Skip to main content
Springer logoLink to Springer
. 2024 Mar 13;229(4):879–896. doi: 10.1007/s00429-024-02767-7

Hayling and stroop tests tap dissociable deficits and network-level neural correlates

Margaret Jane Moore 1, Jessica Byrne 2, Emily C Gibson 2, Lucy Ford 1,2, Gail A Robinson 1,2,
PMCID: PMC11004053  PMID: 38478051

Abstract

Although many executive function screens have been developed, it is not yet clear whether these assessments are equally effective in detecting post-stroke deficits of initiation and inhibition. This study presents a comparative analysis of the Stroop and Hayling tests aiming to evaluate whether these tests measure the same underlying cognitive functions and to identify the neural correlates of the deficits detected by both tasks. Sixty six stroke survivors and 70 healthy ageing controls completed the Hayling and Stroop tests. Stroke patients were found to exhibit qualitative performance differences across analogous Stroop and Hayling Test metrics intended to tap initiation and inhibition. The Stroop test was found to have high specificity to abnormal performance, but low sensitivity relative to the Hayling Test. Minimal overlap was present between the network-level correlates of analogous Stroop and Hayling Test metrics. Hayling Task strategy use metrics were significantly associated with distinct patterns of disconnection in stroke survivors, providing novel insight into the neural correlates of fine-grained behavioural patterns. Overall, these findings strongly suggest that the functions tapped by the Stroop and Hayling Test are both behaviourally and anatomically dissociable. The Hayling Test was found to offer improved sensitivity and detail relative to the Stroop test. This novel demonstration of the Hayling Test within the stroke population suggests that this task represents an effective measure for quantifying post-stroke initiation and inhibition deficits.

Keywords: Inhibition, Initiation, Strategy use, Lesion mapping

Introduction

Executive dysfunction is a common and debilitating consequence of stroke affecting up to 50% of stroke survivors (Blake et al. 2002; Nys et al. 2006). The occurance of post-stroke executive function impairment acts as a robust predictor of poor long-term functional recovery outcomes and has a significant negative impact on stroke survivors’ ability to lead normal lives (Jefferson et al. 2006; Leśniak et al. 2008; Pohjasvaara et al. 2002). It is, therefore, critically important to reliably detect and diagnose post-stroke executive function deficits to inform prognoses and guide targeted rehabilitation. Although many standardised assessments of post-stroke executive function have been developed, it is not yet clear whether these assessments are equally effective in detecting specific aspects of executive dysfunction within the stroke population.

Broadly, executive function includes a range of cognitive abilities responsible for regulating, controlling, and managing other cognitive processes (Diamond 2013). Following stroke, executive dysfunction frequently manifests as an inability to effectively initiate or supress (i.e., inhibit) cognitive functions (Barker et al. 2017). Patients within initiation deficits struggle to begin actions (e.g., movements, verbal responses, activities), despite an intact understanding of when and how to do the correct actions (Zinn et al. 2007). Conversely, patients with suppression deficits struggle to stop or override dominant responses and have difficulties with interference control (selective attention/cognitive inhibition) and self-control (monitoring behaviour) (Chiappe et al. 2000; Diamond 2013; Guarino et al. 2020). Several standardised tools have been developed to assess initiation and suppression functions, but it is not yet clear whether each of these tools is equally effective within the stroke population.

First, the Stroop test (Stroop 1935) is commonly used to assess initiation and inhibition abilities within clinical populations. In this classical test, accuracy and response time are recorded as an individual reports the colour of non-word (e.g., dots) or word stimuli.

(Jensen and Rohwer 1966; Scarpina and Tagini 2017). Next, the person is asked to name the ink colour of colour words which are printed in incongruent colours (e.g., GREEN is printed in blue ink) (Jensen and Rohwer 1966). This task yields the control colour (non-word or word) response time as a measure of initiation and difference between this control and incongruent colour word conditions as a measure of suppression ability (Scarpina and Tagini 2017). Although this task is commonly used, it is not clear whether it serves as an optimal screen for post-stroke inhibition and suppression deficits.

By definition, to effectively assess cognitive inhibition (i.e., suppression), a task must involve the stopping of a response that has been initiated (Aron et al. 2014). It is unclear whether the Stroop test meets this criterium as successful completion of this task does not necessarily require subjects to supress an initiated response (Stuss and Alexander 2007). Cognitive interference from incongruent colour words cannot be clearly conceptualised as an “initiated response” as this information is task irrelevant and does not need to be considered for an appropriate response to be activated (Stuss and Alexander 2007). For example, subjects could adopt a strategy of naming the ink colour of single letters (e.g., the D in RED) to reduce interreference from incongruent colour words. As it is unclear whether incongruent responses are initiated in the Stroop test, it is unclear whether this task can adequately measure cognitive suppression abilities (Aron et al. 2014).

The Hayling Sentence Completion test (Burgess and Shallice 1996; 1997) is an alternative measure which has the potential to address many of the potential weaknesses of the Stroop test. In the first section of the Hayling test (sensible completion), participants are instructed to listen to 15 verbally presented sentence fragments and to verbally complete the sentences with a thematically related, sensible ending (e.g., The captain stayed with the sinking…ship). In the second section (unconnected completion), participants are instructed to verbally complete 15 presented sentence fragments with completely unrelated endings (e.g., for the above example…table). Critically, for subjects to complete this unconnected completion section, participants must successfully supress the automatic responses initiated by the semantic content of presented sentences (Aron et al. 2014). Accuracy on connected and unconnected response sections are compared to provide quantitative measurements of initiation and inhibition while qualitative response data can be used to provide insight into the strategies being used. Specifically, Hayling test responses can be classified into strategy type as identified by previous studies (Burgess and Shallice 1996; Robinson et al. 2015). For example, Burgess and Shallice (Burgess and Shallice 1996) observed that control participants tended to generate an unrelated word by naming visual objects around the room. Patients with frontal lobe damage have also been found to be less likely to implement strategies compared to healthy controls (Burgess and Shallice 1996; Robinson et al. 2015). Similar findings have been documented in older adults relative to younger adults. Hayling test strategy use metrics provide an additional and potentially valuable source of information which helps provide insight into the exact cognitive processes facilitating normal and abnormal initiation/inhibition (Gibson et al. 2018; Martin et al. 2021). However, the utility of the Hayling test has not yet been thoroughly investigated in the stroke population. Some previous studies have reported that stroke patients exhibit worse Hayling test performance relative to healthy controls (Laakso et al. 2019; Nijsse et al. 2019), but more fine-grained strategy and error type comparisons have not yet been reported. Therefore, the present study aims to address this knowledge gap.

Employing more precise diagnostic measurements of initiation can help further understanding of the neural correlates of these behaviours. While the neural correlates of general initiation and inhibition abilities have been investigated (Nathaniel-James et al. 1997), no previous investigation has conducted statistical lesion mapping analyses aiming to identify the correlates of Hayling task strategy use variables. Although initiation/inhibition deficits are traditionally associated with frontal lesions (Aron et al. 2014), recent work has suggested that different components of cognitive inhibition may be linked to separable frontal lobe correlates (Hung et al. 2018). Cipolotti and colleagues (Cipolotti et al. 2016) found that suppression on the Hayling test significantly relies on the right lateral prefrontal cortex (superior and middle frontal gyri), whereas the Stroop test was correlated with the left lateral superior and middle frontal gyri. This finding is in parallel with previous studies that link the right lateral frontal region with suppression on the Hayling test (Robinson et al. 2015) and left inferior frontal regions with the Stroop test (George et al. 1994; Taylor et al. 1997). This previous work provides preliminary evidence that the neural correlates associated with the Stroop and Hayling task may be independent, but more detailed analyses are needed to confirm this finding.

Complex functions such as inhibition and inhibition are not only supported by localised brain areas, but also rely on effective connection and communication between a distributed network of brain structures (Sadaghiani and Kleinschmidt 2016; Thiebaut de Schotten et al. 2008). Previous studies investigating the neural correlates of inhibition have employed mainly focal lesion location analyses (e.g., PET, ROI-level lesion overlay comparisons), and have not explored the role of network-level disconnections in these deficits (Robinson et al. 2015). While focal lesion approaches are important for identifying critical lesion sights, network analyses provide additional insight by quantifying how disruptions in communication between multiple spatially distinct regions is associated with neuropsychological deficits. Network-level lesion mapping is a powerful methodology for identifying statistically significant disconnection correlates of cognitive impairment (Gleichgerrcht et al. 2017). This method takes spatially distributed, disconnection-related effects into account when identifying brain-behaviour relationships (Foulon et al. 2018; Fox 2018; Gleichgerrcht et al. 2017). These methodologies consider probability of disconnection of each pre-defined network edge or tract as behavioural predictors rather than the presence/absence of damage on a voxel-by-voxel basis (Foulon et al. 2018). Network-based symptom mapping approaches have been applied to investigate the anatomy of many common post-stroke deficits but have not yet been applied to explore initiation and inhibition deficits. Therefore, this study aims to employ network-level lesion analyses to identify disconnection correlates of specific behavioural patterns on the Hayling and Stroop tests to elucidate the neural structures underlying common error patterns.

