Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2023 Dec 18;18(12):e0295943. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0295943

The social readjustment rating scale: Updated and modernised

Denise Wallace 1,*, Nicholas R Cooper 1,#, Alejandra Sel 1,#, Riccardo Russo 1,2,#
Editor: Adetayo Olorunlana3
PMCID: PMC10727443  PMID: 38109368

Abstract

The Social Readjustment Rating Scale, originally devised in 1967 by Holmes and Rahe, measures the impact of life events stress. At the time, the SRRS advanced its field of research by standardising the impact of stress with a set of independently derived weights called ‘life change units’ (LCUs) for 43 life events found to predict illness onset. The scale has been criticised for being outdated, e.g. “Mortgage over $10,000” and biased, e.g. “Wife begin or stop work”. The aim of this cross-sectional survey study is to update and improve the SRRS whilst allowing backwards compatibility. We successfully updated the SRRS norms/LCUs using the ratings of 540 predominantly UK adults aged 18 to 84. Moreover, we also updated wording of 12 SRRS items and evaluated the impact of demographics, personal experience and loneliness. Using non-parametric frequentist and Bayesian statistics we found that the updated weights were higher but broadly consistent with those of the original study. Furthermore, changes to item wording did not affect raters’ evaluations relative to the original thereby ensuring cross-comparability with the original SRRS. The raters were not unduly influenced by their personal experiences of events nor loneliness. The target sample was UK rather than US-based and was proportionately representative regarding age, sex and ethnicity. Moreover, the age range was broader than the original SRRS. In addition, we modernised item wording, added one optional extra item to the end of the scale to evaluate the readjustment to living alone and identified 3 potential new items proposed by raters. Backwards-compatibility is maintained.

Introduction

The Social Readjustment Rating Scale (SRRS) is a 43-item list of typically experienced life change events commonly used by researchers interested in the impact of stress on health and well-being. It was designed to predict the allostatic load (physiological cost) of the transient social adjustment required when certain life events occur (e.g. marriage, traffic ticket or a loan). It is well-validated and is cited in over 6000, widely varied, scientific publications. For example, it has been used to measure the association between experienced stress and accelerated cognitive ageing [14], to measure suicide risk [5] and to evaluate the impact of stress severity on dermatitis [6]. The life events were chosen based on sound empirical evidence [see references 1–12 cited in 7] that is arguably still relevant today [e.g. 811]. Numerous updates to the rating norms and modifications to the scale items have been undertaken [10,1215] to address validity and reliability concerns [e.g. 1618], yet many researchers still use the original version [e.g. 1,19,20]. A brief history of the SRRS’ development is provided, highlighting reasons for updates and modifications and why these may have failed to persuade researchers to deviate from the original. The proposed updates are then outlined.

The SRRS evolved from the Schedule of Recent Life Events (SRE) [21,22] which captures a broad spectrum of 42 positively and negatively valenced items which require some level of social readjustment, desirable or undesirable, life-changing or minor [23]. Social readjustment refers to the amount and duration of change in one’s usual routine resulting from various life events. Holmes and Rahe’s SRRS comprises the 42 SRE items plus “Christmas”. Holmes and Rahe’s [7] SRRS study revealed marked similarity between sub-groups in terms of the relative significance of the life events (e.g. ‘marriage’ vs. ‘death of a spouse’), indicating some level of universal agreement for certain experiences. The primary aim of the SRRS was to improve the precision with which the impact of life events on illness onset was measured. Each item has an averaged weighting based on the estimated magnitude of change assigned by a convenience sample (n = 394) of males (n = 179) and females (n = 215) who varied in age, class, education, marital status, religion and race. The raters were asked to rate the magnitude of social readjustment required for each life event irrespective of the desirability of the event, using all their experience as well as what they had learned to be the case for others, relative to the social readjustment needed after marriage. Marriage served as the anchor item with an arbitrary value of 500. The weight for each item was then derived by taking the raters’ average weight and dividing by 10. These weights represent ‘Life Change Units’ (LCU). This set of 43 LCUs provides a set of norms that accompany the SRRS. Social Readjustment Rating Scale respondents would indicate which of the 43 items they have experienced over a certain time-frame (e.g. the previous 12 months). All the LCUs corresponding to the respective items are then summed to produce a total LCU value, which may be used to predict physiological and/or psychological impact for each respondent. For example, a respondent might tick “Death of Spouse” which is 100 LCUs, “Troubles with the boss” (30 LCUs) and “Change in residence” (32 LCUs) giving a total of 162 LCUs. Based on empirical work, Rahe [24] found that a score of about 150 suggested that the respondent would remain healthy over the next 12 months while those falling ill over the same period were typically found to score > 300.

The SRRS and SRE were applauded for adding an objective element to the study of life stress and its impact on health. However it was also argued that the scale was inherently flawed because the event items were not equally well-comprehended by less educated samples [e.g. 25] and the accuracy of event-reporting varied [17]. Furthermore, operationalising “illness” and “life events” are difficult [26]. One review indicated life events likely explained no more than 9% of illness variance [27] and Rahe and colleagues themselves stated that precipitating stressful life events were a necessary but not sufficient antecedent to illness onset [28]. Researchers have sought to address some of these and other concerns as described below.

Muhlenkamp and colleagues [12] noted that the SRRS normative sample did not include those over age 70. They published an extension, providing independent ratings from a sample (n = 41) of 65 to 84 year-olds and modified the instructions by assigning a value of 50 for marriage, rather than 500, to provide a more meaningful and familiar anchor for participants. The study found that raters gave higher ratings for most items relative to the original but there was significant agreement regarding the rank ordering of items. However, these weights were never used in conjunction with any subsequent application of the SRRS by researchers including Miller and Rahe’s [13] update, which replicated the characteristics of the original sample. Moreover, no further validation was undertaken of Miller and Rahe’s [13] update, which may have hindered its adoption in future studies. To our knowledge, researchers have sought only to apply the original weights though modifications to the scale items were undertaken on an ad hoc basis. For example Komaroff and colleagues [25] substituted “marital reconciliation with spouse” with “getting back together” as it was more meaningful to the target population.

Hobson and colleagues [14,15] addressed sample and content criticisms of the original SRRS with an extended, modified “Social Readjustment Rating Scale Revised” (SRRS-R). To address the SRRS’ outdated and insufficiently representative sample the SRRS-R was based on norms derived from a larger sample (n = 3122), representative of a cross-section of Americans regarding age, race, gender, ethnicity, income and geographical location. To address criticisms around content, Hobson and colleagues asked a 30-member expert panel to add, amend or remove existing items, which produced a 51-item scale. Other criticisms of the original version were that some items can be interpreted as symptoms/outcomes rather than precipitating events—the ‘contamination hypothesis’, e.g. “Change in sleeping habits”, could indicate that a new job, like shift work (precipitating event), has occurred or it could indicate the symptom/outcome of a stressful experience. Some items lack representativeness in modern, multi-cultural societies (e.g. “Christmas”) and some items’ wording is ambiguous, biased or out-dated (e.g. “Mortgage or loan greater than $10,000”). Using their extended, modified scale, Hobson et al. [14] found that there were significant differences in the way individuals evaluated the stressfulness of different events. On this basis they concluded that using simple unitary weights (occurred vs. not occurred) risked masking these differences and that further work needs to assess the impact of using group-based weights vs. individually derived weights vs. unit weights. Whilst they found that results were statistically significant, effect sizes were very small and ratings were remarkably similar across age, gender and income categories. Their approach validly addressed concerns however the SRRS-R departed notably from the original SRRS negating any opportunity for cross-comparability and, consequently, the SRRS-R has not been incorporated into any subsequent publications to the best of our knowledge.

Around the same time, Scully and colleagues [10] published updated SRRS ratings and addressed 3 content-related criticisms of the SRRS. They assessed the validity of the contamination hypothesis, mentioned previously. In addition, some evidence suggest that undesirable life events would have a stronger stress response than desirable ones [29,30] though not all findings agree [31]. Similarly, uncontrollable life events would have a more potent stress impact than controllable ones (ibid). In phase 1 of Scully and colleagues’ study [10], the original SRRS instructions were administered to a random sample of Florida residents (n = 200) whose ratings were used to derive updated weights (LCUs) for all items. In phase 2, another sample completed the SRRS, reporting experienced events a) within the last 12 months and b) ever. They also completed a modified version of the Symptom Checklist-90 which measures stress-related symptoms. A group of university staff and student raters (n = 7) categorised all the SRRS items as desirable, undesirable or neutral. A separate group of student raters (n = 7) categorised the items as either controllable or uncontrollable. Comparisons of symptom reporting were conducted based on these categorisations. Regression analyses revealed that the SRRS in its original form was predictive of stress symptoms. In addition, consistently more variance was explained when regression models included all items than when only respective undesirable/uncontrollable items were included. Thus, including only negative items was found to limit the utility of the SRRS. They also found that symptoms associated with events reported over the last 12 months had greater predictive power, suggesting that the stress impact of life events diminished with passing time. They [10] concluded that “the SRRS is a robust instrument for identifying the potential for stress-related outcomes” (p.875).

Twenty years on from the last attempts to modernise the SRRS, the primary aim of the current study was to update and improve the SRRS without fundamentally changing the scale to allow for cross-comparison of studies, which may have played a role in previous updates not being incorporated into subsequent versions. Six areas of focus were identified: First, the original weightings are 5 decades old and required updating. Second, biases in item wording were removed. Third, the complete and accurate wording from the raters’ version was re-instated. As Holmes and David [32] pointed out: “We regret the decision to save space on the Social Readjustment Rating Scale, because the complete wording is the accurate and more helpful form.” (p.30). The SRRS ‘rating’ questionnaire comprised detailed statements, along with examples in some cases. The actual scale’s wording is much simplified. For example:

  • Raters assessed: “Major change in usual type and/or amount of recreation”. The final SRRS used to evaluate illness onset was simplified to: “Change in recreation”.

  • Raters assessed: “Minor violations of the law e.g. traffic tickets, jay walking, disturbing the peace”. The final version was simplified to: “Minor violations of the law”.

Thus, the final version leaves the reader to make assumptions about what ‘counts’ and what does not, resulting in increased inter-individual differences in responding. This portion of inter-individual variability was reduced by reinstating the ‘rater’ version of items.

Fourth, information was collected regarding the potential for systematic bias that may have affected the magnitude of weight that raters assigned to each item. Raters were asked to indicate the extent to which their rating was based on their own personal experiences of events. They were also asked how lonely they were and how frequently they felt lonely. Loneliness was chosen for two reasons, firstly as part of the evaluation of a new item, “Single person, living alone”, that was added to the end of the scale and secondly, as a proxy for depression which is associated with loneliness and stress [3336]. Thus, loneliness allowed the evaluation of whether ratings varied based on emotional state at the time of rating.

Fifth, the rater sample was made more representative, proportionately reflecting the demographics within the UK regarding age, gender and ethnicity.

Sixth, the need for new items was considered. A rating was added to the norms set for being single and living alone, with its inclusion as optional at the end of the SRRS. In addition, an opportunity was provided for raters to add an item and its weight, which they believed could improve future work regarding what people find difficult to adjust to at the current time.

In this cross-sectional survey study, an assessment was made of the extent to which the sample’s ratings deviated from those of the original, replicating earlier similar analyses. Previous work indicates that this is likely. For example, Miller and Rahe [13] in their update found ratings differed when comparing males and females and married with unmarried individuals. Women’s ratings were, on average, 17% higher than those of men. Muhlenkamp et al. [12] measured differences between their elderly sample and the original raters with items categorised into ‘family’, ‘personal’, ‘work’ and ‘finance’. A replication of this analysis was undertaken. The extent to which the rank order of items from the updated SRRS agreed with that of the original was also evaluated.

Materials and method

Study design

A survey method was used comprising a series of questionnaires administered via Qualtrics in a single, online-only session. The present study broadly replicates that of Holmes and Rahe [7] who recruited a convenience sample of adults aged ≥ 18 years to rate a list of 42 life events, using a proportional scaling method.

Participants

Six hundred and thirty adults aged 18 to 85 accepted the invitation to participate. The sample selection criteria were based on the UK’s current age distribution and gender breakdown and England and Wales’ ethnicity breakdown published by the ONS [37]. Based on ONS estimates for England and Wales, 84.8% of the population is white. Roughly that proportion of Caucasians was recruited with the remaining proportion comprising non-Caucasian ethnic groups. Regarding sex, a 50/50 split was targeted, reflecting a similar split within the UK population. Ethnic and sex breakdowns were nested within age bands proportioned as per ONS statistics.

Participants were recruited via social media, word-of-mouth, SONA (local university student recruiting platform) and Prolific, an online participant recruitment platform. Participants had to be ≥ 18 years to be included in this study. Within the Prolific platform participants currently located in the UK were selected and anyone who had taken part in any of our previous studies were excluded. No other exclusion criteria were applied. Participants were recruited and data collected from February 2021 to May 2021. Respondents were anonymous; no personally identifiable information was collected. Thus, participants could not be identified during or after data collection. Participants volunteered either without payment, received a small payment or course credits. The study was approved by the University of Essex Faculty of Science and Engineering Ethics Committee (ETH2021-0829). All participants gave written informed consent using an online form, which had to be read and agreed to before they could gain access to the study.

