Skip to main content
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America logoLink to Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
. 2012 May 3;109(21):E1344-E1351. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1202426109

Phantom tones and suppressive masking by active nonlinear oscillation of the hair-cell bundle

Jérémie Barral 1, Pascal Martin 1,1
PMCID: PMC3361408  PMID: 22556264

Abstract

Processing of two-tone stimuli by the auditory system introduces prominent masking of one frequency component by the other as well as additional “phantom” tones that are absent in the sound input. Mechanical correlates of these psychophysical phenomena have been observed in sound-evoked mechanical vibrations of the mammalian cochlea and are thought to originate in sensory hair cells from the intrinsic nonlinearity associated with mechano-electrical transduction by ion channels. However, nonlinearity of the transducer is not sufficient to explain the rich phenomenology of two-tone interferences in hearing. Here we show that active oscillatory movements of single hair-cell bundles elicit two-tone suppression and distortions that are shaped by nonlinear amplification of periodic stimuli near the characteristic frequency of spontaneous oscillations. When both stimulus frequencies enter the bandwidth of the hair-bundle amplifier, two-tone interferences display level functions that are characteristic both of human psychoacoustics and of in vivo mechanical measurements in auditory organs. Our work distinguishes the frequency-dependent nonlinearity that emerges from the active process that drives the hair bundle into spontaneous oscillations from the passive nonlinear compliance associated with the direct gating of transduction channels by mechanical force. Numerical simulations based on a generic description of an active dynamical system poised near an oscillatory instability—a Hopf bifurcation—account quantitatively for our experimental observations. In return, we conclude that the properties of two-tone interferences in hearing betray the workings of self-sustained “critical” oscillators, which function as nonlinear amplifying elements in the inner ear.

Keywords: auditory amplification, combination tones, hair-bundle motility, Tartini tones


The human ear does not work as a high-fidelity sound receiver. First, the perceived loudness of a tone diminishes in the presence of a second tone at a nearby frequency, a phenomenon called masking (1). Second, a person listening simultaneously to two pure tones f1 and f2 can hear not only these frequencies but also additional tones that are not present in the acoustic stimulus (2, 3). These “phantom” tones have been recognized as distortion products of the input; they are produced at linear combinations of f1 and f2. Any device evinces nonlinearity when the stimulus becomes too intense to elicit a proportional response. In the inner ear, mechano-electrical transduction by sensory hair cells is usually accepted as the dominant source of two-tone interferences, for the sigmoidal relation between hair-bundle deflections and transduction currents provides a saturating nonlinearity that distorts receptor potentials (4, 5). Because gating of transduction channels is directly coupled to tip-link tension, the hair-cell bundle also acts as a nonlinear spring (6) that produces mechanical distortions of sound-evoked vibrations (7). If the transduction process only displayed a static nonlinearity, for which the output can be expressed as a power-series expansion of the input (see section 1 in SI Text), the distortion would vanish for weak stimuli and become more and more pronounced as the magnitude of stimulation increases. For a two-tone stimulus, the level of distortion relative to that of the primaries at f1 and f2 would in turn grow superlinearly.

The behavior of the auditory nonlinearity is in striking contrast to that expected from a static nonlinearity (3, 8). First, the most prominent “phantom tone” in the auditory percept, which occurs at 2f1 - f2 when the frequency ratio f2/f1 is near 1.2, is detected even at low levels of stimulation. Second, its magnitude increases at about the same rate as the stimulus, so that its relative level remains practically constant at 15–20% over a broad range of sound levels. In addition, both masking and phantom tones display a sharp dependence on the frequency separation between the two components of the stimulus, indicating that the auditory nonlinearity must be coupled to a filter that allows interaction of neighboring frequency components only.

Two-tone suppression and distortions have been observed in vivo on the basilar membrane that supports the organ of Corti of the mammalian cochlea (9, 10). These phenomena appear to be intimately related to the nonlinear, frequency-selective active process that enhances sensitivity, sharpens frequency tuning and compresses six orders of magnitudes of sound-pressure levels into a much narrower range of vibration amplitudes (11). Distorted cochlear vibrations are strong enough to be reemitted in the ear canal as distortion-product oto-acoustic emissions (DPOAEs) that contain the cubic difference tone 2f1 - f2 (12, 13). Notably, the ears of nonmammalian vertebrates produce similar distortion-product oto-acoustic emissions (1416). Like in mammals, this manifestation of nonlinear inner-ear mechanics is associated with high sensitivity and sharp frequency selectivity (17), which are key specifications of active hearing.

Two forms of motility by mechanosensory hair cells have been implicated in the active process (18). The hair cell can power active movements of its mechanosensory hair bundle, including spontaneous oscillations that have been shown in nonmammalian vertebrates to provide nonlinear and frequency-selective amplification of sinusoidal stimuli (1922). In mammals, active hair-bundle motility coexists with membrane-based somatic motility (23, 24), a biological form of piezoelectricity by which the soma of an outer hair cell changes length in response to variations of its membrane potential (25). Although electromotility appears to be necessary for cochlear amplification (26, 27), this process is by itself linear over the physiological range of receptor potentials. It is likely that hair-bundle and somatic motility cooperate to mediate the active process in the mammalian cochlea (2830), with the hair bundle bringing the nonlinearity that is associated with both cochlear amplification and two-tone interferences (31, 32).

In this work, we evaluate the effects of active hair-bundle oscillations on two-tone suppression and distortions by a single hair cell. For stimulation near the characteristic frequency of spontaneous oscillations, we show that a hair bundle produces two-tone interferences with level functions that are characteristic of both human psychoacoustics and in vivo measurements of inner-ear mechanics. We interpret our observations within the general theoretical framework of “critical oscillators,” which are active dynamical systems poised near an oscillatory instability called a Hopf bifurcation.

Results

Two-Tone Suppression.

We took advantage of a two-compartment preparation of the bullfrog’s sacculus (33) in which hair cells routinely power spontaneous oscillations of their hair bundle at a characteristic frequency fC = 5–80 Hz. Using a flexible glass fiber, we applied periodic forces F to the top of a hair bundle. Previous work indicates that oscillatory hair bundles actively resonate with pure tones F(t) = F1 sin(2πf1t) and amplify their responsiveness at the frequency f1 of stimulation (20, 34). Significant amplification happens only when stimulus frequencies fall within a limited bandwidth that is centered at the characteristic frequency of oscillation fC. Accordingly, for small stimulus amplitudes F1 of a test tone, a hair bundle displayed a larger response near its frequency of spontaneous oscillation (f1 ≅ fC; Fig. 1A, black disks) than at a higher frequency (f1 ≅ 10 × fC; Fig. 1B, black disks). Amplification at resonance (f1 ≅ fC) was associated with a compressive nonlinearity that accommodated a large range of stimulus amplitudes (0.5–50 pN) into a narrower range of hair-bundle movements (3–40 nm). In contrast, stimulation off-resonance (f1 ≅ 10 × fC) elicited passive hair-bundle movements that grew almost in proportion to the stimulus, although a small, but noticeable, deviation from linearity was observed for forces near 10 pN (Fig. 1B, black disks).

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1.

