Skip to main content
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America logoLink to Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
. 2023 Apr 27;120(18):e2216792120. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2216792120

M1-selective muscarinic allosteric modulation enhances cognitive flexibility and effective salience in nonhuman primates

Seyed A Hassani a, Adam Neumann a, Jason Russell b,c, Carrie K Jones b,c, Thilo Womelsdorf a,d,1
PMCID: PMC10161096  PMID: 37104474

significance

Muscarinic receptors mediate the procognitive effects of acetylcholine, but it has remained unclear whether they differentially affect the cognitive subfunctions of attentional filtering, set shifting, and learning. To clarify the functional specificity of M1 mAChRs, we assessed these diverse functions using a recently developed, highly selective M1 PAM. This M1 PAM caused domain-specific cognitive improvement of flexible learning and extradimensional set shifting, reduced perseverations and enhanced target recognition during learning without altering attentional filtering functions. These domain-specific improvements contrasted to effects of a nonselective acetylcholinesterase inhibitor that primarily enhanced attention and caused dose-limiting adverse side effects. These results demonstrate domain-specific improvements in cognitive flexibility suggesting M1 PAMs are versatile compounds for treating cognitive deficits in schizophrenia and Alzheimer’s disease.

Keywords: acetylcholine, attention, donepezil, cognitive control, learning

Abstract

Acetylcholine (ACh) in cortical neural circuits mediates how selective attention is sustained in the presence of distractors and how flexible cognition adjusts to changing task demands. The cognitive domains of attention and cognitive flexibility might be differentially supported by the M1 muscarinic acetylcholine receptor (mAChR) subtype. Understanding how M1 mAChR mechanisms support these cognitive subdomains is of highest importance for advancing novel drug treatments for conditions with altered attention and reduced cognitive control including Alzheimer’s disease or schizophrenia. Here, we tested this question by assessing how the subtype-selective M1 mAChR positive allosteric modulator (PAM) VU0453595 affects visual search and flexible reward learning in nonhuman primates. We found that allosteric potentiation of M1 mAChRs enhanced flexible learning performance by improving extradimensional set shifting, reducing latent inhibition from previously experienced distractors and reducing response perseveration in the absence of adverse side effects. These procognitive effects occurred in the absence of apparent changes of attentional performance during visual search. In contrast, nonselective ACh modulation using the acetylcholinesterase inhibitor (AChEI) donepezil improved attention during visual search at doses that did not alter cognitive flexibility and that already triggered gastrointestinal cholinergic side effects. These findings illustrate that M1 mAChR positive allosteric modulation enhances cognitive flexibility without affecting attentional filtering of distraction, consistent with M1 activity boosting the effective salience of relevant over irrelevant objects specifically during learning. These results suggest that M1 PAMs are versatile compounds for enhancing cognitive flexibility in disorders spanning schizophrenia and Alzheimer’s diseases.


Cholinergic activity has far reaching consequences on attention and attentional control functions (1, 2) with long-standing suggestions that cholinergic modulation is involved in faster updating of expectations during learning (35). Depleting cholinergic innervation to the prefrontal cortex compromises while stimulation of cholinergic activity can enhance attentional control functions (610). These cholinergic effects have been suggested to be supported differently by nicotinic versus muscarinic receptors (11, 12). Antagonizing muscarinic cholinergic action with scopolamine in healthy humans and nonhuman primates (NHPs) increases false alarm rates and impairs sustained attention by slowing response times and impairing signal detection in two-alternative choice tasks (1317). Consistent with these behavioral effects, neuronal recordings in the prefrontal cortex of NHPs have shown that attentional signaling depends on muscarinic receptor activation (18). One key open question from these insights is to what extent are attentional subcomponent processes supported by muscarinic signaling and whether there are subreceptors of the muscarinic receptor family that differentially support separable subcomponent processes underlying attention, such as filtering of distracting information, enhancing the flexible updating and shifting of attention sets, or supporting robust goal representations during goal-directed behavior. Each of these subcomponent processes has been associated in prior studies with the M1 mAChR, which is widely expressed in the cortex, striatum, and hippocampus (19, 20) and may thus mediate some of these muscarinic procognitive effects (2, 21).

One set of prior studies has implicated the M1 mAChR in memory processes because M1-selective ligands can restore deficits in novel object recognition (22, 23) and can partially reverse scopolamine-induced deficits in contextual fear conditioning consistent with M1-selective compounds enhancing the salience of the (aversive) outcomes during learning (22, 24, 25). But it has remained unclear whether the effects described in these studies are best accounted for by an improvement of memory, or whether enhanced cognitive control processes contribute to more effective encoding of stimuli as opposed to enhancing learning processes. A similar question about the specific cognitive process that is modulated arises when considering the M1 mAChR effects on different forms of attentional performance. While some studies have shown that M1 mAChR modulation is important for attentional modulation of neural firing (18, 26), behavioral studies using M1-selective PAMs in NHPs (27) and rodents (28) have not found primary improvements of sustained attention performance. Rather than modulating attention, the M1 mAChR actions improved behavior only in more demanding task conditions in which M1 modulation enhanced the adjustment of performance when task requirements changed (28). These results are consistent with findings showing that selective M1 mAChR ligands can facilitate the use of complex sensorimotor transformations to reach a goal (as in object-retrieval detour tasks) (29), and improve odor-based reversal learning of objects (30). These cognitive enhancing effects suggest that M1 mAChRs may be particularly important for higher cognitive control processes that go beyond attentional focusing or the filtering of distraction (2). However, it is not apparent which particular control processes might be supported by M1 mAChRs as the existing studies used widely varying tasks and a study using one of the classical cognitive control task (the antisaccade task) was not successful in identifying neural correlates of M1 mAChR–specific effects in the prefrontal cortex of NHPs (31).

The current study set out to address these questions about the specific procognitive role of the M1 mAChR in supporting attention and learning functions. Firstly, to distinguish cognitive subcomponent processes we devised two tasks. A visual search task allowed for distinguishing attentional subcomponent processes by varying distractor load and perceptual interference. And a intra-/extradimensional set-shifting learning task distinguishing cognitive control processes and cognitive flexibility. Secondly, we assessed NHP performance in these tasks using VU0453595, which is a recently developed subtype selective PAM for the M1 mAChR that does not activate the receptor directly but substantially potentiates the M1 mAChR response to endogenous ACh (22, 32, 33). This selective M1 PAM mechanism does not produce the dose limiting side effects which are prevalent with existing orthosteric mAChR agonists and AChEIs (34, 35), and which has the potential to treat deficits in attention control and cognitive rigidity prevalent in schizophrenia, Alzheimer’s disease, and substance use disorders (3640). Assessing the procognitive profile of VU0453595 for these higher cognitive functions is therefore pivotal to advance therapeutic solutions for these widespread neuropsychiatric conditions (22, 41, 42).

We found that the M1 PAM VU0453595 exerts an inverted-U–shaped improvement of cognitive flexibility with increasing dose, causing faster learning, extradimensional set shifting, and reduced perseverations (i.e., enhanced flexibility), while leaving attentional filtering during visual search unaffected. These results are contrasted to the nonselective AChEI donepezil which improved attentional filtering with only marginal effects on cognitive flexibility (43).

Results

We used four male rhesus macaques, with ages ranging from 7 to 11 y old, as subjects. Each of the four monkeys completed 60 sessions composed of 40 vehicle days and 7 d for each of the three tested doses of VU0453595 (0.3, 1, and 3 mg/kg). No dose-limiting adverse effects were observed in any of the 21 VU0453595 dosing days in any of the monkeys.

M1 PAM VU0453595 Enhances Learning.

