Abstract
The hypnogenic properties of cannabis have been recognized for centuries, but endogenous cannabinoid (endocannabinoid) regulation of vigilance states is poorly characterized. We report findings from a series of experiments in mice measuring sleep with polysomnography after various systemic pharmacological manipulations of the endocannabinoid system. Rapid, unbiased scoring of vigilance states was achieved using an automated algorithm that we devised and validated. Increasing endocannabinoid tone with a selective inhibitor of monoacyglycerol lipase (JZL184) or fatty acid amide hydrolase (AM3506) produced a transient increase in non-rapid eye movement (NREM) sleep due to an augmentation of the length of NREM bouts (NREM stability). Similarly, direct activation of type 1 cannabinoid (CB1) receptors with CP47,497 increased NREM stability, but both CP47,497 and JZL184 had a secondary effect that reduced NREM sleep time and stability. This secondary response to these drugs was similar to the early effect of CB1 blockade with the antagonist/inverse agonist AM281, which fragmented NREM sleep. The magnitude of the effects produced by JZL184 and AM281 were dependent on the time of day this drug was administered. While activation of CB1 resulted in only a slight reduction in gamma power, CB1 blockade had dramatic effects on broadband power in the EEG, particularly at low frequencies. However, CB1 blockade did not significantly reduce the rebound in NREM sleep following total sleep deprivation. These results support the hypothesis that endocannabinoid signaling through CB1 is necessary for NREM stability but it is not necessary for sleep homeostasis.
Introduction
Since antiquity cannabinoids have been used as a treatment for insomnia [1], and the first reports in western medical literature regarding the therapeutic utility and physiological effects of cannabis preparations note their hypnogenic properties [2–5]. Additionally, this effect appears to be conserved across mammalian species [6–11]. Given the long standing recognition of cannabinoids as sleep promoting substances, it is surprising that relatively few studies have examined the role of the endogenous cannabinoid (endocannabinoid; eCB) system in regulating vigilance states.
Cannabinoids produce the majority of their central effects by activating the cannabinoid 1 receptor (CB1), and activation of this G-protein-coupled receptor (GPCR) reduces neurotransmitter release at many synapses [12]. CB1 is a central molecular component of the eCB system, an increasingly well characterized, lipid-based neuromodulatory system. The predominant transmitters for the eCB system are N-arachidonyl ethanolamide (anandamide; AEA) and 2-archidonylglycerol (2-AG). These molecules are released during periods of neuronal activity, and their inactivation occurs largely via distinct hydrolytic pathways. AEA is primarily inactivated via fatty acid amide hydrolase (FAAH), and 2-AG signaling is terminated by monoacyglycerol lipase (MAGL). Of the relatively few studies that have been performed, administration of exogenous AEA consistently increases rapid eye movement (REM) sleep and non-REM (NREM) sleep [13–16]. However, conflicting results arise from attempts to increase endogenous AEA levels. Some studies indicate that FAAH inhibition promotes wake [17, 18], but other reports show that blocking the AEA membrane transporter facilitates NREM sleep [19, 20]. Additionally, mice with a constitutive knockout of FAAH have increased NREM sleep time and reduced wake [21]. The effects of MAGL inhibition on sleep have not been examined.
While cannabinoids have been used by humans for many years to increase sleep, patients in clinical trials for the CB1 antagonist/inverse agonist, rimonabant, commonly reported sleep disturbances [22, 23]. In support of a sleep promoting role of eCB signaling, several studies have found fragmented sleep in CB1-null mutant mice [24, 25]. However, studies with constitutive knockout mice are always subject to confounds arising from developmental adaptations, and this has been confirmed for the CB1 knockout mice used in these studies [26, 27]. On the other hand, studies with CB1 antagonists in rodents have had conflicting results with some reporting a weak reduction in NREM sleep [15, 19, 28–30] and others finding no effects on sleep [13, 31, 32]. Of note, all of these studies were performed over short time windows (< 8 Hr recordings), and eCB levels are known to fluctuate over the circadian cycle [33, 34]. Thus, differences in the time of day these experiments were performed could explain some of this discrepancy.
As there is a poor consensus regarding the effects of eCB signaling on sleep, we performed a series of experiments comprising over 11,000 Hr of polysomnographic recordings in mice following a variety of pharmacological manipulations probing different aspects of the eCB system. To more fully account for the time course of effects, sleep measures were assessed over a 23.5 Hr period following all manipulations. To analyze this large volume of data, we developed and validated a novel automated state-scoring algorithm. In addition to a description of sleep, we also report results from power spectral analyses of electroencephalographic (EEG) recordings. Finally, we directly tested whether eCB signaling is necessary for homeostatic regulation of sleep by blocking CB1 signaling during recovery from total sleep deprivation (TSD). Our findings indicate that eCB signaling is both necessary and sufficient to promote long (stable) bouts of NREM sleep, but eCBs are not necessary for sleep homeostasis. These findings constitute a thorough characterization of eCB modulation of vigilance states that should provide a platform for future studies examining the physiological mechanisms of eCB regulation of sleep.
Methods
Ethics Statement
This research involved the use of vertebrate animals (mice), including survival surgical procedures to implant electrodes. All methods were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (protocol #: LIN-DL-22) and hewed to guidelines specified in the Guide to the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals [35]. Survival surgeries were performed under isoflurane anesthesia, and ketoprofen (5 mg/kg i.p.) analgesic solution was administered immediately after surgery and every 24 hr for the next two days.
Subjects
Male C57BL/6J mice (103, 8–10 week olds) were obtained from the Jackson Laboratory (Bar Harbor, ME), and initially group housed, 2–4 mice per cage. Mice weighed 25–30 g at the beginning of the study, and body weight did not change substantially over the course of experiments. Following surgery, subjects were single housed for the remainder of the study. At all times, subjects were provided with ad libitum food and water. The colony and sleep recording environment were maintained on a 12 hr light:dark cycle with the light photoperiod (LP) starting at 06:30 and the dark photoperiod (DP) beginning at 18:30. For the experiment where JZL 184 was administered prior to the LP, mice were housed in reverse cycle conditions with lights turning on at 18:30 and off at 06:30 for 2 weeks prior to recordings and throughout the recording period. Time of day is expressed throughout this manuscript relative to the light zeitgeber (ZT) with ZT 00:00 coinciding with beginning of LP and ZT 12:00 coinciding with the beginning of the DP. The colony and recording environment were maintained at 22.2°C and 50% humidity.
Surgical Implantation of Electrodes
Prior to surgery, custom implants were prepared. One end of three single-stranded, Teflon coated stainless steel wires (#791500, A-M Systems, Sequim, WA) was soldered to individual gold-plated sockets (E363/0, PlasticsOne, Roanoke, VA). These three gold sockets and the socket attached to a stainless steel suture pad (E363T/2, PlasticsOne) were arranged in a plastic 6 channel connector (MS363, PlasticsOne) and secured with non-conductive epoxy. During surgery, two of the stainless steel wires emerging from the implant were wrapped, separately, around the frontal electrodes to provide two EEG channels. The ground electrodes were shorted together with the remaining wire. To ensure electrical connectivity with the EEG and ground electrodes a small amount of electrically conductive glue (Bare Paint, Bare Conductive Ltd., London, UK) was applied at the junction between wires and the stainless steel screws.
Stereotaxic surgery was performed to implant subjects with EEG/EMG electrodes. EEG electrodes consisting of stainless steel screws (Small Parts# AMS90/1P-25, Amazon Supply, Seattle, WA) were implanted supradurally through the skull. Two electrodes were implanted over frontal cortex (B: RC +2.64, ML ± 1.38) and referenced to two, connected ground electrodes implanted over occipital cortex (B: RC—2.5, ML ± 2). The EMG electrode (metal suture pad, PlasticsOne, Roanoke, VA) was implanted underneath the nuchal muscle. A head cap was formed with standard, cold-cure dental acrylic, and subjects were allowed to recuperate for two weeks in their home cages.
Sleep Recordings
Following recuperation from surgery, subjects were lightly anesthetized with isoflurane and connected to a non-motorized commutator (SL6C/SB, Plastics One) via an electrical tether. Subjects were placed into a recording home cage fabricated from a 4 liter, clear polycarbonate bucket (Cambro RFSCW4135, Webstaurant Store, Lancaster, PA). These cages contained standard corncob bedding, and food pellets were placed on the cage floor. Access to water was provided via glass liquid diet feeding tubes (#9019, Bio-serve, Frenchtown, NJ) inserted through a hole drilled through the side of each cage. The commutators were secured to a hole in the cage lid thus ensuring that mice did not become entangled in their tethers. Five cages were placed inside sound and light attenuating chambers equipped with a fan and white LED light strips (# 10434, General Electric, Fairfield, CT). The lights were on a timer synchronized with the colony lights and the data acquisition PC clock. Additionally, the inside of the chamber was lined with either copper mesh or coated in conductive paint to shield the inside from electromagnetic interference.
Prior to recording, mice were habituated to this environment for 7 days. On recording days, data were collected over a 23.5 Hr period. The time at which recordings were initiated depended on the experimental protocol, but this was generally either before the onset of the LP (ZT 00:00) or before the onset of the DP (ZT 12:00). For the sleep deprivation experiment, data collection was initiated in the middle of the LP (ZT 06:00). Individual cages were removed from the recording chambers during the 30 min between recording sessions, and subjects were weighed and administered an i.p. injection of saline, vehicle, or a drug. After injections, subjects were placed back into their respective cages, and these were returned to the recording chambers. In studies with multiple doses of drugs, we explicitly chose to use a schedule of escalating doses over a series of days. Cannabinoids can rapidly induce tolerance, and thus, if a counter-balanced design was implemented, any effect of low-dosage on sleep parameters would likely be prevented by the preceding administration of a high dose. Additionally, compensatory effects were observed following high dose administration of cannabinoids (for example, see results from the recovery day in experiments with JZL184 and AM3506), so a counterbalanced design on successive days could lead to erroneous conclusions that low dosage administration promotes effects opposing those of high-doses.
EEG and EMG signals were amplified 1000x (20x HST/16V-G20 headstage followed by 50x wide-band PBX, Plexon, Dallas, TX). The amplified signals were digitized using a National Instruments digitizer (PCI-6071E, National Instruments, Austin, TX) connected to a standard PC computer (Optiplex GX620, Dell Computers, Round Rock, TX) running Recorder v2 (Plexon). Data were visualized as they were being collected using Recorder software’s built-in oscilloscope, and in cases where signals were observed to be of poor quality (e.g. low signal-to-noise ratio, low dynamic range), the subject was not used for analysis of sleep or drug-induced changes in EEG spectral power. This was almost always the result of a faulty EMG electrode. As an additional check to ensure subjects with corrupt (unscorable) signals were not included in analysis, subsequent visual inspection of the scoring results (including raw EEG/EMG signals) for each subject alerted the investigator to any problems of low signal quality that could arise at a point in the experiment when online data collection was not being monitored. All data were sampled at 1 kHz, and a 60 Hz notch filter was applied to eliminate line noise. EEG signals were low-pass filtered online at 120 Hz with a 2 pole Bessel filter, and EMG signals were high pass filtered at 40 Hz with a 4 pole Bessel filter. Data were saved for offline analysis.
Sleep Deprivation
The custom sleep deprivation apparatus (S1 Fig, panel A) used in this report was constructed from a 10 in long clear acrylic tube with an internal diameter of 5 in that was suspended slightly above an epoxy-sealed ABS disc that served as the chamber floor. The disc and other structural components of the apparatus were custom designed in CAD software (Sketchup 8, Trimble Navigation Ltd, Sunnyvale, CA) and constructed from 3D printed ABS plastic (3D XL printer, Airwolf3D, Costa Mesa, CA). The cylinder had an acrylic divider that extended the length of its radius from the interior wall directly into the center of the chamber (S1 Fig, panel B). This divider ensured the animal would move when the floor of the device rotated. The chambers contained standard corncob bedding, and a sufficient amount of standard mouse chow was placed on the floor to provide ad libitum access to food. A glass liquid diet feeding tube provided ad libitum access to water. The bottom of ABS disc was attached to a 360 degree servo motor (DF15RSMG, DFRobot, Shanghai, China), and the motors of five chambers were controlled by an Arduino UNO board (Adafruit Industries, New York, NY) that received commands via a serial connection with a laptop running MATLAB (S1 Fig, panel C). The signal from the Arduino board and the servos were calibrated to rotate at approximately 15 rpm. Rotation was engaged for a random time interval between 10–15 sec and was turned off for a random interval between 5–10 sec. Additionally, the rotational direction of the disc was randomly alternated to prevent habituation to movement in one direction. In preliminary studies, we found this intermittent schedule to be effective in producing near total sleep deprivation (< 1% of time spent in NREM sleep) for up to 6 Hr in most subjects. Importantly, the devices were constructed so that a commutator (SL6C/SB, Plastics One) could be connected to the chamber lid. This allowed continuous recording of the EEG and EMG signals while the subject was housed in the apparatus.