Measures

Social Readjustment Rating Questionnaire (SRRQ)

The updated SRRQ with instructions administered to the rater sample is provided in Appendix 1 (S1 Appendix). For comparison, the original SRRQ instructions are provided in Appendix 2 (S2 Appendix). To reduce potential variations in interpretation, the SRRQ was administered with modified instructions based on those of Muhlenkamp et al. [12] who changed the weight for marriage from 500 to 50 and simplified the instructions themselves. Using marriage (50) as the anchor point, participants were instructed to rate each item from 0 to 100. Some wording was simplified but kept as close to the original as possible, asking participants to draw on their experience and those of others when giving their ratings, as in the original version. Note that in the original SRRQ participants rated 42 items (relative to marriage). The updated SRRQ includes a 43rd item to be rated: ‘Single person, living alone’. The outcome variables for the SRRQ were the mean weights assigned to each of 43 items (range: 0–100). The mean ratings were derived by averaging the ratings given across participants for each respective item.

Social Readjustment Rating Scale 2022 updated

The updated SRRS, used in conjunction with the SRRQ ratings, is provided in Appendix 3 (S3 Appendix). In accordance with the SRRQ, the updated SRRS also contains the new item at the end of the scale, ‘Single person, living alone’. Consequently, the total number of life change units for a given participant is based on 44 items rather than the original 43. Participants were asked to respond ‘yes’ or ‘no’ with the instruction: “Please indicate which of the following events have occurred in your whole life”. The order of items were randomised and then presented in the same order across participants. Some subtle updates or clarifications to wording were applied e.g. ‘spouse’ became ‘spouse/life partner’. Due to inflation the monetary value used for loans was removed, as recommended by Holmes and David [32]. The outcome variable was the binary value for each of the items multiplied by the corresponding item ‘weight’ (life change units). The products were then summed to provide a total life change units score which represents one’s life change intensity [38]. A higher value indicates greater intensity (i.e. a greater level of adaptation to change was needed).

Invitation to add own item

In this single-item questionnaire respondents were asked: “If you could add one more item to the list, what would it be?”. Participants used the free text box to provide a response or they could leave it blank and continue. In the follow-up question, participants were asked to provide a rating for this item relative to marriage. Valid responses therefore required 2 components: a life event (given in their own words) and a corresponding rating.

Experiential basis for SRRS ratings

An instruction was given to measure the extent to which the participants’ ratings were based on their own experience: “At the start of this survey you were asked to rate a range of life events by comparing them to marriage. To what extent was your chosen rating based on your own personal experience? Please slide the scale to indicate as best you can how much your rating was based on your own experience from ’not at all based on my own experience’ (0) to ’completely based on my own experience’ (100).” Appendix 4 (S4 Appendix) provides a copy of this questionnaire. The outcome variable was the value given for each item (range: 0 to 100).

Loneliness questionnaire

The ONS recommends the following 4 questions to measure loneliness: “How often do you feel that you lack companionship?”, “How often do you feel left out?” and “How often do you feel isolated from others?” with response options of ‘hardly ever or never = 1’, ‘some of the time = 2’ and ‘often = 3’. Scores for these 3 items are summed (range: 3 to 9). A higher score indicates a greater degree of loneliness. The 4th question asked: “How often do you feel lonely?” with 6 response options ranging from ‘often/always’ = 1 to ‘never’ = 5 and ‘prefer not to say’ = 6 [37]. A lower score indicates a greater level of loneliness. The first 3 questions were taken from the University of California, Los Angeles loneliness scale (UCLA v3) [39] which was adapted to a 3-item scale: R-UCLA [40] as used in English Longitudinal Study of Ageing [33]. The UCLA scale has good reliability (coefficient α range: 0.89 to 0.94) and test re-test reliability (r = 0.73). The scale’s reliability and validity was tested on students, teachers, nurses and elderly participants (> 65 years). The R-UCLA has an alpha coefficient of 0.72 with good internal consistency [40]. The final question forms part of the Community Life Survey [41]. Appendix 5 (S5 Appendix) provides a copy of these survey items. Thus, loneliness was measured with two outcome measures: loneliness level as a summed value (range: 3 to 9); loneliness frequency as a single-item value (range: 1 to 5).

Procedure

Participants read the information sheet, accepted the invitation to take part, gave online informed consent by completing a check-list then provided biographical details, namely age, ethnicity, gender, religion, relationship status and employment status (see Results). They were presented with the following in sequential order: the updated SRRQ (rating questionnaire), updated SRRS, new item with corresponding rating, personal experience questionnaire, 4-item loneliness questionnaire.

Statistical analysis

Descriptive statistics are provided for all items. Data were analysed using parametric and/or non-parametric analyses alongside equivalent Bayesian comparisons, depending on whether distributions were normal or skewed. Where any disparity existed between frequentist and Bayesian results, conclusions were based on the Bayes factors (BF), which is not subject to the stopping rule as with the frequentist method. Bayesian analyses were conducted in JASP 0.16.2.0 [42]. Sensitivity analyses for BFs for between-groups comparisons were also conducted to ensure that expected effect sizes were robust.

Numerous previous SRRS studies used geometric means. However, the original SRRS weights were derived using arithmetic weights therefore these were used (see the Results section). For frequentist analyses, alpha levels at < .05 were applied to control for type 1 error. To be comparable to Miller and Rahe (1997), 99% confidence intervals were used.

Gpower 3.1.9.7 [43] indicated that for correlational analyses a sample size of 319 would be required to achieve a power of .80 assuming a small effect size (0.2); for chi square analyses with 1 degree of freedom it was 197. However, an independent samples t-test comparison would require a sample size of 788 and a Mann-Whitney U test would require 824 participants. Due to budget and time constraints, we set the N value at the top end of these constraints. For the Bayesian analyses sensitivity analyses were also performed to evaluate the robustness of BFs. In JASP, the default prior effect size is modelled on a Cauchy distribution scaled to 22 which captures small effects efficiently but has fatter tales than the normal distribution to also capture larger effects. JASP performs sensitivity analyses automatically for parametric tests (i.e. independent t-tests) by varying the scale. Thus, 22 is substituted with a wide (Chaucy prior scale = 1.00) and then an ultra-wide (Chaucy prior scale = 2) scale. Bayes factors are robust when they remain stable under these varied conditions. When non-parametric between-groups tests are performed, a similar approach will be taken by manually performing these additional BF calculations using the different scaling parameters. The latter mitigates the limitation posed by the smaller-than-needed sample size for between-groups comparisons.

The open-ended item (‘Invitation to add own item’ outlined earlier) was analysed by a simple frequency method where the number of times a particular event was given was counted as ‘1’. Only events with an accompanying rating were counted.

Results

Six hundred and thirty respondents were recruited and consented to take part in the study. Ninety of 630 respondents logged off from the study part-way and their data could not be used. Of the remaining 540 respondents, all completed the study in full apart from 5 who completed most of the study (≥ 90%). As part of the informed consent, all participants had agreed that any data collected up to the point of withdrawal may be used. These 5 respondents’ data were therefore retained. None provided data for the loneliness questionnaire, which was the last item of the study. Four of the 5 respondents rated the SRRQ and gave responses to the SRRS but logged off without completing the remaining items (personal experience, selecting own item, loneliness). Two further participants did not answer the question regarding loneliness frequency. Thus for loneliness frequency, 7 respondents’ data are missing. Two participants identified as ‘gender fluid’. For clarity, participants were therefore grouped by biological sex (male vs. female) and the gender-fluid participants’ data were excluded from all gender/sex-based analyses. Analyses were conducted with all available data using list-wise or pair-wise deletion, as appropriate. Sample sizes are given for each table.

Descriptive statistics for demographic details are given in Table 1. There were 453 Prolific participants (84%), SONA (8%) and social media/word-of-mouth (8%). Eighty-seven percent of the sample were British, 4% were EU nationals, 2% were USA nationals and 7% were from other countries. Most (95%) reported English as their first language.

Table 1. Descriptive statistics for biodemographic details of the total sample.

mean (min-max) median (IQR) n* (%)
Age < 30 years 23.08 (18–29) 22 (20–27) 116 (21.5)
30 to 60 years 45.18 (30–60) 45 (37–53) 291 (53.9)
> 60 years 69.81 (61–84) 70 (65–75) 133 (24.6)
Gender female 308 (57)
male 230 (42.6)
gender-fluid 2 (0.4)
Education years 15.31 (12–21) 15 (14–17) 540 (100)
Relationship status married 236 (43.7)
long-term relationship 107 (19.8)
in a relationship 30 (5.6)
separated 5 (0.9)
divorced 21 (3.9)
widowed 11 (2)
life partner died 5 (0.9)
single 125 (23.2)
Ethnicity white 455 (84.3)
mixed race (all) 23 (4.3)
Asian (southern/southeastern Asia) 24 (4.4)
Chinese (east Asian) 7 (1.3)
black (any region) 31 (5.7)
Religion no religion 266 (49.3)
Christian 234 (43.3)
Buddhist 4 (0.7)
Hindu 5 (0.9)
Jewish 6 (1.1)
Muslim 17 (3.2)
Sikh 5 (0.9)
any other religion 3 (0.6)
Employment status full-time or part-time employed 348 (64.4)
currently unemployed, looking for work 14 (2.6)
long-term sick or disabled 11 (2)
looking after home or family 28 (5.2)
retired 97 (18)
have never worked 2 (0.4)
student (p/t or f/t) and currently unemployed 39 (7.2)
other 1 (0.2)

*N = 540.

Wording was adjusted/modernised on 12 items of the rating questionnaire, as shown in Table 2. To assess whether this may have caused those weights to change by more than the unchanged items a Mann-Whitney U test was conducted on the difference scores (new minus original weight) by wording-changed vs. wording-unchanged items. The difference was not statistically significant (p > .685) (Mdnchanged 10.2 vs. Mdnunchanged 8.8) though the equivalent BF of 0.35 was within the anecdotal range, approaching the null, suggesting that participants’ ratings were unlikely to vary with the wording changes.

Table 2. Changed items.

Original item wording New item wording
1. Death of spouse Death of a spouse or life partner
2. Minor violations of the law (e.g. traffic ticket, jay walking, disturbing the peace) Minor violations of the law (e.g. traffic ticket, disturbing the peace)
3. Pregnancy Pregnancy (either yourself or being the father)
4. Gaining a new family member (e.g. through birth, adoption, oldster moving in, etc.) Gaining a new family member (e.g. through birth, adoption, grandparent moving in, etc.)
5. Marital separation from mate Marital separation
6. Major change in church activities (e.g. a lot more or a lot less than usual) Major change in religious activities (e.g. a lot more or a lot less than usual)
7. Marital reconciliation with mate Marital reconciliation
8. Being fired from work Losing your job (redundancy, dismissal, etc.)
9. Major change in the number of arguments with spouse (e.g. either a lot more or a lot less than usual regarding child-rearing, personal habits, etc.) Major change in the number of arguments with spouse or life partner (e.g. either a lot more or a lot less than usual regarding child-rearing, personal habits, etc.)
10. Spouse begins or stops working outside the home Spouse or life partner begins or stops working
11. Taking on a mortgage greater than $10,000 (e.g. purchasing a home, business, etc.) Taking on a mortgage or loan for a major purchase (e.g. purchasing a home, business, etc.)
12. Taking on a mortgage or loan less than $10,000 (e.g. purchasing a car or furniture, paying for college fees, etc.) Taking on a loan for a lesser purchase (e.g. purchasing a car or furniture, paying for college fees, etc.)

SRRS ratings then and now: A comparison with Holmes & Rahe (1967)

The original scale had a score-range of 0 to 1466, while the newly weighted version’s range extended to 1871, increasing the total by 405 life change units (LCUs). Of the original 42 event items rated, 39 increased and 4 decreased. Of the items that increased, 3 items increased by ≥ 25 LCUs relative to the original scale: ‘Foreclosure/repossession on mortgage or loan’ (62new vs. 30original), ‘Death of a close friend’ (64new vs. 37original) and ‘Pregnancy’ (65new vs. 40original). When including the new 43rd rater item, ‘Single person, living alone’, the range increases to 1909 (1871+38). A Mann-Whitney U test found that total LCUs were, on average, higher in the current (Mdn 40.1, n = 43) relative to the original scale (Mdn 29, n = 43) (z = -2.807, p = .005, r = .3). The Bayesian Mann-Whitney U test supported this finding with substantial evidence (BF = 6.22). Bayesian sensitivity analyses are provided in Appendix 6 (S6 Appendix) and indicate that applying different Bayesian priors did not affect the outcome, therefore the reported BFs are reliable. A Kendall’s tau correlation coefficient was conducted to evaluate the level of agreement between the two scales. This revealed that the original weights were strongly, positively associated with the new weights (r = .751, p < .001). The corresponding Bayesian Kendall’s tau provided decisive evidence for this finding (BF > 100), suggesting that the respondents in the new scale and the original rating sample were comparable in the hierarchy of change for their evaluations. Table 3 presents the descriptive statistics and rank order of the SRRS items for the original and new scale weights. The table provides the arithmetic means with their standard errors and 99% confidence intervals, plus the geometric means and median values. The events are ordered by the rank of absolute change in number of LCUs, from highest to lowest (1 to 43) to provide a visual comparison of change between original and new weights. Regarding how consistent participants’ ratings were for each item, it was observed that the range of ratings spanned the full range of 0 to 100 on most items (38/43). The magnitude of interquartile ranges (IQR) and 99% bias-corrected and accelerated (BCa) confidence intervals for each of the 43 items were therefore inspected to better ascertain consensus of ratings. Table 3 shows that the largest IQR magnitude was 40, which was for ‘Outstanding personal achievement’ and ‘Retirement from work’. Next largest were ‘Gaining a new family member’ (39), ‘Death of a close family member’ (35), ‘Son or daughter leaving home’ (35), ‘Losing your job’ (35), ‘Taking on a mortgage or loan for a major purchase’ (35), ‘Major business readjustment’ (35) and ‘Single person, living alone’ (35). The smallest IQR was for ‘Major change in social activities’ (20). Table 3 shows that the largest BCa confidence interval was for ‘Detention in jail or other institution’ (73.79–79.66) and the smallest was for ‘Revision of personal habits’ (20.95–24.85). Of the largest BCa confidence intervals, 3 coincided with some of the largest IQR items: ‘gaining a new family member’ (49.05–54.65), ‘Retirement from work’ (46.73–52.45) and ‘Single person, living alone’ (35.41–41.16). These results suggest that for 79% of items (34/43) participants were consistent in their ratings across items but for the remaining 21% (9/43) respondents were relatively less consistent.