The present study aims to employ both the Hayling and Stroop test to assess initiation and inhibition functions in a large sample of healthy controls and stroke survivors. This study aims to provide a detailed investigation of the Hayling test in stroke survivors by conducting group-level comparisons of standard initiation/inhibition scores as well as more fine-grained comparisons of strategy use differences. This behavioural data will be used to conduct network-level lesion mapping analyses to identify and compare disconnection correlates of initiation/inhibition impairments as reported by the Stroop and Hayling tests. Overall, this study aims to provide a novel and detailed exploration of Hayling test performance and correlates in a large and representative sample of stroke survivors.

Methods

Participants

Sixty-nine acute stroke patients were recruited from the Royal Brisbane and Women’s Hospital and the Princess Alexandra Hospital (Brisbane, Australia). Patients were considered for inclusion in this investigation if they (1) were diagnosed with a first-time stroke as confirmed by routine clinical imaging (CT or MR) and (2) were within six weeks of hospital admission due to a stroke event. Patients were excluded from all conducted analyses if they exhibited evidence of previous strokes, were diagnosed with a Transient Ischaemic Attack, were > 90 years of age, were not fluent in English, and/or were affected by other psychiatric or neurological diagnoses (e.g., schizophrenia, substance use disorder, dementia). Participants were excluded from neuroimaging analyses (but included in behavioural analyses) if they lacked available neuroimaging data of suitable quality for the lesion analyses (e.g., poor quality scans). All participants provided informed written consent (in line with the Declaration of Helsinki) and approval for the study was provided by the Metro South and Metro North Health Human Research Ethics Committees and the University of Queensland (UQ) Human Research Ethics Committee.

Three patients were excluded from behavioural analyses due to having English as a second language (n = 2) or severe fatigue that precluded completion of most tasks (n = 1), whilst 12 participants were excluded from lesion-mapping analyses. Participant demographics and stroke descriptive statistics are reported in Table 1. Overall, 66 patients (mean age = 61.6 (SD = 13.9, range = 23–86), 33.3% female) were included in behavioural analyses and a subset of 54 (mean age = 60.5 (SD = 12.4, range = 23–84), 32.1% female) of these participants were included in lesion analyses. There was no significant differences in age (t(97.5) = 0.86, p = 0.39), gender (X2(1) = 0.06, p = 0.81), time between stroke and assessment (t(101.9) = 0.82, p = 0.41) or lateralisation of stroke (X2(3) = 0.20, p = 0.91) across these participant groups.

Table 1.

Demographics for each of the patients included in this study

ID Age Sex Handed Education Chronicity Stroke type Stroke side Artery Volume
P01 50 M Right 12 19 Ischaemic LH MCA 61.1
P02 74 F Right 10 19 Haemorrhagic RH
P03 69 M Right 16 5 Ischaemic RH MCA 46.7
P04 59 M Right 13 17 Ischaemic LH PICA 331.5
P05 63 M Right 14 15 Ischaemic RH PCA 344.5
P06 84 F Right 13 16 Ischaemic RH MCA 307.7
P07 28 F Right 16 11 Ischaemic LH
P08 66 M Right 12 16 Ischaemic LH Brainstem 8.0
P09 54 M Right 15 39 Ischaemic RH MCA 183.5
P10 69 F Left 17 21 Ischaemic RH MCA 81.7
P11 70 M Right 15 25 Haemorrhagic LH MCA 236.8
P12 40 F Right 16 48 Ischaemic LH MCA 10.4
P13 70 M Right 10 62 Ischaemic LH PICA 23.7
P14 76 F Right 14 26 Ischaemic LH PCA 654.0
P15 68 M Right 10 46 Ischaemic LH PCA 524.2
P16 73 F Right 16 46 Ischaemic RH MCA 0.09
P17 58 M Right 11 45 Ischaemic RH MCA 17.4
P18 64 M Right 10 26 Ischaemic RH MCA 225.8
P19 62 M Right 18 24 Ischaemic LH PCA
P20 67 M Right 12 9 Ischaemic RH MCA/ICA 63.0
P21 72 F Right 10 21 Ischaemic LH MCA 53.4
P22 50 M Left 12 41 Ischaemic RH ACA 34.7
P23 86 F Left 8 36 Ischaemic LH MCA
P24 60 M Right 15 21 Ischaemic LH MCA 19.8
P25 71 M Right 20 26 Haemorrhagic RH ACA 103.7
P26 59 M Right 16 14 Ischaemic LH/RH PICA 278.0
P27 86 M Right 11 21 Ischaemic RH MCA
P28 37 M Right 12 8 Ischaemic LH MCA 413.2
P29 70 F Right 16 27 Haemorrhagic RH MCA 545.4
P30 60 F Right 14.5 35 Ischaemic RH MCA 42.9
P31 55 M Left 12 26 Ischaemic RH
P32 81 F Right 10 37 Ischaemic RH MCA 135.8
P33 55 F Right 12 31 Ischaemic LH MCA 210.4
P34 41 M Right 12 5 Ischaemic RH/LH PICA 180.9
P35 65 F Right 12 32 Ischaemic RH/LH PICA 291.5
P36 64 M Right 9 6 Ischaemic RH MCA 38.2
P37 57 M Right 18 18 Ischaemic RH MCA 83.4
P38 51 M Left 10 6 Ischaemic LH MCA 48.5
P39 65 M Right 10 25 Ischaemic RH MCA 34.3
P40 70 F Right 10 9 Ischaemic RH/LH PICA 107.3
P41 59 M Right 10 6 Ischaemic LH MCA 236.8
P42 51 M Left 10 5 Ischaemic RH MCA 31.3
P43 79 M Right 8 4 Ischaemic RH/LH MCA
P44 43 M Right 12 6 Ischaemic RH MCA 80.3
P45 62 M Right 7 3 Ischaemic RH ACA 9.6
P46 70 F Right 12 6 Ischaemic RH 10.0
P47 65 M Right 10 9 Ischaemic LH PCA 343.3
P48 59 M Right 12 10 Ischaemic RH Brainstem 31.7
P49 69 M Right 13 13 Ischaemic LH MCA
P50 45 F Right 11 6 Ischaemic RH MCA/ICA 73.5
P51 61 M Right 10 4 Ischaemic RH MCA 29.2
P52 74 M Right 11 4 Ischaemic RH MCA 722.4
P53 81 M Right 12 1 Ischaemic RH PCA 17.9
P54 61 M Left 16 1 Haemorrhagic LH PCA 2.9
P55 76 M Right 12 3 Ischaemic RH MCA 642.1
P56 58 M Right 10 8 Ischaemic RH MCA 10.6
P57 48 F Right 12 4 Ischaemic LH MCA 158.5
P58 68 M Right 10 6 Ischaemic LH ICA 517.1
P59 68 M Right 12 2 Ischaemic RH ACA 416.9
P60 49 M Right 12 38 Ischaemic RH Lacunar 20.0
P61 41 F Right 16 4 Ischaemic RH MCA 69.0
P62 23 F Right 12 10 Ischaemic LH/RH MCA 1655.3
P63 43 F Right 12 2 Ischaemic LH PCA 112.0
P64 69 F Right 12 49 Ischaemic RH MCA
P65 64 M Right 9 32 Ischaemic RH Lacunar
P66 60 M Right 10 34 Ischaemic RH MCA

Education is reported in years, Chronicity reports the number of days between stroke and assessment, Stroke Side reports the hemisphere impacted by stroke, and Artery reports the vascular territory impacted by each stroke (if known). Volume reports the lesion volume (in cm3) for the 54 patients who were included in this study’s lesion analyses

Behavioural data from 70 healthy ageing control subjects (mean age = 64.5 (SD = 10.3, range = 41–88), 42.8% Female, 5.7% left-handed) were also collected. These controls were recruited through the University of Queensland (UQ) networks under the above UQ Human Research Ethics. Controls were excluded if they reported a history of major neurological of psychiatric disorder. Control participants did not significantly differ from the included stroke participants in terms of age (t(123.6) =  − 1.42, p = 0.159, 95% CI =  − 6.90 to 1.41), sex (X2(2) = 0.958, p = 0.328 (Yates corrected)), or handedness (X2(2) = 1.054, p = 0.305 (Yates corrected)).

Behavioural assessment

Each participant completed a series of standardised neuropsychological assessments aiming to determine pre-morbid cognitive abilities and to quantify executive function abilities. Specifically, each participant completed the National Adult Reading Test (NART) which, in line with standard protocols (Bright et al. 2002, 2018), was used to estimate pre-morbid optimal levels of functioning (Nelson and Willison 1991). In this task, participants are instructed to read a series of 50 increasingly difficult, irregularly spelled words aloud whilst a trained administrator scores each response for accuracy. This task is commonly employed in clinical environments to estimate pre-morbid intelligence (Nelson and Willison 1991).

Next, the Stroop Test (Victoria Version) was used to provide a standard measure of executive function abilities (Strauss et al. 2006). In this task, participants are first presented with 24 coloured dots arranged in a grid and are asked to name the colours of the dots as quickly as possible. Next, participants are presented with a grid of 24 non-colour words (e.g., FISH) printed in colour ink and are asked to name the ink colour of these stimuli as quickly as possible. Finally, participants are then presented with 24 colour words printed in an incongruent colour and are asked to name the ink colours as quickly as possible. According to standard guidelines, participants are scored according to the time required to complete each task, accuracy, and the interference time (time on the word condition divided by time on the colour dot condition) (Strauss et al. 2006).

