Abstract
High-throughput experiments are becoming increasingly common, and scientists must balance hypothesis-driven experiments with genome-wide data acquisition. We sought to predict novel genes involved in Drosophila learning and long-term memory from existing public high-throughput data. We performed an analysis using PILGRM, which analyzes public gene expression compendia using machine learning. We evaluated the top prediction alongside genes involved in learning and memory in IMP, an interface for functional relationship networks. We identified Grunge/Atrophin (Gug/Atro), a transcriptional repressor, histone deacetylase, as our top candidate. We find, through multiple, distinct assays, that Gug has an active role as a modulator of memory retention in the fly and its function is required in the adult mushroom body. Depletion of Gug specifically in neurons of the adult mushroom body, after cell division and neuronal development is complete, suggests that Gug function is important for memory retention through regulation of neuronal activity, and not by altering neurodevelopment. Our study provides a previously uncharacterized role for Gug as a possible regulator of neuronal plasticity at the interface of memory retention and memory extinction.
Keywords: Drosophila melanogaster, learning and memory, long-term memory, behavior, histone modification, machine-learning, gene prediction
A unique and fundamental characteristic of higher order organisms is the capability to recall and remember past experiences. The ability to remember past experiences is advantageous to an organism when it is presented with challenges bearing resemblance to previous experiences. The biology of memory formation and memory retention has been probed with a vast array of experimental techniques that range from behavioral testing, to physiologically correlates, to experimental perturbation through the use of chemical, anatomical, genetic, and pharmacological stresses (Greenspan 1995). Genetic manipulations, coupled with behavioral assays, have provided important insight into memory input and output, allowing one to understand the genetic and molecular factors that govern memory formation, retention, and retrieval. These experimental approaches provide clues to evolutionarily conserved mechanisms that could allow for vertical integration (Dubnau and Tully 1998; Tully et al. 1994).
Drosophila melanogaster has proven to be an important genetic model system for understanding the genetic basis of memory (Greenspan 1995; Davis 2005; Margulies et al. 2005; McGuire et al. 2005; Davis and Zhong 2017). The biological process of long-term memory in Drosophila has been analyzed through the use of associative and nonassociative assays. Work using associative learning paradigms has provided genes and genetic functions that are now looked upon as “classical” learning. These memory genes were found through the use of associative-fear-conditioning assays (McGuire et al. 2005; Tully 1987; Tully et al. 1994; Dubnau and Tully 1998). These gene candidates were further elucidated and expanded in other assays, including the use of nonassociative olfactory memory (Das et al. 2011; McCann et al. 2011; Ramaswami 2014).
Social learning, or information exchange between a trained teacher and a naïve student, has been studied in insects, including bees (Alem et al. 2016; Loukola et al. 2017) and Drosophila. Studies involving food choice demonstrate that naïve, student flies can gain information through visual cues by observing trained, teacher flies, informing students that subsequently prefer the food of the trainer (Battesti et al. 2012). Another form of social learning in Drosophila involves the use of the courtship-conditioning paradigm. Here, male flies that unsuccessfully court either virgin females or mated females learn to associate pheromonal cues with the courtship rejection and subsequently suppress courtship of all female flies (Ejima et al. 2005; Ejima et al. 2007; Siegel and Hall 1979a,b).
Collectively, these studies in memory formation, retention, and social learning are critical in elucidating genetic and physiological mechanisms of learning and memory. However, these assays use nonecologically relevant forms of stimuli, and, thus, the genes identified might only represent a small fraction of learning and memory genes in the insect brain.
Other forms of memory have also been investigated utilizing what are now known as “classical” memory genes, which involve ecologically relevant forms of stimuli to the fly life cycle. In nature, Drosophila larvae are regularly infected by endoparasitoid wasps. In wild D. melanogaster populations, upwards of 90% of fly larvae are found in a wasp-infected state, suggesting they exert extremely strong selection pressures on Drosophila populations (Driessen et al. 1989; Fleury et al. 2004; LaSalle 1993). Drosophila larvae can mount a cellular immune response following infection (Carton and Nappi 1997), or adult D. melanogaster females can alter egg-laying (oviposition) behavior after an encounter with endoparasitoid wasps. A change in oviposition behavior entails at least two very different and quantifiable behavioral responses. First, if high ethanol containing food is made available to adult Drosophila, then female flies in the presence of wasps will actively prefer to lay eggs on ethanol-laden food (Kacsoh et al. 2013, 2015a). This behavior persists even after the wasp threat is removed, demonstrating a memory formation and retention (Kacsoh et al. 2013, 2015a). Second, Drosophila females depress their oviposition rate in the presence of wasps and maintain this depression following wasp removal. Wasp-exposed flies will also communicate the wasp threat to naïve flies that have never seen the wasp threat, which in turn will also depress their oviposition rate, demonstrating a form of social learning (Kacsoh et al. 2015b; Lefevre et al. 2012; Lynch et al. 2016). Visual cues from the wasp or wasp-exposed teachers are sufficient to trigger these behaviors, suggesting the presence of an innate circuit yielding these behaviors (Kacsoh et al. 2013, 2015a, 2015b). These behaviors persist in wild-type flies for multiple days after wasp encounter, allowing one to probe questions regarding memory formation and retention. The formation of these long-term memories is regulated, in part, by previously identified learning and memory genes, such as Orb2 and rutabaga (Kacsoh et al. 2013, 2015a, 2015b).
To date, the identified learning and memory genes may only represent a small fraction of the range of learning and memory genes and gene functions in the insect brain required for memory acquisition, retention, and recall. Thus, it is valuable to define and mechanistically identify novel genes and gene products. In order to facilitate a broader understanding of genes and gene products that govern memory formation, retention, and recall, the development of new methodologies are needed to identify biologically important functions that are independent of classical mutagenesis-based approached.
In the sequencing age, vast compendiums of existing and publically available data present the opportunity to address important biological questions (Faith et al. 2007; Yan et al. 2010; Chikina et al. 2009). Given the data abundance and availability, the question becomes, how does one utilize so much data to ask specific questions? Genome-wide expression data capture a vast array of conditions in a wide range of organisms, biological processes, and tissues. Data-mining algorithms applied to such large data sets can uncover novel, biologically relevant functions of genes that might be otherwise overlooked as candidates (Greene et al. 2014; Greene and Troyanskaya 2012). PILGRM (the Platform for Interactive Learning by Genomics Results Mining) is a data-mining platform that allows its users to encode specific questions about a biological function of interest into user-created gene sets. Probing for a specific biological function is performed by the user curating a gold standard of genes that are relevant to a particular pathway or process, which PILGRM uses for a supervised machine learning analysis of global microarray expression collections (Greene and Troyanskaya 2011). The user can also provide a list of negative control genes, allowing for improved prediction specificity. PILGRM then trains a support vector machine (SVM; Joachims 2006) classifier with these gene sets to discover novel relevant genes (Greene and Troyanskaya 2011). The SVM analysis assigns relatively high weights to conditions that differentiate gold standard (positive) genes from those in the negative standard (Greene and Troyanskaya 2011). Thus, this machine learning approach can be used to identify novel gene targets/functions in order to further elucidate a biologically meaningful process, such as memory.