Table 3. Present study vs. original weights and rank order of SRRS events*.

Holmes & Rahe Present Study Difference Score
Event original rank weight present rank Mean (SE)a 99% CIb Geometric Meanc Median (IQR) present—original weight rank: absolute changed
Foreclosure/repossession on mortgage or loan 21 30 9 62.39 (1.07) 59.65, 65.09 32.71 40 (25–60) 32.4 1
Death of a close friend 17 37 8 64.05 (1.12) 61.08, 66.8 44.90 50 (40–70) 27.1 2
Pregnancy 12 40 6 64.66 (1.06) 62.03, 67.27 47.63 60 (40–70) 24.7 3
Change in residence 32 20 19 42.69 (0.95) 40.33, 44.99 34.54 40 (30–59.8) 22.7 4
Major change in work hours or conditions 31 20 27 37.09 (0.84) 34.76, 39.32 20.44 25 (10–40) 17.1 5
Major change in sleeping habits 38 16 30 31.92 (0.84) 29.83, 34.3 24.85 30 (20–40) 15.9 6
Changing to a new school 33 20 28 34.6 (0.93) 32.29, 36.95 29.50 35 (20–50) 14.6 7
Major change in living conditions 28 25 24 39.36 (0.9) 37.01, 41.75 23.76 30 (20–50) 14.4 8
Spouse/life partner begins or stops working 26 26 22 40.06 (0.9) 37.72, 42.36 21.22 30 (10–50) 14.1 9
Major change in financial state 16 38 12 52.02 (0.94) 49.68, 54.68 35.47 50 (30–65) 14.0 10
Losing your job 8 47 10 60.97 (1.03) 58.2, 63.5 24.33 30 (20–50) 14.0 11
Detention in jail or other institution 4 63 2 76.88 (1.14) 73.79, 79.66 61.65 80 (70–99) 13.9 12
Death of a spouse or life partner 1 100 1 86.83 (0.98) 84.16, 89.21 73.78 95 (80–100) -13.2 13
Death of a close family member 5 63 3 75.84 (1.02) 73.13, 78.46 67.57 80 (60.5–95) 12.8 14
Gaining a new family member 14 39 13 51.81 (1.11) 49.05, 54.65 29.09 40 (20–50) 12.8 15
Son or daughter leaving home 23 29 21 41.66 (0.99) 39.2, 44.19 21.75 30 (15–40) 12.7 16
Major change in eating habits 40 15 35 27.39 (0.77) 25.2, 29.47 15.72 20 (10–30) 12.4 17
Major change in the health or behaviour of a family member 11 44 11 55.73 (0.98) 53.06, 58.24 38.01 50 (30–70) 11.7 18
Major personal injury or illness 6 53 7 64.36 (0.98) 61.9, 66.91 55.98 70 (50–80) 11.4 19
Taking on a mortgage or loan for a major purchase 20 31 20 42.22 (0.99) 39.78, 44.79 34.98 45 (30–60) 11.2 20
Minor violations of the law 43 11 40 22.14 (0.76) 20.12, 24.19 14.99 20 (10–30) 11.1 21
Major change in usual type and/or amount of recreation 34 19 34 29.08 (0.78) 27.01, 31.04 11.43 15 (5–30) 10.1 22
Major change in the number of arguments with spouse-life partner 19 35 18 44.21 (0.92) 41.75, 46.56 31.10 40 (25.5–50) 9.2 23
Major change in responsibilities at work 22 29 26 37.8 (0.83) 35.54, 39.94 50.24 70 (50–80) 8.8 24
Taking on a loan for a lesser purchase 37 17 36 25.12 (0.84) 22.97, 27.6 17.36 20 (10–35) 8.1 25
Beginning or ceasing formal schooling 27 26 29 33.88 (0.91) 31.57, 36.14 31.07 40 (25–55) 7.9 26
Major change in number of family get-togethers 39 15 38 22.88 (0.76) 20.9, 24.92 20.74 25 (10–40) 7.9 27
Christmas 42 12 43 19.78 (0.84) 17.79, 21.93 11.80 10 (5–30) 7.8 28
Major business readjustment 15 39 16 46.73 (1.06) 43.96, 49.49 40.18 50 (30.8–70) 7.7 29
Vacation 41 13 42 20.09 (0.81) 18.03, 22.25 12.64 10 (6–30) 7.1 30
Major change in social activities 36 18 37 24.39 (0.8) 22.31, 26.45 17.35 20 (10–30) 6.4 31
Troubles with the boss 30 23 33 29.15 (0.9) 26.72, 31.74 15.66 20 (10–30) 6.2 32
Divorce 2 73 4 67.86 (1.03) 65.2, 70.41 56.03 70 (52.8–85) -5.1 33
Retirement from work 10 45 15 49.64 (1.08) 46.73, 52.45 35.58 50 (30–60) 4.6 34
Changing to a different line of work 18 36 23 39.48 (0.85) 37.3, 41.68 52.64 70 (45.8–80) 3.5 35
Outstanding personal achievement 25 28 32 30.94 (0.96) 28.49, 33.55 30.56 38.5 (21.3–50) 2.9 36
In-law troubles 24 29 31 30.94 (0.91) 28.62, 33.26 31.31 40 (25–60) 1.9 37
Marital separation 3 65 5 66.9 (1.01) 64.28, 69.33 55.98 70 (50–80) 1.9 38
Marital reconciliation 9 45 17 46.24 (0.97) 43.77, 48.59 51.32 65 (45–80) 1.2 39
Revision of personal habits 29 24 39 22.8 (0.77) 20.95, 24.85 30.96 40 (25–53.8) -1.2 40
Major change in religious activities 35 19 41 20.09 (0.8) 18.01, 22.2 22.07 30 (15–40) 1.1 41
Sexual difficulties 13 39 25 38.07 (0.92) 35.74, 40.61 53.44 70 (50–80) -0.9 42
Marriage (pre-set weight) 7 50 14 50.00 0.0 43
Single person, living alone 38.16 (1.13) 35.41, 41.16 24.14 40 (15–50)

* Table’s values are ordered by absolute change in weights.

a Mean (SE). Standard error obtained via BCa Bootstrap with 1000 samples.

b 99% confidence intervals obtained via BCa Bootstrap with 1000 samples.

c Where participants responded with a zero value, these were replaced with ’1’ to allow this value to be calculated.

d Rank is based on absolute change (difference score: 2022 weight—original weight). Negative signs were ignored in creating the rank order. Rank values in red indicate that the weight upon which it is based was higher in the original version.

The SRRS categorised as ‘family’, ‘personal’, ‘financial’ or ‘work’ life events

Rahe and colleagues delineated their 43 SRRS items in terms of ‘family’, ‘personal’, ‘work’ and ‘financial’ life events [24,38]. A copy is provided in Appendix 7 (S7 Appendix). The correlations between the original and new weightings were evaluated by these categories, using Kendall’s tau. The result revealed strong, positive associations between original and new weights for family (r = 0.818, p < .001), personal (0.638, p < .001) and work (0.905, p = .004) events. However, original and new weights showed no statistically significant association for financial items (p > .4). Bayesian Kendall’s tau correlations confirmed these findings with decisive evidence for family and personal categories (BF > 100) and strong evidence for work-related events (BF = 12.24). For financial items, the Bayesian Kendall’s tau revealed anecdotal evidence (BF = 0.68). Thus, original and new samples co-varied on all categories except financial events for which evidence was inconclusive.

Demographic differences were examined within each of the 4 categories. Table 4 provides medians with interquartile ranges for all variables analysed. Appendix 8 (S8 Appendix) provides medians and interquartile ranges for all broad categories for demographical variables.

Table 4. Descriptive statistics for SRRS events categorised by family, financial, personal and work.

Mean SRRS weights
sub-groups overall weight family items financial items personal items work items
Age Median (IQR) Median (IQR) Median (IQR) Median (IQR)
< 30 years (n = 116) 40.3 (30.3–54.3) 53.6 (44.8–61) 50 (33.8–55.8) 35.5 (28.6–45.3) 42.1 (31.6–54.3)
30 to 60 years (n = 291) 39.7 (29.7–57.8) 54.6 (45.7–64.3) 45 (35–52.5) 34.7 (28.9–43.3) 43.6 (32.1–52.9)
> 60 years (n = 133) 39.4 (28.7–57.1) 53.9 (46.1–61.1) 46.5 (35.6–58.8) 33.4 (26.4–46.6) 42.9 (33.2–54.7)
Sex
female (n = 308) 43.9 (32.4–58.9) 57.1 (48.9–64.9) 47.5 (37.5–56.2) 36.9 (30.6–46.5) 45.7 (35.7–55.7)
male(n = 230) 35.4 (25.3–50.8) 50.4 (42.4–58.8) 42.5 (30–55) 32.3 (24.7–40.5) 40 (29.3–50)
Ethnicity
white (n = 455) 39.3 (28.7–56.3) 54.3 (46.4–61.8) 46.3 (35–55) 34.2 (27.9–43.7) 42.9 (32.9–52.9)
non-whitea (n = 85) 40.7 (32.5–53) 55.4 (44.8–63.8) 50 (37.4–59.4) 37.9 (27.4–47.9) 45.7 (31.8–55.7)
Religion
no religion (n = 274) 39 (28.1–55.4) 55.5 (46.4–64.3) 47.5 (35–57.5) 35.7 (27.7–46.6) 44.3 (33.4–54.4)
religionb (n = 266) 40.9 (30.1–56) 53.6 (45.6–60.7) 45 (35–54.1) 33.9 (28–41.1) 42.1 (32.1–52.1)
Relationship status
marriedc (n = 343) 40.7 (30.6–57.6) 55 (46.8–64.5) 46.5 (35–55) 34.7 (28.4–45.3) 42.9 (32.9–54.3)
unmarried (n = 197) 37.7 (29.8–53.2) 53.6 (44.6–60.7) 45.8 (34.8–55.6) 34.7 (27.1–44.2) 43.6 (32.5–52.9)
Employment status
employed (n = 348) 39.8 (28.3–55.9) 54.3 (46.1–63) 46 (35–55) 34.7 (28.2–44.5) 42.9 (32.9–55)
unemployede (n = 192) 39.2 (30.7–55.4) 54.3 (45.8–61.1) 47 (35–57.5) 34.7 (27.3–44.7) 43.2 (31.6–52.6)

a ’Non-white’ included all ethnicities: mixed race, Asian (southern/southeastern Asia), Chinese (east Asian), black (any region).

b ’Religious’ includes all religions: Christian, Buddhist, Hindu, Jewish, Muslim, Sikh, any other religion.

c ’married’ includes married and long-term partners.

d ’unmarried’ includes those who don’t qualify as ’c’: divorced, in a relationship, life partner died, separated, single, widowed.

e currently unemployed and looking for work, have never worked, long-term sick/disabled, looking after home or family, retired, student (p/t or f/t) and currently unemployed, other.

For age, ratings of young, middle-aged and older-aged groups were compared for each of the 4 categories with Kruskal-Wallis tests, which revealed no statistically significant differences between groups (p’s > .2). Bayesian Mann-Whitney U tests were used to compare the respective groups as there is no corresponding Bayesian non-parametric one-way ANOVA equivalent. The outcomes were congruent with the frequentist findings and consistently supported the null (BFs ≤ 0.26). Sensitivity analyses are provided in Appendix 6 (S6 Appendix) and support these BF results. Comparing female and male average ratings, in contrast, revealed a statistically significant difference using Mann-Whitney U tests for family events (Mdn 57.1 vs. 50.4), personal events (Mdn 36.9 vs. 32.3), financial events (Mdn 47.5 vs. 42.5) and work events (Mdn 45.7 vs. 40) (p’s < .001) with females’ ratings being consistently higher than males’, respectively. Bayesian Mann-Whitney U tests revealed strong evidence for financial items (BF = 27.22) and decisive evidence for all other categories (BF > 100). Sensitivity analyses are provided in Appendix 6 (S6 Appendix) and support these BFs. These results are shown in Fig 1. Ethnicity, religion, relationship status and employment variables were collapsed into dichotomised variables to simplify comparison. Details are given in Table 4. Appendix 8 (S8 Appendix) provides comparisons for full variables. For ethnicity, a Mann-Whitney U test of white vs. (combined) non-white sub-sets indicated no statistically significant between-groups differences for any of the 4 categories (p’s > .1). Likewise, the Bayesian analyses revealed evidence for the null for all comparisons (BFs ≤ 0.20). For religion, a Mann-Whitney U test comparing no-religion vs. (combined) religion groups revealed a statistically significant difference for personal events (Z = -2.006, p = .047) with the no-religion group assigning a higher average weight to this category of events than the religion group (Mdn 35.7 vs. 33.9, respectively). However, the Bayesian Mann-Whitney U test revealed anecdotal evidence (BF = 0.59). Comparisons for family, work and financial categories were not statistically significant (p’s > .1) which was confirmed by the Bayesian results which supported the null (BFs ≤ 0.28). For relationship status, a Mann-Whitney U test comparing (combined) married vs. (combined) unmarried groups showed a statistically significant difference for family events (Z = -2.144, p = .032) only with the married group giving higher ratings than the unmarried group (Mdn 55 vs. 53.6, respectively). However, the Bayesian Mann-Whitney U test revealed anecdotal evidence (BF = 1.74). The comparisons for personal, financial and work were not statistically significant (p’s > .3), confirmed by Bayesian evidence for the null (BFs ≤ 0.16). For employment status, a Mann-Whitney U test comparing employed vs. (combined) unemployed groups revealed no statistically significant differences (p’s > .2) for any of the categories. Congruent with this outcome, Bayesian Mann-Whitney U tests revealed evidence for the null for all comparisons (BFs ≤ 0.20). Sensitivity analyses are provided in Appendix 6 (S6 Appendix) for all the above-mentioned Bayesian Mann-Whitney U comparisons and support the reported BFs.