Animals performed and consistently completed all 21 blocks of the feature learning task per session in all experimental conditions and expectedly showed faster learning in the easier low distractor load condition versus the more difficult high distractor load condition (Fig. 1 C and D). Administration of VU0453595 improved multiple measures of the learning performance compared to the vehicle control condition. In order to capture changes in learning speed, we calculated the trials-to-criterion as the first trial in a block that led to >70% accurate performance in the subsequent 10 trials. To reveal any temporally specific effects on learning, we implemented a linear mixed effects model on the median trials-to-criterion for the feature-reward learning (FRL) task (SI Appendix, Supplemental Methods). We found faster learning with 1 mg/kg dosing as evident in the early, middle, and last thirds of the 21 learning blocks per session with the first third of blocks showing the strongest effects (1 mg/kg, fixed effect: t(3674) = −2.67, P = 0.008; first third Cohen’s d = −0.228; overall Cohen’s d = −0.061) (SI Appendix, Fig. S1A). For this reason, all future analyses of the FRL task use the first third of blocks. Faster learning was particularly evident at low distractor load for which animals reached the trials-to-criterion at 7.93 (SE: 0.81) trials after a block switch with 1 mg/kg, compared to 11.03 (SE: 0.38) trials with vehicle [F(3,691) = 3.54, P = 0.01; η2 = .015; Tukey’s, P = 0.028; Cohen’s d = −0.352] (Fig. 1E). After the performance criterion was reached, VU0453595 also enhanced plateau performance (SI Appendix, Fig. S1B) and increased the proportion of blocks in which the animals reached the learning criterion at the 1 mg/kg dose (SI Appendix, Fig. S1C).

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1.

Task Design and Feature-Reward Learning Task Performance Enhancement by VU0453595 (A) Images of cage-mounted kiosk and monkey Ba utilizing the touch screen to perform the feature-reward learning (FRL) task taken via the video monitoring system. Both images are taken from the same time point from different angles. (B) Trial progression of the FRL task (Top) and the VS task (Bottom). The example FRL task here is a block with “high distractor load” where objects vary in both color and pattern. Each object contains only 1 feature from each feature dimension. Although the red checkered object was correctly chosen in this trial, the animal would need to learn through trial and error if the red or checkered feature was correct in order to optimally acquire reward from future objects in this block. The example VS block here shows a trial with three distractors and a target object that is defined by three features: blue, striped, and straight conical arms. The red distractor has zero features in common with the target, the yellow distractor has one feature in common with the target (striped pattern) and the blue distractor has two features in common with the target (blue color and straight conical arms). Trials in either task were initiated by a 0.3 to 0.5 s touch and hold of a central blue square (3° visual radius wide) after which the square disappears (for 0.3 to 0.5 s) and task objects (2.5° visual radius wide) are presented on screen. For the VS task, a structured background scene (here: a lawn) was used to distinguish the VS from the FRL task (which had a grey uniform background). For each visual search block, we drew a different random background scene from a set of five backgrounds independent of the target search feature. In either task, subjects have 5 s to select one of the objects with a 0.2 s touch and hold. Failure to choose an object resulted in an aborted trial which was ignored. Feedback for choice selection was provided 0.2 s after object selection for 0.5 s via both a visual halo around the chosen object as well as a auditory cue alongside any earned fluid. Both the frequency of the audio feedback and color of the feedback halo differed based on outcome. (C) Block-wise average learning curves for the low distractor load blocks of the FRL task aligned to block start for vehicle, 0.3, 1, and 3 mg/kg VU0453595, smoothed after the first three trials with a sliding window (shaded area: SE). Dotted horizontal lines signify 0.33 and 0.66 probabilities. (D) The same as C but for the high distractor load blocks. (E) Median trials-to-criterion, calculated as the first trial in a block that led to >70% performance over 10 subsequent trials, for the low and high distractor load blocks of the FRL task. For the low distractor load blocks, trials-to-criterion were 11.03 (SE: 0.38), 8.94 (SE: 0.75), 7.93 (SE: 0.81) and 10.88 (SE: 0.94) for vehicle, 0.3, 1, and 3 mg/kg doses of VU0453595, respectively. Only the 1 mg/kg dose was significantly different from vehicle [F(3,691) = 3.54, P = 0.01; η2 = .015; Tukey’s, P = 0.028; Cohen’s d = −0.352). For the high distractor load blocks, trials-to-criterion were 12.85 (SE: 0.43), 13.56 (SE: 1.03), 11.65 (SE: 1.00), and 12.92 (SE: 0.98) for vehicle, 0.3, 1, and 3 mg/kg doses of VU0453595, respectively, with no significant effect [F(3,565) = .40, n.s.].

Faster learning and improved performance accuracy in the 1 mg/kg dose condition were accompanied by faster response times (RTs). Over the course of a learning block, subjects showed a characteristic change of RTs with fast RTs early in the block that slowed down until an inflection point around the trial within the block when animals began to more consistently choose the rewarded target feature (Fig. 2 A and B). Notably, administering the middle (1 mg/kg) dose of VU0453595 led to significantly faster RTs of 870 ms (SE: 23 ms; low load) and 960 ms (SE: 23 ms; high load) in the low- and high-load conditions relative to vehicle RTs of 960 ms (SE: 11 ms; low load) and 984 ms (SE: 11 ms; high load) [F(3,1672) = 2.97, P = 0.03; η2 = 0.005; Tukey’s, P = 0.04; Cohen’s d = −0.350] (Fig. 2C). Moreover, the number of trials needed for the RTs to reach this inflection point was significantly fewer with the 1 mg/kg dose taking until trial 6.5 (SE: 0.5) relative to vehicle taking until trial 8.7 (SE: 0.3) [F(3,193) = 2.67, P < 0.05; η2 = 0.040; Tukey’s, P = 0.03; Cohen’s d = −0.674) (Fig. 2D).

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2.

Feature-Reward Learning Task Efficiency and cognitive flexibility improvements with VU0453595 (A) The average RT curve of each session (correct trials only) aligned to block start for the low distractor load blocks of the FRL task for vehicle, 0.3, 1 and 3 mg/kg doses of VU0453595 (shaded area: SE) (B) The same as A but for high distractor load blocks of the FRL task. (C) Block-wise averages of the traces plotted in A and B visualized to compare RTs between distractor load conditions. Low distractor load blocks had RTs of 960 ms (SE: 11), 923 ms (SE: 24), 870 ms (SE: 23) and 974 ms (SE: 23) for vehicle, 0.3, 1, and 3 mg/kg doses of VU0453595, respectively. High distractor load blocks had RTs of 984 ms (SE: 11), 965 ms (SE: 26), 960 ms (SE: 23), and 937 ms (SE: 22). Only the 1 mg/kg dose of VU0453595 was significantly different from vehicle [F(3,1672) = 2.97, P = 0.03; η2 = 0.005; Tukey’s, P = 0.04; Cohen’s d = −0.350] (D) Trials-to-inflection for RTs in the low distractor load blocks defined as the first trial per block (excluding trial 2) that RTs become faster (error bars: SE). Trials-to-inflection was 8.7 (SE: 0.3), 8.0 (SE: 0.6), 6.5 (SE: 0.5) and 8.5 (SE: 0.5) for vehicle, 0.3, 1, and 3 mg/kg doses of VU0453595, respectively. Only the 1 mg/kg dose was significantly different from vehicle [F(3,193) = 2.68, P < 0.05; η2 = .040; Tukey’s, P = 0.017; Cohen’s d = −0.674]. (E) Block-wise average trials-to-criterion after extradimensional shifts were 12.2 (SE:1.0), 8.9 (SE: 2.4), 4.0 (SE: 0.7) and 9.3 (SE: 1.4) for vehicle, 0.3, 1, and 3 mg/kg doses of VU0453595, respectively. Only the 1 mg/kg dose showed a significant difference from vehicle [F(3,122) = 3.15, P = 0.03; η2 = 0.072; Tukey’s, P = 0.02; Cohen’s d = −0.868]. (F) Block-wise average trials-to-criterion after intradimensional shifts were 12.6 (SE: 0.5), 10.0 (SE: 0.7), 9.3 (SE: 0.7) and 12.3 (SE: 1.1) for vehicle, 0.3, 1, and 3 mg/kg doses of VU0453595, respectively. Only the 1 mg/kg dose showed a significant difference from vehicle [F(3,518) = 3.26, P = 0.02; η2 = .019; Tukey’s, P = 0.04; Cohen’s d = −0.349]. (G) The proportion of errors that were perseverative in nature with the feature that was perseverated being from the same feature dimension as the target feature. The proportion of perseverative errors from the target feature dimension were 10.7% (SE: 0.2), 11.5% (SE: 0.7), 8.5% (SE: 0.6) and 11.0% (SE: 0.7) for vehicle, 0.3, 1, and 3 mg/kg doses of VU0453595, respectively, with only the 1 mg/kg dose being significantly different from vehicle [F(3,1679) = 3.74, P = 0.01; η2 = 0.007; Tukey’s, P = 0.01; Cohen’s d = −0.243]. (H) The same as G but with the feature that was perseverated being from the distracting feature dimension (different from the target feature dimension). Proportions of perseverative errors from the distracting feature dimension were 17.3% (SE: 0.3), 19.6% (SE: 0.8), 15.6% (SE: 1.0) and 15.6% (SE: 1.0) for vehicle, 0.3, 1, and 3 mg/kg doses of VU0453595, respectively. There was a nonsignificant trend for a main effect of experimental condition [F(3,844) = 2.36, P = 0.07; η2 = 0.008].