Sleep deprivation experiments consisted of three phases: baseline, deprivation, and recovery. Polysomnographic measures of sleep were obtained across all phases of the experiment. Following habituation to the standard recording environment, 48 Hr baseline recordings were obtained from all subjects following an i.p. injection of vehicle (1:1:18 mixture DMSO: Cremaphor: 0.9% Saline) at ZT 06:00. After the baseline recordings, subjects were transferred into the sleep deprivation devices (S1 Fig). Importantly, their original recording cages (including bedding, food, and water bottles) were retained and labelled according to subject. TSD via forced locomotion was initiated at the onset of the LP (ZT 00:00), 18 Hr after the subjects were placed into the deprivation device. The TSD protocol continued for 6 Hr, and afterwards subjects were immediately removed from the deprivation chambers, weighed, and received either an i.p. injection of the vehicle solution (control) or 5 mg/kg AM281. Subjects were then returned to their original recording cage for 48 Hr (recovery phase). TSD was defined as spending less than 1% of total time (< 3.6 minutes) in NREM sleep, and based on this criterion, we disqualified 5 out of 14 mice in the AM281 group and 3 out of 14 mice in the vehicle group.
Vigilance State Scoring
To obtain an unbiased estimate of sleep-wake states, we devised an automated algorithm to score polysomnographic data as either wake, NREM, or REM sleep (Fig 1A and 1B). Importantly, this software arrives at a deterministic score of a 24 Hr single-subject recording in less than 5 min, assigning scores to 2 sec epochs, and it performs as well as trained human scorers (Fig 1C and S3 Fig).
Calculation of the State-Space
The first step, deriving the state-space (Fig 1A), was heavily influenced by the state-space methodology reported by Gervasoni et al [36], but in addition to electrographic signals from the brain, we also incorporated EMG activity to conform with standard polysomnography techniques used in mice [37]. However, our methodology differed somewhat from other state-space based approaches. Because we were recording from two EEG channels, we first compressed this data by taking the first principle component (PC) of the raw data. By only performing analysis on the 1st PC of the two frontal EEG signals, computational overhead is reduced. Next, power spectra were obtained for the EEG and EMG waveforms using a 4 sec sliding window FFT with a 2 sec step. This was implemented using the spectrogram() function that is part of the signal processing toolbox in MATLAB (The Mathworks Inc, Natick, MA). This provided frequency domain data in 2 sec epochs with roughly 0.25 Hz bin resolution. Two power spectral ratios (R1 & R2) were calculated from the EEG data for each epoch:
Ratio 1 (R1) provides good separation between NREM and wake epochs [36, 38]. However, separating REM from the other two clusters is challenging due to the sparse nature of REM sleep and the similarity of the power spectra between REM and wake in mice. However, theta rhythms (5–8 Hz) are prominent during REM sleep, and this characteristic has been used as a means to separate REM epochs into a distinct, if somewhat diffuse cluster [38–40]. Thus, we defined Ratio 2 (R2) to help pull epochs with high theta power away from the major clusters representing NREM and wake. In addition to prominent theta, REM sleep is also distinguished from wake by significantly reduced muscle tonus which can be measured with EMG. Therefore, we incorporated this criterion into the state-space by including the RMS value of the power spectra of the EMG waveform. In summary, three criteria were used to separate epochs of polysomnographic data into a 3D state-space: (1) Prominence of the frequencies between 0.5 and 20 Hz relative to the entire power spectra (0.5–100 Hz), (2) Prominence of theta power relative to delta (0.5-4Hz), (3) RMS of the power spectra of the EMG waveform.
To provide better cluster separation, the state-space coordinates are smoothed with a 10 sec Hann function and log transformed. To standardize the range of axes occupied by the state-space the log transformed data were median centered and normalized to the max absolute value on each axis. This bounds the state-space between -1 and +1 across all axes for all subjects. The first graph in Fig 1B provides an example of the state-space at this stage of algorithm.
Classification of Points in State-Space as Vigilance States
Once the state-space is computed, three distinct clusters can be observed, and the second step of the classification software (Fig 1B) serves to automatically define these clusters. This process occurs over three sequential steps, where a rough description of clusters based on hard cutoffs is used to establish statistical boundaries (confidence intervals) that reclassify the data using an inclusion/exclusion test. In the final step, the remaining epochs that are not assigned to wake, NREM, or REM states are restricted to epochs that are ambiguous to classify because they occur during transitions between vigilance states.
First, a rough estimate of cluster boundaries is obtained by establishing threshold values based on the distribution of points along each axis (Fig 1B, step 1). Specifically, a univariate kernel density estimate is performed independently for EMG and R1. The distribution along R1 is bimodal, so the threshold separating wake and REM from NREM along the R1 axis is defined as the local minimum between these modes. The distribution of EMG values is not consistently bimodal, so NREM epochs are constrained by the third quartile along this axis. Similarly, wake was defined as epochs on the opposite side of the R1 distribution threshold with EMG values greater than the median. REM was defined as values in the same mode of R1 as wake with EMG values within the first tercile. Additionally, REM was constrained to only those epochs above the median value on the R2 axis. The purpose of the these hard-cut criteria is to seed clusters based on the consistent topology of the state-space. The next two steps of the classification process substantially refine this initial classification. The values for thresholds were visually determined by the experimenter after trial and error with many data sets as reasonable thresholds to capture the majority of each cluster across the overwhelming majority of datasets tested. Importantly, not all points are classified in this step (see Fig 1B, step 1), and there is a buffer of unclassified points left between wake and REM that is subsequently incorporated into these clusters after completing steps 2 & 3 of the classification process. After inspecting the classified state-space for each data file, there were some rare instances where it was clear that the REM cluster was not adequately defined by this step of the algorithm, and in these instances, a custom software routine allowed for the manual selection of the REM cluster.
Based upon this initial classification, confidence intervals were calculated for each cluster by estimating the probability density function (PDF) for each cluster separately () and the whole state-space (), using kernel estimation with a Gaussian kernel:
The smoothing parameter was determined based on the dataset using Scott’s Rule:
Because some points were not assigned a state in the first classification step, there was a separate PDF calculated for these points in addition to the estimates for wake, NREM, and REM. After all kernel estimates had been obtained, they were scaled so the maximum value of each component PDF was equal to the corresponding grid location in . In this way, the PDF of the entire state-space was decomposed into component densities representing the different states (Fig 1B, step 2a).
To determine the probability that a given point belonged to a specified cluster, the component PDFs were subtracted from one another and normalized to the absolute value of the resulting maxima:
Where A, B, C, and D represent different states (Wake, NREM, REM, and Unassigned), and x is a three dimensional feature vector for a specified epoch of the state-space. This subtraction and normalization step was performed for each component density yielding four probability matrices. The subtraction step was important to delineate clean borders between states.
At this stage of processing, points in the state-space were reclassified using the probability matrices defined above. To accomplish this, each epoch of the state-space was indexed into the four probability matrices, to determine the probability that it belonged to each state. The epoch was assigned to the state with the highest probability if it fell within confidence intervals specified a priori. We established 99.9% confidence intervals for all states. Points that fell outside of these confidence intervals were assigned to the unclassified cluster, and similarly, points that had equivalent probability of belonging to two or more clusters were assigned to the unclassified cluster (Fig 1B, step 2b).
As can be seen in the results following classification with 99.9% confidence intervals (Fig 1B, step 2b), unclassified epochs comprised points on the periphery of clusters and transitional epochs between clusters. To further refine the state assignment, a final classification step was performed using a transitional classifier (Fig 1B, step 3). The point of this last step was to reduce unclassified epochs to only those epochs representing transitions between states where state scoring is inherently ambiguous. Consequently, this classification step assigned all unclassified epochs bounded by an epoch of the same state, while unclassified epochs bounded by different states would remain unclassified. Thus, the sequence [wake, unclassified, unclassified, wake] would become [wake, wake, wake, wake], while [wake, unclassified, unclassified, NREM] would remain the same. As shown in the last graph of Fig 1B, the result of this classification step was to eliminate the penumbra of unclassified epochs surrounding the clusters, while leaving the unclassified epochs between cluster boundaries unchanged.
Most of this analysis was coded in MATLAB and C/C++ (MEX file libraries). However, a parallelized kernel density estimation method, GPUML, was implemented in CUDA/C (NVIDIA Corp, Santa Clara, CA) to speed computation of non-parametric density estimation [41]. Throughout the algorithm, operations were parallelized where possible, and this was achieved via explicit coding of parfor and spmd loops using MATLAB’s Parallel Computing Toolbox. Importantly, for each data set (all subjects * days of experiment), the scoring results were visually examined to ensure there were no obvious defects in scoring.
The prevalence of each state was calculated as the percent of time spent in that state. Additionally, the number and duration of bouts of NREM and REM were calculated (sleep architecture) with one bout defined as a consecutive series of epochs in the same state.
Drugs
CP47,497 (CP47), AM281, JZL184 (JZL), and URB597 (URB) were all obtained from Tocris Bioscience (Bristol, UK). These compounds are highly lipophilic and only sparingly soluble in aqueous solution. Therefore, these drugs were dissolved in a vehicle solution consisting of a 1:1:18 mixture of DMSO, Cremaphor, and normal saline. Administration of this vehicle had no effect on either sleep or EEG power spectra (S2 Fig). AM3506 was synthesized in the laboratory of Dr. Alexandros Makriyannis (Northeastern University), and was prepared in a 1:1:8 vehicle of DMSO:Cremaphor:Saline because it tends to precipitate when prepared in the standard vehicle solution. For all experiments, 24 Hr recordings of polysomnographic indices following administration of the appropriate vehicle solution were used as a within-subject baseline for comparison. All drug and vehicle solutions were administered via i.p. injections given at a volume of 0.02 mL per gram body weight. Drug were prepared fresh on the day of the experiment.
Statistics
In all experiments, time of day was included as a factor, but it was necessary to take into account the effect of photoperiod as well. Therefore, all analyses of time course data utilized a hierarchical linear mixed model (HLM) approach. For most experiments, a model with three repeated, fixed factors was implemented. Specifically, the model tested for the interaction between drug treatment and time of day nested within photoperiod. Because we followed subjects across different treatment conditions, all analyses contained repeated measures, and post-hoc comparisons were performed within-subjects. For the sleep deprivation experiment, there were two experimental groups (AM281 treated and vehicle control). For simplicity, only data from the first day following baseline vehicle injection (baseline day 1) and the first day following sleep deprivation (recovery day 1) were statistically compared. In this case, the model design examined the between groups interaction of treatment group (vehicle vs AM281) with time of day nested within photoperiod (light vs dark) nested within experimental phase (baseline vs recovery), where time of day, photoperiod, and experimental phase were all repeated measures. Analysis with HLM was performed in SPSS (IBM, Bethesda, MD). A Bonferroni correction was applied to all pair-wise comparisons of the model-derived estimated marginal means, and all reported P-values reflect this correction. For all analyses α = 0.05.
Results
Validation of Unsupervised Sleep Staging Algorithm
To validate our method of scoring, the computer-derived vigilance state classification results from 6 datasets were compared against scoring results from three trained humans. Because manual scoring by humans will always be sensitive to issues of subjectivity and scorer vigilance, an appropriate validation of automated methods should take into account how the computer-derived score compares to the inter-rater reliability of manual scoring. Consequently, the percent agreement between scores obtained from the computer algorithm and manual sleep staging were compared to the percent agreement between the manually-derived scores (inter-rater reliability; Fig 1C). There was no interaction between scorer (human vs. computer) and data file (repeated measure; 2-way ANOVA, F(5, 20) = 1.05, p = 0.42), and there was no effect of scorer (F(1, 4) = 1.01, p = 0.37). However, there was an effect of data file (F(5, 20) = 20.76, p < 0.001), because data file 4 was intentionally included as it had a noisy EMG signal. Compared to the other files that were scored, there was a marked reduction in the inter-rater reliability between humans and between human vs computer derived scores. Comparisons of scoring reliability for each vigilance state also found no difference between humans and the computer (S3 Fig). Consequently, we conclude that this algorithm performs comparably to manual sleep staging.
Fig 2 shows example scoring results with raw data traces and power spectra including state transitions. One important feature of this vigilance state-scoring program is the necessary inclusion of unclassified/transitional epochs that cannot be assigned to specific states with any rigor (note the black points between clusters in Fig 2A). This derives naturally from the fact that state clusters are not cleanly segregated in the state-space, which is consistent with the intuitive notion that state-transitions are not instantaneous (i.e. falling asleep or waking up takes some time as cortical ensembles synchronize or desynchronize, respectively). Thus, the algorithm conservatively estimates vigilance states by only assigning a score when an epoch registers within some statistical bounds of certainty.
Direct Activation of CB1 Receptors Facilitates NREM Sleep
To determine how activation of CB1 affects sleep, the full CB1 agonist, CP47, was administered just prior to the DP. Consistent with reports that CB1 activation reduces locomotor activity, phasic muscle movements in the EMG were reduced after injection of CP47, and the amount of high voltage, low frequency activity in the EEG was increased (Fig 3). In this experiment, a 0.1 (low) and a 1.0 (high) mg/kg dose of CP47 were administered on subsequent recording days following a baseline day where vehicle was injected (Fig 4A). We assessed the percent time spent in NREM sleep (Fig 4B) and found a significant overall interaction (treatment x time of day within photoperiod, F(18, 142.63) = 9.804, p < 0.001), secondary interaction (treatment x photoperiod, F(2, 96.81) = 26.63, p < 0.001), and a main effect of photoperiod (F(1, 116.62) = 284.59, p < 0.001). High dose CP47 had biphasic effects on sleep time, inducing significantly more NREM during the DP (t(85.57) = 5.71, p < 0.001) and reducing NREM during the LP (t(85.57) = -6.046, p = 0.006). NREM sleep time was increased over the first 6 Hr of the DP (low dose, ZT12-15: t(191.94) = 2.89, p = 0.009; high dose, ZT12-18: t(191.94) ≥ 6.21, p < 0.001), and high dose CP47 significantly reduced NREM during the first 3 Hr of the LP (ZT00-03: t(191.94) = -2.54, p = 0.024). Thus, the synthetic cannabinoid CP47 biphasically modulates NREM sleep time.