Finally, each participant completed the Hayling Sentence Completion Test which was administered and scored in line with the published, standard guidelines (Burgess and Shallice 1997). Specifically, each participant was presented with 30 sentence frames which were missing the final word. In the first condition (Initiation) each sentence was read aloud and the participant was instructed to provide a word which completed each sentence frame in a logical and sensible manor (e.g., prompt = He posted a letter without a …, response = stamp). In the second condition (Suppression), this procedure was repeated but participants were instructed to complete each sentence with an unconnected response (e.g., prompt = He posted a letter without a…, response = banana). For each sentence, response time and response accuracy were recorded. These data were used to calculate average response time differences between Initiation and Suppression condition responses and to convert response time and accuracy scores into scaled scores (in line with standard scoring guidelines). Each scaled score ranged between 1 and 10, corresponding to the following normative data percentiles: 1 = out of normal range or < 1st percentile, 2 = 1st percentile, 3 = 5th percentile, 4 = 10th percentile, 5 = 25th percentile, 6 = 50th percentile, 7 = 75th percentile, 8 = 90th percentile, 9 = 95th percentile, 10 = 99th percentile (Burgess and Shallice 1997). All Suppression condition responses were categorised into eight possible response types (Robinson et al. 2015) (Table 2) to facilitate the analysis of strategy use. The frequency of each response category was recorded for each participant. The order of cognitive tests was counterbalanced to prevent systematic variance due to testing order.

Table 2.

Categories used to categorise responses from the Hayling Test Suppression condition. These categories were defined by Robinson et al. (Robinson et al. 2015) based on the data reported by Burgess and Shallice (Burgess and Shallice 1996)

Response category definition Description and examples
Category A error (Blatant connected response)
A1. Sensible sentence completion “The dough was put in the hot…oven
Category B error (Partially connected response)

B2. Semantically related/opposite to response

B3. Semantically related to sentence

B4. Semantically related and bizarre

“The dough was put in the hot…sink/freezer

“The dough was put in the hot…bread

“The whole town came to hear the mayor…cry

Category C correct response (unconnected to sentence)

C5. Correct and visible

C6. Correct and semantically related to previous response

C7. Correct and both visible and semantically related to previous response

C8. Correct with no obvious strategy

C9. Correct with other strategy

“The dough was put in the hot…table

“The dough was put in the hot…orange” (previous response “banana”)

“The dough was put in the hot…chair” (previous response “table”)

“The dough was put in the hot…train

Neuroimaging data

Routinely collected clinical neuroimaging data (10 CT, 1 T1 MR, 43 FLAIR) was employed to create binarized lesion masks for each included patient (Fig. 1). Previous research has demonstrated that similar routinely collected neuroimaging data are of sufficient quality to reliably create lesion masks which are able to accurately identify the correlates of a wide range of cognitive impairments in acute stroke populations (Moore et al. 2023a, b, c; Moore and Demeyere 2022). Further, Moore et al. (2023a, b, c) have demonstrated that lesion mapping results yielded from CT-based analyses agree well with those produced using MR modalities. Within this study, all lesion masks were processed using the standard processing protocol reported by Moore (2022). Specifically, all lesions were manually delineated on native space scans by trained experts and smoothed at 5 mm full width at half maximum in the z-direction using MRIcron (Rorden 2007). These native-space masks and scans were then reoriented to the anterior commissure and warped into 1 × 1x1 MNI space using Statistical Parametric Mapping (Ashburner et al. 2016) and age-matched templates from Clinical Toolbox (Rorden et al. 2012). All normalised scans were visually inspected for quality prior to inclusion in lesion mapping analyses.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Lesion overlay for the 54 patients included in this study’s lesion analyses. Colour represents the number of lesions overlapping at each location. MNI z-coordinates -22–41 are visualised

Lesion mapping analyses

As previous research has strongly suggested that initiation and inhibition require collaboration between a distributed network of neural correlates, network-level lesion mapping analyses were employed in this study. This technique is commonly employed and offers key insight into how disruptions in brain networks are associated with cognitive impairments (Cohen et al. 2021; Moore et al. 2023a, b, c; Saxena et al. 2022).

First, lesion quantification toolkit (Griffis et al. 2021) was used to generate disconnection statistics by overlaying each binarised lesion masks onto the Schaefer-Yeo Atlas Parcellation (100 parcels, 7 Networks) (Schaefer et al. 2018). An additional 35 subcortical ROIs derived from the Harvard–Oxford Subcortical Atlas and the Automated Anatomical Labelling Atlas (version 3). These atlases were employed as, together, they offer an optimal balance between complexity and interpretability. Lesion quantification toolkit (Griffis et al. 2021) estimates parcel-wise disconnection severities by calculating the number of normative HCP-842 streamlines which bilaterally terminate within each pair of atlas-defined grey matter parcels. This process yields disconnection matrices in which the value in each cell represents the percentage of disconnected streamlines (edges) connecting each of the defined grey matter parcels (nodes) per patient (135 nodes, 18,225 edges) (Griffis et al. 2021). For each network edge a regression was conducted to compare percent disconnection to behavioural scores. Each regression controlled for lesion volume, and only edges damaged in > 10% of included patients were analysed (1766 edges included). All conducted comparisons employ 5% false discovery rate to correct for inflated false positive rates due to multiple comparisons. This approach is similar to network-level lesion-mapping methodologies employed in previous studies (Gleichgerrcht et al. 2017; Saxena et al. 2022).

In total, 21 network-level lesion mapping analyses were conducted using the following outcome variables: Stroop Dot test (mean RT), Stroop colour-word test (mean RT, interference score), Hayling Initiation (mean RT, total Correct), Hayling Inhibition (mean RT, total correct), Hayling Strategy use (A1-C8, no obvious strategy count, proportion of responses using strategies), Hayling Scaled Score (calculated in line with standard scoring procedures), Hayling difference between Initiation/Suppression response times, and Hayling Global Error Score (total number of errors).

In addition to this, network-level overlap comparisons were conducted to evaluate the degree of similarity between the correlates associated with Stroop and Hayling Test scores aiming to assess analogous functions. Specifically, three overlap comparisons were conducted to compare Stroop Dot Time score versus Hayling Initiation time, and Stroop Colour-Word Time versus Hayling Inhibition time, Stroop Interference Score versus Hayling Inhibition versus Initiation time difference. In each case, the number and identity of any network edges impacted in both compared tests are reported.

Statistical analyses

First, included behavioural data were inspected for statistical outliers which deviated from score means by > 3.5 standard deviations. This more liberal inclusion threshold was adopted to account for the large amount of behavioural variance expected within the acute stroke population (Demeyere et al. 2015). As the majority of considered behavioural variables were not normally distributed (p < 0.05), non-parametric tests were used (i.e., Spearman’s correlations and Mann–Whitney U tests for between-group comparisons). All reported confidence intervals (CI) refer to 95% confidence intervals.

Next, differences in demographic and clinical characteristics were examined between the acute stroke and healthy control groups. Specifically, Mann–Whitney U tests were used for continuous variables (i.e., age, years of education, estimated premorbid intelligence) and Chi-square tests for independence (with Yates continuity correction) were used for categorical variables (i.e., biological sex and handedness).

To identify significant Stroop and Hayling test performance differences between control and stroke patients, Mann–Whitney U tests were conducted on each of the 21 outcome variables of interest (Stroop Dot test (mean RT), Stroop colour-word test (mean RT, interference score), Hayling Initiation (mean RT, total Correct), Hayling Inhibition (mean RT, total correct), Hayling Strategy use (A1-C8, no obvious strategy count, agrammatical correct responses count, proportion of responses using strategies), Hayling Scaled Score (calculated in line with standard scoring procedures), Hayling difference between Initiation/Suppression response times), and Hayling Global Error Score (total number of errors).

Within strategy use variables, prevalence of each response type was calculated as a proportion of either correct or incorrect responses. For example, incorrect answers (categories A and B) were calculated by dividing the subcategory raw score by the total number of errors. Conversely, prevalence of correct answer types (Category C) was calculated by dividing the subcategory raw score by the total number correct. This strategy was adopted to ensure these analyses were investigating proportion of reliance on each strategy/error type independent of overall accuracy. Where appropriate, Bonferroni corrections for multiple comparisons are employed. Each applied alpha threshold is reported and justified prior to reporting results of each conducted comparison.

Performance on analogous Stroop and Hayling Test variables was compared both in terms of score correlations (non-parametric Spearman rank correlations) and in terms of sensitivity/specificity. These sensitivity/specificity calculations are not meant to be interpreted in terms of diagnostic accuracy but are instead reported to comprehensively summarise the degree of agreement between normal/abnormal performance categorisations reported by each of these tasks. These comparisons report the sensitivity/specificity of Stroop Test metrics relative to the “standard” of Hayling test metrics.