Improved Cognitive Control with M1 PAM VU0453595.

Learning a new feature-reward rule following a block switch entailed either identifying a target feature that was new or from a different feature dimension as in the previous block (extradimensional switches, ED), or from the same feature dimension as the previous target (intradimensional switches, ID). We found that the 1 mg/kg dose with VU0453595 significantly improved learning for both, ED and ID switches (Fig. 2 E and F) but not switches where the current target was from a novel feature dimension (data not shown). A large improvement was evident for ED switches with the average trials-to-criterion of 4.0 (SE: 0.7) after 1 mg/kg dose administration being significantly lower than the average 12.2 (SE: 1.0) trials-to-criterion of the vehicle condition [F(3,122) = 3.15, P = 0.03; η2 = .072; Tukey’s, P = 0.02; Cohen’s d = −0.868] (Fig. 2E). Please note that ED switches reported in our task were to a target of the previous distractor feature dimension and thus required disengaging from that dimension in addition to identifying the newly rewarded dimension. ID switches had a more moderate but still significant advantage after administration of 1 mg/kg dose of VU0453595 with a trials-to-criterion of 9.3 (SE: 0.7) relative to 12.6 (SE: 0.5) with vehicle [F(3,518) = 3.26, P = 0.02; η2 = 0.019; Tukey’s, P = 0.04; Cohen’s d = −0.349] (Fig. 2F).

The learning advantage after ED and ID switches indicates that VU0453595 at the 1 mg/kg dose enhanced cognitive control. Cognitive control also entails the ability to avoid erroneous perseverative responding. We quantified the perseverative responses as the proportion of repeated unrewarded choices to a feature in the target-feature dimension or in distractor-feature dimensions. We found that VU0453595 reduced perseverative responding to other features in the target feature dimension at 1 mg/kg from the 10.7% (SE: 0.2) of vehicle down to 8.5% (SE: 0.6) [F(3,1679) = 3.74, P = 0.01; η2 = 0.007; post hoc analysis of 1 mg/kg condition Tukey’s, P = 0.01; Cohen’s d = −0.243] (Fig. 2G). Perseverative responding to objects with features of the distractor dimension was moderately, but nonsignificantly reduced with VU0453595 [F(3,844) = 2.36, P = 0.07; η2 = 0.008] (Fig. 2H).

M1 PAM VU0453595 Has No Consistent Effect on Interference Control.

Cholinergic compounds modulate attention and interference control (12, 43, 44). We evaluated these functions using a visual search (VS) task that varied the requirements to control interference from increasing numbers of distractor objects during search, and from increasing the number of features that were shared between target and distractors (target-distractor similarity, see Methods and Materials).

Animals showed prominent slowing of target detection times with increasing number of distractors from 3, 6, 9 to 12. VU0453595 did not consistently modulate this slowing with increasing distractor set size as evident by the similar slope of the linear fit across increasing numbers of distractor (Fig. 3 A and B). Similarly, the accuracy of target detection was not consistently affected by VU0453595 with no change of set size effects. For both, the raw values of target detection times and accuracy, some significant changes were observed (see Supplemental Results) but no systematic pattern could be extracted (Fig. 3 C and D). Similarly, VU0453595 did not consistently alter perceptual interference operationalized as changes in performance with increasing similarity between the target and distractors (Fig. 3 E and F). There were no changes in the set size effect for target detection times [first block: F(3,236) = 0.54, n.s.; second block: F(3,236) = 1.81, n.s.; Fig. 3E] or accuracy [first block: F(3,236) = 0.53, n.s.; second block: F(3,236) = 0.51, n.s.; Fig. 3F]. Similar to the distractor effect, the comparisons of how perceptual interference impacted raw target detection times and performance showed no systematic improvements (SI Appendix, Supplemental Results). We also looked at the relationship between search times and performance in both VS blocks independently and found no significant change in relationship at any of the tested doses (n.s.; fisher r to z transformation; SI Appendix, Fig. S2). No changes to speed of processing, operationalized as the time to response during familiarization trials (Methods and Materials) were observed with VU0453595 at any dose for neither the first VS block [F(3,236) = 0.56, n.s.] nor the second block [F(3,236) = 0.35, n.s.] (SI Appendix, Fig. S3).

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3

Distractor Effect and Interference Control Are Not Consistently Impacted by VU0453595 (A) Target detection durations (reaction times) as a function of distractor number for the second VS block. There was a significant main effect of experimental condition with a significant different between the 3 mg/kg dose of VU0453595 compared with vehicle [F(3,944) = 3.67, P = 0.01; η2 = .008; Tukey’s, P < 0.05]. The 3 mg/kg dose improved search times from 1.16 s (SE: 0.02), 1.37 s (SE: 0.02), 1.54 s (SE: 0.03) and 1.72 (SE: 0.03) with vehicle to 1.11 s (SE: 0.04), 1.30 s (SE: 0.04), 1.48 s (SE: 0.05) and 1.58 s (SE: 0.05) for 3, 6, 9, and 12 distractors, respectively. There was no significant change in the first VS block (data not shown). (B) The set size effect, operationalized as the slope of the linear fit of search times as a function of distractor numbers for the second VS block (0.057 (SE: 0.003), 0.060 (SE: 0.005), 0.058 (SE: 0.005) and 0.049 (SE: 0.005) for vehicle, 0.3, 1, and 3 mg/kg doses of VU0453595) was not significant [F(3,236) = 0.67, n.s.]. There was also no significant set size effect in the first VS block (data not shown). (C) VS task performance as a function of distractor number for the first VS block. There was a significant main effect of experimental condition with a significant different between the 3 mg/kg dose of VU0453595 compared with vehicle [F(3,944) = 3.80, P = 0.01; η2 = .010; Tukey’s, P = 0.04]. The 3 mg/kg dose reduced performance from 95.8% (SE: 0.4), 91.8% (SE: 0.7), 88.3% (SE: 0.8) and 84.1% (SE: 1.0) with vehicle to 94.5% (SE: 1.2), 89.4% (SE: 2.0), 83.1% (SE: 2.2) and 81.8% (SE: 2.7) for 3, 6, 9 and 12 distractors, respectively. There was no significant change in the second VS block (data not shown). (D) The set size effect, operationalized as the slope of the linear fit of performance as a function of distractor numbers for the first VS block [−0.013 (SE: 0.001), −0.011 (SE: 0.003), −0.014 (SE: 0.002) and −0.015 (SE: 0.002) for vehicle, 0.3, 1, and 3 mg/kg doses of VU0453595] was not significant [F(3,236) = 0.60, n.s.]. There was also no significant set size effect in the second VS block (data not shown). (E) VS task search times as a function of target-distractor similarity for the second VS block. There was a significant main effect of experimental condition with a significant different between the 1 mg/kg dose of VU0453595 compared with vehicle [F(3,708) = 4.67, P = 0.003; η2 = 0.018; Tukey’s, P = 0.02] but no significant set size effect [F(3,236) = 1.81, n.s.]. Search times were faster from 1.29 s (SE: 0.02), 1.48 s (SE: 0.02), and 1.49 (SE: 0.03) with vehicle to 1.18 s (SE: 0.04), 1.42 s (SE: 0.05) and 1.33 s (0.05) for low, medium, and high average target-distractor similarity, respectively. (F) VS task performance as a function of target-distractor similarity for the second VS block. There was a significant main effect of experimental condition but no significant post hoc comparison was found [F(3,708) = 2.84, P = 0.04; η2 = 0.011; Tukey’s, n.s.]. We also failed to find a significant set size effect [F(3,236) = 0.53, n.s.].