We next examined effects on NREM architecture. For NREM bout duration, there was an overall interaction (treatment x time of day within photoperiod, F(18, 143.87) = 3.854, p < 0.001), a secondary interaction (treatment x photoperiod, F(2, 78.01) = 4.85, p = 0.010), and a main effect of photoperiod (F(1, 113.84) = 26.537, p < 0.001). CP47 significantly increased NREM bout duration during the second quarter of the DP (ZT15-18: t(157.46) = 3.49, p = 0.001). In contrast, NREM bout duration was reduced across the LP (t(62.72) = 3.23, p = 0.032), specifically during the first quarter of the LP (ZT00-03: t(157.46) = 3.16, p = 0.004). The number of NREM bouts was also affected by an overall interaction (F(18, 143.48) = 1.96, p = 0.016) and a main effect of photoperiod (F(1, 113.30) = 17.81, p < 0.001). However, the only significant difference between treatment conditions occurred during the first quarter of the LP when high dose CP47 increased the number of NREM bouts (t(157.72) = 3.74, p < 0.001). Treatment with CP47 did not affect REM sleep (Fig 4C).
To confirm that CP47’s effects on sleep were mediated through the CB1 receptor, a separate cohort of subjects was administered the vehicle solution followed 24 Hr later by an injection containing a mixture of CP47 (1 mg/kg) and the selective CB1 antagonist, AM281 (5 mg/kg; CP47+AM281; Fig 4D). As an internal positive control, the experiment was continued for a third day when CP47 (1 mg/kg) was administered alone. There was an overall interaction for NREM sleep time (Fig 4E; treatment x time of day within photoperiod, F(18, 198.22) = 11.31, p < 0.001), a secondary interaction (treatment x photoperiod, F(2, 133.28) = 43.47, p < 0.001), and a main effect of photoperiod (F(1, 160.97) = 364.21, p < 0.001). While CP47 alone increased sleep time during the DP (ZT12-15 & ZT15-18: t(263.34) ≥ 6.43, p < 0.001) and reduced it during the LP (ZT00-03, 03–06, and 06–09: t(263.34) ≤ -3.12, p ≤ 0.006), co-administering AM281 significantly attenuated the CP47 effects during the DP (AM281+CP47 vs CP47, ZT12-15, 15–18, and 18–21: t(263.34) ≤ -2.94, p ≤ 0.011) and reversed them completely during the LP (ZT00-03 and 03–06: t(263.34) ≥ 2.57, p ≤ 0.033). Compared to vehicle, CP47+AM281 had mixed effects on NREM in the DP (ZT12-15 (increased): t(263.34) = 4.14, p < 0.001; ZT18-21(decreased): t(263.34) = -2.79, p = 0.017), but there were no differences during the LP. These results replicated our previous findings on CP47’s biphasic effect on sleep, and co-administration of AM281 blocked this effect suggesting that CP47’s effects on NREM sleep are mediated through CB1.
The CP47-induced changes in sleep architecture were similarly blunted by co-administration of AM281. For NREM bout duration, there was a significant overall interaction (treatment x time of day within photoperiod, F(18, 202.64) = 5.487, p < 0.001), secondary interaction (treatment x photoperiod, F(2, 103.25) = 13.06, p < 0.001), and main effects of both drug treatment (F(2, 61.707) = 4.376, p = 0.017) and photoperiod (F(1, 169.28) = 45.05, p < 0.001). CP47 produced a large decrease in bout duration across all time points of the LP (CP47 vs vehicle, ZT00-12: t(182.30) ≤ -2.69, p ≤ 0.025) that was blocked by co-administration of AM281 (CP47+AM281 vs CP47, ZT00-09:: t(184.77) ≥ 3.30, p ≤ 0.003). CP47+AM281 did not change NREM bout duration relative to vehicle. The number of NREM bouts was affected by a significant overall interaction (treatment x time of day within photoperiod, F(18, 192.19) = 5.20, p < 0.001) and main effects of both photoperiod (F(1, 155.99) = 11.18, p = 0.001) and drug treatment (F(2, 54.76) = 4.79, p = 0.012). Across the entire recording day, CP47 produced an increase in the number of NREM bouts (t(48.65) = 2.98, p = 0.013), but the only specific time point with significant differences between treatments was the first quarter of the DP where both CP47 (t(176.71) = 4.58, p < 0.001) and CP47+AM281 (t(178.81) = -4.57, p < 0.001) produced a significant increase in NREM bouts. As discussed later, AM281 alone increases the number NREM bouts during the first quarter of the DP, so the increased number of NREM bouts is confounded by AM281’s effect. Thus, CP47’s effects on sleep architecture are largely mediated through the CB1 receptor.
Inhibition of Monoacylglycerol Lipase Stabilizes NREM and Suppresses REM
Sleep Measurements
Considering that activation of CB1 receptors with exogenous ligands can facilitate sleep, we next sought to test the hypothesis that eCBs could similarly promote NREM sleep. Increasing endogenous 2-AG tone with JZL, a selective MAGL inhibitor, reduced phasic EMG activity and increased the amount of low-frequency high-voltage EEG activity characteristic of NREM sleep (Fig 5). To quantify vigilance states after pharmacologically increasing 2-AG levels, subjects were sequentially given a 1.6 (low), 8.0 (moderate), and 16.0 (high) mg/kg doses of JZL, after which an additional 24 Hr recording with no injection (recovery) was obtained (Fig 6A). Within-subject comparisons were made using sleep measures obtained during a 24 Hr baseline recording that followed a vehicle injection. Several reports have suggested that endocannabinoid levels fluctuate across the circadian cycle [33, 34, 42], but it is unclear how or if this may be related to sleep. Therefore, two experiments were performed with JZL in separate groups of mice. In one, JZL was administered before the DP, when mice are most active (Fig 6B), and in the other, JZL was administered prior to the LP (Fig 6C).
When given just before the onset of the DP (between ZT 11:30 and 12:00), JZL administration had obvious effects on NREM sleep that mirrored those seen with CP47 (Fig 6B; top row). This was evidenced by an overall interaction (treatment x time of day within photoperiod, F(30, 269.56) = 12.29, p < 0.001), secondary interaction (treatment x photoperiod, F(4, 198.66) = 48.31, p < 0.001), and a main effect of photoperiod (F(1, 234.92) = 596.81, p < 0.001). JZL had biphasic effects on NREM with increased sleep during the DP (moderate: t(172.80) = 4.49, p < 0.001; high: t(175.26) = 6.71, p < 0.001) and a suppression of NREM during the LP (moderate: t(172.80) = -3.72, p = 0.001; high: t(172.80) = -4.62, p < 0.001). Specifically, JZL increased NREM sleep during the middle of the DP (moderate, ZT15-18: t(357.53) = 4.25, p < 0.001; high, ZT15-21: t(357.53) ≥ 4.38, p < 0.001) and reduced it during the LP (moderate, ZT00-03: t(357.53) = -3.17, p = 0.007; high, ZT06-09: t(357.53) = -2.93, p = 0.014). In contrast, on the recovery day NREM was reduced during the DP (recovery vs vehicle, t(172.80) = -3.66, p = 0.001) and increased during the LP (t(172.8) = 2.55, p = 0.047), specifically during the last quarter of the LP (ZT09-12: t(357.53) = 2.64, p = 0.035). Thus, inhibition of MAGL has biphasic effects on NREM sleep, initially increasing the time in NREM, followed by a decrease in NREM that extends into the recovery day.
JZL also produced alterations in NREM architecture similar to CP47. For NREM bout duration, there was an overall interaction (treatment x time of day within photoperiod, F(30, 272.08) = 3.55, p < 0.001), a secondary interaction (treatment x photoperiod, F(4, 153.85) = 20.92, p < 0.001), and main effects of both treatment (F(4, 93.89) = 3.60, p = 0.009) and photoperiod (F(1, 230.16) = 4.065, p = 0.045). High dose JZL increased NREM bout duration across the DP (t(115.00) = 2.83, p = 0.022), and both moderate and high doses reduced it across the LP (t(115.00) ≤ -2.58, p ≤ 0.044). Specifically, high dose JZL increased bout duration during the second quarter of the DP (ZT15-18: t(286.90) = 4.29, p < 0.001) and reduced it across most of the LP (ZT00-09: t(286.90) ≤ -2.75, p ≤ 0.026). On the recovery day, NREM bout duration was increased across most of the LP (ZT03-12: t(287.54) ≥ 2.56, p ≤ 0.044). For the number of NREM bouts, there was also an overall interaction (treatment x time of day within photoperiod, F(30, 286.26) = 3.19, p < 0.001), a secondary interaction (treatment x photoperiod, F(4, 153.85) = 3.23, p = 0.014), and main effects of both drug treatment (F(4, 75.96) = 6.15, p < 0.001) and photoperiod (F(1, 272.44) = 99.44, p < 0.001). High dose JZL increased NREM bouts across the LP (t(95.33) = 3.57, p = 0.002) with pair-wise differences at two time points (ZT03-06 & ZT09-12: t(218.71) ≥ 2.77, p ≤ 0.024). Thus, the effects of JZL treatment on NREM sleep were closely mirrored by effects on NREM bout duration, suggesting that MAGL inhibition-induced changes in sleep are due to modulation of NREM stability.
In contrast, administration of JZL before the DP produced only a slight reduction in REM sleep parameters (Fig 6B; bottom row). There was no effect of JZL on REM sleep time. For the duration of REM bouts, there was a nested interaction (time of day within photoperiod, F(6, 238.62) = 10.81, p < 0.001), and main effects of treatment (F(4, 82.54) = 7.01, p < 0.001) and photoperiod (F(1,238.51) = 34.78, p < 0.001). 16.0 mg/kg JZL reduced REM bout duration during across the DP (t(110.53) = -2.56, p = 0.047), specifically during the third quarter of the DP (ZT18-21: t(235.00) = -2.80, p = 0.022), and REM bout duration was increased across the LP on the recovery day (t(99.54) = 2.77, p = 0.027), specifically during the second quarter of the LP (ZT03-06: t(237.32) = 2.71, p = 0.022). For the number of REM bouts, there was a nested interaction (time of day within photoperiod, (F(6, 268.06) = 14.44, p < 0.001) and main effects of treatment (F(4, 81.95) = 3.17, p = 0.018) and photoperiod (F(1,254.72) = 55.42, p < 0.001). The high dose of JZL increased the number of REM bouts during the last 3 Hr of the LP (ZT09-12: t(240.24) = 3.72, p = 0.001).
When JZL was administered before the LP, NREM sleep was augmented (Fig 6C; top row), but the magnitude of this effect (deviation from vehicle baseline) was not as great as when the drug was given prior to the DP. The effect was also not biphasic within a circadian cycle. For the percent of time spent in NREM sleep, there was a secondary interaction (treatment x photoperiod, F(4, 165.01) = 5.00, p = 0.001) and a nested interaction (time of day within photoperiod, F(6, 209.40) = 22.04, p < 0.001) along with main effects of both treatment (F(4, 126.24) = 33.05, p < 0.001) and photoperiod (F(1, 192.72) = 522.51, p < 0.001). Moderate and high dose JZL increased NREM sleep time across the LP (t(145.25) ≥ 4.92, p < 0.001), while NREM sleep time was reduced on the recovery day during both the LP (t(145.26) = -3.36, p = 0.004) and DP (t(145.26) = -3.61, p = 0.002). Specifically, all three doses of JZL increase NREM sleep time during the first 3 Hr of the LP (ZT 00–03: t(274.85) ≥ 2.59, p ≤ 0.040) with the moderate dose increasing NREM sleep up to 6 Hr after administration (t(274.85) = 3.06, p = 0.010) and the high dose increasing NREM up to 9 Hr into the LP (t(274.85) = 2.52, p < 0.050). For NREM bout duration, there was an overall interaction (treatment x time of day within photoperiod; F(24, 218.31) = 1.67, p = 0.030), a secondary interaction (treatment x photoperiod; F(4, 120.79) = 2.80, p = 0.029), a nested interaction (time of day within photoperiod; F(6, 20.50) = 8.02, p < 0.001), and main effects of both treatment (F(4, 74.15) = 6.34, p < 0.001) and photoperiod (F(1, 179.88) = 132.73, p < 0.001). High and moderate dose JZL increased NREM bout duration across the LP (t(91.06) ≥ 2.94, p ≤ 0.016). The moderate dose of JZL increased NREM bout duration across the first 6 Hr of the LP (ZT00-06: t(225.33) ≥ 2.59, p ≤ 0.041), while high dose JZL only increased NREM bout duration later in the LP (ZT06-09: t(225.33) = 3.12, p = 0.008). The number of NREM bouts was not affected when JZL was administered before the LP.