In this study, we use PILGRM to identify novel genetic candidates by using a gold standard comprised of classically identified learning and memory genes. We examined the top prediction from PILGRM, a histone-deacetylase, Grunge (gug, CG6964), in a D. melanogaster functional relationship network, and found that it was highly connected to learning and memory genes and that the network was enriched for that particular process. We then used the natural wasp predator system to probe a role for this previously untested gene in D. melanogaster learning and memory by using three distinct learning and memory paradigms. These three assays probed for long-term memory formation, long-term memory retention, and social learning. Collectively, we test the hypothesis that gug may be a high-level regulator of learning and memory. Histone-acetylation activity has been previously identified as a regulator of mouse hippocampal learning and memory (Mews et al. 2017), where histone acetylation yields activation of early memory genes. Perturbation of histone acetylation activity presents defects in memory formation. A functional role for histone deacetylase activity in Drosophila memory is consistent previous reports showing that Rpd3 (HDAC1) functions in the Drosophila mushroom body to facilitate long-term memory formation (Fitzsimons and Scott 2011), suggesting that gug is a strong candidate for investigation.
Materials and Methods
Insect species/strains
The D. melanogaster strains Canton-S (CS), and a Histone-RFP transgenic line, was used as the wild-type strain for oviposition preference after wasp exposure. The mushroom body Gene-Switch line (MB GeneSwitch) and the MB247 were kindly provided by Greg Roman (Baylor College of Medicine). Flies were maintained at room temperature with ∼30% humidity. UAS-GugRNAi, Gug/TM3 (Spradling et al. 1999), and OK-107 were acquired from the Bloomington Drosophila Stock Center (stock numbers 32961, 11622, and 854, respectively) (Supplemental Material, Table S1). Flies aged 3–5 d were used for all experiments.
All species and strains used were maintained in fly bottles (Genesse catalog number 32–130) containing 50 ml of standard Drosophila medium. Bottles were supplemented with three Kimwipes rolled together and placed into the center of the food.
The Figitid larval endoparasitoid Leptopilina heterotoma (strain Lh14; Schlenke et al. 2007) was used in all experiments. In order to culture wasps, batches of 14 female and five male adult flies (D. melanogaster, strain Canton-S) were allowed to lay eggs in standard Drosophila vials containing standard Drosophila medium supplemented with activated yeast for 5 d. After 5 d, flies were removed and replaced with adult wasps (15 female, six male), which then attacked the developing fly larvae. Wasp vials were supplemented with ∼500 µl of a 50% honey/water solution applied to the inside of the vial plugs. Wasps aged 3–7 days posteclosion were used for all experiments.
PILGRM/IMP analysis
For the PILGRM analysis, we utilized the PILGRM version one data-mining algorithm (http://pilgrm.princeton.edu) (Greene and Troyanskaya 2011). The data set used in the analysis was derived from the Fruit Fly Compendium collected in August 2011. This data were collected from the Gene Expression Omnibus. The compendium used contained a total of 3139 arrays from 186 different experiments. This data covers 12,864 Entrez gene identifiers. Within experiments, arrays were processed using the affy (Gautier et al. 2004) bioconductor (Gentleman et al. 2004) package. The gene-expression values were then normalized and summarized using the medianpolish method (Irizarry et al. 2003). The resulting experimental collections were then combined for learning using C++ Sleipnir library (Huttenhower et al. 2008).
Our gold standard was curated to include known learning and long-term memory genes. Our negative standards were selected by PILGRM in a randomized manner. Following the identification of gug, we input our curated gold standard and gug into IMP (http://imp.princeton.edu) (Wong et al. 2012, 2015) with a stringent minimum gene connection of 0.85 confidence.
Fly oviposition ethanol choice
For fly memory assays, we used modified Petri dishes, termed Fly Corrals, as previously used and described (Kacsoh et al. 2015a). Holes are drilled into the Petri dish, where the center of the two holes is 6 cm apart. The diameter of each hole is ∼1.2 cm. A nitex nylon mesh is melted onto the top of the dish in order to allow for ventilation. The mesh used has 120 µm openings (Genesee Scientific catalog number 57–102). Dishes were cleaned using the Fisher Brand Sparkleen powder (catalog number 04-320-4) using a 10% Sparkleen solution. Dishes are allowed to soak in the cleaning solution for at least 2 hr, and subsequently rinsed in distilled water. Plates are then allowed to air dry for 24 hr. This cleaning protocol is followed both before and after an experiment was performed (Kacsoh et al. 2015a).
For preparation of the oviposition media in the Fly Corrals, we measured ∼0.375 g of instant blue Drosophila medium (Fisher Scientific Catalog number S22315C) into the caps of 15 ml Falcon Tubes (S22315C, Biological Resource Center, No.:22315C). For control (0% ethanol) food, we pipette 2250 µl of distilled water onto the instant food. For 6% ethanol food, we pipette 1966.5 µl of distilled water onto the instant food. Subsequent to this, we pipette 141.75 µl of 95% ethanol [190 Proof (95%), USP/NF/FCC/EP/BP/JP] onto the food. After mixing the liquid and the food, we immediately place one cap containing ethanol and one cap containing no ethanol onto the cage with laboratory tape (VWR) (Kacsoh et al. 2015a).
For memory assays utilizing the fly corral, 50 female flies and 10 male flies were coincubated with 50 female Lh14 wasps for 24 hr in 2.25 cm diameter vials or sham-exposed (control). Flies and wasps are then separated and flies are placed into fly corrals after the 24-hr exposure period, with five females and one male fly placed per dish. Following a 24-hr period, caps are removed and replaced with freshly prepared caps (prepared in the exact manner the original caps are made). This process is repeated for 3 d following wasp exposure. Once caps are removed, the number of eggs on both the ethanol cap and the control cap are counted. Ten replicates are performed. All egg counts are blinded such that the counter is unaware of experimental condition and genotype (Kacsoh et al. 2015a).
Fly oviposition depression
We measured fly oviposition rates were using The Fly Condo (Genesee Scientific Cat # 59–110), as previously described (Kacsoh et al. 2015b). The Fly Condo contains 24 independent chambers, where each chamber is 7.5 cm long by 1.5 cm diameter. Each condo has a 24-well food plate into which we dispensed 2 ml of standard Drosophila medium. In order to assay egg retention of flies in the presence of wasps (acute exposure), we place five female flies and one male fly into one chamber of the Fly Condo. For exposed units, three female Lh14 wasps are also placed in the units. The oviposition plate from control and experimental condos are counted 24 hr later. To assay memory of wasp exposure, after the 24-hr exposure, we remove all wasps and transfer all flies to a new Fly Condo. The oviposition food plate is replaced with freshly poured food (2 ml). We repeat this process for 3 d following the wasp exposure, and egg counts are performed every 24-hr. All egg counts are blinded such that the counter is unaware of experimental condition.
Social communication and social learning was tested as previously described (Kacsoh et al. 2015b). In order to assay fly–fly communication and the social learning period, five female flies and one male fly are placed into one chamber of The Fly Condo in the control, while three female Lh14 wasps are placed with the flies in the experimental setting for 24 hr. After the 24-hr wasp or sham (control) exposure, wasps are removed and replaced with three female “student” flies. These are naïve flies, never having seen a wasp. Flies are placed into new, clean fly condos for the second 24-hr period. For experiments with His-RFP teachers, the experiment terminates after one batch of students. For experiments using His-RFP as students, we replace the student flies with new batches of three female naïve His-RFP flies. This is repeated for three 24-hr periods following the wasp exposure. This experiment measures teaching ability of flies. The oviposition plates contain 2 ml of standard Drosophila medium, replaced every 24 hr. Fly embryo counts from each plate are made at each 24-hr time points. All egg counts are blinded such that the counter is unaware of experimental condition and genotype.
