Skip to main content
UKPMC Funders Author Manuscripts logoLink to UKPMC Funders Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2018 Oct 19.
Published in final edited form as: Schizophr Bull. 2018 Oct 17;44(6):1301–1311. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbx180

Defining the Locus of Dopaminergic Dysfunction in Schizophrenia: A Meta-analysis and Test of the Mesolimbic Hypothesis

Robert McCutcheon 1,2,3,4, Katherine Beck 1,2,3,4, Sameer Jauhar 1,2,3,4, Oliver D Howes 1,2,3,4,*
PMCID: PMC5933516  EMSID: EMS75942  PMID: 29301039

Abstract

Background

Studies using positron emission tomography to image striatal dopamine function, have demonstrated that individuals with schizophrenia display increases in presynaptic function. Mesolimbic dysfunction specifically, has previously been suggested to underlie psychotic symptoms. This has not been directly tested in vivo, and the precise anatomical locus of dopamine dysfunction within the striatum remains unclear. The current article investigates the magnitude of dopaminergic abnormalities in individuals with schizophrenia, and determines how the magnitude of abnormality varies across functional subdivisions of the striatum.

Methods

EMBASE, PsychINFO, and MEDLINE were searched from January 1, 1960, to December 1, 2016. Inclusion criteria were molecular imaging studies that had measured presynaptic striatal dopamine functioning. Effects sizes for whole striatum and functional subdivisions were calculated separately. The magnitude of difference between functional subdivisions in patients and controls was meta-analyzed.

Results

Twenty-one eligible studies were identified, including 269 patients and 313 controls. Individuals with schizophrenia (Hedges’ g = 0.68, P < .001) demonstrated elevated presynaptic dopamine functioning compared to controls. Seven studies examined functional subdivisions. These demonstrated significant increases in patients compared to controls in associative (g = 0.73, P = .002) and sensorimotor (g = 0.54, P = .005) regions, but not limbic (g = 0.29, P = .09). The magnitude of the difference between associative and limbic subdivisions was significantly greater in patients compared to controls (g = 0.39, P = .003).

Conclusion

In individuals with schizophrenia dopaminergic dysfunction is greater in dorsal compared to limbic subdivisions of the striatum. This is inconsistent with the mesolimbic hypothesis and identifies the dorsal striatum as a target for novel treatment development.

Keywords: PET, neuroimaging, nigrostriatal, F-DOPA, amphetamine

Introduction

Dysfunction of the dopamine system is one of the most well established findings in schizophrenia.14 Initial evidence was mostly indirect: based on preclinical work, the behavioral effects of drugs, and post-mortem studies.5 The development of positron emission tomography (PET) and single-photon emission computed tomography (SPECT), allowed the dopamine system to be studied in vivo in individuals with schizophrenia.6 Initial studies employed ligands specific to dopamine receptors, and allowed the quantification of receptor availability, while later work was able to investigate dopamine synthesis and release, and other aspects of dopaminergic function. Previous meta-analyses of these imaging studies have found that the major dopaminergic abnormality in schizophrenia is increased presynaptic activity in the striatum.1,3 While an elevation of postsynaptic D2 receptors has also been proposed, meta-analytic findings have been less convincing,1 although the presynaptic results raise the possibility that receptor differences may be masked by increased endogenous dopamine levels.79

Although cortical dopaminergic functioning has also been studied in schizophrenia,10,11 the main anatomical focus for investigations of dopamine dysfunction has been the striatum. Animal research has demonstrated that the striatum can be divided into 3 distinct subregions based on function and the predominant topography of brain projections from limbic, associative, and sensorimotor cortical areas to the striatum (figure 1).12,13 The antero-ventral striatum receives projections from limbic areas such as the orbital frontal cortex and medial temporal lobe, and consequently has been termed the limbic striatum. Anatomically it comprises the nucleus accumbens, and ventral parts of the caudate and putamen. The associative striatum, involved in higher cognitive function, receives projections primarily from cortical regions involved in executive and other higher cognitive processes, such as the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, and is made up of the majority of the caudate, and the pre-commisural putamen. Finally, the sensorimotor striatum, involved in sensory and motor processing, receives afferent projections predominantly from sensory, motor, and premotor areas and consists of the postcommisural putamen. More recent imaging studies have indicated that this topography is paralleled in the human brain.14,15

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

The topography of cortical afferents to the striatum illustrating the functional subdivisions.

Primarily based on preclinical research, dopaminergic hyperactivity of the limbic striatum has long been hypothesized as underlying psychotic symptoms.1619 In vivo evidence for a specific mesolimbic abnormality has, however, been lacking. Initial imaging studies did not have sufficient resolution to visualize these subdivisions, and so reported values for either the whole striatum, or the anatomical divisions of caudate and putamen. However, improvements in PET cameras over the past decade have subsequently allowed dopaminergic function to be measured in these functional subdivisions. Work undertaken by Laruelle, Mawlawi, Martinez and colleagues,20,21 defined these subregions based on anatomical landmarks to allow the consistent reporting of subdivision findings in PET studies, and Howes, Egerton and colleagues determined the reliability of this approach.22 Initial studies using these functional divisions suggested that the greatest abnormality was within the associative striatum.8,23 Several further studies have since been performed, but the results have not been meta-analyzed.

In the current article, we aim to test the mesolimbic hypothesis by comparing the magnitude of dysfunction between the limbic and other striatal subdivisions. We also provide an update to previous meta-analyses of striatal dopamine function given that a significant number of studies have been published since previous reviews.

