, controlled studies with multimodal measures that aim to better understand the effectiveness of food exposure, and the mechanisms underlying exposure therapy’s efficacy, for individuals with EDs.
Public Significance Statement:
This study provides some evidence that partial hospitalization programs that include food exposure may be useful for adolescents with eating disorders (EDs) who are experiencing weight restoration. Between-session habituation, but not within-session habituation, predicted favorable treatment outcomes. Further research is needed to determine whether clinicians can disregard within-session habituation when conducting food exposure for EDs, and understand the importance of between-session habituation as a potential mechanism of food exposure.
Funding:
This work was supported by the National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences, National Institutes of Health, through Grant UL1 TR002014.
Footnotes
Conflict of Interest Statement: All authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.
IRB Statement: This study was approved and monitored by the Institutional Review Board of Penn State University.
Data Availability Statement:
The data that support the findings of this study are available on request from the corresponding author. The data are not publicly available due to privacy or ethical restrictions.
References
- Abramowitz JS (2013). The practice of exposure therapy: Relevance of cognitive-behavioral theory and extinction theory. Behavior Therapy, 44(4), 548–558. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arend MG, & Schäfer T (2019). Statistical power in two-level models: A tutorial based on Monte Carlo simulation. Psychological Methods, 24, 1–19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bates D, Maechler M, Bolker B, & Walker (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 1–48. [Google Scholar]
- Boutelle KN (1998). The use of exposure with response prevention in a male anorexic. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 29(1), 79–84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bouton ME (1993). Context, time, and memory retrieval in the interference paradigms of Pavlovian learning. Psychological Bulletin, 114(1), 80–99. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bryson AE, Scipioni AM, Essayli JH, Mahoney JR, & Ornstein RM (2018). Outcomes of low-weight patients with avoidant/restrictive food intake disorder and anorexia nervosa at long-term follow-up after treatment in a partial hospitalization program for eating disorders. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 51(5), 470–474. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bulik CM, Marcus MD, Zerwas S, Levine MD, & La Via M (2012). The changing “weightscape” of bulimia nervosa. American Journal of Psychiatry, 169(10), 1031–1036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Byrne CE, Accurso EC, Arnow KD, Lock J, & Le Grange D (2015). An exploratory examination of patient and parental self-efficacy as predictors of weight gain in adolescents with anorexia nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 48(7), 883–888. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cardi V, Leppanen J, Mataix-Cols D, Campbell IC, & Treasure J (2019). A case series to investigate food-related fear learning and extinction using in vivo food exposure in anorexia nervosa: A clinical application of the inhibitory learning framework. European Eating Disorders Review, 27(2), 173–181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carter FA, & Bulik CM (1994). Exposure treatments for bulimia nervosa: Procedure, efficacy, and mechanisms. Advances in Behavior Research and Therapy, 16(2), 77–129. [Google Scholar]
- Carter FA, Bulik CM, McIntosh VV, & Joyce PR (2002). Cue reactivity as a predictor of outcome with bulimia nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 31(3), 240–250. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Craske MG, Kircanski K, Zelikowsky M, Mystkowski J, Chowdhury N, & Baker A (2008). Optimizing inhibitory learning during exposure therapy. Behavior Research and Therapy, 46(1), 5–27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Craske MG, Treanor M, Conway CC, Zbozinek T, & Vervliet B (2014). Maximizing exposure therapy: An inhibitory learning approach. Behavior Research and Therapy, 58, 10–23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deacon B, Kemp JJ, Dixon LJ, Sy JT, Farrell NR, & Zhang AR (2013). Maximizing the efficacy of interoceptive exposure by optimizing inhibitory learning: A randomized controlled trial. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 51(9), 588–596. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Essayli JH, & Vitousek KM (2020). Cognitive behavioral therapy with eating disordered youth. In Cognitive behavioral therapy in youth: Tradition and innovation (pp. 163–187). Humana. [Google Scholar]
- Fairburn CG (2008). Cognitive behavior therapy and eating disorders. New York, NY: Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
- Farrell NR, Bowie OR, Cimperman MM, Smith BER, Riemann BC, & Levinson CA (2019a). Exploring the preliminary effectiveness and acceptability of food-based exposure therapy for eating disorders: A case series of adult inpatients. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology, 10(1), 1–9. [Google Scholar]
- Farrell NR, Brosof LC, Vanzhula IA, Christian C, Bowie OR, & Levinson CA (2019b). Exploring mechanisms of action in exposure-based cognitive behavioral therapy for eating disorders: The role of eating-related fears and body-related safety behaviors. Behavior Therapy, 50(6), 1125–1135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foa EB, & Kozak MJ (1986). Emotional processing of fear: Exposure to corrective information. Psychological Bulletin, 99(1), 20–35. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Geller J, & Srikameswaran S (2006). Treatment non-negotiables: Why we need them and how to make them work. European Eating Disorders Review, 14(4), 212–217. [Google Scholar]
- Graham BM, & Milad MR (2013). Blockade of estrogen by hormonal contraceptives impairs fear extinction in female rats and women. Biological Psychiatry, 73(4), 371–378. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gray JJ, & Hoage CM (1990). Bulimia nervosa: Group behavior therapy with exposure plus response prevention. Psychological Reports, 66(2), 667–674. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grilo CM, Masheb RM, Wilson GT, Gueorguieva R, & White MA (2011). Cognitive–behavioral therapy, behavioral weight loss, and sequential treatment for obese patients with binge-eating disorder: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 79(5), 675–685. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hildebrandt T, Bacow T, Greif R, & Flores A (2013). Exposure-Based Family Therapy (FBT-E): An open case series of a new treatment for anorexia nervosa. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 21, 470–484. [Google Scholar]