Fig 1. Box plots showing differences between males and females based on event categories.

Fig 1

From top-left to bottom-right the box plots shown are: ‘family’, ‘personal’, ‘work’ and ‘financial’.

SRRS weights: Comparing normative and 70+ sub-samples

Muhlenkamp and colleagues [12] who extended the original SRRS by adding weights to represent those aged ≥ 65 to 84 years compared the original 1967 normative sample’s ratings (<30 years to > 60 years, n = 394) with those from their new, older group (65 to 84 yrs, n = 41). A similar approach was followed here, however the present study’s older group was 70 to 84 years to minimise overlap with previously represented older age groups (e.g. 65 to 69 year-olds). The original study stated that there were 51 raters > 60 years (i.e. no maximum age was reported) while Muhlenkamp and colleagues [12] stated in their report that the original SRRS did not include adults over 70 years. Thus, in the present study, the sample was grouped as those aged 18 to 69 years (‘normative sample’, n = 473) vs. 70+ years (‘70+ sample’, n = 67). Between-groups differences were evaluated overall as well as within the previously mentioned 4 life events categories: ‘family’, ‘personal’, ‘work’ and ‘financial’.

In the overall assessment the normative sample’s summed total LCUs were higher (LCUtotal 1829) than that of the 70+ group (LCUtotal 1759), however a Mann-Whitney U test found no statistically significant difference between the two groups, on average (p > .7). Likewise, the Bayesian equivalent supported the null (BF = 0.24). Sensitivity analyses (S6 Appendix) were congruent with this outcome. Further, the Kendall’s tau indicated that the lists were strongly, positively correlated (r = .884, p < .001). The corresponding Bayesian Kendall’s tau provided decisive evidence for this finding (BF > 100). Of the 42 original items rated, only 12 were higher for the 70+ group. Thus, the normative and older samples co-varied strongly regarding the ratings, though the normative sample’s ratings were consistently higher for most items. To assess whether there were any systematic differences in ratings between the normative group and 70+ group based on the 4 categories, a chi-square was conducted with event category (family, personal, financial, work) and proportion of change (> adjustment required in 70+ participants vs. > adjustment required in normative participants). The association was not statistically significant (p = .648). Likewise, the Bayesian contingency tables test supported the null (BF = 0.17). These findings suggest that there were no significant age-based differences in ratings across the different categories of events.

SRRS weights: Comparing young, middle-aged and older adults

A set of SRRS weights and ranks for each age group were created and are provided in Table 5. Summing the weights by age group, it was found that YAs’ summed weights value or total life change units (LCUtotal) was 1866, for MAs the LCUtotal was 1875 and for OAs it was 1867. A Kruskal-Wallis test revealed no statistically significant differences (p > .997). Bayesian non-parametric Mann-Whitney U comparisons agreed with these findings, showing evidence for the null (BFs = 0.23). Bayes factor sensitivity analyses (S6 Appendix) were comparable. These results indicated comparable overall weights across the life span. In assessing the strength of association across all items between the pairs of age groups (YA vs. MA; MA vs. OA; YA vs. OA), Kendall’s tau correlation coefficients indicated very strong, positive correlations (r’s ≥ .835, p’s < .001). Bayesian Kendall’s tau coefficients agreed with these findings (BFs > 100).

Table 5. SRRS events by young, middle-aged and older adults’ weights and ranks.

Holmes & Rahe Present study
Young adults Middle-aged adults Older adults
Event original rank rank Mean (SE)a 99% CIb rank Mean (SE)a 99% CIb rank Mean (SE)a 99% CIb
Death of a spouse or life partner 1 1 85.65 (2.1) 79.53, 90.65 1 87.72 (1.24) 84.24, 90.8 1 85.92 (2.07) 80.27, 91.08
Divorce 2 6 65.13 (2.15) 59.34, 70.87 4 68.28 (1.39) 64.66, 71.64 4 69.34 (2.05) 63.45, 74.43
Marital separation 3 7 62.17 (2.15) 56.95, 67.54 5 68.09 (1.33) 64.49, 71.59 5 68.44 (1.92) 63.1, 73.18
Detention in jail or other institution 4 3 70.65 (2.53) 63.62, 76.69 2 79.74 (1.38) 76.4, 83.08 2 76.08 (2.4) 69.22, 81.99
Death of a close family member 5 2 75.26 (2.44) 68, 80.98 3 76.92 (1.38) 73.26, 80.32 3 74 (2.02) 68.54, 79.04
Major personal injury or illness 6 8 59.84 (2.14) 53.53, 65.63 6 65.72 (1.31) 62.46, 68.98 7 65.33 (1.89) 60.53, 70.24
Marriage (pre-set weight) 7 14 50 14 50 13 50
Losing your job 8 9 59.05 (2.22) 52.93, 64.54 10 61.7 (1.37) 57.99, 65.51 9 61.06 (2.2) 55.08, 67.03
Marital reconciliation 9 18 44.11 (1.95) 38.77, 49.37 16 47.01 (1.36) 43.26, 50.26 17 46.41 (1.9) 41.07, 51.2
Retirement from work 10 13 50.16 (2.38) 44.24, 56.17 15 49.9 (1.47) 46.34, 53.55 16 48.63 (2.18) 42.8, 53.99
Major change in the health or behaviour of a family member 11 15 49.13 (2.08) 43.87, 54.63 11 57.75 (1.31) 54.44, 61.2 11 57.06 (1.9) 51.71, 62.19
Pregnancy 12 5 66.61 (2.45) 60.49, 72.75 8 64.19 (1.36) 60.74, 67.54 8 63.97 (1.99) 58.47, 68.63
Sexual difficulties 13 30 34.47 (1.9) 29.8, 39.8 25 38.94 (1.28) 35.54, 42.3 24 39.29 (1.91) 33.98, 44.56
Gaining a new family member 14 12 52.8 (2.38) 47.02, 59.34 12 52.52 (1.55) 48.36, 56.6 15 49.41 (2.02) 44.34, 54.25
Major business readjustment 15 17 44.69 (2.27) 37.94, 50.73 17 46.19 (1.43) 42.65, 49.8 14 49.68 (2.24) 44.15, 55.21
Major change in financial state 16 11 54.3 (2.04) 49.06, 59.65 13 51.59 (1.24) 48.35, 54.94 12 50.96 (1.95) 45.2, 56.64
Death of a close friend 17 4 69.68 (2.37) 63.06, 75.74 7 64.99 (1.54) 60.83, 68.64 10 57.09 (2.17) 51.23, 62.53
Changing to a different line of work 18 22 40.88 (2) 35.67, 45.97 23 39.66 (1.14) 36.82, 42.83 26 37.86 (1.83) 33.26, 42.16
Major change in the number of arguments with spouse/life partner 19 19 42.55 (1.91) 37.52, 47.67 18 44.42 (1.27) 41.01, 47.6 19 45.22 (2.01) 39.13, 50.98
Taking on a mortgage or loan for a major purchase 20 16 46.09 (2.25) 40.23, 52.17 22 40 (1.28) 37.09, 44.12 20 43.71 (2) 38.3, 48.95
Foreclosure/repossession on mortgage or loan 21 10 55.22 (2.23) 49.49, 61.59 9 63.09 (1.41) 59.57, 66.72 6 67.11 (2.27) 60.81, 72.56
Major change in responsibilities at work 22 26 38.16 (1.79) 33.27, 42.88 28 37.09 (1.1) 34.21, 40.3 25 39.02 (1.73) 34.25, 43.78
Son or daughter leaving home 23 21 40.92 (1.97) 35.67, 45.87 19 41.98 (1.38) 38.62, 45.75 22 41.61 (1.99) 36.39, 46.26
In-law troubles 24 34 30.3 (1.99) 25.21, 35.09 32 30.25 (1.18) 27.05, 33.34 31 33.02 (1.85) 28.48, 38.37
Outstanding personal achievement 25 36 29.21 (2.07) 24.57, 34.16 34 29.66 (1.23) 26.45, 33.05 28 35.26 (1.98) 30.36, 40.52
Spouse/life partner begins or stops working 26 23 40.34 (1.89) 35.55, 45.12 20 41.88 (1.25) 38.85, 45.73 27 35.84 (1.85) 31.21, 40.86
Beginning or ceasing formal schooling 27 28 36.34 (2) 30.93, 41.56 31 33.07 (1.18) 30.26, 36.41 30 33.51 (2.02) 28.31, 39.14
Major change in living conditions 28 24 39.28 (2.03) 34.28, 45.03 24 39.38 (1.24) 36.05, 42.25 23 39.38 (1.87) 34.36, 44.22
Revision of personal habits 29 38 27.25 (1.93) 22.7, 32.39 41 21.56 (1) 19.19, 24.34 42 21.65 (1.39) 17.89, 25.55
Troubles with the boss 30 40 24.69 (1.84) 20.51, 29.09 33 30.03 (1.25) 26.94, 33.46 33 31.12 (2.01) 25.7, 36.14
Major change in work hours or conditions 31 25 38.64 (1.8) 33.95, 43.11 26 38.41 (1.15) 35.22, 42.12 32 32.85 (1.69) 28.43, 37.43
Change in residence 32 20 41.01 (2.04) 36.02, 46.2 21 41.75 (1.29) 38.69, 44.92 18 46.23 (1.81) 41.48, 50.53
Changing to a new school 33 27 37.5 (2.02) 32.94, 42.68 29 33.51 (1.25) 30.37, 36.51 29 34.44 (2.12) 28.5, 40.14
Major change in usual type and/or amount of recreation 34 31 33.24 (1.86) 28.83, 38.04 35 28.65 (1.02) 26.2, 31.28 35 26.38 (1.5) 22.8, 30.77
Major change in religious activities 35 39 26.08 (1.7) 22.08, 30.2 44 18.05 (1.02) 15.55, 20.85 44 19.33 (1.7) 14.88, 24.21
Major change in social activities 36 37 28.78 (1.9) 24.2, 33.82 38 23.3 (1.06) 20.31, 26.5 39 22.95 (1.4) 19.4, 26.91
Taking on a loan for a lesser purchase 37 35 29.83 (1.95) 25.3, 35.2 37 23.51 (1.1) 20.85, 26.6 36 24.55 (1.66) 20.33, 28.88
Major change in sleeping habits 38 32 32.26 (2.13) 27.13, 37.73 30 33.25 (1.12) 30.21, 36.13 34 28.74 (1.49) 25.24, 32.57
Major change in number of family get-togethers 39 41 24.53 (1.88) 20.13, 29.1 40 21.73 (0.94) 19.44, 24.4 38 23.97 (1.56) 20.28, 28.06
Major change in eating habits 40 33 30.68 (1.94) 26.24, 36.11 36 27.63 (1.06) 24.81, 30.71 37 24 (1.36) 20.63, 28.16
Vacation 41 42 21.45 (2.05) 16.28, 26.5 43 19.13 (1.07) 16.33, 22.04 43 21 (1.51) 17.29, 25.52
Christmas 42 44 16.95 (1.7) 12.65, 21.35 42 19.79 (1.09) 16.76, 22.86 41 22.23 (1.93) 17.62, 27.65
Minor violations of the law 43 43 19.85 (1.47) 16.66, 24.09 39 22.72 (1.04) 20, 25.45 40 22.87 (1.64) 18.4, 27.3
Single person, living alone 29 34.96 (2.59) 28.55, 41.39 27 37.84 (1.49) 33.92, 41.51 21 41.66 (2.28) 35.82, 48.26

a SE = Standard Error.

b CI = Confidence Intervals.

SRRS weights: Comparing males and females

Miller and Rahe (1997) found that females’ ratings were 17% higher on average than that of males. Table 6 provides the descriptive statistics for the present samples’ weights and ranking by sex. Females’ summed weights (LCUtotal 1992) were found to be 14% higher than those for males (LCUtotal 1708). The corresponding Mann-Whitney U test revealed a trend (z = -1.840, p = .066, r = .2), suggesting that the average difference between males’ (Mdn 35.3, n = 43) and females’ (Mdn 45.1, n = 43) LCUtotal was statistically comparable. The corresponding Bayesian test revealed anecdotal evidence for this finding (BF = 1.42). Bayes factor sensitivity analyses (S6 Appendix) were comparable. A Kendall’s tau correlation coefficient indicated a very strong, positive correlation between males’ and females’ ratings (r = .892, p < .001), as indicated by Fig 2. The corresponding Bayesian Kendall’s tau correlation revealed decisive evidence for this finding (BF > 100). These results suggest that whilst females’ ratings were higher than males’ on all items, there was strong covariance between them. The 3 items with the greatest difference in ratings, with males’ ratings being lower in each case, were ‘Death of a close friend’ (-15 LCU), ‘Spouse/life partner begins or stops working’ (-13 LCU) and ‘Son or daughter leaving home’ (-12 LCU). A likewise comparison regarding the 3 items with the smallest difference were ‘Gaining a new family member’ (-1 LCU), ‘Retirement from work’ (-2 LCU) and ‘Marital reconciliation’ (-3 LCU).