Comparison of VU0453595 Effects with the Literature and Consistency of Effects across Monkeys.

To evaluate how our study compared to previous studies, we surveyed 20 studies in NHPs (six using M1-selective PAMs) and thirty-two studies in rodents ((28) using M1-selective PAMs) and summarize them in Table 1 (study details are described in SI Appendix, Tables S2 and S3 for NHP and rodent studies, respectively). This survey highlighted two main ways the current study is distinct from existing studies beyond the use of the compound VU0453595.

Table 1.

Summary of studies in NHPs and rodents assessing cognitive and behavioral effects using M1 PAMs specifically. For details about the individual NHP studies, see SI Appendix, Table S2 and for individual rodent studies, see SI Appendix, Table S3

graphic file with name pnas.2216792120table01.jpg

First, the type and number of tasks used here differ from typical behavioral assays. None of the surveyed nonhuman primate studies used a set-shifting task or varied the number and similarity of distractors to a target stimulus in an attention task. Of the six studies using M1 PAM compounds (PQCA, TAK-071, MK-7622, or VU0453595), two contained no behavior, four tested cognitive effects including problem solving (3/4), working memory (2/4), and vigilance/attention (1/4). Critically, this study shows cognitive enhancement relative to vehicle and does not involve reversing pharmacological challenge-mediated deficits in cognition unlike the other M1 PAM NHP studies surveyed here. Similarly, of the 28 rodent studies using M1 PAMs (VU compounds: 9/28; BQCA/PQCA: 9/28 studies; PF compounds: 6/28 studies; TAK-071: 4/28; MK-7622: 3/28; Other: 5/28) in diverse pharmacological challenge paradigms and disease models (Table 1 and SI Appendix, Table S3). Across these rodent M1 PAM studies, none quantified ED/ID set shifting, while one reported reversal learning performance (30), alongside behavioral assays testing learning and memory (19/28), locomotion/ motor control (9/28), working memory (6/28), attention/vigilance (3/28), social behavior (3/28), misc. exec. function (2/28), or satiety/drug abuse (1/28).

Second, our approach to test individual monkeys seven times per dose differs from all six previous NHP M1 PAM studies, which used one determination per dose per monkey. These previous studies focused analyses on the group level with on average eight NHPs used per dose (SI Appendix, Table S2), compared to four monkeys per dose in our study. This difference raises the question whether our study design succeeded to find consistent effects not only in individual subjects (which we aimed for by repeating each dose seven times), but also across subjects. We address this question by summarizing in Fig. 4 the average effect of 1 mg/kg VU0453595 relative to the vehicle condition for each subject (marked in different colors) and across all major performance metrics of the set-shifting task and the visual search task. The figure illustrates the consistency of the effects across monkeys and performance metrics, supporting the main conclusions of the previous sections that the M1 PAM enhanced metrics indexing cognitive flexibility (reduced switch costs, reduced perseverative errors and led to faster learning) with less consistent effects on metrics indexing distractor filtering.

Fig. 4.

Fig. 4.

Results Summary and Consistency across Monkeys. Key results from the feature-reward learning task (Top) and the visual search task (Bottom). Measures and the respective figures showing each result (if applicable) are stated on the y-axis; asterisks indicate a significant effect for all monkeys combined. Values for each monkey represent the average change at the 1 mg/kg VU0453595 dose relative to vehicle scaled arbitrarily for each measure. The scaling for each measure is indicated on the right for 1 arbitrary unit along the x-axis. At the 1 mg/kg dose, VU0453595 enhances virtually all measures of the feature-reward learning task to some degree while no reliable changes in the visual search task were observed.

Double Dissociation of VU0453595 and Donepezil for Cognitive Flexibility and Interference Control

VU0453595 improved learning and reduced perseveration, but without reducing interference from distracting objects and features. This pattern of results contrasts to the effects of nonselective AChEI donepezil for which a prior study using the same tasks as in the current study found that an optimal dose range improved VS performance but without affecting reward learning and perseveration (43). To quantify this difference, we re-analyzed the performance of reward learning and visual search with donepezil in the prior study using the best dose for VS improvements (0.3 mg/kg) (43). This comparative approach revealed a double dissociation (Table 2). VU0453595 enhanced metrics of learning efficiency and cognitive flexibility but not metrics of interference control during VS, while donepezil made the animals more robust against distraction (improved interference control) during visual search but did so without improving feature-reward learning performance. Furthermore, at this dose, donepezil slowed down response times in the FRL task as well as search times in the VS task and even slowed the speed of processing early, partially as a consequence of dose-limiting side effects that accompanied donepezil. In contrast, VU0453595 at 1 mg/kg sped up response times in the FRL task without slowing down VS search times or the speed of processing and without any observable side effects (Table 2).

Table 2.

Comparison of performance metrics with the best doses of VU0453595 and Donepezil

graphic file with name pnas.2216792120table02.jpg

Discussion

Here, we found that healthy adult rhesus monkeys show M1 mAChR–specific improvements of cognitive flexibility in a feature-reward learning task, while leaving attentional filtering unaffected. In particular, the middle of three doses of the M1 PAM VU0453595 increased the speed of learning a new feature-reward rule, particularly with extradimensional rule changes. At the same dose animals showed less perseveration on unrewarded features. These procognitive effects contrasted to the absence of consistent distractor-dependent changes in accuracy or search times during VS. Although we did find significantly faster search times in the second VS block only and a significant reduction in performance in the first VS block only (both at 3 mg/kg), no attentional effect, i.e., shift in the slope of performance or search times dependent on the number of distractors or on target-distractor similarity was found. At the dose range tested no adverse side effects were noted. This result pattern contrasts with the effects of donepezil which improved attentional filtering during VS at a dose at which it did not affect cognitive flexibility, but already resulted in dose-limiting side effects. Taken together, these findings document a functional dissociation of the role of M1 mAChR modulation with highly selective M1 PAMs, suggesting it is a versatile treatment target for disorders suffering from inflexible, rigid cognition and behavior including schizophrenia, Alzheimer’s disease, and addiction.

M1 PAM Enhances Learning and Extradimensional shifts.