Methods

EMBASE, PsychINFO and MEDLINE were searched from 1960 (or 1974 in the case of EMBASE), to December 31, 2016. Titles and abstracts were searched for the words: (“schizophrenia” or “psychosis” or “schizophreniform”) AND (“Positron Emission Tomography” or “PET” or “Single photon emission tomography” or SPET or “Single Photon Emission Computed Tomography” or SPECT) AND (Dopamine).

For the meta-analysis of presynaptic dopamine function in schizophrenia the inclusion criteria were: (1) studies of patients with schizophrenia diagnosed in accordance with criteria specified in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual for Mental Disorders (DSM), or the International Classification of Diseases (ICD)24,25 and a control group; (2) reporting molecular imaging measures of presynaptic dopaminergic function (see supplementary methods for further details) for both the patient and control groups; (3) providing data enabling the estimation of mean difference between control and clinical groups for the dopaminergic measure; and (4) For the subdivision analysis only studies reporting all 3 subdivisions (limbic, associative, and sensorimotor subdivisions) were included to enable comparisons across regions.

Studies reporting data on dopaminergic functioning in individuals with treatment resistant schizophrenia, or co-morbid substance dependence, were excluded. This is because the primary neurobiological abnormality in these patients may not involve striatal hyperdopaminergia.2629

Data Extraction

The primary outcome of interest was the dopamine imaging parameter reported for the patient and control groups. For studies using labeled L-DOPA this was the influx constant in the region of interest relative to uptake in the reference region, while for studies using a release or depletion paradigm this was percent change in binding potential. In addition, author, year of study, number of participants, participant age and gender, illness duration, antipsychotic treatment, symptom scores, scan length, and whether an arterial input function was used were extracted.

Two studies23,30 reporting data in individuals with schizophrenia were not included due to sample overlap with Howes et al 2013.31 Where values for the whole striatum were not given but data for the caudate and putamen were reported, whole striatum values were calculated as described previously1 by weighting these values by their volumes as reported in the Oxford-GSK-Imanova Structural–Anatomical Striatal Atlas (43% and 57% respectively). If the ventral striatum was also reported the following weightings were used to derive a summary outcome for the whole striatum: caudate – 36%, putamen-putamen – 48%, ventral striatum – 16%.32

Statistical Analysis

All statistical analyses were carried out using the “meta-for” package (version 1.99) in the statistical programming language R (version 3.3.1). A minimum of 3 studies was required for meta-analysis. Standard effect sizes (Hedges’ g) for individual studies were estimated. The individual study effect sizes were then entered into a random effects meta-analytic model using restricted maximum likelihood estimation. I2 values were calculated to estimate between study heterogeneity. Where there were at least 10 studies included in a meta-analysis, funnel plots were constructed and visually inspected, and Egger’s regression test performed to check for the possibility of publication bias.33 Secondary subgroup and meta-regression analyses were undertaken to investigate the relationship between dopaminergic function and antipsychotic treatment (studies where ≥75% of patients were antipsychotic naïve were grouped as studies of predominantly antipsychotic naïve patients), scan length, paradigm type, modeling techniques, patient age and severity of symptoms.34 The statistical significance of differences between subgroups was tested for by fitting separate random effects models for each subgroup, and then comparing the subgroup estimates in a fixed effects model with a Wald-type test. A significance level of P < .05 (2-tailed) was used for all analyses.

To test the hypothesis that dopamine dysfunction is primarily located in limbic regions we first determined if there was a significant difference between patients and controls for each individual subdivision. We next calculated the magnitude of subdivision differences within group, and then determined whether the size of these differences significantly differed between groups (see below and supplementary information for further details).

In order to contrast and quantify the degree of dysfunction between subdivisions, a meta-analysis of difference was undertaken. In this we performed an inter-group (patient vs control) comparison of the magnitude of intra-group subdivision differences (eg, associative vs limbic). This approach employs methods used to quantify the propagation of errors.35 For each study, mean within subject differences in presynaptic function between subdivisions were calculated for both patient and control groups. For example, for patients the mean difference between associative and limbic measurements (al) equals:

P¯al=P¯aP¯l(P¯a=meanassociativevalue)(P¯l=meanlimbicvalue)

In order to calculate the standard deviation of this mean difference, a correlation coefficient for presynaptic functioning between subdivisions is required (see supplementary information and supplementary eFigure 1 for full methods).36 We estimated this correlation coefficient from individual data for 37 subjects (21 controls and 16 individuals with schizophrenia).37 This showed Pearson’s coefficients of 0.72, 0.84, and 0.87 for correlations between sensorimotor-limbic, associative-limbic, and associative-sensorimotor divisions respectively. To be conservative the lowest of these values (0.72) was used for all comparisons. For example, to calculate the standard deviation of the limbic-associative difference in a patient group35:

σPal=σPa2+σPl22ralσPaσPlσPal=StandarddeviationoflimbicassociativedifferenceσPa=StandarddeviationofassociativesubdivisionvaluesσPl=Standarddeviationoflimbicsubdivisionvaluesral=Correlationbetweenlimbicandassociativesubdivsionvalues

We repeated the exercise to calculate the control mean difference (al), and standard deviation (σCal), and then calculated the combined standard deviation of both groups (σPCal) .

σPCal=(nP1)σPal2+(nC1)σCal2nP+nC2nP=NumberofpatientsnC=Numberofcontrols

The between groups effect size for the study was then calculated for each subdivision using this standard deviation as follows:

ES=P¯alC¯alσPCal

This was converted to the bias corrected Hedges g,38 which was then entered into the standard meta-analytic model described above. For further information regarding methods see supplementary information.

Results

A total of 1798 papers were identified. 21 of these met inclusion criteria (PRISMA flow diagram in supplementary eFigure 2).