Table 6. SRRS events by females’ and males’ weights and ranks.

Female Male
Event rank M (SE) 99% CI rank M (SE) 99% CI
Death of a spouse or life partner 1 89.73 (1.03) 86.48, 92.62 1 82.96 (1.75) 78.09, 87.37
Divorce 5 69.91 (1.31) 66.38, 73.15 4 65.1 (1.68) 61.02, 69.41
Marital separation 6 69.09 (1.23) 65.79, 72.11 5 64.34 (1.68) 59.93, 68.35
Detention in jail or other institution 3 79.28 (1.38) 75.21, 83.25 2 73.78 (1.77) 69.04, 78.63
Death of a close family member 2 80.85 (1.22) 77.52, 84.07 3 69.23 (1.76) 64.72, 74.2
Major personal injury or illness 8 67.33 (1.21) 64.07, 70.62 7 60.52 (1.57) 56.38, 64.28
Marriage (arbitrary weight) 16 50 15 50
Losing your job 10 63.43 (1.3) 60.34, 66.66 9 58.09 (1.66) 53.8, 61.99
Marital reconciliation 17 47.5 (1.16) 44.07, 51.14 16 44.83 (1.52) 41.11, 48.59
Retirement from work 15 50.39 (1.37) 46.91, 54.32 14 48.51 (1.7) 44.07, 52.58
Major change in the health or behaviour of a family member 11 60.28 (1.15) 56.98, 63.32 10 49.82 (1.64) 45.73, 53.98
Pregnancy 7 67.81 (1.35) 64.44, 71.52 6 60.78 (1.73) 56.34, 65.43
Sexual difficulties 27 39.24 (1.17) 35.88, 42.18 26 36.52 (1.47) 32.68, 40
Gaining a new family member 13 52.21 (1.44) 48.95, 55.73 12 51.09 (1.8) 45.9, 55.72
Major business readjustment 14 51.84 (1.33) 48.6, 55.11 13 39.73 (1.61) 35.45, 44.14
Major change in financial state 12 54.58 (1.21) 51.52, 57.64 11 48.44 (1.49) 44.56, 52.34
Death of a close friend 4 70.24 (1.36) 66.8, 73.76 10 55.62 (1.88) 51.5, 60.37
Changing to a different line of work 24 42.67 (1.16) 39.52, 45.87 23 35.33 (1.26) 32.05, 38.56
Major change in the number of arguments with spouse-life partner 18 47.12 (1.23) 43.79, 50.4 17 40.27 (1.42) 36.54, 43.59
Taking on a mortgage or loan for a major purchase 22 45.12 (1.25) 42.06, 48.23 21 38.09 (1.49) 33.95, 41.64
Foreclosure/repossession on mortgage or loan 9 65.23 (1.44) 61.56, 68.54 8 58.65 (1.75) 54.29, 63.21
Major change in responsibilities at work 26 40.72 (1.13) 37.81, 43.77 25 33.86 (1.23) 30.51, 37.15
Son or daughter leaving home 19 47.02 (1.27) 44.07, 50.45 18 34.71 (1.45) 30.96, 38.58
In-law troubles 30 35.04 (1.18) 32.08, 38.33 29 25.56 (1.27) 22.25, 28.63
Outstanding personal achievement 32 33.43 (1.35) 30.1, 36.98 31 27.58 (1.31) 23.73, 31.23
Spouse/life partner begins or stops working 20 45.73 (1.17) 42.81, 48.54 19 32.25 (1.24) 29.04, 35.63
Beginning or ceasing formal schooling 29 36.79 (1.2) 33.98, 40.25 28 30.07 (1.38) 26.45, 33.54
Major change in living conditions 23 42.76 (1.2) 39.5, 46.39 22 34.89 (1.4) 31, 38.96
Revision of personal habits 39 25.59 (1.11) 22.56, 28.84 38 18.97 (1.03) 16.52, 21.82
Troubles with the boss 33 32.07 (1.26) 28.61, 35.55 32 25.19 (1.3) 21.83, 28.38
Major change in work hours or conditions 25 41.06 (1.11) 37.97, 44.21 24 31.7 (1.23) 28.6, 34.82
Change in residence 21 45.63 (1.22) 42.38, 48.8 20 38.79 (1.5) 34.66, 42.62
Changing to a new school 28 38.2 (1.29) 34.82, 41.54 27 29.76 (1.34) 26.23, 33.14
Major change in usual type and/or amount of recreation 34 31.81 (1.03) 29.2, 34.92 33 25.28 (1.11) 22.33, 28.14
Major change in religious activities 42 21.8 (1.08) 19.25, 24.56 41 17.77 (1.2) 14.86, 21.02
Major change in social activities 36 26.93 (1.03) 24.45, 29.45 35 20.73 (1.12) 17.94, 23.51
Taking on a loan for a lesser purchase 37 26.53 (1.12) 23.56, 29.65 36 23.19 (1.24) 19.64, 26.52
Major change in sleeping habits 31 34.81 (1.15) 31.64, 37.69 30 27.77 (1.22) 24.9, 31.18
Major change in number of family get-togethers 38 25.93 (1.08) 23.17, 28.91 37 18.78 (1) 16.27, 21.23
Major change in eating habits 35 29.11 (1.03) 26.46, 31.62 34 24.89 (1.15) 22.09, 27.91
Vacation 41 22.01 (1.12) 19.34, 24.97 40 17.21 (1.1) 14.33, 20.1
Christmas 43 21.66 (1.16) 18.87, 24.53 42 17.38 (1.18) 14.5, 20.74
Minor violations of the law 40 23.82 (1.06) 21.32, 26.44 39 19.87 (1.14) 17.18, 22.73
Single person, living alone* 40.10 (1.55) 35.91, 44.52 35.37 (1.76) 30.48, 39.73

*’Single person, living alone’ was not included in the ranking as it was not included in the original rating of the SRRS.

Fig 2. Scatter plot showing the covariance of females’ and males’ weights by items.

Fig 2

Scatter plot shows the covariance of females’ and males’ weights by items with weights (life change units) ranging from 0 to 100, grouped by category: ‘family’, ‘financial’, ‘personal’ and ‘work’.

Impact of personal experience on SRRS ratings

To ascertain whether there was a link between ratings for each life event and personal experience of that item a series of correlations using Kendall’s tau were conducted. Table 7 provides the descriptive statistics for personal experience. Of 42 items, 18 showed a statistically significant correlation (p’s ≤ .042). However, all coefficients were very small (r ≤ .146). The Bayesian Kendall’s tau similarly found evidence ranging from substantial to decisive (BFs ≥ 3.0) for 15 items. Of these, 6 items were supported by ≥ very strong evidence but correlation coefficients remained small with magnitudes ranging from 0.108 to 0.146 as shown in Appendix 9 (S9 Appendix). These results suggest that there may have been some events for which personal experience were weakly, positively associated with event ratings. Overall, however, participants’ personal experiences did not appear to systematically bias their ratings.

Table 7. Descriptive statistics showing the extent to which ratings were based on personal experience.

Life event Mean % (SE)* Median % (IQR)
Christmas 83.39 (1.14) 99 (76–100)
Vacation 81.35 (1.1) 93 (68–100)
Death of a close family member 73.11 (1.55) 90.5 (60–100)
Change in residence 72.96 (1.42) 85 (59–100)
Beginning or ceasing formal schooling 68.05 (1.56) 80 (45.5–100)
Pregnancy 56.17 (1.95) 77.5 (0–100)
Major change in the health or behaviour of a family member 65.4 (1.53) 75 (41–100)
Taking on a mortgage or loan for a major purchase 58.39 (1.81) 74 (2–100)
Gaining a new family member 58.74 (1.78) 70.5 (7.5–100)
Major change in sleeping habits 62.54 (1.53) 70 (29–100)
Changing to a new school 58.56 (1.7) 70 (13–100)
Troubles with the boss 58.63 (1.63) 70 (19.5–99)
Major change in financial state 62.59 (1.43) 70 (39–94.5)
Taking on a loan for a lesser purchase 54.91 (1.8) 68 (1.5–100)
Changing to a different line of work 56.08 (1.6) 66 (17–89.5)
Major change in number of family get-togethers 57.12 (1.49) 63.5 (26–86)
Major change in eating habits 56.3 (1.55) 63 (20.5–90)
Single person, living alone 51.99 (1.89) 61.5 (0–100)
Major change in responsibilities at work 53.77 (1.55) 61 (18–86)
Major change in work hours or conditions 54.15 (1.5) 61 (21–81)
Outstanding personal achievement 51.31 (1.62) 56 (13–85)
Major change in usual type and/or amount of recreation 49.05 (1.51) 55 (12–77)
Major personal injury or illness 49.93 (1.73) 54.5 (4–95.5)
Major change in social activities 48.51 (1.5) 53 (13.5–76)
Major change in living conditions 47.19 (1.66) 51 (1–82)
Losing your job 46.81 (1.81) 50.5 (0–93)
Major change in the number of arguments with spouse/life partner 43.12 (1.58) 44 (1–74)
Revision of personal habits 42.1 (1.56) 41.5 (3–72)
Minor violations of the law 43.8 (1.8) 35 (0–89)
Spouse/life partner begins or stops working 42.17 (1.81) 26.5 (0–90)
Death of a close friend 40.33 (1.84) 17 (0–90)
Sexual difficulties 33.85 (1.64) 15 (0–69)
In-law troubles 32.57 (1.67) 11 (0–66)
Major change in religious activities 25.72 (1.51) 2.5 (0–51)
Son or daughter leaving home 34.66 (1.86) 1 (0–87)
Marital separation 26.48 (1.7) 0 (0–55.5)
Death of a spouse or life partner 17.71 (1.48) 0 (0–11)
Divorce 25.52 (1.71) 0 (0–53)
Marital reconciliation 15.4 (1.33) 0 (0–10)
Major business readjustment 20.75 (1.37) 0 (0–31)
Retirement from work 28.6 (1.75) 0 (0–61)
Foreclosure/repossession on mortgage or loan 11.04 (1.11) 0 (0–3)
Detention in jail or other institution 10.58 (1.18) 0 (0–0)

*Ordered from highest to lowest mean % personal experience.

A value of 100 indicates that the rating for an item was completely based on personal experience.

A value of 0 indicates that the rating for an item was not at all based on personal experience.

N = 536.

Impact of loneliness on SRRS ratings

Overall, participants’ average loneliness score (loneliness level), as measured by the R-UCLA (higher value = higher level of loneliness), was M 5.1 (SE .08), ranging from 3 to 9. In response to how often respondents felt lonely (loneliness frequency), with a lower value indicating feeling lonely more often, 9.8% (n = 53) were lonely ‘often/always’, 24.1% (n = 130) ‘some of the time, 24.3% (n = 131) ‘occasionally’, 27.6% (n = 149) ‘hardly ever’ while 13% (n = 70) indicated ‘never’.

To explore whether loneliness affected SRRS ratings, Kendall’s tau correlational analyses were conducted to test the association between the R-UCLA and loneliness frequency measures and each of the 43 SRRS rating items. The frequentist analyses for both level and frequency of loneliness revealed statistically significant correlations for 7 items (p’s ≤ .039), however the correlation coefficients were very small (r ≤ .128). For both level and frequency, 6 items were ‘Revision of personal habits’; ‘Foreclosure/repossession on mortgage or loan’; ‘Detention in jail or other institution’; ‘Major change in usual type and/or amount of recreation’; ‘Major change in work hours or conditions’ and ‘Major change in religious activities’. For loneliness level only, ‘Major change in sleeping habits’ was. For loneliness frequency only ‘Major change in social activities’ was significant. Bayesian analysis was only conducted for loneliness level as there was no equivalent non-parametric Bayesian analysis for loneliness frequency in JASP. The Bayesian Kendall’s tau conducted between R-UCLA scores and ratings revealed only two noteworthy results: decisive evidence for a small positive correlation (r = 0.128, BF > 100) between ‘Revision of personal habits’ and R-UCLA scores and substantial evidence for ’Single person, living alone’ and R-UCLA scores (r = 0.082, BF = 3.18). These outcomes indicated that an increase in level of loneliness experienced was associated with an increase in the respective ratings. All other associations either supported the null (nratings = 32; BF ≤ 0.30) or evidence was anecdotal (nratings = 9; ≤ 0.35 BF ≤ 2.42).