As past literature has suggested that Category B Hayling errors are subtle semantically related responses rather than blatant inhibition failures, the underlying cognitive process may differ from inhibition per se. In focal frontal lesion patients, all frontal patients make a high number of Category A Hayling errors (i.e., blatant inhibitory failures) but only the right lateral frontal patients make a high number of Category B Hayling errors, which are subtle and likely implicate a failure of other processes such as monitoring (Robinson et al. 2015, 2016). Further, healthy older adults increasingly produce category B but not A errors (Gibson et al. 2018). Taken together, these findings suggest differing cognitive and neural underpinnings. A series of secondary, exploratory analyses were, therefore, conducted to identify patterns of disconnection associated with these errors in more detail. First, linear regression and ANOVA analyses were conducted to identify stroke-related and clinical factors associate with the occurrence of Category B Hayling errors. These analyses included age, stroke side, stroke territory, and lesion volume as predictors. Next, lesion overlap images were created to visualise qualitative difference between patients in the top and bottom quantiles of Hayling Category B error commission. A similar analysis was repeated within the network-level data to identify connections that, when disrupted, resulted in high versus low occurrence of Category B errors. Specifically, the value of each considered edge was determined by calculating the number of Category B errors which occurred in patients with damage to the edge and dividing this by the number of patients with damage to this edge. This score summarizes the prevalence of Category B errors at each edge whilst accounting that some edges were damaged more frequently in the sample than others. Importantly, these visualisations are not meant to identify statistically significant correlates, but instead aim to identify potential trends which can then be further investigated in larger samples.

Results

Behavioural results

Stroke patients were found to commit significantly more errors on the NART than controls (patient mean = 22.9 (SD = 7.05, range = 3–42) versus control mean = 16.6 (SD = 6.94, range = 4–36) (W = 2968.5, p < 0.001, 95% CI 5.00–10.00)). In line with this, controls had a higher average estimated IQ than stroke patients (patient mean = 102 (SD = 8.73, range = 79–127) versus control mean = 110 (SD = 8.65, range = 86–126))(W = 953.5, p < 0.001, 95% CI: − 12.0 to − 6.0).

Next, control and stroke patient Stroop Test performance was compared. As 4 Stroop Test variables were compared across groups, a Bonferroni-corrected alpha threshold of 0.0125 was used for these analyses. Patients took significantly more time than controls to complete the Stroop test Dot condition (patient mean = 16.6 s, control mean = 12.5 s, W = 2766, p < 0.001, CI 1.81–4.37). Patients and controls completion times were also significantly different within the Stroop Colour-Word condition (patient mean = 35.7 s, control mean = 26.5 s, W = 2486, p < 0.001, CI 2.45–9.07). However, Stroop Colour-Word Interference scores were not significantly different between patient and control subjects (patient mean = 2.25, control mean = 2.15, W = 1830.5, p = 0.8505, CI − 0.17 to 0.22) and patients and controls were not found to have significantly different Stroop Colour-Word accuracy scores (patient mean = 64.61% correct, control mean = 75.5% correct, W = 1412, p = 0.0176, CI − 2.40 to − 1.9 × 10–5).

Control data were then used to calculate Stroop Test impairment thresholds and identify patients exhibiting abnormal performance (Fig. 2). Stroop Dot times > 17.5 s, Colour-Word times greater than 43.6 s, interference scores > 3.40 s, and cumulative percent correct scores < 9.86 were considered to represent abnormal performance. In line with these thresholds, 18 patients exhibited abnormal scores on Stroop Dot Time, 8 on Colour-Word Time, 12 on Colour-Word Percent Correct, and 5 on Interference Scores.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

A visualisation of control and patient performance on the Stroop Test. Number of participants is represented on the y-axis and impairment cut-off thresholds are marked by dotted lines

Next, control and participant Hayling Test performance was compared. As seven independent Hayling performance metrics were included in these analyses, a Bonferroni corrected alpha level of 0.006 was employed. Table 3 summarizes the results of these comparisons. Hayling Overall Scaled Score was found to be significantly lower within patients than within control participants. Correspondingly, Hayling Global Error score was significantly higher in patients than in controls. Hayling Initiation Total Correct Scores were not found to be significantly different between patients and controls, but patients exhibited significantly lower Hayling Inhibition Total Scores than controls. Control participants were found to employ strategies in a significantly higher proportion of correct response trials. All other considered that general Hayling performance variables were not found to differ significantly across patients and controls (see Table 3 for details).

Table 3.

A comparison of patient versus control group performance on the Stroop and Hayling Test general performance metrics

Metric Patient Control W p CI Cut Nimp
Stroop test
 Dot time 16.64 (6.6) 12.5 (2.3) 2766.0  < 0.001 1.81 to 4.37  > 17.05 18
 Colour-word time 36.84 (24.4) 26.5 (8.6) 2486.0  < 0.001 2.45 to 9.07  > 43.6 8
 Colour word percent correct 58.64 (75.5) 75.5 (32.8) 1412.0 0.018 − 24.00 to 0.00  < 9.86 12
 Interference score 2.27 (0.9) 2.15 (0.64) 1830.5 0.850 − 0.17 to 0.22  > 3.4 5
Hayling test
 Overall scaled score 4.21 (1.8) 5.63 (1.28) 1156.0  < 0.001 − 2.00 to 0.00  < 3.1 21
 Global error score 12.69 (11.9) 5.6 (5.36) 2747.5  < 0.001 1.00 to 7.00  > 16.3 20
 Proportion strategy use 0.43 (0.3) 0.63 (0.25) 1294.0  < 0.001 − 0.30 to − 0.08  < 0.13 12
 Initiation mean RT 1.31 (0.9) 0.94 (0.39) 2310.5 0.031 0.02–0.33  > 1.72 10
 Initiation mean correct 14.35 (1.5) 14.8 (0.55) 1749.0 0.109 0.00–0.00  < 13.6 7
 Inhibition mean RT 4.43 (3.8) 2.86(1.96) 2384.0 0.012 0.21–1.61  > 6.8 10
 Inhibition/initiation RT difference 47.6 (66.0) 29.1 (28) 2298.5 0.199 − 3.00 to 18.00  > 85.1 14
 Inhibition total correct 8.5 (4.5) 11.4 (3.01) 1248.0  < 0.001 − 4.00 to − 1.00  < 4.7 18

Group means are presented alongside standard deviations (in parentheses)

P p-value, CI 95% confidence interval boundaries, max cut control mean plus two standard deviations, min cut control mean minus two standard deviations, N imp number of patients with scores outside this normative performance range

Notably, significant disparity was present within the specific patients who met abnormal performance thresholds on analogous Stroop and Hayling Test metrics. Only 6/18 of the patients exhibiting abnormal performance on the Stroop Dot Time metric also exhibited abnormal performance on Hayling Initiation Time (sensitivity = 66.6%, specificity = 75.0%). Of the 8 patients exhibiting impairment on the Stroop Colour-Word Time metric, 2 were also impaired on Hayling Inhibition Response Time (sensitivity = 20.0%, specificity = 86.0%). Of the 5 patients with abnormal Stroop Interference Scores, 1 exhibited abnormal performance on the Hayling Response Time Difference Metric (sensitivity = 10.0%, specificity = 90.5%). Finally, 6/12 patients with abnormal Stroop Test Percent Correct also exhibited abnormal performance according to the Hayling Global Error Score (sensitivity = 27.8%, specificity = 80.0%). In addition to this, regression analyses revealed that scores analogous Stroop and Hayling metrics measuring initiation time and overall percent correct were significantly correlated, but analogous scores aiming to tap inhibition time and interference were not significantly correlated (Bonferroni-corrected alpha = 0.0125, see Fig. 3).

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3

A comparison of analogous metrics derived from Stroop and Hayling Tests. Outcome categories are calculated using Hayling Scores as references. R reports rho values from Spearman’s correlations. Comparisons surviving multiple corrections are starred

Next, differences in Hayling Strategy use were compared across patient and control groups. As 10 distinct error/strategy categories were considered, a Bonferroni-corrected significance threshold of 0.005 was used within these analyses (Table 4). Within incorrect responses, patients were found to commit a greater proportion of errors in which the response was semantically related (B3) to a sensible completion. Within correct responses, controls exhibited a greater proportion of responses using visible objects (C5) and visible objects which were related to previous responses (C7). Conversely, stroke patients exhibited a higher proportion of correct responses which did not use a strategy than controls (C8). All other considered variables were not found to be significantly different across groups (Table 4).

Table 4.