We found that the medium dose of VU0453595 improved learning of feature values. Compared to the vehicle control, the medium dose allowed subjects to reach the performance criterion 3.10 trials earlier at the low distractor load condition (Fig. 1E) and the number of trials to reach RT inflection decreased by 2.20 trials for low distractor load blocks (Fig. 2D) (see SI Appendix, Supplemental Discussion with regard to dose specificity of the effects). The learning improvement was particularly apparent with extradimensional (ED) switches, i.e., when the target feature in a block was from a different feature dimension as the target in the preceding block (Fig. 2E). Typically, ED switches take longer and are more difficult than intradimensional switches by requiring the recognition of a new dimension and integrating it in a new attentional set (45), suggesting that VU0453595 particularly benefits the flexible updating and switching of attention sets. This finding in NHPs extends the insights that the M1-selective ago-PAM BQCA can restore odor-based reversal learning of objects in transgenic mice (30).

Computationally, human and animal studies support the suggestion that an M1 PAM mechanism might enhance the updating of attention sets. Enhanced learning following ED switches in our task paradigm suggests that the M1 PAM VU0453595 allowed the animals to more effectively recognize that previously unrewarded, distracting, features became rewarded. The effect of VU0453595 is therefore akin to increasing the effective salience of those targets that were “learned distractors” from the previous block while suppressing the salience of current distractor features (Fig. 1). Recent modeling suggests that increasing effective salience is achieved with an attention–augmentation mechanism that enhances learning from attended features by actively unlearning (forgetting) values of unattended features (46). Various studies have documented that such an attention–augmentation mechanism is important for fast learning in complex tasks like the one used here (4651). The effect of VU0453595 may thus enhance the effective salience of target features, consistent with neuronal recordings that show M1 mAChR activation in the prefrontal cortex is necessary during the early processing of targets (52). Support for this suggestion comes from an elegant multitask study in NHPs that found compromising muscarinic activity with scopolamine increased the proactive interference of prior spatial information onto current performance in a self-ordered search task (53). The current findings support the interpretation that potentiation of M1 mAChR activity reduces proactive interference with the net effect of enhanced effective salience.

Recent human studies found that the learning of stimulus-response reward probabilities is enhanced with the AChE inhibitor galantamine (5) and impaired when antagonizing muscarinic receptors with biperiden (54). In a Bayesian framework, these performance improvements were linked specifically to enhanced versus reduced weighting of top-down expectancies and prediction errors during learning (5, 54). In this framework, muscarinic receptor activity determined how fast prediction errors led to belief updating about how stimuli are linked to reward. The results of the current study is consistent with that framework by suggesting that enhanced belief updating and effective salience is mediated specifically through the potentiation of the M1 mAChR. Supporting this conclusion, in rodents, the M1-selective ago-PAM BQCA reverses scopolamine-induced deficits in a contextual fear conditioning consistent with M1 mAChR enhancing the salience of the (aversive) outcomes during learning (22, 24, 25).

These functions of the M1 mAChR may be realized in the prefrontal cortex. In primates, reversal learning and the extradimensional updating of attentional sets depend on dissociable subareas of the prefrontal cortex with ED shifting and the recognition of attention sets depending particularly on the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (55, 56). Support for such a prefrontal mechanism comes from a rodent study that found the M1-selective PAM TAK-701 can partially reverse a deficit of target detection selectively on signal trials that followed no-signal trials when the deficit was induced by partially (~60%) depleting ACh afferents to the prefrontal cortex (28). These considerations support the notion that M1 mAChRs in prefrontal cortex are pivotal for the improved updating of attentional sets (2).

In previous work, faster learners of feature-reward associations were shown to have improved working memory capacity (46), which raises the possibility that M1 mAChR allosteric modulation may have affected learning through enhanced short-term memory of targets. We believe this is unlikely. While the nonselective muscarinic antagonist scopolamine impairs short term memory retention and nonselective AChE inhibitors partially reverse the deficit (5761), the short-term deficits can be independent of the delay and more prominent for short or intermediate delays, making it unlikely that muscarinic receptors have primary effects on recurrent persistent delay representations (53, 59, 62, 63).

M1 PAM Reduces Perseverative Responding.

A second main result of the current study is VU0453595 reduced response perseveration, allowing animals to avoid repeating erroneous responses to objects with the same nonrewarded features (Fig. 2E). This finding supports early insights into the effects of the muscarinic antagonist scopolamine in the prefrontal cortex to increase omissions (64), suggesting that it is the M1 mAChR that is particularly important for minimizing error rates. Support for the M1 specificity of these effects also comes from a study treating transgenic mice with an M1-selective ago-PAM which resulted in reduced erroneous choices of compound object discrimination in the trials after reversing object-reward associations (30).

Perseverative responding is the key characteristic of inflexible, habitual responding because it reflects that performance feedback is not utilized for adjusting behavior. It has been shown that performance feedback triggers transient activation of cholinergic neurons in the basal forebrain in mice (65) and activates the basal forebrain in humans (66). In the prefrontal cortex, cholinergic transients trigger gamma activity (67) that depends specifically on local M1 mAChRs (52), and thus could be a mechanism underlying improved recognizing feedback that leads to avoiding perseveration in our study.

Taken together, the reduction of perseverative responding with VU0453595 implicates the M1 mAChRs in the effective processing of feedback to adjust future performance. Perseverative, habitual responding is a hallmark of multiple psychiatric disorders including schizophrenia, obsessive compulsive disorder and substance use disorders (68, 69). The current result therefore bears particular relevance by suggesting that potentiating the M1 mAChR critically reduces perseverative response tendencies (70).

M1 PAM Has No Consistent Effect on Interference Control over Distractors.

We found that VU0453595 did not affect VS performance differently with few or many distractors. Target detection response times were moderately faster and accuracy was moderately lower to a similar extent for 3, 6, 9, or 12 distractors (Fig. 3 AD). This finding shows that the VU0453595 dose range that improved cognitive flexibility did not alter attentional filtering of distracting information. This finding adds clarity to diverse results in previous studies. Firstly, the absence of M1-specific distractor effects resonates with a recent finding in rodents that the M1-selective PAM, TAK-071, did not modulate the distracting effects of light on/off switches during a sustained attention task, but started to improve performance in the second half of testing when distraction ended, and the animals adjusted to a no-distractor regime (28). This result pattern is congruent with our result pattern. Allosteric modulation of the M1 mAChR improved adjusting behavior to challenges, but without improving interference control from distraction. A similar lack of effects of muscarinic modulation on distractor interference control were found in other task contexts. Scopolamine-induced deficits of continuous recognition performance can be partially reversed with an M1-selective agonist (71) or the nonselective muscarinic agonist milameline (72, 73), but this deficit reversal is independent of the similarity between distracting and target objects (27). Similarly, scopolamine does not alter distractor effects in an attentional flanker task, but rather causes an overall slowing and selective impairment of learning reminiscent of the reward learning effect we found (74).

The observed result pattern with the M1 PAM VU0453595 contrasts to apparent effects to reduce distraction with nicotinic modulation (12, 44, 75, 76), with nonselective cholinergic increases using donepezil (43) (Table 2), or with the improvement of target detection accuracy and visuospatial attentional orienting when enhancing cholinergic transmission from the basal forebrain (1, 7779). Particularly relevant in this context is a prior NHP study that found the nicotinic alpha-4/beta-2 receptor agonist selectively enhanced distractor filtering when two stimuli underwent salient changes but had no effect on reversal learning speed (44).

One caveat when interpreting the absence of an effect with the M1 PAM VU0453595 is that it is unclear whether a higher dose of this ligand would have affected distractor filtering during VS performance. The highest dose used in this study (3 mg/kg, oral) is a magnitude lower than the maximum 30 mg/kg doses that previous studies found to have only mild adverse side effects (80), suggesting that future studies will need to identify possible dose-specific effects on attention functions.