In contrast to the modest effects of JZL on REM sleep when the drug was given before the DP, administration of JZL before the LP produced a marked reduction in REM sleep (Fig 6C; bottom row). For the percent of time spent in REM sleep, there was an overall interaction (treatment x time of day within photoperiod, F(24, 224.83) = 1.61, p = 0.040), secondary interaction (treatment x photoperiod, F(4, 116.88) = 13.58, p < 0.001), nested interaction (time of day within photoperiod, F(6, 212.50) = 9.60, p < 0.001), and main effects of both treatment (F(4, 61.32) = 7.17, p < 0.001) and photoperiod (F(1, 200.12) = 10.77, p = 0.001). Both moderate and high dose JZL reduced REM sleep time across the LP (t(77.11) ≤ -4.81, p < 0.001). Specifically, REM sleep was diminished across most time points in the LP following administration of both doses (moderate dose, ZT00-09: t(184.07) ≤ -2.81, p ≤ 0.022; high dose, ZT00-12: t(184.05) ≤ -3.87, p ≤ 0.001). For REM bout duration, there was an overall interaction (treatment x time of day within photoperiod, F(24, 192.48) = 2.02, p = 0.005), secondary interaction (treatment x photoperiod, F(4, 113.74) = 6.52, p < 0.001), and main effects of both treatment (F(4, 70.319) = 11.95, p < 0.001) and photoperiod (F(1, 165.44) = 15.90, p < 0.001). REM bout duration was suppressed by both moderate and high dose JZL across the LP (t(85.623) ≤ -27.46, p ≤ 0.001). Specifically, the moderate dose of JZL reduced REM bout duration during the first 6 Hr of the LP (ZT00-06: t(217.98) ≤ −3.27, p ≤ 0.005) and increased REM bout duration during the middle of the DP (ZT18-21: t(217.49) = 2.75, p = 0.026). High dose JZL reduced REM bout duration across the entire LP and into the first 3 Hr of the DP (ZT00-15: t(214.55) ≤ −3.21, p ≤ 0.006). For the number of REM bouts, there was a secondary interaction (treatment x photoperiod, F(4, 119.91) = 10.01, p < 0.001), nested interaction (time of day within photoperiod, F(6, 206.20) = 11.17, p < 0.001), and main effects of both treatment (F(4, 74.09) = 3.14, p = 0.019) and photoperiod (F(1, 177.54) = 27.29, p < 0.001). Again, moderate and high dose JZL reduced the number of REM bouts across the LP (t(90.894) ≤ -3.41, p ≤ 0.002). The number of REM bouts was reduced at multiple time points during the LP following JZL administration (moderate dose, ZT03-06: t(227.24) = 3.19, p < 0.001); high dose, ZT00-09: t(227.24) ≤ -2.85, p ≤ 0.019). Thus, REM sleep is markedly suppressed by acute augmentation of 2-AG tone, but only when this drug is administered immediately before the time of day when mice engage in most of their REM sleep.
EEG Power Spectral Measurements
Given the effects of increased 2-AG signaling on NREM and REM sleep described above, we examined the spectral content of the EEG signal from the experiment where JZL was administered before the DP (Fig 7). Similar results were obtained when JZL was administered before the LP (S4 Fig) and when CP47 was administered before the DP (S5 Fig). Despite the robust effects on sleep, JZL produced relatively modest effects on 12 Hr averages of EEG power spectrum from epochs of any state (Fig 7A–7C). To quantify JZL’s effects on EEG power spectra with higher temporal precision, we summed across well-described power spectral bandwidths (delta: 0-4Hz, theta: 4–8 Hz, and gamma: 30–70 Hz) in 3 Hr time bins (Fig 7D–7F). These bandwidths are routinely associated with sleep homeostasis (delta [43, 44], theta [45]), pneumonic processes (theta [46]), and attention (gamma [47]). Treatment with JZL had no effect on delta, theta, or gamma power during wake epochs (Fig 7D). For NREM epochs (Fig 7E), there was no effect of JZL on delta power, but for theta power there was a significant overall interaction (treatment x time of day within photoperiod, F(24, 335.61) = 1.84, p = 0.010), a nested interaction (time of day within photoperiod, F(6, 304.79) = 9.24, p < 0.001), and a main effect of photoperiod (F(1, 159.84) = 85.90, p < 0.001). However, there were no specific time points where JZL significantly altered NREM theta power relative to vehicle. For NREM gamma power, there was an overall interaction (treatment x time of day within photoperiod, F(24, 344.26) = 3.21, p < 0.001), a secondary interaction (treatment x photoperiod, F(4, 354.88) = 14.62, p < 0.001), a nested interaction (time of day within photoperiod, F(6, 304.49) = 25.78, p < 0.001), and main effects of both drug treatment (F(4, 220.36) = 10.85, p < 0.001) and photoperiod (F(1, 168.31) = 5.16, p = 0.024). JZL184 produced a dose-dependent reduction of NREM gamma power, with 8.0 mg/kg JZL184 decreasing gamma during the first 9 Hr of the DP (ZT 12–21: t(240.60) ≤ -2.67, p ≤ 0.032) and 16.0 mg/kg JZL reducing NREM gamma across the entire DP (ZT 12–00: t(159.50) ≤ -4.14, p ≤ 0.001). NREM gamma was no different from vehicle following the 1.6 mg/kg dose or on the recovery day. For REM epochs (Fig 7F), there was an there was an overall interaction (treatment x time of day within photoperiod, F(24, 284.87) = 1.71, p ≤ 0.022) and a main effect of photoperiod (F(1, 306.75) = 16.23, p < 0.001) for delta power. However, there were no pair-wise differences between treatment/recovery conditions and vehicle. Similarly, for REM theta power, there was an overall interaction (treatment x time of day within photoperiod, F(24, 293.65) = 2.36, p < 0.001) and a nested interaction (time of day within photoperiod, F(6, 292.49) = 8.09, p < 0.001), but there were no pair-wise differences between treatment/recovery conditions and vehicle. For REM gamma, there was a secondary interaction (treatment x photoperiod, F(4, 252.78) = 5.03, p = 0.001) and a nested interaction (time of day within photoperiod, F(6, 292.39) = 10.94, p < 0.001) with main effects of drug treatment (F(4, 83.77) = 7.39, p < 0.001) and photoperiod (F(1, 235.60) = 15.65, p < 0.001). JZL reduced REM gamma in a dose-dependent manner with 8.0 mg/kg JZL184 decreasing REM gamma across the first half of the DP (ZT12-18: t(121.00) ≤ -3.04, p ≤ 0.011) and 16.0 mg/kg JZL184 decreasing REM gamma across the entire DP (ZT12-00: t(85.92) ≤ -3.64, p ≤ 0.002). There was no difference in REM gamma power following low dose JZL184 or on the recovery day. Thus, increasing endogenous 2-AG tone with JZL has little effect on delta or theta power, but it attenuates gamma oscillations, particularly during sleep. Additionally, this effect is consistent irrespective of the time of day JZL is administered (see S4 Fig).
Inhibition of Fatty Acid Amide Hydrolase Stabilizes NREM and Suppresses REM
Sleep Measurements
Next, we tested the hypothesis that endogenous N-acylethanolamines, including AEA, could modulate sleep. The FAAH inhibitor URB was injected systemically at three different doses (0.1, 1.0, and 10.0 mg/kg) over successive days (S6 Fig, panel A), but URB did not have a substantial effect on either NREM (S6 Fig, panel B) or REM (S6 Fig, panel C) sleep, in contrast to the counterintuitive effects of i.c.v. injection reported previously [18]. These data would appear to suggest that N-acylethanolamines are not important for the regulation of vigilance states. However, application of exogenous AEA is known to facilitate NREM sleep [14, 15], and the elevation of N-acylethanolamines in rodent brain tissue by URB lasts only a few hours [48]. Thus, we performed a separate experiment with a single dose of the selective, long-lasting FAAH inhibitor AM3506 (10.0 mg/kg; Fig 8A) that reduces FAAH activity for up to 10 days after administration [49]. In this experiment, subjects were administered a vehicle injection followed 24 Hr later by an injection of AM3506, and polysomnographic measures of sleep were obtained over the following 48 Hr. As shown in Fig 8B, AM3506 significantly altered NREM sleep. For NREM sleep time, there was a significant overall interaction (treatment x time of day within photoperiod, F(18, 142.90) = 3.68, p < 0.001), a secondary interaction (treatment x photoperiod, F(2, 80.60) = 20.57), and main effects of both treatment (F(2, 56.53) = 11.63, p < 0.001) and photoperiod (F(1, 111.44) = 231.09, p < 0.001). During the DP, AM3506 significantly augmented NREM sleep time (t(66.28) = 5.16, p < 0.001), and similar to the effect of JZL, there was a significant reduction in NREM sleep during the DP on the recovery day (t(66.63) = -2.41, p = 0.038). In contrast to the effects of JZL and CP47, NREM sleep time during the LP was unaffected. Pair-wise comparisons at individual time bins found AM3506 significantly increased NREM sleep across the first 9 Hr of the DP (ZT12-21: t(168.29) ≥ 2.64, p ≤ 0.018), and on the recovery day, NREM sleep time was significantly reduced during the first three hours of the DP (ZT12-15: t(168.35) = -3.25, p = 0.003). Thus, increasing N-acylethanolamine signaling with long-lasting inhibition of FAAH increases NREM sleep time, but this does not produce the biphasic effect seen with JZL and CP47.
To ascertain the effect of FAAH inhibition on NREM architecture, we measured the number and duration of NREM bouts at 3 Hr time points across the circadian cycle (Fig 8B). There was an overall interaction for NREM bout duration (treatment x time of day within photoperiod, F(18, 148.23) = 2.79, p < 0.001) and a secondary interaction between treatment and photoperiod (F(2, 75.68) = 3.54, p = 0.034). There was a significant increase in NREM bout duration during the second quarter of the dark photoperiod (ZT15-18: t(135.38) = 2.77, p = .013). The number of NREM bouts was not affected by treatment with AM3506. These findings demonstrate that FAAH inhibition promotes sleep by increasing NREM stability shortly after drug administration.
Similar to JZL, AM3506 reduced REM sleep (Fig 8C). For the percent time spent in REM, there was a nested interaction (time of day within photoperiod, F(6, 139.71) = 7.51, p < 0.001) and main effects of treatment (F(2, 51.93) = 4.399, p = 0.017) and photoperiod (F(1, 112.07) = 227.69, p < 0.001). Overall, AM3506 reduced REM sleep (t(47.65) = -2.75, p = 0.017), specifically during the third quarter of the DP (ZT18-21: t(158.67) = -2.54, p = 0.024) and first quarter of the LP (ZT00-03: t(158.67) = -3.05, p = 0.005). For the duration of REM bouts, there was a nested interaction (time of day within photoperiod, F(6, 132.64) = 3.99, p = 0.001) and main effects of treatment (F(2, 59.13) = 10.66, p < 0.001) and photoperiod (F(1,105.96) = 7.72, p = 0.006). Overall, REM bouts were longer on the recovery day (t(60.507) = 2.74, p = 0.016), but AM3506 reduced REM bout duration across the DP (t(73.75) = -2.70, p = 0.017), specifically during the middle of the DP (ZT15-21: t(170.06) ≤ -2.61, p ≤ 0.020). Finally, for the number of REM bouts, there was a nested interaction (time of day within photoperiod, F(6, 142.95) = 5.23, p < 0.001) and main effects of treatment (F(2, 46.33) = 4.39, p = 0.018) and photoperiod (F(1, 120.03) = 84.76, p < 0.001). The number of REM bouts during the LP was reduced by AM3506 and on the recovery day (t(55.64) ≤ -2.80, p ≤ 0.014), specifically during the third quarter of the LP (ZT06-09: t(139.96) ≤ -2.68, p ≤ 0.017).
EEG Power Spectral Measurements
The results of power spectral analysis of the EEG from the AM3506 experiment were very similar to those obtained following CP47 and JZL administration (S7 Fig). Specifically, increasing AEA tone with AM3506 had modest effects on delta and theta bandwidths, but it reduced gamma power during NREM and REM epochs. Thus, the attenuation of gamma oscillations, particularly during sleep, seems to be a consistent effect of increased eCB signaling.
Blockade of CB1 Fragments NREM Sleep and Substantially Alters Power Spectral Features of the EEG
Sleep Measurements
To determine if eCB/CB1 signaling is necessary for the normal circadian fluctuation in NREM and REM sleep, we performed experiments with the full, selective CB1 antagonist/inverse agonist AM281. Following AM281 administration, there was a substantial fragmentation of NREM sleep and a loss of REM, particularly when this drug was administered prior to the LP (Fig 9). Again, given that eCBs exhibit a circadian fluctuation that differs by brain region, it was not easy to predict a priori an optimal time to administer the drug, so two separate experiments were performed where AM281 was given at opposite points in the circadian phase, immediately before either the LP or DP. In both experiments, two doses of AM281, 0.5 mg/kg (low) and 5.0 mg/kg (high), were administered sequentially on consecutive days following a baseline day when vehicle was given (Fig 10A).