Studies of the Whole Striatum

21 studies of individuals with schizophrenia met inclusion criteria (see table 1 for study details). The studies included a total of 269 patients (256 with a diagnosis of schizophrenia, 3 schizoaffective disorder, and 10 a mixture of schizophrenia/ schizophreniform disorder) and 313 controls. Presynaptic dopamine function was significantly elevated in individuals with schizophrenia relative to controls with a summary effect size of 0.68 (see figure 2, 95% CI 0.44–0.91; P < .001). Egger’s regression test was not significant (z = 1.21, P = .23), indicating publication bias was unlikely. Visual inspection of the funnel plot potentially suggested asymmetry (supplementary eFigure 3), but a trim and fill analysis did not indicate any missing studies. The I2 value was 42.5%, suggesting a low to moderate level of heterogeneity. Subgroup meta-analysis of studies of predominantly drug naïve patients, and of patients who were receiving antipsychotic treatment, found a greater effect size in drug naïve patients (g = 0.78, P < .001 and .64, P < .001 respectively, see supplementary eFigure 5) but this difference was not statistically significant (P = .59). Studies using a challenge or depletion paradigm (g = 0.95, P < .001) showed a greater effect size when compared to those using labeled L-DOPA (g = 0.52, P < .001), and this difference was statistically significant (P = .049, see supplementary eFigure6). Neither scan time (P = .44) nor the use of an arterial input function (P = .55) was significantly associated with magnitude of effect size in the labeled L-DOPA studies. Meta-regressions of effect sizes against age (P = .29), total symptoms (P = .16), and positive symptoms (P = .39) were not significant.

Table 1.

Studies of Presynaptic Dopamine Function in Individuals With Schizophrenia

Controls
Patients
Scan Details
Study N Age
Mean
(SD)/yr
N Age Diagnosis Illness
Duration/
mo
Antipsychotic
Treatment
Total
Symptom
Score
Positive
Symptom
Score
Negative
Symptom
Score
Outcome
Measure
PET Tracer
and Method
Reith 199439 13 36(13)   5 38(4) Scz 168 4 naïve, 1 free >3 yr PANSS 58 PANSS 14(3) PANSS 12(2) K3 [18F]DOPA
Hietala 199540   8 27 (7)   7 26 (7) Scz   24 All naive PANSS 81(14) na na Ki [18F]DOPA
Dao-Castellana 199741   7 25 (5)   6 26 (9) Scz   72 2 naïve 4 free ≥4 mo PANSS 94 (na) PANSS 21 (12) PANSS 33 (7) Ki [18F]DOPA
Breier 199742 12 29.2 (9.0) 11 32.4 (10.0) Scz   79.2 4 naïve, 7 free for >14 d BPRS 28.8 (7.2) BPRS 6.7 (2.8) na % Δ BPND [11C]Raclopride AMPH challenge
Hietala 199943 13 30.4 (9.4) 10 29.6 (8.8) 7 Scz 3 SczAf     7 All naïve PANSS 77.6 (na) Na na Ki [18F]DOPA
Lindström 199944 10 n/a 12 31(na) Scz   31 12 naïve, 2 drug free > 2 yr na na na Ki [11C]DOPA
Laruellea 199945 36 40 (9) 34 40 (9) Scz na 7 naïve, 27 free mean 104 d na 17.5 (6.2) 16.8(6.6) % Δ BPND [123I]IBZM AMPH
Elkashef 200046 13 34.6 (10.8) 19 36.3 (na) Scz 207.6 10 medicated 10 drug free na na na uptake ratio:str/ref [18F]DOPA
Abi-Dargham 20009 18 31 (8) 18 31 (8) Scz na 8 naïve, 10 free for mean 139 d 66.6 18.2 (6) 13.8(5.4) % Δ BPND [123I]IBZM AMPt
Meyer Lindenberg 200247   6 34 (na)   6 35 (na) Scz na All free ≥ 6 wk na na Na Ki [18F]DOPA
Kumakura 200748 15 37.3 (6.4)   8 37.3 (6.3) Scz na 3 naïve, 6 free for ≥ 6 mo PANSS 80.2 (4.7) PANSS 15.4 (3.5) PANSS 23.6 (4.0) Kinapp [18F]DOPA
Nozaki 200949 20 35.1 (9.5) 18 35.6 (7.4) Scz   26.4 14 naïve, 4 free PANSS 79.2 (21.4) PANSS 22.6(7.3) PANSS 17.1(6.5) Ki [11C]DOPA
Kegeles 20108 18 29 (7) 18 29 (8) Scz na 6 naïve, 4 free ≥1 yr, 8 free for ≥20 d PANSS 78.6 (20.6) PANSS 21.7 (7.1) PANSS 17.1 (5.9) % Δ BPND [11C]Raclopride AMPT depletion
Shotbolt 201150 20 43 (12)   7 43 (12) Scz na All medicated PANSS 56.8 (25.4) PANSS 13.5 (6.7) PANSS 15 (4.9) Ki [18F]DOPA
Pogarell 201251   7 3.6 (2.7)   8 25.4 Scz   24 Free for 1 wk PANSS 76(18) na na % Δ BPND [123I]IBZM AMPH
Mizrahi 201252 12 26.1 (3.83) 10 24.1 (5.0) Scz/Sczform na All naïve na PANSS 19.0 (3.8) na % Δ BPND [11C]-(+)-PHNO MIST
Demjaha 201226 12 44.0 (11.9) 12 44.2 (8.9) Scz 194.4 All medicated PANSS 50.7 (5.8) PANSS 11.9 (2.4) na Ki [18F]DOPA
Howesb 201331 29 29.3 (7.5) 29 33.7 (10.6) Scz na 16 medicated, 8 free, 5 naïve CASH 77.6 (47.6) CASH 38.3 (30) CASH 31.9 (22.9) Ki [18F]DOPA
Caravaggio 201553 10 29.1 (8.4)   3 30 (16) Scz na All medicated na na Na % Δ BPND [11C]-(+)-PHNO AMPT depletion
Kim 201654 12 30.3 (8.4) 12 31.1 (9.8) Scz 111.3 All medicated PANSS 50.3 (11.1) PANSS 10.8 (2.7) PANSS 13.2 (5.2) Ki [18F]DOPA
Jauhar 201737 22 24.5 (4.5) 16 26.3 (4.4) Scz   24 11 naïve, 3 free PANSS 72.9 (16.5) PANSS 17.8(6.3) PANSS 18.8(4.1) Ki [18F]DOPA