All treatments are run at 25°, at 40% humidity, with a 12:12 light:dark cycle in 24 replicates. Fly condos and oviposition plates are soaked thoroughly with 10% Sparkleen (Fisher catalog number 04-320-4) solution, rinsed with distilled water after every use, and allowed to air-dry (Kacsoh et al. 2015b).
RU486 feeding
We perform RU486 feedings in both assays used, as previously described (Kacsoh et al. 2015a, b). The method in which the RU486 is administered is similar in each behavior assay, but warrants two detailed methods. RU486 (Mifepristone) is used from Sigma (Lot number SLBG0210V).
For the ethanol-seeking memory assay, we apply RU486 or vehicle only directly into the media. The caps for the fly corrals are made in the exact same manner as described above, but instead of using distilled water, an RU486 solution is used. The RU486 solution is prepared by dissolving 3.575 mg of RU486 in 800 µl methanol (Fisher Scientific, Lot number 141313). This solution is added to 15.2 ml of distilled water. The total solution (16 ml) is thoroughly mixed and pipetted onto control food caps. For caps containing ethanol, the total solution of RU486 is changed to 14.992 ml and then pipetted onto the instant food. After pipetting the RU486 solution, we pipette 1.008 ml of 95% ethanol directly onto the food. Fresh RU486 solutions are prepared daily for experiments (Kacsoh et al. 2015a).
For the oviposition depression, teaching, and social learning assays involving inducible knock-down, we prepare the fly condos by measuring 0.375 g of flaky instant blue Drosophila medium (Fisher Scientific Catalog number S22315C) into each well of The Fly Condo plates. For all food treatments, a total liquid volume of 2250 µl is directly pipetted onto the instant food. For experiments with RU486, an RU486 solution is used that is prepared by dissolving 3.575 mg of RU486 in 800 µl methanol (Fisher Scientific, Lot number 141313). This solution is added to 15.2 ml of distilled water. The total solution (16 ml) is thoroughly mixed. From this mixed solution, we pipette 2250 µl onto the instant food. For plates containing no RU486 (vehicle, methanol only) 800 µl methanol is mixed with 15.2 ml of distilled water. The total solution (16 ml) is thoroughly mixed. From this mixed solution, we pipette 2250 µl onto the instant food. RU486 solutions are prepped daily for experiments (Kacsoh et al. 2015b).
Statistical analysis
Statistical tests for behavioral assays are performed in Microsoft Excel. We use Welch’s two-tailed t-tests for all egg count data. P-values are calculated for comparisons between paired treatment-group and unexposed (Kacsoh et al. 2015a, b).
Data availability
Stocks are available upon request or from the Bloomington Drosophila Stock Center, with stock numbers listed in Table S1. File S1 contains all inputs used for PILGRM analysis. File S2 contains the full data set for the PILGRM analysis output. File S3 contains legends for all Supplemental Material.
Results
PILGRM analysis reveals novel gene targets
In order to identify novel genes involved in learning and memory, we turned to the PILGRM data-mining tool (http://pilgrm.princeton.edu). The system requires a list of positive and negative standards. We input a positive, gold standard as well as a list of randomly selected negatives provided by the PILGRM server. Our positive standard list was comprised of previously identified learning and memory genes. Specifically, we input the genes: for (Donlea et al. 2012), Akt1 (Guo and Zhong 2006), Fmr1 (Kanellopoulos et al. 2012; Kacsoh et al. 2015a, b), S6k (Vargas et al. 2010), CrebB-17A (Perazzona et al. 2004), Adf1 (DeZazzo et al. 2000; Kacsoh et al. 2013, 2015a), amn (Aldrich et al. 2010; Kacsoh et al. 2015a, b), rut (Scheunemann et al. 2013; Kacsoh et al. 2015a, b), Nf1 (Li et al. 2013), CaMKII (Mehren and Griffith 2006), orb2 (Krüttner et al. 2012; Kacsoh et al. 2015a; Kacsoh et al. 2015b), and shi (Iyengar et al. 2011) (File S1). This list of gold standards is comprised of learning and memory genes that have been validated in associative and nonassociative memory assays. A subset of the genes selected here has been shown to be involved in the paradigms used in this study. We hypothesized that the use of genes that have been validated in multiple assays would provide a list of output genes most likely to provide a novel hit.
We performed the analysis utilizing PILGRM’s D. melanogaster compendium, which consists of gene expression data sets from the Gene Expression Omnibus (GEO). Following the analysis, PILGRM provides a visual interpretation for precision of predictions at different recall thresholds (Figure 1A). Following analysis, users are also provided a visual representation of true positive rate at various false positive rate thresholds. We find in our analysis that the area under the curve (AUC), shown as the shaded region, is 0.8130 (Figure 1B). PILGRM also provides a list of novel predictions (Figure 1C and File S2). In this case, the top novel prediction is the gene Grunge (gug), whose molecular function is listed as a histone deacetylase (Wang et al. 2006; Zhang et al. 2002; Zhang et al. 2013; Wang et al. 2008; Yeung et al. 2017). This gene has no previously described role in learning and memory in Drosophila. However, the Drosophila gene expression atlas (Fly:Atlas) reports Gug expression as most highly enriched in the fly brain (Chintapalli et al. 2007). We identify other genes in our top novel predictions from PILGRM that may also have a role in memory. For example, our third target, Twins (tws), is shown to be involved in neuroblast development (Chabu and Doe 2009). Our fourth target is TBP-associated factor 4 (Taf4), important in dendrite morphogenesis (Parrish et al. 2006). Also in our top 10, we find Syndecan (Sdc), a gene involved in synapse growth (Chanana et al. 2009). Collectively, we find our top candidates highly enriched in neurons and brain tissue with previously characterized genes having neuronal function.
We became intrigued with the possibility of histone modifications playing a role in fly memory. Recent work has shown that aberrations in mouse hippocampal histone acetylation yields mice with impaired long-term spatial memory formation—a cognitive process that relies on histone acetylation to activate early memory genes (Mews et al. 2017; Kandel et al. 2014; Korzus et al. 2004; Gräff and Tsai 2013; Wood et al. 2005). In Drosophila, the HDAC1 homolog, Rpd3, also is implicated in learning and memory functions (Fitzsimons and Scott 2011). Given these observations, in conjunction with observations showing epigenetic deregulation as a mechanism for neuropsychiatric diseases (Kandel et al. 2014; Gräff and Tsai 2013; Zovkic et al. 2013; Walker et al. 2015), we wished to elucidate the possibility of Gug playing a role in learning and long-term memory.
Gug shows high connectivity to known learning and memory genes
In order to further examine Gug before moving to genetic experiments, we used the integrative multi-species prediction (IMP) webserver to determine the extent to which Gug is connected with our known learning and memory gene set (Wong et al. 2012, 2015). IMP integrates gene-pathway annotations from the selected organism in addition to mapping functional analogs. This system has been shown to provide accurate gene-process predictions (Chikina and Troyanskaya 2011).
We queried the network with Gug and our gold standard set. We set the network filter to only show interactions whose minimum relationship confidence was 0.85, a stringent threshold. We find that Gug is connected, both directly and indirectly, to our known learning and memory genes (Figure 2). Unsurprisingly, this network is enriched for the biological processes of long-term memory (26.3%, P-value: 1.29e−8), short-term memory (15.8%, P-value: 1.85e−6), learning (21.1%, P-value: 1.21e−5), and associative learning (18.4%, P-value: 4.51e−5). Given the PILGRM and IMP results, we sought to test the role of Gug in a variety of assays probing for its role in memory formation, retention, and social learning.
Gug has a role in long-term memory retention
Given our bioinformatics results, we hypothesized that gug has a role in long-term memory formation and retention. We tested this question using two distinct assays, both involving the predatory wasp Leptopilina heterotoma (strain Lh14). Both behavioral assays utilize adult D. melanogaster, who can alter egg-laying behavior when encountering predatory wasps.
We first used an ethanol-seeking food preference assay where, following a wasp exposure, female flies will actively prefer to lay eggs on ethanol-laden food (Kacsoh et al. 2013, 2015a). Briefly, we exposed 3- to 5-d-old flies in three batches of 100 female flies and 20 male flies with 50 female wasps. Control flies underwent the same process, but lacked wasps. Following a 24-hr exposure, flies and wasps were separated. Exposed and unexposed flies are transferred in batches of five female and one male flies to a fly corral, a Petri dish with holes, where the center of the two holes were 6 cm apart, and the edge of the two holes were 7.2 cm apart. We place Falcon tube caps to the holes. The caps contain 0.375 g of flaky instant blue hydrated fly food with either distilled-water or distilled water with ethanol (6% by volume). Caps are changed every 24-hr for 3-d, and egg counts were performed on caps. All counts are blinded (see Materials and Methods) (Kacsoh et al. 2015a).
We find that wild-type D. melanogaster continue to oviposit on ethanol-laden food across each of the three, 24-hr time points tested following wasp exposure (Figure 3A) (Kacsoh et al. 2013, 2015a). Interestingly, we find that Gug heterozygous mutants (Spradling et al. 1999) (Gug/+) show a defect in persistence of oviposition on ethanol-laden food (Figure 3B). These flies show a defect in memory retention, starting on day 2. Day 1 of the assay does not show a memory defect, suggesting that memory formation is not affected. By day 3, we observe no difference in exposed and unexposed flies in the mutants, but still have a strong difference in wild-type flies, suggesting memory extinction has been achieved in the heterozygotes (Figure S2). However, these experiments cannot exclude the possibility that the Gug gene product is required in non-neural tissues, and that the observed behavior is not specific to neuronal function.
The mushroom body (MB) of the adult brain is thought to be required for behaviors that are dependent on learning and memory (Aso et al. 2009; Claridge-Chang et al. 2009; Schwaerzel et al. 2003; Masse et al. 2009). The behaviors we set out to test Gug’s role in have been shown to be MB dependent, demonstrating a bona fide memory paradigm (Kacsoh et al. 2015a, 2015b). A critical question arising after testing the Gug heterozygotes was whether Gug is required in the MB for memory formation and/or retention. We used the GAL4/UAS system to drive expression of an RNA-hairpin targeting Gug mRNA (GugRNAi) in conjunction with either the MB driver OK107 or MB247. We find that driver lines alone, and the line containing the Gug hairpin alone have wild-type memory retention (Figure S1, A–C). However, flies expressing the GugRNAi in the MB phenocopy the Gug heterozygote phenotype (Figure 3, C–D), where we observe no defect in memory formation, only a defect in memory retention beginning on d 2 of observation. Again, we observe memory ablation in these flies by d 3, suggesting a memory retention defect (Figure S2).
While the MB drivers in conjunction with the GugRNAi presented a memory retention phenotype, the constitutive expression of the hairpin presents the possibility of Gug’s function being essential for early neuronal development. This suggestion means that the knockdown phenotypes may simply reflect developmental defects that preclude proper adult MB functions. In order to test elucidate this possibility, we turned to the GAL4-based Gene-Switch system where the GAL4 transcription factor is fused to the human progesterone ligand-binding domain (Burcin et al. 1999). We used flies expressing the Gene-Switch transgene specifically in the MB, where only upon an administration of the pharmacological Gene-Switch ligand, RU486, GAL4 transcription factor would become active (Mao et al. 2004). We find that feeding of RU486 to the outcrossed MB GeneSwitch driver line yields a wild-type memory retention phenotype. This demonstrates that feeding of the GeneSwitch ligand does not perturb memory formation or retention (Figure S1D). We also observe wild-type memory retention when the MB GeneSwitch is in combination with the GugRNAi when fed vehicle control (methanol) (Figure 3E). When fed the RU486 solution, these flies express the GugRNAi hairpin and show the same memory retention defect as when the GugRNAi transgene is in conjunction with constitutively active MB driver, and the Gug/+ heterozygote (Figure 3E). Again, these flies show no memory formation defect when compared to wild-type. However, there is accelerated memory decay at d 2 and the memory is ablated by d 3, demonstrating a defect in memory retention (Figure S2). Thus, we believe that the memory retention defect observed is a result of Gug being a necessary gene product in the MB to facilitate memory retention in the adult Drosophila brain. Given these data, we use the MB GeneSwitch line to drive expression of the Gug hairpin for all subsequent experiments. This approach allows us to delineate the role of Gug in memory retention from other important functions it may also perform during development.
We decided validate our conclusion indicative of the role of Gug in memory retention by utilizing a second, distinct memory assay. This second assay utilizes another D. melanogaster behavior following wasp exposure, where Drosophila female flies depress their oviposition rate following wasp exposure. Flies are given only standard Drosophila media as a substrate, with no food choice in this assay. The oviposition depression following wasp exposure is observed for multiple days after the predator threat is removed (Kacsoh et al. 2015b; Lefevre et al. 2012). We utilized Fly Condos to measure oviposition rate (Kacsoh et al. 2015b). Briefly, D. melanogaster were exposed for 24 hr to wasps in cylindrical 7.5 cm long by 1.5 cm diameter tubes of the Fly Condos (Genesse). Each tube contains five female flies and one male fly, either with three female wasps (exposed) or with no wasps at all (unexposed) (see Materials and Methods). After 24-hr, wasps are removed, and flies are placed into new, clean Fly Condos. We repeat this transfer for each of 3 d following wasp exposure. At every 24-hr time point, food-plates are removed, replaced with new food plates, and embryos are counted in a blinded manner.
Consistent with previous observations, wild-type flies depress oviposition in the presence of wasps and following wasp removal (Figure 4A and Figure S2B) (Lefevre et al. 2012; Kacsoh et al. 2015b). We find that Gug heterozygotes (Gug/+) have wild-type memory on d 1 following wasp exposure, but show accelerated memory decay, where by d 2 there is no difference between exposed and unexposed groups (Figure 4B and Figure S4). In order to ascertain the specificity of the phenotype with respect to memory, we again utilized the MB GeneSwitch line. We find that the GugRNAi construct outcrossed to Canton-S has wild-type memory retention (Figure S3A) (Kacsoh et al. 2015b). When the GugRNAi is expressed in conjunction with the MB GeneSwitch fed vehicle only, we observe wild-type memory retention (Figure 4C). When this line is fed RU486, leading to Gug knock-down, we observe a memory retention defect comparable to the heterozygote, where d 1 is unaffected, suggesting that memory formation is wild-type, but d 2 begins to show accelerated memory decay, where again we observe no difference between exposed and unexposed (Figure 4D and Figure S4). These data show no defect in memory formation, only memory retention. Given the data from the food choice and egg retention assays, we observe that Gug has a functional role in the MB, independent of memory formation, and modulates memory retention.