When AM281 was administered prior to the DP (Fig 10B, top row), NREM sleep time was affected by an overall interaction (treatment x time of day within photoperiod, F(18, 198.82) = 10.27, p < 0.001) with a main effect of photoperiod (F(1, 169.19) = 836.77, p < 0.001). Only the high dose of AM281 produced effects on NREM sleep time, and the magnitude of these effects was small. Specifically, there was an increase in NREM sleep time during the first quarter of the DP (t(261.07) = 2.93, p = 0.007) and a decrease during the second quarter (t(261.09) = -3.20, p = 0.003). The effect on NREM bout duration was more pronounced with a significant overall interaction (treatment x time of day within photoperiod, F(18, 201.36) = 7.20, p < 0.001) and main effects of both treatment (F(2, 69.38) = 3.86, p = 0.026) and photoperiod (F(1, 163.20) = 86.17, p < 0.001). Following administration of high dose AM281, there was an overall reduction in NREM bout duration (t(72.79) = -2.64, p = 0.020) with a significant reduction during the second quarter of the DP (ZT15-18: t(205.68) = -3.02, p = 0.006). The number of NREM bouts was also affected by an overall interaction (F(18, 204.63) = 3.46, p < 0.001) with main effects of both treatment (F(2, 61.58) = 3.30, p = 0.043) and photoperiod (F(1, 173.11) = 25.90, p < 0.001). However, this result was largely driven by an increased number of NREM bouts during the first quarter of the DP following high dose AM281 (ZT612-15: t(181.59) = 4.81, p < 0.001). Thus, administration of AM281 prior to the DP yields subtle effects on sleep time but decreases the stability of NREM bouts.
REM sleep was disrupted by AM281, but the effect size was small as baseline REM is normally very low during the DP, when mice are most active (Fig 10B, bottom row). For the percent of time spent in REM, there was a nested interaction (time of day within photoperiod, F(6, 187.08) = 16.15, p < 0.001) and main effects of both treatment (F(2, 85.82) = 4.28, p = 0.017) and photoperiod (F(1, 149.93) = 377.93, p < 0.001). Overall, 5 mg/kg AM281 decreased REM sleep (t(89.77) = -2.92, p = 0.009), particularly during the third quarter of the DP (ZT18-21: t(241.64) = -2.88, p = 0.009). REM bout duration was affected by an overall interaction (treatment x time of day within photoperiod, F(12, 176.73) = 2.60, p = 0.003), nested interaction (time of day within photoperiod, F(6, 169.25) = 6.37, p < 0.001), and main effect of photoperiod (F(1, 151.57) = 24.16, p < 0.001). The 0.5 mg/kg dose of AM281 increased REM bout duration during the second quarter of the DP (ZT15-18: t(179.07) = 3.04, p = 0.005). There was no effect of AM281 on the number of REM bouts.
When AM281 was administered prior to the LP, overall sleep time did not change substantially, but there was a profound fragmentation of NREM (Fig 10C, top row). For NREM sleep time, there was an overall interaction (treatment x time of day within photoperiod, F(18, 146.08) = 8.76, p < 0.001), a secondary interaction (treatment x photoperiod, F(2, 102.87) = 3.31, p = 0.040) and main effects of both treatment (F(2, 81.59) = 3.97, p = 0.023) and photoperiod (F(1, 122.15) = 383.92, p < 0.001). When delivered at the onset of the LP, AM281 increased overall NREM sleep time (t(84.01) = 2.74, p = 0.015), but a comparison between photoperiods found that NREM sleep time was only increased during the DP (low dose: t(91.65) = 2.38, p = 0.039; high dose: t(91.65) = 2.84, p = 0.011). More specifically, there was a significant increase in NREM following high dose AM281 during the first 3 Hr of the DP (ZT12-15: t(191.99) = 3.15, p = 0.004), which was the same point in the circadian cycle when NREM sleep time was increased in the experiment where AM281 was delivered before the DP. For NREM bout duration, there was an overall interaction (treatment x time of day within photoperiod, F(18, 141.98) = 4.74, p < 0.001), a secondary interaction (treatment x photoperiod, F(2, 82.68) = 12.61, p < 0.001), and main effects of both treatment (F(2, 59.88) = 9.86, p < 0.001) and photoperiod (F(1, 109.96) = 31.83, p < 0.001). High dose AM281 substantially reduced NREM bout duration (t(62.07) = -4.42, p < 0.001), particularly during the LP (t(69.68) = -6.23, p < 0.001). More specifically, NREM bout duration was reduced for the first 3 Hr of the LP following low dose AM281 (ZT00-03: t(176.12) = -2.82, p = 0.011) and for the first 9 Hr following high dose AM281 (ZT00-09: t(176.12) ≤ -3.46, p ≤ 0.001). The number of NREM bouts was affected in the opposite manner. There was an overall interaction (treatment x time of day within photoperiod, F(18, 144.093) = 3.266, p < 0.001), a secondary interaction (treatment x photoperiod, F(2, 78.77) = 13.65, p < 0.001), and main effects of both treatment (F(2, 53.14) = 19.99, p < 0.001) and photoperiod (F(1, 113.94) = 148.145, p < 0.001). High dose AM281 increased the number of NREM bouts (t(63.61) = 6.79, p < 0.001) particularly during the first 9 Hr of the LP (ZT00-09: t(159.32) ≥ 4.22, p < 0.001), and both doses increased the number of NREM bouts during the second quarter of the DP (ZT15-18: t(159.32) ≥ 2.31, p ≤ 0.045). Thus, blockade of CB1 receptors greatly fragments NREM sleep, but opposing effects on NREM bout duration and the number of NREM bouts result in subtle changes in total sleep time.
In addition to the effects on NREM sleep, REM sleep time was significantly reduced following AM281 administration prior to the LP (Fig 10C, bottom row). For the percent of time spent in REM sleep, there was a secondary interaction (treatment x photoperiod, F(2, 95.51) = 36.30, p < 0.001), nested interaction (time of day within photoperiod, F(6, 143.275) = 11.15, p < 0.001), and main effects of both treatment (F(2, 74.2) = 21.38, p < 0.001) and photoperiod (F(1, 116.42) = 68.40, p < 0.001). Both low and high dose AM281 reduced REM sleep during the LP (t(84.01) ≤ -4.22, p < 0.001). Low dose AM281 reduced REM sleep during the first 6 Hr of the LP (ZT00-06: t(190.28) ≤ -3.30, p ≤ 0.002), and high dose AM281 reduced REM at all times during the LP (ZT00-12: t(190.28) ≤ -3.40, p ≤ 0.002). For REM bout duration, there was a secondary interaction (treatment x photoperiod, F(2, 81.71) = 7.10, p = 0.001), nested interaction (time of day within photoperiod, F(6, 139.18) = 5.34, p < 0.001), and a main effect of treatment (F(2, 56.07) = 3.35, p = 0.042). The high dose of AM281 reduced REM bout duration during the LP (t(64.52) = -4.05, p < 0.001), particularly during the first 9 Hr of the LP (ZT00-09: t(156.05) ≤ -2.31, p ≤ 0.044). For the number of REM bouts, there was an overall interaction (treatment x time of day within photoperiod, F(12, 133.54) = 1.85, p = 0.046), secondary interaction (treatment x photoperiod, F(2, 68.89) 7.463, p = 0.001), nested interaction (time of day within photoperiod, F(6, 127.15) = 3.99, p = 0.001), and main effects of both treatment (F(2, 46.46) = 6.39, p = 0.004) and photoperiod (F(1, 99.89) = 59.86, p < 0.001). The number of REM bouts were reduced by high dose AM281 during the LP (t(54.99) = -4.49, p < 0.001) and increased by low dose AM281 during the DP (t(54.65) = 2.44, p = 0.036). At the high dose, AM281 reduced the number of REM bouts across most of the LP (ZT00-06 & 09–12: t(162.69) ≤ -2.67, p = 0.002), while low dose AM281 decreased the number of REM bouts during the second quarter of the LP (ZT03-06: t(162.62) = -3.30, p = 0.002) and increased REM bouts during the third quarter of the DP (ZT18-21: t(157.68) = 2.51, p = 0.026). Thus, blockade of CB1 signaling fragments NREM and decreases REM sleep, suggesting that this receptor is necessary for NREM stability.
EEG Power Spectral Measurements
Given that blockade of endocannabinoid signaling through CB1 fragments NREM sleep, we hypothesized that power spectral features associated with sleep might be disrupted after acute administration of CB1 antagonists. Administration of AM281 before the LP had large effects on power spectral features of the EEG across vigilance states, but the nature of these effects was different across states (Fig 11A–11C). Notably, the power of low frequencies (< 8 Hz) was consistently increased, and high frequencies (gamma, > 30 Hz) were much less affected by CB1 blockade. These effects lasted for most of the day, and a similar time course was observed in experiments where AM281 was administered before the DP (S8 Fig), suggesting that this effect is not modulated by circadian processes.
As for we did for CP47, AM3506, and JZL184 we quantified power spectral bandwidths (delta, theta, and gamma) in 3 Hr time bins over the entire recording (Fig 11D–11F). During wake epochs (Fig 11D) there was a significant interaction (treatment x time of day within photoperiod, F(24, 229.63) = 5.26, p < 0.001) with main effects of both treatment (F(3, 73.99) = 42.19, p < 0.001) and photoperiod (F(1, 154.27) = 127.39, p < 0.001) for delta power. Only the 5.0 mg/kg dose significantly elevated wake delta relative to vehicle (t(53.52) = 7.21, p < 0.001), and comparisons at individual time points found that this effect lasted for 18 Hr after drug administration (ZT00-18: t(83.70) > 3.13, p ≤ 0.007). Wake theta power was also modulated by a significant interaction (F(24, 228.72) = 3.23, p < 0.001) and main effects of both treatment (F(3, 69.952) = 20.74, p < 0.001) and photoperiod (F(1, 157.78) = 13.20, p < 0.001). Again, only the high dose of AM281 significantly elevated theta power over the circadian cycle (t(50.532) = 5.35, p < 0.001), and theta power was increased over the first 18 Hr of the recording period (ZT00-18: t(80.45) > 3.97, p < 0.001). Analysis of wake gamma power also found an overall interaction (F(24, 227.65) = 8.013, p < 0.001) with a main effect of photoperiod (F(1, 143.48) = 89.70, p < 0.001), but no pair-wise comparisons between treatment conditions and the vehicle baseline reached significance.
AM281 also altered EEG power spectra during NREM epochs (Fig 11E). For NREM delta power, there was a significant overall interaction (F(24, 229.40) = 9.84, p < 0.001) with a main effect of treatment (F(3, 80.45) = 28.89, p < 0.001). 5 mg/kg AM281 produced an overall increase in NREM delta power (t(58.23) = 5.54, p < 0.001) with pair-wise differences noted across the vast majority of the recording (ZT00-21: t(88.09) ≥ 2.53, p ≤ 0.039). There was also an overall interaction for NREM theta power (F(24, 235.54) = 6.31, p < 0.001) with main effects of both treatment (F(3, 148.62) = 35.06, p < 0.001) and photoperiod (F(1, 135.42) = 5.438, p = 0.21). 5 mg/kg AM281 increased the overall power in the theta bandwidth during NREM sleep (t(118.00) = 5.01, p < 0.001) with specific pair-wise comparisons over the majority of the recording (ZT00-21: t(150.13) ≥ 2.86, p ≤ 0.014). Finally, analysis of NREM gamma found an overall interaction (F(24, 233.59) = 14.55, p < 0.001) with main effects of both treatment (F(3, 121.33) = 7.128, p < 0.001) and photoperiod (F(1, 127.916) = 93.21, p < 0.001), but there were no significant differences between AM281 and vehicle.
The CB1 antagonist also affected EEG spectral content during REM (Fig 11F). For REM delta power, there was an overall interaction (F(24, 217.83) = 1.68, p = 0.028) with a main effect of treatment (F(3, 54.98) = 7.64, p < 0.001). 5.0 mg/kg AM281 increased delta power during REM sleep epochs across the day (t(40.91) = 2.82, p < 0.022), but this was mainly due to increased REM delta during the first 9 Hr (ZT00-09: t(76.78) ≥ 3.54, p ≤ 0.002). For REM theta power, there was a secondary interaction (treatment x photoperiod, F(3, 146.49) = 9.23, p < 0.001) with main effects of both treatment (F(3, 51.66) = 19.22, p < 0.001) and photoperiod (F(1, 219.58) = 18.05, p < 0.001). Overall, 5.0 mg/kg AM281 increased REM theta power (t(39.91) = 5.23, p < 0.001), particularly during the first 15 Hr of the recording (ZT00-15: t(85.58) ≥ 3.92, p ≤ 0.001). There was a treatment x photoperiod interaction for REM gamma power (F(3, 168.32) = 3.61, p = 0.015) with a main effect of photoperiod (F(1, 196.66) = 8.13, p = 0.005). Overall, REM gamma was augmented by the high dose of AM281 during the light photoperiod (t(51.71) = 3.12, p = 0.009), but there were no differences at specific time points. Thus, blockade of CB1 receptors produces broadband changes in the EEG waveform that are particularly evident in lower frequencies irrespective of vigilance state.
eCB Signaling is Not Necessary for Sleep Homeostasis but is Required for the Stability of Rebound Sleep
Measures of EEG delta and theta power are frequently used as an index of sleep drive [44, 50, 51], and according to this interpretation, the large increase in NREM delta power following administration of AM281 may be indicative of augmented sleep homeostatic drive. Thus, we sought to test this possibility by administering AM281 following 6 Hr of total sleep deprivation (TSD) using a rotating disc paradigm (Fig 12A & S1 Fig). Measurements of sleep during the deprivation period clearly show that this method was successful in achieving TSD for 6 Hr (Fig 12B).