A comparison of patient versus control group performance on Hayling Test error type and strategy use metrics

Strategy use variables
Category Patient Control W p CI Max cut Min cut Nimp
A: Blatant connected 0.35 (0.3) 0.26 (0.3) 1638.5 0.0530 − 0.20 to 0.00 0.92 − 0.40 5
B: Partially connected 0.6 (0.3) 0.54 (0.4) 1942 0.6710 − 0.17 to 0.07 1.36 − 0.28 0
B2: Related/opposite 0.25 (0.3) 0.28 (0.3) 2066.5 0.8590 − 0.03 to 0.06 0.90 − 0.34 2
B3: Related 0.2 (0.3) 0.08 (0.2) 1511.5 0.0040 − 0.08 to 0.00 0.41 − 0.26 11
B4: Related/bizarre 0.15 (0.2) 0.18 (0.2) 2181 0.4460 0.00 to 0.06 0.66 − 0.29 3
C5: Visible 0.2 (0.3) 0.34 (0.2) 2779.5  < 0.001 0.08 to 0.22 0.82 − 0.13 2
C6: Previous response 0.16 (0.2) 0.16 (0.1) 2295.5 0.1950 0.00 to 0.08 0.45 − 0.12 7
C7: Visible and related 0.05 (0.1) 0.1 (0.1) 2692  < 0.001 0.00 to 0.08 0.34 − 0.14 2
C8: No strategy 0.55 (0.3) 0.37 (0.3) 1351.5 0.0012 − 0.29 to − 0.07 0.87 − 0.12 11
C9: Other strategy 0.03 (0.1) 0.02 (0.08) 1915 0.3400 0.00 to 0.00 0.17 − 0.14 2

Group means are presented alongside standard deviations (in parentheses)

P p-value, CI 95% confidence interval boundaries, max cut control mean plus two standard deviations, min cut control mean minus two standard deviations, N imp number of patients with scores outside this normative performance range

Lesion-mapping results

Next, network-level lesion mapping analyses were conducted to identify patterns of dysconnectivity significantly associated with performance on each considered behavioural metric. Full lists of the network edges associated with each considered outcome measure can be found in supplementary materials. Within the Stroop Test, response time within the dot condition was significantly associated with dysconnectivity in 27 edges spanning left hemisphere temporo-parietal networks including the visual, default, and limbic networks as well as several subcortical nodes (lenticular and caudate nuclei) (Fig. 4). Response time within the Stroop Colour-Word Interference condition was also associated with dysconnectivity within left hemisphere temporo-parietal networks, but was also linked to disconnection within cross-hemisphere connections linking left and right hemisphere fronto-temporal areas. Accuracy within the Stroop Colour-Word Interference condition was primarily associated with damage to these cross-hemisphere fronto-parietal network connections. Finally, overall percent correct on the Stroop Test was associated with disconnection within left hemisphere temporo-parietal areas as well as some cross-hemisphere structural connections (Fig. 4).

Fig. 4.

Fig. 4

Schaefer-Yeo Atlas network edges significantly associated with Stroop Test metric. Nodes are plotted as white dots and edge colour represents adjusted R2 value for each base regression analysis. On each slice, node locations are collapsed into 2 dimensions and plotted in order of ascending R2 value (highest R2 values on top). Names, MNI coordinates, and statistics for each significant node are openly available https://osf.io/mbsd4/

Hayling Overall Scaled Score was associated with disconnection within 66 network edges, mainly involving dysconnectivity between left hemisphere posterior and frontal areas as well as disconnection between right hemisphere frontal areas and the brainstem. Accuracy within the Initiation Condition was linked to disconnection within a large number of network edges (n = 302) spanning a diverse range of correlates in both the right and left hemispheres. Notably, no network edges were found to be associated with number of correct responses within the Hayling Suppression Condition. Mean response time within the Hayling Initiation Condition was associated with damage to 59 network edges spanning left hemisphere language areas and cross-hemisphere fronto-temporal connections. Conversely, mean response time within the Suppression condition was linked to damage to 11 edges mainly linking left hemisphere ventral attention network nodes to right hemisphere dorsal attention, limbic, visual, and default network nodes. Finally, response time difference between the suppression and initiation conditions was associated with disconnection within 6 edges, connecting a diverse range of right and left network nodes (Fig. 5).

Fig. 5.

Fig. 5

Schaefer-Yeo Atlas network edges significantly associated with Hayling Test general performance metrics. Nodes are plotted as white dots and edge colour represents adjusted R2 value for each base regression analysis. On each slice, node locations are collapsed into 2 dimensions and plotted in order of ascending R2 value (highest R2 values on top). Names, MNI coordinates, and statistics for each significant edge are openly available https://osf.io/mbsd4/

Finally, five of the considered ten Hayling Strategy use variables were significantly associated with localised patterns of structural dysconnectivity (Fig. 6). Inhibition section errors involving errantly reporting a response associated with the sentence (Category B3) were linked with disconnection in a single edge connecting the left hemisphere somatic motor network (division 4) to the left dorsal attention network posterior division 4. Errors involving bizarre strategies (B4) were associated with disconnection in 12 network edges connecting a range of right and left hemisphere structures. Inhibition section correct responses involving reporting an unrelated, but visible object response were linked to disconnection within two edges connecting the left hemisphere somatic motor network (division 3) to the left dorsal attention network (posterior divisions 2 and 4). Correct responses which were semantically connected to previous responses and visible (category C7) were associated an edge connecting the right caudate nucleus to the right cerebellum (division 9). Finally, correct answers employing other strategies were linked to disconnection in three edges connecting the left visual network divisions 4 and 8, the left default network precuneus/posterior cerebral cortex (division 1) and the right default temporal division 3, and connecting the left cerebellum (crus) to the left cerebellum (division 6).

Fig. 6.

Fig. 6

Schaefer-Yeo Atlas network edges significantly associated with Hayling Test strategy use metrics. Nodes are plotted as white dots and edge colour represents adjusted R2 value for each base regression analysis. On each slice, node locations are collapsed into 2 dimensions and plotted in order of ascending R2 value (highest R2 values on top). Names, MNI coordinates, and statistics for each significant node are openly available https://osf.io/mbsd4/

Comparison of stroop and hayling neural correlates

Four network-level overlap comparisons were conducted to evaluate the degree of similarity between the correlates associated with Stroop and Hayling Test scores aiming to assess analogous functions. First, the network level correlates of Stroop Dot Time and Hayling Initiation response time were compared. Of the 86 network-edges found to be significantly related to these variables, 6 were common across both tests. These common edges mainly involved connections between left hemisphere visual areas (divisions 4 and 5) and the caudate nucleus and brainstem. The network edge connecting the left caudate nucleus and left default network precuneus/posterior parietal cortex component was also significantly associated with performance on both tests.

Next, networks associated with Stroop Colour-Word time and Hayling Inhibition time were compared. Of the 78 edges associated with these conditions, only one connection between the left caudate nucleus and left default network (posterior parietal/precuneus division 2) was common across both tests. No common edges were present between the networks associated with Stroop Interference Scores and Hayling Inhibition versus Initiation response time difference.

Secondary analyses: neural basis of category B hayling errors

Finally, a series of exploratory analyses were conducted to identify neural correlates associated with the commission of category. Within patients, the proportion of errors classed as Category B was not found to be significantly predicted by stroke volume (F(1,50) = 1.197, adjusted R2 = 0.004, p = 0.2791) but was predicted by age (F(1,54) = 5.552, Adjusted R2 = 0.07644, p = 0.022) (Bonferroni-corrected alpha = 0.05). Patients with left hemisphere strokes committed a significantly higher proportion of Category B errors than patient with right hemisphere strokes (0.742 vs 0.525 respectively, F(1,53) = 7.61, p = 0.008). However, this proportion was not significantly different across patients with MCA (0.535), ACA (0.543), PCA (0.751), lacunar (0.641), and PICA (0.639) territory stokes (F(4,46) = 0.943, p = 0.448). Notably, when all these considered factors were entered into a single regression analysis, no factors were found to be significantly predictive of the proportion of Category B errors committed (all p values > 0.60). This relationship remained unchanged when the raw number of Category B errors committed (instead of proportion of Category B errors) was used as the outcome variable (all p values > 0.027, Bonferroni-corrected alpha = 0.006).

Figure 7 presents an overlap of the lesions from patients who committed the highest and lowest proportions of Category B errors. Specifically, the top panel visualises the lesions from patients in the top quantile (proportion of Category B errors > 0.875, n = 10) whilst the bottom panel visualises lesions from the bottom quantile (proportion < 0.667, n = 10). Both patient groups exhibited a wide variance in lesion locations with the highest degree of overlap (n = 3) centred within the left supramarginal gyrus. Notably, the patients in the top quantile exhibited no frontal lesions.

Fig. 7.

Fig. 7

A comparison of the lesion overlays across patients committing the highest and lowest proportion of Category B errors on the Hayling test. Colour represents number of overlapping lesions. MNI slices − 22 to 36 are visualised

Similar analyses were conducted to identify patterns of disconnection associated with the highest and lowest proportion of Category B Errors (Fig. 8). In total, 317 network edges were associated with high Category B error prevalence, whilst 337 edges were associated with comparatively low Category B error prevalence. Both these high and low prevalence edges spanned an expansive range of partially overlapping brain regions. However, the edges associated with high Category B prevalence appeared to involve slightly more anterior connections (e.g., frontal disconnection) as compared to the network associated with low Category B error prevalence (Fig. 8).

Fig. 8.

Fig. 8

A visualisation of the network edges associated with comparatively high and low Category B error prevalence. Edge values represent the number of Category B errors committed in patients with damage to each edge divided by the number of times this edge was impacted in the full sample. All network nodes and edge locations are defined using the Schaefer-Yeo Atlas

Discussion

Overall, this project provides a novel investigation of Hayling test performance and strategy use within the stroke population. Although the Stroop test is commonly used to assess initiation and inhibition cognitive abilities, the results of this study strongly suggest that this task taps behaviourally and neuroanatomically distinct functions than those assessed in the Hayling test. Behavioural metrics derived from the Hayling test were found to provide detailed insight into abnormal patterns of both general performance impairment and strategy use in stroke survivors. These findings are important when considered in the context of clinical practice, as they highlight potential avenues for improving detection and diagnosis of common post-stroke cognitive impairments.