Two-tone suppression. (A) The magnitude of the bundle motion’s Fourier component at the frequency f1 = 13 Hz of a test tone is plotted as a function of the amplitude F1 of this component of the two-tone stimulus. The various symbols correspond to different amplitudes F2 (•, 0 pN; △, 2 pN; ⧫, 5 pN; ▿, 10 pN) of the masker tone that was added to the test tone at a nearby frequency f2 = 12 Hz. Stimulation was here delivered at resonance—i.e., near the bundle’s characteristic frequency fC = 11 Hz of spontaneous oscillation. (B) Same as in A but for stimulation off-resonance, with f1 = 130 Hz and f2 = 120 Hz that are both one order of magnitude larger than fC. (C) Using the same data as in A, the response at f2, normalized to its value when F1 = 0, is here plotted as a function of the amplitude F1 for different amplitudes F2. (D) Same as in C but for stimulation at f1 = 130 Hz and f2 = 120 Hz. All data from same cell.

By applying two-tone stimuli F(t) = F1 sin(2πf1t) + F2 sin(2πf2t), we studied how the hair-bundle response to a test tone (frequency f1) was affected by the addition of a second tone (frequency f2) at a nearby frequency. For stimuli at resonance (f1 and f2 near fC), we observed that the response to the test tone was partially suppressed by the additional tone (Fig. 1A). For intense masker levels, the test tone displayed a level function that was nearly linear, resembling that obtained off-resonance in response to the test tone only (Fig. 1B). Amplification of the test tone thus appeared to be turned off by the masker tone. As expected from a complete loss of gain from active hair-bundle motility, the amplitude of stimulation that was required to elicit a response beyond a given threshold was increased by as much as 10-fold. The response at f2 was also suppressed by the stimulus at f1 (Fig. 1C). When the amplitudes of the two tones were dissimilar (e.g., F2/F1 ≅ 10 with F1 = 1 pN in Fig. 1A), the response to the stronger tone was almost unaffected whereas the response to the weaker tone was largely suppressed, decreasing by about 90%. The contrast between the frequency components of the two-tone stimulus was thus enhanced. Remarkably, the phenomenon of two-tone suppression, which was so compelling at resonance, was barely observed when the hair bundle was stimulated off-resonance (Fig. 1B and D).

To further characterize the frequency specificity of two-tone suppression, we measured how the amplified response to a test tone at resonance was affected by an additional tone with the same amplitude but varying frequency (Fig. 2). We used low stimulation levels, for which amplification was maximal (Fig. 1). When the frequency difference between the two tones was large enough, the response to the test tone was the same as when this tone was presented alone. As the frequency mismatch was reduced, we observed that the response to the test tone was increasingly suppressed. This response reached a minimum, and two-tone suppression was thus maximal, when both stimuli had about the same frequency. Correspondingly, the response to the masker tone increased as its frequency approached the characteristic frequency of the hair-bundle oscillator but, as the result of mutual suppression of the two tones, never rose to the maximal amplified value that would have been reached, had this tone been presented alone.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2.

Frequency specificity of two-tone suppression. The two components of the stimulus had here the same amplitude F1 = F2 = 2 pN. The frequency f1 = 24 Hz of the test tone was fixed near the bundle’s characteristic frequency of oscillation fC = 23 Hz, while the frequency f2 of the masker tone was varied. The magnitude of bundle motion, at frequencies f1 (●) and f2 (○), is plotted as a function of the frequency separation Δf = f2 - f1. The dashed line represents the response level when the test tone was presented alone.

Two-Tone Distortions.

A two-tone stimulus constructed from the sum of two sinusoids with equal amplitudes Inline graphic and nearby frequencies produces a beating pattern. Although this pattern dominated the waveform of hair-bundle movement, filtering the primary frequencies of motion at f1 and f2 revealed that the hair bundle distorted its response to the stimulus (Fig. 3). The distortion was significantly larger in magnitude for stimulation at resonance than off-resonance and its waveform was qualitatively different in the two cases. The spectral density of hair-bundle motion disclosed the presence of discrete frequency components that were not present in the stimulus. In particular, for moderate to intense stimuli (Inline graphic), the hair bundle produced a series of odd distortion products at frequencies f1 - kΔf and f2 + kΔf, where k is a positive integer (k < 6) and Δf = f2 - f1. Distortion products of increasing order k displayed a hierarchy of magnitude Xk that was well described by an exponential decay Xk+1/Xk = exp(-λ), where λ is a fit parameter.

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3.

Two-tone distortions. Hair-bundle response to the sum Inline graphic of two sinusoidal forces with equal amplitudes Inline graphic (2 pN, A and C; 10.1 pN, B and D). Two-tone stimulation was delivered at resonance (A and B), with f1 = 20 Hz and f2 = 22 Hz near the bundle’s characteristic frequency of spontaneous oscillations fC = 20 Hz, or off-resonance (C and D; f1 = 200 Hz and f2 = 220 Hz). The hair-bundle motion displayed a periodic beating pattern of magnitude; the ensemble average of 40 (A and B) or 400 (C and D) beats is plotted here (black line). The spectral density of bundle movement is shown on the side of each temporal trace for 20 s of recording. The frequencies of the distortion products are labeled in red. Keeping only the Fourier components f1 and f2 of the primaries resulted in a beating pattern of motion with an envelope (blue line) that is superimposed on the bundle’s global response. Conversely, filtering the primaries disclosed, in the temporal domain, the waveform of distortion (red line) that the hair bundle introduced into its response to the stimulus.

At resonance (f1 and f2 near fC), a condition that maximizes amplification by active hair-bundle motility (34), stimulation of increasing magnitude enriched the spectrum of hair-bundle movements almost exclusively near the primaries (Fig. 3 A and B). The most prominent components of distortion were the cubic difference products at frequencies 2f1 - f2 and 2f2 - f1. These products, which had nearly the same magnitude, could be as much as fourfold more intense than the quadratic summation tone f1 + f2 for weak stimuli, but cubic and quadratic products reached similar levels for intense stimuli (Fig. 4A). In addition, the cubic products appeared at low level of stimulation (Inline graphic), showed a linear growth for weak stimuli and a compressive growth for intermediate stimuli (Fig. 4B) that was well described by a power law Inline graphic with α = 0.26 ± 0.08 (mean ± SD, n = 11). This level function resembled that of the primaries (α = 0.41 ± 0.07, n = 11) or of the response to a pure tone (α = 0.38 ± 0.07, n = 43). Correspondingly, the level of the cubic difference products relative to that of the primaries showed little variation over the range of stimulus amplitudes that we explored, remaining approximately constant at 10–20% (Fig. 4C).

Fig. 4.

Fig. 4.

Level function of cubic difference products. (A) The level of the cubic-difference products at 2f1 - f2 and 2f2 - f1, for which we computed the arithmetic mean, is here compared to that of the quadratic-summation product f1 + f2. The ratio of these two quantities is plotted as a function of the magnitude Inline graphic of the external force for stimulation at resonance (black) and off-resonance (red). (B) Arithmetic-mean level of the two cubic difference products 2f1 - f2 and 2f2 - f1 (filled circles) and of the two primaries f1 and f2 (open circles) at resonance (black) and off-resonance (red) as a function of the stimulus amplitude Inline graphic. Dashed horizontal lines mark noise-floor levels when distortion products first appeared. Data in A and B from the same dataset as that used to plot Fig. 3. (C) For n = 11 cells with f2/f1 = 1.08 - 1.15, the arithmetic-mean level of the two cubic difference products was divided by that of the primaries at resonance (black) and off-resonance (red). This relative level is plotted as a function of the stimulus amplitude Inline graphic. The continuous lines correspond to a moving average over a window comprising seven data points. The shaded areas represent 95% confidence intervals.