Examination of the percent of time subjects spent in NREM sleep (Fig 12C, top) found an overall interaction (group x time of day within photoperiod within experimental phase, F(24, 213.06) = 10.34, p < 0.001) with main effects of experimental phase (F(1, 112,24) = 8.26, p < 0.005) and photoperiod (F(1, 165.42) = 187.28, p < 0.001). In general, subjects engaged in more NREM sleep following sleep deprivation (t(112.24) = 2.88, p = 0.005), indicating a significant NREM rebound. Compared to baseline, the vehicle group spent significantly more time in NREM during the first 3 Hr of recovery (ZT06-09: t(286.99) = 2.40, p = 0.017) and during the first 3 Hr of the DP (ZT12-15: t(286.99) = 3.08, p = 0.002). Similarly, the AM281 group had increased NREM sleep during the first 3 Hr of the DP (ZT12-15: t(286.99) = 1.99, p = 0.47). There were no differences in the amount of NREM sleep between the vehicle and AM281 treatment groups during either baseline or recovery. Additionally, there was no difference between treatment groups in the recovery of the sleep deficit induced by the 6 Hr TSD protocol (S9 Fig). Thus, both groups displayed an augmentation of NREM sleep time following 6 Hr sleep deprivation, and the two groups did not differ in regards to the total amount of sleep obtained during recovery from sleep deprivation.
In contrast, there were significant between-group differences for NREM architecture during recovery from sleep deprivation. For NREM bout duration (stability; Fig 12C, middle), there was an overall interaction (group x time of day within photoperiod within experimental phase, F(24, 213.74) = 8.66, p < 0.001) with main effects of treatment group (F(1, 76.81) = 7.14, p = 0.009) and photoperiod (F(1, 158.63) = 37.26, p < 0.001). The AM281 group had reduced NREM stability compared to the vehicle group (t(76.81) = -2.67, p = 0.009), with specific differences evident at two time points of recovery (ZT06-09: t(256.30) = -3.63, p < 0.001; ZT00-03: t(256.30) = -2.47, p = 0.014). There were no between-group differences during the baseline recordings. Relative to their own baseline, the vehicle group had increased NREM stability during the first 3 Hr of recovery (ZT06-09: t(265.32) = 3.95, p < 0.001), but the AM281 treated mice did not show increased NREM stability following TSD. Rather, the AM281 group had decreased NREM stability during the second half of the recording (ZT18-21 & 00–03: t(265.32) ≤ -1.99, p ≤ 0.048). For the number of NREM bouts (Fig 12C, bottom), there was an overall interaction (group x time of day within photoperiod within experimental phase, F(24, 220.93) = 2.95, p < 0.001) and a main effect of photoperiod (F(1, 174.17) = 17.77, p < 0.001). There were no differences between groups during baseline, but the AM281 group had significantly more NREM bouts than the vehicle group during the first three hours of recovery (ZT06-09: t(169.85) = 2.16, p = 0.032). Relative to their own baseline, the vehicle group increased the number of NREM bouts during the first quarter of the DP (ZT12-15: t(177.13) = 2.20, p = 0.029). These findings show that AM281 blocks the increase in NREM stability during recovery from TSD and increases the number of NREM bouts to compensate.
REM sleep was also affected by TSD with reduced REM early in recovery in both the AM281 and vehicle groups (S10 Fig). Late in the recovery, there was increase in REM sleep in the AM281 treated group due to an increase in the number of REM bouts. Consistent with our previous experiments, blockade of CB1 receptors produced a large increase in low frequency oscillations that occluded any changes due to sleep deprivation (S11 Fig). However, wake theta power did increase during the sleep deprivation period (S11 Fig, panel B). Thus, power spectral results cannot be used as an index of homeostatic drive in this experiment as they are confounded by the baseline effects of CB1 antagonism.
Discussion
The major findings of this work are that eCB signaling through the CB1 receptor is necessary and sufficient for the stability of NREM sleep. Direct activation of CB1 with CP47 or increasing eCB tone with JZL or AM3506 augmented the time spent in NREM primarily due to increased NREM bout length. This suggests that increasing eCB signaling stabilizes the NREM state. Notably these effects were biphasic, where the initial increase in sleep and NREM stability gave way to a secondary reduction and destabilization of NREM bouts suggesting that homeostatic regulation of sleep remained intact. Further support for the role of eCBs in regulating NREM stability comes from experiments with the CB1 antagonist AM281, where blockade of CB1 reduced the duration of NREM bouts. Importantly, this effect was largely offset by a concomitant increase in the number of NREM bouts resulting in minimal changes in overall sleep time. Again, the opposing effects of reduced NREM bout duration and number suggest homeostatic regulation of sleep remains intact in the absence of CB1 signaling. However, CB1 blockade consistently resulted in substantial broadband changes to EEG power spectra, particularly in low frequency bandwidths that are often used as correlates of sleep drive. To test the involvement of eCB signaling in sleep homeostatic processes, we administered AM281 immediately prior to a recovery from TSD. Blockade of CB1 during recovery from TSD did not occlude the rebound in NREM sleep time following TSD, but it did completely block the increased stability (bout duration) of NREM sleep. Unfortunately, the baseline effects of CB1 antagonism on EEG power spectra hobbled our attempts to use this as a metric of sleep drive. Thus, the major conclusion from this work is that eCBs regulate sleep stability, but eCB signaling is not necessary for sleep homeostasis.
Validity and Robustness of State-Scoring Algorithm
In order to perform the series of experiments described herein, we needed to find a way to score mouse polysomnographic data in an accurate and timely manner, so we developed and validated the state-space based approach. Our method was heavily influenced by previous reports, but we modified and extended this work, in part by automating the state-assignment process after state-space coordinates have been defined. Discrete fourier analysis is the basis of this approach, because the state-space coordinates are derived from power spectral ratios similar to those used by others [36, 38, 39, 52, 53]. The use of power spectral ratios (effectively normalizing the FFT) to define state-space coordinates, as opposed to raw power spectral values [54], means that our method of state-scoring is robust to changes in the FFT, and this is evidenced by our experimental results. Activating CB1/eCB signaling with CP47, JZL, or AM3506 produced only modest effects on power spectral measurements, but these drugs had large biphasic effects on measures of sleep time. Importantly, the effects of these drugs on sleep and EEG power spectra were not temporally aligned. In contrast, CB1 blockade consistently produced broadband changes in EEG power spectra that were most evident for low frequency bandwidths, and these effects were evident irrespective of the time of day of drug administration or if CB1 was blocked following TSD. However, CB1 antagonism did not produce substantial effects on total sleep time and the magnitude of effects on sleep architecture varied with time of drug administration. Thus, while CB1 antagonism consistently and substantially alters EEG power spectra, the time course of these effects are not aligned with changes in the sleep score or even sleep architecture. Consequently, the various data sets presented here show large changes in sleep in the absence of substantial changes in state-dependent EEG power spectra (direct and indirect CB1 agonists) and large changes in power spectra in the absence of substantive changes in sleep time (CB1 antagonism). Therefore, we conclude that in addition to performing comparably to human scoring, our vigilance state scoring algorithm is robust to cannabinoid pharmacological manipulations that alter EEG power spectra.
The Biphasic Effects of Augmented eCB Signaling
The early facilitation of NREM sleep seen in the present study following activation of CB1 is largely in agreement with earlier findings [6–11, 13–15, 29, 32], but the secondary (wake promoting/sleep fragmenting) effect of CP47 and JZL to reduce NREM sleep at a time of day when mice normally engage in most of their daily sleep is a novel finding. It should be mentioned that JZL had lower magnitude effects (change from vehicle) on NREM when it was administered before the LP, and did not produce substantial fragmentation of sleep during the subsequent DP. It is unclear why a secondary process was not evident in this experiment, but the acute augmentation of sleep stability was, in this case, synchronized with the normal time of day when mice are inactive. Thus, there was no perturbation in the circadian timing or homeostatic regulation of NREM and wake as in experiments where JZL, CP47, and AM3506 were administered before the DP.
Based on several observations, we hypothesize that the sleep fragmentation during the secondary phase of CP47, JZL, and AM3506 effects is due to a rapid reduction in CB1 signaling. First, JZL selectively increases 2-AG levels via inhibition of MAGL, and both CP47 and 2-AG are highly potent, full agonists at the CB1 receptor [55–57]. In contrast, as an irreversible inhibitor of FAAH, AM3506 increases N-acylethanolamine transmitters for several days [49], and AEA is only a weakly efficacious agonist at CB1 [58]. This discrepancy could explain the difference in the magnitude of effects seen between these drugs. On the other hand, the lack of any effect of URB597 even during the first few hours of the recording is surprising, and it is possible that the early induction of sleep by AM3506 could be due to off-target augmentation of 2-AG. However, administration of exogenous AEA increases NREM sleep [13, 14], and administration of THC, a partial agonist at CB1, also increases NREM sleep (unpublished observation). Thus, partial activation of CB1 with endogenous N-acylethanolamines is likely to have hypnogenic effects. Second, following acute activation, CB1 receptors are rapidly down-regulated on a time scale compatible with the biphasic effects of CP47 and JZL [59, 60]. As these compounds are slowly eliminated in the hours following injection, receptor function will be decreased compared to baseline. Although data on diurnal expression of CB1 are extremely limited, a few studies have provided evidence that CB1 accumulates in brain and neuroendocrine tissue during the DP [61, 62]. Thus, it is possible that diurnal fluctuations in CB1 expression could account for the blunted biphasic effects seen with JZL administration before the LP. Third, NREM fragmentation was also observed in the present study following administration of CB1 antagonists, and CB1 knockout mice also have fragmented sleep [24, 25].
On the Timing of Drug Administration
Although CB1 blockade consistently reduced NREM bout duration, the effects were greatest when AM281 was administered at the onset of the LP. Similarly, JZL had differential effects on sleep depending on the circadian timing of drug administration. When JZL was administered either before the LP or the DP, it increased NREM sleep time and bout duration, but the magnitude of these effects was blunted when the drug was given before the LP. Consequently, the timing of maximal effects for activation and inhibition of eCB signaling peaks at opposite poles of the circadian cycle, suggesting that eCB signaling interacts with or is controlled by circadian processes. These are not mutually exclusive possibilities, and there is evidence supporting both mechanisms. First, cannabinoids alter the light-induced phase-shift in activity in free running rodents [63, 64], suggesting that CB1 can influence entrainment to photic stimulation. Additionally, eCB levels fluctuate over the circadian period in a brain-region dependent manner [33, 34], suggesting that eCB signaling may be under circadian regulation. In fact, levels of AEA and 2-AG are lowest in cortical and hypothalamic tissues at the onset of the DP [33], and in the present work, administration of CB1 antagonists at this time point produced smaller effects on sleep architecture. Thus, our findings provide evidence that changing levels of the molecular components of the eCB system have functional consequences for eCB regulation of vigilance states.
eCBs and EEG Power Spectra
NREM delta power has been used as an index for sleep homeostatic drive because of the correlation of this measure with the difference in cumulative time awake versus asleep [44, 50, 51]. However, as Davis et al [65] point out, some pharmacological manipulations can dissociate alterations in delta power and changes in vigilance states, thus confounding NREM delta power as a metric of sleep homeostatic drive. This situation is clearly applicable to the current dataset, where CB1 antagonists increased delta power across all vigilance states without changing total sleep time. On the contrary, CB1 antagonists fragment NREM sleep, and this may lead one to speculate that the augmentation of delta is a response to poor sleep quality, and therefore, an increase in sleep drive. In fact, sleep fragmentation protocols are associated with increasing homeostatic sleep drive [66–68]. However, evidence from the current dataset argues against the hypothesis that the CB1 antagonist-induced increase in delta power is indicative of an increase in sleep homeostatic processes. First, the time course for augmented delta does not reflect a gradual increase that one would expect if homeostatic sleep drive were building over time with reduced sleep quality. Second, the rebound in total NREM sleep following 6 Hr sleep deprivation was not occluded by CB1 antagonists. Third, blockade of CB1 did not alter the rate of recovery from sleep deprivation.
It should be noted that we did not normalize our power spectral data as in other reports [17, 28, 29, 32], and our presentation of raw power spectral results may explain some discrepancies between our findings and those of others. It is not clear why other researchers have not observed the large augmentation of delta and theta power following administration of CB1 antagonists reported here. In fact, several reports have observed reduced delta and theta power following administration of CB1 antagonists [19, 28] while others have concluded that these drugs have little or no effect on the EEG power spectrum [29, 32]. We can only speculate that the variability in methods used to normalize power spectral bandwidths has contributed to these inconsistencies.
Although we observed no changes in low-frequency oscillations following drugs that activate eCB signaling (JZL, AM3506, CP47), gamma oscillations were consistently reduced, particularly during NREM and REM sleep. This result is consistent with previous reports suggesting reduced gamma power is a robust effect of increased CB1 activation [69–73]. Another consistent effect of cannabinoid agonists is to facilitate high-voltage spindles/spike wave discharges (HVS) [74–76], but in the present study, power spectral analysis found no changes in the HVS bandwidth. This is not surprising given that spindles are rare in C57 mice under baseline conditions [77, 78], so at 3–12 Hr time bins used here, spindle events would only account for a small fraction of the NREM epochs. Thus, it is highly likely that any changes would be averaged out with the analysis performed here. In all cases where cannabinoids have been reported to augment spindles and their respective power spectral correlates [74–76], some prior detection method has been implemented to isolate epochs containing these events before comparing power spectral features across drug conditions. Consequently, no conclusions regarding cannabinoid-induced changes in the incidence of spindles can be derived from the present results.
eCB Signaling and Sleep
Previous reports of CB1 antagonist effects in animal studies of sleep indicated either subtle augmentation of wake at the expense of NREM [15, 19, 28, 29] or no effect [13, 31, 32]. The subtle differences observed with respect to CB1 antagonist effects on sleep time across these studies are likely due to different doses and times of administration. Additionally, most of these studies used relatively short recordings (4–8 Hr). In the reports indicating increased wake time following administration of CB1 antagonists, the arousing properties of these drugs were only observed when summating across the entire recording. On the other hand, fragmentation of NREM has not been reported, but only a few studies have examined metrics of sleep architecture following administration of CB1 antagonists [29, 32]. Studies in CB1 KO mice have found significantly reduced NREM bout duration with an increased number of NREM bouts [24, 25]. REM sleep is consistently reduced following administration of CB1 antagonists [28, 29, 32, 79, 80], and reduced REM sleep is frequently associated with NREM fragmentation [67, 68]. In the present study, reductions in REM sleep time were associated with time points where there was noticeable fragmentation of NREM. Thus, it is likely that NREM fragmentation is a common outcome of CB1 antagonism, and this may give rise to reduced REM.