Note: AMPH, amphetamine; AMPT, alpha-methyl-para-tyrosine; BP, Binding Potential; BPRS, Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale; CASH, Comprehensive Assessment of Symptoms and History; Ki, utilization rate constant of DOPA relative to a reference region; Kinapp, net blood-brain DOPA clearance; MIST, Montreal Imaging Stress Test; na, not available; PANSS, Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale; Ref, reference region; Scz, schizophrenia; Sczform, Schizophreniform; SczAf, Schizoaffective disorder; Str, striatum.

a

Includes all subjects from Laruelle et al55 and Abi-Dargham et al.56

b

Includes the entire sample from McGowan et al.30

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Forest plot of studies investigating presynaptic dopaminergic function in the whole striatum for individuals with schizophrenia. The forest plot shows the effect size (hedges g) and 95% CI for the difference between patients and controls. There is a significant elevation in schizophrenia with a summary effect size of 0.68.

Studies of Limbic, Associative, and Sensorimotor Subdivisions

Seven studies of individuals with schizophrenia reported associative, sensorimotor and limbic subdivisions. These reported data on a total of 104 patients with schizophrenia (schizophrenia or schizophreniform disorder), and 174 controls. All 7 studies used the subdivision definitions proposed by Mawlawi et al,21 and Martinez et al.20

Significant differences were found between patients and controls for associative (schizophrenia – g = 0.73, P = .002) and sensorimotor (schizophrenia – g = 0.54, P = .009) subdivisions, but not for the limbic subdivision (schizophrenia – g = 0.29, P = .09) (see figures 3A–C). The results for the associative subdivision showed the greatest heterogeneity (I2 = 58.3%), with sensorimotor (I2 = 37.7%), and limbic subdivisions (I2 = 29.5%) showing relatively low levels of heterogeneity.

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3

Studies of presynaptic dopamine function in individuals with schizophrenia by functional subdivisions. Significant elevations are seen for the associative and sensorimotor, but not limbic subdivisions. In schizophrenia the associative-limbic and sensorimotor-limbic differences are significantly greater in patients than in controls. (A) Effect size and 95% CI of difference in dopamine function in schizophrenia between patients and controls showing significant elevations in patients in associative (g = 0.73) and sensorimotor (g = 0.54) subdivisions but not limbic. (B) Effect sizes and 95% CIs of subdivision differences in schizophrenia between patients and controls. Patients show significantly greater associative-limbic (d = 0.38) and sensorimotor-limbic (d = 0.29) differences compared to controls. (C) Magnitude of patient-control differences in presynaptic dopamine functioning for striatal subdivisions in individuals with schizophrenia (*P < .05 for patient-control comparison), error bars represent 1 SE).

In individuals with schizophrenia, the difference between associative and limbic subdivisions was significantly greater in patients compared to controls (see figure 3C; effect size, g = 0.38, P = .004). Presynaptic dopaminergic function in schizophrenia was also significantly greater in the sensorimotor compared to the limbic subdivision compared to the difference in controls (g = 0.29, P = .03). There were no significant patient-control differences as regards the comparisons between dopamine function in the associative and sensorimotor subdivision (g = 0.08, P = .55). These comparisons showed low levels of heterogeneity (associative-limbic I2 = 25.5%, other comparisons I2 = 0).

Discussion

Our main finding is that individuals with schizophrenia display greater elevation in dopaminergic functioning in the dorsal (sensorimotor and associative) relative to limbic striatum compared controls (figure 3). Moreover, there was no significant difference in presynaptic dopaminergic functioning between patients and controls for the limbic subdivision. This is, to our knowledge, the first study to meta-analyze differences between functional subdivisions of the striatum. Our analysis of the whole striatum included 8 additional studies published since previous reviews but is consistent with their findings in showing an increase in schizophrenia.1,3

Methodological Considerations

Moderate heterogeneity was seen in the studies of individuals with schizophrenia. Methodological factors such as differences in the resolution of scanners, measurement time, experimental paradigm, and modeling technique may contribute to this heterogeneity. In addition, differences in the clinical characteristics of patients could contribute to between study heterogeneity, given findings that increased dopaminergic activity is linked to acute psychosis.45,57,58 Some studies included antipsychotic treated patients. However, our sub-analysis in antipsychotic free/naïve patients showed no statistically significant difference between these groups, and the elevation in presynaptic dopamine function was numerically larger in naïve patients than in antipsychotic treated patients, indicating antipsychotic treatment is unlikely to account for the elevation we see. Moreover chronic antipsychotic treatment may reduce dopamine synthesis capacity in some patients.59

We combined studies using challenge and depletion paradigms with those using radiolabeled DOPA. While there is some evidence that results from challenge paradigms are directly related to results from radiolabeled DOPA studies,60,61 it should be recognized that these measures are indexing different, albeit related, aspects of dopaminergic function, and could be influenced by different factors. Interestingly our sensitivity analysis found that effects were greater for the challenge/depletion studies (supplementary efigure 6), which could suggest that these aspects of the dopamine system are particularly affected in schizophrenia.