- Iniesta-Sepúlveda M, Nadeau JM, Whelan MK, Oiler CM, Ramos A, Riemann BC, & Storch EA (2017). Intensive family exposure-based cognitive-behavioral treatment for adolescents with anorexia nervosa. Psicothema, 29(4), 433–439. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knowles KA, & Olatunji BO (2019). Enhancing inhibitory learning: The utility of variability in exposure. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 26(1), 186–200. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuznetsova A, Brockhoff PB, & Christensen RHB (2017). lmerTest package: Tests in linear mixed effects models. Journal of Statistical Software, 82(13), 1–26. [Google Scholar]
- Levinson CA, & Byrne M (2014). The fear of food measure: A novel measure for use in exposure therapy for eating disorders. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 48(3), 271–283. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levinson CA, Sala M, Murray S, Ma J, Rodebaugh TL, & Lenze EJ (2019). Diagnostic, clinical, and personality correlates of food anxiety during a food exposure in patients diagnosed with an eating disorder. Eating and Weight Disorders – Studies on Anorexia, Bulimia and Obesity, 24(6), 1079–1088. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levinson CA, Vanzhula IA, & Christian C (2019). Development and validation of the eating disorder fear questionnaire and interview: Preliminary investigation of eating disorder fears. Eating Behaviors, 35, 101320. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Linardon J, Brennan L, & de la Piedad Garcia X (2016). Rapid response to eating disorder treatment: A systematic review and meta-analysis. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 49(10), 905–919. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lock J, & Le Grange D (2015). Treatment manual for anorexia nervosa: A family-based approach. New York, NY: American Psychiatric Association. [Google Scholar]
- Maloney MJ, McGuire JB, & Daniels SR (1988). Reliability testing of a children’s version of the Eating Attitude Test. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 27(5), 541–543. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maples-Keller JL, & Rauch SAM (2020). Habituation. In Abramowitz JS & Blakey SM (Eds.), Clinical handbook of fear and anxiety: Maintenance processes and treatment mechanisms (pp. 249–263). American Psychological Association. 10.1037/0000150-014 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Miller RR, & Matzel LD (1988). The comparator hypothesis: A response rule for the expression of associations. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 22, 51–92. [Google Scholar]
- Murray SB, Treanor M, Liao B, Loeb KL, Griffiths S, & Le Grange D (2016). Extinction theory and anorexia nervosa: Deepening therapeutic mechanisms. Behavior Research and Therapy, 87, 1–10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ornstein RM, Essayli JH, Nicely TA, Masciulli E, & Lane-Loney S (2017). Treatment of avoidant/restrictive food intake disorder in a cohort of young patients in a partial hospitalization program for eating disorders. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 50(9), 1067–1074. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pallister E, & Waller G (2008). Anxiety in the eating disorders: Understanding the overlap. Clinical Psychology Review, 28(3), 366–386. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pender S, Gilbert SJ, & Serpell L (2014). The neuropsychology of starvation: Set-shifting and central coherence in a fasted nonclinical sample. PLoS One, 9(10), e110743. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peterman JS, Carper MM, & Kendall PC (2019). Testing the habituation-based model of exposures for child and adolescent anxiety. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 48(sup1), S34–S44. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- R Core Team (2021). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. [Google Scholar]
- Reilly EE, Anderson LM, Gorrell S, Schaumberg K, & Anderson DA (2017). Expanding exposure-based interventions for eating disorders. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 50(10), 1137–1141. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson AL, Strahan E, Girz L, Wilson A, & Boachie A (2013). ‘I know I can help you’: Parental self-efficacy predicts adolescent outcomes in family-based therapy for eating disorders. European Eating Disorders Review, 21(2), 108–114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schaumberg K, Reilly EE, Gorrell S, Levinson CA, Farrell NR, Brown TA, Smith KM, Schaefer LM, Essayli JH, Haynos AF, & Anderson LM (2021). Conceptualizing eating disorder psychopathology using an anxiety disorders framework: Evidence and implications for exposure-based clinical research. Clinical Psychology Review, 83, 101952. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shufelt CL, Torbati T, & Dutra E (2017). Hypothalamic amenorrhea and the long-term health consequences. Seminars in Reproductive Medicine, 35(3), 256–262. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steinglass JE, Albano AM, Simpson HB, Wang Y, Zou J, Attia E, & Walsh BT (2014). Confronting fear using exposure and response prevention for anorexia nervosa: A randomized controlled pilot study. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 47(2), 174–180. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vall E, & Wade TD (2015). Predictors of treatment outcome in individuals with eating disorders: A systematic review and meta-analysis. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 48(7), 946–971. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Waller G, Cordery H, Corstorphine E, Hinrichsen H, Lawson R, Mountford V, & Russell K (2007). Cognitive behavioral therapy for eating disorders: A comprehensive treatment guide. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Waller G, & Raykos B (2019). Behavioral interventions in the treatment of eating disorders. Psychiatric Clinics, 42(2), 181–191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Waller G, Turner HM, Tatham M, Mountford VA, & Wade TD (2019). Brief cognitive behavioural therapy for non-underweight patients. New York, NY: Routledge. [Google Scholar]
- Wickham H (2016). ggplot2: Elegant graphics for data analysis. Springer-Verlag: New York. [Google Scholar]
- Wickham H, François R, Henry L, & Müller K (2021). dplyr: A grammar of data manipulation. R package version 1.0.5. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=dplyr
- Zwipp J, Hass J, Schober I, Geisler D, Ritschel F, Seidel M, … & Ehrlich S (2014). Serum brain-derived neurotrophic factor and cognitive functioning in underweight, weight-recovered and partially weight-recovered females with anorexia nervosa. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 54, 163–169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
The data that support the findings of this study are available on request from the corresponding author. The data are not publicly available due to privacy or ethical restrictions.