Extending the SRRS: New items

Proposed new item: ‘Single person, living alone’

Table 8 provides descriptive statistics and rank for this new item relative to the existing 43 life events. As the table shows, the overall averaged weight based on the arithmetic mean was 38 (SE 1.13), which places its rank as lower than marriage. Non-parametric frequentist and Bayesian statistics were used to evaluate whether ratings for this event differed depending on age, sex, ethnicity, relationship status, employment status or religion. Table 9 presents their descriptive statistics. A Kruskal Wallis test revealed no statistically significant differences between age groups (p > .07). Bayesian Mann-Whitney U tests compared all 2-way age group combinations and similarly found evidence supporting the null regarding YA vs. MA and MA vs. OA (BFs ≤ 0.17). Evidence comparing YA and OA was anecdotal (BF = 0.43). Comparing males and females, a Mann-Whitney U test revealed a statistically significant difference (z = -2.086, p = .034) with females (Mdn 40) rating this item higher than males (Mdn 30). The Bayesian Mann-Whitney U revealed anecdotal evidence (BF = 0.52), however. For ethnicity (white vs. non-white) and employment (employed vs. unemployed) ratings between groups were comparable for both frequentist (p > .3) and Bayesian (BF ≤ 0.14) tests. In contrast, both relationship status (married vs. unmarried) and religion (religion vs. no-religion) frequentist Mann Whitney tests revealed statistically significant outcomes. The married group (Mdn 40) assigned a higher rating than the unmarried group (Mdn 35) (z = -2.578, p = .01) and for the religion comparison, the religion group (Mdn 40) (z = -2.615, p = .009) rated this item higher than the no-religion group (Mdn 30). However, the corresponding Bayesian Mann-Whitney U results revealed anecdotal evidence for both relationship status (BF = 1.77) and religion (BF = 1.68). Thus, it remains unclear if participants from these respective sub-groups differ systematically regarding these items.

Table 8. New vs. original weights and rank order of SRRS events, including new item: ’Single person, living alone’.
Holmes & Rahe Present Study
Event rank weight rank Mean (SE)a 99% CIb Geometric Meanc Mdn (IQR)d
Death of a spouse or life partner 1 100 1 86.83 (0.98) 84.16, 89.21 73.78 95 (80–100)
Detention in jail or other institution 4 63 2 76.88 (1.14) 73.79, 79.66 61.65 80 (70–99)
Death of a close family member 5 63 3 75.84 (1.02) 73.13, 78.46 67.57 80 (60.5–95)
Divorce 2 73 4 67.86 (1.03) 65.2, 70.41 56.03 70 (52.8–85)
Marital separation 3 65 5 66.9 (1.01) 64.28, 69.33 55.98 70 (50–80)
Pregnancy 12 40 6 64.66 (1.06) 62.03, 67.27 47.63 60 (40–70)
Major personal injury or illness 6 53 7 64.36 (0.98) 61.9, 66.91 55.98 70 (50–80)
Death of a close friend 17 37 8 64.05 (1.12) 61.08, 66.8 44.9 50 (40–70)
Foreclosure/repossession on mortgage or loan 21 30 9 62.39 (1.07) 59.65, 65.09 32.71 40 (25–60)
Losing your job 8 47 10 60.97 (1.03) 58.2, 63.5 24.33 30 (20–50)
Major change in the health or behaviour of a family member 11 44 11 55.73 (0.98) 53.06, 58.24 38.01 50 (30–70)
Major change in financial state 16 38 12 52.02 (0.94) 49.68, 54.68 35.47 50 (30–65)
Gaining a new family member 14 39 13 51.81 (1.11) 49.05, 54.65 29.09 40 (20–50)
Marriage (pre-set weight) 7 50 14 50
Retirement from work 10 45 15 49.64 (1.08) 46.73, 52.45 35.58 50 (30–60)
Major business readjustment 15 39 16 46.73 (1.06) 43.96, 49.49 40.18 50 (30.8–70)
Marital reconciliation 9 45 17 46.24 (0.97) 43.77, 48.59 51.32 65 (45–80)
Major change in the number of arguments with spouse-life partner 19 35 18 44.21 (0.92) 41.75, 46.56 31.1 40 (25.5–50)
Change in residence 32 20 19 42.69 (0.95) 40.33, 44.99 34.54 40 (30–59.8)
Taking on a mortgage or loan for a major purchase 20 31 20 42.22 (0.99) 39.78, 44.79 34.98 45 (30–60)
Son or daughter leaving home 23 29 21 41.66 (0.99) 39.2, 44.19 21.75 30 (15–40)
Spouse/life partner begins or stops working 26 26 22 40.06 (0.9) 37.72, 42.36 21.22 30 (10–50)
Changing to a different line of work 18 36 23 39.48 (0.85) 37.3, 41.68 52.64 70 (45.8–80)
Major change in living conditions 28 25 24 39.36 (0.9) 37.01, 41.75 23.76 30 (20–50)
Single person, living alone 25 38.16 (1.13) 35.41, 41.16 24.14 40 (15–50)
Sexual difficulties 13 39 26 38.07 (0.92) 35.74, 40.61 53.44 70 (50–80)
Major change in responsibilities at work 22 29 27 37.8 (0.83) 35.54, 39.94 50.24 70 (50–80)
Major change in work hours or conditions 31 20 28 37.09 (0.84) 34.76, 39.32 20.44 25 (10–40)
Changing to a new school 33 20 29 34.6 (0.93) 32.29, 36.95 29.5 35 (20–50)
Beginning or ceasing formal schooling 27 26 30 33.88 (0.91) 31.57, 36.14 31.07 40 (25–55)
Major change in sleeping habits 38 16 31 31.92 (0.84) 29.83, 34.3 24.85 30 (20–40)
Outstanding personal achievement 25 28 32 30.94 (0.96) 28.49, 33.55 30.56 38.5 (21.3–50)
In-law troubles 24 29 33 30.94 (0.91) 28.62, 33.26 31.31 40 (25–60)
Troubles with the boss 30 23 34 29.15 (0.9) 26.72, 31.74 15.66 20 (10–30)
Major change in usual type and/or amount of recreation 34 19 35 29.08 (0.78) 27.01, 31.04 11.43 15 (5–30)
Major change in eating habits 40 15 36 27.39 (0.77) 25.2, 29.47 15.72 20 (10–30)
Taking on a loan for a lesser purchase 37 17 37 25.12 (0.84) 22.97, 27.6 17.36 20 (10–35)
Major change in social activities 36 18 38 24.39 (0.8) 22.31, 26.45 17.35 20 (10–30)
Major change in number of family get-togethers 39 15 39 22.88 (0.76) 20.9, 24.92 20.74 25 (10–40)
Revision of personal habits 29 24 40 22.8 (0.77) 20.95, 24.85 30.96 40 (25–53.8)
Minor violations of the law 43 11 41 22.14 (0.76) 20.12, 24.19 14.99 20 (10–30)
Major change in religious activities 35 19 42 20.09 (0.8) 18.01, 22.2 22.07 30 (15–40)
Vacation 41 13 43 20.09 (0.81) 18.03, 22.25 12.64 10 (6–30)
Christmas 42 12 44 19.78 (0.84) 17.79, 21.93 11.8 10 (5–30)

Table ordered by Present Study’s ranks, including ’Single person, living alone’.

a Mean (SE). Standard error obtained via BCa Bootstrap with 1000 samples.

b 99% confidence intervals obtained via BCa Bootstrap with 1000 samples.

c Where participants responded with a zero value, these were replaced with ’1’ to allow this value to be calculated.

d Mdn (IQR) = Median and inter-quartile range in brackets.

Table 9. Descriptive statistics for ’Single person, living alone’.
sub-groups Mean (SE) Median (IQR)
Age
< 30 years 34.96 (2.61) 30 (10–50)
30 to 60 years 37.84 (1.52) 35 (18–50)
> 60 years 41.66 (2.31) 45 (20–65)
Sex
female 40.1 (1.51) 40 (20–60)
male 35.37 (1.73) 30 (10–50)
Ethnicity
white 38.62 (1.25) 40 (15–52)
non-whitea 35.72 (2.81) 30 (15.5–50)
Religion
no religion 35.35 (1.67) 30 (10–50)
religiousb 40.88 (1.56) 40 (20–60)
Relationship status
marriedc 40.22 (1.41) 40 (20–60)
unmarriedd 34.57 (1.93) 35 (10–50)
Employment status
Employed 37.26 (1.42) 35 (15–50)
unemployede 39.79 (1.93) 40 (15.3–60)

a ’non-white’ included all ethnicities: mixed race, Asian (southern/southeastern Asia), Chinese (east Asian), black (any region).

b ’religious’ includes all religions: Christian, Buddhist, Hindu, Jewish, Muslim, Sikh, any other religion.

c ’married’ includes married and long-term partners.

d ’unmarried’ includes those who don’t qualify as ’c’: divorced, in a relationship, life partner died, separated, single, widowed.

e ’unemployed’ includes those currently unemployed and looking for work, have never worked, long-term sick/disabled, looking after home or family, retired, student (p/t or f/t) and currently unemployed, other.

These results indicate that ratings for ‘Single person, living alone’ were comparable across age groups, ethnicity and employment status. However, for sex, religion and relationship status, evidence was inconclusive. Bayes factor sensitivity analyses (S6 Appendix) were comparable for all Mann-Whitney U comparisons reported above.

Items proposed by respondents

Participants were asked to provide a new item along with a corresponding rating for the amount of adjustment it required. Of 540 participants 259 (48%) gave no response, 50 (9.3%) suggested an item that was already in the SRRS. Further, one respondent gave a comment rather than an item, leaving 230 (42.6%) responses. Of 230 responses 43 items were ‘one-off’ suggestions (e.g. ‘Brexit’), which were grouped as ‘other’ and a further 11 participants offered more than one item but with only one rating. These 54 responses were excluded from consideration because their respective ratings were either unclear or could not be averaged. The final list comprised 176 respondents’ proposed new items along with their weights, given in Table 10. Items were given in participants’ own words therefore item wording was chosen as appropriate. For example, the item: ‘Death of a pet’ was based on statements including ‘loss of a pet’, ‘losing a pet’, ‘death of a family pet’ and ‘death of a pet’. The top 3 items were ‘Mental health issue’ (17%), ‘Death of a pet’ (14.8%) and ‘Emigration’ (8.5%). The averaged weights were 77, 72 and 69, respectively, as the table shows.

Table 10. Mean and standard errors (SE) for own items and frequency of raters per item.
Suggested additional items for SRRS Number of raters (n = 176) Mean (SE)
Mental health difficulties 30 77.2 (3.56)
Death of pet 26 72.31 (4.43)
Emigration 15 69.27 (4.93)
Relationship break-up 12 71.25 (6.8)
Covid (having the illness/unspecified) 10 76.5 (7.15)
Covid restrictions (e.g. lock-down) 7 70 (7.94)
Accident (e.g. car accident) 6 57.17 (10.59)
Becoming a carer (e.g. elderly relative) 6 81.67 (5.43)
Getting a pet 6 38.33 (4.94)
Infidelity (having affair) 6 78.67 (7.23)
Relocation 5 62 (3.39)
War/conflict 5 95 (5)
Addiction 4 62.5 (8.54)
Change in state policy/regime (e.g. Brexit) 4 60.75 (14.08)
Natural disaster 4 80 (4.08)
Sexuality/gender-identity (e.g. identifying as gay) 4 57.5 (12.5)
Trouble with neighbours 4 48.75 (14.35)
Victim of crime 4 48.75 (14.78)
Abuse 3 88.33 (4.41)
Adjusting to older age 3 60 (5.77)
Bullying 3 75 (2.89)
Domestic violence 3 91.67 (4.41)
Assault 2 47.5 (17.5)
Terminal illness 2 82.5 (7.5)
Wedding 2 80 (20)

Discussion

The SRRS weights were successfully updated using the ratings of 540 predominantly UK respondents aged 18 to 84. In addition, item wording was modernised, one optional extra item was added to the end of the scale and 3 potential new items proposed by raters were identified, namely: ‘Mental health issue’, ‘Death of a pet’ and ‘Emigration’. Changes/modernisations made did not affect the meaning of any of the original items. No items were removed. By doing so, the SRRS was improved and backwards-compatibility with the original scale, that continues to be widely used, was maintained.

The main findings were that the updated SRRS yielded a significantly higher total score on average than the original scale. However, the new weights were broadly consistent with the original weights, indicating continuity between US and UK samples. Comparing all life events in the original and new scales using Rahe’s [24] 4 categories, ‘personal’, ‘family’, ‘work’ and ‘finance’ it was found, as with the overall correlation, that there was a strong covariance for all categories except finance. Focusing on the present study’s sample, young, middle-aged and older adults were comparable in their total LCU scores. Females assigned, on average, 14% higher weight to life events than males but the statistical evidence for this gender-based variation in life change units was inconclusive. Similarly, while the sample’s +70s group assigned, on average, a 4% lower weight to life events than the normative-aged sample, this difference was statistically negligible. Regarding the possible influence of personal experience on ratings, some significant associations were found, though coefficient sizes were very small. The new additional item, ‘Single person, living alone’ required less adjustment relative to marriage. Most commonly, participants felt lonely occasionally and average level of loneliness was rated towards the lower end of the range. Loneliness was, at best, weakly associated with the SRRS ratings given. Thus, participants’ ratings, particularly for ‘Single person, living alone’ were unlikely to have been influenced by their experience of loneliness.