Importantly, a large reduction in REM sleep was seen following JZL administration before the LP. This finding was not as evident when this drug was administered prior to the DP, likely due to a floor effect as REM is very infrequent the DP. However, some impairment of REM was seen in all experiments in the present study with eCB/CB1 system activation and by others with THC administration [81], suggesting that suppression of REM is a consistent effect of globally increasing eCB signaling. Nevertheless, the similarity between the effect of JZL and AM281 to reduce REM is striking. In contrast to the NREM fragmentation evoked by AM281, facilitating eCB/CB1 signaling stabilizes NREM sleep, and the similar suppression of REM seen with these two opposite manipulations indicates that an optimal level of NREM stabilization must be achieved to allow for REM to emerge. On the neurobiological level, it is possible that the REM suppressing effects of CB1 antagonism and activation arise from different circuit elements controlling vigilance states. This would not be surprising for the eCB system, considering its molecular constituents are widespread throughout structures known to control sleep-wake transitions and considering CB1-activation reduces neurotransmission at both excitatory and inhibitory synapses. However, all drug administrations were performed systemically, so no specific conclusions about neurobiological loci can be drawn from the results presented herein.
According to the influential two-process model, sleep timing is regulated by interactions between homeostatic and circadian processes, where homeostatic control of sleep is defined as a mechanism that increases the propensity to sleep as a function of the amount of time spent awake [44, 51]. Changes in the amount of sleep arise from modulation of one of these two processes. Our data do not support a role for eCB signaling in sleep homeostasis. First, CB1 activation causes a biphasic response in NREM sleep time, increasing sleep during the DP and reducing it during the LP. This reduction in sleep time during the LP (secondary response) is the expected homeostatic response to increased sleep during the DP. Second, blockade of CB1 signaling does not substantially alter sleep time. While the duration of NREM bouts is reduced by CB1 antagonism, the compensatory increase in the number of bouts argues for an intact homeostatic mechanism. Finally, there was not a robust effect of CB1 blockade on rebound sleep time or the amount of sleep recovered following TSD. Consequently, we conclude that it is unlikely that eCB signaling is an essential component of sleep homeostatic machinery.
Relevance and Future Directions
In both rodents and humans, chronic cannabinoid administration produces down-regulation of CB1 signaling [82, 83], and although sleep disturbances are a central feature of cannabis withdrawal in humans [84], there is marked paucity of objective studies describing exactly how sleep is disrupted much less a mechanism for these disruptions [85]. Interestingly, insomnia and poor sleep quality were commonly reported adverse effects in clinical trials with the CB1 antagonist rimonabant, but again, no objective measures of sleep were obtained [22, 23]. The rimonabant trial and trials with other CB1 antagonists were terminated due to the increased depression, anxiety, and suicidality that was associated with these drugs, so it is unlikely that any data pertaining to how CB1 antagonists affect human sleep will be forthcoming in the near future. Nevertheless, the self-reports of sleep disturbances from these clinical trials are interesting considering the association between insomnia and depression [86]. Our findings demonstrate that eCB signaling is necessary and sufficient for the control of sleep stability, but this neurotransmitter system is not necessary for sleep homeostasis.
Supporting Information
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by the United States National Institutes of Health. DML and MJP received support from the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism's Division of Intramural Clinical and Biological Research, award number ZIA AA000416 (http://www.niaaa.nih.gov/). AM received support from National Institute on Drug Abuse Grant DA003801 (http://www.drugabuse.gov/). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript. We wish to thank Dr. Shih-Chieh Lin (NIA, NIH) for his advice on the computational data analysis used here and for his feedback on this manuscript. Additionally, we wish to thank Dr. John J. Woodward (Medical University of South Carolina, Charleston, SC) and Dr. Igor Timofeev (Université Laval, Québec, Canada) for their comments during the preparation of this manuscript.
Data Availability
All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.
Funding Statement
This work was supported by the United States National Institutes of Health. DML and MJP received support from the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism's Division of Intramural Clinical and Biological Research, award number ZIA AA000416 (http://www.niaaa.nih.gov/). AM received support from National Institute on Drug Abuse Grant DA003801 (http://www.drugabuse.gov/). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1.Russo E. Cannabis in India: ancient lore and modern medicine. In: Mechoulam R, editor. Cannabinoids as Therapeutics. Milestones in drug therapy. Basel; Boston: Birkhäuser; 2005. p. 1–22.
- 2.O'Shaughnessy WB. On the Preparations of the Indian Hemp, or Gunjah: Cannabis Indica Their Effects on the Animal System in Health, and their Utility in the Treatment of Tetanus and other Convulsive Diseases. Prov Med J Retrosp Med Sci. 1843;5(123):363–9. PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC2490264. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Wallich GC. Cannabis Indica. Br Med J. 1883;1(1173):1224 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Bradbury JB. The Croonian Lectures on some Points Connected with Sleep, Sleeplessness, and Hypnotics: Delivered before the Royal College of Physicians of London. Br Med J. 1899;2(2011):134–8. . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Clendinning J. Observations on the medicinal properties of the Cannabis Sativa of India. Medico-chirurgical transactions. 1843;26:188–210. PMC2116906. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Moreton JE, Davis WM. Electroencephalographic study of the effects of tetrahydrocannabinols on sleep in the rat. Neuropharmacology. 1973;12(9):897–907. Epub 1973/09/01. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Watanabe K, Narimatsu S, Yamamoto I, Yoshimura H. Difference in tolerance development of hypothermia and pentobarbital-induced sleep prolongating effect of 11-hydroxy-delta 8-tetrahydrocannabinol and 11-oxo-delta 8-tetrahydrocannabinol in mice. European journal of pharmacology. 1982;77(1):53–6. Epub 1982/01/08. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Watanabe K, Narimatsu S, Yamamoto I, Yoshimura H. Cross-tolerance development to the prolongation of pentobarbitone-induced sleep by delta 8-tetrahydrocannabinol and 11-hydroxy-delta 8-tetrahydrocannabinol in mice. The Journal of pharmacy and pharmacology. 1987;39(11):945–7. Epub 1987/11/01. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Zarcone VP Jr. Marijuana and ethanol: effects on sleep. Int J Psychiatry Med. 1973;4(2):201–12. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Fujimori M, Himwich HE. Delta 9-tetrahydrocannabinol and the sleep-wakefulness cycle in rabbits. Physiology & behavior. 1973;11(3):291–5. Epub 1973/09/01. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Wallach MB, Gershon S. The effects of delta8-THC on the EEG, reticular multiple unit activity and sleep of cats. European journal of pharmacology. 1973;24(2):172–8. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Kano M, Ohno-Shosaku T, Hashimotodani Y, Uchigashima M, Watanabe M. Endocannabinoid-mediated control of synaptic transmission. Physiol Rev. 2009;89(1):309–80. 10.1152/physrev.00019.2008 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Murillo-Rodriguez E, Cabeza R, Mendez-Diaz M, Navarro L, Prospero-Garcia O. Anandamide-induced sleep is blocked by SR141716A, a CB1 receptor antagonist and by U73122, a phospholipase C inhibitor. Neuroreport. 2001;12(10):2131–6. Epub 2001/07/12. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Murillo-Rodriguez E, Sanchez-Alavez M, Navarro L, Martinez-Gonzalez D, Drucker-Colin R, Prospero-Garcia O. Anandamide modulates sleep and memory in rats. Brain research. 1998;812(1–2):270–4. Epub 1998/11/14. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Murillo-Rodriguez E, Blanco-Centurion C, Sanchez C, Piomelli D, Shiromani PJ. Anandamide enhances extracellular levels of adenosine and induces sleep: an in vivo microdialysis study. Sleep. 2003;26(8):943–7. Epub 2004/01/30. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Rueda-Orozco PE, Soria-Gomez E, Montes-Rodriguez CJ, Perez-Morales M, Prospero-Garcia O. Intrahippocampal administration of anandamide increases REM sleep. Neuroscience letters. 2010;473(2):158–62. Epub 2010/03/02. 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.02.044 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Murillo-Rodriguez E, Palomero-Rivero M, Millan-Aldaco D, Arias-Carrion O, Drucker-Colin R. Administration of URB597, oleoylethanolamide or palmitoylethanolamide increases waking and dopamine in rats. PloS one. 2011;6(7):e20766 10.1371/journal.pone.0020766 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Murillo-Rodriguez E, Vazquez E, Millan-Aldaco D, Palomero-Rivero M, Drucker-Colin R. Effects of the fatty acid amide hydrolase inhibitor URB597 on the sleep-wake cycle, c-Fos expression and dopamine levels of the rat. European journal of pharmacology. 2007;562(1–2):82–91. Epub 2007/03/06. 10.1016/j.ejphar.2007.01.076 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Murillo-Rodriguez E, Millan-Aldaco D, Di Marzo V, Drucker-Colin R. The anandamide membrane transporter inhibitor, VDM-11, modulates sleep and c-Fos expression in the rat brain. Neuroscience. 2008;157(1):1–11. Epub 2008/09/30. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.08.056 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Murillo-Rodriguez E, Palomero-Rivero M, Millan-Aldaco D, Di Marzo V. The administration of endocannabinoid uptake inhibitors OMDM-2 or VDM-11 promotes sleep and decreases extracellular levels of dopamine in rats. Physiology & behavior. 2013;109:88–95. Epub 2012/12/15. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2012.11.007 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Huitron-Resendiz S, Sanchez-Alavez M, Wills DN, Cravatt BF, Henriksen SJ. Characterization of the sleep-wake patterns in mice lacking fatty acid amide hydrolase. Sleep. 2004;27(5):857–65. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Nathan PJ, O'Neill BV, Napolitano A, Bullmore ET. Neuropsychiatric adverse effects of centrally acting antiobesity drugs. CNS Neurosci Ther. 2011;17(5):490–505. 10.1111/j.1755-5949.2010.00172.x . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Steinberg BA, Cannon CP. Cannabinoid-1 receptor blockade in cardiometabolic risk reduction: safety, tolerability, and therapeutic potential. Am J Cardiol. 2007;100(12A):27P–32P. 10.1016/j.amjcard.2007.10.011 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Pava MJ, den Hartog CR, Blanco-Centurion C, Shiromani PJ, Woodward JJ. Endocannabinoid modulation of cortical up-states and NREM sleep. PloS one. 2014;9(2):e88672 Epub 2014/02/13. 10.1371/journal.pone.0088672 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Silvani A, Berteotti C, Bastianini S, Lo Martire V, Mazza R, Pagotto U, et al. Multiple sleep alterations in mice lacking cannabinoid type 1 receptors. PloS one. 2014;9(2):e89432 Epub 2014/03/04. 10.1371/journal.pone.0089432 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Zimmer A, Zimmer AM, Hohmann AG, Herkenham M, Bonner TI. Increased mortality, hypoactivity, and hypoalgesia in cannabinoid CB1 receptor knockout mice. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 1999;96(10):5780–5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Wu CS, Zhu J, Wager-Miller J, Wang S, O'Leary D, Monory K, et al. Requirement of cannabinoid CB(1) receptors in cortical pyramidal neurons for appropriate development of corticothalamic and thalamocortical projections. The European journal of neuroscience. 2010;32(5):693–706. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07337.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Santucci V, Storme JJ, Soubrie P, Le Fur G. Arousal-enhancing properties of the CB1 cannabinoid receptor antagonist SR 141716A in rats as assessed by electroencephalographic spectral and sleep-waking cycle analysis. Life sciences. 1996;58(6):PL103–10. Epub 1996/01/01. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Goonawardena AV, Plano A, Robinson L, Ross R, Greig I, Pertwee RG, et al. Modulation of food consumption and sleep-wake cycle in mice by the neutral CB1 antagonist ABD459. Behav Pharmacol. 2015;26(3):289–303. 10.1097/FBP.0000000000000108 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Perez-Morales M, Alvarado-Capuleno I, Lopez-Colome AM, Mendez-Diaz M, Ruiz-Contreras AE, Prospero-Garcia O. Activation of PAR1 in the lateral hypothalamus of rats enhances food intake and REMS through CB1R. Neuroreport. 2012;23(14):814–8. Epub 2012/08/15. 10.1097/WNR.0b013e328357615a . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Mendelson WB, Basile AS. The hypnotic actions of oleamide are blocked by a cannabinoid receptor antagonist. Neuroreport. 1999;10(15):3237–9. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Goonawardena AV, Plano A, Robinson L, Platt B, Hampson RE, Riedel G. A Pilot Study into the Effects of the CB1 Cannabinoid Receptor Agonist WIN55,212–2 or the Antagonist/Inverse Agonist AM251 on Sleep in Rats. Sleep disorders. 2011;2011:178469 Epub 2011/01/01. 10.1155/2011/178469 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Liedhegner ES, Sasman A, Hillard CJ. Brain region-specific changes in N-acylethanolamine contents with time of day. J Neurochem. 2014;128(4):491–506. 10.1111/jnc.12495 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Valenti M, Vigano D, Casico MG, Rubino T, Steardo L, Parolaro D, et al. Differential diurnal variations of anandamide and 2-arachidonoyl-glycerol levels in rat brain. Cell Mol Life Sci. 2004;61(7–8):945–50. 10.1007/s00018-003-3453-5 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.National Research Council Committee for the Update of the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals, Institute for Laboratory Animal Research, National Academies Press. Guide for the care and use of laboratory animals 8th ed. Washington, D.C.: National Academies Press; 2011. xxv, 220 p. p. [Google Scholar]
- 36.Gervasoni D, Lin SC, Ribeiro S, Soares ES, Pantoja J, Nicolelis MA. Global forebrain dynamics predict rat behavioral states and their transitions. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2004;24(49):11137–47. Epub 2004/12/14. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3524-04.2004 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Mang GM, Franken P. Sleep and EEG Phenotyping in Mice. Curr Protoc Mouse Biol. 2012;2(1):55–74. 10.1002/9780470942390.mo110126 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Diniz Behn CG, Klerman EB, Mochizuki T, Lin SC, Scammell TE. Abnormal sleep/wake dynamics in orexin knockout mice. Sleep. 2010;33(3):297–306. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Bastianini S, Berteotti C, Gabrielli A, Del Vecchio F, Amici R, Alexandre C, et al. SCOPRISM: a new algorithm for automatic sleep scoring in mice. J Neurosci Methods. 2014;235:277–84. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2014.07.018 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Weber F, Chung S, Beier KT, Xu M, Luo L, Dan Y. Control of REM sleep by ventral medulla GABAergic neurons. Nature. 2015;526(7573):435–8. 10.1038/nature14979 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Srinivasan BV, Qi H, Duraiswami R. GPUML: Graphical processors for speeding up kernel machines. Siam Conference on Data Mining; April 2010; Columbus, OH2010.