The findings of this study are in line with previous research suggesting that the Stroop and Hayling tests likely tap dissociable components of executive functions (Cipolotti et al. 2016). This conclusion is supported by both the behavioural and anatomical findings of this study. First, stroke patients were found to exhibit qualitative performance differences across Stroop and Hayling Test metrics intended to tap similar functions. Only minimal overlap was present between the specific patients exhibiting abnormal performance on analogous Stroop and Hayling test measures. While Stroop test metrics were found to have relatively high specificity to abnormal performance (specificity range = 75.0–90.5), this test was found to have very low sensitivity compared to analogous Hayling measures (sensitivity range = 10.0–66.6). This difference cannot be explained as a function of task difficulty, as performance on analogous Stroop and Hayling interference and inhibition measures were not found to be significantly correlated. In terms of neural correlates, only minimal overlap was present between the network-level correlates of analogous Stroop and Hayling test metrics. The highest degree of overlap was present between Stroop Dot Time and Hayling Initiation Time metrics, which tap initiation, with 6 (6.97%) of the 86 implicated network edges being common across both conditions. Overall, the findings of this investigation strongly suggest that the functions tapped by the Stroop and Hayling test are both behaviourally and anatomically dissociable.

There are several potential explanations for this dissociation. First, past research has suggested that it is possible for patients to complete the Stroop test without relying on the initiation/inhibition executive functions this test is designed to assess (Stuss and Alexander 2007). This is because participants can reduce interference from incongruent colour words by adopting strategies in which they do not read the full word whilst naming ink colours. This strategy may be more commonly adopted in the stroke population, as comorbid visual, spatial attention, language, or acquired dyslexia deficits would be expected to reduce the degree to which word stimuli are “automatically” read (Coslett and Turkeltaub 2016; Leff and Starrfelt 2014; Vallar et al. 2010). This potential confound may help explain the documented disparity between performance on purportedly analogous Stroop and Hayling test metrics. Additionally, Stroop test performance was found to vary widely in this study’s sample of healthy ageing controls, with many controls committing more errors than what was average within the patient sample. This is in line with previous work documenting declines in initiation/inhibition abilities within healthy ageing populations (Cervera-Crespo and González-Alvarez 2017; Gibson et al. 2018; West and Alain 2000). However, this high control variability does suggest that the Stroop test may not adequately be able to distinguish between age-related performance decrements and stroke-specific cognitive impairment. These implications are critically important when considered in the context of current clinical practice.

Variations of classical Stroop tasks are commonly used as screening tools for inhibition/initiation impairments within stroke survivors (Troyer et al. 2006). The findings of this study are critically important when considered in the context of this practice, as they suggest that abnormal performance on the Stroop test may not serve as an effective method for detecting these impairments in the stroke population. First, healthy ageing controls and stroke patients were not found to perform significantly differently on the Stroop colour-word task accuracy and interference scores. These metrics are the key measures expected to differ between clinical and control populations (Troyer et al. 2006). This lack of significant difference is likely due to the high score variance present within the control population coupled with the fact that not all stroke patients would be expected to perform abnormally on this task. This lack of difference alone does not undermine the validity of the Stroop Test but does suggest that cut-offs derived from this variable control performance may lack sensitivity to detect abnormal performance relative to other measures. This implication is supported by this study as only 5 patients met impairment criteria on Stroop-Colour Word Interference Scores, versus 14 on the analogous Hayling Response Time difference measure. This finding implies that more sensitive and targeted assessments are needed to detect inhibition/initiation deficits with high sensitivity in the stroke population.

The findings of this project suggest that the Hayling test may offer an effective alternative screening method for initiation and inhibition deficits in the stroke population. First, Hayling test metrics were found to be significantly different across stroke patient and control populations. Specifically, stroke survivors exhibited worse performance on Hayling Overall Scaled Scores, Global Error Scores, and Inhibition Accuracy versus control subjects. This is important when compared to the Stroop Test in which the only metric which was significantly different between controls and patients was the baseline dot-colour naming response time measure. In line with this, more patients were categorized as demonstrating abnormal performance on Hayling test measures relative to Stroop test measures, suggesting a comparative increase in screening sensitivity. In addition to this improved general sensitivity to potential impairment, Hayling strategy-use variables highlight additional differences between patient and control populations.

As found in previous studies, stroke participants committed more Hayling test suppression errors than controls (Laakso et al. 2019; Nijsse et al. 2019). However, this is the first investigation to explore Hayling task strategy use in stroke survivors. Previous work has explored differences in general strategy formation skills in stroke patients (e.g., Laakso et al. 2019), but this study did not quantify how differences in strategy formation may impact performance on the Hayling task. This analysis adds to this existing literature by providing novel insight into strategy differences underlying normal and abnormal Hayling test performance. First, a significantly higher proportion of control participant responses involved using a strategy to facilitate a correct response. Specifically, controls were significantly more likely to use strategies involving reporting visual items (See Table 3). While no other statistically significant group-level strategy-use differences emerged between patients and controls, strategy-use analyses facilitated the identification of individual patients exhibiting abnormal response types across a range of detailed, response-type metrics.

Notably, different Hayling test strategy-use measures were found to be associated with distinct network-level neural correlates. Specifically, correct responses involving reporting visual objects, visual objects related to previous responses, and other (undefined) strategy use were each significantly associated with non-overlapping patterns of network disconnection. Errors involving reporting responses semantically related to the sentence or bizarre errors were also found to be related to significant and distinct patterns of disconnection. These identified network-level correlates were found to be relatively restricted with comparatively few significantly involved nodes compared to general task metrics. However, these results did reach statistical significance following very strict corrections for lesion volume and multiple comparisons. Future, targeted studies are needed to fully quantify the patterns of disconnection associated with different Hayling test strategies and to interpret the functional significance of the identified network correlates. However, the present investigation is important in that it provides preliminary evidence that these different behavioural patterns are linked to distinct patterns of dysconnectivity. This is an important finding as it suggests that differences in strategy use are not just a result of patients preferred approaches but instead may be linked to distinct patterns of stroke-specific disconnection. This finding emphasizes the added utility of employing the Hayling Test in stroke populations, as it provides insight into what strategies patients are employing as well as assessing general initiation and suppression abilities.

This information can be used to direct more in-depth neuropsychological assessments and to detect abnormal strategy use patterns even in the absence of overall Hayling test impairment. The ability to generate and implement a strategy is crucial for rehabilitation to compensate for weaknesses and to facilitate the successful completion of everyday activities. Strategy training has been shown to be effective at overcoming suppression failures (Robinson et al. 2016), as well as multitasking (Rand et al. 2009) and cognitive flexibility and disability in stroke (Skidmore et al. 2015). We propose that analysis of strategy generation via the Hayling Test not only provides clinicians information regarding another aspect of executive functioning, but also provides information to support rehabilitation planning. That is, it can provide an indication of an individual’s capacity to problem solve and compensate for deficits. In this case, strategy generation and use appear to assist with overcoming difficulties with inhibitory control, which is important for goal-directed behaviour in daily life.

Notably, no significant network level correlates of Hayling Test Category B errors were identified in this study. This finding is surprising when considered in the context of previous functional imaging studies which have suggested that suppression abilities (as quantified by the Hayling test) are related to activity within a network of left prefrontal areas (Collette et al. 2001). There are several reasons why these previous findings were not replicated in this study. First, functional imaging is able to identify correlates involved in cognitive processes but cannot clearly distinguish whether these correlates are merely involved with or are necessary for the cognitive function of interest. It is possible that documented activation of left fronto-temporal areas may be related to the language component of the Hayling test (e.g., verbalising responses) rather than to cognitive inhibition functions. Qualitative analysis of Hayling test and lesion data suggest that disconnection of frontal network edges is associated with a disproportionately high prevalence of Category B Hayling errors, but further data is needed before this implication can be either confidently supported or refuted. It is also possible that this negative finding is related to variability in network-level statistical power, as the probability of committing network-edge level false negative detection varies as a function of how many patients have damage at each specific edge (Griffis et al. 2021).

Finally, it is possible that the null result produced by this analysis indicates that a wide range of lesions (and potentially underlying mechanisms) may modulate the occurrence of Hayling Category B Errors. In terms of lesions, the diversity in lesion sites associated with the occurrence of Hayling Category B errors is evidenced by Fig. 7. Contrary to expectations, many patients with posterior lesions were found to commit the highest proportion of Category B errors on the Hayling. This may indicate that critical networks underlying suppression ability may be disrupted at many, spatially distinct locations or that lesion location is not the only factor which modulates the occurrence of Hayling Category B errors. Past work has found that general measures of cortical atrophy and white matter integrity act as more effective predictors of executive function impairment than lesion-location metrics (Hobden et al. 2022). It is possible that a similar relationship may be present between pre-morbid atrophy/white matter integrity and Hayling test performance. Additional research is needed to explore each of these possibilities in detail and to further fundamental insight into the neural correlates underlying error commission on the Hayling test.

This novel demonstration of the Hayling Test within the stroke population suggests that this task represents an effective alternative measure which can detect initiation and inhibition deficits. The findings of this study are in line with past research indicating that the Stroop and Hayling Tests assess behaviourally and anatomically dissociable components of executive function. Behavioural metrics derived from the Hayling Test were found provide detailed insight into abnormal patterns of both general performance impairment and strategy use in stroke survivors and were able to link these patterns to distinct neural correlates. These findings suggest that the Hayling Test can be employed in acute stroke settings to provide a detailed and practical screen of initiation, inhibition and strategy use abilities in stroke survivors.