SRRS life events require more adjustment now than in 1967

A comparison with the original SRRS weights indicated an overall average increase of 28%, which suggests that, on average, adults find SRRS events more taxing now than in 1967. Consistent with this increase, Miller and Rahe [13]’s update (n = 426) revealed a 45% overall increase. However, as in their update, a strong covariance pattern across ratings was found, but with some exceptions. Three of the items increased by ≥ 25 life change units (LCUs) relative to the original scale. They were also among the 6 items that had increased by ≥ 25 LCUs in the Miller and Rahe [13] update. Taken together, the aforementioned outcomes suggest that since the original weights were derived there has been considerable change in perceived adjustment needed regarding certain key life events such as pregnancy and foreclosure on a loan, but on the whole, there is overall consensus regarding relative importance of the life events in the scale. The exception being financial items. The new sample’s weights for the 4 financial life events did not correlate with the original sample and, as a category, showed the largest average increase in weights while family items showed the smallest increase. By comparison, the Miller and Rahe [13] update also showed the largest average increase in weights for financial items but the smallest increase was for personal items. Interestingly, the Scully et al. [10] update, relative to the original, indicated that across categories, weights consistently decreased but the smallest decrease was for financial items. Scully’s sample comprised 200 Florida residents, however, which makes their result less generalisable. The relative volatility of the financial items may be because there are only 4 items included in this sub-set. In addition, one could argue that economic factors are generally more volatile than social factors though a thorough examination of this would be required.

Females’ ratings for SRRS life events were slightly higher

Females’ total LCUs were 14% higher on average relative to males’ LCUs, though this difference was inconclusive. However, an in-depth Bayesian analysis for each of the 4 categories revealed substantial to very strong evidence that females’ weights were consistently higher than males’ weights. This pattern of higher weights among females is in line with Miller and Rahe [13]’s update reporting that their females’ weights were 17% higher. They did not report whether this difference was statistically significant. This pattern indicating that females typically assign a greater level of adjustment to change relative to males, warrants more detailed investigation in future work. Research investigating the gender-specific profiles of psychiatric disorders indicate that stressed women become hyper-aroused, which is a common feature of depression [44]. However, stressed men’s cognitive function can be differentially disrupted [44,45] and evidence shows that these differences associated with stress may be consequent to sex-based differences within the locus coeruleus-norepinephrine arousal system [44,46]. These results show that there are numerous underlying psychological, physiological and environmental factors to consider regarding sex-based differences in stress-reactivity and, by implication, perceived adjustment to different life events.

Adults aged 70+ rate SRRS items similarly to adults aged 18 to 69

The normative and 70+ groups gave comparable ratings on average. In contrast, Muhlenkamp et al.’s [12] study found that older participants assigned higher weights relative to the original 1967 sample. However, in line with the present study’s results, they concluded that there was agreement regarding relative importance of some life events. They argued that their older group’s higher ratings for ‘personal’ life events were consistent with literature indicating that adults become more self-orientated (egocentric) with age [12]. By comparison, the present results are congruent with the consistent finding in the literature that social and emotional functioning does not change significantly across the lifespan and that self-regulation, including coping with stressors, improves with age [47]. Further support of this is that the young, middle-aged and older adults were comparable in their ratings, which yielded very similar averages and age did not reveal any systematic differences when considering items as categories. However, this does not mean that age should be ignored. While older age is associated with improved emotional well-being, they respond less well compared to the young in some situations. For example, older adults show more pronounced psychological and physiological reactions relative to younger adults when stressful events are complex, affecting multiple life domains [48]. It is also worth noting that middle adulthood is associated with increased roles and responsibilities such as career progression and having a family. Moreover, they are pivotal to the younger and older members of their family [49] which can be very stressful particularly for those caring for children and elderly parents/relatives [50]. These points highlight the complexity of measuring the impact of life events at different points across the lifespan.

Adding ratings for a new item: ‘Single person, living alone’

‘Single person, living alone’, the new item, yielded some differences in ratings between age groups with the greatest disparity between young and older adults (OA>YA). However, the result was inconclusive. The same was true for comparisons by relationship status, religion and sex. For ethnicity and employment status evidence supported the null. Particularly regarding the age-related finding, this life event is important to monitor in future work. The ONS [51] data indicate a continuing upward trend in the number of one-person households. Moreover, the ONS data agrees with the findings of a recent study showing that among those in western countries, such as Europe and North America, more males than females live alone among young (25 to 29) and middle-aged adults (50 to 54) while more females live alone among older adults (75 to 79) [52]. Their review of global patterns further revealed that family-based living is most common, except in older age. Indeed, among ageing populations around the world, the number of older adults living alone is likely to increase (ibid). It is important to distinguish between sub-populations of older adults who live alone, as those with a diverse social network (e.g. adult children, siblings, friends and neighbours to socialise with and ask for support) have been shown to have better well-being than those with a restricted network (few family and friends/neighbours to reach out to) [53].

Raters’ 3 new items: ‘Mental health issue’, ‘Death of a pet’, ‘Emigration’

Roughly half the respondents added an additional item to the given list of events to rate. The 3 most common items are indicated in the sub-title above. The full table of items provides further opportunities for future investigation.

The impact on ratings of personal experience and loneliness

No clear pattern of association was found between ratings and personal experience of the life events nor between ratings and loneliness scores. Regarding personal experiences, 35% of items were significantly correlated but coefficient sizes were of no practical significance, suggesting that respondents were not unduly influenced by their own personal experiences (i.e. they adhered to the instructions to consider their own experiences as well as those of others). The results for loneliness revealed that for the 2 items, ‘Revision of personal habits’ and ‘Single person, living alone’, there was a significant association with loneliness. Again, however, the coefficients were very small, providing confidence that ratings were not biased by loneliness scores suggesting that ratings did not vary systematically with emotional state.

Limitations

A convenience sample rather than a randomly selected sample was used, which may have introduced selection bias. One could speculate that those who completed the survey were relatively more pro-active because participation required time and effort and had more resources because they would need basic computer skills, access to the internet and an internet-accessible device and time. Based on these assumptions, bias would be introduced by not including the segment of the UK population who were too stressed or struggling either financially or in other ways to take part. Their ratings may have been somewhat higher compared to our sample’s for items such as ‘Foreclosure/repossession on mortgage or loan’, ‘Detention in jail or other institution’ and ‘Losing your job’. Using the present study’s updated weights are likely suitable for the general population but if researchers intend to use these with specific sub-populations such as trauma survivors or those living in severe poverty, additional validation is advised. The same is true regarding ethnic and religious minority groups who represent a relatively small proportion within the UK population. Secondly, the updated weights agree broadly with the original study based on a US sample and would therefore probably generalise to populations culturally similar to the UK and US. Care should therefore be taken when using the updated weights with non-Western cultures. In addition, future work could include more extended differential item functioning analysis in these subgroups to explore the universality of stressful life events and their ratings. Thirdly, the statistical power was sub-optimal for between-groups comparisons, based on the Gpower calculations. However, Bayesian analyses together with accompanying sensitivity analyses provides valid statistical evidence for the findings reported.

Conclusion

This study’s aim was to update and improve the SRRS weights without fundamentally changing the scale to allow for backwards-compatibility of studies. This was achieved by providing new weights for all original items. The updated weights were higher but broadly consistent with those of the original study except for financial items, which may vary considerably over time. Cross-comparability with the original version was allowed for by retaining all original items, making helpful changes to the wording of some items without changing their meaning and adding a new item to the end, which can be excluded for comparisons. This sample’s ratings were not systematically influenced by personal experiences of events or loneliness. Such factors are important indicators of consistency across ratings but were not considered in the original scale nor subsequent versions.

The present study provides updated weights derived from a predominantly UK sample which is broadly proportionately representative regarding age, gender and ethnicity. Moreover, the age-range was broader and sample size slightly larger than the original. Though it is noted that this study was conducted using a convenience sample with a modest sample size. A new item was added to the end of the scale, ‘Single person, living alone’, which will benefit future work given that single-person households have increased in recent years. Three potential new items submitted by respondents have been identified: ‘Mental health issue’, ‘Death of a pet’ and ‘Emigration’. Further work may be undertaken in future studies to determine whether these items would be beneficial to add. What is known about the impact of demographics, such as gender and age, on the rating of life events was updated and extended, providing additional scope for future work.

Supporting information

S1 Checklist. The SRRS updated and modernised: STROBE checklist.

(DOCX)

S1 Appendix. SRRQ (updated version).

(PDF)

S2 Appendix. Original Instructions for SRRS.

(PDF)

S3 Appendix. SRRS updated version.

(PDF)

S4 Appendix. Personal experience of SRRS life events.

(PDF)

S5 Appendix. Loneliness questionnaire.

(PDF)

S6 Appendix. Bayes Factor sensitivity analysis for all Mann-Whitney U comparisons.

(PDF)

S7 Appendix. Social Readjustment Rating Scale by category.

(PDF)

S8 Appendix. Descriptive statistics for SRRS events by unabridged sub-group demographics.

(PDF)

S9 Appendix. Bayesian Kendall’s tau correlation between event ratings and degree to which these were based on personal experience.

(PDF)

Data Availability

All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.

Funding Statement

The author(s) received no specific funding for this work.