Gug has a role in teaching behavior
We observe a memory retention, but not formation, defect in Gug heterozygous mutants and Gug knockdown in the MB. We sought to ask whether this memory retention defect translates to other behaviors that are present following wasp exposure, including a social learning phenomenon. Following wasp exposure, flies depress oviposition and communicate the threat of wasps to naïve, unexposed flies. We term the experienced flies as “teachers” and the naïve flies “students.” We hypothesized that the memory retention defects observed (Figure 3, Figure 4, Figure S2, and Figure S4) may translate to defects in teaching behavior. To ask this, we utilized the Fly Condos (Kacsoh et al. 2015b). Briefly, we exposed wild-type flies to wasps for 24-hr, followed by wasp removal. We then placed the exposed and unexposed teacher flies into new Fly Condos with three naïve female flies expressing Histone-RFP (His-RFP) for an additional 24-hr (see Materials and Methods). Following a 24-hr coincubation, we removed His-RFP students, and placed exposed and unexposed teachers into new Fly Condos containing a new batch of three female naïve His-RFP flies. We repeated this process three times following wasp exposure as a means of testing the maintenance and ability of teaching behavior. New batches of students were used at each 24-hr time point. At every 24-hr time point, food-plates were removed, replaced with new food plates, and embryos were counted in a blinded manner. The His-RFP line was ideal for discriminating mixed populations of non-RFP and RFP embryos, allowing us to probe specificity of student and teacher behavior.
Consistent with previous data, we find that wild-type flies can instruct multiple batches of students across the 3 d tested (Figure 5A) (Kacsoh et al. 2015b). We find that Gug heterozygotes (Gug/+) have wild-type teaching ability on d 1, which quickly decays on d 2 and 3 (Figure 5B). To again probe for neuronal specificity, we turned to the MB GeneSwitch line. We find that outcrossed GugRNAi flies have wild-type teaching behavior across each time-point tested (Figure S5). When the GugRNAi is expressed in conjunction with the MB GeneSwitch line fed vehicle only, we observe wild-type teaching behavior across all 3 d (Figure 5C). When this line is fed RU486, allowing for expression of GugRNAi transgene, we find wild-type teaching ability on d 1, but find decaying teaching ability across d 2 and 3, similar to the heterozygote (Figure 5D and Figure S6). Collectively, the data suggests that memory of the wasp exposure must be maintained in order to exhibit persistent teaching behavior. Gug is not required for teaching behavior on d 1, where the flies still depress oviposition. Once the memory begins to its erasure, we observe the accelerated decay in teaching behavior. Interestingly, in Gug deficient flies, we observe no difference between exposed and unexposed oviposition on d 2. However, these flies are still able to teach, albeit not as efficiently as wild-type. By d 3, Gug deficient flies are unable to teach naïve students, while wild-type flies are still efficient teachers (Figure 5 and Figure S6). The data suggest that there may be two circuits in the fly brain governing (1) wasp memory and (2) teaching behavior, both of which require Gug to maintain signaling.
Gug has a role in student behavior
We observe a memory retention phenotype when flies lack Gug in the MB. We observe no defect in memory formation. Given these results, we wished to ask whether a different type of learning and memory paradigm is also affected following Gug removal. To do this, we utilized the social learning paradigm described above, but used Gug deficient flies as students to see how they learn from wild-type teachers. Since both Gug heterozygotes and RNAi depleted Drosophila were able to perceive and respond to wasp presence and form the initial memory, we hypothesized that Gug deficient flies may also be able to learn from wasp-exposed teacher flies. An alternative hypothesis is that learning in the context of social interactions with other flies could be different from learning that takes place directly from wasp-exposure. In this latter case, the molecular, cellular, and/or neuronal requirements for social learning might be different than those for nonsocial learning. We propose this given that the inputs, a wasp or a teacher fly, are very different for the observer fly given the differences in the visual, olfactory, and other understudied cues being detected.
In order to test the role of Gug in social learning, we modified the Fly Condo oviposition depression assay. We exposed His-RFP flies to wasps for 24-hr, followed by wasp removal. We then placed the exposed and unexposed teacher flies into new Fly Condos with three naïve female flies that were either wild-type, or Gug deficient, for an additional 24-hr. Following a 24-hr coincubation, food-plates were removed, and embryos were counted in a blinded manner.
Consistent with previous results, we find that wild-type flies are able to learn from His-RFP teacher flies (Figure 6A) (Kacsoh et al. 2015b). Interestingly, we find that Gug heterozygous (Gug/+) students have an impaired social learning ability when compared to wild-type students (Figure 6B). These flies are still able to learn from teachers, but at a much less efficient rate when compared to wild-type students. We wished to test the role of Gug in the MB in a social learning context. We find that the outcrossed GugRNAi show wild-type learning ability (Figure S7). When expressed in combination with the MB GeneSwitch line and fed vehicle only, we observe wild-type learning (Figure 6C). When this line is fed RU486, inducing GugRNAi transgene expression, we observe an impaired social learning ability, similar to the heterozygote (Figure 6D). We note that the ability to learn from a teacher is not ablated in Gug-deficient flies, but instead impaired (Figure S8). In other assays used, the d 1 phenotype has always been similar to wild-type, but in this assay, there is a distinct difference in the d 1 phenotype of these deficient flies. Collectively, these data strongly suggest that Gug has an important function in social learning and information processing. Again, this protein function occurs specifically in the MB information-processing center of the fly brain. The data also suggest that the type of learning taking place from wasp exposure may be fundamentally different from the type of learning taking place during social learning.
Discussion
In this study, we have presented the use of a machine-learning platform, PILGRM, in concert with a functional network analysis tool, IMP, to identify and evaluate a novel regulator of learning and memory in Drosophila. We identified Gug, a transcriptional repressor known to complex with the Rpd3/HDAC histone deacetylase (Fitzsimons and Scott 2011). To our knowledge, Gug itself has not been previously implicated to function in Drosophila memory.
We tested Gug’s role in memory formation and memory retention through two unique assays. Through these assays we find wild-type memory formation following training. However, memory retention is severely perturbed when the protein is missing in the fly mushroom body. We note that this function in memory retention for Gug is different from what has previously been reported for Rpd3, and therefore constitutes a novel function for Gug specifically in maintenance of long-term memory, rather than formation. Our observation raises the interesting possibility that the HDAC activity of Rpd3 recruited to chromatin by Gug could also be important in memory retention. It is important to note that the memory defects in Gug deficient flies we observe are not simply a result of developmental defects, given the data utilizing the MB GeneSwitch lines. In these lines, the depletion of Gug in adult MB neurons takes place after cell division and development has been completed. These data suggest that Gug has an active role in post-mitotic neurons throughout memory retention and/or retrieval in the fly MB, most likely via its transcriptional repressor function.
We highlight that memory formation is not affected in flies lacking Gug, as the d 0 and d 1 data looks like wild-type. This also lack of difference also suggests that the sensory cues required to identify a wasp as a threat, visual or otherwise, are intact in these flies lacking Gug. Thus, these flies are able to process the relevant input information from the wasp signal to form a memory. Thus, Gug is not required to detect the input or to make the initial memory. This result also suggests that the wiring and development of the fly brain remains intact to produce a functioning brain, given that the initial signal and interpretation are wild-type. We observe phenotypic differences on d 2 and 3, where we find the rapid extinction of memory, or accelerated memory decay. Wild-type flies show strong memory retention following the exposure, but in each of the assays we use, we find total memory ablation by d 3.
Additionally, we find that perturbations in Gug yield differences in social behavior. We find that teaching behavior in Gug-deficient flies that is normal on d 1 when compared to wild-type. As the memory decays in these flies, so does the teaching behavior, suggesting that the retention of both memory and teaching behavior is modulated by Gug activity. Of greatest interest, we find that social learning is also perturbed in Gug deficient flies, where students are not able to effectively gain information from wild-type teacher flies. These flies still have an ability to function as a student, but this ability is severely impaired. This observation is important because it suggests that molecular requirements for learning in a social context from experienced flies may require fundamentally different molecular and/or neuronal circuitry, and it is therefore distinct from learning in nonsocial contexts. Interestingly, humans with trinucleotide expansions within the Atrophin 1 gene, the human Gug homolog, exhibit autism-like behaviors (Licht and Lynch 2002). Thus, the observation of a social learning deficiency in flies could serve as a model for studying autism.
Recent work has shown that visual cues are sufficient to elicit a wasp-response and a teacher-student response (Kacsoh et al. 2013, 2015b). In this study, for each assay used, we provided direct interaction between insects (either fly-wasp or teacher-student), where visual, olfactory, tactile, and auditory cues may be exchanged. Thus, we cannot rule out the possibility that Gug may have an effect on certain sensory components of the nervous system. It remains possible that a chromatin transcriptional repressor may alter the excitability of the neurons involved in each of these circuits to make the neuron less excitable. A less excitable neuron may lead to a less responsive fly, where wild-type flies achieve maximal excitation and form a stable memory, while Gug deficient flies are trained to a lesser degree resulting in an unstable memory that decays faster. This lowered neuron excitation may also account for the student-teacher interaction, where the Gug deficient flies do not learn as effectively as wild-type due to sensory dampening. This alternative hypothesis does not affect the conclusion that Gug is required in the MB for memory retention, only that Gug might also function elsewhere in the learning circuitry. Future experiments that utilize isolated sensory stimuli would be able to examine these possibilities.
Previous work using a murine hippocampal model demonstrates a role for histone acetylation and subsequent expression of neuronal genes involved in memory formation (Mews et al. 2017). In mammals, Sp3 has been shown to recruit HDAC2 to synaptic specific genes, without interfering in other HDAC2 gene targets (Yamakawa et al. 2017). Together, these findings suggest the presence of specific recruitment factors that dictate the acetylation state of cell-type specific genes. In Drosophila, Gug (Atrophin) may act in a similar way by recruiting HDAC1/2 in neurons specifically (Licht and Lynch 2002). We propose a model where histone acetylation and deacetylation are acting in concert to promote neuronal plasticity (Figure 7). Given our results, we hypothesize that deacetylase activity is important in the memory retrieval process, where certain gene activity is turned off, or repressed. This repression may help elongate the memory retention, as without deacetlyase activity, we observe accelerated memory decay. The targets of Gug in the MB may serve as memory extinction genes (Abel and Lattal 2001; Rudenko et al. 2013), or genes that promote memory decay, and repression of these genes promotes memory maintenance. Our data also suggest that histone deacetylase activity is dispensable for memory formation, suggesting that other mechanisms govern the initial input acquisition.
Collectively, our data highlights the value of machine-learning approaches for biological function prediction via biologist-driven analyses. PILGRM enables data-driven experimentation in conjunction with knowledge-based discovery through the selection of a biologically relevant gold standard. Given that PILGRM utilizes a large collection of public gene expression data, it may identify genes that are not differentially regulated in an investigator’s own experiment, but act in unison with curated genes of interest. It can also identify consistent patterns across a compendium. This approach may help to identify regulators where small, potentially nonstatistically significant changes in transcript abundances can have biologically meaningful changes. These changes can be subtle, yet could lead to neuron plasticity. Epigenetic mechanisms are continually found to be important regulators of neuronal plasticity and functional output. These mechanisms are implicated in maintaining neuronal homeostasis, and perturbations have been implicated in neuropsychiatric diseases (Walker et al. 2015; Mews et al. 2017; Gräff and Tsai 2013; Kandel et al. 2014). Our study provides a previously uncharacterized role for Gug as a possible regulator of neuronal plasticity at the intersection of memory retention and memory extinction.
Supplementary Material
Supplemental material is available online at www.g3journal.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1534/g3.117.300172/-/DC1.
Acknowledgments
We thank Greg Roman, FlyBase, and the Bloomington Drosophila Stock Center, for stocks. We thank Sassan Hodge for technical assistance. We thank Julianna Bozler for technical and graphical assistance. We thank two anonymous reviewers for helpful comments on the manuscript. We acknowledge grants from Geisel School of Medicine at Dartmouth, a Gordon and Betty Moore Foundation Data-Driven Discovery Investigator grant GBMF4552 (C.S.G.), the National Institutes of Health Pioneer grant 1DP1MH110234 (G.B.), the National Science Foundation DBI-145830 (C.S.G. and G.B.), and the Defense Advanced Research Projects Agency grant HR0011-15-1-0002 (G.B.).
Footnotes
Communicating editor: A. Bashirullah
Literature Cited
- Abel T., Lattal K. M., 2001. Molecular mechanisms of memory acquisition, consolidation and retrieval. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 11: 180–187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aldrich B. T., Kasuya J., Faron M., Ishimoto H., Kitamoto T., 2010. The amnesiac gene is involved in the regulation of thermal nociception in Drosophila melanogaster. J. Neurogenet. 24: 33–41. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alem S., Perry C. J., Zhu X., Loukola O. J., Ingraham T., et al. , 2016. Associative mechanisms allow for social learning and cultural transmission of string pulling in an insect. PLoS Biol. 14: e1002564. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aso Y., Grübel K., Busch S., Friedrich A. B., Siwanowicz I., et al. , 2009. The mushroom body of adult Drosophila characterized by GAL4 drivers. J. Neurogenet. 23: 156–172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Battesti M., Moreno C., Joly D., Mery F., 2012. Spread of social information and dynamics of social transmission within Drosophila groups. Curr. Biol. 22: 309–313. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burcin M. M., Schiedner G., Kochanek S., Tsai S. Y., O’Malley B. W., 1999. Adenovirus-mediated regulable target gene expression in vivo. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 96: 355–360. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carton Y., Nappi A., 1997. Drosophila cellular immunity against parasitoids. Parasitol. Today 13: 218–227. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chabu C., Doe C. Q., 2009. Twins/PP2A regulates aPKC to control neuroblast cell polarity and self-renewal. Dev. Biol. 330: 399–405. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chanana B., Steigemann P., Jackle H., Vorbruggen G., 2009. Reception of slit requires only the chondroitin-sulphate-modified extracellular domain of Syndecan at the target cell surface. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 106: 11984–11988. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chikina M. D., Troyanskaya O. G., 2011. Accurate quantification of functional analogy among close homologs. PLoS Comput. Biol. 7: e1001074. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chikina M. D., Huttenhower C., Murphy C. T., Troyanskaya O. G., 2009. Global prediction of tissue-specific gene expression and context-dependent gene networks in Caenorhabditis elegans. PLoS Comput. Biol. 5: e1000417. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chintapalli V. R., Wang J., Dow J. A., 2007. Using FlyAtlas to identify better Drosophila melanogaster models of human disease. Nat. Genet. 39: 715–720. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Claridge-Chang A., Roorda R. D., Vrontou E., Sjulson L., Li H., et al. , 2009. Writing memories with light-addressable reinforcement circuitry. Cell 139: 405–415. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Das S., Sadanandappa M. K., Dervan A., Larkin A., Lee J. A., et al. , 2011. Plasticity of local GABAergic interneurons drives olfactory habituation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 108: E646–E654. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davis R. L., 2005. Olfactory memory formation in Drosophila: from molecular to systems neuroscience. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 28: 275–302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davis R. L., Zhong Y., 2017. The biology of forgetting—a perspective. Neuron 95: 490–503. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DeZazzo J., Sandstrom D., de Belle S., Velinzon K., Smith P., et al. , 2000. nalyot, a mutation of the Drosophila myb-related Adf1 transcription factor, disrupts synapse formation and olfactory memory. Neuron 27: 145–158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Donlea J., Leahy A., Thimgan M. S., Suzuki Y., Hughson B. N., et al. , 2012. Foraging alters resilience/vulnerability to sleep disruption and starvation in Drosophila. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 109: 2613–2618. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Driessen G., Hemerik L., Van Alphen J. J., 1989. Drosophila species, breeding in the stinkhorn (Phallus impudicus Pers.) and their larval parasitoids. Neth. J. Zool. 40: 409–427. [Google Scholar]
- Dubnau J., Tully T., 1998. Gene discovery in Drosophila: new insights for learning and memory. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 21: 407–444. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ejima A., Smith B. P., Lucas C., Levine J. D., Griffith L. C., 2005. Sequential learning of pheromonal cues modulates memory consolidation in trainer-specific associative courtship conditioning. Curr. Biol. 15: 194–206. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ejima A., Smith B. P. C., Lucas C., van der Goes van Naters W., Miller C. J., et al. , 2007. Generalization of courtship learning in Drosophila is mediated by cis-vaccenyl acetate. Curr. Biol. 17: 599–605. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Faith J. J., Hayete B., Thaden J. T., Mogno I., Wierzbowski J., et al. , 2007. Large-scale mapping and validation of Escherichia coli transcriptional regulation from a compendium of expression profiles. PLoS Biol. 5: e8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fitzsimons H. L., Scott M. J., 2011. Genetic modulation of Rpd3 expression impairs long-term courtship memory in Drosophila. PLoS One 6: e29171. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fleury F., Ris N., Allemand R., Fouillet P., Carton Y., et al. , 2004. Ecological and genetic interactions in Drosophila–parasitoids communities: a case study with D. melanogaster, D. simulans and their common Leptopilina parasitoids in South-eastern France. Genetica 120: 181–194. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gautier L., Cope L., Bolstad B. M., Irizarry R. A., 2004. affy–analysis of Affymetrix GeneChip data at the probe level. Bioinformatics 20: 307–315. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gentleman R. C., Carey V. J., Bates D. M., Bolstad B., Dettling M., et al. , 2004. Bioconductor: open software development for computational biology and bioinformatics. Genome Biol. 5: R80. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gräff J., Tsai L., 2013. Histone acetylation: molecular mnemonics on the chromatin. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 14: 97–111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greene C. S., Troyanskaya O. G., 2011. PILGRM: an interactive data-driven discovery platform for expert biologists. Nucleic Acids Res. 39: W368–W374. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greene C. S., Troyanskaya O. G., 2012. Accurate evaluation and analysis of functional genomics data and methods. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1260: 95–100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greene C. S., Tan J., Ung M., Moore J. H., Cheng C., 2014. Big data bioinformatics. J. Cell. Physiol. 229: 1896–1900. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greenspan R. J., 1995. Flies, genes, learning, and memory. Neuron 15: 747–750. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guo H. F., Zhong Y., 2006. Requirement of Akt to mediate long-term synaptic depression in Drosophila. J. Neurosci. 26: 4004–4014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huttenhower C., Schroeder M., Chikina M. D., Troyanskaya O. G., 2008. The Sleipnir library for computational functional genomics. Bioinformatics 24: 1559–1561. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Irizarry R. A., Gautier L., Cope L. M., 2003. An R package for analyses of Affymetrix oligonucleotide arrays, pp. 102–119 in The Analysis of Gene Expression Data: Methods and Software, edited by Parmigiani R. I. G., Garrett E. S., Ziegler S. Springer, Berlin. [Google Scholar]
- Iyengar B., Chou C. J., Vandamme K., Klose M., Zhao X., et al. , 2011. Silencing synaptic communication between random interneurons during Drosophila larval locomotion. Genes Brain Behav. 10: 883–900. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Joachims T., 2006. Training linear SVMs in linear time. Paper presented at the Proceedings of the 12th ACM SIGKDD International Conference on Knowledge Discovery and Data Mining, 217–226. [Google Scholar]
- Kacsoh B. Z., Lynch Z. R., Mortimer N. T., Schlenke T. A., 2013. Fruit flies medicate offspring after seeing parasites. Science 339: 947–950. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kacsoh B. Z., Bozler J., Hodge S., Ramaswami M., Bosco G., 2015a A novel paradigm for nonassociative long-term memory in Drosophila: predator-induced changes in oviposition behavior. Genetics 199: 1143–1157. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kacsoh B. Z., Bozler J., Ramaswami M., Bosco G., 2015b Social communication of predator-induced changes in Drosophila behavior and germ line physiology. Elife 4: e07423. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kandel E. R., Dudai Y., Mayford M. R., 2014. The molecular and systems biology of memory. Cell 157: 163–186. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kanellopoulos A. K., Semelidou O., Kotini A. G., Anezaki M., Skoulakis E. M., 2012. Learning and memory deficits consequent to reduction of the fragile X mental retardation protein result from metabotropic glutamate receptor-mediated inhibition of cAMP signaling in Drosophila. J. Neurosci. 32: 13111–13124. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Korzus E., Rosenfeld M. G., Mayford M., 2004. CBP histone acetyltransferase activity is a critical component of memory consolidation. Neuron 42: 961–972. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krüttner S., Stepien B., Noordermeer J. N., Mommaas M. A., Mechtler K., et al. , 2012. Drosophila CPEB Orb2A mediates memory independent of Its RNA-binding domain. Neuron 76: 383–395. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LaSalle J., 1993. Parasitic Hymenoptera, biological control and biodiversity, pp. 197–215 in Hymenoptera and Biodiversity, edited by LaSalle J., Gauld I. D. CAB International, Wallingford, CT. [Google Scholar]