Limitations

Executive dysfunction is a highly complex disorder and any one test in isolation is insufficient to fully characterise the pattern of impairment present in a patient. The results of this study suggest that the Hayling Test may serve as an effective first-line screen for initiation/inhibition impairment, but in-depth cognitive assessment is needed to fully characterise behaviour. First-line neuropsychological screens are important and practical clinical tools which are compatible with the time and resource limitations of real-world clinical environments (Moore et al. 2022). It, therefore, remains important to improve the efficiency of this first-line screening by identifying the tools which can provide the most detailed information within a short testing period.

Importantly, the Hayling Test may not be a suitable screen for all stroke survivors. A high percentage of the stroke population exhibit either language comprehension or production deficits which may preclude assessing initiation/inhibition performance on this language-dependent measure (Demeyere et al. 2015, 2020), although constraining an auditory context (i.e., sentence completion) has been found to facilitate production (Berndt et al. 2002; Robinson et al. 2005). Nevertheless, future studies can aim to develop and validate novel initiation/inhibition screens which are not dependent on language and can therefore be used in a greater portion of the stroke population.

The network-level analyses employed in this investigation use normative tractography atlases which may not exactly map onto the connectivity structure of the patients included in this analysis (Gleichgerrcht et al. 2017; Griffis et al. 2021). Similarly, number of disconnected streamlines can be unrelated to the strength of connectivity between regions (Fox 2018; Griffis et al. 2021). Despite these potential limitations, past studies employing similar normative atlas-based tractography atlases have agreed well with studies employing in-vivo tractography approaches.

Finally, past work has suggested that the non-random spatial distributions of stroke lesions may yield results which are displaced relative to the true underlying neural correlates of behavioural deficits in voxel-level lesion mapping analyses (Mah et al. 2014; Moore et al. 2023a, b, c). It is plausible that this effect is present in network-level analyses as well but is unlikely to have significantly impacted the main conclusions drawn in this study. Importantly, the aim of this study is to identify preliminary evidence of connectivity profile differences related to select tasks rather than to quantify the exact anatomy of each considered deficit. Future investigations can aim to expand on these findings by employing in-vivo tractography methods to improve fundamental understanding of the anatomy of suppression and initiation functions.

Conclusion

Overall, this project provides a novel investigation of Hayling Test performance and strategy use within the stroke population. The results of this study strongly suggest that the Stroop and Hayling tests measure functions which are both behaviourally and neuroanatomically distinct. The Hayling Test was found to provide detailed insight into abnormal patterns of both general performance impairment and strategy use in stroke survivors. This novel demonstration of the Hayling Test within the stroke population suggests that this task represents an effective alternative measure which can detect initiation and inhibition deficits. These findings are important when considered in the context of clinical practice, as they highlight potential avenues for improving detection of common and debilitating post-stroke cognitive impairments and informing management and rehabilitation.

Author contributions

MJM: was responsible for methodology, formal analysis, investigation, writing—original draft, writing—reviewing & editing, visualisation. JB: was responsible for data curation, methodology, writing—original draft, and writing—reviewing & editing. EG: was responsible for data curation and writing—reviewing & editing. LF: was responsible for writing—data curation, reviewing & editing. GR: was responsible for conceptualisation, methodology, writing—review & editing, supervision, funding acquisition, project administration, and resources.

Funding

Open Access funding enabled and organized by CAUL and its Member Institutions. The authors thank all of the stroke survivors and participants for their time. We also thank Mia Phillips for assistance with data cleaning. This work was supported by the Brazil Family Program for Neurology and a NHMRC Boosting Dementia Research Leadership Fellowship (APP1135769) to GAR.

Data availability

All anonymised behavioural data, binarized lesion files, and analysis code have been made openly available on the Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/mbsd4/). All data which have not been made openly available due to copyright (e.g., test materials) and/or patient anonymity concerns (e.g., brain scans) are available on request from the authors or from the copyright holders where relevant.

Declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors report no conflicts of interest.