These properties are in stark contrast with those elicited by off-resonance stimuli (f1 and f2 near 10 × fC), which probe passive hair-bundle mechanics (34). In this case, the stimulus elicited distortion products that were uniformly distributed across the spectrum (Fig. 3 C and D). In particular, harmonic distortions were clearly visible as well as even distortion products at f1 + f2 ± mΔf (m < 3). The quadratic summation product f1 + f2 often emerged first (Fig. 3C). The cubic difference products at 2f1 - f2 and 2f2 - f1 surfaced from the noise floor for relatively intense stimuli (Inline graphic). These cubic products grew more rapidly than the quadratic summation product, resulting in a relative level that increased (Fig. 4A). For Inline graphic, the relative levels of distortion at and off-resonance were similar, suggesting that two-tone distortions no longer depended on the frequency of stimulation. The cubic products showed a steep increase with the stimulus amplitude that followed a power law Inline graphic (α = 2.74 ± 0.34, n = 11), until saturation afforded no further increase of distortion magnitude (Fig. 4 B and C). At any given stimulation level, these products were fainter than those observed at resonance, with a difference that was more pronounced for smaller stimuli. Notably, in four of the 11 cells that we analyzed, the level function was nonmonotonic and displayed a notch.

We then measured how distortion products depended on the frequency separation between the two components of the stimulus. When a test tone was applied at resonance, the addition of a second tone with the same amplitude produced cubic difference products with a magnitude that displayed a maximum when the frequency mismatch approached zero (Fig. 5A). The bell-shaped behavior mirrored that observed for two-tone suppression (Fig. 2, black disks), indicating a reciprocal relation between the two nonlinear phenomena. In a second set of experiments, we kept Inline graphic constant. For stimulation about resonance (Inline graphic), the magnitude of the cubic difference products decreased by 50% when f2/f1 was raised from 1.05 to 6 (Fig. 5B). In addition, higher-order odd products near the primaries became much less prominent: The parameter λ, which characterizes the exponential decay rate of the relation between a product’s magnitude Xk and its order k, increased from 0.4 to 1.2 (Fig. 5C). Correspondingly, the relative magnitude Xk+1/Xk decreased from 67% to 30%. Off-resonance (Inline graphic), the cubic difference products as well as higher-order products were instead insensitive to variations of the frequency separation between the two components of the stimulus (Fig. 5 B and C).

Fig. 5.

Fig. 5.

Frequency specificity of odd distortion products near the primaries. (A) Arithmetic-mean level of the two cubic difference products 2f1 - f2 and 2f2 - f1 as a function of the frequency separation Δf = f2 - f1. Same cell and stimulus series as in Fig. 2. (B) Level of the cubic difference products as a function of the frequency ratio f2/f1. We varied f1 and f2 in a way that kept Inline graphic constant and stimulated the hair bundle about resonance (black; Inline graphic), or off-resonance (red; Inline graphic). (C) In this semilogarithmic plot, the parameter λ that characterizes the exponential hierarchy of distortion products is represented as a function of the relative frequency separation Inline graphic, where Δf = f2 - f1. The continuous lines result from moving averages over a window of five data points obtained from five hair bundles; 95% confidence intervals are shown as shaded areas. Black and red plots are for stimulation about resonance (Inline graphic) and off-resonance (Inline graphic), respectively. In B and C, the stimulus amplitude was Inline graphic.

The endolymphatic Ca2+ concentration acts as a control parameter of active hair-bundle motility (33, 35, 36). Because raising this concentration in the millimolar range brings spontaneous oscillations to a halt, we could compare, with the same hair bundle, two-tone distortions in oscillatory and quiescent regimes of hair-bundle mechanics (Fig. 6). In agreement with a previous study (7), we found that a quiescent hair bundle still produced distortion products. However, the cubic difference products were much weaker than in the oscillatory regime and the spectrum of distortion was in this case dominated by quadratic products (Fig. 6 A and B). In addition, the level function of cubic products had lost frequency specificity and displayed a superlinear growth (Fig. 6C). This behavior was similar to that observed off-resonance with oscillatory hair bundles.

Fig. 6.

Fig. 6.

Effects of Ca2+ on cubic difference products. This hair bundle was subjected to the sum of two sinusoidal forces with equal amplitudes Inline graphic. (A) At an endolymphatic Ca2+ concentration of 250 μM, the hair bundle displayed spontaneous oscillations at a characteristic frequency fC = 19 Hz. The spectral density of hair-bundle movement is plotted for stimulation at resonance (f1 = 18 Hz and f2 = 19 Hz) with a stimulus amplitude Inline graphic. (B) When the Ca2+ concentration was raised to a value of 1 mM, the hair bundle became quiescent. The spectral density of motion resulted from the same stimulus as in A. (C) The arithmetic-mean level of the two cubic difference products is plotted as a function of the stimulus amplitude Inline graphic for f1 = 18 Hz and f2 = 19 Hz (black) and f1 = 180 Hz and f2 = 190 Hz (red). Close and open symbols correspond to oscillatory and quiescent conditions, respectively. Note the notch in the level function depicted by red disks. Data in AC from the same cell.

Discussion

Static and Passive Nonlinearity from Gating Compliance.

What is the mechanical nonlinearity that underlies the production of two-tone interferences? The nonlinear compliance that ensues from direct gating of the transduction channels by tip-link tension is a well-recognized source of mechanical distortion by the hair bundle (7). Gating compliance is clearly sufficient to explain two-tone suppression and distortions under ionic conditions for which the hair bundle does not oscillate, or for off-resonance stimulation of an oscillatory hair bundle (see section 2 in SI Text and Figs. S1 and S2). In particular, this nonlinearity produces distortion products at 2f1 - f2 and 2f2 - f1 that grow superlinearly, until saturation of mechano-electrical transduction affords no further increase of their magnitude. Simulations also indicate that when gating compliance is strong enough to elicit negative stiffness, a condition associated with the production of spontaneous hair-bundle oscillations (37), the level function of these cubic products displays a pronounced notch. Interestingly, in the example shown in Fig. 6C, the notch that was apparent for stimulation off resonance in the regime of spontaneous oscillation was lost in the quiescent regime, suggesting that the property of negative stiffness had vanished at higher Ca2+ concentrations.

Near the characteristic frequency of spontaneous hair-bundle oscillations, two-tone interferences acquire three main features that, together, are inconsistent with static nonlinearities such as that provided by gating compliance. First, the cubic difference products, as well as the other odd distortion products near the primaries, appear at lower levels of stimulation and become prominent in the spectral density of hair-bundle movements (Figs. 3 and 4). Second, both cubic difference products and primaries show a compressive growth over a large range of stimulus magnitudes, yielding a relative level that is nearly constant (Fig. 4). Finally, the level of these cubic products (Figs. 4 and 5), the exponential hierarchy of odd distortion products near the primaries (Fig. 5C), as well as the phenomenon of two-tone suppression (Figs. 1 and 2) display pronounced frequency specificity.

Dynamic and Active Nonlinearity from Critical Oscillation.

Clearly, the oscillatory activity of the hair bundle harnesses the intrinsic nonlinearity associated with mechano-electrical transduction to produce a dynamic nonlinearity with new properties. This makes sense because active hair-bundle motility introduces an extended compressive nonlinearity of responsiveness for frequency components of the stimulus that fall near the characteristic frequency of oscillation (20) (Fig. 1 A and B, black disks). This behavior is characteristic of “critical” oscillators, which are active dynamical systems that operate in the vicinity of a Hopf bifurcation (3841). A Hopf bifurcation corresponds to an oscillatory instability by which the system changes abruptly from quiescence to a regime of spontaneous oscillations as a control parameter is varied continuously (42). At the characteristic frequency of the instability, an energy-transducing mechanism cancels the impedance of the system to sinusoidal stimuli. The system’s response is in turn amplified and under the control of an essential nonlinearity, for which there is no stimulus weak enough to evoke a linear response. Evidently, an active system of this sort should produce conspicuous two-tone interferences for stimulation near the characteristic frequency of the oscillator (43, 44). Simulations (Fig. 7) that are based on a generic description of a critical oscillator (Eq. 1; see Methods) demonstrate that these interferences are in qualitative agreement with those elicited by an oscillatory hair bundle (Fig. 1, 2, 4 and 5). In particular, the frequency specificity of two-tone suppression and distortions as well as the parallel compressive growth of the cubic difference products and of the primaries for stimulation at resonance emerge as properties that should be shared by all dynamical systems that operate near a Hopf bifurcation.

Fig. 7.

Fig. 7.

Simulations of two-tone interferences by a critical oscillator. (A) Two-tone suppression. The response at the frequency f1 = 20 Hz (black) or f1 = 200 Hz (red) of a test tone is plotted as a function of the amplitude F1 of this component of the two-tone stimulus for various amplitudes F2 (•, 0 pN; △, 1 pN; ⧫, 5 pN; ▿, 25 pN) of a masker tone at f2 = 22 Hz (black) or f2 = 220 Hz (red). The oscillator displayed a characteristic frequency of spontaneous oscillation fC = 20.9 Hz. (B) Here F1 = F2 = 0.2 pN, the test-tone frequency f1 = 21 Hz ≅ fC was fixed while the masker-tone frequency f2 was varied. The responses at frequencies f1 (black disks) and f2 (white disks), as well as the arithmetic-mean level of the cubic difference products at frequencies 2f1 - f2 and 2f2 - f1 (red disks), are plotted as a function of the frequency separation Δf = f2 - f1. (C) Two-tone distortions. The arithmetic-mean level of the cubic difference products is plotted as a function of the stimulus amplitude Inline graphic at resonance (black; f1 = 20 Hz and f2 = 22 Hz) and off-resonance (red; f1 = 200 Hz and f2 = 220 Hz). (D) The arithmetic-mean level of the two cubic difference products was divided by that of the primaries and plotted as a function of the stimulus amplitude Inline graphic. The system was deterministic, except in C and D (open symbols) where the oscillator was subjected to random noise. Thin blue lines represent power laws (power indicated next to the line). Parameter values: μ = 0, b = 1017 m-2·s-1, Λ = 1·10-7 N·s·m-1.

The theoretical framework provided by the generic behavior of a critical oscillator explains why the cubic difference tones dominate the spectrum of distortion products. If both components of a two-tone stimulus approximate the characteristic frequency of the oscillator, a condition that requires a small frequency separation, so will the cubic difference tones. Because they enter the active bandwidth of the oscillator, these distortion products are amplified and in turn contribute significantly to the hair-bundle motion. Moreover, the amplified response to a test tone by the active oscillator is readily extinguished by adding a masker tone at a nearby frequency, because the masker tone increases the impedance to the test tone by effectively detuning the oscillator away from the bifurcation point (44).

Some features of the experimental recordings, however, are not reproduced by the simulations of a critical oscillator. These include the notch that was observed in the level function of distortion products at 2f1 - f2 and 2f2 - f1 for stimulation off-resonance (Fig. 6C), as well as the saturation that was measured in response to intense stimuli both for the suppression of the response to a test tone by a masker tone (Fig. 1A) and in the level functions of distortion products (Fig. 4). These two properties can be interpreted as consequences of negative hair-bundle stiffness and of the saturation of mechano-electrical transduction, respectively (Figs. S1 and S2). Off-resonance or intense stimuli thus provide specific information about passive hair-bundle mechanics. In addition, active oscillations by a single hair bundle are noisy. By limiting the phase coherence of active force production, intrinsic fluctuations conceal the bifurcation at which a deterministic dynamical system might operate and restore a linear regime of responsiveness to weak stimuli (45, 46). In contrast to a truly critical oscillator, which can distort arbitrarily small inputs for stimulation at resonance, a noisy system thus evinces nonlinearities only for stimuli with magnitudes beyond some threshold under the control of the noise strength (Fig. 7 C and D). If noise is weak enough, however, the signatures of critical oscillation remain (46). Notably, when the noise strength matches that expected for a hair bundle (see Methods), cubic difference products by a critical oscillator can arise at stimulus magnitudes that accord with those observed experimentally (Fig. 7 C and D, open disks).

Active and Passive Distortions in Vivo.

The unusual properties of the phantom tones that are heard in the human percept in response to two-tone stimulation had early been recognized as indirect evidence for the existence of an essential nonlinearity in hearing (3, 8). Yet, the physical origin of this nonlinearity remained elusive. The compressive and frequency-specific nonlinearity evinced by an oscillatory hair bundle (Fig. 1 A and B, black disks), or more generally by any self-sustained oscillator operating near a Hopf bifurcation (47), is ideally suited to account for the auditory nonlinearity. In response to two-tone stimuli of increasing magnitudes, this dynamic nonlinearity produces distorted vibrations at 2f1 - f2 that appear at low levels and grow nearly in parallel with vibrations at the primary frequencies f1 and f2 (Figs. 4 and 7). In agreement with psychoacoustical studies (3), the subjective intensity of the corresponding phantom tone would thus be expected to remain practically proportional to that of the stimulus over the extended range of stimulation for which the oscillator’s response remains compressive. In addition, because this compressive nonlinearity holds only near the condition of resonance (f1 and f2 near fC), two-tone interferences by the active oscillator are inherently dependent on the frequency separation between the primary frequencies.

Signatures of critical oscillators can also be found in vivo on the basilar membrane of the mammalian cochlea. First, for two-tone stimuli at nearby frequencies, the cubic difference product 2f1 - f2 appears at its characteristic place for stimulation levels as low as 30 dB and displays a compressive growth that parallels that observed in response to a pure tone at the same frequency over a 50-dB variation of sound-pressure levels (10). Assuming that the pure tone response provides a fair estimate for the level function of the primaries, this behavior conforms to that shown in Figs. 4 and 7. Second, the cubic-difference product displays a power-law relation to the stimulus level that is similar to that anticipated for an active oscillator near a Hopf bifurcation (Fig. 7). Third, moderate to intense sound stimuli produce an exponential hierarchy of distortion products, as measured with a single hair bundle (Fig. 3) and expected for a critical oscillator (44). Finally, these properties strongly depend on the frequency separation between the primary frequencies.

Distortion-product oto-acoustic emissions afford another noninvasive means to assay the properties of the mechanical nonlinearity that underlies two-tone distortions in vivo. The spectrum of DPOAEs in the bobtail lizard shows prominent cubic difference tones in live animals, whereas passive mechanics instead favors quadratic products postmortem (16). This change in spectral content agrees with that reported here for a single oscillatory hair bundle for stimulation at and off resonance, respectively (Fig. 3), and in response to an increase in Ca2+ concentration that stops spontaneous oscillations (Fig. 6). In mammals, furosemide or anoxia, which both depress cochlear amplification, evoke a progressive loss of the cubic difference tones at low levels of stimulation and a transition from a compressive to a superlinear level function for these distortion products (48). Similarly, prestin knockout mice lacking electromotility still display DPOAEs, but the cubic difference tone at 2f1 - f2 appears at higher sound levels than in wild-type animals and displays a superlinear level function (49) that is characteristic of passive nonlinear mechanics. The qualitative resemblance between the two-tone distortions that are measured in vivo as oto-acoustic emissions in the ear canal and those evoked in vitro by a single oscillatory hair bundle is remarkable considering that the former are not evaluated at the site of production but after propagation through the hearing organ.

Integration of the Hair-Bundle Oscillator in Hearing Organs.

We have demonstrated here that a single oscillatory hair bundle qualitatively recapitulates the most salient features of phantom tones and suppressive masking in the field of psychoacoustics as well as of two-tone distortions and suppression that characterize active inner-ear mechanics. Being observed over a relatively narrow range of stimulus amplitudes, two-tone interferences from active oscillations of a single hair bundle are significantly weaker than those observed in intact organs. Understandably, intrinsic noise limits the nonlinear amplification that a single hair bundle can achieve for weak stimuli (45, 46). Mechanical coupling of noisy hair-bundle oscillators by overlying membranous structures, however, has been shown to enlarge the compressive nonlinearity produced by the hair-bundle amplifier (50, 51). An oscillatory module comprising a few tens of hair cells is predicted to produce similar amplificatory gains as those measured in vivo in the mammalian cochlea and in turn to elicit two-tone suppression and distortions over a much wider range of stimulus amplitudes than that reported here with a single hair bundle.

Our results do not pinpoint active hair-bundle motility as the only player in the active process that both boosts the ear’s technical specifications as a sound detector and fosters nonlinear interference in hearing. In the mammalian cochlea, a complex interplay between active hair-bundle and somatic motility of outer hair cells with the micromechanical environment may bring the cochlear partition on the brink of a Hopf bifurcation (32, 47). Any critical oscillator must show the same generic behavior as that observed here experimentally with a single oscillatory hair bundle. In return, we argue that the properties of two-tone interferences in hearing betray the workings of active mechanical oscillators that provide nonlinear amplification of sound-evoked vibrations in the inner ear.

Methods

Experimental Preparation.

Details of the experimental procedure have been published elsewhere (35). An excised preparation of the bullfrog’s (Rana catesbeiana) saccule was mounted on a two-compartment chamber. The basal bodies of hair cells were bathed in standard saline containing (in mM): 110 NaCl, 2 KCl, 4 CaCl2, 3 D-glucose, 2 Na2-creatine phosphate, 2 Na-pyruvate and 5 Na-HEPES. Hair bundles instead projected into an artificial endolymph of composition (in mM): 2 NaCl, 118 KCl, 0.25 CaCl2, 3 D-glucose and 5 Na-HEPES. To disconnect the hair bundles from the overlying otolithic membrane, the apical surface of the preparation was exposed for 20 min to endolymph supplemented with 67 mg·mL-1 of the protease subtilisin (type XXIV, Sigma). The otolithic membrane was then peeled off to obtain access to individual hair bundles.

Microscopic Apparatus and Mechanical Stimulation.

The preparation was viewed through a × 60 water-immersion objective of an upright microscope (BX51WI, Olympus). The tip of a stimulus fiber attached to the hair bundle was imaged at a magnification of × 1,000 onto a displacement monitor that included a dual photodiode. For movements that did not exceed ± 150 nm, this photometric system reported the fiber motion with no detectable distortion and was thus linear. Calibration was performed by measuring the output voltages of the photometric system in response to a series of offset displacements.

Flexible glass fibers were fabricated from borosilicate capillaries and coated with a thin layer of gold-palladium to enhance contrast. The stiffness KF and drag coefficient λF of the fibers were, respectively, 0.1–0.15 pN·nm-1 and 40–200 nN·s·m-1, as determined by power spectral analysis of Brownian motion of the fiber’s tip in water. The fiber was secured by its base to a stack type piezoelectric actuator (PA-4/12, Piezosystem Jena) driven by a custom-made power supply (Elbatech). The fiber’s tip was affixed to the kinociliary bulb of an individual hair bundle. Neglecting viscous components, the force FHB = KF·(Δ - X) exerted by the stimulus fiber at the bundle’s top was proportional to the deflection of the fiber, in which X and Δ are the positions of the hair bundle and of the fiber’s base, respectively.

We define here the stimulus as F = KF·Δ = FHB + KF·X, which corresponds to the force effectively applied to the top of the hair bundle when this organelle is loaded by a fiber. The mechanical system under study is therefore composed of the parallel arrangement of the hair bundle with the elastic fiber; this system is slightly stiffer than the hair bundle alone. Using flexible fibers ensured that spontaneous hair-bundle oscillations were only weakly disturbed by the elastic load (33).

To study two-tone interferences, the hair bundle was subjected to the time-dependent sum F(t) = F1 sin(2πf1t) + F2 sin(2πf2t) of two sinusoidal forces at frequencies f1 and f2, with a ratio f2/f1 = 1.08–1.15, unless explicitly stated (Figs. 2 and 5). Here, F1 and F2 are the amplitudes of the two tones. To make sure that the frequency components of the stimulus, as well as their linear combinations, were precisely represented by a single data point in the Fourier domain, we used only stimulus frequencies and recording durations that were integer numbers of hertz and seconds, respectively. A typical recording lasted 10–30 s.

Signal Generation, Acquisition.

All signals were generated and acquired under the control of a computer running a user interface programmed with LabVIEW software (version 8.6; National Instruments). The command signal controlling the external force F applied to the hair bundle was produced by a 16-bit interface card at a sampling rate of 2.5 kHz (PCI-6733, National Instruments). A second interface card (PCI-6250, National Instruments) conducted signal acquisition with a precision of 16 bits and a sampling rate of 2.5 kHz. Signals coming from the displacement monitor or going to the stimulation apparatus were conditioned with an eight-pole Bessel antialiasing filter adjusted to a low-pass half-power frequency of 1 kHz.

Data Analysis.

We defined the characteristic frequency fC of spontaneous oscillations as the peak frequency of the bundle’s power spectrum of spontaneous motion or of the bundle’s frequency-dependent linear response function to small (1–2 pN) sinusoidal forces (34).

In response to two-tone stimulation, the hair bundle elicited distortion products at linear combinations nf1 + mf2 of the stimulus frequencies f1 and f2, where n and m are integers. The amplitude Xnf1+mf2 of the bundle’s motion Fourier component at frequency nf1 + mf2 was calculated as Inline graphic, where Inline graphic is the Fourier component at frequency f of the time-varying hair-bundle motion X(t) and Texp is the duration of the recording. The minimal distortion amplitude that could be measured was limited by the noise-floor level at the frequency of the distortion product. This noise level was estimated by computing the average power of bundle motion in a frequency window of 2·(f2 - f1) centered at the frequency of the distortion product, with a stimulus magnitude just below the threshold value for which a clear peak of distortion emerged in the power spectrum. Because the cubic difference products 2f1 - f2 and 2f2 - f1 were not significantly different, we considered only their arithmetic-mean level (X2f1-f2 + X2f2-f1)/2. The amplitude Xnf1+mf2 of each product was averaged over 4–6 realizations of the stimulation procedure. Error bars in Figs. 2 and 46 represent standard errors to the mean value.

At intermediate-to-large stimulus intensities, a hierarchy of odd distortion products was observed on either side of the primaries at frequencies f1 - kΔf and f2 + kΔf, respectively, where k is a positive integer. For each side, the decrease in distortion amplitude with increasing order k was described by an exponential decay Xk = X0· exp(-λ·k), where X0 and λ are fit parameters. In the main text, we consider the arithmetic mean of the two decay coefficients λ that resulted from this fitting procedure.

Simulations of Two-Tone Interferences by a Critical Oscillator.

An active dynamical system undergoes a Hopf bifurcation when its behavior exhibits an abrupt change from quiescence to spontaneous oscillations upon continuous variation of a parameter μ describing some component of the system (42). When this control parameter is poised near the critical value μ = μC where spontaneous oscillations just emerge, the system is called a critical oscillator. The generic behavior of a critical oscillator is described by a dynamic equation of a complex variable Z:

graphic file with name pnas.1202426109eq36.jpg [1]

where Inline graphic is the time derivative of Z, fC is the characteristic frequency of the oscillator, b is the magnitude of the cubic nonlinear term that stabilizes the amplitude of spontaneous oscillations when μ < 0, and i2 = -1. In simulations, we took μC = 0 and b was a real number of positive sign. The real part of Z represents the hair-bundle displacement X(t), F(t) = F1 sin(2πf1t) + F2 sin(2πf2t) describes the two-tone stimulus applied to the system, and Λ is a friction coefficient. In some cases, a complex noise term η(t) = η1(t) + 2(t) was added to account for fluctuations. We considered Gaussian white noise with zero mean Inline graphic and with a strength characterized by correlations Inline graphic, where δ(t) is the Dirac distribution and δij denotes the Kronecker delta (i = 1, 2). We chose Λ = 1·10-7 N·s·m-1 to accord with measured values of a hair bundle’s friction coefficient (52). Note that by imposing the value of the parameter Λ, we also set the magnitude of the noise terms ηi(t). The value b = 1017 m-2·s-1 ensured that the oscillator entered the nonlinear regime of responsiveness to sinusoidal stimuli at a vibration magnitude that agreed with those measured experimentally with oscillatory hair bundles, typically 10 nm (20). The two coupled first-order differential equations describing the time evolution of the real and imaginary part of Z were integrated by the Euler method.

Supplementary Material

Supporting Information

Acknowledgments.

We thank Frank Jülicher for discussions, Geoffrey Manley for his comments on an early version of the manuscript, and Benoît Lemaire and Rémy Fert from the machine shop of the Curie Institute. This work was supported by the Laboratoire Européen Associé “Active Cellular Structures” of the Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique. J.B. is an alumnus of the Frontiers in Life Science PhD program of the University Paris Diderot and thanks the Pierre-Gilles de Gennes Fondation for a doctoral fellowship.

Footnotes

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

*This Direct Submission article had a prearranged editor.

See Author Summary on page 7959 (volume 109, number 21).

This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1202426109/-/DCSupplemental.