Another factor contributing to heterogeneity could be the inclusion of individuals with treatment resistant schizophrenia, or with co-morbid substance dependence, given recent findings these groups may show reduced presynaptic dopamine functioning.26,28,29,62 While we excluded studies specifically including these patients, many studies pre-dated these recent findings and did not specify these as exclusion criteria. As such it is likely that some of the included studies may have contained treatment resistant patients; indeed 2 studies report including patients taking clozapine.9,46 However this would, if anything reduce effect sizes given treatment resistant patients do not seem to show presynaptic dopamine elevation.26,54

We examined the difference between subdivisions, as, in the absence of individual patient data, this measure can be more accurately estimated than the ratio between subdivisions. A potential drawback of our measure is that if, eg, associative values are greater than limbic values, then a uniform proportionate increase in dopaminergic function across the whole striatum in the clinical group would lead to a greater absolute increase in the associative striatum, and thus give a larger associative-limbic difference. In our case, however, only 2 of the 7 control groups had a value for the associative region that was greater than the limbic value.31,37 Therefore, if anything, effects related to general increases in striatal functioning would reduce the magnitude of our findings.

When examining the differences between subdivisions, the assumed correlation between subdivisions has an influence on the precision of the estimated magnitude of difference between subdivisions, with a stronger correlation leading to larger effect sizes. The correlation coefficient we employed, however, was conservative, using the lowest of the correlation coefficients between subdivisions that we found in individual participant data. Using the largest coefficient of 0.87 gave an effect size of 0.50 (P = .01 for associative limbic measure, and 0.29 (P = .01) for the sensorimotor-limbic measure (supplementary efigure 5). Thus, the differences we report may underestimate the magnitude of the true difference.

The limbic striatum has a smaller volume than either the associative or sensorimotor subdivisions. As a result it is more susceptible to partial volume effects whereby its true activity may be diluted by spill over and spill in from adjacent regions.63 However, given that there is no consistent evidence of reduced limbic striatal volumes in schizophrenia this would be expected to affect measures in patients and controls equally.6467 Moreover one study employed partial volume correction and found a significant elevation in the associative striatum, but not in the limbic striatum in schizophrenia and clinical high risk groups relative to controls,52 consistent with our meta-analytic findings. The fact that measures of dopamine functioning in the limbic striatum may be less reliable compared to measures in other subdivisions does mean, however, that it is possible the reduced limbic effect size (figure 3A) could be at least partially due to the increased noise inherent in measuring this region.22,68 This possibility is supported by some26,52 (but not all31,37,54) studies where the variance of the limbic measure, is noticeably greater than the variance of the associative measure.

Neither partial volume effects, nor reduced signal-to-noise, however, would account for the patient-control differences found when examining subdivision differences directly (figure 3B). In this case we are, eg, looking at limbic-associative differences in patients, and comparing this to the limbic-associative differences in controls. A reduction in signal-to-noise for the limbic measure will therefore affect patient and control findings equally, and will not bias the results. This means that while the reduced reliability of limbic measurements may increase the risk of a false negative, in this specific analysis it will not increase the likelihood of a false positive.

The Anatomical Locus of Dopaminergic Dysfunction in Psychosis

Our meta-analysis confirms, using a larger sample, the previous meta-analytic findings of increased presynaptic dopamine functioning in schizophrenia in the striatum.1 Moreover, our meta-analysis extends understanding of the nature of dopamine dysfunction in psychosis by showing that the degree of dopaminergic dysfunction varies across the striatum, and identifies the dorsal striatum as the predominant locus of dopamine dysfunction in psychosis. Although patients showed no significant alteration in the limbic striatum relative to controls, we cannot rule out the possibility of a small difference in this subdivision. Nevertheless, in patients the dorsal to ventral balance was significantly shifted dorsally in patients when compared to controls. While a small mesolimbic abnormality may exist, overall these findings are not consistent with a hypothesis which proposes that the predominant locus of dopamine dysfunction is the limbic striatum.

Our findings thus suggest that models highlighting a primary role for excessive mesolimbic dopamine transmission in psychosis may need to be revised.1214,29 The associative subdivision receives dopaminergic innervation from the substantia nigra,12 suggesting that nigrostriatal pathways may be disrupted in schizophrenia. This hypothesis is in keeping with findings of increases in some,31,48 although not all,10 aspects of dopamine functioning within the substantia nigra in schizophrenia. The elevation was greatest in the associative striatum, although this was not significantly greater than the elevation in the sensorimotor striatum.

It should be noted, that while our findings support the hypothesis that dopaminergic functioning within the associative striatum may be abnormal in schizophrenia, this does not preclude the possibility that the primary site of dysfunction exists in another brain region.5 The associative part of the dorsal striatum receives projections predominantly from dorso-lateral prefrontal cortex.13 Thus the dorsal locus of dopamine abnormality is consistent with the hypothesis that frontal cortical dysfunction underlies striatal dopamine abnormalities,16,69 although causality remains to be established in clinical studies.