References

  • 1.Marshall AC, Cooper NR, Segrave R, Geeraert N. The effects of long-term stress exposure on aging cognition: a behavioral and EEG investigation. Neurobiol Aging. 2015;36(6):2136–44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Marshall AC, Cooper NR, Geeraert N. Experienced stress produces inhibitory deficits in old adults’ Flanker task performance: First evidence for lifetime stress effects beyond memory. Biol Psychol. 2016. b;113:1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2015.10.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Marshall AC, Cooper NR, Geeraert N. The impact of experienced stress on aged spatial discrimination: Cortical overreliance as a result of hippocampal impairment. Hippocampus. 2016. a;26(3):329–40. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22525 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Marshall AC, Cooper NR. The association between high levels of cumulative life stress and aberrant resting state EEG dynamics in old age. Biol Psychol. 2017;127(Supplement C):64–73. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2017.05.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Blasco-Fontecilla H, Delgado-Gomez D, Legido-Gil T, de Leon J, Perez-Rodriguez MM, Baca-Garcia E. Can the Holmes-Rahe Social Readjustment Rating Scale (SRRS) Be Used as a Suicide Risk Scale? An Exploratory Study. Arch Suicide Res. 2012;16(1):13–28. doi: 10.1080/13811118.2012.640616 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Wardhana M, Suryawati N, Rusyati LMM. The role of psychological stress in atopic dermatitis through an increase of il-31. Bali med j. 2020;9(1):27–30. [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Holmes TH, Rahe RH. The Social Readjustment Rating Scale. J Psychosom Res. 1967;11(2):213–8. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(67)90010-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Jiang X, Olsson T, Hillert J, Kockum I, Alfredsson L. Stressful life events are associated with the risk of multiple sclerosis. Eur J Neurol. 2020;27(12):2539–48. doi: 10.1111/ene.14458 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Salleh MR. Life event, stress and illness. Malays J Med Sci. 2008;15(4):9–18. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Scully JA, Tosi H, Banning K. Life Event Checklists: Revisiting the Social Readjustment Rating Scale after 30 Years. Educational and Psychological Measurement. 2016;60(6):864–76. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Hulbert-Williams L, Hastings RP. Life events as a risk factor for psychological problems in individuals with intellectual disabilities: a critical review. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2008;52(11):883–95. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2008.01110.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Muhlenkamp AF, Gress LD, Flood MA. Perception of life change events by the elderly. Nurs Res. 1975;24(2):109–13. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Miller MA, Rahe RH. Life changes scaling for the 1990s. J Psychosom Res. 1997;43(3):279–92. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3999(97)00118-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Hobson CJ, Kamen J, Szostek J, Nethercut CM, Tiedmann JW, Wojnarowicz S. Stressful Life Events: A Revision and Update of the Social Readjustment Rating Scale. Int J Stress Manag. 1998;5(1):1–23. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Hobson CJ, Delunas L. National Norms and Life-Event Frequencies for the Revised Social Readjustment Rating Scale. Int J Stress Manag. 2001;8(4):299–314. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Murphy E, Brown GW. Life events, psychiatric disturbance and physical illness. Br J Psychiatry. 1980;136(4):326–38. doi: 10.1192/bjp.136.4.326 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Cooper CL, Cooper R, Faragher B. Stress and life event methodology. Stress Medicine. 2006;1(4):287–9. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Cleary PJ. Problems of internal consistency and scaling in life event schedules. J Psychosom Res. 1981;25(4):309–20. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(81)90008-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Sesar AP, Sesar A, Bucan K, Sesar I, Cvitkovic K, Cavar I. Personality Traits, Stress, and Emotional Intelligence Associated with Central Serous Chorioretinopathy. Med Sci Monit. 2021;27:e928677. doi: 10.12659/MSM.928677 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Dong Y, Baumeister D, Berens S, Eich W, Tesarz J. Stressful Life Events Moderate the Relationship Between Changes in Symptom Severity and Health-related Quality of Life in Patients With Irritable Bowel Syndrome. J Clin Gastroenterol. 2020;54(5):445–51. doi: 10.1097/MCG.0000000000001261 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Rahe RH. Life change measurement clarification. Psychosom Med. 1978;40(2):95–8. doi: 10.1097/00006842-197803000-00001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Hawkins NG, Davies R, Holmes TH. Evidence of psychosocial factors in the development of pulmonary tuberculosis. Am Rev Tuberc. 1957;75(5):768–80. doi: 10.1164/artpd.1957.75.5.768 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Amundson ME, Hart CA, Holmes TH. Manual for the Schedule of Recent Experience (SRE). In: Holmes TH, David EM, editors. Life Change, Life Events, and Illness: Selected Papers. New York: Praeger Publishers; 1989. p. 125–48. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Rahe RH. Subjects’ recent life changes and their near-future illness reports. Ann Clin Res. 1972;4(5):250–65. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Komaroff AL, Masuda M, Holmes TH. The social readjustment rating scale: a comparative study of Negro, Mexican and white Americans. J Psychosom Res. 1968;12(2):121–8. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(68)90018-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Sarason I G., de Monchaux C, Hunt T. Methodological issues in the assessment of life stress. In: Levi L, editor. Emotions: Their Parameters and Measurement. New York: Raven Press; 1975. p. 499–509. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Rabkin JG, Struening EL. Life Events, Stress, and Illness. Science. 1976;194(4269):1013–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Rahe RH, Meyer M, Smith M, Kjaer G, Holmes TH. Social Stress and Illness Onset. J Psychosom Res. 1964;8(1):35–44. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(64)90020-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Mueller DP, Edwards DW, Yarvis RM. Stressful life events and psychiatric symptomatology: change or undesirability? J Health Soc Behav. 1977;18(3):307–17. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Ross CE, Mirowsky J, 2nd. A comparison of life-event-weighting schemes: change, undesirability, and effect-proportional indices. J Health Soc Behav. 1979;20(2):166–77. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Stallings MC, Dunham CC, Gatz M, Baker LA, Bengtson VL. Relationships Among Life Events and Psychological Well-Being: More Evidence for a Two-Factor Theory of Well-Being. J APPL GERONTOL. 2016;16(1):104–19. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Holmes TH, David EM, editors. Life change, life events, and illness: selected papers: Praeger Publishers; 1989.
  • 33.Lee SL, Pearce E, Ajnakina O, Johnson S, Lewis G, Mann F, et al. The association between loneliness and depressive symptoms among adults aged 50 years and older: a 12-year population-based cohort study. The Lancet Psychiatry. 2021;8(1):48–57. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(20)30383-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Eres R, Lim MH, Bates G. Loneliness and social anxiety in young adults: The moderating and mediating roles of emotion dysregulation, depression and social isolation risk. Psychology and Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice. 2023;n/a(n/a). doi: 10.1111/papt.12469 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Brown EG, Gallagher S, Creaven AM. Loneliness and acute stress reactivity: A systematic review of psychophysiological studies. Psychophysiology. 2018;55(5):e13031. doi: 10.1111/psyp.13031 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Lasgaard M, Friis K, Shevlin M. "Where are all the lonely people?" A population-based study of high-risk groups across the life span. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2016;51(10):1373–84. doi: 10.1007/s00127-016-1279-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Robards J. Principal projection for the UK—population by five-year age groups and sex. ONS website: Office for National Statistics; 2022. [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Rahe RH. Life changes and near-future illness reports. In: Levi L, Euler USvUS, editors. Emotions—Their Parameters and Measurement. New York: Raven Press; 1975. p. 511–29. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Russell DW. UCLA Loneliness Scale (Version 3): reliability, validity, and factor structure. J Pers Assess. 1996;66(1):20–40. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 40.Hughes ME, Waite LJ, Hawkley LC, Cacioppo JT. A Short Scale for Measuring Loneliness in Large Surveys: Results From Two Population-Based Studies. Res Aging. 2004;26(6):655–72. doi: 10.1177/0164027504268574 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Community Life Survey [Internet]. GOV.UK. [cited 07/12/2020]. Available from: https://www.gov.uk/government/collections/community-life-survey—2#community-life-survey:-focus-reports.
  • 42.JASP_Team. JASP. 0.16.1 ed: JASP team; 2022.
  • 43.Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang AG, Buchner A. G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods. 2007;39(2):175–91. doi: 10.3758/bf03193146 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Bangasser DA, Eck SR, Telenson AM, Salvatore M. Sex differences in stress regulation of arousal and cognition. Physiol Behav. 2018;187:42–50. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2017.09.025 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Shields GS, Sazma MA, Yonelinas AP. The effects of acute stress on core executive functions: A meta-analysis and comparison with cortisol. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2016;68:651–68. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.06.038 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Bangasser DA, Eck SR, Ordones Sanchez E. Sex differences in stress reactivity in arousal and attention systems. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2019;44(1):129–39. doi: 10.1038/s41386-018-0137-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Charles ST, Carstensen LL. Social and emotional aging. Annu Rev Psychol. 2010;61:383–409. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100448 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Wrzus C, Müller V, Wagner GG, Lindenberger U, Riediger M. Affective and cardiovascular responding to unpleasant events from adolescence to old age: complexity of events matters. Dev Psychol. 2013;49(2):384–97. doi: 10.1037/a0028325 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Lachman ME, Teshale S, Agrigoroaei S. Midlife as a Pivotal Period in the Life Course: Balancing Growth and Decline at the Crossroads of Youth and Old Age. Int J Behav Dev. 2015;39(1):20–31. doi: 10.1177/0165025414533223 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Gillett JE, Crisp DA. Examining coping style and the relationship between stress and subjective well-being in Australia’s ’sandwich generation’. Australas J Ageing. 2017;36(3):222–7. doi: 10.1111/ajag.12439 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.ONS. Living alone in the UK [dataset]. Office of National Statistics; 2019 [Available from: https://www.ons.gov.uk/peoplepopulationandcommunity/birthsdeathsandmarriages/families/datasets/livingaloneintheuk.
  • 52.Esteve A, Reher DS, Treviño R, Zueras P, Turu A. Living Alone over the Life Course: Cross-National Variations on an Emerging Issue. Popul Dev Rev. 2020;46(1):169–89. [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Djundeva M, Dykstra PA, Fokkema T. Is Living Alone "Aging Alone"? Solitary Living, Network Types, and Well-Being. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2019;74(8):1406–15. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gby119 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Adetayo Olorunlana

18 Oct 2023

PONE-D-23-17307The Social Readjustment Rating Scale: updated and modernisedPLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Wallace,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Dec 02 2023 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Adetayo Olorunlana, Ph.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf.

2. Did you know that depositing data in a repository is associated with up to a 25% citation advantage (https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0230416)? If you’ve not already done so, consider depositing your raw data in a repository to ensure your work is read, appreciated and cited by the largest possible audience. You’ll also earn an Accessible Data icon on your published paper if you deposit your data in any participating repository (https://plos.org/open-science/open-data/#accessible-data).

3. Please review your reference list to ensure that it is complete and correct. If you have cited papers that have been retracted, please include the rationale for doing so in the manuscript text, or remove these references and replace them with relevant current references. Any changes to the reference list should be mentioned in the rebuttal letter that accompanies your revised manuscript. If you need to cite a retracted article, indicate the article’s retracted status in the References list and also include a citation and full reference for the retraction notice.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: Dear author, thank you for the opportunity to review your manuscript. I can say that the main goals of this study are accomplished. There are good results, highlighted from the findings. The methodology is well described and the research questions are clear. Moreover, statistical analyses is well conducted and the literature is carefully selected. I can say that the manuscript is well revised and it is ready for the publication. Thank you.

Reviewer #2: Dear authors,

Thank you for giving me the opportunity to review your manuscript. Frankly, it was a joy to read. I have worked with the old version of the Holmes & Rahe scale and had wondered when somebody would manage to update this.

I only have minor comments to make.

(a) I understand that you have essentially done a quota sampling approach. While this can be representative as a sampling approach to an extent, it is not a random sampling approach per se. Particularly regarding the content validity of stressful life events and whether ratings can be extrapolated to the whole population, I was wondering whether it would have been worthwhile to

- oversample certain ethnic and minority groups

- do a more extended DIF analysis in these subgroups to understand further whether both stressful life events and their ratings are truly universal like you seem to imply.

I understand that this comment refers to a different sampling approach than the one you have done. I'd appreciate if you could go into more detail on problems regarding the universality of the ratings, particularly on certain minority ethnic groups. Given that the UK population is multi-cultural (as other societies are), it would pay to think through how to ensure better validity and representation for all groups involved. It would also create further discussions and might give you ideas for further research studies.

(b) I would appreciate some more thoughts on using marriage as the anchor. Why have you done so, given that marriage in our society does not hold the same meaning than it did when the original version of the scale was developed? What does it mean for your ratings that you picked an anchor item with quite a low rating?

As a knock-on effect - what does it mean that the most stressful event is not anchored to a 100% anymore?

I have a personal preference for using item response theory to better understand item difficulty. Might this be an approach you could take to reanalyse the data to better understand the respective item difficulty and the corresponding person abilities in your sample?

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

**********

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2023 Dec 18;18(12):e0295943. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0295943.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


29 Nov 2023

MS # PONE-D-23-17307: Our response to reviewer comments

Dear Editor,

Thank you for the opportunity to re-submit our revised manuscript.

Our submission includes a rebuttal letter and a marked up and unmarked copy of our manuscript. We address specific points below.

We would like to thank the reviewers for their time and thoughtfulness in reviewing our manuscript. The feedback from both reviewers is gratefully received.

To Reviewer 1, thank you for giving our study a clean bill of health. We really appreciate receiving such positive feedback from a reviewer.

To Reviewer 2, thank you for your positive feedback and insightful suggestions for future follow-up studies. We reply to each comment point by point below:

Reviewer 2 Point 1:

(a) I understand that you have essentially done a quota sampling approach. While this can be representative as a sampling approach to an extent, it is not a random sampling approach per se. Particularly regarding the content validity of stressful life events and whether ratings can be extrapolated to the whole population, I was wondering whether it would have been worthwhile to

- oversample certain ethnic and minority groups

- do a more extended DIF analysis in these subgroups to understand further whether both stressful life events and their ratings are truly universal like you seem to imply.

I understand that this comment refers to a different sampling approach than the one you have done. I'd appreciate if you could go into more detail on problems regarding the universality of the ratings, particularly on certain minority ethnic groups. Given that the UK population is multi-cultural (as other societies are), it would pay to think through how to ensure better validity and representation for all groups involved. It would also create further discussions and might give you ideas for further research studies.

Reply to Point 1:

Thank you for raising this very important point regarding representation. The purpose of our study was to update the SRRS which is a population-based measure. Our study was therefore designed to represent (proportionately) the UK’s current population regarding age, gender and ethnicity. Researchers can and do use the SRRS in other contexts and should note the point regarding representation which we discuss in our limitations section. We agree that research questions concerning differences between ethnic, cultural and religious groups would add value to the understanding of life events stress. I have made two amendments in the Limitations section noting that some groups within the UK population represent a relatively small proportion of the UK population and to recommend more in-depth DIF analysis in future work:

[lines 870-871] “…living in severe poverty, additional validation is advised. The same is true regarding ethnic and religious minority groups who represent a small proportion within the UK population.”

[lines 875-877] “…updated weights with non-Western cultures. In addition, future work could include more extended differential item functioning analysis in these subgroups to explore the universality of stressful life events and their ratings.”

Reviewer 2 Point 2:

(b) I would appreciate some more thoughts on using marriage as the anchor. Why have you done so, given that marriage in our society does not hold the same meaning than it did when the original version of the scale was developed? What does it mean for your ratings that you picked an anchor item with quite a low rating?

As a knock-on effect - what does it mean that the most stressful event is not anchored to a 100% anymore?

Reply to Point 2:

It is fundamentally important to provide an update that is compatible with the original version of the SRRS otherwise researchers may not want to use it. For this reason, we used a proportionate scaling approach with marriage as an anchor to align our update with the original Holmes and Rahe (1967) SRRS. Marriage was not a rated item in the original scale, the value was pre-assigned and so, our approach is in line with the original one taken by Holmes and Rahe.

Regarding the knock-on effect, we used marriage as the anchor point. ‘Death of a spouse/life-partner’ while no longer at 100 life change units is still ranked 1st. Its relative position remains unchanged.

Reviewer 2 Point 3:

I have a personal preference for using item response theory to better understand item difficulty. Might this be an approach you could take to reanalyse the data to better understand the respective item difficulty and the corresponding person abilities in your sample?

Reply to Point 3:

The design and purpose of this study was to update and improve the SRRS in a backwards-compatible way. We agree that the issue of item difficulty is a valid concern with survey research. This research question would be important to address in a future study.

Once again, our thanks to both reviewers for their valued contribution to our manuscript.

With kind regards,

Denise Wallace

On behalf of the authors

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response to reviewers_28.11.23.docx

Decision Letter 1

Adetayo Olorunlana

4 Dec 2023

The Social Readjustment Rating Scale: updated and modernised

PONE-D-23-17307R1

Dear Dr. Wallace,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Adetayo Olorunlana, Ph.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #2: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #2: Thank you for providing such a thoughtful reply. All my comments have been addressed. This was a joy to re-review.

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #2: Yes: Christina Ramsenthaler

**********

Acceptance letter

Adetayo Olorunlana

7 Dec 2023

PONE-D-23-17307R1

The Social Readjustment Rating Scale: updated and modernised

Dear Dr. Wallace:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at customercare@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Associate Professor Adetayo Olorunlana

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Checklist. The SRRS updated and modernised: STROBE checklist.

    (DOCX)

    S1 Appendix. SRRQ (updated version).

    (PDF)

    S2 Appendix. Original Instructions for SRRS.

    (PDF)

    S3 Appendix. SRRS updated version.

    (PDF)

    S4 Appendix. Personal experience of SRRS life events.

    (PDF)

    S5 Appendix. Loneliness questionnaire.

    (PDF)

    S6 Appendix. Bayes Factor sensitivity analysis for all Mann-Whitney U comparisons.

    (PDF)

    S7 Appendix. Social Readjustment Rating Scale by category.

    (PDF)

    S8 Appendix. Descriptive statistics for SRRS events by unabridged sub-group demographics.

    (PDF)

    S9 Appendix. Bayesian Kendall’s tau correlation between event ratings and degree to which these were based on personal experience.

    (PDF)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to reviewers_28.11.23.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.


    Articles from PLOS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES