Abstract
Subjective memory complaints (SMCs) are common in older adults that can often predict further cognitive impairment. No proven effective agents are available for SMCs. The effect of BrainPower Advanced, a dietary supplement consisting of herbal extracts, nutrients, and vitamins, was evaluated in 98 volunteers with SMCs, averaging 67 years of age (47–88), in a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial. Subjective hypomnesis/memory loss (SML) and attention/concentration deficits (SAD) were evaluated before and after 12-week supplementation of BrainPower Advanced capsules (n = 47) or placebo (n = 51), using a 5-point memory questionnaire (1 = no/slight, 5 = severe). Objective memory function was evaluated using 3 subtests of visual/audio memory, abstraction, and memory recall that gave a combined total score. The BrainPower Advanced group had more cases of severe SML (severity ⩾ 3) (44/47) and severe SAD (43/47) than the placebo group (39/51 and 37/51, < 0.05, < 0.05, resp.) before the treatment. BrainPower Advanced intervention, however, improved a greater proportion of the severe SML (29.5%)(13/44) (P < 0.01) and SAD (34.9%)(15/43)(P < 0.01) than placebo (5.1% (2/39) and 13.5% (5/37), resp.). Thus, 3-month BrainPower Advanced supplementation appears to be beneficial to older adults with SMCs.
1. Introduction
Memory is the ability of an individual to record, retain, and recall sensory stimuli, events, and information over short and long periods of time. Deficits in memory function can compromise one's quality of life and ability to work.
Hypomnesis/forgetfulness/memory decline can occur with aging or as results of subhealth conditions [1]. Complaints about memory impairment, or subjective memory complaints (SMCs), are common in elderly people but are rarely detected by clinicians using objective memory function tests due to the subtle and heterogeneous nature of SMC [2–5]. SMCs are a criterion of mild cognitive impairment (MCI), which is common in older adults and in people who have experienced subhealth conditions [6–12].
MCI is an intermediary status between normal aging and prodromal memory decline [6, 13]. Individuals with MCI appear to have intact general cognitive function and activities of daily living, but their memory is impaired for normal age. The prevalence of MCI is estimated at 3%–19% in adults older than 65 years and 15% in adults older than 75 years [14–18]. Less than half of people with MCI are stable or able to reverse back to normal memory function again within 5 years [19–21].
SMC and MCI are often associated with a decline in episodic memory (a recollection of specific past events) [22–24], which is most frequently found in those with the amnesic subtype MCI [22–28]. Positron emission tomography (PET) brain image studies show that people with SMC are characterized with elevated brain beta-amyloid [25–27]. Increased amyloid deposition has an early and subclinical impact on cognition that precedes hypometabolism [28] and impairs blood vessel functions [29] that could contribute to increased inflammation in amnestic mild cognitive impairment [30].
Latent insufficiency of cerebrovascular circulation and loss of phospholipid asymmetry may underlie early manifestation of SMC and MCI [31] and could be a risk factor for episodic nonspecific complaints of mild cognitive deficit, regional hypoperfusion, and hypometabolism [32–38].
Episodic memory processes depend on the integrity of the medial temporal lobe, hippocampus, the posterior parietal cortex, and lateral prefrontal cortex (PFC) [33–35]. Imaging studies have shown an asymmetric hemispheric encoding/retrieval (HERA) pattern in young adults where the left PFC and temporo-occipital cortex are involved in encoding and the right PFC is involved in retrieval of the stored information [36–39]. During normal aging, PFC activation becomes less asymmetric during memory tasks. Normal or high-performing older adults balance age-related neural decline through neuroplasticity, which reorganizes neurocognitive networks. The subnormal or low-performing older adults use a network similar to young adults, but inefficiently [36, 40].
Abnormal cholinergic and glutamatergic neurotransmissions are thought to be involved in SMC and MCI [41–43]. Cognitive decline in older adults is associated with a loss of cholinergic function (cholinergic hypofunction) including a reduction in choline acetyltransferase (ChAT), muscarinic and nicotinic acetylcholine receptor binding sites, and concentrations of acetylcholine in the synaptic clefts [43, 44]. Glutamatergic overstimulation (excitotoxicity) of the postsynaptic NMDA receptors could also lead to memory impairment [45].
Currently, there are no effective and safe pharmaceutical drugs for SMC and MCI. Prevention of the progression of the symptomatic development could be the best strategy [46]. Although acetylcholinesterase inhibitors (AChEIs) and N-methyl D-aspartate (NMDA) receptor antagonists have been used for treatment of people with varying degree of memory deficits, their effects on cognition and memory improvement are often negative [47–54]. A portion of people with MCI, however, may respond favorably to AChEIs (15%–35%) and NMDA receptor antagonists (30%) [55–57], but usually after high doses or long treatment, and with potential adverse effects, such as nausea, vomiting, diarrhea, headache, hypertension, and hepatotoxicity [58–64].
A variety of plant-derived compounds have been studied as potential enhancers of memory and cognitive function [65] with potential mechanisms on (1) modulation of neuronal membrane integrity; (2) modulation of the cholinergic system through inhibition of acetylcholinesterase (AChE) or stimulation of muscarinic and nicotinic receptors; (3) neuroprotection against NMDA receptor excitotoxicity; (4) anti-inflammatory and antioxidant activities; (5) improved cerebral blood flow and microperfusion.
Table 1 lists some herbal extracts and compounds that are active ingredients of BrainPower Advanced, the formulation used in this pilot study. BrainPower Advanced is a dietary supplement formulated to support healthy memory and cognitive function in adults. Its key ingredients include extracts of Ginkgo biloba (flavonoids, terpenoids, and terpene lactones), Camellia sinensis leaf (tea polyphenols), Catharanthus roseus (vinpocetine), kola nut (Cola nitida, caffeine), Huperzia serrata (hup A and hup B), phosphatidylserine (PS), L-tyrosine, L-pyroglutamic acid, acetyl-L-carnitine, choline bitartrate, L-glutamine, L-phenylalanine, L-cysteine, vitamin B6, and vitamin B12 (Table 1).
Table 1.
Ingredients | Main active compounds | Effects and possible mechanisms of actions |
---|---|---|
Phosphatidylserine | Phosphatidylserine | Improves cognitive performance in elderly adults with memory deficits |
Enhances cognitive performance in school children and adults | ||
Restores impaired neuronal calcium and glucose uptake and metabolism in aged brain | ||
Precursor of neuronal membrane phospholipid that is responsible for neuroplasticity, learning, and memory | ||
Neuroprotection | ||
| ||
Catharanthus roseus | Vinpocetine | Enhance memory function in young healthy volunteers and in animals |
Protect against ischemia by improving blood perfusion and cerebral blood flow | ||
Increase glucose and oxygen consumption, cerebral ATP, and cAMP levels | ||
Improve cerebral microcirculation by inhibiting platelet aggregation | ||
Reduce red blood cell deformability and cerebral vascular resistance | ||
Enhance neurotransmitter production, release, and concentration in the brain | ||
Block voltage-gated sodium channels and potentiate the neuroprotective effect of adenosine in hypoxia | ||
| ||
Huperzia serrata (whole plant) | Huperzine A Huperzine B |
Inhibitor of AChE and NMDAR |
Inhibitor of b-amyloid neurotoxicity | ||
Strong antioxidative, antiapoptotic, and neuroprotective activities | ||
Improve cognition in healthy people | ||
Reverse or attenuate cognitive deficits in older adults | ||
| ||
Ginkgo biloba | Flavonoids Terpenoids Terpene lactones (ginkgolides and bilobalide) |
Large dose may improve cognition, daily living activities, and mood |
Dose-dependent and specific enhancing effects on memory, cognitive performance, and alertness in healthy adults | ||
Delay cognitive decline in elderly population | ||
Potentiate the cognitive-enhancing effects of phosphatidylserine | ||
Memory improving effect in older people with memory deficits | ||
Increase blood supply, vasodilation, reduce blood viscosity, balance neurotransmitter systems, and reduce free radicals | ||
Inhibitor of platelet activating factor | ||
| ||
Vitamin B6 | Pyridoxal, pyridoxamine, and pyridoxine | Homocysteine remethylation cofactor |
Reduce blood homocysteine level which is a risk factor for cerebrovascular disease and neuron toxicity | ||
| ||
Vitamin B12 | Required for the methylation of homocysteine to methionine and needed for myelin, neurotransmitters, and membrane phospholipids for maintaining the integrity of the central nervous system | |
Protects against brain atrophy | ||
Protects mood and memory function | ||
| ||
L-Tyrosine | L-Tyrosine | Promotes protein utilization and enhances IgG antibody induction |
| ||
L-Pyroglutamic acid | L-Pyroglutamic acid | Makes N-terminal modification in neuronal peptides, hormones and peptides, and analogue/reservoir of glutamate |
| ||
Green tea extract (Camellia sinensis, leaf) |
Tea polyphenols (epigallocatechin gallate) | Free-radical scavengers, strong antioxidants, and neuroprotection |
Anti-inflammatory; improve vasodilation and normal blood pressure, normal glucose, and lipid metabolism | ||
| ||
Acetyl-L-carnitine | Acetyl-L-carnitine | Regulates neuroplasticity, membrane function, and neurotransmitter release; reduces pain and depression activity at cholinergic neurons; membrane stabilization; and enhancing mitochondrial function |
| ||
Cola nut extract (kola nitida) | Caffeine Theobromine Theophylline |
Decrease brain beta-amyloid |
| ||
Choline bitartrate | A precursor of acetylcholine, a cholinergic neurotransmitter that declines with advancing age | |
Improves auditory and visual word recognition at a dose of 12 g per day for 2 weeks | ||
| ||
L-Glutamine | Reduces beta-amyloid and H2O2-induced stress and DNA damage | |
| ||
L-Phenylalanine | An essential amino acid that can be converted to tyrosine and other excitatory neurotransmitters (dopamine, norepinephrine, and epinephrine) | |
| ||
L-Cysteine | A precursor of the antioxidant glutathione and a flavor |
PS is a key integrative phospholipid component of neuronal cell membranes and represents 15% of the total phospholipid pool. PS acts to maintain membrane integrity and neuroplasticity, buffer oxidative stress, facilitate neurotransmitter release, and increase brain glucose metabolism [66–69]. Phospholipid deficits in neuronal membranes are involved in age-related brain-structural and cognitive decline. A decline of PS and other phospholipids in neuronal membranes has been associated with memory impairment and cognitive deficits whereas dietary PS and phospholipid supplements have prevented or reversed such deficits [70–75]. PS supplementation improved learning capacity and memory in rodents [76, 77] and improved physical and mental performance such as long-term memory and recognition in elderly people with cognitive decline [73, 78–80], in stressed young adults [81], and in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder [82].
Vinpocetine (ethyl apovincaminate) is synthesized from the alkaloid vincamine, an extract from the leaf of the lesser periwinkle plant (Catharanthus roseus). Vinpocetine has been used widely in Japan and Europe for the treatment of cognitive decline since the late 1970s. Vinpocetine can improve cerebral blood flow and glucose metabolism in the thalamus and basal ganglia and the occipital, parietal, and temporal cortex [83–86]. Previous studies have confirmed that vinpocetine can inhibit beta-amyloid-induced activation of NF-κB, inflammation, and cytokine production and interferes with many stages of the ischemic cascade: ATP depletion, activation of voltage-sensitive Na(+) and Ca(++) channels, and glutamate and free-radical release (antioxidant activity) [87–91]. Vinpocetine treatment for 18 months significantly improved cognitive functions, overall health status, and quality of life in people with chronic cerebral hypoperfusion [92]. Vinpocetine is considered prophylactic for MCI [92–94].
Huperzine A (hup A), is a quinolizidine-related alkaloid isolated from Huperzia serrata (Thunb.) Trevis. Hup A is a competitive, reversible, and well-tolerated inhibitor of AChE that is more potent than current memory-promoting agents (donepezil, rivastigmine, and galantamine) [95–98]. Hup A is also a competitive NMDA receptor inhibitor and an antagonist of Aβ-induced neurotoxicity [99–108]. It has significant antioxidative, antiapoptotic, and neuroprotective effects [100]. Hup A has been used for enhancing memory and mental function in healthy people [101] and in animal models of cognitive deficits [97] without showing any obvious serious adverse effects [102]. Other studies suggest, however, that hup A has limited effects on cognition. One recent multicenter, randomized, placebo-controlled trial showed that a dose of 0.4 mg daily, but not 0.2 mg daily, of hup A was effective in improving cognition in people with moderate cognition decline [103].
The active components of the leaf extract of Ginkgo biloba (EGb) consist of flavonoids, terpenoids, and terpene lactones (ginkgolides and bilobalide). EGb has long been used in traditional Chinese medicine [104, 105]. EGb has also been prescribed for memory and concentration complaints in Germany and France since the 1960s [106, 107]. EGb protects neuronal cell membranes and mitochondria from free-radical damage [108]; reduces the aggregation and toxicity of beta-amyloid [109, 110]; promotes hippocampal neurogenesis [111–116]; decreases blood viscosity [117, 118]; enhances microperfusion [119]; improves neurotransmission of glutamatergic [120–122], dopaminergic, and cholinergic systems [123, 124]; and improves learning and memory in animal models [125–129].
Recent meta-analyses of multiple randomized, controlled trials suggest that EGb may be effective for reversing or delaying age-related memory deficits [130–135]. However, two recent large-scale studies, conducted in more than 6000 participants aged 70 years and over [136, 137], failed to confirm such effects. One explanation is that, due to the particularly long incubation phase of severe memory decline (20–30 years), a quick reduction in the incidence of severe memory deficits by EGb is unrealistic, or EGb is only effective in subjects at early stages of memory decline such as SMC and/or MCI, but not in advanced stage of memory deficits. To support this, short-term and acute EGb administration could be used to enhance specific memory and cognitive functions in cognitively intact older, middle-aged, and younger healthy volunteers [138]. In addition, coadministration of EGb and PS improved secondary memory and the speed of memory task performance [125, 126].
Other studies show that deficits or dysregulation in brain metabolism is linked with aging-related cognition decline and that decline could be reversed through nutrients and vitamin supplements such as acetyl-L-carnitine, vitamin B6, and vitamin B12 [139–151]. L-Tyrosine is a precursor to L-dopa and catecholamines, an imbalance of which may be involved in cognitive dysfunction [127, 128].
The preventive effects of coffee, tea, and caffeine consumption on late-life cognitive decline and dementia have been extensively reviewed recently [129]. Besides the well-known short-term enhancing effects, some studies examined the long-term effects and showed that coffee, tea, and caffeine consumption could protect against late-life cognitive impairment/decline and dementia. These findings, however, are still considered preliminary [129].
Green tea (Camellia sinensis) extract enhances parietofrontal connectivity during working memory processing [152] and protects against okadaic acid-induced acute learning and memory impairments in rats [153]. A recent review suggests that the green tea constituent theanine could have therapeutic effects against psychiatric and neurodegenerative disorders including mild cognitive impairment and dementia through multiple mechanisms such as inhibition of NMDA-induced neurotoxicity, enhanced brain GABA and glycine content, and enhanced BDNF-related neuroprotection [154]. Most of these mechanistic studies, however, were done on animals.
Coadministration of multiple medicinal herbal ingredients is a common practice in traditional Chinese medicine. The main goal of such practices is to potentiate the bioavailability, activity, and efficacy of the key therapeutic ingredients and to minimize or antagonize potential toxicities associated with the ingredients.
The objective of this exploratory, randomized, placebo-controlled trial was to evaluate the safety and effectiveness of short-term administration of BrainPower Advanced, a multi-ingredient dietary supplement, on SMCs in older adults.
2. Methods
2.1. Samples and Participants Recruitment
This study was conducted between December 15, 2011, and April 10, 2012, at the Si Tang Community Health Service Center in Baoshan district in Shanghai, China. Community volunteers of older adults were recruited by self-referral in response to media coverage and word of mouth. All study procedures were conducted in accordance with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975 and were approved by the Shanghai Jiao Tong University Medical Center Institutional Review Board. Informed consent was obtained from all participants prior to enrollment into the study.
Subjects who met the following criteria were eligible for the study: inclusion criteria: (1) healthy males or females at least 50 years of age; (2) having self-reported hypomnesis, forgetfulness, memory loss, or impaired attention/concentration as determined by a standard medical examination questionnaire; exclusion criteria: (1) having been diagnosed with neurological diseases such as Alzheimer's Disease, Parkinson's Disease, migraine, and epilepsy; (2) having taken any medication likely to affect brain and nervous system function such as L-dopa, MAO inhibitors, modafinil, and amphetamine, within 30 days before the start of the trial; (3) having a history of severe cerebral-cardiovascular events (myocardial infarction, cerebrovascular disorder, acute coronary syndrome, or other cerebrovascular diseases); (4) other severe medical conditions (severe diseases of the liver, kidneys, or lungs; malignant tumors; motor impairment; dysphonia; visual impairment; etc.).
2.2. Randomization and Blindness
Participants were randomly assigned to the BrainPower Advanced treatment group or placebo treatment group. The randomization was performed using a predetermined randomization code which was generated by a random number generator. The numbers generated were placed in sealed envelopes and a serial number was assigned to each envelope according to the sequence of allocation of the randomized number. Each envelope was then opened sequentially according to the admission sequence of subjects.
Trial participants and community doctors were both blinded from the treatment (double-blind trial). Of the 101 enrolled participants, 98 participants completed the 12-week follow-up, including 47 subjects in the BrainPower Advanced group and 51 subjects in the placebo group. Three subjects withdrew from the study (2 due to symptoms of diarrhea and 1 due to family objection).
The participants received similar-looking capsules in color-coded bottles (white bottles for BrainPower Advanced and yellow bottles for placebo). Neither the subjects nor the medical doctors, including the study principal investigator (Rong Shi), knew the specific color code until the end of the study. Both the BrainPower Advanced capsules and the placebo (which was mainly composed of flour) were manufactured and supplied by Robinson Pharma, Inc. (Costa Mesa, California, USA).
Each participant was instructed to take 2 capsules with meals daily for 12 weeks and a new batch of supplements was dispensed every 4 weeks during follow-up sessions. Changes in subjective hypomnesis/memory loss and attention/concentration deficits were recorded using a self-administrated medical questionnaire. Memory function capacity was evaluated using a subset of tester-administered memory tests before and after the 12-week intervention. All participants were followed up each month in order to check compliance and adverse effects.
2.3. Evaluation of Subjective Memory Complaints
Two aspects of subjective memory complaints, that is, subjective hypomnesis/forgetfulness/memory loss (SML) and subjective attention/concentration deficits (SAD), were screened using a self-administered 5-point scale (1 = no symptoms or occasional slight symptoms complaints; 2 = slight/mild symptom complaints; 3 = moderate severe symptom complaints; 4 = severe symptom complaints; 5 = very severe symptom complaints) included in a medical questionnaire that included the demographics and medication history of the participants. Because relatively few participants scored 1 and 2 points in the memory questionnaire, participants who scored 1 and 2 points were combined as the “slight symptom” group, and participants who scored 3, 4, and 5 points were combined as the “severe symptom” group for further statistical analysis.
2.4. Evaluation of Objective Memory Function
The short-term working memory of the participants was evaluated using three simple tester-administered subtests constructed and validated for the Chinese population [155].
(1) Visual/Auditory Memory (1 Point for Each Correct Answer, a Maximal Total of 5 Points). Subjects were allowed to watch 5 film clips in 5 minutes (1 minute per film clip). The films were selected from a pool of popular old Chinese films that all participants should have been familiar with. The subjects were then asked to recall the correct order and content of the films based on the movie listings provided by the tester.
(2) Abstracting (1 Point for Each Correct Answer, a Maximal Total of 5 Points). Subjects were shown 5 different pictures/images of cartoon characters (such as a policeman, soldier, medical doctor, nurse, taxi driver, bus driver, school teacher, university professor, cook, waitress, and tour guide) on a computer screen and were asked to recall them immediately.
(3) Memory Recall (1 Point for Each Correct Answer, a Maximal Total of 5 Points). Subjects were shown 5 consecutive pictures of famous historical landscapes located in different Chinese cities and were asked to recall the pictures' contents and their locations.
2.5. Statistics Analysis
EpiData 3.02 software was used for the establishment of the database. SPSS 20.0 software was used for statistical analysis. Group data were presented as the mean ± s.d. Mean differences of the variables between the BrainPower Advanced and placebo groups were compared using Student's t-test for variables with normal distribution or using nonparametric tests for variables with nonnormal distributions. Ridit scoring test, which is a nonparameter test for comparing two or more sets of ordered qualitative data, was used for evaluating the changes in the symptom severity score of SML and SAD in response to the intervention. The alpha level of P > 0.05 was chosen as being statistically significant. All P values reported were 2-sided.
3. Results
3.1. Participants' Characteristics
The baseline characteristics of age and gender and histories of alcohol intake, disease, and medication of the participants are shown in Table 2. There were 14 males (29.8%) and 33 females (70.2%) in the BrainPower Advanced group and 17 males (33.3%) and 34 females (66.7%) in the placebo group. The gender distribution between the two groups was not significantly different (χ 2 = 0.142, P > 0.05), with females accounting for more than two-thirds of the total participants. The overall average age of all participants was 67.1 ± 10.5, with no significant difference found between the BrainPower Advanced group (69.1 ± 9.5 years) and the placebo group (65.2 ± 11.1 years) (t = 1.839, P > 0.05). The BrainPower Advanced and placebo groups also showed similar patterns of alcohol intake history (χ 2 = 0.542, P > 0.05). However, a greater proportion of the participants in the BrainPower Advanced group had disease history (39/47, 83%) and medication history (35/47, 74.5%) compared to the placebo group (31/51, 60.8%, χ 2 = 5.904, P < 0.05 and 23/51, 45.1%, χ 2 = 8.734, P < 0.01, resp.).
Table 2.
Treatment | Male | Female | Subtotal | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Gender, N (% of subtotal) | BrainPower | 14 (29.8%) | 33 (70.2%) | 47 (100%) |
Placebo | 17 (33.3%) | 34 (66.7%) | 51 (100%) | |
Combined | 31 (31.6%) | 69 (68.4) | 98 (100%) | |
| ||||
Age, year (mean ± s.d.) | BrainPower | 69.68 ± 9.52 | 68.80 ± 9.60 | |
Placebo | 64.03 ±10.73 | 65.81 ± 11.41 | ||
Combined | 69.07 ± 9.48 | 65.22 ± 11.1 | 67.07 ± 10.49 | |
| ||||
Age, year (range) | BrainPower | 52.87–82.80 | 53.18–84.86 | |
Placebo | 47.28–83.47 | 49.03–88.43 | ||
Combined | 52.87–84.46 | 47.28–88.43 | 47.28–88.43 | |
| ||||
History of chronic disease, yes/total (%) | BrainPower | 39/47 (83.0%) | ||
Placebo | 31/51 (60.8%) | |||
Combined | 70/98 (71.4%) | |||
| ||||
History of alcohol use, yes/total (%) | BrainPower | 5/47 (10.6%) | ||
Placebo | 8/51 (15.7%) | |||
Combined | 13/98 (13.3%) | |||
| ||||
History of medication, yes/total (%) | BrainPower | 35/47 (74.5%) | ||
Placebo | 23/51 (45.1%) | |||
Combined | 58/98 (59.2%) |
3.2. Subjective Memory Complaints (SMC)
The mean value, distribution pattern, and the differences between the BrainPower Advanced group and placebo group in the severity level of subjective hypomnesis/forgetfulness/memory loss (SML) and subjective attention/concentration deficit (SAD) before and after the 12-week intervention are shown in Tables 3, 4(a), and 5(a).
Table 3.
Self-reported deficits | Intervention | Before intervention | After intervention | Relative to baseline (= 1) |
---|---|---|---|---|
Subjective hypomnesis/memory loss (SML) | BrainPower | 3.77 ± 0.89 | 2.94 ± 0.94 | 0.779840849 |
Placebo | 3.43 ± 1.19 | 2.88 ± 0.77 | 0.839650146 | |
| ||||
Subjective concentration/attention deficit (SAD) | BrainPower | 3.62 ± 0.99 | 2.68 ± 0.89 | 0.740331492 |
Placebo | 3.25 ± 1.35 | 2.92 ± 0.94 | 0.898461538 |
Table 4.
(a).
Distribution of SML symptom severity | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Group | 1, none or slight | 2, mild | 3, moderate | 4, severe | 5, very severe | |
Before intervention | BrainPower group | 0 (0%) | 3 (6.4%) | 16 (34.0%) | 17 (36.2%) | 11 (23.4%) |
Placebo group | 5 (9.8%) | 7 (13.7%) | 7 (13.7%) | 25 (49.0%) | 7 (13.7%) | |
Total | 5 (5.1%) | 10 (10.2%) | 23 (23.5%) | 42 (42.9%) | 18 (18.4%) | |
| ||||||
After intervention | BrainPower group | 2 (4.3%) | 14 (29.8%) | 18 (38.3%) | 11 (23.4%) | 2 (4.3%) |
Placebo group | 2 (3.9%) | 12 (23.5%) | 27 (52.9%) | 10 (19.6%) | 0 (0%) | |
Total | 4 (4.1%) | 26 (26.5%) | 45 (45.9%) | 21 (21.4%) | 2 (2.0%) |
(b).
Symptom score change | BrainPower group | Placebo group | Total | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
N | % | N | % | N | % | |
−3 | 2 | 4.3 | 1 | 2.0 | 3 | 3.1 |
−2 | 12 | 25.5 | 10 | 19.6 | 22 | 22.4 |
−1 | 20 | 42.6 | 12 | 23.5 | 32 | 32.7 |
0 | 12 | 25.5 | 15 | 29.4 | 27 | 27.6 |
+1 | 1 | 2.1 | 10 | 19.6 | 11 | 11.2 |
+2 | 0 | 0.0 | 2 | 3.9 | 2 | 2.0 |
+3 | 0 | 0.0 | 1 | 2.0 | 1 | 1.0 |
Total | 47 | 100.0 | 51.0 | 100.0 | 98 | 100.0 |
(c).
Treatment group | Before intervention | After intervention | P value of McNemar's test | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
No or slight | Severe | No or slight | Severe | ||
BrainPower group | 3 (6.4%) | 44 (93.6%) | 16 (34.0%) | 31 (66.0%) | 0.001 |
Placebo group | 12 (23.5%) | 39 (76.5%) | 14 (27.5%) | 37 (72.5%) | 0.791 |
Pearson χ 2 | 5.547 | 0.5 | |||
P | 0.019 | 0.479 |
Table 5.
(a).
Distribution of SAD symptom severity | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Group | 1, none or slight | 2, mild | 3, moderate | 4, severe | 5, very severe | |
Before intervention | BrainPower group | 1 (2.1%) | 3 (6.4%) | 16 (34.0%) | 19 (40.4%) | 8 (17.0%) |
Placebo group | 7 (13.7%) | 7 (13.7%) | 8 (15.7%) | 22 (43.1%) | 7 (13.7%) | |
Total | 8 (8.2%) | 10 (10.2%) | 24 (24.5%) | 41 (41.8%) | 15 (15.3%) | |
| ||||||
After intervention | BrainPower group | 4 (8.5%) | 15 (31.9%) | 21 (44.7%) | 6 (12.8%) | 1 (2.1%) |
Placebo group | 2 (3.9%) | 17 (33.3%) | 16 (31.4%) | 15 (29.4%) | 1 (2.0%) | |
Total | 6 (6.1%) | 32 (32.7%) | 37 (37.8%) | 21 (21.4%) | 2 (2.0%) |
(b).
Symptom score change | BrainPower group | Placebo group | Total | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
N | % | N | % | N | % | |
−3 | 2 | 4.3 | 2 | 3.9 | 4 | 4.1 |
−2 | 14 | 29.8 | 8 | 15.7 | 22 | 22.4 |
−1 | 20 | 42.6 | 10 | 19.6 | 30 | 30.6 |
0 | 9 | 19.1 | 15 | 29.4 | 14 | 14.3 |
+1 | 2 | 4.3 | 11 | 21.6 | 13 | 13.3 |
+2 | 0 | 0.0 | 3 | 5.9 | 3 | 3.1 |
+3 | 0 | 0.0 | 2 | 3.9 | 2 | 2.0 |
Total | 47 | 100.0 | 51 | 100.0 | 98 | 100.0 |
(c).
Treatment group | Before intervention | After intervention | P value of McNemar's test | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
No or slight | Severe | No or slight | Severe | ||
BrainPower group | 4 (8.5%) | 43 (91.5%) | 19 (40.4%) | 28 (59.6%) | 0.001 |
Placebo group | 14 (27.5%) | 37 (72.5%) | 19 (37.3%) | 32 (62.7%) | 0.302 |
Pearson χ 2 | 5.852 | 0.104 | |||
P | 0.016 | 0.748 |
The baseline symptom severities of SML and SAD in the BrainPower Advanced group (3.77 ± 0.89; 3.62 ± 0.99, resp.) were about 10% greater than those of the placebo group (3.43 ± 1.19; 3.25 ± 1.35, resp.) (Table 3). These differences in SML and SAD between the two groups disappeared after the 12-week intervention, primarily due to a greater proportion of the BrainPower Advanced group showing greater reduction of symptom severity than the placebo group (Tables 4(b) and 5(b)). The placebo group had more participants that showed worsened symptom severity than the BrainPower Advanced group.
Ridit scoring test shows a significant and differential reduction in the SML (mean ± s.d. = 0.418 ± 0.236 versus 0.575 ± 0.299, P < 0.01) and SAD symptom severity (means = 0.424 ± 0.229 versus 0.612 ± 0.283, P < 0.01) after BrainPower Advanced and placebo treatment. Table 4(b) shows that a total of 34 people reported reduced SML symptom severity after BrainPower Advanced intervention (2 people by 3 points, 12 people by 2 points, and 20 people by 1 point), 12 people reported no change, and 1 person reported worsened symptom severity (by 1 point). In comparison, only 23 people reported reduced SML severity after placebo intervention (1 person by 3 points, 10 people by 2 points, and 12 people by 1 point), 15 people reported no change in symptom severity, and 13 people reported worsened symptom severity (10 people by 1 point, 2 people by 2 points, and 1 person by 3 points).
Similarly, Table 5(b) shows that a total of 36 people reported different reductions in SAD symptom severity after BrainPower Advanced intervention (2 people by 3 points, 14 people by 2 points, and 20 people by 1 point), 9 people reported no change, and 2 people reported worsened symptom severity (by 1 point) (Table 5(b)). After placebo intervention, 20 people reported different reductions in SAD symptom severity (2 people by 3 points, 8 people by 2 points, and 10 people by 1 point), 15 people reported no change, and 16 people reported worsened symptoms (11 people by 1 point, 3 people by 2 points, and 2 people by 3 points).
Because the symptom severity scores are nominal variables and because only few participants of the BrainPower Advanced group scored 1 and 2 points (Tables 4(a) and 5(a)) of SML and SAD, participants with the severity scores of 1 and 2 were combined as the “slight symptom” group and participants with the severity scores of 3, 4, and 5 were combined as the “severe symptom” group for further chi-square test (Tables 4(c) and 5(c)).
It was noted that a greater proportion of the BrainPower Advanced group (44/47, 93.6%) than the placebo group (39/51, 76.5%) had severe SML symptoms (severity scores ⩾ 3), or a smaller proportion of the BrainPower Advanced group had slight SML symptoms (severity scores ⩽ 2) (3/47, 6.4%) than the placebo group (12/51, 23.5%) (Pearson χ 2 = 5.547, P = 0.019) before the start of the intervention (Tables 4(a) and 4(c)). Similar pretreatment differences in severe SAD distribution existed between the BrainPower Advanced (43/47, 91.5%) and placebo groups (37/51, 72.5%) (severity scores ⩾ 3) (Pearson χ 2 = 5.852, P = 0.016) (Tables 5(a) and 5(c)).
After the intervention, however, the BrainPower Advanced group had fewer cases of severe SML (31) and SAD (28) than the placebo group (37 for SML and 32 for SAD, resp.) and the difference in SML and SAD between the BrainPower Advanced and placebo groups was no longer significant (Pearson χ 2 = 0.5, P = 0.479; χ 2 = 0.104, P = 0.748, resp.) (Tables 4(c) and 5(c)).
Placebo group analysis of the combined data showed that the proportion of people with severe SML showed little change after placebo intervention (reduced by 2 people, from 39 to 37, P > 0.05) whereas the proportion of people with severe SML dropped significantly after BrainPower Advanced intervention (reduced by 13 people, from 44 to 31, P < 0.001) (Table 4(c)). Similarly, the proportion of severe SAD did not change significantly after placebo intervention (reduced by 5 people, from 37 to 32, P > 0.05) whereas the proportion of severe SAD decreased significantly after BrainPower Advanced intervention (reduced by 15 people, from 43 to 28, P < 0.001) (Table 5(c)).
3.3. Memory Function Test Scores
The memory function test focused on 3 subareas: visual/auditory impression memory, abstract thinking, and immediate memory recall. The subtest scores and the combined total test scores are shown in Tables 6(a) and 6(b), respectively. The BrainPower Advanced group had about 10% lower baseline scores than placebo group in audio/visual memory (2.28 ± 1.06 versus 2.51 ± 0.81), abstract thinking (2.40 ± 1.08 versus 2.73 ± 1.08), and combined total memory function scores (6.85 ± 2.46 versus 7.37 ± 2.32), but similar scores in memory retrieval (2.17 ± 1.15 versus 2.18 ± 1.14). These baseline differences disappeared after the 12-week intervention (3.43 ± 0.83 versus 3.41 ± 1.00, 3.45 ± 0.75 versus 3.47 ± 0.92, 9.66 ± 1.85 versus 9.39 ± 1.98, and 2.81 ± 0.92 versus 2.47 ± 0.83, resp.). Both the BrainPower Advanced and the placebo groups produced significant improvements (129%–150%) (P < 0.01, each) of the subtests and combined total scores in the older adults, with the BrainPower Advanced group producing better improvements (1.50-, 1.35-, 1.43-, and 1.29-fold relative to the baseline values of audio/visual memory, abstract thinking, memory retrieval, and combined total function test scores) than the placebo group (1.35-, 1.27-, 1.13-, and 1.27-fold, resp.) (Tables 6(a) and 6(b)).
Table 6.
(a).
Memory function subtest scores | Before intervention | After intervention | Improvement relative to baseline (= 1) |
t | P | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Audio/visual memory | BrainPower | 2.28 ± 1.06 | 3.43 ± 0.83 | 1.50 | −8.247 | <0.001 |
Placebo | 2.51 ± 0.81 | 3.41 ± 1.00 | 1.36 | −5.014 | <0.001 | |
t | −1.232 | 0.074 | ||||
P | 0.221 | 0.941 | ||||
| ||||||
Abstracting ability | BrainPower | 2.40 ± 1.08 | 3.45 ± 0.75 | 1.44 | −6.602 | <0.001 |
Placebo | 2.73 ± 1.08 | 3.47 ± 0.92 | 1.27 | −4.791 | <0.001 | |
t | −1.474 | 0.081 | ||||
P | 0.144 | 0.889 | ||||
| ||||||
Memory retrieval | BrainPower | 2.17 ± 1.15 | 2.81 ± 0.92 | 1.29 | −3.526 | 0.001 |
Placebo | 2.18 ± 1.14 | 2.47 ± 0.83 | 1.13 | −1.820 | 0.075 | |
t | −.027 | 0.410 | ||||
P | 0.979 | 0.060 |
(b).
Before intervention | After intervention | t | P | |
---|---|---|---|---|
BrainPower | 6.85 ± 2.46 | 9.66 ± 1.85 | 8.478 | <0.001 |
Placebo | 7.37 ± 2.32 | 9.39 ± 1.98 | 6.988 | <0.001 |
t | −1.08 | 0.692 | ||
P | 0.283 | 0.491 |
(c).
Score change | BrainPower intervention | Placebo control | Total | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
N | % | N | % | N | % | |
−5 | 1 | 2.1 | 0 | 0 | 1 | 1 |
−3 | 0 | 0 | 1 | 2 | 1 | 1 |
−2 | 1 | 2.1 | 2 | 3.9 | 3 | 3.1 |
−1 | 1 | 2.1 | 5 | 9.8 | 6 | 6.1 |
0 | 2 | 4.3 | 2 | 3.9 | 4 | 4.1 |
+1 | 6 | 12.8 | 5 | 9.8 | 11 | 11.2 |
+2 | 7 | 14.9 | 17 | 33.3 | 24 | 24.5 |
+3 | 12 | 25.5 | 8 | 15.7 | 20 | 20.4 |
+4 | 9 | 19.1 | 5 | 9.8 | 14 | 14.3 |
+5 | 2 | 4.3 | 5 | 9.8 | 7 | 7.1 |
+6 | 4 | 8.5 | 0 | 0 | 4 | 4.1 |
+7 | 2 | 4.3 | 1 | 2 | 3 | 3.1 |
Total | 47 | 100.0 | 51 | 100.0 | 98 | 100.0 |
Ridit scoring test showed that the improvement in the combined total scores of the memory function subtests was significantly better after the BrainPower Advanced intervention (mean = 0.56 ± 0.287) than after the placebo intervention (0.445 ± 0.276) (P < 0.05) (Table 6(c)).
4. Discussion
In this exploratory randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled study, we evaluated the effectiveness of a proprietary dietary supplement, BrainPower Advanced, in older adults with SMCs. The results show that BrainPower Advanced intervention for 12 weeks was safe and effective in improving the symptoms of SMC in older adults.
SMCs are defined as self-awareness of memory loss that can be assessed by a simple “yes” or “no” questionnaire but are often not detected by clinicians using objective memory scales [2]. SMC is also a predictor of mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and of future cognitive decline [156, 157]. There are studies showing that people with SMC are 3–6 times more likely to develop MCI than people without SMC [158–162].
Because SMCs are often reversible, improvement of SMC would represent a good opportunity to intervene prodromal MCI and cognitive decline. So far, no proven agents are available for SMC. BrainPower Advanced is a dietary supplement based on polyherbal extracts, nutrients, and vitamins. Previous studies have shown that the active ingredients of BrainPower Advanced could potentially improve cognitive function in different age groups of healthy people in the presence or absence of SMCs through various mechanisms including anti-inflammation, improved cerebral blood flow and perfusion, and improved glucose/energy metabolism (Table 1).
In the present study, BrainPower Advanced treatment reversed 13 of the 44 (29.5%) cases of severe SML and 15 of the 43 (34.9%) cases of severe SAD. In contrast, placebo treatment reversed only 2 of the 39 (5.1%) cases of severe SML (P > 0.05) and 5 of the 37 (13.5%) cases of severe SAD (P > 0.05). Furthermore, a greater proportion of the participants showed various degrees of symptom improvement after BrainPower Advanced intervention (72.3%, 34/47 for SML and 76.6%, 36/47 for SAD) than after placebo treatment (49%, 25/51 for SML and 39.2%, 20/51 for SAD).
In contrast, a greater proportion of participants in the placebo group reported worsened symptoms of SML (25.5%, 13/51) and SAD (31.4%, 16/51) or no change in SML (29.4%, 15/51) and SAD (29.4%, 15/51) compared to the BrainPower Advanced group that reported worsened SML (2.1%, 1/47) and SAD (2%, 2/47) or no change in SML (25.5%, 12/47) and SAD (19.1%, 9/47) after the intervention.
The 29.5% reversion rate (13/44) of severe SML and 34.9% reversion rate (15/43) of severe SAD by BrainPower Advanced are comparable to the reported 15%–35% response rate of MCI to AChEIs treatment and the 30% response rate of MCI to NMDA receptor antagonist treatment [139]. Given that there is no standard anti-SMC treatment yet, the current results are very promising [140–142].
These results suggest that BrainPower Advanced is not only effective in reducing a significant portion (about 30%) of severe SMC and in reducing symptom severity in about 70% of the subjects with SMCs, but also effective in reducing the progression or worsening of SMCs. It is noted, however, that 25.5% of the people with severe SML showed no response to BrainPower Advanced intervention.
In this study, no significant differences were found in visual/auditory memory, abstract thinking, and memory retrieval and in the combined total memory function testing scores between the BrainPower Advanced and placebo groups before or after the intervention. Both BrainPower Advanced and placebo interventions resulted in significantly improved memory function performance, albeit with the BrainPower Advanced group showing better improvement than the placebo group at a nonsignificant level.
Because both placebo and BrainPower Advanced intervention enhanced the performance scores of the memory function tests, this raised the possibility that factors other than the intervention per se may be responsible for the improvement. One such possibility is that the postintervention memory test score was unintendedly “enhanced” by the preintervention test. Like any learning activity, practice or repetition of the same learning task would improve performance and produce better scores. This may not mean that the memory was “getting better” but simply reflecting the remembered answers from the last test. There are suggestions that the same memory tests should not be given within a short period of time (3–6 months) or that only the tests taken the first time should be taken seriously as a true measure of memory abilities.
It is the current view that SMC is a subjective experience that cannot be reliably measured by objective memory scales due to the subtle and heterogeneous nature of SMC [2–5]. Nevertheless, more accurate diagnosis of SMC and/or MCI could be corroborated by informants and by using different questionnaires and memory scales before and after an intervention such as the use of Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), Six-Item Screener, Subjective Memory Rating Scale (SMRS) and Deterioration Cognitive Observee (DECO) [143], Memory Complaint Questionnaire (MAC-Q), and Subjective Cognitive Decline Questionnaire (SCD-Q) [144]. The updated Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS) also has a battery of subtests for evaluating multiple aspects of learning and memory including immediate and delayed memory for visual working memory and auditory memory [145–148].
There are other limitations of this study. The sample size is too small to detect potential performance differences in the memory function tests. Despite the randomization process, more participants in the BrainPower Advanced group had a history of disease and medication use than the placebo group. Due to the multi-ingredient nature of BrainPower Advanced, it was difficult to determine whether any single ingredient (Ginkgo biloba, Camellia sinensis, vinpocetine, kola nut, hup A, PS, L-tyrosine, L-pyroglutamic acid, acetyl-L-carnitine, choline bitartrate, L-glutamine, L-phenylalanine, L-cysteine, vitamin B6, or vitamin B12) was primarily responsible for BrainPower Advanced overall effects or, more likely, whether it was a synergistic combination of the ingredients working together that produced the observed results. Because SMCs are heterogeneous and potentially affected by a range of genetic and epigenetic factors including lifestyle [149], daily activity/exercise [150, 151], APOE genotype [163], affective status [164–166], education achievements [11, 167], inflammation, and alcohol use, controlling these factors would allow better understanding of BrainPower Advanced intervention in further studies [168].
5. Conclusion
Twelve-week BrainPower Advanced intervention was effective and safe in reducing the progression of symptom severity in older adults with severe SML and SAD. As no proven agents are currently available in reversing or delaying the progression of SMC in the increasing aging population, further well-controlled, large-scale studies could validate if long-term dietary supplementation of combined polyherbal ingredients, nutrients, and vitamins could be an alternative prophylactic strategy for older adults with SMCs.
Acknowledgments
The authors thank Reza Emaddudin for proofreading the paper. This study was sponsored by DRM Resources (Costa Mesa, California, USA).
Disclosure
The sponsor had no role in the design or analysis of this study or the interpretation of the findings. Mina Shariff and Tuong Nguyen are employees of DRM Resources.
Competing Interests
Jingfen Zhu, Guoqiang Xing, Su Chen, Lihua Dai, Tian Shen, Yi Feng, Yeats Ye, Jianyu Rao, and Rong Shi have declared that they have no competing or potential conflict of interests in the study.
Authors' Contributions
Jingfen Zhu, Guoqiang Xing, and Su Chen have contributed equally to this work. All authors have read the paper and had full access to the study data.
References
- 1.Hänninen T., Koivisto K., Reinikainen K. J., et al. Prevalence of ageing-associated cognitive decline in an elderly population. Age and Ageing. 1996;25(3):201–205. doi: 10.1093/ageing/25.3.201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Abdulrab K., Heun R. Subjective memory impairment. A review of its definitions indicates the need for a comprehensive set of standardised and validated criteria. European Psychiatry. 2008;23(5):321–330. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2008.02.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Pearman A., Storandt M. Self-discipline and self-consciousness predict subjective memory in older adults. Journals of Gerontology B Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. 2005;60(3):P153–P157. doi: 10.1093/geronb/60.3.p153. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Snitz B. E., Morrow L. A., Rodriguez E. G., Huber K. A., Saxton J. A. Subjective memory complaints and concurrent memory performance in older patients of primary care providers. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2008;14(6):1004–1013. doi: 10.1017/S1355617708081332. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Snitz B. E., Yu L., Crane P. K., Chang C.-C. H., Hughes T. F., Ganguli M. Subjective cognitive complaints of older adults at the population level: an item response theory analysis. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders. 2012;26(4):344–351. doi: 10.1097/wad.0b013e3182420bdf. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Burns A., Zaudig M. Mild cognitive impairment in older people. The Lancet. 2002;360(9349):1963–1965. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(02)11920-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Roberts R. O., Knopman D. S., Geda Y. E., Cha R. H., Roger V. L., Petersen R. C. Coronary heart disease is associated with non-amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Neurobiology of Aging. 2010;31(11):1894–1902. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.10.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Grambaite R., Hessen E., Auning E., Aarsland D., Selnes P., Fladby T. Correlates of subjective and mild cognitive impairment: depressive symptoms and CSF biomarkers. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders Extra. 2013;3:291–300. doi: 10.1159/000354188. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Mielke M. M., Savica R., Wiste H. J., et al. Head trauma and in vivo measures of amyloid and neurodegeneration in a population-based study. Neurology. 2014;82(1):70–76. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000438229.56094.54. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Sajjad A., Mirza S. S., Portegies M. L. P., et al. Subjective memory complaints and the risk of stroke. Stroke. 2015;46(1):170–175. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.114.006616. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Alagoa João A., Maroco J., Ginó S., Mendes T., de Mendonça A., Martins I. P. Education modifies the type of subjective memory complaints in older people. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2016;31(2):153–160. doi: 10.1002/gps.4305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Rowell S. F., Green J. S., Teachman B. A., Salthouse T. A. Age does not matter: memory complaints are related to negative affect throughout adulthood. Aging & Mental Health. 2015 doi: 10.1080/13607863.2015.1078284. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Petersen R. C. Early diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease: is MCI too late? Current Alzheimer Research. 2009;6(4):324–330. doi: 10.2174/156720509788929237. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Bischkopf J., Busse A., Angermeyer M. C. Mild cognitive impairment—a review of prevalence, incidence and outcome according to current approaches. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 2002;106(6):403–414. doi: 10.1034/j.1600-0447.2002.01417.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Busse A., Bischkopf J., Riedel-Heller S. G., Angermeyer M. C. Subclassifications for mild cognitive impairment: prevalence and predictive validity. Psychological Medicine. 2003;33(6):1029–1038. doi: 10.1017/s0033291703007839. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Panza F., Capurso C., D'Introno A., Colacicco A. M., Capurso A., Solfrizzi V. Prevalence rates of mild cognitive impairment subtypes and progression to dementia. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society. 2006;54(9):1474–1475. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2006.00874.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Panza F., Capurso C., D'Introno A., Colacicco A. M., Capurso A., Solfrizzi V. Mild cognitive impairment: risk of Alzheimer disease and rate of cognitive decline. Neurology. 2007;68(12):964–965. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000259691.26674.d3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Gauthier S., Reisberg B., Zaudig M., et al. Mild cognitive impairment. The Lancet. 2006;367(9518):1262–1270. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(06)68542-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Meguro K., Akanuma K., Meguro M., Yamaguchi S., Ishii H., Tashiro M. Prevalence and prognosis of prodromal Alzheimer's disease as assessed by magnetic resonance imaging and f-fluorodeoxyglucose-positron emission tomography in a community: reanalysis from the Osaki-Tajiri project. Psychogeriatrics. 2015 doi: 10.1111/psyg.12131. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Vos S. J. B., Verhey F., Frölich L., et al. Prevalence and prognosis of alzheimer's disease at the mild cognitive impairment stage. Brain. 2015;138(5):1327–1338. doi: 10.1093/brain/awv029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Ward A., Tardiff S., Dye C., Arrighi H. M. Rate of conversion from prodromal Alzheimer's disease to Alzheimer's dementia: a systematic review of the literature. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders Extra. 2013;3(1):320–332. doi: 10.1159/000354370. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Darby D. G., Brodtmann A., Pietrzak R. H., et al. Episodic memory decline predicts cortical amyloid status in community-dwelling older adults. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease. 2011;27(3):627–637. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2011-110818. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Erk S., Spottke A., Meisen A., Wagner M., Walter H., Jessen F. Evidence of neuronal compensation during episodic memory in subjective memory impairment. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2011;68(8):845–852. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Gifford K. A., Liu D., Damon S. M., et al. Subjective memory complaint only relates to verbal episodic memory performance in mild cognitive impairment. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease. 2015;44(1):309–318. doi: 10.3233/JAD-140636. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Forsberg A., Engler H., Almkvist O., et al. PET imaging of amyloid deposition in patients with mild cognitive impairment. Neurobiology of Aging. 2008;29(10):1456–1465. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2007.03.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Snitz B. E., Weissfeld L. A., Cohen A. D., et al. Subjective cognitive complaints, personality and brain amyloid-beta in cognitively normal older adults. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2015;23(9):985–993. doi: 10.1016/j.jagp.2015.01.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Lim Y. Y., Ellis K. A., Harrington K., et al. Cognitive decline in adults with amnestic mild cognitive impairment and high amyloid-β: prodromal Alzheimer's disease? Journal of Alzheimer's Disease. 2013;33(4):1167–1176. doi: 10.3233/jad-121771. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Landau S. M., Mintun M. A., Joshi A. D., et al. Amyloid deposition, hypometabolism, and longitudinal cognitive decline. Annals of Neurology. 2012;72(4):578–586. doi: 10.1002/ana.23650. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Smith E. E., Greenberg S. M. β-Amyloid, blood vessels, and brain function. Stroke. 2009;40(7):2601–2606. doi: 10.1161/strokeaha.108.536839. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Karim S., Hopkins S., Purandare N., et al. Peripheral inflammatory markers in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2014;29(3):221–226. doi: 10.1002/gps.3988. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Bader Lange M. L., Cenini G., Piroddi M., et al. Loss of phospholipid asymmetry and elevated brain apoptotic protein levels in subjects with amnestic mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer disease. Neurobiology of Disease. 2008;29(3):456–464. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2007.11.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Hadjiev D. Asymptomatic ischemic cerebrovascular disorders and neuroprotection with vinpocetine. Ideggyogyaszati Szemle. 2003;56(5-6):166–172. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Douaud G., Menke R. A. L., Gass A., et al. Brain microstructure reveals early abnormalities more than two years prior to clinical progression from mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33(5):2147–2155. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4437-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Leal S. L., Yassa M. A. Perturbations of neural circuitry in aging, mild cognitive impairment, and Alzheimer's disease. Ageing Research Reviews. 2013;12(3):823–831. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2013.01.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Zhuang L., Sachdev P. S., Trollor J. N., et al. Microstructural white matter changes, not hippocampal atrophy, detect early amnestic mild cognitive impairment. PLoS ONE. 2013;8(3) doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0058887.e58887 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Cabeza R., Grady C. L., Nyberg L., et al. Age-related differences in neural activity during memory encoding and retrieval: a positron emission tomography study. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1997;17(1):391–400. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-01-00391.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Loewenstein D. A., Acevedo A., Potter E., et al. Severity of medial temporal atrophy and amnestic mild cognitive impairment: selecting type and number of memory tests. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2009;17(12):1050–1058. doi: 10.1097/jgp.0b013e3181b7ef42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Okamoto M., Wada Y., Yamaguchi Y., et al. Process-specific prefrontal contributions to episodic encoding and retrieval of tastes: a functional NIRS study. NeuroImage. 2011;54(2):1578–1588. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.08.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Prendergast G., Limbrick-Oldfield E., Ingamells E., Gathercole S., Baddeley A., Green G. G. R. Differential patterns of prefrontal MEG activation during verbal & visual encoding and retrieval. PLoS ONE. 2013;8(12) doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0082936.e82936 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Manenti R., Cotelli M., Miniussi C. Successful physiological aging and episodic memory: a brain stimulation study. Behavioural Brain Research. 2011;216(1):153–158. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.07.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Lockhart B. P., Lestage P. J. Cognition enhancing or neuroprotective compounds for the treatment of cognitive disorders: why? when? which? Experimental Gerontology. 2003;38(1-2):119–128. doi: 10.1016/s0531-5565(02)00163-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Taffe M. A., Weed M. R., Gutierrez T., Davis S. A., Gold L. H. Differential muscarinic and NMDA contributions to visuo-spatial paired-associate learning in rhesus monkeys. Psychopharmacology. 2002;160(3):253–262. doi: 10.1007/s00213-001-0954-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Schaeffer E. L., Gattaz W. F. Cholinergic and glutamatergic alterations beginning at the early stages of Alzheimer disease: participation of the phospholipase A2 enzyme. Psychopharmacology. 2008;198(1):1–27. doi: 10.1007/s00213-008-1092-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Wenk G. L. Neuropathologic changes in Alzheimer's disease: potential targets for treatment. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 2006;67(supplement 3):3–7. doi: 10.4088/jcp.0706e03. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Francis P. T. Glutamatergic systems in Alzheimer's disease. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2003;18(1):S15–S21. doi: 10.1002/gps.934. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.McVeigh C., Passmore P. Vascular dementia: prevention and treatment. Clinical Interventions in Aging. 2006;1(3):229–235. doi: 10.2147/ciia.2006.1.3.229. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Cosman K. M., Boyle L. L., Porsteinsson A. P. Memantine in the treatment of mild-to-moderate Alzheimer's disease. Expert Opinion on Pharmacotherapy. 2007;8(2):203–214. doi: 10.1517/14656566.8.2.203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Peters O., Lorenz D., Fesche A., et al. A combination of galantamine and memantine modifies cognitive function in subjects with amnestic MCI. Journal of Nutrition, Health and Aging. 2012;16(6):544–548. doi: 10.1007/s12603-012-0062-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Salloway S., Correia S., Richardson S. Key lessons learned from short-term treatment trials of cholinesterase inhibitors for amnestic MCI. International Psychogeriatrics. 2008;20(1):40–46. doi: 10.1017/S1041610207005650. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Petersen R. C., Thomas R. G., Grundman M., et al. Vitamin E and donepezil for the treatment of mild cognitive impairment. The New England Journal of Medicine. 2005;352(23):2379–2388. doi: 10.1056/nejmoa050151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Feldman H. H., Ferris S., Winblad B., et al. Effect of rivastigmine on delay to diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease from mild cognitive impairment: the InDDEx study. The Lancet Neurology. 2007;6(6):501–512. doi: 10.1016/s1474-4422(07)70109-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.O'Brien J. T., Burns A., Group BAPDC Clinical practice with anti-dementia drugs: a revised (second) consensus statement from the British Association for Psychopharmacology. Journal of Psychopharmacology. 2011;25(8):997–1019. doi: 10.1177/0269881110387547. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Russ T. C. Cholinesterase inhibitors should not be prescribed for mild cognitive impairment. Evidence-Based Medicine. 2014;19(3, article 101) doi: 10.1136/eb-2013-101687. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Russ T. C., Morling J. R. Cholinesterase inhibitors for mild cognitive impairment. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. 2012;9 doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD009132.pub2.CD009132 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Servello A., Andreozzi P., Bechini F., et al. Effect of ache and BuChE inhibition by rivastigmin in a group of old-old elderly patients with cerebrovascular impairment (SIVD type) Minerva Medica. 2014;105(2):167–174. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Lane R. M., Darreh-Shori T. Understanding the beneficial and detrimental effects of donepezil and rivastigmine to improve their therapeutic value. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease. 2015;44(4):1039–1039. doi: 10.3233/JAD-142268. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Heckman P. R., Wouters C., Prickaerts J. Phosphodiesterase inhibitors as a target for cognition enhancement in aging and Alzheimer's disease: a translational overview. Current Pharmaceutical Design. 2014;21(3):317–331. doi: 10.2174/1381612820666140826114601. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Singer M., Romero B., Koenig E., Förstl H., Brunner H. Nightmares in patients with Alzheimer's disease caused by donepezil. Therapeutic effect depends on the time of intake. Nervenarzt. 2005;76(9):1128–1129. doi: 10.1007/s00115-004-1856-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Mimica N., Presečki P. Side effects of approved antidementives. Psychiatria Danubina. 2009;21(1):108–113. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Gareri P., Putignano D., Castagna A., et al. Retrospective study on the benefits of combined memantine and cholinesterase inhibitor treatment in aged patients affected with Alzheimer's disease: the memage study. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease. 2014;41(2):633–640. doi: 10.3233/JAD-132735. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Nguyen M. D., Salbu R. L. Donepezil 23 mg: a brief insight on efficacy and safety concerns. Consultant Pharmacist. 2013;28(12):800–803. doi: 10.4140/tcp.n.2013.800. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Anand P., Singh B. A review on cholinesterase inhibitors for Alzheimer's disease. Archives of Pharmacal Research. 2013;36(4):375–399. doi: 10.1007/s12272-013-0036-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Di Santo S. G., Prinelli F., Adorni F., Caltagirone C., Musicco M. A meta-analysis of the efficacy of donepezil, rivastigmine, galantamine, and memantine in relation to severity of Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease. 2013;35(2):349–361. doi: 10.3233/JAD-122140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Mehta S., Chandersekhar K., Prasadrao G., et al. Safety and efficacy of donepezil hydrochloride in patients with mild to moderate Alzheimer's disease: findings of an observational study. Indian Journal of Psychiatry. 2012;54(4):337–343. doi: 10.4103/0019-5545.104820. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Russo P., Frustaci A., Del Bufalo A., Fini M., Cesario A. Multitarget drugs of plants origin acting on Alzheimer's disease. Current Medicinal Chemistry. 2013;20(13):1686–1693. doi: 10.2174/0929867311320130008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Pepeu G., Pepeu I. M., Amaducci L. A review of phosphatidylserine pharmacological and clinical effects. Is phosphatidylserine a drug for the ageing brain? Pharmacological Research. 1996;33(2):73–80. doi: 10.1006/phrs.1996.0013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Kim H.-Y., Huang B. X., Spector A. A. Phosphatidylserine in the brain: metabolism and function. Progress in Lipid Research. 2014;56(1):1–18. doi: 10.1016/j.plipres.2014.06.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Starks M. A., Starks S. L., Kingsley M., Purpura M., Jäger R. The effects of phosphatidylserine on endocrine response to moderate intensity exercise. Journal of the International Society of Sports Nutrition. 2008;5, article 11 doi: 10.1186/1550-2783-5-11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Escribá P. V., Busquets X., Inokuchi J., et al. Membrane lipid therapy: modulation of the cell membrane composition and structure as a molecular base for drug discovery and new disease treatment. Progress in Lipid Research. 2015;59:38–53. doi: 10.1016/j.plipres.2015.04.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Crook T., Petrie W., Wells C., Massari D. C. Effects of phosphatidylserine in Alzheimer's disease. Psychopharmacology Bulletin. 1992;28(1):61–66. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Crook T. H., Tinklenberg J., Yesavage J., et al. Effects of phosphatidylserine in age-associated memory impairment. Neurology. 1991;41(5):644–649. doi: 10.1212/WNL.41.5.644. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Schreiber S. An open trial of plant-source derived phosphatydilserine for treatment of age-related cognitive decline. Israel Journal of Psychiatry and Related Sciences. 2000;37(4):302–307. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Kato-Kataoka A., Sakai M., Ebina R., Nonaka C., Asano T., Miyamori T. Soybean-derived phosphatidylserine improves memory function of the elderly Japanese subjects with memory complaints. Journal of Clinical Biochemistry and Nutrition. 2010;47(3):246–255. doi: 10.3164/jcbn.10-62. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Yurko-Mauro K. Cognitive and cardiovascular benefits of docosahexaenoic acid in aging and cognitive decline. Current Alzheimer Research. 2010;7(3):190–196. doi: 10.2174/156720510791050911. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Richter Y., Herzog Y., Lifshitz Y., Hayun R., Zchut S. The effect of soybean-derived phosphatidylserine on cognitive performance in elderly with subjective memory complaints: a pilot study. Clinical Interventions in Aging. 2013;8:557–563. doi: 10.2147/cia.s40348. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Park H.-J., Lee S. Y., Shim H. S., Kim J. S., Kim K. S., Shim I. Chronic treatment with squid phosphatidylserine activates glucose uptake and ameliorates TMT-induced cognitive deficit in rats via activation of cholinergic systems. Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine. 2012;2012:8. doi: 10.1155/2012/601018.601018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Lee B., Sur B.-J., Han J.-J., et al. Oral administration of squid lecithin-transphosphatidylated phosphatidylserine improves memory impairment in aged rats. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry. 2015;56:1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2014.07.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Amaducci L. Phosphatidylserine in the treatment of Alzheimer's disease: results of a multicenter study. Psychopharmacology Bulletin. 1988;24(1):130–134. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Amaducci L., Crook T. H., Lippi A., et al. Use of phosphatidylserine in Alzheimer's disease. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1991;640:245–249. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1991.tb00227.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Cenacchi T., Bertoldin T., Farina C., et al. Cognitive decline in the elderly: a double-blind, placebo-controlled multicenter study on efficacy of phosphatidylserine administration. Aging Clinical and Experimental Research. 1993;5(2):123–133. doi: 10.1007/bf03324139. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Kingsley M. I., Miller M., Kilduff L. P., McEneny J., Benton D. Effects of phosphatidylserine on exercise capacity during cycling in active males. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise. 2006;38(1):64–71. doi: 10.1249/01.mss.0000183195.10867.d0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Hirayama S., Terasawa K., Rabeler R., et al. The effect of phosphatidylserine administration on memory and symptoms of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: a randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled clinical trial. Journal of Human Nutrition and Dietetics. 2014;27(supplement 2):284–291. doi: 10.1111/jhn.12090. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Tamaki N., Kusunoki T., Matsumoto S. The effect of vinpocetine on cerebral blood flow in patients with cerebrovascular disorders. Therapia Hungarica. 1985;33(1):13–21. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Tamaki N., Matsumoto S. Agents to improve cerebrovascular circulation and cerebral metabolism—vinpocetine. Nippon Rinsho. 1985;43(2):376–378. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Bönöczk P., Panczel G., Nagy Z. Vinpocetine increases cerebral blood flow and oxygenation in stroke patients: a near infrared spectroscopy and transcranial Doppler study. European Journal of Ultrasound. 2002;15(1-2):85–91. doi: 10.1016/s0929-8266(02)00006-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Szilágyi G., Nagy Z., Balkay L., et al. Effects of vinpocetine on the redistribution of cerebral blood flow and glucose metabolism in chronic ischemic stroke patients: a PET study. Journal of the Neurological Sciences. 2005;229-230:275–284. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2004.11.053. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Pereira C., Agostinho P., Moreira P. I., Duarte A. I., Santos M. S., Oliveira C. R. Neuroprotection strategies: effect of vinpocetinein in vitro oxidative stress models. Acta Medica Portuguesa. 2003;16(6):401–406. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Pereira C., Agostinho P., Oliveira C. R. Vinpocetine attenuates the metabolic dysfunction induced by amyloid β-peptides in PC12 cells. Free Radical Research. 2000;33(5):497–506. doi: 10.1080/10715760000301041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Solntseva E. I., Bukanova I. V., Skrebitskiǐ V. G. Memory and potassium channels. Uspekhi Fiziologicheskikh Nauk. 2003;34(4):16–25. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Liu R. T., Wang A., To E., et al. Vinpocetine inhibits amyloid-beta induced activation of NF-κB, NLRP3 inflammasome and cytokine production in retinal pigment epithelial cells. Experimental Eye Research. 2014;127:49–58. doi: 10.1016/j.exer.2014.07.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Zhang L., Yang L. Anti-inflammatory effects of vinpocetine in atherosclerosis and ischemic stroke: a review of the literature. Molecules. 2015;20(1):335–347. doi: 10.3390/molecules20010335. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Valikovics A., Csányi A., Németh L. Study of the effects of vinpocetin on cognitive functions. Ideggyogyaszati Szemle. 2012;65(3-4):115–120. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Szatmari S. Z., Whitehouse P. J. Vinpocetine for cognitive impairment and dementia. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. 2003;(1) doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD003119.CD003119 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Valikovics A. Investigation of the effect of vinpocetine on cerebral blood flow and cognitive functions. Ideggyógyászati Szemle. 2007;60(7-8):301–310. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Liang Y. Q., Tang X. C. Comparative effects of huperzine A, donepezil and rivastigmine on cortical acetylcholine level and acetylcholinesterase activity in rats. Neuroscience Letters. 2004;361(1–3):56–59. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2003.12.071. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Liang Y.-Q., Tang X.-C. Comparative studies of huperzine A, donepezil, and rivastigmine on brain acetylcholine, dopamine, norepinephrine, and 5-hydroxytryptamine levels in freely-moving rats. Acta Pharmacologica Sinica. 2006;27(9):1127–1136. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-7254.2006.00411.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Wang R., Yan H., Tang X.-C. Progress in studies of huperzine A, a natural cholinesterase inhibitor from Chinese herbal medicine. Acta Pharmacologica Sinica. 2006;27(1):1–26. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-7254.2006.00255.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Xing S.-H., Zhu C.-X., Zhang R., An L. Huperzine A in the treatment of Alzheimer's disease and vascular dementia: a meta-analysis. Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine. 2014;2014:10. doi: 10.1155/2014/363985.363985 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Ha G. T., Wong R. K., Zhang Y. Huperzine a as potential treatment of Alzheimer's disease: an assessment on chemistry, pharmacology, and clinical studies. Chemistry and Biodiversity. 2011;8(7):1189–1204. doi: 10.1002/cbdv.201000269. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Zhu N., Lin J., Wang K., Wei M., Chen Q., Wang Y. Huperzine a protects neural stem cells against Aβ-induced apoptosis in a neural stem cells and microglia co-culture system. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Pathology. 2015;8(6):6425–6433. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Xu Z.-Q., Liang X.-M., Juan-Wu, Zhang Y.-F., Zhu C.-X., Jiang X.-J. Treatment with huperzine a improves cognition in vascular dementia patients. Cell Biochemistry and Biophysics. 2012;62(1):55–58. doi: 10.1007/s12013-011-9258-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Li J., Wu H. M., Zhou R. L., Liu G. J., Dong B. R. Huperzine a for Alzheimer's disease. Cochrane database of Systematic Reviews. 2008;(2) doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD005592.pub2.CD005592 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Rafii M. S., Walsh S., Little J. T., et al. A phase II trial of huperzine A in mild to moderate Alzheimer disease. Neurology. 2011;76(16):1389–1394. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e318216eb7b. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Ihl R. Gingko biloba extract EGb 761®: clinical data in dementia. International Psychogeriatrics. 2012;24(supplement 1):S35–S40. doi: 10.1017/s1041610212000543. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Kasper S. Clinical data in early intervention. International Psychogeriatrics. 2012;24(supplement 1):S41–S45. doi: 10.1017/s1041610212000555. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Serby M. J., Yhap C., Landron E. Y. A study of herbal remedies for memory complaints. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences. 2010;22(3):345–347. doi: 10.1176/appi.neuropsych.22.3.345. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Gertz H.-J., Kiefer M. Review about Ginkgo biloba special extract EGb 761 (Ginkgo) Current Pharmaceutical Design. 2004;10(3):261–264. doi: 10.2174/1381612043386437. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Stoll S., Scheuer K., Pohl O., Müller W. E. Ginkgo biloba extract (EGb 761) independently improves changes in passive avoidance learning and brain membrane fluidity in the aging mouse. Pharmacopsychiatry. 1996;29(4):144–149. doi: 10.1055/s-2007-979561. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Liu C.-S., Hu J.-F., Chen N.-H., Zhang J.-T. Comparison of the inhibitory activities of salvianolic acid B and Ginkgo biloba extract EGb 761 on neurotoxicity of beta-amyloid peptide. Yao Xue Xue Bao. 2006;41(8):706–711. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110.Longpré F., Garneau P., christen Y., Ramassamy C. Protection by EGb 761 against β-amyloid-induced neurotoxicity: Involvement of NF-κB, SIRT1, and MAPKs pathways and inhibition of amyloid fibril formation. Free Radical Biology and Medicine. 2006;41(12):1781–1794. doi: 10.1016/j.freeradbiomed.2006.08.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Abdel-Salam O. M. E. Stem cell therapy for Alzheimer's disease. CNS and Neurological Disorders-Drug Targets. 2011;10(4):459–485. doi: 10.2174/187152711795563976. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112.Osman N. M., Amer A. S., Abdelwahab S. Effects of Ginko biloba leaf extract on the neurogenesis of the hippocampal dentate gyrus in the elderly mice. Anatomical Science International. 2015 doi: 10.1007/s12565-015-0297-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113.Sun L., Zhuang W., Xu X., Yang J., Teng J., Zhang F. The effect of injection of EGb 761 into the lateral ventricle on hippocampal cell apoptosis and stem cell stimulation in situ of the ischemic/reperfusion rat model. Neuroscience Letters. 2013;555:123–128. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2013.09.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114.Tchantchou F., Lacor P. N., Cao Z., et al. Stimulation of neurogenesis and synaptogenesis by bilobalide and quercetin via common final pathway in hippocampal neurons. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease. 2009;18(4):787–798. doi: 10.3233/jad-2009-1189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115.Tchantchou F., Xu Y., Wu Y., Christen Y., Luo Y. EGb 761 enhances adult hippocampal neurogenesis and phosphorylation of CREB in transgenic mouse model of Alzheimer's disease. The FASEB Journal. 2007;21(10):2400–2408. doi: 10.1096/fj.06-7649com. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116.Yoo D. Y., Nam Y. Y., Kim W., et al. Effects of ginkgo biloba extract on promotion of neurogenesis in the hippocampal dentate gyrus in C57BL/6 mice. Journal of Veterinary Medical Science. 2011;73(1):71–76. doi: 10.1292/jvms.10-0294. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117.Santos R. F., Galduróz J. C. F., Barbieri A., Castiglioni M. L. V., Ytaya L. Y., Bueno O. F. A. Cognitive performance, SPECT, and blood viscosity in elderly non-demented people using Ginkgo biloba. Pharmacopsychiatry. 2003;36(4):127–133. doi: 10.1055/s-2003-41197. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118.Huang S.-Y., Jeng C., Kao S.-C., Yu J. J.-H., Liu D.-Z. Improved haemorrheological properties by Ginkgo biloba extract (Egb 761) in type 2 diabetes mellitus complicated with retinopathy. Clinical Nutrition. 2004;23(4):615–621. doi: 10.1016/j.clnu.2003.10.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 119.Koltringer P., Eber O., Klima G., et al. Microcirculation under therapy of parenteral Ginkgo biloba extract. Wiener Klinische Wochenschrift. 1989;101(6):198–200. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120.Esposito M., Carotenuto M. Ginkgolide B complex efficacy for brief prophylaxis of migraine in school-aged children: an open-label study. Neurological Sciences. 2011;32(1):79–81. doi: 10.1007/s10072-010-0411-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 121.Nooshinfar E., Lashgari R., Haghparast A., Sajjadi S. NMDA receptors are involved in Ginkgo extract-induced facilitation on memory retention of passive avoidance learning in rats. Neuroscience Letters. 2008;432(3):206–211. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2007.12.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122.Veerman S. R., Schulte P., Begemann M., de Haan L. Non-glutamatergic clozapine augmentation strategies: a review and meta-analysis. Pharmacopsychiatry. 2014;47(7):231–238. doi: 10.1055/s-0034-1385930. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 123.Cong W. H., Liu J. X., Xu L. Effects of extracts of ginseng and ginkgo biloba on hippocampal acetylcholine and monoamines in pdap-pv717i transgenic mice. Zhongguo Zhong Xi Yi Jie He Za Zhi. 2007;27(9):810–813. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 124.Kehr J., Yoshitake S., Ijiri S., Koch E., Nöldner M., Yoshitake T. Ginkgo biloba leaf extract (EGb 761®) and its specific acylated flavonol constituents increase dopamine and acetylcholine levels in the rat medial prefrontal cortex: possible implications for the cognitive enhancing properties of EGb 761®. International Psychogeriatrics. 2012;24(supplement 1):S25–S34. doi: 10.1017/s1041610212000567. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 125.Kaschel R. Specific memory effects of Ginkgo biloba extract EGb 761 in middle-aged healthy volunteers. Phytomedicine. 2011;18(14):1202–1207. doi: 10.1016/j.phymed.2011.06.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 126.Kennedy D. O., Jackson P. A., Haskell C. F., Scholey A. B. Modulation of cognitive performance following single doses of 120 mg Ginkgo biloba extract administered to healthy young volunteers. Human Psychopharmacology. 2007;22(8):559–566. doi: 10.1002/hup.885. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 127.Kulisevsky J. Role of dopamine in learning and memory: implications for the treatment of cognitive dysfunction in patients with Parkinson's disease. Drugs and Aging. 