References

  • 1.Moore BCJ. An Introduction to the Psychology of Hearing. London: Elsevier; 2004. pp. 65–126. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Tartini G. Trattato di musica secondo la vera scieza dell’armonia. Padova: Kessinger Publishing; 1754. Treatise on music according to the true science of harmony. Italian. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Goldstein JL. Auditory nonlinearity. J Acoust Soc Am. 1967;41:676–689. doi: 10.1121/1.1910396. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Patuzzi RB, Yates GK, Johnstone BM. Outer hair cell receptor current and sensorineural hearing loss. Hear Res. 1989;42:47–72. doi: 10.1016/0378-5955(89)90117-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Lukashkin AN, Russell IJ. Analysis of the f2-f1 and 2f1-f2 distortion components generated by the hair cell mechanoelectrical transducer: Dependence on the amplitudes of the primaries and feedback gain. J Acoust Soc Am. 1999;106:2661–2668. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Howard J, Hudspeth AJ. Compliance of the hair bundle associated with gating of mechanoelectrical transduction channels in the bullfrog’s saccular hair cell. Neuron. 1988;1:189–199. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(88)90139-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Jaramillo F, Markin VS, Hudspeth AJ. Auditory illusions and the single hair cell. Nature. 1993;364:527–529. doi: 10.1038/364527a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Smoorenburg GF. Combination tones and their origin. J Acoust Soc Am. 1972;52:615–632. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Ruggero MA, Robles L, Rich NC. Two-tone suppression in the basilar membrane of the cochlea: Mechanical basis of auditory-nerve rate suppression. J Neurophysiol. 1992;68:1087–1099. doi: 10.1152/jn.1992.68.4.1087. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Robles L, Ruggero MA, Rich NC. Two-tone distortion on the basilar membrane of the chinchilla cochlea. J Neurophysiol. 1997;77:2385–2399. doi: 10.1152/jn.1997.77.5.2385. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Robles L, Ruggero MA. Mechanics of the mammalian cochlea. Physiol Rev. 2001;81:1305–1352. doi: 10.1152/physrev.2001.81.3.1305. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Kim DO, Molnar CE, Matthews JW. Cochlear mechanics: Nonlinear behavior in two-tone responses as reflected in cochlear-nerve-fiber responses and in ear-canal sound pressure. J Acoust Soc Am. 1980;67:1704–1721. doi: 10.1121/1.384297. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Probst R, Lonsbury-Martin BL, Martin GK. A review of otoacoustic emissions. J Acoust Soc Am. 1991;89:2027–2067. doi: 10.1121/1.400897. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Meenderink SW, van Dijk P. Level dependence of distortion product otoacoustic emissions in the leopard frog, Rana pipiens pipiens. Hear Res. 2004;192:107–118. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2004.01.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Vassilakis PN, Meenderink SW, Narins PM. Distortion product otoacoustic emissions provide clues hearing mechanisms in the frog ear. J Acoust Soc Am. 2004;116:3713–3726. doi: 10.1121/1.1811571. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Manley GA, Köppl C, Johnstone BM. Distortion-product otoacoustic emissions in the bobtail lizard. I: General characteristics. J Acoust Soc Am. 1993;93:2820–2833. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Manley GA. Evidence for an active process and a cochlear amplifier in nonmammals. J Neurophysiol. 2001;86:541–549. doi: 10.1152/jn.2001.86.2.541. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Hudspeth AJ. Making an effort to listen: Mechanical amplification in the ear. Neuron. 2008;59:530–545. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.07.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Martin P, Hudspeth AJ. Active hair-bundle movements can amplify a hair cell’s response to oscillatory mechanical stimuli. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1999;96:14306–14311. doi: 10.1073/pnas.96.25.14306. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Martin P, Hudspeth AJ. Compressive nonlinearity in the hair bundle’s active response to mechanical stimulation. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2001;98:14386–14391. doi: 10.1073/pnas.251530498. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Martin P. In: Active Processes and Otoacoustic Emissions in Hearing. Manley GA, Popper AN, Fay RR, editors. New York: Springer; 2008. pp. 93–143. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Manley GA, Kirk DL, Koppl C, Yates GK. In vivo evidence for a cochlear amplifier in the hair-cell bundle of lizards. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2001;98:2826–2831. doi: 10.1073/pnas.041604998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Kennedy HJ, Crawford AC, Fettiplace R. Force generation by mammalian hair bundles supports a role in cochlear amplification. Nature. 2005;433:880–883. doi: 10.1038/nature03367. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Chan DK, Hudspeth AJ. Ca2+ current-driven nonlinear amplification by the mammalian cochlea in vitro. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8:149–155. doi: 10.1038/nn1385. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Ashmore J. Cochlear outer hair cell motility. Physiol Rev. 2008;88:173–210. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00044.2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Dallos P, et al. Prestin-based outer hair cell motility is necessary for mammalian cochlear amplification. Neuron. 2008;58:333–339. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.02.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Mellado Lagarde MM, Drexl M, Lukashkina VA, Lukashkin AN, Russell IJ. Outer hair cell somatic, not hair bundle, motility is the basis of the cochlear amplifier. Nat Neurosci. 2008;11:746–748. doi: 10.1038/nn.2129. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Peng AW, Ricci AJ. Somatic motility and hair bundle mechanics, are both necessary for cochlear amplification? Hear Res. 2011;273:109–122. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2010.03.094. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Barral J, Martin P. The physical basis of active mechanosensitivity by the hair-cell bundle. Curr Opin Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg. 2011;19:369–375. doi: 10.1097/MOO.0b013e32834a8c33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Ashmore J, et al. The remarkable cochlear amplifier. Hear Res. 2010;266:1–17. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2010.05.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Nobili R, Mammano F. Biophysics of the cochlea. II: Stationary nonlinear phenomenology. J Acoust Soc Am. 1996;99:2244–2255. doi: 10.1121/1.415412. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.O’ Maoileidigh D, Jülicher F. The interplay between active hair bundle motility and electromotility in the cochlea. J Acoust Soc Am. 2010;128:1175–1190. doi: 10.1121/1.3463804. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Martin P, Bozovic D, Choe Y, Hudspeth AJ. Spontaneous oscillation by hair bundles of the bullfrog’s sacculus. J Neurosci. 2003;23:4533–4548. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-11-04533.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Martin P, Hudspeth AJ, Jülicher F. Comparison of a hair bundle’s spontaneous oscillations with its response to mechanical stimulation reveals the underlying active process. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2001;98:14380–14385. doi: 10.1073/pnas.251530598. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Tinevez JY, Jülicher F, Martin P. Unifying the various incarnations of active hair-bundle motility by the vertebrate hair cell. Biophys J. 2007;93:4053–4067. doi: 10.1529/biophysj.107.108498. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Manley GA, Sienknecht U, Koppl C. Calcium modulates the frequency and amplitude of spontaneous otoacoustic emissions in the bobtail skink. J Neurophysiol. 2004;92:2685–2693. doi: 10.1152/jn.00267.2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Martin P, Mehta AD, Hudspeth AJ. Negative hair-bundle stiffness betrays a mechanism for mechanical amplification by the hair cell. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2000;97:12026–12031. doi: 10.1073/pnas.210389497. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Eguíluz VM, Ospeck M, Choe Y, Hudspeth AJ, Magnasco MO. Essential nonlinearities in hearing. Phys Rev Lett. 2000;84:5232–5235. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.84.5232. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Choe Y, Magnasco MO, Hudspeth AJ. A model for amplification of hair-bundle motion by cyclical binding of Ca2+ to mechanoelectrical-transduction channels. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1998;95:15321–15326. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.26.15321. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Camalet S, Duke T, Jülicher F, Prost J. Auditory sensitivity provided by self-tuned critical oscillations of hair cells. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2000;97:3183–3188. doi: 10.1073/pnas.97.7.3183. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Duke T, Jülicher F. In: Active Processes and Otoacoustic Emissions. Manley GA, Popper AN, Fay RR, editors. New York: Springer; 2008. pp. 63–92. [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Strogatz ST. Nonlinear Dynamics and Chaos. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Stoop R, Kern A. Two-tone suppression and combination tone generation as computations performed by the Hopf cochlea. Phys Rev Lett. 2004;93:268103. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.93.268103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Jülicher F, Andor D, Duke T. Physical basis of two-tone interference in hearing. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2001;98:9080–9085. doi: 10.1073/pnas.151257898. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Nadrowski B, Martin P, Jülicher F. Active hair-bundle motility harnesses noise to operate near an optimum of mechanosensitivity. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2004;101:12195–12200. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0403020101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Jülicher F, Dierkes K, Lindner B, Prost J, Martin P. Spontaneous movements and linear response of a noisy oscillator. Eur Phys J E. 2009;29:449–460. doi: 10.1140/epje/i2009-10487-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Hudspeth AJ, Jülicher F, Martin P. A critique of the critical cochlea: Hopf—a bifurcation—is better than none. J Neurophysiol. 2010;104:1219–1229. doi: 10.1152/jn.00437.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Frolenkov GI, Belyantseva IA, Kurc M, Mastroianni MA, Kachar B. Cochlear hair cell electromotility can provide force for both low and high intensity distortion product otoacoustic emission. Hear Res. 1998;126:67–74. doi: 10.1016/s0378-5955(98)00150-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Liberman MC, Zuo J, Guinan JJ., Jr Otoacoustic emissions without somatic motility: can stereocilia mechanics drive the mammalian cochlea? J Acoust Soc Am. 2004;116:1649–1655. doi: 10.1121/1.1775275. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Dierkes K, Lindner B, Jülicher F. Enhancement of sensitivity gain and frequency tuning by coupling of active hair bundles. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2008;105:18669–18674. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0805752105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Barral J, Dierkes K, Lindner B, Jülicher F, Martin P. Coupling a sensory hair-cell bundle to cyber clones enhances nonlinear amplification. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2010;107:8079–8084. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0913657107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Kozlov AS, Baumgart J, Risler T, Versteegh CP, Hudspeth AJ. Forces between clustered stereocilia minimize friction in the ear on a subnanometre scale. Nature. 2011;474:376–379. doi: 10.1038/nature10073. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2012 May 22;109(21):7959-7960.