Limitations.

While our study already tested multiple markers of cognitive flexibility and attention, it was not yet incorporating tests of other domains that M1 mAChR modulating ligands might affect and which are compromised in psychiatric patient populations such as long-term memory and motivation (13, 8183). Further tasks, where we can extract measures for these domains would be important additions for a more comprehensive characterization of possible M1 mAChR dependent behaviors (SI Appendix, Supplemental Discussion). Such an expansion of extracted measures would align well with efforts to develop multi-task batteries for NHPs covering a wide range of cognitive domains (53, 8487).

Conclusion.

In summary, the M1 PAM VU0453595 produced selective improvements in cognitive flexibility in the absence of adverse side effects. The results were obtained with cognitive tasks that tap into real-world cognitive demands for adjusting to the changing relevance of visual objects. This result pattern suggests that M1 PAMs will be powerful targets for drug discovery efforts to augment cognitive flexibility.

Methods and Materials

Subjects.

Four adult male rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) were separately given access to a touchscreen Kiosk Station attached to their housing unit where they performed a visual search attention task and a feature-reward learning task (85) (Fig. 1A) (SI Appendix, Supplemental Materials).

Compounds and Procedures.

The scale up of the M1 PAM VU0453595 used in the present study was synthesized at the Molecular Design and Synthesis Center within the Vanderbilt Institute of Chemical Biology, Vanderbilt University, School of Medicine (30, 33) and mixed with a vehicle of 18 g of strawberry yogurt and 2 g of honey provided to the monkeys in a small paper cup (oral administration). All monkeys received vehicle or VU0453595 2 h prior to the start of behavioral performance and were observed to ensure full consumption of vehicle or VU0453595. VU0453595 was administered once per week to allow appropriate washout. Based on the weight of each animal, its volume was calculated for 0.3, 1, and 3 mg/kg doses. Side effects were assessed 15 min following VU0453595 administration and after completion of the behavioral performance with a modified Irwin Scale for rating autonomic nervous system functioning (e.g., salivation) and somatomotor system functioning (e.g., posture and unrest) (43, 8890). Furthermore, monkeys’ behavioral status was video-monitored throughout task performance.

The pK for VU0453595 has previously been reported in cynomolgus macaques where peak concentrations at 3 mg/kg dosing occurred ~2 h after oral administration (80). The same study found changes to qEEG spectral power 0 to 4 h after VU0453595 administration in macaques, consistent with the time window for behavioral performance in our study. While changes in qEEG spectral power were also observed in mice, M1-KO mice did not exhibit these changes. Furthermore, the agonist and PAM activity of VU0453595, through calcium mobilization assays have been previously reported in M1-expressing CHO cells (22).

Behavioral Paradigms.

Monkeys performed a sequence of two tasks in a single behavioral session including an initial VS task block, 21 reward learning task blocks and finally, a second visual task block. Rewarded and unrewarded objects in the VS task and FRL task were multidimensional, 3D rendered objects (named “Quaddles”) (91) that shared a variable number of different feature dimensions (colors, shapes, arms and/or body patterns). The VS task varied the perceptual target-distractor similarity by changing the average number of common features between distractors and the target object. The FRL task varied the complexity of the feature space by varying features of objects in only one or two feature dimensions from trial to trial.

At the start of each session, animals performed a VS task block and again at the end of each session, they performed a second VS task block. Each VS block contained an initial ten “familiarization trials” followed by 100 search trials. During the familiarization trials, only the rewarded object was presented on screen, without any distracting objects. The rewarded object was made up of three features of three different feature dimensions. The 10 familiarization trials were followed by a set of 100 search trials, where the rewarded object (learned during the familiarization trials) was always presented amongst 3, 6, 9, or 12 (counter-balanced and randomly selected) distracting objects (Fig. 1B, Bottom). These distracting objects could each share 0, 1, or 2 of the three features with the target. Animals received fluid reward for touching the learned target object presented during that block’s familiarization trials. Each of the two VS blocks in a daily session was accompanied by one of five patterned background images, selected without replacement daily. These images bore no relationship to the target objects and served to cue the animal to the task rule in contrast to the FRL task which contained a neutral gray background.

Between the two VS blocks, the animals performed 21 blocks of the FRL task where they had to identify the single feature value associated with high reward probability (85%). In each block (35 to 60 trials) of the FRL task, animals were required to learn, by trial-and-error, which single feature was associated with the reward. The FRL task indexes cognitive flexibility by testing how fast subjects learn which feature is rewarded when the feature-reward rule switched between blocks. The newly rewarded feature after the uncued block switches could be from the same or from a different feature dimension than the previously rewarded feature. This makes the task similar to attentional set-shifting tasks, but different by using a larger set of features that varied within and across sessions in order to control task difficulty. In each trial, three objects were shown that varied either in the features of one feature dimension (e.g., each object having different colors or body shapes), or that varied in features of two feature dimensions (e.g., each object having different colors and body shapes). Thus, in a single trial, no two objects contained any overlap in the presented features (Fig. 1B, Top). Choosing the object with the correct feature was rewarded with a probability of 0.85. Blocks where only 1 feature dimension varied (low distractor load) were easier as there were less distracting features than in blocks with two varying feature dimensions (high distractor load). Blocks switched after the completion of a minimum of 35, 40, or 45 trials (random jittering) if a performance threshold of 80% in the previous 10 trials was reached. Otherwise, blocks would switch after the completion of 60 trials.

Statistical Analysis.

Data were analyzed with standard nonparametric and parametric tests with test statistics, P values, and effect sizes reported where appropriate in text. For detailed statistical methods, please see the SI Appendix.

Financial Disclosures.

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

Supplementary Material

Appendix 01 (PDF)

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health of the NIH under Award Number R01MH129641 (T.W.) and the National Institute of Aging under Award Number R01AG054622 (C.J.K.). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the NIH.

Author contributions

S.A.H., J.R., C.K.J., and T.W. designed research; S.A.H. and A.N. performed research; J.R. and C.K.J. contributed new reagents/analytic tools; S.A.H. analyzed data; and S.A.H., A.N., J.R., C.K.J., and T.W. wrote the paper.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interest.

Footnotes

This article is a PNAS Direct Submission.

Data, Materials, and Software Availability

.mat and .m files data have been deposited in Github (https://github.com/att-circ-contrl/PNAS_VU595_code-data) (92).

Supporting Information

References

  • 1.Levin E. D., Bushnell P. J., Rezvani A. H., Attention-modulating effects of cognitive enhancers. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 99, 146–154 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Sarter M., Lustig C., Cholinergic double duty: Cue detection and attentional control. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 29, 102–107 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Doya K., Metalearning and neuromodulation. Neural Netw. 15, 495–506 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Hirayama J., Yoshimoto J., Ishii S., Bayesian representation learning in the cortex regulated by acetylcholine. Neural Netw. 17, 1391–1400 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Vossel S., et al. , Cholinergic stimulation enhances Bayesian belief updating in the deployment of spatial attention. J. Neurosci. 34, 15735–15742 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Sarter M., Lustig C., Blakely R. D., Koshy Cherian A., Cholinergic genetics of visual attention: Human and mouse choline transporter capacity variants influence distractibility. J. Physiol. Paris 110, 10–18 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Botly L. C. P., de Rosa E., Cholinergic deafferentation of the neocortex using 192 IgG-saporin impairs feature binding in rats. J. Neurosci. 29, 4120–4130 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Ljubojevic V., Luu P., de Rosa E., Cholinergic contributions to supramodal attentional processes in rats. J. Neurosci. 34, 2264–2275 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Turchi J., Sarter M., Cortical cholinergic inputs mediate processing capacity: Effects of 192 IgG-saporin-induced lesions on olfactory span performance. Euro. J. Neurosci. 12, 4505–4514 (2000). [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Turchi J., Sarter M., Cortical acetylcholine and processing capacity: Effects of cortical cholinergic deafferentation on crossmodal divided attention in rats. Cogn. Brain Res. 6, 147–158 (1997). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Bertrand D., Terry A. v., The wonderland of neuronal nicotinic acetylcholine receptors. Biochem. Pharmacol. 151, 214–225 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Thiele A., Bellgrove M. A., Neuromodulation of Attention. Neuron 97, 769–785 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Ellis J. R., et al. , Muscarinic and nicotinic receptors synergistically modulate working memory and attention in humans. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 9, 175–189 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Furey M. L., Pietrini P., Haxby J. V., Drevets W. C., Selective effects of cholinergic modulation on task performance during selective attention. Neuropsychopharmacology 33, 913–923 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Bushnell P. J., Oshiro W. M., Padnos B. K., Detection of visual signals by rats: Effects of chlordiazepoxide and cholinergic and adrenergic drugs on sustained attention. Psychopharmacology 134, 230–241 (1997). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Davidson M. C., Marrocco R. T., Local infusion of scopolamine into intraparietal cortex slows covert orienting in rhesus monkeys. J. Neurophysiol. 83, 1536–1549 (2000). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Davidson M. C., Cutrell E. B., Marrocco R. T., Scopolamine slows the orienting of attention in primates to cued visual targets. Psychopharmacology 142, 1–8 (1999). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Dasilva M., et al. , Cell class-specific modulation of attentional signals by acetylcholine in macaque frontal eye field. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 116, 20180–20189 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Levey A. I., Muscarinic acetylcholine receptor expression in memory circuits: Implications for treatment of Alzheimer disease. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 93, 13541–13546 (1996). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Rapan L. J., Froudist-walsh S., Niu M., Xu T., Zhao L., Cytoarchitectonic, receptor distribution and functional connectivity analyses of the macaque frontal lobe. bioRxiv [Preprint] (2022). 10.1101/2022.06.27.497757 (Accessed 11 April 2023). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 21.Jones C. K., Byun N., Bubser M., Muscarinic and nicotinic acetylcholine receptor agonists and allosteric modulators for the treatment of schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology 37, 16–42 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Moran S. P., et al. , M1-positive allosteric modulators lacking agonist activity provide the optimal profile for enhancing cognition. Neuropsychopharmacology 43, 1763–1771 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Rook J. M., et al. , Diverse effects on M1 signaling and adverse effect liability within a series of M1 ago-PAMs. ACS Chem. Neurosci. 8, 866–883 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Ma L., et al. , Selective activation of the M1 muscarinic acetylcholine receptor achieved by allosteric potentiation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 15950–15955 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Chambon C., Wegener N., Gravius A., Danysz W., A new automated method to assess the rat recognition memory: Validation of the method. Behav. Brain Res. 222, 151–157 (2011). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Herrero J. L., et al. , Acetylcholine contributes through muscarinic receptors to attentional modulation in V1. Nature 454, 1110–1114 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Lange H. S., Cannon C. E., Drott J. T., Kuduk S. D., Uslaner J. M., The M1 muscarinic positive allosteric modulator PQCA improves performance on translatable tests of memory and attention in rhesus monkeys. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Therapeut. 355, 442–450 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Kucinski A., Phillips K. B., Koshy Cherian A., Sarter M., Rescuing the attentional performance of rats with cholinergic losses by the M1 positive allosteric modulator TAK-071. Psychopharmacology 237, 137–153 (2020). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Uslaner J. M., et al. , Preclinical to human translational pharmacology of the novel M1positive allosteric modulator MK-7622. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Therapeut. 365, 556–566 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Shirey J. K., et al. , A selective allosteric potentiator of the M1 muscarinic acetylcholine receptor increases activity of medial prefrontal cortical neurons and restores impairments in reversal learning. J. Neurosci. 29, 14271–14286 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Vijayraghavan S., Major A. J., Everling S., Muscarinic M1 receptor overstimulation disrupts working memory activity for rules in primate prefrontal cortex. Neuron 98, 1256–1268.e4 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Rook J. M., et al. , A novel M 1 PAM VU0486846 exerts efficacy in cognition models without displaying agonist activity or cholinergic toxicity. ACS Chem. Neurosci. 9, 2274–2285 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Ghoshal A., et al. , Potentiation of M1 muscarinic receptor reverses plasticity deficits and negative and cognitive symptoms in a schizophrenia mouse model. Neuropsychopharmacology 41, 598–610 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Li D. D., Zhang Y. H., Zhang W., Zhao P., Meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials on the efficacy and safety of donepezil, galantamine, rivastigmine, and memantine for the treatment of Alzheimer’s disease. Front. Neurosci. 13, 472 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Korczyn A. D., Muscarinic M1 agonists in the treatment of Alzheimer’s disease. Expert Opin. Invest. Drugs 9, 2259–2267 (2000). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Perry R. J., Hodges J. R., Attention and executive deficits in Alzheimer’s disease. A critical review. Brain 122, 383–404 (1999). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Waltz J. A., The neural underpinnings of cognitive flexibility and their disruption in psychotic illness. Neuroscience 345, 203–217 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Rohlf H., et al. , Set shifting and working memory in adults with attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disorder. J. Neural. Transm. 119, 95–106 (2012). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Woicik P. A., et al. , A pattern of perseveration in cocaine addiction may reveal neurocognitive processes implicit in the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Neuropsychologia 49, 1660–1669 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Tobin A. B., “Targeting the M1 muscarinic receptor in Alzheimer’s Disease” in Proc. Annu. Meeting Japanese Pharmacol. Soc.WCP2018, SY85-2 (2018). [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Foster D. J., Bryant Z. K., Conn P. J., Targeting muscarinic receptors to treat schizophrenia. Behav. Brain Res. 405, 113201 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Ferreira-Vieira T. H., Guimaraes I. M., Silva F. R., Ribeiro F. M., Alzheimer’s disease: Targeting the Cholinergic System. Curr. Neuropharmacol. 14, 101–115 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Hassani S. A., et al. , Dose-dependent dissociation of pro-cognitive effects of donepezil on attention and cognitive flexibility in rhesus monkeys. Biol. Psychiatry Global Open Sci. 3, 68–77 (2023). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Azimi M., Oemisch M., Womelsdorf T., Dissociation of nicotinic α7and α4/β2 sub-receptor agonists for enhancing learning and attentional filtering in nonhuman primates. Psychopharmacology 237, 997–1010 (2019). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Izquierdo A., Brigman J. L., Radke A. K., Rudebeck P. H., Holmes A., The neural basis of reversal learning: An updated perspective. Neuroscience 345, 12–26 (2016), 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.03.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Womelsdorf T., Watson M. R., Tiesinga P., Learning at variable attentional load requires cooperation of working memory, meta-learning, and attention-augmented reinforcement learning. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 34, 79–107 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Hassani S. A., et al. , A computational psychiatry approach identifies how alpha-2A noradrenergic agonist Guanfacine affects feature-based reinforcement learning in the macaque. Sci. Rep. 7, 40606 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Oemisch M., et al. , Feature-specific prediction errors and surprise across macaque fronto-striatal circuits. Nat. Commun. 10, 1–15 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Niv Y., et al. , Reinforcement learning in multidimensional environments relies on attention mechanisms. J. Neurosci. 35, 8145–8157 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Wilson R. C., Niv Y., Inferring relevance in a changing world. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 5, 1–14 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Radulescu A., Daniel R., Niv Y., The effects of aging on the interaction between reinforcement learning and attention. Psychol. Aging 31, 747–757 (2016). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Howe W. M., et al. , Acetylcholine release in prefrontal cortex promotes gamma oscillations and theta-gamma coupling during cue detection. J. Neurosci. 37, 3215–3230 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Taffe M. A., Weed M. R., Gold L. H., Scopolamine alters rhesus monkey performance on a novel neuropsychological test battery. Cogn. Brain Res. 8, 203–212 (1999). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Marshall L., et al. , Pharmacological fingerprints of contextual uncertainty. PLoS Biol. 14, 1–31 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Rudebeck P. H., Saunders R. C., Lundgren D. A., Murray E. A., Specialized representations of value in the orbital and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex: Desirability versus availability of outcomes. Neuron 95, 1208–1220.e5 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Friedman N. P., Robbins T. W., The role of prefrontal cortex in cognitive control and executive function. Neuropsychopharmacology 47, 72–89 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Buccafusco J. J., et al. , The scopolamine-reversal paradigm in rats and monkeys: The importance of computer-assisted operant-conditioning memory tasks for screening drug candidates. Psychopharmacology 199, 481–494 (2008). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Buccafusco J. J., Terry A. v., Donepezil-induced improvement in delayed matching accuracy by young and old rhesus monkeys. J. Mol. Neurosci. 24, 85–91 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Knakker B., et al. , Delay-dependent cholinergic modulation of visual short-term memory in rhesus macaques. Behav. Brain Res. 396 (2021). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Bartus R. T., Johnson H. R., Short-term memory in the rhesus monkey: Disruption from the anti-cholinergic scopolamine. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 5, 39–46 (1976). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Aigner T. G., Mishkin M., The effects of physostigmine and scopolamine on recognition memory in monkeys. Behav. Neural. Biol. 45, 81–87 (1986). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Glick S. D., Jarvik M. E., Differential effects of amphetamine and scopolamine on matching performance of monkeys with lateral frontal lesions. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 73, 307–313 (1970). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Aigner T. G., Walker D. L., Mishkin M., Comparison of the effects of scopolamine administered before and after acquisition in a test of visual recognition memory in monkeys. Behav. Neural. Biol. 55, 61–67 (1991). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Robbins T. W., et al. , Neural systems underlying arousal and attention: Implications for drug abuse. Ann. N Y Acad. Sci. 846, 222–237 (1998). [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Hangya B., Ranade S. P., Lorenc M., Kepecs A., Central cholinergic neurons are rapidly recruited by reinforcement feedback. Cell 162, 1155–1168 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Iglesias S., et al. , Cholinergic and dopaminergic effects on prediction error and uncertainty responses during sensory associative learning. Neuroimage 226, 117590 (2021). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Lin S. C., Gervasoni D., Nicolelis M. A. L., Fast modulation of prefrontal cortex activity by basal forebrain noncholinergic neuronal ensembles. J. Neurophysiol. 96, 3209–3219 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Floresco S. B., Zhang Y., Enomoto T., Neural circuits subserving behavioral flexibility and their relevance to schizophrenia. Behav. Brain Res. 204, 396–409 (2009). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Moritz S., et al. , Perseveration and not strategic deficits underlie delayed alternation impairment in obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD). Psychiatry Res. 170, 66–69 (2009). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Lustig C., Sarter M., Attention and the cholinergic system: Relevance to schizophrenia. Curr. Top. Behav. Neurosci. 28, 327–362 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.O’Neill J., et al. , Reduction in distractibility with AF102B and THA in the macaque. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 76, 301–306 (2003). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Schwarz R. D., et al. , Milameline (CI-979/RU35926): A muscarinic receptor agonist with cognition-activating properties: Biochemical and in vivo characterization. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Therapeut. 291, 812–822 (1999). [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Callahan M. J., Combining tacrine with milameline reverses a scopolamine-induced impairment of continuous performance in rhesus monkeys. Psychopharmacology 144, 234–238 (1999). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Thienel R., et al. , Muscarinic antagonist effects on executive control of attention. International J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 12, 1307–1317 (2009). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Parikh V., Ji J., Decker M. W., Sarter M., Prefrontal β2 subunit-containing and α7 nicotinic acetylcholine receptors differentially control glutamatergic and cholinergic signaling. J. Neurosci. 30, 3518–3530 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Terry A. v., Plagenhoef M., Callahan P. M., Effects of the nicotinic agonist varenicline on the performance of tasks of cognition in aged and middle-aged rhesus and pigtail monkeys. Psychopharmacology 233, 761–771 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.lou Voytko M., et al. , Basal forebrain lesions in monkeys disrupt attention but not learning and memory. J. Neurosci. 14, 167–186 (1994). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Chiba A. A., Bushnell P. J., Oshiro W. M., Gallagher M., Selective removal of cholinergic neurons in the basal forebrain alters cued target detection. Neuroreport 10, 3119–3123 (1999). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Phillips J. M., McAlonan K., Robb W. G. K., Brown V. J., Cholinergic neurotransmission influences covert orientation of visuospatial attention in the rat. Psychopharmacology 150, 112–116 (2000). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Gould R. W., et al. , Modulation of arousal and sleep/wake architecture by M1 PAM VU0453595 across young and aged rodents and nonhuman primates. Neuropsychopharmacology 45, 2219–2228 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Millan M. J., et al. , Cognitive dysfunction in psychiatric disorders: Characteristics, causes and the quest for improved therapy. Nat. Rev. Drug. Discov. 11, 141–168 (2012). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Edginton T., Rusted J. M., Separate and combined effects of scopolamine and nicotine on retrieval-induced forgetting. Psychopharmacology 170, 351–357 (2003). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Broks P., et al. , Modelling dementia: Effects of scopolamine on memory and attention. Neuropsychologia 26, 685–700 (1988). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Wither R. G., Boehnke S. E., Lablans A., Armitage-Brown B., Munoz D. P., Behavioral shaping of rhesus macaques using the Cambridge neuropsychological automated testing battery. J. Neurosci. Methods 342, 108803 (2020). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Womelsdorf T., et al. , A kiosk station for the assessment of multiple cognitive domains and enrichment of monkeys. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 15, 721069 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Palmer D., et al. , Touchscreen cognitive testing: Cross-species translation and co-clinical trials in neurodegenerative and neuropsychiatric disease. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 182, 107443 (2021). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Weed M. R., et al. , Performance norms for a rhesus monkey neuropsychological testing battery: Acquisition and long-term performance. Cogn. Brain Res. 8, 185–201 (1999). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Irwin S., Comprehensive observational assessment: Ia. A systematic, quantitative procedure for assessing the behavioral and physiologic state of the mouse. Psychopharmacologia 13, 222–257 (1968). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Patel S., et al. , Biological profile of L-745,870, a selective antagonist with high affinity for the dopamine D4 receptor. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Therapeut. 283, 636–647 (1997). [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Andersen M. B., et al. , The muscarinic M1/M4receptor agonist xanomeline exhibits antipsychotic-like activity in cebus apella monkeys. Neuropsychopharmacology 28, 1168–1175 (2003). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Watson M. R., Voloh B., Naghizadeh M., Womelsdorf T., Quaddles: A multidimensional 3-D object set with parametrically controlled and customizable features. Behav. Res. Methods 51, 2522–2532 (2019). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Hassani S. A., Neumann A., Russell J., Jones C. K., Womelsdorf T., PNAS 2023 VU0453595 behavioral data and omni-analysis script. Github. https://github.com/att-circ-contrl/PNAS_VU595_code-data. Deposited 13 April 2023.

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Appendix 01 (PDF)

Data Availability Statement

.mat and .m files data have been deposited in Github (https://github.com/att-circ-contrl/PNAS_VU595_code-data) (92).


Articles from Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America are provided here courtesy of National Academy of Sciences

RESOURCES