- Lefevre T., de Roode J. C., Kacsoh B. Z., Schlenke T. A., 2012. Defence strategies against a parasitoid wasp in Drosophila: fight or flight? Biol. Lett. 8: 230–233. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li W., Cressy M., Qin H., Fulga T., Van Vactor D., et al. , 2013. MicroRNA-276a functions in ellipsoid body and mushroom body neurons for naive and conditioned olfactory avoidance in Drosophila. J. Neurosci. 33: 5821–5833. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Licht D. J., Lynch D. R., 2002. Juvenile dentatorubral-pallidoluysian atrophy: new clinical features. Pediatr. Neurol. 26: 51–54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Loukola O. J., Perry C. J., Coscos L., Chittka L., 2017. Bumblebees show cognitive flexibility by improving on an observed complex behavior. Science 355: 833–836. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lynch Z. R., Schlenke T. A., de Roode J. C., 2016. Evolution of behavioural and cellular defences against parasitoid wasps in the Drosophila melanogaster subgroup. J. Evol. Biol. 29: 1016–1029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mao Z., Roman G., Zong L., Davis R. L., 2004. Pharmacogenetic rescue in time and space of the rutabaga memory impairment by using Gene-Switch. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 101: 198–203. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Margulies C., Tully T., Dubnau J., 2005. Deconstructing memory in Drosophila. Curr. Biol. 15: R700–R713. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Masse N. Y., Turner G. C., Jefferis G. S., 2009. Olfactory information processing in Drosophila. Curr. Biol. 19: R700–R713. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCann C., Holohan E. E., Das S., Dervan A., Larkin A., et al. , 2011. The Ataxin-2 protein is required for microRNA function and synapse-specific long-term olfactory habituation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 108: E655–E662. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGuire S. E., Deshazer M., Davis R. L., 2005. Thirty years of olfactory learning and memory research in Drosophila melanogaster. Prog. Neurobiol. 76: 328–347. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mehren J. E., Griffith L. C., 2006. Cholinergic neurons mediate CaMKII-dependent enhancement of courtship suppression. Learn. Mem. 13: 686–689. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mews P., Donahue G., Drake A. M., Luczak V., Abel T., et al. , 2017. Acetyl-CoA synthetase regulates histone acetylation and hippocampal memory. Nature 546: 381–386. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parrish J. Z., Kim M. D., Jan L. Y., Jan Y. N., 2006. Genome-wide analyses identify transcription factors required for proper morphogenesis of Drosophila sensory neuron dendrites. Genes Dev. 20: 820–835. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perazzona B., Isabel G., Preat T., Davis R. L., 2004. The role of cAMP response element-binding protein in Drosophila long-term memory. J. Neurosci. 24: 8823–8828. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ramaswami M., 2014. Network plasticity in adaptive filtering and behavioral habituation. Neuron 82: 1216–1229. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rudenko A., Dawlaty M. M., Seo J., Cheng A. W., Meng J., et al. , 2013. Tet1 is critical for neuronal activity-regulated gene expression and memory extinction. Neuron 79: 1109–1122. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scheunemann L., Skroblin P., Hundsrucker C., Klussmann E., Efetova M., et al. , 2013. AKAPS act in a two-step mechanism of memory acquisition. J. Neurosci. 33: 17422–17428. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schlenke T. A., Morales J., Govind S., Clark A. G., 2007. Contrasting infection strategies in generalist and specialist wasp parasitoids of Drosophila melanogaster. PLoS Pathog. 3: e158. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schwaerzel M., Monastirioti M., Scholz H., Friggi-Grelin F., Birman S., et al. , 2003. Dopamine and octopamine differentiate between aversive and appetitive olfactory memories in Drosophila. J. Neurosci. 23: 10495–10502. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Siegel R., Hall J., 1979a Conditioned courtship in Drosophila and its mediation by association of chemical cues. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 76: 3430–3434. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Siegel R. W., Hall J. C., 1979b Conditioned responses in courtship behavior of normal and mutant Drosophila. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 76: 3430–3434. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spradling A. C., Stern D., Beaton A., Rhem E. J., Laverty T., et al. , 1999. The Berkeley Drosophila genome project gene disruption project: single P-element insertions mutating 25% of vital Drosophila genes. Genetics 153: 135–177. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tully T., 1987. Drosophila learning and memory revisited. Trends Neurosci. 10: 330–335. [Google Scholar]
- Tully T., Preat T., Boynton S., Del Vecchio M., 1994. Genetic dissection of consolidated memory in Drosophila. Cell 79: 35–47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vargas M. A., Luo N., Yamaguchi A., Kapahi P., 2010. A role for S6 kinase and serotonin in postmating dietary switch and balance of nutrients in D. melanogaster. Curr. Biol. 20: 1006–1011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walker D. M., Cates H. M., Heller E. A., Nestler E. J., 2015. Regulation of chromatin states by drugs of abuse. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 30: 112–121. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang L., Rajan H., Pitman J. L., McKeown M., Tsai C. C., 2006. Histone deacetylase-associating Atrophin proteins are nuclear receptor corepressors. Genes Dev. 20: 525–530. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang L., Charroux B., Kerridge S., Tsai C. C., 2008. Atrophin recruits HDAC1/2 and G9a to modify histone H3K9 and to determine cell fates. EMBO Rep. 9: 555–562. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wong A. K., Park C. Y., Greene C. S., Bongo L. A., Guan Y., et al. , 2012. IMP: a multi-species functional genomics portal for integration, visualization and prediction of protein functions and networks. Nucleic Acids Res. 40: W484–W490. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wong A. K., Krishnan A., Yao V., Tadych A., Troyanskaya O. G., 2015. IMP 2.0: a multi-species functional genomics portal for integration, visualization and prediction of protein functions and networks. Nucleic Acids Res. 43: W128–W133. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wood M. A., Kaplan M. P., Park A., Blanchard E. J., Oliveira A. M., et al. , 2005. Transgenic mice expressing a truncated form of CREB-binding protein (CBP) exhibit deficits in hippocampal synaptic plasticity and memory storage. Learn. Mem. 12: 111–119. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yamakawa H., Cheng J., Penney J., Gao F., Rueda R., et al. , 2017. The transcription factor Sp3 cooperates with HDAC2 to regulate synaptic function and plasticity in neurons. Cell Rep. 20: 1319–1334. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yan H., Venkatesan K., Beaver J. E., Klitgord N., Yildirim M. A., et al. , 2010. A genome-wide gene function prediction resource for Drosophila melanogaster. PLoS One 5: e12139. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yeung K., Boija A., Karlsson E., Holmqvist P. H., Tsatskis Y., et al. , 2017. Atrophin controls developmental signaling pathways via interactions with Trithorax-like. Elife 6: e23084. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang S., Xu L., Lee J., Xu T., 2002. Drosophila atrophin homolog functions as a transcriptional corepressor in multiple developmental processes. Cell 108: 45–56. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang Z., Feng J., Pan C., Lv X., Wu W., et al. , 2013. Atrophin-Rpd3 complex represses Hedgehog signaling by acting as a corepressor of CiR. J. Cell Biol. 203: 575–583. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zovkic I. B., Guzman-Karlsson M. C., Sweatt J. D., 2013. Epigenetic regulation of memory formation and maintenance. Learn. Mem. 20: 61–74. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
Stocks are available upon request or from the Bloomington Drosophila Stock Center, with stock numbers listed in Table S1. File S1 contains all inputs used for PILGRM analysis. File S2 contains the full data set for the PILGRM analysis output. File S3 contains legends for all Supplemental Material.