Our findings also question the proposal that mesolimbic selectivity is a desirable property for pharmacological treatments of schizophrenia,70 and suggest instead that selectivity for the dorsal, particularly associative, striatum may show advantages in both efficacy and tolerability. Treatment strategies may be able to make use of the neurochemical distinctions found across striatal subdivisions. For example, dopamine transporter densities are greater in the ventral, compared to dorsal, striatum.71 Due to this variable distribution, combination therapy with a dopamine reuptake inhibitor and D2 antagonist could potentially reduce dopaminergic neurotransmission to a greater degree in the dorsal, as opposed to ventral striatum. There are potential risks to this approach, but evidence suggests that in some patients it may have benefits for the amelioration of negative symptoms.72

In conclusion, current molecular neuroimaging studies suggest that in individuals with schizophrenia the major locus of dopamine dysfunction is the dorsal striatum, and significant elevations were not seen in the limbic striatum. These findings are inconsistent with the mesolimbic hypothesis of schizophrenia, and suggest treatments showing nigro-striatal rather mesolimbic selectivity may have better efficacy and tolerability.

Supplementary Material

Supplementary material is available at Schizophrenia Bulletin online.

Supplementary Material

Acknowledgments

R.M., K.B., and S.J. declare no conflicts of interest. O.D.H. has received investigator-initiated research funding from and/or participated in advisory/ speaker meetings organized by Astra-Zeneca, Autifony, BMS, Eli Lilly, Heptares, Jansenn, Lundbeck, Lyden-Delta, Otsuka, Servier, Sunovion, Rand, and Roche. Neither O.D.H. or his family have been employed by or have holdings/ a financial stake in any biomedical company.

Funding

This work was supported by Medical Research Council-UK (no. MC-A656-5QD30), Maudsley Charity (no. 666), Brain and Behavior Research Foundation, and Wellcome Trust (no. 094849/Z/10/Z) grants to O.D.H., a Wellcome trust (no. 200102/Z/15/Z) grant to R.M., and the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Biomedical Research Centre at South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust and King’s College London. The views expressed are those of the author(s) and not necessarily those of the NHS, the NIHR or the Department of Health.