2000;16(5):365–379. doi: 10.2165/00002512-200016050-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 128.Chen C., Zhu Y.-F., Wilcoxen K. An improved synthesis of selectively protected l-dopa derivatives from l-tyrosine. Journal of Organic Chemistry. 2000;65(8):2574–2576. doi: 10.1021/jo9913661. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 129.Panza F., Solfrizzi V., Barulli M. R., et al. Coffee, tea, and caffeine consumption and prevention of late-life cognitive decline and dementia: a systematic review. Journal of Nutrition, Health and Aging. 2015;19(3):313–328. doi: 10.1007/s12603-014-0563-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 130.Birks J., Grimley Evans J. Ginkgo biloba for cognitive impairment and dementia. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. 2009;(1) doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD003120.pub3.CD003120 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 131.Gauthier S., Schlaefke S. Efficacy and tolerability of Ginkgo biloba extract EGb 761Ⓡ in dementia: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized placebo-controlled trials. Journal of Clinical Interventions in Aging. 2014;9:2065–2077. doi: 10.2147/CIA.S72728. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 132.Kasper S., Schubert H. Ginkgo biloba extract EGb 761 in the treatment of dementia: evidence of efficacy and tolerability. Fortschritte der Neurologie-Psychiatrie. 2009;77(9):494–506. doi: 10.1055/s-0028-1109504. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 133.Weinmann S., Roll S., Schwarzbach C., Vauth C., Willich S. N. Effects of Ginkgo biloba in dementia: systematic review and meta-analysis. BMC Geriatrics. 2010;10, article 14 doi: 10.1186/1471-2318-10-14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 134.Charemboon T., Jaisin K. Ginkgo biloba for prevention of dementia: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of the Medical Association of Thailand. 2015;98(5):508–513. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 135.Yang G., Wang Y., Sun J., Zhang K., Liu J. Ginkgo biloba for mild cognitive impairment and alzheimer’s disease: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Current Topics in Medicinal Chemistry. 2015;16(5):520–528. doi: 10.2174/1568026615666150813143520. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 136.Snitz B. E., O'Meara E. S., Carlson M. C., et al. Ginkgo biloba for preventing cognitive decline in older adults: a randomized trial. The Journal of the American Medical Association. 2009;302(24):2663–2670. doi: 10.1001/jama.2009.1913. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 137.Amieva H., Meillon C., Helmer C., Barberger-Gateau P., Dartigues J. F. Ginkgo biloba extract and long-term cognitive decline: a 20-year follow-up population-based study. PLoS ONE. 2013;8(1) doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0052755.e52755 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 138.Mix J. A., Crews W. D., Jr. An examination of the efficacy of Ginkgo biloba extract EGb 761 on the neuropsychologic functioning of cognitively intact older adults. The Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine. 2000;6(3):219–229. doi: 10.1089/acm.2000.6.219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 139.Doody R. S., Cummings J. L., Farlow M. R. Reviewing the role of donepezil in the treatment of Alzheimer's disease. Current Alzheimer Research. 2012;9(7):773–781. doi: 10.2174/156720512802455412. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 140.MacPherson H., Ellis K. A., Sali A., Pipingas A. Memory improvements in elderly women following 16 weeks treatment with a combined multivitamin, mineral and herbal supplement: a randomized controlled trial. Psychopharmacology. 2012;220(2):351–365. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2481-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 141.Macpherson H., Silberstein R., Pipingas A. Neurocognitive effects of multivitamin supplementation on the steady state visually evoked potential (SSVEP) measure of brain activity in elderly women. Physiology and Behavior. 2012;107(3):346–354. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2012.08.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 142.Solfrizzi V., Panza F. Plant-based nutraceutical interventions against cognitive impairment and dementia: meta-analytic evidence of efficacy of a standardized Gingko biloba extract. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease. 2015;43(2):605–611. doi: 10.3233/jad-141887. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 143.Ramlall S., Chipps J., Bhigjee A. I., Pillay B. J. Screening a heterogeneous elderly South African population for cognitive impairment: the utility and performance of the mini-mental state examination, six item screener, subjective memory rating scale and deterioration cognitive observee. African Journal of Psychiatry. 2013;16(6):445–455. doi: 10.4314/ajpsy.v16i6.57. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 144.Rami L., Mollica M. A., Garcfa-Sanchez C., et al. The subjective cognitive decline questionnaire (SCD-Q): a validation study. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease. 2014;41(2):453–466. doi: 10.3233/jad-132027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 145.Rabin L. A., Barr W. B., Burton L. A. Assessment practices of clinical neuropsychologists in the United States and Canada: a survey of INS, NAN, and APA division 40 members. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 2005;20(1):33–65. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2004.02.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 146.Brooks B. L., Iverson G. L., Holdnack J. A., Feldman H. H. Potential for misclassification of mild cognitive impairment: a study of memory scores on the Wechsler Memory Scale-III in healthy older adults. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2008;14(3):463–478. doi: 10.1017/s1355617708080521. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 147.D'Amato C. P., Denney R. L. The diagnostic utility of the rarely missed index of the wechsler memory scale-third edition in detecting response bias in an adult male incarcerated setting. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 2008;23(5):553–561. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2008.05.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 148.Seelye A. M., Howieson D. B., Wild K. V., Moore M. M., Kaye J. A. Wechsler memory scale-III faces test performance in patients with mild cognitive impairment and mild Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 2009;31(6):682–688. doi: 10.1080/13803390802484763. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 149.Sohrabi H. R., Bates K. A., Rodrigues M., et al. The relationship between memory complaints, perceived quality of life and mental health in apolipoprotein eε4 carriers and non-carriers. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease. 2009;17(1):69–79. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2009-1018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 150.Lee P.-L. The relationship between memory complaints, activity and perceived health status. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology. 2014;55(2):136–141. doi: 10.1111/sjop.12107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 151.Zuniga K. E., Mackenzie M. J., Kramer A., McAuley E. Subjective memory impairment and well-being in community-dwelling older adults. Psychogeriatrics. 2016;16(1):20–26. doi: 10.1111/psyg.12112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 152.Schmidt A., Hammann F., Wölnerhanssen B., et al. Green tea extract enhances parieto-frontal connectivity during working memory processing. Psychopharmacology. 2014;231:3879–3888. doi: 10.1007/s00213-014-3526-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 153.Li H., Wu X., Wu Q., et al. Green tea polyphenols protect against okadaic acid-induced acute learning and memory impairments in rats. Nutrition. 2014;30(3):337–342. doi: 10.1016/j.nut.2013.08.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 154.Lardner A. L. Neurobiological effects of the green tea constituent theanine and its potential role in the treatment of psychiatric and neurodegenerative disorders. Nutritional Neuroscience. 2014;17(4):145–155. doi: 10.1179/1476830513y.0000000079. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 155.Xu S., Wu Z. The construction of the ‘clinical memory teste’. Acta Psychologica Sinica. 1986;8:100–108. [Google Scholar]
- 156.Jacinto A. F., Brucki S. M. D., Porto C. S., Martins M. D. A., Nitrini R. Subjective memory complaints in the elderly: a sign of cognitive impairment? Clinics. 2014;69(3):194–197. doi: 10.6061/clinics/2014(03)09. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 157.Jonker C., Launer L. J., Hooijer C., Lindeboom J. Memory complaints and memory impairment in older individuals. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society. 1996;44(1):44–49. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1996.tb05636.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 158.Abner E. L., Kryscio R. J., Caban-Holt A. M., Schmitt F. A. Baseline subjective memory complaints associate with increased risk of incident dementia: the preadvise trial. The Journal of Prevention of Alzheimer's Disease. 2015;2:11–16. doi: 10.14283/jpad.2015.37. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 159.Kim J.-M., Stewart R., Kim S.-W., Yang S.-J., Shin I.-S., Yoon J.-S. A prospective study of changes in subjective memory complaints and onset of dementia in South Korea. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2006;14(11):949–956. doi: 10.1097/01.JGP.0000214857.66638.ed. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 160.Luck T., Riedel-Heller S. G., Luppa M., et al. Risk factors for incident mild cognitive impairment—results from the german study on ageing, cognition and dementia in primary care patients (AgeCoDe) Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 2010;121(4):260–272. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2009.01481.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 161.Rönnlund M., Sundström A., Adolfsson R., Nilsson L.-G. Subjective memory impairment in older adults predicts future dementia independent of baseline memory performance: evidence from the Betula prospective cohort study. Alzheimer's & Dementia. 2015;11(11):1385–1392. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2014.11.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 162.Zonderman A. B., Grimmer T. Risk of mild cognitive impairment: the olmsted county mci risk score. Neurology. 2015;84(14):1392–1393. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000001452. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 163.Ramanan V. K., Risacher S. L., Nho K., et al. APOE and BCHE as modulators of cerebral amyloid deposition: a florbetapir PET genome-wide association study. Molecular Psychiatry. 2014;19(3):351–357. doi: 10.1038/mp.2013.19. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 164.Arbabi M., Zhand N., Eybpoosh S., Yazdi N., Ansari S., Ramezani M. Correlates of memory complaints and personality, depression, and anxiety in a memory clinic. Acta Medica Iranica. 2015;53(5):270–275. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 165.Zandi T. Relationship between subjective memory complaints, objective memory performance, and depression among older adults. American Journal of Alzheimer's Disease and other Dementias. 2004;19(6):353–360. doi: 10.1177/153331750401900610. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 166.Polyakova M., Sonnabend N., Sander C., et al. Prevalence of minor depression in elderly persons with and without mild cognitive impairment: a systematic review. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2014;152–154(1):28–38. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2013.09.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 167.Montenegro M., Montejo P., Claver-Martín M. D., et al. Relationship between memory complaints and memory performance, mood and sociodemographic variables in young adults. Revista de Neurologia. 2013;57(9):396–404. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 168.Roussotte F. F., Gutman B. A., Madsen S. K., Colby J. B., Narr K. L., Thompson P. M. The apolipoprotein E epsilon 4 allele is associated with ventricular expansion rate and surface morphology in dementia and normal aging. Neurobiology of Aging. 2014;35(6):1309–1317. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2013.11.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]