- 42.Vaughn LK, Denning G, Stuhr KL, de Wit H, Hill MN, Hillard CJ. Endocannabinoid signalling: has it got rhythm? British journal of pharmacology. 2010;160(3):530–43. Epub 2010/07/02. 10.1111/j.1476-5381.2010.00790.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Vyazovskiy VV, Riedner BA, Cirelli C, Tononi G. Sleep homeostasis and cortical synchronization: II. A local field potential study of sleep slow waves in the rat. Sleep. 2007;30(12):1631–42. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Borbely AA, Achermann P. Sleep homeostasis and models of sleep regulation. J Biol Rhythms. 1999;14(6):557–68. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Kim T, Ramesh V, Dworak M, Choi DS, McCarley RW, Kalinchuk AV, et al. Disrupted sleep-wake regulation in type 1 equilibrative nucleoside transporter knockout mice. Neuroscience. 2015;303:211–9. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.06.037 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Jacobs J. Hippocampal theta oscillations are slower in humans than in rodents: implications for models of spatial navigation and memory. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2014;369(1635):20130304 10.1098/rstb.2013.0304 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Fries P. Neuronal gamma-band synchronization as a fundamental process in cortical computation. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2009;32:209–24. 10.1146/annurev.neuro.051508.135603 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Fegley D, Gaetani S, Duranti A, Tontini A, Mor M, Tarzia G, et al. Characterization of the fatty acid amide hydrolase inhibitor cyclohexyl carbamic acid 3'-carbamoyl-biphenyl-3-yl ester (URB597): effects on anandamide and oleoylethanolamide deactivation. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 2005;313(1):352–8. 10.1124/jpet.104.078980 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Gunduz-Cinar O, MacPherson KP, Cinar R, Gamble-George J, Sugden K, Williams B, et al. Convergent translational evidence of a role for anandamide in amygdala-mediated fear extinction, threat processing and stress-reactivity. Mol Psychiatry. 2013;18(7):813–23. 10.1038/mp.2012.72 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Dijk DJ, Brunner DP, Beersma DG, Borbely AA. Electroencephalogram power density and slow wave sleep as a function of prior waking and circadian phase. Sleep. 1990;13(5):430–40. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Borbely AA. A two process model of sleep regulation. Hum Neurobiol. 1982;1(3):195–204. . [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Dzirasa K, Ribeiro S, Costa R, Santos LM, Lin SC, Grosmark A, et al. Dopaminergic control of sleep-wake states. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2006;26(41):10577–89. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1767-06.2006 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Lin SC, Gervasoni D. Defining Global Brain States Using Multielectrode Field Potential Recordings. In: Nicolelis MAL, editor. Methods for Neural Ensemble Recordings. Frontiers in Neuroscience. 2nd ed. Boca Raton (FL)2008. [PubMed]
- 54.Sunagawa GA, Sei H, Shimba S, Urade Y, Ueda HR. FASTER: an unsupervised fully automated sleep staging method for mice. Genes to cells: devoted to molecular & cellular mechanisms. 2013;18(6):502–18. 10.1111/gtc.12053 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Savinainen JR, Jarvinen T, Laine K, Laitinen JT. Despite substantial degradation, 2-arachidonoylglycerol is a potent full efficacy agonist mediating CB(1) receptor-dependent G-protein activation in rat cerebellar membranes. British journal of pharmacology. 2001;134(3):664–72. 10.1038/sj.bjp.0704297 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Melvin LS, Milne GM, Johnson MR, Subramaniam B, Wilken GH, Howlett AC. Structure-activity relationships for cannabinoid receptor-binding and analgesic activity: studies of bicyclic cannabinoid analogs. Mol Pharmacol. 1993;44(5):1008–15. . [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Compton DR, Johnson MR, Melvin LS, Martin BR. Pharmacological profile of a series of bicyclic cannabinoid analogs: classification as cannabimimetic agents. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 1992;260(1):201–9. . [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Mackie K, Devane WA, Hille B. Anandamide, an endogenous cannabinoid, inhibits calcium currents as a partial agonist in N18 neuroblastoma cells. Mol Pharmacol. 1993;44(3):498–503. . [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Garzon J, de la Torre-Madrid E, Rodriguez-Munoz M, Vicente-Sanchez A, Sanchez-Blazquez P. Gz mediates the long-lasting desensitization of brain CB1 receptors and is essential for cross-tolerance with morphine. Mol Pain. 2009;5:11 10.1186/1744-8069-5-11 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Zhuang S, Kittler J, Grigorenko EV, Kirby MT, Sim LJ, Hampson RE, et al. Effects of long-term exposure to delta9-THC on expression of cannabinoid receptor (CB1) mRNA in different rat brain regions. Brain Res Mol Brain Res. 1998;62(2):141–9. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Martinez-Vargas M, Morales-Gomez J, Gonzalez-Rivera R, Hernandez-Enriquez C, Perez-Arredondo A, Estrada-Rojo F, et al. Does the neuroprotective role of anandamide display diurnal variations? Int J Mol Sci. 2013;14(12):23341–55. 10.3390/ijms141223341 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Yasuo S, Koch M, Schmidt H, Ziebell S, Bojunga J, Geisslinger G, et al. An endocannabinoid system is localized to the hypophysial pars tuberalis of Syrian hamsters and responds to photoperiodic changes. Cell Tissue Res. 2010;340(1):127–36. 10.1007/s00441-010-0930-7 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Acuna-Goycolea C, Obrietan K, van den Pol AN. Cannabinoids excite circadian clock neurons. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2010;30(30):10061–6. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5838-09.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Sanford AE, Castillo E, Gannon RL. Cannabinoids and hamster circadian activity rhythms. Brain research. 2008;1222:141–8. 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.05.048 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Davis CJ, Clinton JM, Jewett KA, Zielinski MR, Krueger JM. Delta wave power: an independent sleep phenotype or epiphenomenon? J Clin Sleep Med. 2011;7(5 Suppl):S16–8. 10.5664/JCSM.1346 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Baud MO, Magistretti PJ, Petit JM. Sustained sleep fragmentation induces sleep homeostasis in mice. Sleep. 2015;38(4):567–79. 10.5665/sleep.4572 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.He J, Kastin AJ, Wang Y, Pan W. Sleep fragmentation has differential effects on obese and lean mice. J Mol Neurosci. 2015;55(3):644–52. 10.1007/s12031-014-0403-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Ringgold KM, Barf RP, George A, Sutton BC, Opp MR. Prolonged sleep fragmentation of mice exacerbates febrile responses to lipopolysaccharide. J Neurosci Methods. 2013;219(1):104–12. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2013.07.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Hajos M, Hoffmann WE, Kocsis B. Activation of cannabinoid-1 receptors disrupts sensory gating and neuronal oscillation: relevance to schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;63(11):1075–83. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.12.005 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Hajos N, Katona I, Naiem SS, MacKie K, Ledent C, Mody I, et al. Cannabinoids inhibit hippocampal GABAergic transmission and network oscillations. The European journal of neuroscience. 2000;12(9):3239–49. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Kucewicz MT, Tricklebank MD, Bogacz R, Jones MW. Dysfunctional prefrontal cortical network activity and interactions following cannabinoid receptor activation. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2011;31(43):15560–8. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2970-11.2011 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Cortes-Briones J, Skosnik PD, Mathalon D, Cahill J, Pittman B, Williams A, et al. Delta(9)-THC Disrupts Gamma (gamma)-Band Neural Oscillations in Humans. Neuropsychopharmacology: official publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology. 2015;40(9):2124–34. 10.1038/npp.2015.53 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Robbe D, Montgomery SM, Thome A, Rueda-Orozco PE, McNaughton BL, Buzsaki G. Cannabinoids reveal importance of spike timing coordination in hippocampal function. Nat Neurosci. 2006;9(12):1526–33. 10.1038/nn1801 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Buonamici M, Young GA, Khazan N. Effects of acute delta 9-THC administration on EEG and EEG power spectra in the rat. Neuropharmacology. 1982;21(8):825–9. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Sales-Carbonell C, Rueda-Orozco PE, Soria-Gomez E, Buzsaki G, Marsicano G, Robbe D. Striatal GABAergic and cortical glutamatergic neurons mediate contrasting effects of cannabinoids on cortical network synchrony. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2013;110(2):719–24. Epub 2012/12/28. 10.1073/pnas.1217144110 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Turkanis SA, Karler R. Central excitatory properties of delta 9-tetrahydrocannabinol and its metabolites in iron-induced epileptic rats. Neuropharmacology. 1982;21(1):7–13. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Kim D, Hwang E, Lee M, Sung H, Choi JH. Characterization of topographically specific sleep spindles in mice. Sleep. 2015;38(1):85–96. 10.5665/sleep.4330 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Ryan LJ. Characterization of cortical spindles in DBA/2 and C57BL/6 inbred mice. Brain Res Bull. 1984;13(4):549–58. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Perez-Morales M, De La Herran-Arita AK, Mendez-Diaz M, Ruiz-Contreras AE, Drucker-Colin R, Prospero-Garcia O. 2-AG into the lateral hypothalamus increases REM sleep and cFos expression in melanin concentrating hormone neurons in rats. Pharmacology, biochemistry, and behavior. 2013;108:1–7. Epub 2013/04/23. 10.1016/j.pbb.2013.04.006 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Jacobson LH, Commerford SR, Gerber SP, Chen YA, Dardik B, Chaperon F, et al. Characterization of a novel, brain-penetrating CB1 receptor inverse agonist: metabolic profile in diet-induced obese models and aspects of central activity. Naunyn-Schmiedeberg's archives of pharmacology. 2011;384(6):565–81. Epub 2011/09/29. 10.1007/s00210-011-0686-y . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Feinberg I, Jones R, Walker J, Cavness C, Floyd T. Effects of marijuana extract and tetrahydrocannabinol on electroencephalographic sleep patterns. Clin Pharmacol Ther. 1976;19(6):782–94. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Ceccarini J, Kuepper R, Kemels D, van Os J, Henquet C, Van Laere K. [18F]MK-9470 PET measurement of cannabinoid CB1 receptor availability in chronic cannabis users. Addict Biol. 2015;20(2):357–67. 10.1111/adb.12116 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Sim LJ, Hampson RE, Deadwyler SA, Childers SR. Effects of chronic treatment with delta9-tetrahydrocannabinol on cannabinoid-stimulated [35S]GTPgammaS autoradiography in rat brain. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 1996;16(24):8057–66. . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5 5th ed. Washington, D.C.: American Psychiatric Association; 2013. xliv, 947 p. p. [Google Scholar]
- 85.Gates P, Albertella L, Copeland J. Cannabis withdrawal and sleep: A systematic review of human studies. Subst Abus. 2015:0 10.1080/08897077.2015.1023484 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Fernandez-Mendoza J, Shea S, Vgontzas AN, Calhoun SL, Liao D, Bixler EO. Insomnia and incident depression: role of objective sleep duration and natural history. J Sleep Res. 2015;24(4):390–8. 10.1111/jsr.12285 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.