References

  • 1.Howes OD, Kambeitz J, Kim E, et al. The nature of dopamine dysfunction in schizophrenia and what this means for treatment. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2012;69:776–786. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2012.169. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Howes OD, McCutcheon R, Owen MJ, Murray RM. The role of genes, stress, and dopamine in the development of schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2017;81:9–20. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2016.07.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Fusar-Poli P, Meyer-Lindenberg A. Striatal presynaptic dopamine in schizophrenia, part II: meta-analysis of [(18)F/(11)C]-DOPA PET studies. Schizophr Bull. 2013;39:33–42. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbr180. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Fusar-Poli P, Meyer-Lindenberg A. Striatal presynaptic dopamine in schizophrenia, Part I: meta-analysis of dopamine active transporter (DAT) density. Schizophr Bull. 2013;39:22–32. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbr111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Howes O, McCutcheon R, Stone J. Glutamate and dopamine in schizophrenia: an update for the 21st century. J Psychopharmacol. 2015;29:97–115. doi: 10.1177/0269881114563634. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Howes OD, Egerton A, Allan V, McGuire P, Stokes P, Kapur S. Mechanisms underlying psychosis and antipsychotic treatment response in schizophrenia: insights from PET and SPECT imaging. Curr Pharm Des. 2009;15:2550–2559. doi: 10.2174/138161209788957528. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Seeman P, Kapur S. Schizophrenia: more dopamine, more D2 receptors. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2000;97:7673–7675. doi: 10.1073/pnas.97.14.7673. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Kegeles LS, Abi-Dargham A, Frankle WG, et al. Increased synaptic dopamine function in associative regions of the striatum in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67:231–239. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Abi-Dargham A, Rodenhiser J, Printz D, et al. Increased baseline occupancy of D2 receptors by dopamine in schizophrenia. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2000;97:8104–8109. doi: 10.1073/pnas.97.14.8104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Slifstein M, van de Giessen E, Van Snellenberg J, et al. Deficits in prefrontal cortical and extrastriatal dopamine release in schizophrenia. JAMA Psychiatry. 2015;10032:1–9. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.2414. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Kambeitz J, Abi-Dargham A, Kapur S, Howes OD. Alterations in cortical and extrastriatal subcortical dopamine function in schizophrenia: systematic review and meta-analysis of imaging studies. Br J Psychiatry. 2014;204:420–429. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.113.132308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Haber SN, Fudge JL, McFarland NR. Striatonigrostriatal pathways in primates form an ascending spiral from the shell to the dorsolateral striatum. J Neurosci. 2000;20:2369–2382. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-06-02369.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Haber SN. Corticostriatal circuitry. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2016;18:7–21. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2016.18.1/shaber. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Di Martino A, Scheres A, Margulies DS, et al. Functional connectivity of human striatum: a resting state FMRI study. Cereb Cortex. 2008;18:2735–2747. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Draganski B, Kherif F, Klöppel S, et al. Evidence for segregated and integrative connectivity patterns in the human Basal Ganglia. J Neurosci. 2008;28:7143–7152. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1486-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Weinberger D. Implications of normal brain development for the pathogenesis of schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1987;45:1055. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1987.01800190080012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Davis KL, Kahn RS, Ko G, Davidson M. Dopamine in schizophrenia: a review and reconceptualization. Am J Psychiatry. 1991;148:1474–1486. doi: 10.1176/ajp.148.11.1474. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Sadock BJ, Sadock VA. Kaplan & Sadock’s Concise Textbook of Clinical Psychiatry. Philadelphia: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Stahl S. Stahl’s Essential Psychopharmacology: Neuroscientific Basis and Practical Applications. Cambridge University Press; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Martinez D, Slifstein M, Broft A, et al. Imaging human mesolimbic dopamine transmission with positron emission tomography. Part II: amphetamine-induced dopamine release in the functional subdivisions of the striatum. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. 2003;23:285–300. doi: 10.1097/01.WCB.0000048520.34839.1A. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Mawlawi O, Martinez D, Slifstein M, et al. Imaging human mesolimbic dopamine transmission with positron emission tomography: I. Accuracy and precision of D(2) receptor parameter measurements in ventral striatum. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. 2001;21:1034–1057. doi: 10.1097/00004647-200109000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Egerton A, Demjaha A, McGuire P, Mehta MA, Howes OD. The test-retest reliability of 18F-DOPA PET in assessing striatal and extrastriatal presynaptic dopaminergic function. Neuroimage. 2010;50:524–531. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Howes OD, Montgomery AJ, Asselin MC, et al. Elevated striatal dopamine function linked to prodromal signs of schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2009;66:13–20. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2008.514. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.World Health Organization. International Classification of Diseases, 10th Edition (ICD-10): Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders. Geneva: World Health Organization; 1992. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5®) Washington: American Psychiatric Pub; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Demjaha A, Murray RM, McGuire PK, Kapur S, Howes OD. Dopamine synthesis capacity in patients with treatment-resistant schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2012;169:1203–1210. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12010144. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Howes OD, Kapur S. A neurobiological hypothesis for the classification of schizophrenia: type A (hyperdopaminergic) and type B (normodopaminergic) Br J Psychiatry. 2014;205:1–3. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.113.138578. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Thompson JL, Urban N, Slifstein M, et al. Striatal dopamine release in schizophrenia comorbid with substance dependence. Mol Psychiatry. 2013;18:909–915. doi: 10.1038/mp.2012.109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Bloomfield MA, Morgan CJ, Egerton A, Kapur S, Curran HV, Howes OD. Dopaminergic function in cannabis users and its relationship to cannabis-induced psychotic symptoms. Biol Psychiatry. 2014;75:470–478. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.05.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.McGowan S, Lawrence AD, Sales T, Quested D, Grasby P. Presynaptic dopaminergic dysfunction in schizophrenia: a positron emission tomographic [18F]fluorodopa study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2004;61:134–142. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.61.2.134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Howes OD, Williams M, Ibrahim K, et al. Midbrain dopamine function in schizophrenia and depression: a post-mortem and positron emission tomographic imaging study. Brain. 2013;136:3242–3251. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt264. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Tziortzi AC, Searle GE, Tzimopoulou S, et al. Imaging dopamine receptors in humans with [11C]-(+)-PHNO: dissection of D3 signal and anatomy. Neuroimage. 2011;54:264–277. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.06.044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Sterne JA, Sutton AJ, Ioannidis JP, et al. Recommendations for examining and interpreting funnel plot asymmetry in meta-analyses of randomised controlled trials. BMJ. 2011;343:d4002. doi: 10.1136/bmj.d4002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Higgins JP, Green S. Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions. London: John Wiley & Sons; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Ku HH. Notes on the use of propagation of error formulas. J Res Natl Bur Stand Sect C Eng Instrum. 1966;70C:263. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Borenstein M, Hedges LV, Higgins JPT, Rothstein HR. Introduction to Meta-Analysis - Complex Data Structures. London: John Wiley & Sons; 2009. [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Jauhar S, Nour MM, Veronese M, et al. A test of the transdiagnostic dopamine hypothesis of psychosis using positron emission tomographic imaging in bipolar affective disorder and schizophrenia. JAMA Psychiatry. 2017 doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2017.2943. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Hedges LV. Distribution theory for glass’s estimator of effect size and related estimators. J Educ Stat. 1981;6:107. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Reith J, Benkelfat C, Sherwin A, et al. Elevated dopa decarboxylase activity in living brain of patients with psychosis. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1994;91:11651–11654. doi: 10.1073/pnas.91.24.11651. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Hietala J, Syvälahti E, Vuorio K, et al. Presynaptic dopamine function in striatum of neuroleptic-naive schizophrenic patients. Lancet. 1995;346:1130–1131. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(95)91801-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Dao-Castellana MH, Paillère-Martinot ML, Hantraye P, et al. Presynaptic dopaminergic function in the striatum of schizophrenic patients. Schizophr Res. 1997;23:167–174. doi: 10.1016/S0920-9964(96)00102-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Breier A, Su TP, Saunders R, et al. Schizophrenia is associated with elevated amphetamine-induced synaptic dopamine concentrations: evidence from a novel positron emission tomography method. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1997;94:2569–2574. doi: 10.1073/pnas.94.6.2569. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Hietala J, Syvälahti E, Vilkman H, et al. Depressive symptoms and presynaptic dopamine function in neuroleptic-naive schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 1999;35:41–50. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(98)00113-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Lindström LH, Gefvert O, Hagberg G, et al. Increased dopamine synthesis rate in medial prefrontal cortex and striatum in schizophrenia indicated by L-(beta-11C) DOPA and PET. Biol Psychiatry. 1999;46:681–688. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(99)00109-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Laruelle M, Abi-Dargham A, Gil R, Kegeles L, Innis R. Increased dopamine transmission in schizophrenia: relationship to illness phases. Biol Psychiatry. 1999;46:56–72. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(99)00067-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Elkashef AM, Doudet D, Bryant T, Cohen RM, Li SH, Wyatt RJ. 6-(18)F-DOPA PET study in patients with schizophrenia. Positron emission tomography. Psychiatry Res. 2000;100:1–11. doi: 10.1016/s0925-4927(00)00064-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Meyer-Lindenberg A, Miletich RS, Kohn PD, et al. Reduced prefrontal activity predicts exaggerated striatal dopaminergic function in schizophrenia. Nat Neurosci. 2002;5:267–271. doi: 10.1038/nn804. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Kumakura Y, Cumming P, Vernaleken I, et al. Elevated [18F] fluorodopamine turnover in brain of patients with schizophrenia: an [18F]fluorodopa/positron emission tomography study. J Neurosci. 2007;27:8080–8087. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0805-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Nozaki S, Kato M, Takano H, et al. Regional dopamine synthesis in patients with schizophrenia using L-[beta-11C] DOPA PET. Schizophr Res. 2009;108:78–84. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2008.11.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Shotbolt P, Stokes PR, Owens SF, et al. Striatal dopamine synthesis capacity in twins discordant for schizophrenia. Psychol Med. 2011;41:2331–2338. doi: 10.1017/S0033291711000341. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Pogarell O, Koch W, Karch S, et al. Dopaminergic neurotransmission in patients with schizophrenia in relation to positive and negative symptoms. Pharmacopsychiatry. 2012;45(suppl 1):S36–S41. doi: 10.1055/s-0032-1306313. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Mizrahi R, Addington J, Rusjan PM, et al. Increased stress-induced dopamine release in psychosis. Biol Psychiatry. 2012;71:561–567. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.10.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Caravaggio F, Borlido C, Wilson A, Graff-Guerrero A. Examining endogenous dopamine in treated schizophrenia using [11C]-(+)-PHNO positron emission tomography: a pilot study. Clin Chim Acta. 2015;449:60–62. doi: 10.1016/j.cca.2015.03.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Kim E, Howes OD, Veronese M, et al. Presynaptic dopamine capacity in patients with treatment-resistant schizophrenia taking clozapine: an [18F]DOPA PET study. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2017;42:941–950. doi: 10.1038/npp.2016.258. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Laruelle M, Abi-Dargham A, van Dyck CH, et al. Single photon emission computerized tomography imaging of amphetamine-induced dopamine release in drug-free schizophrenic subjects. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1996;93:9235–9240. doi: 10.1073/pnas.93.17.9235. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Abi-Dargham A, Gil R, Krystal J, et al. Increased striatal dopamine transmission in schizophrenia: confirmation in a second cohort. Am J Psychiatry. 1998;155:761–767. doi: 10.1176/ajp.155.6.761. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Valli I, Howes O, Tyrer P, McGuire P, Grasby PM. Longitudinal PET imaging in a patient with schizophrenia did not show marked changes in dopaminergic function with relapse of psychosis. Am J Psychiatry. 2008;165:1613–1614. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08071109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Howes O, Bose S, Turkheimer F, et al. Progressive increase in striatal dopamine synthesis capacity as patients develop psychosis: a PET study. Mol Psychiatry. 2011;16:885–886. doi: 10.1038/mp.2011.20. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Gründer G, Vernaleken I, Müller MJ, et al. Subchronic haloperidol downregulates dopamine synthesis capacity in the brain of schizophrenic patients in vivo. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2003;28:787–794. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Abi-Dargham A, van de Giessen E, Slifstein M, Kegeles LS, Laruelle M. Baseline and amphetamine-stimulated dopamine activity are related in drug-naïve schizophrenic subjects. Biol Psychiatry. 2009;65:1091–1093. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.12.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Piccini P, Pavese N, Brooks DJ. Endogenous dopamine release after pharmacological challenges in Parkinson’s disease. Ann Neurol. 2003;53:647–653. doi: 10.1002/ana.10526. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Mizrahi R, Kenk M, Suridjan I, et al. Stress-induced dopamine response in subjects at clinical high risk for schizophrenia with and without concurrent cannabis use. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2014;39:1479–1489. doi: 10.1038/npp.2013.347. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Kim E, Shidahara M, Tsoumpas C, et al. Partial volume correction using structural-functional synergistic resolution recovery: comparison with geometric transfer matrix method. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. 2013;33:914–920. doi: 10.1038/jcbfm.2013.29. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.van Haren NEM, Schnack HG, Koevoets MGJC, Cahn W, Hulshoff Pol HE, Kahn RS. Trajectories of subcortical volume change in schizophrenia: a 5-year follow-up. Schizophr Res. 2016;173:140–145. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2015.09.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Gunduz H, Wu H, Ashtari M, et al. Basal ganglia volumes in first-episode schizophrenia and healthy comparison subjects. Biol Psychiatry. 2002;51:801–808. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(01)01345-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Bois C, Levita L, Ripp I, et al. Hippocampal, amygdala and nucleus accumbens volume in first-episode schizophrenia patients and individuals at high familial risk: a cross-sectional comparison. Schizophr Res. 2015;165:45–51. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2015.03.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Lauer M, Senitz D, Beckmann H. Increased volume of the nucleus accumbens in schizophrenia. J Neural Transm (Vienna) 2001;108:645–660. doi: 10.1007/s007020170042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Yoder KK, Albrecht DS, Kareken DA, et al. Test-retest variability of [11C]raclopride-binding potential in nontreatment-seeking alcoholics. Synapse. 2011;65:553–561. doi: 10.1002/syn.20874. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Howes OD, Kapur S. The dopamine hypothesis of schizophrenia: version III–the final common pathway. Schizophr Bull. 2009;35:549–562. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbp006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Kinon BJ, Lieberman JA. Mechanisms of action of atypical antipsychotic drugs: a critical analysis. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1996;124:2–34. doi: 10.1007/BF02245602. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Haber SN, Knutson B. The reward circuit: linking primate anatomy and human imaging. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35:4–26. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.129. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Lindenmayer JP, Nasrallah H, Pucci M, James S, Citrome L. A systematic review of psychostimulant treatment of negative symptoms of schizophrenia: challenges and therapeutic opportunities. Schizophr Res. 2013;147:241–252. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2013.03.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary Material

RESOURCES