Footnotes

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

References

  1. Aron AR, Robbins TW, Poldrack RA. Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex: one decade on. Trends Cogn Sci. 2014;18(4):177–185. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.12.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Ashburner, J., Barnes, G., Chen, C., Daunizeau, J., Flandin, G., Friston, K., Kiebel, S., Kilner, J., Litvak, V., & Moran, R. (2016). SPM12 manual. URL: http://Www.Fil.Ion.Ucl.Ac.Uk/Spm/Doc/Spm12 Manual. Pdf. Accessed 28 Mar 2023
  3. Barker MS, Young B, Robinson GA. Cohesive and coherent connected speech deficits in mild stroke. Brain and Language. 2017;168:23–36. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2017.01.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Berndt RS, Burton MW, Haendiges AN, Mitchum CC. Production of nouns and verbs in aphasia: effects of elicitation context. Aphasiology. 2002;16(1–2):83–106. doi: 10.1080/02687040143000212. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  5. Blake H, McKinney M, Treece K, Lee E, Lincoln NB. An evaluation of screening measures for cognitive impairment after stroke. Age Ageing. 2002;31(6):451–456. doi: 10.1093/ageing/31.6.451. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bright P, Jaldow ELI, Kopelman MD. The National adult reading test as a measure of premorbid intelligence: a comparison with estimates derived from demographic variables. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2002;8(6):847–854. doi: 10.1017/S1355617702860131. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bright P, Hale E, Gooch VJ, Myhill T, van der Linde I. The National adult reading test: restandardisation against the Wechsler adult intelligence scale—fourth edition. Neuropsychol Rehabil. 2018;28(6):1019–1027. doi: 10.1080/09602011.2016.1231121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Burgess PW, Shallice T. Response suppression, initiation and strategy use following frontal lobe lesions. Neuropsychologia. 1996;34(4):263–272. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(95)00104-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Burgess PW, Shallice T. The Hayling and Brixton tests: test manual. Bury St Edmunds:UK: Thames Valley Test Company; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  10. Cervera-Crespo T, González-Alvarez J. Age and semantic inhibition measured by the Hayling task: a meta-analysis. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2017;32(2):198–214. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acw088. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Chiappe P, Siegel LS, Hasher L. Working memory, inhibitory control, and reading disability. Mem Cognit. 2000;28(1):8–17. doi: 10.3758/BF03211570. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Cipolotti L, Spanò B, Healy C, Tudor-Sfetea C, Chan E, White M, Biondo F, Duncan J, Shallice T, Bozzali M. Inhibition processes are dissociable and lateralized in human prefrontal cortex. Neuropsychologia. 2016;93(Pt A):1–12. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.09.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Cohen AL, Ferguson MA, Fox MD. Lesion network mapping predicts post-stroke behavioural deficits and improves localization. Brain. 2021;144(4):e35. doi: 10.1093/brain/awab002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Collette F, Van der Linden M, Delfiore G, Degueldre C, Luxen A, Salmon E. The functional anatomy of inhibition processes investigated with the Hayling task. Neuroimage. 2001;14(2):258–267. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0846. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Coslett HB, Turkeltaub P. Chapter 63—acquired dyslexia. In: Hickok G, Small SL, editors. Neurobiology of language. Academic Press; 2016. pp. 791–803. [Google Scholar]
  16. Demeyere N, Riddoch MJ, Slavkova ED, Bickerton W-L, Humphreys GW. The Oxford Cognitive Screen (OCS): validation of a stroke-specific short cognitive screening tool. Psychol Assess. 2015;27(3):883–894. doi: 10.1037/pas0000082. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Demeyere N, Haupt M, Webb SS, Strobel L, Milosevich ET, Moore MJ, Wright H, Finke K, Duta MD. Introducing the tablet-based Oxford Cognitive Screen-Plus (OCS-Plus) as an assessment tool for subtle cognitive impairments. Sci repo. 2021;11(1):8000. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-87287-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Diamond A. Executive functions. Annu Rev Psychol. 2013;64:135–168. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Foulon C, Cerliani L, Kinkingnehun S, Levy R, Rosso C, Urbanski M, Volle E, Thiebaut de Schotten M. Advanced lesion symptom mapping analyses and implementation as BCBtoolkit. Gigascience. 2018;7(3):giy004. doi: 10.1093/gigascience/giy004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Fox MD. Mapping symptoms to brain networks with the human connectome. N Engl J Med. 2018;379(23):2237–2245. doi: 10.1056/NEJMra1706158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. George MS, Ketter TA, Parekh PI, Rosinsky N, Ring H, Casey BJ, Trimble MR, Horwitz B, Herscovitch P, Post RM. Regional brain activity when selecting a response despite interference: An H2 15O PET study of the Stroop and an emotional Stroop. Hum Brain Mapp. 1994;1(3):194–209. doi: 10.1002/hbm.460010305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Gibson EC, Barker MS, Martin AK, Robinson GA. Initiation, inhibition and strategy generation across the healthy adult lifespan. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2018;34(4):511–523. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acy057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Gleichgerrcht E, Fridriksson J, Rorden C, Bonilha L. Connectome-based lesion-symptom mapping (CLSM): A novel approach to map neurological function. NeuroImage Clin. 2017;16:461–467. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2017.08.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Griffis JC, Metcalf NV, Corbetta M, Shulman GL. Lesion Quantification Toolkit: a MATLAB software tool for estimating grey matter damage and white matter disconnections in patients with focal brain lesions. NeuroImage Clin. 2021;30:102639. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2021.102639. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Guarino A, Forte G, Giovannoli J, Casagrande M. Executive functions in the elderly with mild cognitive impairment: A systematic review on motor and cognitive inhibition, conflict control and cognitive flexibility. Aging Ment Health. 2020;24(7):1028–1045. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2019.1584785. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Hobden G, Moore M, Mair G, Pendlebury S, Demeyere N. Post-stroke executive function in relation to white matter damage on clinically acquired CT brain imaging. medRxiv. 2022 doi: 10.1101/2021.11.12.21266247. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Hung Y, Gaillard SL, Yarmak P, Arsalidou M. Dissociations of cognitive inhibition, response inhibition, and emotional interference: Voxelwise ALE meta-analyses of fMRI studies. Hum Brain Mapp. 2018;39(10):4065–4082. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24232. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Jefferson AL, Paul RH, Ozonoff AL, Cohen RA. Evaluating elements of executive functioning as predictors of instrumental activities of daily living (IADLs) Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2006;21(4):311–320. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2006.03.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Jensen AR, Rohwer WD., Jr The Stroop color-word test: a review. Acta Physiol (oxf) 1966;25:36–93. doi: 10.1016/0001-6918(66)90004-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Laakso HM, Hietanen M, Melkas S, Sibolt G, Curtze S, Virta M, Ylikoski R, Pohjasvaara T, Kaste M, Erkinjuntti T. Executive function subdomains are associated with post-stroke functional outcome and permanent institutionalization. Eur J Neurol. 2019;26(3):546–552. doi: 10.1111/ene.13854. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Leff A, Starrfelt R. Hemianopic Alexia. In Alexia. London: Springer; 2014. pp. 31–69. [Google Scholar]
  32. Leśniak M, Bak T, Czepiel W, Seniów J, Członkowska A. Frequency and prognostic value of cognitive disorders in stroke patients. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2008;26(4):356–363. doi: 10.1159/000162262. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Mah Y-H, Husain M, Rees G, Nachev P. Human brain lesion-deficit inference remapped. Brain. 2014;137(9):2522–2531. doi: 10.1093/brain/awu164. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Martin AK, Barker MS, Gibson EC, Robinson GA. Response initiation and inhibition and the relationship with fluid intelligence across the adult lifespan. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2021;36(2):231–242. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acz044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Moore MJ. A practical guide to lesion symptom mapping. PsyArXiv. 2022 doi: 10.31234/osf.io/2jxr9. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  36. Moore MJ, Demeyere N. Lesion symptom mapping of domain-specific cognitive impairments using routine imaging in stroke. Neuropsychologia. 2022;167:108159. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2022.108159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Moore MJ, Jenkinson M, Griffanti L, Huygelier H, Gillebert CR, Demeyere N. A comparison of lesion mapping analyses based on CT versus MR imaging in stroke. Neuropsychologia. 2023;184:108564. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108564. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Moore MJ, Milosevich E, Beisteiner R, Bowen A, Checketts M, Demeyere N, Fordell H, Godefroy O, Laczo J, Rich T. Rapid screening for neglect following stroke: A systematic search and European Academy of Neurology recommendations. Eur j neurol. 2022;29(9):2596–2606. doi: 10.1111/ene.15381. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Moore MJ, Demeyere N, Rorden C, Mattingley JB. Lesion mapping in neuropsychological research: a practical and conceptual guide. Cortex; a J Devoted Study Nerv Sys Behav. 2023 doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2023.10.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Moore MJ, Hearne L, Demeyere N, Mattingley JB. Comprehensive voxel-wise, tract-based and network lesion mapping reveals unique architectures of right and left visuospatial neglect. Brain Struct Funct. 2023;228(9):2067–2087. doi: 10.1007/s00429-023-02702-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Nathaniel-James DA, Fletcher P, Frith CD. The functional anatomy of verbal initiation and suppression using the Hayling test. Neuropsychologia. 1997;35(4):559–566. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(96)00104-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Nelson HE, Willison J (1991) National adult reading test (NART). Windsor: Nfer-Nelson, 1–26
  43. Nijsse B, Spikman JM, Visser-Meily JM, de Kort PL, van Heugten CM. Social cognition impairments in the long term post stroke. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2019;100(7):1300–1307. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2019.01.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Nys GMS, van Zandvoort MJE, van der Worp HB, de Haan EHF, de Kort PLM, Jansen BPW, Kappelle LJ. Early cognitive impairment predicts long-term depressive symptoms and quality of life after stroke. J Neurol Sci. 2006;247(2):149–156. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2006.04.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Pohjasvaara T, Leskelä M, Vataja R, Kalska H, Ylikoski R, Hietanen M, Leppävuori A, Kaste M, Erkinjuntti T. Post-stroke depression, executive dysfunction and functional outcome. Eur J Neurol. 2002;9(3):269–275. doi: 10.1046/j.1468-1331.2002.00396.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Rand D, Weiss PL, Katz N. Training multitasking in a virtual supermarket: a novel intervention after stroke. Am J Occup Ther. 2009;63(5):535–542. doi: 10.5014/ajot.63.5.535. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Robinson G, Shallice T, Cipolotti L. A failure of high level verbal response selection in progressive dynamic aphasia. Cogn Neuropsychol. 2005;22(6):661–694. doi: 10.1080/02643290442000239. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Robinson GA, Cipolotti L, Walker DG, Biggs V, Bozzali M, Shallice T. Verbal suppression and strategy use: a role for the right lateral prefrontal cortex? Brain. 2015;138(4):1084–1096. doi: 10.1093/brain/awv003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Robinson GA, Walker DG, Biggs V, Shallice T. When does a strategy intervention overcome a failure of inhibition? Evidence from two left frontal brain tumour cases. Cortex. 2016;79:123–129. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.03.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Rorden C, Bonilha L, Fridriksson J, Bender B, Karnath H-O. Age-specific CT and MRI templates for spatial normalization. Neuroimage. 2012;61(4):957–965. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.03.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Rorden, C. (2007). MRICron [computer software].
  52. Sadaghiani S, Kleinschmidt A. Brain networks and α-oscillations: structural and functional foundations of cognitive control. Trends Cogn Sci. 2016;20(11):805–817. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2016.09.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Saxena S, Keser Z, Rorden C, Bonilha L, Fridriksson J, Walker A, Hillis AE. Disruptions of the human connectome associated With Hemispatial neglect. Neurology. 2022;98(2):e107–e114. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000013050. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Scarpina F, Tagini S. The stroop color and word test. Front Psychol. 2017;8:557. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00557. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Schaefer A, Kong R, Gordon EM, Laumann TO, Zuo X-N, Holmes AJ, Eickhoff SB, Yeo BTT. Local-global parcellation of the human cerebral cortex from intrinsic functional connectivity MRI. Cereb Cortex. 2018;28(9):3095–3114. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhx179. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Skidmore ER, Dawson DR, Butters MA, Grattan ES, Juengst SB, Whyte EM, Begley A, Holm MB, Becker JT. Strategy training shows promise for addressing disability in the first 6 months after stroke. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2015;29(7):668–676. doi: 10.1177/1545968314562113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Strauss E, Sherman EM, Spreen O (2006) A compendium of neuropsychological tests: administration, norms, and commentary. American chemical society.
  58. Stroop JR. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J Exp Psychol. 1935;18(6):643. doi: 10.1037/h0054651. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  59. Stuss DT, Alexander MP. Is there a dysexecutive syndrome? Philos Trans the R Soc b: Biol Sci. 2007;362(1481):901–915. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2007.2096. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Taylor SF, Kornblum S, Lauber EJ, Minoshima S, Koeppe RA. Isolation of specific interference processing in the Stroop task: PET activation studies. Neuroimage. 1997;6(2):81–92. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1997.0285. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Thiebaut de Schotten M, Kinkingnéhun S, Delmaire C, Lehéricy S, Duffau H, Thivard L, Volle E, Levy R, Dubois B, Bartolomeo P. Visualization of disconnection syndromes in humans. Cortex. 2008;44(8):1097–1103. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2008.02.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Troyer AK, Leach L, Strauss E. Aging and response inhibition: normative data for the Victoria stroop test. Aging Neuropsychol Cogn. 2006;13(1):20–35. doi: 10.1080/138255890968187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Vallar G, Burani C, Arduino LS. Neglect dyslexia: a review of the neuropsychological literature. Exp Brain Res. 2010;206(2):219–235. doi: 10.1007/s00221-010-2386-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. West R, Alain C. Age-related decline in inhibitory control contributes to the increased Stroop effect observed in older adults. Psychophysiology. 2000;37(2):179–189. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.3720179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Zinn S, Bosworth HB, Hoenig HM, Swartzwelder HS. Executive function deficits in acute stroke. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2007;88(2):173–180. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2006.11.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

All anonymised behavioural data, binarized lesion files, and analysis code have been made openly available on the Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/mbsd4/). All data which have not been made openly available due to copyright (e.g., test materials) and/or patient anonymity concerns (e.g., brain scans) are available on request from the authors or from the copyright holders where relevant.


Articles from Brain Structure & Function are provided here courtesy of Springer

RESOURCES