Author Summary

AUTHOR SUMMARY

The ear does not work as a high-fidelity sound receiver (1). A person listening simultaneously to two pure tones can perceive not only those two frequencies but also additional “phantom” tones that are not present in the sound input. In addition, the perceived loudness of a tone diminishes when a second tone is added. Mechanical correlates of these psychophysical phenomena have been observed in the sound-evoked vibrations of the mammalian cochlea (2) and are thought to originate in sensory hair cells from the intrinsic nonlinearity associated with mechanoelectrical transduction by ion channels. Nonlinearity of the transducer, however, is not sufficient to explain the rich phenomenology of two-tone interferences in hearing. Here we show that active oscillatory movements by the hair cell can produce two-tone suppression and distortions with properties that are characteristic of hearing. Our results promote a general principle of sound detection that is based on the nonlinear activity of self-sustained oscillators in the inner ear.

Hearing starts with the deflection of the hair bundle, a tuft of extended microvilli (i.e., cylindrical processes) protruding from the apical surface of each mechanosensory hair cell. We took advantage of an in vitro preparation of a bullfrog’s sacculus, a bed of sensory cells in the inner ear, in which hair cells routinely power spontaneous oscillations of their hair bundle at a characteristic frequency fC = 5–80 Hz. Using a flexible glass fiber, we applied periodic forces F to the top of a hair bundle. Previous work (3) indicates that oscillatory hair bundles actively resonate with pure tones F(t) = F1 sin(2πf1t) and amplify weak stimuli near fC. Amplification is associated with a compressive nonlinearity that accommodates a large range of stimulus magnitudes (0.5–50 pN) into a narrower range of hair-bundle movements (3–40 nm). In this work, we evaluated the effects of active hair-bundle motility on two-tone interferences by a single hair cell.

By applying two-tone stimuli F(t) = F1 sin(2πf1t) + F2 sin(2πf2t), we first studied how the hair-bundle response to a test tone (frequency f1) was affected by the addition of a second tone—i.e., masker tone (frequency f2)—at a nearby frequency. For stimuli at resonance (f1 and f2 near fC), we observed that the response to the test tone was partially suppressed by the additional tone. As expected from a complete loss of amplification, the amplitude of stimulation that was required to elicit a response beyond a given threshold increased by as much as 10-fold for intense masker tones (F2 = 10 pN). Remarkably, the phenomenon of two-tone suppression, which was so strong at resonance, was barely observed when the hair bundle was stimulated off-resonance (f1 and f2 near 10fC) or when the masker tone did not enter the active bandwidth of the hair-bundle amplifier (f1 ≅ fC and f2fC or f2 ≪ fC).

In addition to two-tone suppression, the spectral density of the hair-bundle motion revealed the presence of discrete frequency components that were not present in the stimulus (Fig. P1). The hair bundle thus distorted its input. At resonance (f1 and f2 near fC)—a condition that maximizes nonlinear amplification by active hair-bundle motility (3), the most prominent components of distortion were the cubic-difference products at frequencies 2f1 - f2 and 2f2 - f1 (Fig. P1A). These products appeared at a low level of stimulation (Inline graphic) and showed a compressive growth that was well described by a power law Inline graphic with α = 0.26 ± 0.08 (mean ± SD, n = 11). This level function was nearly parallel to that of the primaries f1 and f2. Correspondingly, their relative level showed little variation over the range of stimulus amplitudes that we explored, remaining approximately constant at 10–20%. Notably, both the most prominent phantom tone in the human percept, which occurs at 2f1 - f2 (1), and in vivo cubic-difference products on the basilar membrane of the mammalian cochlea (2) evince a behavior similar to that observed with a single oscillatory hair bundle.

Fig. P1.

Fig. P1.

Two-tone distortions by a single hair-cell bundle. A hair bundle that oscillated spontaneously at 20 Hz was subjected to the sum of two sinusoidal forces at frequencies f1 and f2 with equal amplitudes (2 pN). (A) When stimulation was applied near the bundle’s characteristic frequency of oscillation—i.e., a condition that maximizes nonlinear amplification via active hair-bundle motility—the spectral density of bundle movement displayed cubic distortion products at 2f1 ± f2. In the temporal domain, the bundle’s global response displayed a beating pattern of motion (black line). Filtering the primaries f1 and f2 in the frequency domain revealed the temporal waveform of distortion (red line) that the hair bundle introduced into its response to the stimulus. (B) For stimulation away from the characteristic frequency, which probes passive hair-bundle mechanics, the movement evinced only a weak quadratic distortion product at f1 + f2.

Two-tone distortions caused by stimulation at resonance were in stark contrast to those measured for off-resonance stimulation (f1 and f2 near 10fC) or at extracellular calcium concentrations for which the hair bundle remained quiescent. In these cases, quadratic distortion products at f1 + f2 and f2 - f1 often emerged first (Fig. P1B). The cubic-difference products at 2f1 - f2 and 2f2 - f1 surfaced from the noise floor for relatively intense stimuli (Inline graphic) and showed a superlinear level function until saturation afforded no further increase in distortion magnitude. Nonlinear compliance of the hair bundle, which ensues from the direct opening or closing of transduction channels by mechanical force, can explain this behavior.

Although previously proposed as a major source of distortion (4), nonlinear gating compliance does not account, by itself, for two-tone interferences evoked by stimulation near the characteristic frequency of an oscillatory hair bundle. Simulations based on a generic description of active dynamical systems poised near an oscillatory instability—called a Hopf bifurcation—instead accounted for the data. Two-tone interferences thus allowed us to distinguish the frequency-dependent nonlinearity that emerges from the active process that drives the hair bundle into spontaneous oscillations (5) from the static nonlinearity associated with mechanoelectrical transduction. Any oscillator operating near a Hopf bifurcation must exhibit the same generic behavior as that observed here experimentally with a single oscillatory hair bundle. In return, we argue that the properties of two-tone interferences in hearing betray the workings of active mechanical oscillators that operate as nonlinear amplifiers of sound-evoked vibrations in the inner ear.

Footnotes

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

This Direct Submission article had a prearranged editor.

See full research article on page E1344 of www.pnas.org.

Cite this Author Summary as: PNAS 10.1073/pnas.1202426109.

References

  • 1.Goldstein JL. Auditory nonlinearity. J Acoust Soc Am. 1967;41:676–689. doi: 10.1121/1.1910396. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Robles L, Ruggero MA, Rich NC. Two-tone distortion on the basilar membrane of the chinchilla cochlea. J Neurophysiol. 1997;77:2385–2399. doi: 10.1152/jn.1997.77.5.2385. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Martin P, Hudspeth AJ. Compressive nonlinearity in the hair bundle’s active response to mechanical stimulation. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2001;98:14386–14391. doi: 10.1073/pnas.251530498. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Jaramillo F, Markin VS, Hudspeth AJ. Auditory illusions and the single hair cell. Nature. 1993;364:527–529. doi: 10.1038/364527a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Jülicher F, Andor D, Duke T. Physical basis of two-tone interference in hearing. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2001;98:9080–9085. doi: 10.1073/pnas.151257898. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America are provided here courtesy of National Academy of Sciences

RESOURCES